You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/284811888

Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: from acute episode to remission

Article in European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience · April 2016


DOI: 10.1007/s00406-015-0657-2

CITATIONS READS

28 269

8 authors, including:

Julia Volkert Miriam A Schiele


University of Wuerzburg University Medical Center Freiburg
21 PUBLICATIONS 659 CITATIONS 95 PUBLICATIONS 1,081 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Juliane Kopf Sarah Kittel-Schneider


University Hospital Frankfurt University Hospital Würzburg
34 PUBLICATIONS 536 CITATIONS 222 PUBLICATIONS 4,760 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

iPSC-derived Models for the Investigation of ADHD View project

R-Link View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Andreas Reif on 01 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci
DOI 10.1007/s00406-015-0657-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

Cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: from acute episode


to remission
J. Volkert1 · M. A. Schiele2 · Julia Kazmaier2 · Friederike Glaser2 · K. C. Zierhut2 ·
J. Kopf1 · S. Kittel‑Schneider1 · A. Reif1

Received: 31 July 2015 / Accepted: 12 November 2015


© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Abstract Considerable evidence demonstrates that neu- impaired. However, we found that subthreshold depressive
ropsychological deficits are prevalent in bipolar disorder symptoms and persisting sleep disturbances in euthymic
during both acute episodes and euthymia. However, it is BP were associated with reduced speed, deficits in atten-
less clear whether these cognitive disturbances are state- tion and verbal memory, while working memory was cor-
or trait-related. We here present the first longitudinal study related with psychotic symptoms (lifetime). This result
employing a within-subject pre- and post-testing examin- indicates working memory as trait related for a subgroup
ing acutely admitted bipolar patients (BP) in depression of BP with psychotic symptoms. In contrast, attention and
or mania and during euthymia, aiming to identify cogni- verbal memory are negatively influenced by state factors
tive performance from acute illness to remission. Cogni- like residual symptoms, which should be more considered
tive performance was measured during acute episodes and as possible confounders in the search of cognitive endophe-
repeated after at least 3 months of remission. To do so, 55 notypes in remitted BP.
BP (35 depressed, 20 hypo-/manic) and 55 healthy con-
trols (HC) were tested with a neuropsychological test bat- Keywords Bipolar disorder · Cognition ·
tery (attention, working memory, verbal memory, executive Neuropsychological functioning · Depression · Mania ·
functioning). The results showed global impairments in Remission
acutely ill BP compared to HC: depressed patients showed
a characteristic psychomotor slowing, while manic patients
had severe deficits in executive functioning. Twenty-nine Introduction
remitted BP could be measured in the follow-up (dropout
rate 48 %), whose cognitive functions partially recovered, Bipolar disorder (BD) is a recurrent and highly disabling
whereas working memory and verbal memory were still affective disorder characterised by mood swings as well as
cognitive disturbances. Impairments in psychomotor speed,
attention, working memory, long-term memory, and execu-
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this tive functions have been consistently reported both dur-
article (doi:10.1007/s00406-015-0657-2) contains supplementary ing acute episodes of depression and mania [1] as well as
material, which is available to authorized users. during euthymia, albeit in a milder form [2]. In contrast
to extensive investigations of neurocognitive functioning
* J. Volkert
julia.volkert@kgu.de in euthymic bipolar patients (BP), relatively few studies
have characterised changes in cognition across different
1
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic phases of the illness. A meta-analysis of cognitive function-
Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe-University,
ing in euthymic, manic/mixed, and depressed BP revealed
Heinrich‑Hoffmann‑Straße 10, 60528 Frankfurt am Main,
Germany severe impairments across neuropsychological domains in
2 all illness phases, with a moderate worsening of a subset
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics
and Psychotherapy, University of Wuerzburg, of deficits in acute states [1]. Cross-sectional studies dem-
Fuechsleinstrasse 15, 97080 Würzburg, Germany onstrated that manic, mixed, and depressed BP perform

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

worse (especially in verbal memory and executive func- and memory. Similarly, a recent longitudinal study by Xu
tioning) compared to healthy controls (HC), whereas no et al. [17] compared 223 bipolar patients and 293 unipolar
or only marginal differences have been reported between depressed patients at baseline and after 6 weeks of treat-
depressed and (hypo-)manic BP [3–5]. This is surprising ment. In comparison with healthy controls (HC), bipo-
regarding the clinical presentation of cognitive patterns in lar and unipolar depressed patients were characterised by
the two phases: during mania, hyperactivity, thought rac- similar patterns of cognitive impairment (reduced speed,
ing, flight of ideas, disinhibited behaviour and pressure memory, verbal fluency, and executive functioning). During
of speech predominate, whereas depression is typically clinical remission, test performance in both patient groups
accompanied by psychomotor slowing, fatigue, egocentric improved, with the exception of deficits in speed and visual
restriction of thinking, and ruminations, all of which impair memory, which points to trait-like cognitive impairments in
cognitive speed. Several neuropsychological studies aimed those domains. However, it should be noted that patients in
to objectify cognitive dysfunctioning during acute episodes this study were in a relatively unstable state as follow-up
of BD and found similar deficits in attention, memory, and took place immediately after remission.
executive functions in bipolar and unipolar depression [6, In summary, longitudinal studies on the course of cog-
7], while cognitive deficits in acute mania seem to overlap nitive impairments in BD are rare, inconsistent, depend on
with impairments found in psychosis [8, 9]. small samples and interpretation is limited due to meth-
In addition, an emerging body of research demonstrates odological issues. The aim of the present study was there-
that cognitive disturbances in BD persist during remission fore to detect changes in cognitive performance from acute
[10], at least in a subgroup of BP [11–13]. However, up episodes to euthymia in BD. We investigated BP suffer-
to now studies in euthymic BP are inconsistent and have ing from acute depression or (hypo-)mania longitudinally
failed to report cognitive deficits specific to euthymic BD. (follow-up after at least 3 months of remission). Based on
The identification of trait-related variables, however, would previous findings, we expected cognitive impairments in
be helpful in studies on the pathophysiology of BD. Fur- acutely ill BP compared to a sample of HC [1]. Specifi-
thermore, stable trait markers could help to identify persons cally, we hypothesised that depressed BP would show psy-
with high risk of the disease and thus could facilitate early chomotor slowing and attentional deficits [7], while (hypo-)
diagnosis, prevention, and treatment options. Furthermore, manic BP would be impaired in frontal-executive functions
cognitive disturbances that persist in the absence of clinical like working memory, cognitive flexibility, and response
symptomatology may point to a genetically driven neuro- inhibition [8]. Regarding cognitive functioning over time,
cognitive endophenotype [14]. Within-subject longitudinal we assumed BP to improve during remission in all cogni-
studies across different mood states are the gold standard tive domains, but to still show mild cognitive disturbances
to determine whether cognitive deficits in BD are trait- or compared to HC. Neuropsychological performance in HC
state-dependent. However, so far there is little systematic should be stable over time. Furthermore, based on previous
research on the longitudinal course of cognitive functioning findings, we hypothesised that only a subgroup of remitted
in different clinical states in BD patients. Chaves et al. [15] BP show cognitive deficits [11]. Finally, we were interested
investigated clinically stable but symptomatic BD outpa- in the relationship between cognitive performance and
tients in a pre- and post-testing over a period of 3 months. demographic, clinical, and treatment variables of the bipo-
The authors assessed changes in mood symptoms in rela- lar sample.
tion to cognitive test performance at baseline and follow-
up. They found that BP showed persistent impairments in
psychomotor speed and attention over time. Neuropsycho- Materials and methods
logical measures were not influenced by variations in affec-
tive symptoms with the exception of verbal fluency and This study was performed at the Bipolar Disorder Program
verbal long-term memory, both of which were influenced of the Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psy-
by depressive symptoms. In contrast, Malhi et al. [16] chotherapy, University Hospital Wuerzburg. All procedures
investigated mood state-related cognitive deficits in BD followed the Declaration of Helsinki in its latest version
over a period of 30 months across all three phases of the ill- and were approved by the ethical committee of the medical
ness. Due to the small sample size, only four patients could faculty at the University of Wuerzburg. Written informed
be followed up in all three phases (depression, mania, and consent was obtained from all participants.
euthymia), and eight patients in two phases of the disorder.
The authors found modest impairments in attention, mem- Participants
ory, and executive tasks in depressed and manic patients,
while cognitive functions were largely normal during A total of 55 inpatients with BD (diagnosed according to
euthymia, showing only subtle impairments in attention DSM-IV criteria by an experienced psychiatrist) and 55

