You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/330179367

The Origin of Man

Chapter · September 2017


DOI: 10.4324/9781315127408-44

CITATIONS READS
13 4,688

1 author:

owen lovejoy
Kent State University
192 PUBLICATIONS 20,218 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by owen lovejoy on 01 May 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


23 January 1981, Volume 211, Number 4480 SCIE: NCE

left no record because of the use of per-


ishable materials, there is still the neces-
sary presumption of a 6- to 10-million-
year period dominated by reliance on
material culture-a view with numerous
shortcomings.
The Origin of M The use of p.imitive tools by extant
pongids (13) supports the contention of
C. Owen Lov ejoy comparable abilit;es in early hominids,
but it also demonst'ates that tool use is a

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


general capacity of pongids, none of
which exhibit the unique characters of
During the last quarter-century, the Material Culture hominids (14). If tools were the primary
study of human origins has proved re- determinant of early hominization, why-
markably successful. Crucial fossils and The most commonly icited distinction should their first appearance be so late in
primate behavioral data are now avail- between man and apes is the former's re- the hominid record? More importantly,
able from which to reconstruct man's liance on material culturn e. The beliefthat what activity requiring tools was"critical
e-volution during the last 15 million tools were pivotal to thie divergence of to early hominid survival and phyletic
years. Equally important is the recogni- hominids was initiated b: y Darwin (3) and origin (15)? It is now clear that hunting
tion of a close genetic relationship be- has remained the most Xpopular view (4- does not qualify as such an activity (16).
tween man and the other extant homi- 6). Darwin was impres;sed by the ab- From the first recognizable tools to the
noids (especially Pan and Gorilla) (1). sence of large canines ini man and attrib- industrial revolution required only 2 mil-
lion years, whereas if tools played a part
in the origin of hominids, they must have
Summary. Five characters separate man from other hominoids-a lrge neocortex, remained primitive and unchanged for at
bipedality, reduced anterior dentition with molar dominance, matetrial culture, and least 5 million years. It is likely that ei-
unique sexual and reproductive behavior. Evidence provided by tlhe fossil record, ther the earliest hominids made no use of
primate behavior, and demographic analysis shows that the traditionwal view that early tools at all, or that such use was com-
human evolution was a direct consequence of brain expansion and nnaterial culture is pamble to that in other extant hominoids
incorrect, and that the unique sexual and reproductive behavior of man may be the and was not critical to their survival or
sine qua non of human origin. pivotal to their origin.

Experiments on DNA hybridization in- uted their reduction to tool use. As Hol- Exnsin of the Neocortex
dicate at least 98 percent identity in non- loway (7) and Jolly (8) have cogently ar-
repeated DNA in man and chimpanzee, gued, however, tool use is not an ex- It is now clear that the marked expan-
sufficient similarity to suggest the possi- planation of canine reduction since there sion of the hominid cerebral cortex took
biliyf a viable hybrid. These data con- is no behavioral contradiction in having place during the last 2 to 3 million years
firm studies by comparative anatomists both functional canines and tools. There (17, 18). Detailed study of the Hadar
who have emphasized the striking ana- is little doubt that material culture has crania from Ethiopia, recently attributed
tomical similarities of apes and man (2). played a role in the evolution of Homo to Australopithecus afarensis (19), has
As a consequence of this physical simi- sapiens and H. erectus, but this does not revealed that they were strikingly primi-
larity, models of human origin must di- require it to have been a significant fac- tive (20). Preliminary estimates of cranial
rectly address the few primary dif- tor in the origin of hominids. In fact, the capacity indicate a brain size well within
ferences separating humans from apes. earliest recognizable tools are only about the range of extant pongids (21). The pel-
Clearly, the rate of acquisition of these 2 million years old (9), but there is con- vis of the skeleton known as "Lucy"
differences, the fossil evidence bearing siderable evidence placing the phyletic from Afar Locality (A.L.) 288 has been
in their first appearance, and their un- origin of hominids in the middle to late fully reconstructed (22). One of its most
derlying selection are crucial to an un- Miocene (12 to 6 million years ago) (10- salient features is a birth canal whose
derstanding of human evolution. 12). Although the earliest tools will have shape and dimensions show little or no
Dr. Lovejoy is professor ot anthropology in the Department of Sociology and Anthropology, Kent State effects of selection for passage of en-
University, Kent, Ohio 44242; professor of human anatomy at the Northeat Ohio Universities Coilege of larged fetal crania, adaptations that so
Medicine; assistant clinical professor of orthopedic surgery at Case Western Reserve University; researchclearly dominate the form of the modem
aociate, Cleveland Museum of Natural History; and chaiman of the Biological Anthropology Area Com-
mittee, division of biomedical sciences, Kent State University. human pelvis (23, 24).
SqIENCE, VOL. 211, 23 JANUARY 1981 003648075/81/0123-0341$02.000 Copyright 0 1981 AAAS 341
Bipedality shear. This phase is associated with for- dominance and became extinct by mid-
est faunas and floras (31) and is shared Pleistocene (A. africanus -- A. robus-
Bipedality is an unusual mode of mam- by all hominoids before 15 million years tus -. "A. boisei"). A second clade, an-
malian locomotion. Contrary to the so- ago (range, 23 to 15 million years) (30- cestral to H. erectus, retained a more
called efficiency argument, energy ex- 32). generalized dentition in the early Pleisto.
penditure for bipedal walking is probably Phase II. This phase shows a shift to- cene but underwent dentognathic reduc-
not significantly different from that dur- ward greater molar dominance. About 14 tion in the middle and upper Pleistocene
ing quadrupedal locomotion (23, 25). Yet million years ago, hominoids fall into as a consequence of reliance on material
the adoption of nonsaltatory bipedal pro- two groups. The first retained phase I culture [for example, reduced dental ma-
grssion is disadvantageous because characters and may constitute ancestral nipulation and greater preoral food prep-
both speed and agility are markedly re- populations of extant apes (Proconsul, aration (3S)]. My view is that this clade
duced (23, 24, 26). All present evidence, "'Rangwapithecus," and Limnopi- occupied more varied habitats. Both
especially that made available by the thecus, Dryopithecus) (30). A second groups are probably directly descendant
postcranium of A. afarensis, confirms an group exhibits enamel thickening, in- from A. afarensis (17).
essentially complete adaptation to biped- creased molar wear gradient, and moder-
al locomotion by at least 4 million years ate anterior dental reduction or in-
ago (22, 27). This conclusion is provided creased relative molar size, or both. Models of Human Origin
unequivocal support by the hominid Mandibles are more robust and progna-
footprints discovered at Laetoli in Tan- thism is reduced. The shift toward great- A model of hominid origin proposed
zania (28). er molar dominance has partially been by Jolly (8) uses analogy to anatomical
attributed to greater reliance on terrestri- and behavioral characters shared by
al food sources. This group includes gen- Theropithecus gelada and some early

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


Detfiton era (Ramapithecus and Sivapithecus) hominids. He suggests that early homi-
probably related to hominids, an extinct nid populations relied on small-object
Additional distinctions between homi- ape (Gigantopithecus), and possibly the feeding, that this dietary specialization
nids and pongids are found in their re- modern orang-utan (range, 14 to 8 mil- led to a suite of adaptations to the grass-
spective jaws and teeth. In fact, these lion years) (12, 30-34). land savannah, and that bipedality devel-
differences have allowed the identifica- Phase III. This phase represents a oped in response to feeding posture. Yet
tion of possible hominids in the Mio- conservative period. The dentition of A. geladas, which do rely on small-object
cen'4-there are no distinctive post- afarensis appears only moderately feeding, are not bipedal and show no sig-
crtnial or cranial remains of undoubted changed in morphology and proportions nifiant adaptations to bipedality. Biped-
hominid affinities before about 4 million from phase II; the features include com- al locomotion is clearly not required for
years ago. As a result of recent field paratively large incisors, fiequently a extensive small-object feeding especially
work in Mio-Pliocene deposits (29, 30), it unicuspid lower first premolar, canines on grasslands where speed and agility
is now possible to suggest a broad sched- of moderate size, molars of moderate are of great value in animals who also
ule of phases in the evolution of the hom- size (relative to body size and later homi- lack wide visual fields and sensitive ol-
inoid dentition that can serve as an out- nids), and loss of canine-premolar shear faction (36). Furthermore, the dental
line of hominoid phyletic events during (range, 7 to 2'/2 million years) (12, 17). morphology of A. afarensis is consid-
the last 23 million years. Phase IV. This phase represents Plio- erably more generalized than that of later
Phase I. This phase has a generaled Pleistocene specialization. The sample in hominids. The dietary specialization
dryopithecine dentition including a dis- this time range is divisible into two seen in A. robustus is possibly account-
tinct Y-5 lower molar cusp pattern with clades or phyletic lines (17). The first able by Jolly's model, but the more gen-
bunodont crowns, thin enamel, and was possibly restricted to savannah and eralized dentition of A. afarensis is not
cheek teeth small relative to body size; grassland. It displays extreme anterior (37). It is more likely that hominids ven-
incisors are broad with canine-premolar tooth reduction and excessive molar turing into open habitats were already bi-
pedal and that their regular occupation of
savannahs was not possible until in-
tensified social behavior was well devel-

