You are on page 1of 9

Received: 10 November 2020 | Revised: 6 January 2021 | Accepted: 9 January 2021

DOI: 10.1002/jnr.24787

REVIEW

Sex differences in the social brain and in social


cognition

Alice Mado Proverbio

Milan Center for Neuroscience, Department


of Psychology, University of Milano-­Bicocca, Abstract
Milan, Italy Many studies have reported sex differences in empathy and social skills. In this review,
Correspondence several lines of empirical evidences about sex differences in functions and anatomy
Alice Mado Proverbio, Department of of social brain are discussed. The most relevant differences involve face processing,
Psychology, University of Milano-­Bicocca,
Piazza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126, Milan, facial expression recognition, response to baby schema, the ability to see faces in
Italy. things, the processing of social interactions, the response to the others’ pain, interest
Email: mado.proverbio@unimib.it
in social information, processing of gestures and actions, biological motion, erotic,
Funding information and affective stimuli. Sex differences in oxytocin-­based parental response are also
Università degli Studi di Milano-­Bicocca
reported. In conclusion, the female and male brains show several neuro-­functional
differences in various aspects of social cognition, and especially in emotional cod-
ing, face processing, and response to baby schema. An interpretation of this sexual
dimorphism is provided in the view of evolutionary psychobiology.

KEYWORDS

baby schema, empathy, facial expression, gender, sex differences

1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N the reactivity to/efficacy of drugs and pharmaceutical molecules, as


well as in the incidence of neurodegenerative, neurological, and psy-
Genetic and hormonal influences are long known to affect the human chiatric diseases (see the entire volume dedicated to sex differences
brain and determine a variety of anatomical and functional differ- in the brain and edited by Cahill, 2017).
ences between the two sexes. These sex differences would concern: Besides anatomical and physiological diversities, some func-
overall brain volume (Zaidi, 2010), neuronal density and white mat- tional and mental differences between men and women have long
ter thickness (Luders & Toga, 2010), microstructure of the thalamus, been identified (e.g., Kimura, 1980) and more recently supported
corpus callosum, and cingulum (Menzler et al., 2011), structure/size by neuroimaging studies (e.g., verbal fluency Sokołowski et al.,
of anterior hypothalamus, of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, 2020) and empathy (Baron-­Cohen, 2002) in favor of female gen-
of the posterodorsal region of the medial amygdala, and of the preop- der and spatial rotation ability (Voyer et al., 1995) in favor of the
tic area (see Cahill, 2006; Hines, 2020) for comprehensive reviews). male gender. Lately an increasingly large amount of evidence has
The cerebral sexual dimorphism would support marked diversities accumulated indicating a sex difference in social and affective pro-
in reproductive, parental, and social behavior. In addition, prenatal cesses (e.g., Anderson et al., 2013; McDuff et al., 2017; Perrett
and postnatal differences in testosterone concentration (between et al., 1998; Stevens & Hamann, 2012; Thompson & Voyer, 2014).
females and males) would heavily affect the brain physiology, also In this review, we will examine a large corpus of behavioral, EEG/
modulating dendritic growth, spine density, density of brain recep- event-­related potentials (ERPs), and neuroimaging evidence in the
tors, neurogenesis, and gliogenesis (McCarthy et al., 2012). A rapidly recent literature supporting the existence of sex differences in the
burgeoning literature now documents significant sex differences in social brain.

Edited by Sara Bulgheroni and Junie Warrington. Reviewed by Larry Cahill and Ruben Gur.

J Neurosci Res. 2021;00:1–9. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jnr© 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC | 1