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

HC participated in the study. Groups were matched for sex, Clinical assessment
age, and years of education. Participants were 18–55 years
old and native German speakers. Intellectual abilities The following clinical characteristics (lifetime presence)
were estimated via the German multiple-choice word of BP were recorded via structured interviews with the
test (MWT-B) [18]. BP were initially assessed (baseline) patients, their relatives, and clinical records: number of
during an acute episode of depression (N = 35), mania episodes (depression, mania/hypomania, mixed episodes),
(N = 15), or hypomania (N = 5), all leading to inpatient bipolar subtype, age at illness onset, number of hospitalisa-
treatment. Within the acutely depressed sample, 20 patients tion, polarity of the first episode, attempted suicides, sub-
were diagnosed with BD type I and 15 with BD type II. stance abuse, psychotic symptoms, medication, ECT, fam-
Of the (hypo-)manic patients, 15 were diagnosed with BD ily history of mental disorders, and comorbid somatic (e.g.
type I and 5 with BD type II. Follow-up assessment took thyroid diseases) or mental illnesses, especially anxiety dis-
place the first time 3 months after discharge. Patients had orders and attention deficit disorder (ADHD). Patients were
to be euthymic for at least 3 months before the follow-up asked whether they had persistent sleep disturbances (ini-
testing was conducted. If patients were not fully remitted tial or middle insomnia) in the week preceding the inter-
at this 3-month mark, they were frequently contacted until view. Additionally, MADRS item 4 (reduced sleep) was
they reported having been euthymic for at least 12 weeks. analysed separately in order to assess sleep disorder in our
On average, follow-up took place after 21.1 ± 5.3 weeks. sample. Furthermore, current mood of all participants was
At both, baseline and follow-up, affective symptoms of BP recorded using the self-report Positive and Negative Affect
were measured with clinical interviews and questionnaires. Scale (PANAS) [23] prior to neuropsychological testing. In
We applied the observer-rating scales Montgomery–Asberg order to assess psychosocial functioning, all patients were
Depression Ratings Scale (MADRS) [19], the Young evaluated by the clinician with the Global Assessment of
Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) [20], and the self-rating Functioning Scale (GAF) [24].
questionnaire Beck Depression Inventory, Second Edition
(BDI-II) [21]. Criteria for euthymia were rating scores of Neurocognitive assessment
MADRS < 12, BDI-II < 15 and YMRS < 5 points. Further-
more, patients had to be on a stable medication for at least We conducted a 60-min neuropsychological testing
4 weeks at follow-up to qualify for follow-up assessment. (including a 5-min break). Five standardised tests were
Exclusion criteria for BP were previous head trauma, neu- administered in a fixed order by the same trained, expe-
rological illnesses, schizoaffective disorder, or present sub- rienced clinical psychologist (JV). Except the California
stance abuse. Patients who had received electroconvulsive Verbal Learning Test, all tests were computerised and par-
therapy (ECT) in the preceding 6 months were excluded as allel forms were used for the follow-up testing. Tests were
well. HC (N = 55) were recruited via Web-based announce- selected based on evidence of sensitivity to cognitive defi-
ments. They were screened for history of DSM-IV Axis I cits in bipolar disorder (see Table 1).
disorder by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inter-
view, German Version 5 [22]. Furthermore, persons with Statistical analysis
a history of substance abuse/taking of psychotropic drugs
and neurological diseases were excluded. Similar to BP, All statistical tests were performed using the Statistical
HC were tested in an initial and a follow-up assessment of Package for Social Sciences (SPSS), version 22.0 (IBM
approximately 3-month interval (13.3 ± 1.3 weeks). Corporation, Armonk NY, USA). For all tests, the signifi-
At the follow-up testing, 26 of 55 BP could not be cance level was set at p < .05 and Bonferroni–Holm cor-
retested in a euthymic mood state for the following reasons: rection for multiple testing was applied. Chi-square tests
7 BP (27 %) did not reach full remission during the full and analyses of variance (one-way ANOVAs) were used
observation period, 9 BP (35 %) had relapsed, 6 BP (23 %) to compare demographic variables and current mood by
were lost during follow-up, and 4 BP (15 %) did not wish group. In case of a significant F test, post hoc t tests were
to participate at follow-up. Comparison of completers and calculated. Neurocognitive and clinical variables were
non-completers revealed that patients who did not take part checked for outliers and normal distribution (Kolmogorov–
again were characterised by a higher number of episodes Smirnov test). Given that all test measures (except compat-
in their life (t = −1.79, p = .079), especially of depressive ible trials of Stroop Test) were not normally distributed, we
phases (t = −2.34, p = .024). Furthermore, they displayed conducted nonparametric tests. We used the Kruskal–Wal-
higher MADRS scores at the initial assessment (t = 1.90, lis one-way analysis of variance to test whether the three
p = .063), indicating a higher load with depression. Groups groups [depressed BP, (hypo-)manic BP, and HC] dif-
did not differ in other clinical variables. fered from each other on the baseline assessment. In case

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

Table 1  Neuropsychological tests


Cognitive domain Test Dependent variables

Psychomotor speed Compatible trials of the Stroop Test [66] Reactions times (RT) in ms
Attention Subtest Divided Attention, Test battery of Attentional Number of omissions
Performance (TAP) [67]
Short- and long-term memory California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) [68] Scores of total verbal learning, immediate and delayed
recall
Executive functioning Subtest Working Memory (n-back task of the TAP) Number of omissions
Cognitive Flexibility (shifting task of the TAP) Number of errors
Incompatible trials of the Stroop Test [66] Response inhibition (interference score)

of significant group differences, we conducted post hoc our sample of euthymic BP was very small, and the corre-
Table 2  Demographic characteristics of depressed and (hypo-)manic bipolar patients (BP) and healthy controls (HC)
Depressed BP (N = 35) Manic BP (N = 20) HC (N = 55) F/χ2 p value Post hoc t tests
N/Mean (SD) N/Mean (SD) N/Mean (SD)