70
'li>&iil
-
c
a
3
oped.
Other theorists have viewed hominiza-
tion as the direct result of savannah oc-
cupation by prehominids. Proponents of

WEI
0
I I 11- I
I
I
Post repro-
duction
Fig. 1. Progressive prolonga-
tion of life phases and gesta-
tion in primates. Note the pro-
portionality of the four in-
this view believe that the selective pres-
sures of life on grassland savannahs di-
rectly produced the human charcter
complex. Bipedal locomotion is posited
I-
Adult dicated phases. The post-re- as sentinel behavior and as an adaptation
productive phase is restricted allowing weapons to be used against
to man and is probably a re- predators. Intelligence is said to be fa-
cent development (101, after vored because highly integrated troop
102). behavior is necessary for predator repul-
Subaduft sion. Differences in some behaviors of
chimpanzee populations now living in
nfancy woodland savannahs versus those in-
1241 ELi[ 1I_JLI >Gestation habiting more forested areas are cited as
evidence (38).
There are many problems with this
view. Bipedality is useless for avoidance Fig. 2. Mechanical model of Bkth Get
or escape from predators. Occasional bi- demographic variables in Lo viSex space
pedality, as seen in many primates, is hominoids. The R is the in- oefant
sufficient for the use of weapons. Most
trinsic rate of population in- .Rpermat 1 -R
crease (I = static population A
importantly, brain expansion and cultur- size). An increase in the
al development remotely postdate homi- lengths of the four periods
nid divergence. on the bar to the right (birth space, gestation, infant dependency, and sexual maturity) is accom-
panied by a comparable shift of longevity to the left, but without realization of that longevity,
Furthermore, Miocene ecology is in- prolonged maturation reduces R and leads to extinction or replacement by-populations in which
consistent with the savannah selection life phases are chronologically shorter. Of the four variables on the right, only birth space can
theory. While cooling, aridity, and in- be significantly shortened (shifted to the left) without alteration of primate aging physiology.
creased seasonality had pronounced ef-
fects on Old World floras, the pre-
dominant effect of these climatic trends, development of major adaptive trends. tain a stable population, she must sur-
in areas where hominids are known to Figure 1 is a well-known diagram of the vive to an age of 21 years (48). Whereas
have been present, appears to have been chronology of life phases in living pri- in rhesus macaques, the age is only
the development of diversified mosaics, mates. There is an obvious trend toward about 9 years (49, 50).
rather than broad-scale forest reduction prolonged life-span, which has both Figure 2 shows a balance depicting the
(39-41). It would be more correct to say physiological and demographic corre- reciprocal relation between longevity
that hominids of the middle and late Mio- lates bearing directly on the phyletic ori- and the primary demographic elements
cene were presented with a greater varie- gin of hominids. of parental investment. The two sides of
ty of possible habitats than to view them The physiological correlates (Fig. 1) this hypothetical balance are physiologi-
as having suffered an imposed "'terrestri- include a longer period of infant de- cally interdependent; as longevity is in-

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


alization." It is also clear that some Mio- pendency, prolonged gestation, single creased, each of the developmental
cene sites at which possible hominids births, and successively greater periods stages is proportionately prolonged. The
have been recovered had canopy forest between pregnancies. Cutler (44) has relationships between these variables, in
conditions (12, 42). While increased sea- demonstrated that such developmental fact, are not exactly linear, but they do
sonality would have imposed a need for parameters are ""qualitatively and se- have remarkably high correlations in
larger feeding ranges, occasional use quentially similar in different mammalian most mammals (44). As the scale in-
of woodlands and edaphic grasslands species" but proceed "at different char- dicates, greater longevity is accom-
would not necessarily impose elevated acteristic rates defined by the reciprocal panied by a proportionate delay in repro-
carnivore pressure. Nor, as was pointed of their MLP" (maximum life potential). ductive rate and therefore requires a fe-
out above, would early hominids be re- The progressive slowing of life phases male to survive to an older age in order
quired to abandon quadrupedality in or- can in turn be accounted for by an in- to maintain the same reproductive value
der to use more orthograde positional be- creasingly K-type demographic strategy (measured at birth) (Si). Put another
havior during feeding. Quite the con- (45). With each step in the scala naturae, way, the total reproductive rate of a pri-
try, it would appear that late Miocene populations devote a greater proportion mate species can remain constant with
habitat mosaics would allow adoption of of their reproductive energy to subadult progressive increases in longevity only if
bipedality (in forests and transition mo- care, with increased investment in the the crude mortality rate is correspond-
saics) rather than directly select for it. survival of fewer offspring. Among chim- ingly reduced. Actual mortality rate is
All present evidence therefore indicates panzee populations, this trend appears to dependent on both maximum life poten-
that hominid clade evolved in forest or have resulted in marginal demographic tial, a genetic factor, and environmental
mosaic conditions, or both (43), rather conditions. Field studies at Gombe in interaction. Deaths caused by predation,
than only on grassland or savannahs. and Tanzania show the average period be- accident, parasitism, infection, failure of
that bipedal locomotion was not a re- tween successful births to be 5.6 years food supply, and so forth, are at least
sponse to feeding posture, material cul- (46). This can be attributed in part to a partially stochastic events beyond the
ture, or predator avoidance. greatly prolonged period of subadult de- complete control ofthe organism. Only if
In summary, four major characterf* pendency. Van Lawick-Goodall's (47) mechanisms are developed to increase
complexes are usually cited as distin- description of the chimpanzee life phases an organism's resistance to such factors,
guishing hominids from pongids. Homi- is instructive: can the effects of increased longevity be
nids have remarkable brain expansion, a reproductively accommodated. Strong
complex material culture, anterior dental The infant does not start to walk until he is six social bonds, high levels of intelligence,
months old, and he seldom ventures more
reduction and molar dominance, and bi- than a few yards from his mother until he is intense parenting, and long periods of
pedal locomotion. Only bipedal locomo- over nine months old. He may ingest a few learning are among factors used by high-
tion and partial dental modifications can scraps of solid food when he is six months, er primates to depress environmentally
be shown to have an antiquity even ap- but solids do not become a significant part of induced mortality. It is of some interest
proximating the earliest appearance of his diet until he is about two years of age and that such factors
he continues to nurse until he is between four- also require greater lon-
unquestioned, developed hominids (A. and-a-half and six years old. Moreover, while gevity (for brain development, learning,
afarensis). he may travel short distances ... when he is acquisition of social and parenting skills)
about four years old, he continues to make and that they constitute reciprocal links
long journeys riding on his mother's back un- leading to greater longevity. This posi-
til he is five or six....
Demographic Strategy and the tive feedback system, however, has an
Evolution of Homrinids This extreme degree of parental invest- absolute limit; environmentally induced
ment has profound demographic con- mortality can never be completely under
The order Primates has long been rec- sequences. A chimpanzee female does organism control, no matter how ef-
ognized to display a scala naturae con- not reach sexual maturity until she is fective the mechanisms developed to re-
sisting of ""intercalary types" extant about 10 years old (46). If she is to repro- sist it.
forms that represent earlier stages in the duce herself and her mate, that is, main- Suppose that late Miocene hominoids
were approaching the effective limit of change in reproductive strategy accom- the altricial chimpanzee infant. The feed-
this feedback system or at least were suf- panied occupation of novel environ- ing and reproductive strategies of higher
ficiently near the limit not to thrive in ments by these hominids. Yet neither primates, however, largely prevent such
novel environments (52). Two demo- brain expansion nor significant material an advancement. The mother must both
graphic variables could be altered to im- culture appear at this time level and were care for the infant and forge for herself.
prove reproductive success-survivor- therefore not responsible for this shift. A common method of altricial infant care
ship (the probability of surviving) and in other mammals is sequestration of
the time period between successive offspring at locations of maximum
births (the birth space). All other factors A Behavioral Model for safety. Nests, lodges, setts, warrens,
are direct linear functions of mammalian Early Hominid Evolution dreys, dens, lairs, and burrows are ex-
developmental physiology and could not amples of this strategy. A similar adap-
be altered. The argument is subject to Any behavioral change that increases tation in primates is usually not possi-
the following simple quantification reproductive rate, survivorship, or both, ble, however, because the need to forage
MNLP
is under selection of maximum intensity. requires both mother and infant to re-
RV = i(s) J l(x)b(x)dx (1) Higher primates rely on social behavior- main mobile. The requirement of mother-
al mechanisms to promote survivorship infant mobility is a significant cause of
during all phases of the life cycle, and mortality and is at the same time the
where RV it reproductive value of a co- one could cite numerous methods by most important restriction on primate
hort measured at birth, that is, the ex- which it theoretically could be increased. birth spacing. X
pected number of offspring produced by Avoidance of dietary toxins, use of more Many primates display significant sex
a unit radix; b(x) is fertility at age x; 1(x) reliable food sources, and increased differences in foraging. Diet composi-.
is survivorship at age x; s is age at sexual competence in arboreal locomotion are tion, selection of food items, feeding