2 | PROVERBIO

The main known sex differences in social cognition refer to: hemi-
spheric lateralization for face processing (Bourne, 2005; Proverbio,
Significance
De Gabriele, et al., 2011; Proverbio et al., 2006a, 2010, 2012), facial
This review is particularly relevant because it focuses on
expression encoding (e.g., Connolly et al., 2019; Orozco & Ehlers,
an extensive neuroimaging and electrophysiological lit-
1998; Sawada et al., 2014; Wingenbach et al., 2018), face gender
erature on functional and anatomical sex differences in
encoding (Proverbio et al., 2010a), interest in social scenes and
social cognition, thus revealing new and hitherto unknown
human faces (Pavlova et al., 2014, 2015; Proverbio, 2017; Proverbio
knowledge.
et al., 2008), emotional response to negative affective informa-
tion (Hofer et al., 2007; Klein et al., 2003), empathy for pain (Han
et al., 2008; Proverbio et al., 2009; Singer et al., 2004), understand-
ing gestures, body language, and intentions (Canessa et al., 2012; would rely more on interhemispheric communication during tasks
Proverbio, De Gabriele, et al., 2011), parental response to baby involving 3D mental rotation (Kurth et al., 2018), and would show
schema (Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Gur, et al., 2009; a more bilateral activation of brain areas devoted to linguistic func-
Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Valdez, et al., 2009; Sander tions (Hausmann, 2016; Kansaku & Kitazawa, 2001; Shaywitz et al.,
et al., 2007; Seifritz et al., 2003). 1995). Bourne (2005) investigated the existence of hemispheric
asymmetries during face processing. The task consisted of viewing
chimeric faces with different facial expressions and determining
1.1 | Hemispheric asymmetries for face processing which one looked more positive. Again, the data provided evidence
of a stronger hemispheric asymmetry in men compared to women,
While the classical neuroimaging literature reports a right hemi- with a perceptual advantage of the left visual field (right hemisphere)
spheric dominance for face processing in humans (Kanwisher in the latter group. Tiedt et al. (2013) reported similar findings in
et al., 1997; Rossion et al., 2003; Sergent et al., 1992), regardless of an ERP study on the interhemispheric transfer-­time (IHTT) of facial
gender of viewers, new recent data have shown that face process- information estimated through N170 latency. They showed a sex
ing is less lateralized in females than males (Kiesow et al., 2020; Liu difference in IHTT with earlier N170 latencies in the right-­to-­left di-
et al., 2020; Proverbio et al., 2010b; Proverbio & Galli, 2016). Others rection (170 ms) as opposed to the left-­to-­right direction (185 ms),
authors suggested that facial processing involved both the right and while IHTT was symmetric in women. Quite similar findings were
left hemispheres (Haxby et al., 1999) but that they would contrib- also reported by Proverbio et al. (2012) who concluded that asym-
ute differently in response to familiar or unfamiliar faces. Overall, metry in callosal transfer times in males might be ascribed to quicker
the literature is very inconsistent about hemispheric lateralization of transfer times of facial inputs through pathways departing from the
facial processing since virtually no neuroimaging study has ever con- more efficient (right) to the less efficient (left) hemisphere. Likewise,
sidered viewer's sex in the past. At this regard, it is of great interest the lack of asymmetry in females might suggest that both face areas
to note that face-­specific N170 responses of ERPs are found to be are equally specialized.
bilateral or even left-­sided in studies in which female participants are
the majority (e.g., Jemel et al., 2005).
Two ERP studies (Proverbio et al., 2006b, 2012) have shown a 1.2 | Affective facial expressions and emotions
face-­specific activation of the left occipito-­temporal area, namely of
the fusiform face area (FFA), in females during processing of facial Empirical evidence of females’ greater accuracy in interpreting fa-
stimuli (as reflected by N170 component of ERPs) and of the right cial mimicry has also been provided (Connolly et al., 2019; Sawada
homologous FFA, in males. More in detail, Proverbio and coworkers et al., 2014; Thompson & Voyer, 2014; Williams et al., 2009;
(2012) recorded ERPs to ~400 stimuli (faces of adults, children, and Wingenbach et al., 2018). This finding is typically interpreted in an
infants, tools) in 50 participants, in a landscape detection task. The evolutionary perspective, considering the primary role of moth-
results showed a lack of difference in the amplitude of N170 to ob- ers in the early interactions with infants. In this regard, Proverbio
jects in the two sexes. However, it was found that N170 to faces was and coworkers (2007) examined the role of biological sex and ex-
of greater amplitude over the right in males, and bilateral in females. pertise in understanding infant expressions in a large group of men
Furthermore, the left face area in males was not responding to peo- and women who differed in their experience with babies. Women
ple's age (it did not discriminate between adult, children, or baby showed a better performance and decoding accuracy than men;
faces) but only the right one, while the age discriminative response furthermore, familiarity with babies (linked to profession) improved
was bilateral in females. decoding of facial expressions but only in women. These results sug-
Overall, these findings fit with previous data providing evidence gest an interaction between expertise and a biologically driven fe-
of a sex difference in the hemispheric lateralization of perceptual male inclination to better comprehend babies’ facial expressions (i.e.,
and cognitive processes. For example, several studies have shown an interaction between sex and cultural factors).
more strongly lateralized functions in males versus females, in- In an electrophysiological study performed on the same set of
cluding language and visuospatial processes. In particular, females stimuli (Proverbio et al., 2006b), it was investigated whether viewers’
PROVERBIO | 3