Age 37.1 (11.7) 43.9 (9.7) 39.5 (9.6) 2.82 .064 –


Gender f/m 17/18 14/6 35/25 2.44 .296 –
Years of education 11.1 (1.7) 10.6 (1.6) 11.1 (1.5) 0.68 .505 –
Verbal IQa 104 [13] 109 (12) 106 (12) 3.18 .080 –
PANASb PAc (baseline) 20.9 (6.4) 33 (7.9) 32.8 (5.3) 45.5 <.001*** M > HC > D
PANAS NAd (baseline) 16.9 (5.8) 15.3 (5.3) 11.1 (1.4) 25.1 <.001*** HC < D&M
PANASPA (follow-up) 28.5 (7.9) 30.9 (6.2) 32.1 (5.5) 2.59 .081 HC > D
PANAS NA (follow-up) 13.5 (3.9) 13.7 (2.2) 11 (1.8) 10.60 <.001*** HC < D&M
a
Verbal IQ via multiple-choice word test (MWT-B)
b
Positive and Negative Affect Scale [momentary mood prior the testing at baseline and follow-up]
c
Score positive affect
d
Score negative affect

Mann–Whitney U tests. lational analysis was not the main hypotheses of the paper.
In a subsequent step, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were
used to check for differences in the repeated measurements
of each group. In order to check the test–retest reliability Results
of the neuropsychological tests, we examined performance
of HC over time and calculated Pearson correlation coef- Sample characteristics
ficients for each test between the initial and follow-up
assessments. Test–retest reliability was considered good Demographic characteristics and current mood at base-
if baseline and follow-up were significantly correlated line and follow-up of depressed/(hypo-)manic BP and HC
(p < 0.01). are presented in Table 2. There were no group differences
Furthermore, we compared the clinical characteristics of regarding age, gender, verbal IQ, and years of education
completers and non-completers (dropout analyses) using between groups. Mood ratings (measured by PANAS score)
Student t tests. Finally, exploratory correlational analyses at the initial assessment showed a significantly reduced
(Pearson coefficients) were conducted to assess associa- positive and increased negative affect in depressed patients
tions between test performance and demographic/clinical compared to (hypo-)manic patients and HC. At follow-up,
and treatment variables of BP. In order to control the influ- BP showed reduced positive and increased negative affect
ence of age on cognitive measures, we calculated partial compared to HC, despite being euthymic.
correlations with age as the covariate. We did not apply a Depressed and (hypo-)manic BP did not differ from
correction of p values in this exploratory analysis, because each other in their drug regimen with the exception of

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

Table 3  Mood ratings and medication of depressed and (hypo-)manic bipolar patients at baseline (acute) and follow-up (euthymic)
Depressed BP (Hypo-)manic BP
2
Baseline (acute) Follow-up (euthymic) t/X p Baseline (acute) Follow-up (euthymic) t/X2 p
(N = 35) (N = 20) (N = 20) (N = 9)

Mood ratings
MADRS 24.3 (5.8) 7.5 (2.9) 15.6 <.001*** 7.6 (4.8) 4.2 (2.7) 4.1 .003**
BDI-II 22.4 (10.1) 8.4 (5.7) 3.7 .002** 5.6 (4.8) 6.2 (3.7) −0.4 .709
YMRS 2.5 (1.5) 0.7 (0.9) 5.9 <.001*** 11.6 (2.7) 1.4 (2.6) 9.6 .000***
Medication
Antidepressants 27 (77.1 %) 15 (75 %) 4.4 .073 3 (15 %) 1 (11.1 %) 5.9 .154
Antipsychotics 25 (71.4 %) 13 (65 %) 12.1 <.001*** 16 (80 %) 6 (66.7 %) 5.1 .083
Lithium 19 (54.3 %) 12 (60 %) 16.3 <.001*** 11 (55 %) 4 (44.4 %) 5.7 .040*
Other MSa 10 (28.6 %) 7 (35 %) 12.4 .001** 5 (25 %) 1 (11.1 %) 9.0 .111
Benzodiazepines 6 (17.1 %) 2 (10 %) 2.1 .284 2 (10 %) 0 (0 %) – –

BP bipolar patients, MADRS Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale, BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory, YMRS Young Mania Rating
Scale
a
Mood stabilisers: valproate, carbamazepine, lamotrigine

antidepressant add-on therapy, which was prescribed sig- four (depressed) BP (7.3 %) showed a test performance in
nificantly more often in acutely depressed patients (see the normal range of the normative data group. These four
Table 3). Overall, 11 (37.9 %) of patients had an altered patients without cognitive deficits differed not in clinical or
drug regimen at follow-up as compared to the initial meas- demographic variables compared to acutely ill patients with
urement. HC were completely medication free. At follow- cognitive deficits.
up, patients reported being in a euthymic state over a period
of 15.4 (SD 2.8) weeks. Mood ratings in the acute and Comparison of cognitive performance
remitted sample are demonstrated in Table 3. GAF scores between euthymic bipolar patients and healthy controls
indicated a marginal reduced psychosocial functioning in (follow‑up testing)
euthymic BP (M = 76.5; SD 9.9).
At follow-up, euthymic BP differed significantly from HC
Comparison of cognitive performance between acutely in Working Memory and delayed verbal memory. Although
ill bipolar patients and healthy controls (baseline we found marginal reduced performances in the other test
testing) measures in euthymic BP compared to HC, differences did
not reach level of significance (see Table 5). In comparison
Acutely depressed and (hypo-)manic BP showed a sig- with the normative data of each test, 17 (58.6 %) euthymic
nificantly reduced test performance compared to HC in BP performed below the average in at least one test meas-
the test measures. However, some group differences did ure (cut-off score: 1.5 SD below the average of normative
not reach level of significance after the correction of the p data group), while 12 BP (41.4 %) showed no cognitive
value according to Bonferroni–Holm method for multiple deficits at all. The extent of cognitive deficits during remis-
testing. The results in Table 4 demonstrate that depressed sion was not associated with the test performance during
BP showed a significantly reduced psychomotor speed and the acute episode.
a lower response inhibition (reading colours, Stroop Test)
compared to HC. (Hypo-)manic BP showed a significantly Change in cognitive performance (baseline to follow‑up)
reduced performance in Working Memory. Both patient in bipolar patients and healthy controls
groups were characterised by more omission errors in the
test assessing divided attention (trend). No significant dif- Differences in repeated measurements of each group
ferences in total verbal learning (CVLT) and Cognitive were analysed using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test.
Flexibility were observed between depressed, (hypo-)manic Depressed BP showed an improvement from baseline
BP, and HC (for more detail, see Table 4). In the acutely to follow-up in the compatible trials of the Stroop Test
admitted bipolar sample, 51 BP (92.7 %) showed cogni- Reading (Z = −2.98, p = .003) and Naming (Z = −2.49,
tive deficits (cut-off score: at least one test measure 1.5 SD p = .013), a marginal improvement in the immediate verbal
below the average of normative data group), while only recall (Z = −1.71, p = .088) and the Stroop Test Naming