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


maturation; and MLP = maximum life obvious examples. Yet these are among time, and canopy levels and sites dffer in
potential. Assuming that a female gives many that have remained under strong some species (55). In at least Pongo pyg.
birth at age s years and subsequently selection throughout much of the course maeus and Colobus badius, males often
every P (birth space) years until reaching of primate evolution, and it is therefore feed at lower canopy levels than females
MLP, her total offspring would be given unlikely that early hominid adaptation (56-58). In the gelada baboon, all-male
by was a product of intensified selection for groups "tended not to exploit quite the
MLP-s adaptations almost universal to anthro- same areas as the reproductive units thus
(2) poid primates. For early hominids we reducing indirect competition for food"
must look beyond such common vari- (59). Clutton-Brock (55) notes that an in-
Fertility is then seen to be dependent on ables to novel forms of behavioral creased separation of males from female-
birth space P8 according to change. The tendency has been to con- offspring foraging sites is advantageous
centrate on singular, extraordinary traits where (i) animals feed outward from a
of later human evolution such as intense fixed base, (ii) the adult sex ratio is close
b(x)dx= MLPs (3) technology, organized hunting, and the to parity, and (iii) feeding rate is limited
massive human brain. Yet these adapta- by search time rather than by handling
A simple solution (but one which is fully tions were not likely to have arisen de time, which is the time spent both pre-
acceptable because of the proportionate novo from elemental behaviors seen in paring and consuming food. Similar feed-
relation between MLP and s) is b(r) = extant nonhuman primates, such as the ing differences by sex are found in birds
1/p. The expression for RV then be- primitive tool using of the chimpanzee, and other mammals (60).
comes in the absence of a broad selective mi- It is reasonable to assume that Mio-
lieu. It is more probable that significant cene hominoids traveled between food
RV *-[l(s) I l(x)dx] (4) preadaptations were present in early sources on the ground and that these pri-
hominids that served as a behavioral mates would be best characterized as
base from which the "breakthrough" ad- omnivores (12). These are ecologically
Because the term in brackets is inde- aptations (53) of later hominids could sound assumptions. Increased season-
pendent of P, RV is inversely propor- progessively develop. We are therefore ality coupled with already occurring lo-
tional to 8, and RV is increased by a in search of a novel behavioral pattern in cal biotic variation (edaphic grasslands,
shorter birth space, by greater values of Miocene hominoids that could evolve savannah, woodland, forest) (8, 12, 39-
I(x) for any age, or by both. Table 1 pro- from typical primate survival strategies, 41) would have presented variable and
vides reproductive values for chim- but that might also include important ele- mosaic conditions. Occupation of heter-
panzees, Old World monkeys, and man ments of other mammalian strategies, ogeneous ("patchy") environments and
from estimated values of (, s, and MLP that is, a behavioral pattern that arose by use of variable food sources favors a
under the simplifying assumption of recombination of common mammalian generalist strategy, whereas reliance on
1(x) = 1x. It can be seen from this table behavioral elements and that increased a homogeneous diet requires high food
that both chimpanzees and humans have survivorship and birthrate. concentrations (61, 62). The time spent
considerably lower reproductive values In her essay on mother-infant relation- searching for food is greatest among gen-
than Old World monkeys for low values ships among chimpanzees, van Lawick- eralists who live in food-sparse environ-
of 1(x). As the values used for calculation Goodall (54) noted two primary causes of ments (63). In short, Miocene ecological
are conservative, the existence of suc- mortality among infants: "inadequacy" conditions support the view that feeding
cessful hominid clades in Pliocene mo- of the mother-infant relationship and rate would have been more dependent oil
saics suggests that both birth space re- "injuries caused by falling from the search time than handling time.
duction and elevation of survivorship mother." An intensification of both the Greater seasonality and the need to in-.
had probably been accomplished. This is quality and quantity of parenting would crease both birthrate and survivorship
without explanation unless a major unquestionably improve survivorship of would also favor at least partial separa-
344 SCIENCE, VOL. 211
tion of male and female day ranges since Table 1. Relative reproductive values of Old nutritional requirements for young and
this strategy would increase carrying ca- World primates calculated from Eq. 4 (see old and the seasonal changes in the quali-
pacity and improve the protein and calo- text) and multiplied by 10 for clarity. ty and quantity of available food." This
ne supply of females and their offspring. Reproductive values same statement could be as well applied
Terrestriality, however, would require a si- Old to early hominids, especially given in-
centrifugW or linear displacement of vorship World Chim- Mant creased Miocene seasonality and the
males, as opposed to vertical strati- monkeys* z St need for a decrease in birth spacing. The
fication in canopy feeding. Given the 17 4 2
altricial infants of Miocene hominoids,
Miocene conditions described above, 92 23 7 4 however, would have required reduced
such separation could become marked .94 31 13 9 mobility and therefore prevented a cal-
especially in the dry season. If such sep- .96 42 25 24 litrichid strategy of male care, with the
aration were primarily due only to an in- .98 58 50 64 simplest solution being the male provi-
crease in the male day range, moreover, *Maximum life potential 20; sexual maturky -4; sioning model proposed above.
the range of the female-offspring group birth
tential
spe- 2 (49, 50, 103).
40; sexual maturity
-
tMaximum hfe po-
10; birth spce
-
3 -

could be proportionately reduced by pro- (4654. 103). tMaximum lif potential - 60; sex-
-
gressive elimination of male competition ual maturity -
15; birth space 2.5. The Origin of Bipedalty
for local resources. This separation
would be under strong positive selection. Provisioning is, of course, the primary
Lowered mobility of females would re- them to the mate and offspring. Contrary parental care strategy of most canids and
duce accident rate during travel, maxi- to the opinion that such behavior would birds (69-71). Both groups exhibit direct
mize familiarity with the core area, re- be altruistic, it would not be so in the male involvement similar to that de-
duce exposure to predators, and allow proposed system, because it would only scribed for callitrichids. Their offspring

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


intensification of parenting behavior, benefit the biological offspring of the are normally immature at birth, immo-
thus elevating survivorship (64). Such a male carrying out the provisioning and bile, and require constant provisioning
division of feeding areas, however, thus would be under powerful, direct se- and parenting. In some species, a sexual
would not genetically favor males unless lection. If this behavior were to become division of labor, like that posited here
it specifically reduced competition with a regular componeq[_of the male's be- for early hominids, is observed. Female
their own biological offspring and did not havioral repertoire, (it would directly in- hornbils (Bucerotidae), for example, de-
reduce their opportunities for consort crease his reproductive rate by corre- pend totally on male provisioning for
relationships. Polygynous mating would spondingly improving the protein and their survival and that of their offspring.
not be favored by this adaptive strategy calorie supply of the female who could Monogamous pair bonding is character-
because the advantage of feeding diver- then accommodate greater gestational istic of 90 percent of bird species (70, 72)
gence is reduced as the number of males and lactation loads and intensify parent- and is the most common mating system
is reduced. Conversely, a sex ratio close ing (66). The behavior, would thus in provisioning canids (69). Both groups,
to parity would select for the proposed achieve both an increase in survivorship- as a fundamental feature of reproductive
feeding strategy. Such a ratio would ob- and a reduction in birth space. It would strategy, commonly sequester their off-
tain if the mating pattern were monoga- allow a progressive increase in the num- spring at home bases (73).
mous pair bonding. In this case, males ber of dependent offspring because their One critical difference separates provi-
would avoid competition with their nutritional and supervisory requirements sioning in birds and canids from that sug-
bonded mates and biological offspring could be met more adequately. gested for early hominids. Birds and can-
(by using alternative feeding sites) and Behaviors associated with similar re- ids can carry in their mouths or regurgi-
not be disadvantaged by physical separa- productive strategies are in fact present tate (or both) a significant proportion of
tion, that is, there would be no loss of in other primates. In both the Calli- their body weight. Oral carrying would
consort opportunity. In short, monoga- trichidae and Aotinae, extensive pater- have been inadequate for early homi-
mous pair bonding would favor feeding nal care of the young constitutes a criti- nids, however, and a strong selection for
divergence by "'assuring" males of bio- cal part of reproductive, strategy in some bipedality, which would allow provisions
logical paternity and by reducing feeding species (67, 68). Among callitrichids, the to be carried "by hand," would thus ac-
competition with their own offspring and social unit is usually an adult male and company provisioning behavior (74).
mates. female, plus one to several subadults. Chimpanzees are fully capable of
Such a system would increase survi- Maternal care is largely restricted to short-range bipedal walking and a varie-
vorship and would also favor any in- suckling and grooming, the male being ty of hindlimb stances (75), but because
crease in the reproductive rate of a mo- responsible for subadults at all other they lack the pelvic and lower limb adap-
nogamous pair so long as feeding strate- times. The modal birth is dizygotic twins tations characteristic of hominids, biped-
gy was sufficient to meet the increased (55, 68). It is likely that this system is a al walking leads to rapid fatigue (23). It
load on the sources of protein and calo- partitioning of care in response to the appears likely that the skeletal altera-
ries. One element of feeding among for- high protein and calorie requirements of tions for bipedality would be under
est chimpanzees is the "food call" some-VI these small species. Male care during strong selection only by consistent, ex-
times made by males upon discovery of a foraging tends to equilibrate the high ca- tended periods of upright walking and
new food source (65). In the proposed loric load imposed on females by lacta- not by either occasional bipedality or up-
system, however, selection would not fa- tion and gestation of two (and sometimes right posture. While primitive material
Vor this behavior; instead, selection three) offspring-the process of twinning culture does not impose this kind of se-
Would favor a behavior that would bene- being an obvious demographic adapta- lection, carrying behavior of the type
fit only the male's own reproductive tion of elevated birthrate. As Hershkov- suggested above, does. It is likely that
ullit. The simple alternative to the food jtz (68) notes: ".survival of a population the need to carry significant amounts of
Call would involve collecting the avail- {[of callitrichids] in the wild depends on food was a strong selection factor in fa-
able food item or items and returning close synchronization between cyclical vor of primitive material culture (76). Al-
23JANUARY 1981 345
though it is not a significant shift from predisposed to assume most, or all, of (and improved survivorship and reprof..
primitive tools of the type used by chim- the parental care. (ii) Parental care re- ductive rate) is maximized.
panzees today, such as '"termite sticks" quirements are minimal. (iii) A super- Man displays a greater elaboration of
and ""leaf sponges," to simple and read- abundant food resource enables a single epigamic characters than any other pri.
ily available natural articles that could be parent to provide full parental care. As mate (8, 59, 83, 84). Frequently, our sex-,
used to enhance carrying ability, it is a has been noted above, however, survi- ual dimorphism is tacitly accepted as evi-
significant shift from such primitive and vorship of offspring must have been criti- dence for a polygynou's mating structure
occasional tool use to the stone tools of cal to Miocene hominoids; further fe- because marked sexual dimorphism is.a
the basal Pleistocene. Development of male parenting is negated by the mobile most often a product of elaboration of
such tools is most likely to have followed feeding strategy; hominoid males may be characters of attraction, display, and
an extended period of more primitive considered an ""untapped pool" of repro- agonistic behavior in males of polyg.
material culture, which was not critical ductive energy; and Miocene ecological ynous species. Among primates, the de-
to survival. It has been suggested fre- conditions required a genealist feeding gree of sexual dimorphism corresponds8?
quently that the earliest tools were weap- strategy. Conditions were prime for the closely to the degree of male competition
ons. However, the progressive develop- establishment of male parental invest- for mates (56, 59, 83). Yet human sexual
ment of more advanced stone tools from ment and a monogamous mating struc- dimorphism is clearly not typical as is
rudimentary weapons is unlikely. A pro- ture. Finally, it should be pointed out even made clear by the fossil record. InX
longed and extensive period of regular that only among primates in which the their discussion of A. afarensis, Johanb.
and habitual use of simple (primitive) male is clearly and directly involvedUn son and White (17, 85) note that although
carrying devices could eventually allow the parenting process should monogamy- this species shows "'marked body size
the coordination and pattern recognition be found. This is exactly the case, as this dimorphism, the metric and morphologi..,
necessary for a more advanced reliance mating structure is found only in gib- cal dimorphism of the canine teeth is not