sex affected the perceptual response at different stages of visual 1.3 | Parental response
processing during a decision task of infant joy/pain expressions.
All infants were unknown to participants. Overall, the occipital P1 A wide literature shows that the visual and the orbitofrontal cortices
response was earlier and much greater in female than male par- of adult viewers are particularly aroused by the view of baby faces
ticipants. Furthermore, P1 latency was faster in response to pain (Nitschke et al., 2004). Viewing an infant would provide a positive
than neutral expressions only in women, thus showing a prioritized feeling (e.g., tenderness or cuteness) especially in women, but also
processing of biologically relevant information in the female brain in men, via the dopaminergic reward system. This effect is known
(Figure 1). as “baby schema” effect, and besides the enhanced affective valance
The role of sex in the emotion comprehension and psycholog- it results in a deeper processing and attentional orienting toward
ical reaction to emotional visual stimuli has been also investigated children versus adult faces (Brosch et al., 2007; Glocker, Langleben,
(Proverbio, 2017; Stevens & Hamann, 2012). Quite recently Li Ruparel, Loughead, Gur, et al., 2009; Luo et al., 2011; Proverbio, De
et al. (2020) analyzed a large data set of 970 subjects (508 women) Gabriele, et al., 2011; Proverbio, Riva, Zani, et al., 2011; see also the
engaged in identifying negative facial emotions and neutral shape review by Hahn & Perrett, 2014). Other behavioral studies showed
targets finding that women were more sensitive to fearful stimuli, how women might be more responsive to baby schema than men
whereas men were more sensitive to anger-­provoking stimuli. and more skilled in comprehending infant expressivity (Babchuk
In general, stimuli producing a larger arousal response would et al., 1985; Proverbio et al., 2007). For example, in an EEG/ERP study
undergo a prioritized processing because of their biological rel- (Proverbio et al., 2006a) aimed at investigating the neural response
evance, for example, baby faces would stimulate an instinctual to baby schema in female and male adult people, data showed the
parental response, especially in women. In contrast, erotic stimuli sensory response to faces (P1) was of greater amplitude in women
(such as nudity) would be particularly arousing for males versus than men. This might suggest a greater perceptual sensitivity or at-
females. Sabatinelli et al. (2004) showed that perceiving erotic tentive response in females versus males at the view of unrelated in-
images induced a much larger activation of associative visual fants. Other studies showed that baby faces were more perceptually
areas in males than women. Indeed, Huynh and coworkers (2012) salient for women than men (Hahn et al., 2013; Parsons et al., 2011,
reported that high-­intensity (vs. low-­intensity and neutral) erotic 2013). In addition, infant faces would capture females’ attention to a
movies deactivated the primary visual area in women. Conversely, greater extent than adult faces; conversely, infant faces would cap-
perception of scenes stimulating an empathic response (such as, ture male's attention much less than faces of the opposite sex, that
body mutilations) triggered a greater activity in the extra-­s triate is, female faces (Cárdenas et al., 2013).
visual area of females versus males. These findings seem to in- Several interesting neuroimaging studies (Glocker, Langleben,
dicate that brain arousal and attentional allocation devoted to Ruparel, Loughead, Valdez, et al., 2009; Kringelbach et al., 2008;
social stimulus depends on their specific biological relevance for Leibenluft et al., 2004) have investigated which neural systems are
the viewers. involved in the “parental response” to infants, and identified a neural