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

Table 4  Group analyses of cognitive performance (baseline) in acutely depressed, (hypo-)manic bipolar patients and healthy controls
Cognitive domain Depressed BP baseline Manic BP baseline HC baseline Analysis of group differences
(N = 35) Mean (SD) (N = 20) Mean (N = 55) Mean Kruskal–Wallis p valuea Post hoc Mann–Whit-
(SD) (SD) X2 test ney U test

Psychomotor speed
Stroop Reading compatible 799 (119) 772 (107) 725 (72) 17.94 .001** HC > D
(RT)
Stroop Naming compatible 770 (118) 720 (116) 673 (81) 12.57 .016* HC > D
(RT)
Attention
Divided Attention (Omis- 2.1 (2.1) 2.2 (1.4) 1.2 (1.3) 9.34 .054 HC > D&M
sions)
Memory
CVLT (total verbal learn- 60.2 (10.1) 57.8 (11.2) 61.2 (7.2) 1.44 .488 –
ing)
CVLT (immediate recall) 11.7 (2.3) 11.7 (2.8) 13.1 (1.8) 7.02 .120 –
CVLT (delayed recall) 11.2 (2.5) 11.8 (2.6) 13.3 (2) 6.67 .180 –
Executive functions
Working Memory (Omis- 2.9 (3) 4.9 (4.3) 1.6 (1.9) 13.5 .009** HC > M
sions)
Cognitive Flexibility 3.1 (5.1) 4.8 (7.6) 1.5 (1.9) 5.5 .128 –
(Errors)
Stroop Reading incompat- 225 (152) 181 (104) 134 (78) 10.58 .028* HC > D
ible (RT)
Stroop Naming incompat- 121 (71) 183 (102) 119 (66) 6.19 .135 –
ible (RT)

BP bipolar patients, HC healthy controls, D depressed patients, M (hypo-)manic patients, CVLT California Verbal Learning Test, RT reaction
time in ms
a
p value corrected by Bonferroni–Holm method

(Z = −1.77, p = .077). (Hypo-)manic patients showed an learning (r12 = .577, p < .001), CVLT immediate recall
improvement in the Working Memory task (Z = −2.67, (r12 = .664, p < .001), CVLT delayed recall (r12 = .579,
p = .008) and the Stroop Test Naming (Z = −2.07, p < .001), Working Memory (r12 = .556, p < .001), Cogni-
p = .038). A detailed table of the test statistics of changes tive Flexibility (r12 = .152, p = .246), and the incompatible
in cognitive performance can be found in the supplemen- trials of the Stroop Test Reading (r12 = .654, p < .001) and
tary material (table S1). Figure 1 illustrates z score trans- Naming (r12 = .531, p < .001).
formation of the test scores (healthy controls as reference)
of acutely depressed, (hypo-)manic, and euthymic patients Association of cognitive performance and clinical
in order to facilitate comparison of the cognitive profile of variables in BP
the different clinical states of illness.
Performance of HC improved in the same tests as BP, In order to test for associations between clinical variables
namely the compatible trials of the Stroop Test (Z = −4.82, and cognitive functioning in the euthymic BP, exploratory
p < .001), the immediate recall of the CVLT (Z = −3.16, correlational analyses were conducted. We found signifi-
p = .002), and the Working Memory task (Z = −2.09, cant influences of subthreshold depressive symptoms and
p = .036). This indicates learning and practice effects in sleep disturbances on cognitive deficits in BD after con-
these subtests. Thus, we calculated test–retest reliability by trolling for age (see Table 6). Psychomotor speed (compat-
Pearson correlations, which revealed that with the exception ible trials of Stroop Reading) was associated with MADRS
of Cognitive Flexibility, initial and follow-up performance score (p = .036) and reduced sleep (p = .015); Stroop Test
scores were highly correlated, indicating good test–retest Naming was correlated with reduced sleep (p = .018).
reliability for the compatible trials of Stroop Test Reading Divided Attention was associated with BDI-II depression
(r12 = .887, p < .001) and Naming (r12 = .815, p < .001), score (p = .036) and reduced sleep (p = .039). Total verbal
Divided Attention (r12 = .490, p < .001), CVLT total verbal learning (CVLT) was correlated with MADRS (p = .013),

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

Table 5  Group analyses of cognitive performance (follow-up testing) in euthymic bipolar patients and healthy controls
Cognitive domain Euthymic BP follow-up HC follow-up Analysis of group differences
(N = 29) Mean (SD) (N = 55) Mean (SD) Mann–Whitney U test p valuea

Psychomotor speed
Stroop Reading compatible (RT) 749 (115) 697 (75) 584.5 .315
Stroop Naming compatible (RT) 717 (129) 656 (84) 597.5 .360
Attention
Divided Attention (Omissions) 1.5 (2.1) 0.9 (1.3) 674.5 .872
Memory
CVLT (total verbal learning) 58.2 (9.1) 59.8 (8.4) 711.5 .836
CVLT (immediate recall) 12.3 (2.1) 13.4 (2.5) 550.0 .152
CVLT (delayed recall) 11.9 (2.5) 13.6 (2.2) 469 .020*
Executive functions
Working Memory (Omissions) 2.0 (1.7) 1.1 (1.4) 500.5 .027*
Cognitive Flexibility (Errors) 2.4 (2.6) 1.6 (1.8) 655.5 .855
Stroop Reading incompatible (RT) 165 (145) 130 (59) 738.5 .579
Stroop Naming incompatible (RT) 139 (113) 107 (64) 693.5 .984

BP bipolar patients, HC healthy controls, CVLT California Verbal Learning Test, RT reaction time in ms
a
p value corrected by Bonferroni–Holm method

Fig. 1  Mean z score trans- 2


formations of test scores of
depressed, (hypo-)manic, and 1.8 *
euthymic bipolar patients (z
scores were calculated using 1.6
mean and SD values of sample Depressed BP
of healthy controls) (*signifi- 1.4
cant differences after Bonfer-
(Baseline)
1.2 * *
roni–Holm correction; for
details, see Tables 4 and 5) 1 ** Manic BP
(Baseline)
0.8 *
* Euthymic BP
0.6
(Follow-up)
0.4
0.2
0

BDI-II (p = .006), and reduced sleep (p = .001), and with YMRS (p = .030). Furthermore, higher BDI-II scores
delayed recall in the CVLT was associated with MADRS were associated with more errors in the Cognitive Flex-
(p = .025), BDI-II (p = .021), and reduced sleep ibility test (p = .019). Therefore, impairment in most test
(p = .009). Interestingly, short-term recall was correlated measures was associated with subclinical depression and/