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


on material culture. bons, siamangs, and the New World taxa as pronounced as in other extant,
The sequential evolution of behavior discussed above (55). ground-dwelling primates. This implies a
proposed in this article has a high proba- functional pattern different from that
bility of mirroring actual behavioral seen in other primates and may have sig-..
events during the Miocene. In most high- Human Sexual Behavior and Anatomy nificant behavioral implications." There,l
er primates, male fitness is largely deter- can be no doubt that large male canines
mined by consort success of one sort or The highly unusual sexual behavior of are part of the "'whole anatomy of bluff,'A
another (77). Male enhancement of off- man may now be brought into focus. Hu- threat, and fighting" (6). The reduction.,
spring survival is for the most part in- man females are continually sexually re- and effective loss of canine dimorphisma
direct and is expressed more in terms of cetive (79) and have essentially'no ex- in early hominids therefore serves as pri-t3
* iclor kin selection by general behav- ternally recognizable estrous cycle; male mary evidence in favor of the proposed I
iors such as territory defense or predator approach may be considered equally behavioral model (86). But it is important-
recognition and repulsion (78). Females stable. Copulation shows little or no syn- to stress that while canine dimorphism
are solely responsible for true parenting chronization with ovulation (80). As was was undergoing reduction, other formsl
and their ability in this is under strong pointed out above, the selective emer- of dimorphism were apparently being ac-i
selection. However, progressive in- gence of a monogamous mating structure centuated, as judged from their ex-
tensification of higher primate K strategy and male provisioning would require that pression in modern man, who remains.
elevates parenting requirements and males not be disadvantaged in obtaining the most epigamically adorned primate.-i
lowers reproductive rate. The most obvi- consorts. Provisioning in birds and can- Since man displays a highly unusual'
ous, and perhaps only, additional mecha- ids is normally made possible by highly mating structure, it is perhaps not sur--
nism available with which to meet this restricted breeding seasons and discrete prising that his epigamic, or perhaps'
"'demographic dilemma" is an increase generations-the female normally is im- parasexual, anatomy is equally unusual
in the direct and continuous participation pregnable for only brief periods during and fully explicable by that mating struc-
by males in the reproductive process. which parental care is not required. The ture. If pair bonding was fundamental
Whatever the actual sequence of events, menstrual cycle of higher primates (81), and crucial to early hominid reproduc-
whether as posed above or by some al- however, requires regular male proxim- tive strategy, the anatomical characters
ternative order, such additional invest- ity for reproductive success. The pro- that could reinforce pair bonds would al-
ment would improve survivorship and gressive elimination of external manifes- so be under strong positive selection.
favor a mating structure that intensified tations of ovulation and the estab- Thus the body and facial hair, distinctive
energy apportionment to the male's bio- lishment of continual receptivity would somatotype, the conspicuous penis of
logical offspring. Two mating patterns require copulatory vigilance in both sex- human males, and the prominent and
satisfy this latter requirement: polygyny es in order to ensure fertilization. More- permanently enlarged mammae of hu-
(one male and several females) or - over, copulation would increase pair- man females are not surprising in light of
nogamy. The former, however, requires bond adhesion and serve as a social dis- Mayr's (87) observation that in ""monog-
male energy to continue to be devoted to play asserting that bond. Indeed, any se- amous species such as herons (egrets) inl
maintaining consorts, and a pool of com- questration of ovulation (82) would seem which the pair bond is continuously test-
peting males is ensured by polygynous to directly imply both regular copulatory ed and strengthened by mutual displays,
structure itself, thereby directing it away behavior and monogamous mating struc- there has been a "transference' of the dis-
from direct enhancement of survivor- ture. It establishes mathematical parity play characters from the males to the fe-
ship. between males restricted to a single mate males with the result that both sexes
In their synthesis of the evolution of and those practicing complete promiscu- have elaborate display plumes." In man,'
mating systems, Emlen and Oring (72) ity, and the balance of selection falls to however, marked epigamic dimorphism
stress three factors common to polyg- the offspring of pair-bonded males, since is achieved by elaboration of parasexual
Ynous mating structure. (i) One sex is their energetic capacity for provisioning characters in both males and females,
346 '
SCIENCE. VOL. 211.w
rather than in males alone. Their display
value is clearly cross-sexual and not in-
trasexual as in other primates. It should
be stressed that these epigamic charac-
ters are highly variable and can thus be
viewed as a mechanism for establishing Fig. 3. Approximate distnbu- /
and displaying individual sexual unique- tion of extant Old World mon-
keys (hatched) and pongids
ness, and that such uniqueness would (gorilla, chimpanzee, orang-
play a major role in the maintenance of utan) (solid) (38,58,102,103).
pair bonds (59). This is especially impor-
tant when other epigamic features of
man (pubic, axillary, and scalp hair),
which have been elaborated in both sex-
es, are considered. Such characters may
also contribute to individual sexual
uniqueness (88). Redolent individuality
is clearly the most probable role of ax- and remain relatively abundant-the the human career. Hunting and toolmak-
illary and urogenital scent "organs" (cc- Hominidae. It is probable that the homi- ing are most frequently cited as "primal
crine and apocrine glands plus hair), noid trends of prolonged longevity and causes" for the Pleistocene acceleration
which are unique among mammals (89). increased parental investment are the in hominid brain development. Yet have
An objection that might be voiced in re- key to the replacement of most pongid these not figured so prominently because
sponse to these suggestions is that such taxa by Old World monkeys, which are they leave ubiquitous evidence-the ar-