F I G U R E 1 Mean latency of P1 component recorded at lateral occipital area (independent of hemispheric site) and analyzed according to
viewer's sex and type of facial expression (taken and modified from Proverbio et al., 2006b study, protected by CC BY 2.0)
4 | PROVERBIO

F I G U R E 2 Event-­related potential (ERP) difference waves obtained by subtracting ERPs to congruent from ERPs to incongruent actions
separately for men and women, over anterior scalp sites. It can be appreciated a much larger N400 response to incongruent actions in
women than men (original figure, relative to Proverbio et al., 2010, courtesy of the authors)

network of structures based primarily on the orbitofrontal cortex. other groups (i.e., males and nulliparous females), possibly indicating
For example, Leibenluft and coauthors (2004) recorded the brain ac- a greater perceptual sensitivity (or enhanced arousal) in mothers, at
tivation of mothers while viewing photographs of their own infants. the view of unknown babies.
They wished to get some knowledge on the neural correlates of “ma- As far as for auditory social information are concerned, the neu-
ternal love.” It was found a focus of activity in the orbitofrontal area roscientific literature reported larger brain responses in women than
and in related regions belonging to the pleasure/reward system, in- men while listening to infant vocalizations such as cry and laugh-
cluding the periaqueductal gray matter. ter (e.g., Sander et al., 2007; Seifritz et al., 2003). These data also
In an electrophysiological study (Peltola et al., 2014), it was seem to suggest the hypothesis of a sex difference in the parental
tested the associations between motherhood and genetic variations response to infantile nonverbal signals. It is conceivable that many
in oxytocin receptors, and bioelectrical and behavioral responses to of the sex differences in social cognition might be related to the ge-
facial expressions of babies and adults. Both mothers (as opposed netically determined role of human females as primary caregivers of
to nulliparous females) and women carrying the rs53576 GG vari- the offspring (instead of fighters/hunters, as suggested by Kuhn &
ant of OXTR gene showed larger discriminative N170 responses to Stiner, 2006). For example, the robust evidence linking testosterone
strong versus mild negative facial expressions of infants. This result exposure to sex-­t yped play (e.g., playing or not with dolls) suggest
suggests that the visual cortex responsivity to baby schema de- how behavior is strongly modulated by sexual genes (Hines, 2020;
pends both on genetic (individual differences) and biological factors Leveroni & Berenbaum, 1998), but it cannot be rule out the role of
(being a mother or not). Overall, being a parent has been associated socialization and upbringing style.
with a greater sensitivity to infant facial expressions. This has been
demonstrated in an electrophysiological study performed in parent
versus nulliparous adults where it was found that perceptual N160 1.4 | Interest in social stimuli
response reflected the earliest discrimination of mild versus strong
painful facial expressions in parents (especially in mothers) but not in Some electrophysiological literature provided evidence of a greater
nulliparous individuals, thus suggesting again a strong interaction of female electrocortical response to social information (e.g., faces or
genetic predisposition and parental status (Proverbio et al., 2006a). people) as opposed to inanimate stimuli. In a study by Proverbio
Again, the data showed larger P3 responses in mothers versus all et al. (2008), participants viewed hundreds of pictures depicting real
PROVERBIO | 5