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

Table 6  Association (rpartial) of clinical characteristics and test performance (follow-up testing) of euthymic bipolar patients
Pearson correlation (rp, age as covariate)
MADRS BDI YMRS Sleep Age at onset Duration Total epi- Depressive Manic Psychotic Anti-psy-
illness sodes episodes episodes symptoms chotics

Psychomotor speed
Stroop .450* .308 .137 .453* −.104 .104 .099 .037 .089 −.166 −.041
Reading
compatible
(RT)
Stroop .322 .367 .232 .445* .046 −.046 −.042 −.124 −.036 −.094 .118
Naming
compatible
(RT)
Attention
Divided .302 .391* .271 .385* .251 −.085 .160 .117 .137 −.245 .160
Attention
(Omis-
sions)
Memory
CVLT (total −.481* −.520** −.033 −.596** −.028 .043 −.185 −.123 −.217 .322 −.364
verbal
learning)
CVLT −.276 −.332 −.427* −.166 −.310 .305 .365 .113 −.016 .221 −.067
(immediate
recall)
CVLT −.438* −.452* −.181 −.502** −.203 .199 −.057 −.079 −.159 .337 −.144
(delayed
recall)
Executive functions
Working .313 .337 .171 .268 .213 −.099 −.048 .006 .091 .432* −.104
Memory
(Omis-
sions)
Cognitive .101 .434* .075 .353 .303 −.295 −.210 −.101 .016 −.140 .125
Flexibility
(Errors)
Stroop .066 .260 .064 −.092 .258 −.258 −.266 −.312 −.188 −.144 .047
Reading
incompat-
ible
Stroop .229 .127 −.139 .160 .185 −.095 −.036 −.071 .012 .027 −.029
Naming
incompat-
ible

Significant correlations are indicated in bold


MADRS Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale, BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory, second edition, YMRS Young Mania Rating Scale,
CVLT California Verbal Learning Test, RT reaction time in ms
* p < .05; ** p < .01

or sleep disturbances except for Working Memory, which BP, we expected to see the same pattern in acutely ill BP.
was significantly associated with psychotic symptoms However, the performance in the test measures was not cor-
(lifetime) (p = .015). There was no association of cogni- related with depression scores in acutely depressed BP or
tive performance and other clinical characteristics indicat- YMRS scores in (hypo-)manic patients, respectively.
ing disease severity such as number of previous episodes Psychosocial functioning was assessed using the GAF.
or age of onset. Given these results suggesting that mood In euthymic BP, we found negative correlations between
symptoms have a negative effect on cognition in euthymic GAF score and omission errors in the Working Memory

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

task (r = −.454, p = .013) and reaction time in compat- showed considerable deficits in divided attention and work-
ible trials of the Stroop Test (r = −.562, p = .002). Fur- ing memory. Accordingly, previous investigations found
thermore, reduced mnestic performance was highly associ- impaired cognition in manic patients, especially regarding
ated with low psychosocial functioning, as indicated by a deficits in frontal-executive measures [8, 33, 34]. There-
positive correlation between GAF score and verbal learning fore, we confirmed a different cognitive profile during
(CVLT) (r = .529, p = .003). depression and (hypo-)mania, in that the test performance
of depressed BP was comparable to the cognitive deficits
found in major depression [6, 7], and the cognitive per-
Discussion formance of (hypo-)manic patients was similar to deficits
found in psychosis [8, 9]. Interestingly, our results showed
The present within-subject longitudinal study investigated that the differences between depressed and (hypo-)manic
the neurocognitive performance of bipolar inpatients in an BP were only marginal, and with the exception of the
acute episode of depression or (hypo-)mania and during Stroop Test, they did not reach significance. Possibly, the
remission. We found broad impairments in cognitive speed, neuropsychological tests are not sensitive enough to depict
attention, working memory, verbal memory, and execu- the marginal differences of a depressive and hypomanic
tive functioning in acutely admitted BP compared to HC. symptomatology.
As expected, depressed and (hypo-)manic patients showed
a different cognitive profile, in that depressed patients had Cognitive deficits in remitted bipolar patients
slowed reaction times while (hypo-)manic patients were
more impaired in executive functioning. However, the dif- Due to these difficulties in interpreting cognitive abilities
ferences were marginal and only the performance in the in acutely ill BP regarding the pathophysiology of the dis-
Stroop Test (Naming of colour words with incongruent ink) order, it is important to investigate the long-term course
differed significantly between depressed and (hypo-)manic within subjects. Unfortunately, longitudinal studies about
BP. After at least 3 months of remission BP performance cognitive deficits in BD are rare and our study is one of
generally improved, albeit we found persisting impairments the few performing a follow-up measurement after stable
in working memory and delayed verbal memory. Further- remission. We found that patients’ neurocognitive perfor-
more, we confirmed previous data in that at least some cog- mance improved after 3 months of remission. Particularly,
nitive measures were associated with subthreshold depres- depressed BP no longer displayed a reduced psychomotor
sive symptoms and sleep disturbances of euthymic BP, but speed compared to HC. (Hypo-)manic patients improved
not with clinical variables indicating severity of illness significantly in the Working Memory task. However, even
[11]. In addition, we found strong associations between in an euthymic mood state, our sample of BP showed sig-
worse cognitive performance and reduced daily life func- nificant deficits in working memory and delayed verbal
tioning (GAF score). This is in line with previous investiga- memory compared to HC. This coincides with previous
tions demonstrating that neuropsychological performance studies which demonstrated an impaired verbal memory
predicts functional adjustment [25]. [35–37] and working memory [5, 38, 39] as prominent
deficits of BD. Furthermore, we found that 58.6 % of our
Cognitive impairments in acutely ill bipolar patients sample showed cognitive impairments, while 41.4 % had a
test performance within the average of the normative data
In detail, we specified the clinical presentation of cogni- group. Therefore, we confirmed previous study results that
tive disturbances in acutely depressed BP, in that patients only a subgroup of euthymic BP seems to have cognitive
suffered of reduced psychomotor speed compared to deficits [11, 12].
(hypo-)manic patients and HC. Furthermore, patients We found that performance in working memory was exten-
were impaired in divided attention and response inhibi- sively reduced in patients with acute (hypo-)mania and in
tion (Stroop Test Reading). This is in line with previ- remitted patients who reported psychotic symptoms (lifetime).
ous studies which demonstrated a cognitive slowing and Working memory deficits have been demonstrated as core
attentional deficits in acutely depressed BP [7, 26–28], impairment in psychotic disorders, and BP were suggested to
and also in executive functioning [29–31]. However, atten- have comparable deficits to schizophrenic patients, albeit less
tion and cognitive speed are very basic processes which severe [40, 41]. Interestingly, Simonsen [42] showed that neu-
underlie other cognitive functions, and deficits in atten- rocognitive dysfunction in BD and schizophrenia was deter-
tion can thus lead to greater distractibility and interference mined more by the history of psychosis than by the diagnos-
effects [32]. Therefore, deficits in executive functioning in tic category or subtype. These results indicate that deficits in
depressed BP could be secondary to attentional deficits. working memory may be related to neuronal changes related
Similarly to the depressed sample, (hypo-)manic patients to the psychotic spectrum. In addition, there is evidence of