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


auxiliary pair-bond "'enhancers" are reproductively more prosperous. If only cheological record? Other human behav-
eclipsed by the paramount role of culture a portion of Miocene hominoids made iors at least as critical to survival (espe-
in the mating practices of non- the adaptations described above, two cially reproductive behavior) are not
technological societies. Quite the con- distinct groups would subsequently re- "fossilized." It is now clear that man
trary, the more that culturre can be sult. One group might counter the ""de- probably remained an omnivore through-
shown to dominate the mating structure mographic dilemma" according to the out the Pleistocene and that hunting may
and process of recent man, the more an- model suggested in this article; a second have always been an auxiliary food
cient must be the anatomical-physiologi- group could survive by occupying habi- source (93).
cal mechanisms involved in the forma- tats with minimal environmental haz- As Reynolds (94) stressed, intense so-
tion and maintainence of pair bonds (90). ards. Hominids, being more demograph- cial behavior would seem the most likely
ically resistant to environmentally induc- single cause of the origin of human in-
ed mortality, would be more capable of telligence if one origin must be isolated.
Higher Primate Paleogeography expanding into novel and varied habi- Tools are used to manipulate the envi-
tats, especially mosaics, and of com- ronment and are thus a vehicle of in-
The present-day geographic distribu- peting with the radiating Old World mon- teligence, not necessarily a cause.
tions of Old World monkeys and apes are keys. Conversely, the extant pongids are Chimpanzees occasionally use tools (a
shown in Fig. 3. The great apes are by implication descendant of populations behavior that has fascinated many early
markedly restricted and occupy only mi- progressively more restricted to highly hominid theorists), but tools are not criti-
nor areas where minimal environmental favorable forest conditions, where mini- cal to their survival. Primates, which are
changes have taken place since the early mal seasonality in food supply, low pre- the most intelligent mammals, have
Miocene. Yet the fossil record shows dation pressure, and limited size of the achieved evolutionary success primarily
that their lineal ancestors (dryopithe- home range would be in effect. These dif- by their social and reproductive behav-
cines, sensu lato) spread throughout the ferences in habitat preference would re- ior, which is their most developed ordi-
Old World following the establishment of sult in a more extensive fossil record for nal character. It seems reasonable there-
a land bridge and forest corridor between hominids than pongids, both by virtue of fore to propose that a further elaboration
Africa and Eurasia about 16 to 17 million the geographic expansion of hominids of this adaptive strategy is the most
years ago, and that they enjoyed consid- and as a consequence of the occupation likely "cause" of early hominid success
erable success after their colonization of of habitats with more favorable condi- and the further development of intel-
Europe and Asia (41, 91). Old World tions of fossilization. It is therefore quite ligence.
monkeys, on the other hand, were much possible that the sivapithecines (sensu It is of interest to explore one further
less abundant during this period (92). Af- lato) of the middle and late Mio ,pe, effect of the proposed model on early
ter the middle and late Miocene, how- which already evince dental modifla- hominid social structure. The strong ma-
ever, a marked reduction in dryopithe- tions adumbrating those of late Pliocene ternal and sibling ties of higher primates
cine numbers occurred. While this can- hominids (12), may have contained are now well documented (47, 54, 94,
not be deduced from the sparse fossil primitive emergent hominids, at least be- 95). The matrifocal unit of chimpanzees
record of the late Miocene and early haviorally, if not phylogenetically. continues throughout the life of the
Pliocene, the distribution of extant de- mother, as do sibling ties. In the pro-
scendants of the dryopithecines is ample posed hominid reproductive strategy,
evidence of their relict status. Today, The Nuclear Family the process of pair bonding would not
Old World monkeys are clearly the only lead to the direct involvement of
dominant and successful group, having Man's most unique character is with- males in the survivorship of offspring, in
replacej-ihe dryojipthecines and their de- out question his enormous intelligence, prmates as intelligent as extant homi-
Scendants during the last 12 million years and its evolutionary pathway has fasci- noids, it would establish paternity, and
(91). One hominoid group did survive nated all who have attempted to explain thus lead to a gradual replacement of the
23 JANUARY 1981 347
matrifocal group by a ""bifocal" one- traditionally been on singular, extraordi- 2. T. H. Huxley, Evidence as to Man's Place in
Nature (Wiliams & Norgate, London, 1863);
the primitive nuclear family (84, 96). The nary traits of later human evolution. The W. K. Gregory, Science 71, 645 (1930); A.
effects of such a social unit on survivor- model proposed in this article has placed Keith, Br. Med. J. 1, 451 (1923); The Antiquity
of Man (Williams & Norgate, London, 1929),
ship and species success could be pro- greater emphasis on a fundamental be- vols. I and 2; W. E. Le Gros Clark, The An-
found. It could lead to a further shorten- havioral base from which these unusual tecedents of Man, (Edinburgh Univ. Press
Edinburgh, ed. 2, 1962).
ing ofbirth sce, which would acceler- adaptations could be directionally se- 3. C. Darwin, The Descent of Man (John Murray,
ate the reproductive rate and amplify sib- lected. London, 1871).
4. G. A. Bartholomew and J. B. Birdsell, Am. An-
ling bonds. Reduction of birth space The proposed model accounts for the thropol. 55, 481 (1953); A. E. Mann, Man 7
would allow coincident protraction of 379 (1972); S. L. Washburn, The Study of Hu:
early origin of bipedality as a locomotor man Evolution (Oregon State System Higher
the subadult (learning) period (97). Be- behavior directly enhancing reproduc- Education, Eugene, 1968); in ClassifJcation
and Human Evolution, S. L. Washburn, Ed.
haviors that in other primates are com- tive fitness, not as a behavior resulting (Aldine, Chicago, 1963), pp. 190-203; Sci. Am.
mon causes of infant death (for example, from occasional upright feeding posture. 203, 3 (September 1960); 1. DeVore, in Hori-
zons in Anthropology, S. Tax, Ed. (Aldine,
agonistic buffering (78), would be largely It accounts for the origin of the home Chicago, 1965), pp. 25-36; C. F. Hockett and
eliminated, while those that might im- base in the same fashion as it has been R. Ascher, Curr. Anthropol. 5. 135 (1964).
5. P. V. Tobias, The Brain in Hominid Evolution
prove survivorship (for example, adop- acquired by numerous other mammals. (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1971).
tion) (46, 98) would be facilitated. The It accounts for the human nuclear fam- 6. S. L. Washburn and R. L. Ciochon, Am. An-
thropol. 76, 765 (1974).
age until which an orphaned chimpanzee ily, for the distinctive human sexual epi- 7. R. L. Holloway, ibid. 0, 63 (1967).
8. C. J. Jolly, Man 5, 5 (1970).
does not survive the death of its mother gamic features, and the species' unique 9. Artifacts have been found in situ in the Gona
is "around 5 years of age, but may sexual behavior. It accounts for a func- region of the Hadar formation by the Inter-
national Afar Research Expedition. Strati-
stretch another 3 to 4 in special circum- tional, rudimentary material culture of fication of the Gona region is at present under
stances" (46). Survival of a second par- long-standing, and it accounts for the investigation and correlations with the KH
member are as yet uncertain. At present the
ent may have been a crucial reproductive greater proportion of r-selected (45) artifacts are thought to overlie deposits equiva-