persons versus sceneries (urban or natural landscapes) while their have found enhanced activity in females versus males especially fo-
EEG signals were recorded. The results showed larger amplitude N2 cused in areas devoted to social perception (such as, the temporal
responses in women, but not in men, in response to social than inani- pole and the medial temporal gyrus).
mate information. Source reconstruction of brain signals evidenced
a bilateral activation of face-­related regions in response to persons.
In addition, sex differences were found with the unique activation of 1.6 | Face pareidolia
the superior temporal gyrus and the right extra-­striate body area in
women, and of the left para-­hippocampal area in men. This was in- Due to the social bias, females would also be more inclined than
terpreted as female greater interest in (or attention to) social signals men at seeing faces in things (face pareidolia), as recently shown by
(i.e., bodies and faces). behavioral and electrophysiological findings (Pavlova et al., 2015;
This interest for social information might draw more attention Proverbio & Galli, 2016; Zhou & Meng, 2020). Pavlova et al. (2015)
to visual stimuli. Indeed, a considerable literature has accumulated compared women and men in a face recognition task involving
showing that females are more accurate than males in decoding fa- Arcimboldo-­like food patterns and found that women were more
cial mimicry. Many studies have reported sex differences in the abil- inclined to recognize a face in the food configuration and gave
ity to perceive, process, express, and experience emotions. Overall, food displays higher face-­likeness ratings. Interestingly, Proverbio
females seem better skilled in the analysis and comprehension of and Galli (2016) further investigated this issue by recording EEG/
social signals, and more interested in conspecifics. Again, female ERP in women and men engaged in viewing photographs of animals
children typically spend more time with infants, younger siblings, (targets), objects, faces in things, and faces. Overall, compared to
and dolls than male children. While it is problematic to determine men, women were more inclined to see faces in perfectly real ob-
whether this socially oriented behavior is due to cultural factors (e.g., jects, as shown in the preliminary face-­likeness ratings assessment.
society models, upbringing style) or to genes, comparative studies Furthermore, vertex positive potential of the ERPs showed a dis-
on monkeys seem to point to the second direction. criminative response between faces and faces in things in men (with
larger responses to faces), while it was of comparable amplitude in
women, clearly reflecting a face pareidolia effect. The analysis of
1.5 | Action and body language understanding intracranial generator of this response in women identified the or-
bitofrontal cortex, the right STS and the posterior cingulate cortex
Several sex differences have been found in the way female and male as the stronger sources of activity for the affective processing (face
brain processes actions and gestures. Proverbio et al. (2010)’s study, likeness) of things. According to the authors, this network would
for example, reported earlier and greater ERP responses to incon- support the anthropomorphizing bias in females.
gruent actions in women than men. Perception of congruent actions
(e.g., a woman looking attentively at road map) was compared with
that of incongruent actions (e.g., a man in a kitchen cutting a carrot 1.7 | Empathy for pain
with an axe). Data showed an earlier discrimination between congru-
ent and incongruent actions in women, indexed by a parietal N200 Recent findings have demonstrated that women might be more re-
to response. The inverse solution applied to this component located active than men at the view of suffering conspecifics (vicarious re-
the neural sources of this effect in the inferior parietal and left in- sponse to pain), and therefore more empathic (Han et al., 2008). This
ferior frontal cortex (among other areas), corresponding to the mir- sex difference fully agrees with Baron-­Cohen model, as well as with
ror neuron network. In addition, the later anterior N400 component a recent survey examining the empathy quotients of 671,606 indi-
(greater to incongruent behavior) was found enhanced in women viduals (Greenberg et al., 2018). In a study by Proverbio et al. (2009)
versus men (see Figure 2). The N400 source analysis showed slightly it was investigated whether the two sexes differed in their cerebral
different neural circuits in two sexes, with an activation of the limbic response to affective pictures, depicting people in different positive
system and cingulate cortex in women, and of the ventromedial pre- or negative contexts compared to natural or urban landscapes. More
frontal cortex in men, which has been interpreted as a more rational than 400 stimuli from the International Affective Picture System
than affective reaction in men than women. (IAPS) were shown to a group of female and male students. An emo-
A conjoined fMRI and ERP study from our group (Canessa tional impact scale was administered to all participants showing
et al., 2012; Proverbio, Riva, Paganelli, et al., 2011) reported a greater higher emotional psychological reactions in women than men to a
female activation of the so-­called action understanding system, the variety of emotional stimuli.
ventral premotor cortex and the superior temporal sulcus (STS) in Visual P1 response was larger in amplitude to persons than to
a task involving the observation of social interactions with a coop- landscapes and was modulated by the emotional valence of the
erative or an affective goal. Other studies provided several lines of social information. A late positive response to negatively valence
evidence of sex differences in the brain development of perceptual social information (suffering humans, visible in Figure 3, blue line)
systems devoted to biological motion. fMRI data collected during far exceeded the response to negative nonsocial information in
visual perception of coherent versus incoherent biological motion females but not in males. Again, a low-­resolution electromagnetic
6 | PROVERBIO