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

structural abnormalities in brain regions associated with work- demonstrated that sleep and circadian rhythms are involved
ing memory in BD [43]. Neuroimaging studies have shown in the cognitive deficits in BD through overlapping neuro-
altered neuronal activation within the brain network involved biological systems [60]. In our bipolar sample, sleep distur-
in working memory tasks [44]. Most imaging studies in bances were associated with impairments in psychomotor
BD reported a loss of connectivity in prefrontal and parietal speed, divided attention, and verbal learning. In a recently
networks, structures that are well known to be involved in published study, we reported accordingly that several cog-
working memory [45]. Taken together, there is evidence that nitive impairments in euthymic BP were predicted by sleep
working memory is a trait biomarker in BD although being disorder [11]. Taken together this underscores the need
aggravated by acute symptomatology in our study. Also our for more extensive research on the influence of sleep dis-
correlational analyses are in favour of this hypothesis: psycho- turbances on cognitive deficits in BD. In conclusion, our
motor speed, attention and verbal memory were significantly results suggest that residual symptoms negatively affect
associated with residual symptoms (MADRS, BDI-II, and cognitive functioning in BD. Therefore, an optimised
sleep disturbances). On the contrary, working memory was not antidepressant treatment and sleep regulation should be
correlated with any actual mood symptoms, which is in agree- enforced in patients complaining about cognitive impair-
ment with a recently published study about predictors of cog- ments. Even though our results do not disprove the idea of
nition in euthymic BP [11]. In line with these results, impair- neurodegenerative processes in BD [1], our data underscore
ments in executive functions including working memory were the importance of residual symptoms. This also has impli-
considered as salient endophenotypic components of cogni- cations for future research and calls for the improvement of
tion in BD as they persist in remission, appear to be heritable, treatment options for cognitively impaired BP.
and hence co-segregate within families [46, 47]. Therefore, Comparable to the influence of subdepressive symptoms
our study provides further evidence that working memory on test performance of euthymic BP, we expected a relation-
could serve as a potential neurocognitive endophenotype in ship between severity of depression and test performance
remitted BP. in acutely ill BP. Very surprisingly though, we did not find
associations between the severity of depression or (hypo-)
Association between cognition and clinical variables mania and neurocognitive measures in acutely admitted
BP. This might well be due to a ceiling effect of the effect
Regarding the clinical characteristics of our bipolar sam- on affective measures on neuropsychological impairment.
ple, we found no relationship between test measures and Finally, it has to be discussed how much time patients need
disease characteristics indicating severity of the disorder to fully recover after an acute episode. We defined a period
(e.g. number of episodes, age of onset, bipolar type). How- of 3 months of euthymia preceding neurocognitive assess-
ever, subclinical mood symptoms and sleep disturbances ment in order to ensure a relatively stable state. Neverthe-
were associated with cognitive speed, attention, and verbal less, some of the remitted BP reported residual symptoms.
memory. Although patients with bipolar disorder have his- Interestingly, in a recent study, Torres et al. [61] investigated
torically been characterised as returning to full remission first-episode patients three times within the first year follow-
between affective episodes, recent studies demonstrated ing their initial manic episode. Patients’ cognitive function-
that some residual symptoms may persist [48, 49]. It has ing was improved 6 months after discharge and even further
been previously demonstrated that subthreshold mood at follow-up after 1 year. Therefore, it seems that it takes a
symptoms are associated with impairments in attention long time to fully cognitively recover after an acute episode,
and verbal memory [11, 50–52]. In a study by Bonnin et al. and we recommend more research on BP who have been
[53], the degree of cognitive functioning was best predicted remitted for a period of at least a year although these studies
by subdepressive symptoms, and a meta-analysis reported are inherently difficult to conduct.
that more rigorously defined euthymia goes along with
smaller cognitive impairments [2]. Therefore, it seems that Limitations
the neuronal circuitry activated in the modulation of emo-
tions overlaps with brain regions involved in neurocogni- Among the limitations of the present study is the rela-
tion. Besides subclinical depressive symptoms, 37 % of our tively small sample size, especially at follow-up, which is
remitted bipolar sample reported sleep disturbances like inherent to these types of studies. This nevertheless raises
initial or middle insomnia or an abnormal increased sleep- the possibility of selective attrition and whether non-sig-
ing time. Rates of sleep disturbances are usually very high nificant findings can be related to insufficient statistical
in remitted BP [54, 55], and BD is highly associated with power. Indeed, we found that patients who dropped out
sleep and circadian rhythm abnormalities [49, 56, 57]. It is had a higher load with depression (more number of pre-
well known that sleep deprivation in healthy people causes vious depressive episodes and higher depression scores at
cognitive deterioration [58, 59]. Furthermore, it has been baseline). Therefore, the study completers may have a less