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


lent to BKT, tuffof the KH member, which has
advance in early hominids (99). Primi- characters in hominids relative to other a potassium-argon age determination of about
parous females are much less adept than hominoids. It accounts for these charac- 2.6 million years. The artifacts may therefore
be older than 2 million years [D. C. Johanson,
multiparous mothers. Drickamer (50) ters with simple behavioral changes personal communication].
found that in free-ranging Macaca mul- common to both primates and other 10. E. L. Simons, Primate Evolution: An In-
troduction to Man's Place in Nature (Macmil-
lata "between 40 and 50%6 of the infants mammals and in relatively favorable en- lan, New York, 1972); in (11), pp. 543-566; D.
born first or second to a female did not vironments, rather than by rapid or Pilbeam, The Ascent of Man: An Introduction
to Human Evolution (Macmillan, New York,
survive their first year, but by the fourth forced occupation of habitats for which I972).
11. C. J. Jolly. Ed., Early Hominids of Africa (St.
infant born to the same female only 9%o early hominoids were clearly not adapt- Martin's Press, London. 1978).
died during the first 12 months." Lancas- ively or demographically equipped. It is 12. L. 0. Greenfield, Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 50,
527 (1979).
ter (100) notes that: ""Recent field and fully consistent with primate paleogeog- 13. A Kortlandt, in Progress in Primatology, D.
laboratory workers have shown that in raphy, present knowledge of higher pri- Starck, R. Schneider, H.-J. Kuhn, Eds. (Fis-
cher, Stuttgart, 1967); J. M. Goodall, in Pri-
many species of mammals, and espe- mate behavior patterns (as well as those mate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and
cially in monkeys and apes, learning and of other mammals), and the hominid fos- Apes, I. DeVore, Ed. (Holt, Rinehart & Win-
ston, 1965), pp. 425-473; R. V. S. Wright, in
experience play vital roles in the devel- sil record. Human Evolution: Biosocial Perspectives, S.
opment of the behavior patterns used in If the model is correct, the conven- L. Washburn and E. R. McCown, Eds. (Ben-
jamin Cummings, Menlo Park, Calif., 1978),
mating and maternal care." The effect of tional concept that material culture is pp. 215-238.
14. A convincing and more detailed argument is
intensified parenting, protracted learn- pivotal to the differentiation and origin of provided by Jolly (8).
ing, and enhanced sibling relationships the primary characters of the Hominidae 15. Whether or not the early evolution of material
culture did in fact proceed in a gradualistic
would have a markedly beneficial effect is probably incorrect. Rather, both ad- manner is difficult to establish. A punctuated
upon survivorship. Such projections of vanced material culture and the Pleisto- equilibrium model is equally applicable to the
early artifact record, and it is not unlikely that
the behavior of developing hominids are cene acceleration in brain development material culture proceeded at variable mtes.
certainly not new, but they have not re- are sequelae to an already established 16. Contrary to popular opinion, there is no evi-
dence whatsoever that early hominids hunted.
ceived their due emphasis. Can the nu- hominid character system, which includ- Bipedality is probably the mode of locomotion
least adapted to hunting, unless sophisticated
clear family not be viewed as a prodi- ed intensified parenting and social rela- technology is available (or unusually high lev-
gious adaptation central to the success of tionships, monogamous pair bonding, els of intelligence, or both). The evidence
made available by A. afarensis is particularly
early hominids? It may certainly be con- specialized sexual-reproductive behav- striking. Further australopithecine evolution
sidered as being within the behavior re- ior, and bipedality. It implies that the nu- from that species is documented by a reduction
of the anterior dentition and further enlarge-
pertoire of hominoid primates, provided clear family and human sexual behavior ment of the grinding teeth. Artifacts do not ap-
that the reproductive and feeding strate- may have their ultimate origin long be- pear until 2 million years ago, and when they
do appear it is difficult to interpret them as
gies commensurate to its development fore the dawn of the Pleistocene. hunting implements. In short, if the evidence
made available by the fossil record is to be
were themselves under strong selection. used in reconstructing early hominid evolu-
This brief review of the fossil record and tion, one of its clearest implications is that
References d Notes hunting was not a dietarily significant behav-
some primate behavioral and ecological ior. See also (8. 41, 62. 91. 102).
adaptations would seem to strongly fa- 1. M. Goodman, in Molecular Anthropology, M. 17. D. C. Johanson and T. D. White, Science 203,
Goodman and R. E. Tashian, Eds. (Plenum, 321 (1979).
vor the correctness of this view. New York, 1977). pp. 321-353; M.-C. King and
A. C. Wilson, Science 138, 107 (1975);--VI.._
18. L. Radinsky, Am. J. Phvs. Anthropol. 41, 15
(1974); Am. Sci. 63, 656 (1975).
Sarich, jn Perspectives on Human Evolutio- 19. D. C. Johanson, T. D. White, Y. Coppens.
S. L. Wshburn and P. C.-Jay,-,Eds.-(ot, Kirtlandia 28, 1 (1978).
Conclusion Rinehart & Winston, New York.A968), vol. 1, 20. Among the salient features of the A. afarensis
pp. 94-121; in Background to Man, V. M. Sar- cranium are a convex nasal clivus, a uniformlY
ich and P. Dolhinow, Eds. (little, Brown, Bos- shallow palate anterior to the incisal foramen,
It is a truism to say that even late ton, 1971), pp. 182-191; D. E. Kohne, in Per- an independent juga with individual sharp lat-
spectives on Human Evolution, S. L. Wash- eral margins of the nasal aperture, a true ca-
Pliocene hominids must have been un- burn and P. Dolhinow. Eds. (Holt, Rinehart nine fossa, a rounded tympanic plate with an
& Winston, New York, 1972), vol. 2. pp. 166 inferiorly directed surface, a compound tem-
usual mammals, both behaviorally and and 168; J, . A. Chiscon, B. H Hoyer, poral-nuchal crest in large and smal individ-to
anatomically. As was pointed out above, Carnegie Inst. Washington Yearb. 69, 488 uals, an occipital plane that is short relative
(1970); C. 0. Lovejoy and R. S. Mcindi, the nuchal plane, and a relatively vertical
emphasis in models of human origin has Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 16, 18 (1972).
l
nuchal plane and posterior positioned foramen
348 SCIENCE, VOL. 211
magnum [W. H. Kimbel, in preparation]. See 43. Greenfield (12) concludes that "Sivapithecus portantly, the adoption of bipedality would
also (17). utilized a broad range of zones including tropi- mean the loss of most or all of the pre-
21. R. L. Holloway, personal communication. cal rain forests (Chinji of India), subtropics hensibility of the infant foot, which is an im-
22. C. 0. Lovejoy, Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 50, (Rudabanya and Chinji-Nagri of India and Pa- portant grasping organ in the chimpanzee in-
460 (1979). kistan), and woodland and bush habitats (Late fant. This, in turn, selects for a more secure
23. , in (11), pp. 403-429. Nagri and Early Dhok Pathan of India and Pa- infant carrying ability in the mother and thus
24. , K. G. Heiple, A. H. Burstein, Am. J. kistan, Fort Ternan)." bipedality. This form of selection can clearly
Phys. Anthropol. 38, 757 (1973); C. 0. Love- 44. R. G. Cutler, J. Hum. Evol. 5, 169 (1976), and not be viewed as the initial selective force for
joy, Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 17, 147 (1974); in references therein. bipedal locomotion, but in conjunction with
Primate Morphology and Evolution, R. H. 45. R. H. MacArthur and E. 0. Wilson, The Theo- others, would certainly contribute to the total
Tuttle, Ed. (Mouton, The Hague, 1975), pp. ry of Island Biogeography (Princeton Univ. selective pattern.
291-326. Press, Princeton, N.J., 1967). The K and r are 55. T. H. Clutton-Brock, in Primate Ecology, T.
25. C. R. Taylor and V. J. Rowntree, Science 179, opposite ends of the continuum of reproduc- H. Clutton-Brock, Ed. (Academic Press, New
186 (1973); G. A. Gavagna, F. P. Saibene, R. tive strategy. In the r strategy, the number of York, 1977), pp. 539-556.
Margaria, J. Appl. Phys. 19, 249 (1964). offspring is maximized at the expense of paren- 56. __, J. Zool. London 183, 1 (1977).
26. I have elsewhere pointed out (23, 24) that the tal care; at the K end (the effective limit of 57. Clutton-Brock (56) notes that "although sex
resting lengths of the major propulsive muscles which is 1), parental care is maximized. differences in feeding behavior are common
about the hip and knee in quadrupedal pri- 46. G. E. Teleki, E. Hunt, J. H. Pfifferling, J. among primates there is little evidence to sug-
mates are so substantially altered by the adop- Hum. Evol. 5, 559 (1976). gest that they have evolved to minimize feed-
tion of erect posture that the regular effective 47. J. Goodall, in Human Origins: Louis Leakey ing competition between the sexes." Yet for
use of both quadrupedal and bipedal locomo- and the East African Evidence, G. L. Isaac and the two species just cited this may indeed be
tion is not possible. Thus the transition to bi- E. R. McCown, Eds. (Benjamin, Menlo Park, the case. Colobus badius uses a more general-
pedality as a habitual mode of locomotion must Calif., 1976), p. 86. ist feeding strategy than its sympatric con-
have been relatively rapid. 48. A female chimpanzee reaches sexual maturity generics, has a larger number of adult males
27. D. C. Johanson, C. 0. Lovejoy, A. H. Burst- at about age 10 years. Using the average span within the troop [T. T. Struhsaker and J. F.
ein, K. G. Heiple, Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 44, of 5.6 years between successful births (46) Oates, in (58), pp. 165-186] and has the highest
188 (1976); C. 0. Lovejoy and D. C. Johanson, gives a required life expectancy of about 21 population density of any Old World monkey
in preparation. years (a chimpanzee infant usually dies after [T. H. Clutton-Brock, in (58), pp. 503-512]. It
28. D. C. Johanson and T. D. White, Science 207, his mother's death if it is not at least 4 years is clearly true of the orang (64).
1104 (1980). old). The authors (46, p. 577) conclude similar- 58. R. W. Sussman, Ed., Primate Ecology: Prob-
29. D. C. Johanson and M. Taieb, Nature (Lon- ly that "The mean generation span, or elapsed lem Oriented Field Studies (Wiley, New York,
don) 260, 293 (1976); D. C. Johanson and Y. time between birth of a female and birth of her 1979).
Coppens, Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 45, 217 median offspring, is about 19.6 years for a 59. J. H. Crook, in (40), pp. 231-281.
(1976); M. D. Leakey, R. L. Hay, G. H. Cur- sample of ten Gombe females with three or 60. R. K. Selander, in (40), pp. 180-230; Condor