F I G U R E 3 Event-­related potentials (ERPs) recorded at midline frontal and centro/parietal sites as a function of stimulus valence and
viewer's sex. It is visible a large effect of both emotional content (that can be appreciated by comparing ERPs to negative vs. positive
unanimated scenes), and an effect of empathy for pain, especially in women (that can be appreciated by comparing ERPs to negative scenes
vs. ERPs to pictures portraying humans). Original figure, relative to Proverbio et al. (2009)’s study, reproduced with permission of the authors
and of Elsevier

tomography applied to ERP data showed an enhanced activation of the women might be more empathic than men at the view of suffer-
the right amygdala and of the right frontal areas in women (compared ing humans (Han et al., 2008; Schulte-­Ruether et al., 2008; Singer
to men) for the contrast suffering-­minus-­happy humans. This func- et al., 2004).
tional difference possibly suggests a stronger empathic response in In summary, we have here outlined a series of sex differences in
the female brain at the sight of the others’ pain. social behavioral and psychological processes that seem biologically
Hofer et al. (2007) found larger activation of the right superior associated with female chromosomal karyotype. They are: efficient
temporal area, right insula, right putamen, and anterior cingulate and bilateral face processing, marked empathic attitude, sensitivity
cortex (ACC) during processing of affective words for women versus to facial mimicry and gestures, attachment to infants (induced by
men, and interpreted this finding as reflecting the greater emotion- oxytocin), early interest for infants, curiosity about people and social
ality of female individuals. Again, Klein et al. (2003) found increased information, emotional responsivity, lesser incidence of autistic, psy-
activation of the ACC and the amygdala in women in response to chopathic, and sociopathic disorders. Knowledge of sex differences
negative IAPS pictures. In the study by Proverbio and coauthors in the brain is essential for understanding how to treat male and fe-
(2009), sex differences in the response to negative versus positive male patients with sex-­specific medications (Cahill, 2017), or how
stimuli were much greater for social than nonsocial information. to recognize and treat gender-­specific symptoms in socially related
This might suggest that visual images of humans have a special disorders (e.g., autistic spectrum disorder, attention deficit/hyperac-
status for the female brain, especially in interaction with negative tivity disorder, personality disorder and psychopathy, conduct disor-
valence. These data are consistent with other literature suggesting der, alexitimia, depression, social anxiety etc., American Psychiatric
PROVERBIO | 7