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

severe progress of disease. Due to the naturalistic nature the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All
of the study, we had only limited control of pharmacologi- authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
cal effects on cognition. Although there were no statistical Compliance with ethical standards
differences between depressed and (hypo-)manic groups
on that regard, the effects of medications for bipolar disor- Conflict of interest None of the authors have conflict of interest with
der on cognition are not totally understood [62]. As some the contents of this paper or financial ties to disclose.
BP had changes in drugs and dosing within the interval
between baseline and follow-up, interpretation of changes
in cognitive performance is limited. In particular, (hypo-) References
manic patients received antipsychotics at baseline and
1. Kurtz MM, Gerraty RT (2009) A meta-analytic investigation of
drugs were tapered at follow-up. A methodological prob- neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of
lem which has to be addressed is to determine the extent of clinical state. Neuropsychology 23:551–562
practice effects in the pre-post-testing. We thus included a 2. Mann-Wrobel MC, Carreno JT, Dickinson D (2011) Meta-analy-
sufficient sample size of HC to calculate test–retest reliabil- sis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disor-
der: an update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar
ity allowing to control for practice effects. Disord 13:334–342
3. Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Reinares M, Colom F, Torrent C,
Summary and conclusions Sanchez-Moreno J et al (2004) Cognitive function across manic
or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disor-
der. Am J Psychiatry 161:262–270
Although these limitations warrant attention, the results of the 4. Basso MR, Lowery N, Neel J, Purdie R, Bornstein RA (2002)
present study add the existing literature to cognitive function- Neuropsychological impairment among manic, depressed, and
ing in BD. To our knowledge, this is the first study present- mixed-episode inpatients with bipolar disorder. Neuropsychol-
ing within-subject pre- and post-testing with acutely admit- ogy 16:84–91
5. Ryan KA, Vederman AC, McFadden EM, Weldon AL, Kamali
ted BP in depression or (hypo-)mania and during euthymia. M, Langenecker SA et al (2012) Differential executive function-
We found that cognitive deficits in BD are exacerbated dur- ing performance by phase of bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord
ing acute episodes and partially recover during remission. 14:527–536
Working memory and verbal memory were still impaired in 6. Wolfe J, Granholm E, Butters N, Saunders E, Janowsky D
(1987) Verbal memory deficits associated with major affective
euthymic BP compared to HC, indicating these two cogni- disorders: a comparison of unipolar and bipolar patients. J Affect
tive domains as trait-related. The present study demonstrated Disord 13:83–92
that “state” factors like subclinical symptoms and persisting 7. Godard J, Grondin S, Baruch P, Lafleur MF (2011) Psycho-
sleep disturbances contribute to cognitive impairment and are social and neurocognitive profiles in depressed patients with
major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res
potential confounders in the search of cognitive endopheno- 190:244–252
types in BD. Furthermore, our results implicate the need for 8. McGrath J, Scheldt S, Welham J, Clair A (1997) Performance
interventions that improve cognitive dysfunctions in remitted on tests sensitive to impaired executive ability in schizophrenia,
BP. In the clinical setting, an optimised antidepressant treat- mania and well controls: acute and subacute phases. Schizophr
Res 26:127–137
ment and sleep regulation could improve cognitive deficits 9. Murphy FC, Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Rogers RD, Robbins
which are caused by residual symptoms. Moreover, some TW, Paykel ES et al (2001) Decision-making cognition in mania
studies demonstrated promising results in that euthymic BP and depression. Psychol Med 31:679–693
benefit from a cognitive training with regard to neurocog- 10. Torres IJ, Boudreau VG, Yatham LN (2007) Neuropsychological
functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta
nitive and psychosocial functioning [63–65]. However, to Psychiatr Scand Suppl 434:17–26
clarify trait versus disease process effects in cognition in BD, 11. Volkert J, Kopf J, Kazmaier J, Glaser F, Zierhut KC, Schiele
more studies with a prospective design, especially with high- MA, et al. (2015) Evidence for cognitive subgroups in bipolar
risk groups and first-episode patients, are needed. disorder and the influence of subclinical depression and sleep
disturbances. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol J Eur Coll Neuropsy-
chopharmacol 25(2):192–202
Acknowledgments We acknowledge all participants who took 12. Aminoff SR, Hellvin T, Lagerberg TV, Berg AO, Andreassen
part in the present study. JV was supported by a grant of the German OA, Melle I (2013) Neurocognitive features in subgroups of
Excellence Initiative to the Graduate School of Life Sciences, Uni- bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 15:272–283
versity of Wuerzburg. JK and AR received support by the DFG and 13. Iverson GL, Brooks BL, Langenecker SA, Young AH (2011)
Laender funds RTG 1253/2 “Gk emotions”, AR and KZ received sup- Identifying a cognitive impairment subgroup in adults with
port by the DFG-funded study “earlyCBT” (BA 1504/7-1), AR and mood disorders. J Affect Disord 132:360–367
MAS received support by the DFG-funded study SFB TRR 58 Z02, 14. Raust A, Daban C, Cochet B, Henry C, Bellivier F, Scott J
and AR was supported by the Comprehensive Heart Failure Center (2014) Neurocognitive performance as an endophenotype for
Wuerzburg funded by the BMBF (project 01EO1004). bipolar disorder. Front Biosci (Elite Ed). 6:89–103
15. Chaves OC, Lombardo LE, Bearden CE, Woolsey MD, Martinez
Author contributions JV and AR designed the study and wrote the DM, Barrett JA et al (2011) Association of clinical symptoms
protocol. JV assisted the literature searches and analyses, undertook