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


tis, R. E. Drake, M. K. Jakes, T. D. White, more recorded births." Their most realistic es- 68, 113 (1966); G. E. Hutchinson, The Ecologi-
Nature (London) 262, 460 (1976); T. D. White, timate of achieved reproduction is three to four cal Theater and the Evolutionary Play (Yale
Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 46, 197 (1977); Y. "offspring that are successfully raised to sex- Univ. Press, New Haven, Conn., 1965).
Coppens, F. C. Howell, G. L. Isaac, R. E. ual maturity" (46, p. 580). 61. This forms the basis for an additional criticism
Leakey, Eds., Earliest Man and Environments 49. Demographic studies of Old World monkey of the hominid model proposed by Jolly (8).
in the Lake Rudolf Basin (Univ. of Chicago populations are at present insufficient to pro- Gelada baboons are highly specialized feeders
Press, Chicago, 1976); P. V. Tobias, Annu. vide accurate data for unprotected and undis- whose feeding rate is largely limited by han-
Rev. Anthropol. 2, 311 (1973); R. E. Leakey, J. turbed populations [D. S. Sade et al., Yearb. dling time.
M. Mungai, A. C. Walker, Am. J. Phys. An- Phys. Anthropol. 20, 253 (1976)], but approxi- 62. "Although nutritional factors alone would not
thropol. 35, 175 (1971); ibid. 36, 235 (1972); M. mations can be made adequately from observa- preclude the possibility that early hominids
H. Day and R. E. Leakey, ibid. 39, 341 (1973); tions of protected or introduced populations. were dietarily quite specialized, [from] the
ibid. 41, 367 (1974); R. E. Leakey and B. A. Ninety percent of adult females in the Chhatari available archaeological evidence and [from]
Wood, ibid. 39, 255 (1973); M. H. Day, R. E. population studied by Southwick and Siddiqi what is known of the dietary patterns of living
Leakey, A. C. Walker, B. A. Wood, ibid. 42, [ibid. 20, 242 (1976)] gave birth yearly during gatherer-hunters and chimpanzees, it appears
461 (1974); ibid. 45, 369 (1976); F. C. Howell, the 14-year study period. Infant mortality aver- unlikely that all early hominids were almost ex-
Nature (London) 223, 234 (1969); __ and B. aged 16.3 percent, and juvenile mortality was clusively carnivorous or herbivorous. It is
A. Wood, ibid. 249, 174 (1974). judged very low (the actual figure was 33 per- more reasonable to suggest that the diet fell
30. D. Pilbeam, G. E. Mayer, C. Badgley, M. D. cent but most of this loss was attributed to within the broad range of today's gatherer-
Rose, M. H. L. Pickford, A. K. Behrens- trapping). A reasonable figure with this data is hunter diets, but that within the wide spectrum
meyer, S. M. Ibrahim Shah, Nature (London) 7 to 9 years, from methods similar to those of this adaptation, local environmental re-
270, 689 (1977). used in (48). L. C. Drickamer (50) found first sources and seasonal scarcity may have forced
31. D. R. Pilbeam, Peabody Mus. Nat. His. Yale birth to occur in females of 4 years on the aver- some individual populations to become more
Univ. Bull. 31, 1 (1969); E. L. Simons and D. age in free-ranging rhesus at La Parguera, dependent on vegetable or animal tissue foods
R. Pilbeam, Folia Primatol. 3, 81 (1965); in The Puerto Rico, and in animals of this age, 68 to 77 than others" [A. E. Mann, personal communi-
Functional and Evolutionary Biology of Pri- percent produced infants each year. Although cation: see "The evolution of hominid dietary
mates, R. Tuttle, Ed. (Aldine, Chicago, 1972), first-year mortality was high for first and sec- patterns" in Primate Dietary Patterns, R. S. 0.
pp. 36-62; D. R, Pilbeam, in (32), pp. 39-59; E. ond born offspring (40 to 50 percent), it was Harding and G. Teleki, Eds. (Columbia Univ.
L. Simons, in (32), pp. 60-72. only 9 percent for third born. These data sup- Press, New York, in press)].
32. P. V. Tobias and Y. Coppens, Eds., Le Plus port the above conclusion as do those of C. B. 63. E. R. Pianka, Evolutionary Ecology (Harper &
Anciens Hominides (Centre National de la Koford [in Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Row, New York, 1974), and references there-
Recherche Scientifique, Paris, 1976). Monkeys and Apes, I. DeVore, Ed. (Holt, in.
33. While there is general agreement as to those Rinehart & Winston, New York, 1965, pp. 64. D. A. Horr [in (58), p. 320] comments with re-
morphological features directly associated 160-174] from rhesus on Cayo Santiago. spect to the orang-utan: '. . . orang social or-
with molar dominance in the group of homi- 50. L. C. Drickamer, Folia Primatol. 21, 61 (1974). ganization might easily be explained as fol-
noids referred to as phase II, there is some dis- 51. R. A. Fisher, Genetical Theory of Natural Se- lows: In order not to overload the food supply,
agreement as to the distinctiveness of other lection (Clarendon, Oxford, 1930). orangs disperse themselves in the jungles. Fe-
dentognathic characters. See P. Andrews and 52. It is in fact more likely that they were not, or at males carrying infants or tending young juve-
I. Tekkaya, in (32), pp. 7-25. least not in the extreme forms seen in extant niles can best survive if they don't have to
34. L. 0. Greenfield, thesis, University of Mich- hominoids. The hominid adaptations proposed move far. Young orangs could also best learn
igan (1976). in this article are more likely to have been de- the jungle in a restricted, familiar area....
35. C. L. Brace and A. Montagu, Human Evolu- veloped to prevent the "demographic dilem- Adult males are unencumbered by young and
tion (Macmillan, New York, ed. 2, 1977). ma." Modern pongids probably represent ter- can more easily move over wider areas. This
36. This is especially true of Erythrocebus patas, minal phases of extreme parental investment means that they compete with females for food
which is both a small-object feeder and the only because of long-term occupation of partic- only for short periods of time, 'and thus do not
fastest ground-living primate [K. R. L. Hall, J. ularly favorable environments, hence their overload her food supply and force her to
Zool. 148, 15 (1965)]. very restricted present-day distribution. move over wider areas."
37. It should also be pointed out that changes in the 53. By breakthrough adaptation is meant that 65. V. Reynolds and F. Reynolds, in Primate Be-
masticatory apparatus of hominids are a reflec- which allows or precedes an adaptive radia- havior, I. DeVore, Ed. (Holt, Rinehart & Win-
tion of changes in habitat and are not necessar- tion. While the Old World monkeys replaced ston, New York, 1965), pp. 368-424; J. Van
ily the initial cause of clade differentiation. all other hominoids, the hominids were suc- Lawick-Goodall, Anim. Behav. Mono. 1, 161
Care must be taken not to view those charac- cessful in many of the same environments oc- (1968); Y. Sugiyama, Primates 9, 225 (1968);
ters that allow identification of early hominids cupied by monkeys, including the Pleistocene ibid. 10, 197 (1969). See R. W. Wrangham [in
as synonomous with acutal forces of diver- savannahs. Primate Ecology, T. H. Clutton-Brock, Ed.
gence. 54. J. Van Lawick-Goodall, in Primate Ethology, (Academic Press, New York, 1977), pp. 504-
38. A. Kortlandt, Sci. Am. 206, 128 (May 1962); D. Morris, Ed. (Doubleday, New York, 1969), 538] for further discussion on the possible
New Perspectives on Ape and Human Evolu- p. 424. It should also be pointed out that falls functions of the "food call."
tion (Stichting Voor Psychobiologie, Amster- as a consequence of mother-infant travel 66. Such loads can become intense (up to 1.5 times
dam, 1972). would be a more critical selection factor in normal resting basal metabolic rate in females)
39. R. E. Moreau, Proc. Zool. Soc. London 121, early hominids than other primates. Van Law- [0. W. Portman, in Feeding and Nutrition of
869 (1951); K. W. Butzer, Am. Sci. 65, 572 ick-Goodall points out that "in striking con- Nonhuman Primates, R. S. Harris, Ed. (Aca-
(1977); B. Campbell, in (40), pp. 40-58. trast to most other primate species, the small demic Press, New York, 1970), pp. 87-116].
40. B. Campbell, Ed., Sexual Selection and the chimpanzee infant often appears unable to re- The modern human preparation for lactation is
Descent of Man 1871-1971 (Aldine, Chicago, main securely attached to its mother if she an average accumulation of 9 pounds subcuta-
1972). makes a sudden movement. For several neous fat [D. B. Jelliffe and E. F. P. Jelliffe,
41. F. Andrews and J. A. H. van Couvering, in Ap- months after the birth of her infant the mother Human Milk in the Modern World (Oxford
proaches to Primate Paleobiology, F. S. may have to support it, thus hindering her Univ. Press, Oxford, 1978)]. A major birth in-
Szalay, Ed. (Karger, Basel, 1975), pp. 62-105. movements...." Mortality as a consequence terval limitation in hominoid primates may well
42. M. Kretzoi, Nature (London) 257, 578 (1975); of falling may thus be viewed as one selection be the lactational loads placed on the mother
G. E. Kennedy, ibid. 271, 11 (1978). factor in favor of terrestrial care, but more im- (in contradistinction to the needs of the infant)
23 JANUARY 1981 349
and any improvement in feeding strategy could Perspectives, S. L. Washburn and E. R. + collecting) without the requirement that hunt-
..support" a reduction in birth space on this McCown, Eds. (Benjamin-Cummings, Menlo ing be critical to human evolution at any point.
basis. Park, Calif., 1978), pp. 123-154. The similarity in social behavior between
67. G. Mitchell and E. M. Brandt, in Primate So- 85. In other primates that are monogamous, there canids and early humans has often been cited
cialization, F. E. Poirier, Ed. (Random House, is little sexual dimorphism. All of these, how- and attributed to hunting. It is more likely
New York, 1972), pp. 173-206; H. Kummer, ever, live in territorial family groups [A. Jolly, that such similarities take origin in reproduc-
Primate Societies (Aldine, Chicago, 1971). The Evolution of Primate Behavior (Macmil- tive strategy (pair bonding, intratroop coopera-
68. P. Hershkovitz, Living New World Monkeys lan, New York, 1972)] and there is therefore no tion, provisioning, male involvement in sub-
(Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1977), vol. intragroup competition for mates. Strong sex- adult care, and so forth) and that hunting
1. ual dimorphism is usually a consequence of ei- merely represents one fbo-d procurement meth-
69. H. C. B. Grzimek, Ed., Animal Life Encyclo- ther differential competition for mates or dif- od that satisfies the economic requirements of
pedia (Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, ferential exploitation of resources (56, 60). In the social system. There are numerous carni-
1975), vol. 12. A. afarensis, according to the proposed model, vores that do not display this form of reproduc-
70. D. Lick, Ecological Adaptations for Breeding dimorphism would be favored on the latter tive strategy, and there are some rodents that
in Birds (Metheun, London, 1968). Monogamy basis. Small female body size would reduce ca- do but, of course, do not hunt (73).
is especially characteristic of long-lived (K-se- loric-protein requirements, while large body 94. V. Reynolds, The Biology of Human Action
lected) birds [J. W. F. Davis, J. Anim. Ecol. size would increase male mobility and predator (Freeman, San Francisco, 1976).
45, 531 (1976); A. Mills, ibid. 42, 147 (1973); J. resistance. 95. J. H. Kaufmann, Ecology 40, 500 (1965); M.
C. Coulson, ibid. 35, 269 (1966)]. 86. This is not meant to imply that it was a cause of Yamada, Primates 4, 43 (1963); C. B. Koford,
71. J. F. Eisenberg, Handb. Zool. 10, 1 (1966). canine reduction, but only that the process Science 141, 356 (1963).
72. S. T. Emlen and L. W. Oring, Science 197, 215 could occur by a combination of relaxation of 96. The term bifocal is preferable to nuclear family
(1977). selection on large male canine size and a posi- because the latter carries manifest connota-
73. Such sequestration is common among rodents tive selective mechanism for reduced canines. tions from its application to Western and non-
as well with perhaps its most classic ex- The latter is most likely to be found in the con- Western modern human cultures, none of
pression in castorids, which are comparatively current dentognathic changes of greater molar which are implied by its use here.
K-selected (requiring 3 to 4 years to mature dominance and general anterior tooth reduc- 97. A. E. Mann's extensive studies of dental devel-
sexually) and live in stable family groups. tion (8, 17). opment and wear in australopithecines ["Some
74. A second, also important, element of food-han- 87. E. Mayr, in (40), p. 97. palaeodemographic aspects of the South Afri-
dling behavior may have been premastication. 88. Modern man displays a remarkable number of can australopithecines" (Univ. Penn. Publ.
Reduction of birth space would have required morphological traits that may be considered Anthropol. 1, 1975)] indicate a prolonged peri-
an earlier reinitiation of ovulation, which epigamic (hair color and type, lip size and od of development was established by about
would in turn have required a reduction of me- form, corporal hair patterning, eyebrows, fa- 2.5 million years ago. K. R. McKinley's survi-
chanical stimuli to the mechanoreceptors of cial countenance, and so forth). Attempts have vorship calculations based upon Mann's data