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

and neurocognitive performance in bipolar disorder: a longitudi- 35. Bourne C, Aydemir O, Balanza-Martinez V, Bora E, Brissos S,
nal study. Bipolar Disord 13:118–123 Cavanagh JT et al (2013) Neuropsychological testing of cog-
16. Malhi GS, Ivanovski B, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Mitchell PB, Vieta nitive impairment in euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual
E, Sachdev P (2007) Neuropsychological deficits and functional patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand 128:149–162
impairment in bipolar depression, hypomania and euthymia. 36. Arts B, Jabben N, Krabbendam L, van Os J (2008) Meta-anal-
Bipolar Disord 9:114–125 yses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and
17. Xu G, Lin K, Rao D, Dang Y, Ouyang H, Guo Y et al (2012) their first-degree relatives. Psychol Med 38:771–785
Neuropsychological performance in bipolar I, bipolar II and 37. Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Torrent C, Sanchez-Moreno J,
unipolar depression patients: a longitudinal, naturalistic study. J Goikolea JM, Salamero M et al (2007) Functional outcome in
Affect Disord 136:328–339 bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipo-
18. Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B (1995) Multiple choice vocabulary lar Disord 9:103–113
test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intel- 38. Hellvin T, Sundet K, Simonsen C, Aminoff SR, Lagerberg TV,
ligence. Acta Neurol Scand 91:335–345 Andreassen OA et al (2012) Neurocognitive functioning in
19. Montgomery SA, Asberg M (1979) A new depression scale patients recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord
designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 14:227–238
134:382–389 39. Thompson JM, Gallagher P, Hughes JH, Watson S, Gray JM,
20. Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA (1978) A rating Ferrier IN et al (2005) Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic
scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychia- patients with bipolar affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry J Ment
try J Ment Sci 133:429–435 Sci 186:32–40
21. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W (1996) Comparison of 40. Bearden CE, Shih VH, Green MF, Gitlin M, Sokolski KN, Lev-
Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpa- ander E et al (2011) The impact of neurocognitive impairment on
tients. J Pers Assess 67:588–597 occupational recovery of clinically stable patients with bipolar
22. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, disorder: a prospective study. Bipolar Disord 13:323–333
Weiller E et al (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric 41. Brandt CL, Eichele T, Melle I, Sundet K, Server A, Agartz I
Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a struc- et al (2014) Working memory networks and activation patterns
tured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: comparison with healthy
J Clin Psychiatry 59(20):22–33 (quiz 4-57) controls. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 204:290–298
23. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A (1988) Development and vali- 42. Simonsen C, Sundet K, Vaskinn A, Birkenaes AB, Engh JA,
dation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the Faerden A et al (2011) Neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar
PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1063–1070 and schizophrenia spectrum disorders depends on history of psy-
24. Hall RC (1995) Global assessment of functioning. A modified chosis rather than diagnostic group. Schizophr Bull 37:73–83
scale. Psychosomatics 36:267–275 43. Strakowski SM, DelBello MP, Adler C, Cecil DM, Sax KW
25. Martino DJ, Marengo E, Igoa A, Scapola M, Ais ED, Perinot L (2000) Neuroimaging in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord
et al (2009) Neurocognitive and symptomatic predictors of func- 2:148–164
tional outcome in bipolar disorders: a prospective 1 year follow- 44. Adler CM, Holland SK, Schmithorst V, Tuchfarber MJ, Stra-
up study. J Affect Disord 116:37–42 kowski SM (2004) Changes in neuronal activation in patients
26. Holmes MK, Erickson K, Luckenbaugh DA, Drevets WC, Bain with bipolar disorder during performance of a working memory
EE, Cannon DM et al (2008) A comparison of cognitive func- task. Bipolar Disord 6:540–549
tioning in medicated and unmedicated subjects with bipolar 45. Cremaschi L, Penzo B, Palazzo M, Dobrea C, Cristoffanini M,
depression. Bipolar Disord 10:806–815 Dell’Osso B et al (2013) Assessing working memory via N-back
27. Burdick KE, Gunawardane N, Goldberg JF, Halperin JM, Garno task in euthymic bipolar I disorder patients: a review of func-
JL, Malhotra AK (2009) Attention and psychomotor functioning tional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neuropsychobiology
in bipolar depression. Psychiatry Res 166:192–200 68:63–70
28. Gallagher P, Gray JM, Watson S, Young AH, Ferrier IN (2014) 46. Glahn DC, Almasy L, Barguil M, Hare E, Peralta JM, Kent JW
Neurocognitive functioning in bipolar depression: a component Jr et al (2010) Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disor-
structure analysis. Psychol Med 44:961–974 der identified in multiplex multigenerational families. Arch Gen
29. Borkowska A, Rybakowski JK (2001) Neuropsychological fron- Psychiatry 67:168–177
tal lobe tests indicate that bipolar depressed patients are more 47. Balanza-Martinez V, Rubio C, Selva-Vera G, Martinez-Aran A,
impaired than unipolar. Bipolar Disord 3:88–94 Sanchez-Moreno J, Salazar-Fraile J et al (2008) Neurocognitive
30. Ha TH, Chang JS, Oh SH, Kim JS, Cho HS, Ha K (2014) Dif- endophenotypes (endophenocognitypes) from studies of rela-
ferential patterns of neuropsychological performance in the tives of bipolar disorder subjects: a systematic review. Neurosci
euthymic and depressive phases of bipolar disorders. Psychiatry Biobehav Rev 32:1426–1438
Clin Neurosci 68:515–523 48. Vieta E, Sanchez-Moreno J, Lahuerta J, Zaragoza S (2008)
31. Rubinsztein JS, Michael A, Underwood BR, Tempest M, Saha- Subsyndromal depressive symptoms in patients with bipolar
kian BJ (2006) Impaired cognition and decision-making in and unipolar disorder during clinical remission. J Affect Disord
bipolar depression but no ‘affective bias’ evident. Psychol Med 107:169–174
36:629–639 49. Samalin L, Llorca PM, Giordana B, Milhiet V, Yon L, El-Hage
32. Sackeim HA, Steif B (1988) Neuropsychology of depression and W et al (2014) Residual symptoms and functional performance
mania. Elsevier, Amsterdam in a large sample of euthymic bipolar patients in France (the
33. Sweeney JA, Kmiec JA, Kupfer DJ (2000) Neuropsychologic OPTHYMUM study). J Affect Disord 159:94–102
impairments in bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the 50. McKay AP, Tarbuck AF, Shapleske J, McKenna PJ (1995) Neu-
CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biol Psychiatry 48:674–684 ropsychological function in manic-depressive psychosis. Evi-
34. Fleck DE, Shear PK, Madore M, Strakowski SM (2008) Wiscon- dence for persistent deficits in patients with chronic, severe ill-
sin Card Sorting Test performance in bipolar disorder: effects of ness. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 167:51–57
mood state and early course. Bipolar Disord 10:539–545 51. Palsson E, Figueras C, Johansson AG, Ekman CJ, Hultman
B, Ostlind J et al (2013) Neurocognitive function in bipolar

13
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci

disorder: a comparison between bipolar I and II disorder and 61. Torres IJ, Kozicky J, Popuri S, Bond DJ, Honer WG, Lam RW
matched controls. BMC Psychiatry 13:165 et al (2014) 12-month longitudinal cognitive functioning in
52. Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, del Mar Bonnin C, Reinares M, patients recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord
Daban C, Sole B et al (2012) Long-term outcome of cognitive 16:159–171
impairment in bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 73:e899–e905 62. Dias VV, Balanza-Martinez V, Soeiro-de-Souza MG, Moreno
53. Bonnin CM, Sanchez-Moreno J, Martinez-Aran A, Sole B, Rein- RA, Figueira ML, Machado-Vieira R et al (2012) Pharmacologi-
ares M, Rosa AR et al (2012) Subthreshold symptoms in bipolar cal approaches in bipolar disorders and the impact on cognition:
disorder: impact on neurocognition, quality of life and disability. a critical overview. Acta Psychiatr Scand 126:315–331
J Affect Disord 136:650–659 63. Deckersbach T, Nierenberg AA, Kessler R, Lund HG, Ametrano
54. Sylvia LG, Dupuy JM, Ostacher MJ, Cowperthwait CM, Hay RM, Sachs G et al (2010) RESEARCH: Cognitive rehabilitation
AC, Sachs GS et al (2012) Sleep disturbance in euthymic bipolar for bipolar disorder: An open trial for employed patients with
patients. J Psychopharmacol 26:1108–1112 residual depressive symptoms. CNS Neurosci Ther 16:298–307
55. Harvey AG (2008) Sleep and circadian rhythms in bipolar disor- 64. Torrent C, Bonnin Cdel M, Martinez-Aran A, Valle J, Amann
der: seeking synchrony, harmony, and regulation. Am J Psychia- BL, Gonzalez-Pinto A et al (2013) Efficacy of functional reme-
try 165:820–829 diation in bipolar disorder: a multicenter randomized controlled
56. Murray G, Harvey A (2010) Circadian rhythms and sleep in study. Am J Psychiatry 170:852–859
bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 12:459–472 65. Preiss M, Shatil E, Cermakova R, Cimermanova D, Ram I (2013)
57. Scott J (2011) Clinical parameters of circadian rhythms in affec- Personalized cognitive training in unipolar and bipolar disorder:
tive disorders. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol J Eur Coll Neuropsy- a study of cognitive functioning. Front Hum Neurosci 7:108
chopharmacol 21(Suppl 4):S671–S675 66. Puhr U, Wagner M (2011) Interferenztest nach Stroop. Version
58. Durmer JS, Dinges DF (2005) Neurocognitive consequences of 24- Rev. 1. Mödling: Schuhfried GmbH
sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol 25:117–129 67. Zimmermann PF (2011) B. Testbattery of attentional perfor-
59. Drummond SP, Brown GG (2001) The effects of total sleep dep- mance, version 2.2. Herzogenrath: Psytest
rivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neu- 68. Niemann H, Sturm W, Thöne-Otto AIT, Wilmes K (2011) Cali-
ropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol fornia verbal learning test, version 21. Mödling: Schuhfried
25:S68–S73 GmbH
60. McKenna BS, Eyler LT (2012) Overlapping prefrontal systems
involved in cognitive and emotional processing in euthymic
bipolar disorder and following sleep deprivation: a review of
functional neuroimaging studies. Clin Psychol Rev 32:650–663

13

View publication stats

You might also like