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on March 10, 2009


the nipple and areola (and thereby a reduction been made to correlate some of these with geo- [Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 34, 417 (1971)] led
in prolactin levels) [R. C. Kolodny, L. S. Ja- graphic variables, but they have been largely him to conclude that australopithecines show a
cobs, W. H. Daughaday, Nature (London) 238, unsuccessful. An alternative explanation is hominid rather than a nonhuman primate
284 (1972)], and hence an earlier age of wean- that disruptive selection acts to maximize the "birth spacing pattern." While these calcu-
ing. Parental premastication would have facili- variability of these features within populations, lations require a number of assumptions about
tated such behavior, at the same time enhanc- thereby enhancing the distinctiveness of poten- the origin and nature of the death assemblages
ing parental bonds (see discussion of the nucle- tial and actual mates in establishing and main- at Swartkrans and Sterkfontein, they are
ar family). This could have also increased the taining pair bonds. The subsequent geographic strong evidence that a major demographic shift
rate of dental wear and have been an auxiliary isolation, whether partial or complete, of a was fully developed by 2.0 to 2.5 million years
selection component of the dentognathic population could then have resulted in a trun- ago which included an extended period of sub-
changes characteristic of early hominids. cation of expression and apparent uniqueness adult dependency.
75. H. R. Bauer, Primates 18, 913 (1977). of some features that maintained their epigam- 98. H. Kummer, in Social Communication Among
76. R. E. Leakey and R. Lewin, People of the ic significance in the population (for example, Primates, S. Altman, Ed. (Univ. of Chicago
Lake (Doubleday, New York, 1978); G. the epicanthic eye fold). The obvious poly- Press, Chicago, 1967), pp. 63-72; D. S. Sade,
Hewes, Am. Anthropol. 63, 687 (1961). genic basis of such traits and their reappearance in ibid., pp. 99-115; see (47, 54).
77. R. L. Trivers, in (40), pp. 136-179; R. P. Mi- in unrelated populations (Bushman, Lapps, in- 99. M. H. Wolpoff, cited in Mosaic 10, 28 (1979).
chael and D. Zumpe, J. Reprod. Fertil. 21, 199 fant Euroamericans) indicate that their ex- 100. J. B. Lancaster, in Primate Socialization, F. E.
(1970); T. E. Rowell, in Social Communication pression is a consequence of elevated frequen- Poirier, Ed. (Random House, New York,
Among Primates, S. Altman, Ed. (Univ. ot cies of genes that may be universal in H. sa- 1972), pp. 83-104.
Chicago Press, Chicago, 1967), pp. 15-32; I. piens, but below an expressive threshold in 101. C. 0. Lovejoy, R. S. Meindl, T. R. Pryzbeck,
Devore, in Sex and Behavior, F. Beach, Ed. some populations. T. S. Barton, K. G. Heiple, D. Kotting, Sci-
(Wiley, New York, 1965), pp. 266-289; see (36, 89. W. Montagna, in Biological Anthropology, S. ence 198, 291 (1977).
67). H. Katz, Ed. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1975), 102. A. H. Schultz, The Life of Primates (Universe,
78. S. H. Hrdy, inAdvances in the Study ofBehav- pp. 341-351. New York, 1969).
ior, J. S. Rosenbaltt, R. A. Hinde, E. Shaw, C. 90. Further evidence of the age of pair bonding is 103. J. R. Napier and P. H. Napier, A Handbook of
Beer, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, provided by the absence of strong canine di- Living Primates (Academic Press, New York,
1976), vol. 6, pp. 101-158. morphism in A. afarensis (17). The only other ed. 3, 1970).
79. D. C. Johanson, personal communication. Old World higher primates without canine di- 104. I thank G. J. Armelagos, T. Barton, B. Camp-
80. R. A. McChance, M. C. Luff, E. E. Widdow- morphism are the gibbon and siamang, which bell, T. Gray, F. C. Howell, K. Jacobs, D. C.
son, J. Hyg. 37, 571 (1937); J. R. Udry and N. are monogamous (6). Johanson, B. Kimbel, A. E. Mann, R. S.
M. Morris [Nature (London) 220, 593 (1968)] 91. E. Delson, in Approaches to Primate Palaeo- Meindl, R. P. Mensforth, M. H. Wolpoff, P.
did find such a relationship, but it is a moot biology, F. S. Szalay, Ed. (Karger, Basel, Shipman, A. C. Walker, T. D. White, and S.
point since with sequestration of ovulation and 1975), pp. 167-217. Ward, who read earlier versions of this paper
its external manifestations, copulation would 92. and P. Andrews, in Phylogeny of the and provided valuable comments. I thank D.
require female initiation. Primates, W. P. Luckett and F. S. Szalay, C. Johanson, C. J. Jolly, J. Lancaster, R. S.
81. F. A. Beach, Psychol. Rev. 54, 297 (1947); M. Eds. (Plenum, New York, 1976), pp. 405-446. Meindl, and T. D. White for valuable dis-
A. Chance, in Culture and the Evolution of 93. R. B. Lee, in Man the Hunter, R. B. Lee and I. cussions about its content. I thank T. Barton
Man, M. F. Ashley Montagu, Ed. (Oxford DeVore, Eds. (Aldine, Chicago, 1968), pp. 30- for discussions and advice with respect to the
Univ. Press, New York, 1962), pp. 84-130. 48. The provisioning model proposed here ef- quantitative approach used, L. Don Carlos and
82. J. Lancaster, personal communication. fectively accounts for the origin of hunting by R. P. Mensforth for research assistance, and
83. D. Morris, The Naked Ape (Cape, London. means of a progressive elaboration of provi- R. S. Meindl for listening to endless anecdotes
1967). sioning behavior (that is, collecting -a sca- about the behavior of canids, rodents, and
84. F. A. Beach, in Human Evolution: Biosocial venging + collecting -a hunting + scavenging birds.

35S0 SCIENCE, VOL. 211

View publication stats

You might also like