Association, 2013). The greater incidence of some of these disorders perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115,
257–­263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-­0310.2008.01603.x
in the male population might ultimately be due to sex differences in
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Valdez,
social cognition. J. N., Griffin, M. D., Sachser, N., & Gur, R. C. (2009). Baby schema
modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women.
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States
of America, 106, 9115–­9119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.08116​
This work was supported by 13974 2015-­ATE-­0 052 grant entitled
20106
“Emotional responses and gender differences in individuals with high Greenberg, D. M., Warrier, V., Allison, C., & Baron-­Cohen, S. (2018).
traits of psychopathy, impulsivity and empathy” from the University Testing the empathizing-­ systemizing theory of sex differences
of Milano-­Bicocca. and the extreme male brain theory of autism in half a million peo-
ple. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 115(48), 12152–­12157. https://doi.org/10.1073/
C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T
pnas.18110​32115
No conflicting interests exist. Hahn, A. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2014). Neural and behavioral responses to
attractiveness in adult and infant faces. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
PEER REVIEW Reviews, 46(4), 591–­603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubi​orev.2014.​
08.015
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publo​
Hahn, A. C., Xiao, D., Sprengelmeyer, R., & Perrett, D. I. (2013). Gender
ns.com/publo​n/10.1002/jnr.24787. differences in the incentive salience of adult and infant faces.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(1), 200–­208. https://
ORCID doi.org/10.1080/17470​218.2012.705860
Han, S., Fan, Y., & Mao, L. (2008). Gender difference in empathy for pain:
Alice Mado Proverbio https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5138-1523
An electrophysiological investigation. Brain Research, 27(1196), 85–­
93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brain​res.2007.12.062
REFERENCES Hausmann, M. (2016). Why sex hormones matter for neuroscience: A
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man- very short review on sex, sex hormones, and functional brain asym-
ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Author. metries. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95(1–­2), 40–­49.
Anderson, L. C., Bolling, D. Z., Schelinski, S., Coffman, M. C., Pelphrey, Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Clark, V. P., Schouten, J. L., Hoffman, E. A.,
K. A., & Kaiser, M. D. (2013). Sex differences in the development of & Martin, A. (1999). The effect of face inversion on activity in human
brain mechanisms for processing biological motion. Neuroimage, 83, neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron, 22, 189–­199.
751–­760. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2013.07.040 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896​-­6273(00)80690​-­X
Babchuk, W. A., Hames, R. B., & Thompson, R. A. (1985). Sex differ- Hines, M. (2020). Neuroscience and sex/gender: Looking back and for-
ences in the recognition of infant facial expressions of emotion: The ward. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(1), 37–­43. https://doi.org/10.1523/
primary caretaker hypothesis. Ethology & Sociobiology, 6, 89–­101. JNEUR​OSCI.0750-­19.2019
https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-­3 095(85)90002​- ­0 Hofer, A., Siedentopf, C. M., Ischebeck, A., Rettenbacher, M. A., Verius,
Baron-­Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends M., Felber, S., & Fleischhacker, W. (2007). Sex differences in brain
in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 248–­254. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364​ activation patterns during processing of positively and negatively
-­6613(02)01904​-­6 valenced emotional words. Psychological Medicine, 37(1), 109–­119.
Bourne, V. J. (2005). Lateralised processing of positive facial emo- https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033​29170​6008919
tion: Sex differences in strength of hemispheric dominance. Huynh, H. K., Beers, C., Willemsen, A., Lont, E., Laan, E., Dierckx, R.,
Neuropsychologia, 43, 953–­ 956. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​ Jansen, M., Sand, M., Weijmar Schultz, W., & Holstege, G. (2012).
psych​ologia.2004.08.007 High-­intensity erotic visual stimuli de-­activate the primary visual
Brosch, T., Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). That baby caught my eye. cortex in women. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(6), 1579–­1587. https://
Attention capture by infant faces. Emotion, 7, 685–­689. https://doi.or doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-­6109.2012.02706.x
g/10.1037/1528-­3542.7.3.685 Jemel, B., Pisani, M., Rousselle, L., Crommelinck, M., & Bruyer, R. (2005).
Cahill, L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature Reviews Exploring the functional architecture of person recognition system
Neuroscience, 7(6), 477–­484. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1909 with event-­ related potentials in a within-­and cross-­ domain self-­
Cahill, L. (2017). An issue whose time has come: Sex/gender influences priming of faces. Neuropsychologia, 43, 2024–­ 2040. https://doi.
on nervous system function. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95, org/10.1016/j.neuro​psych​ologia.2005.03.016
Spc1, 1–­791. Kansaku, K., & Kitazawa, S. (2001). Imaging studies on sex differences in
Canessa, N., Alemanno, F., Riva, F., Zani, A., Proverbio, A. M., Mannara, the lateralization of language. Neuroscience Research, 41(4), 333–­337.
N., Perani, D., & Cappa, S. F. (2012). The neural bases of social in- https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168​- ­0102(01)00292​-­9
tention understanding: The role of interaction goals. PLoS ONE, 7(7), Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face
e42347. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0042347 area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face
Cárdenas, R. A., Harris, L. J., & Becker, M. W. (2013). Sex differences perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 4302–­ 4311. https://doi.
in visual attention toward infant faces. Evolution and Human org/10.1523/JNEUR​OSCI.17-­11-­0 4302.1997
Behavior, 34(4), 280–­287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolh​umbeh​ Kiesow, H., Dunbar, R. I. M., Kable, J. W., Kalenscher, T., Vogeley, K.,
av.2013.04.001 Schilbach, L., Marquand, A. F., Wiecki, T. V., & Bzdok, D. (2020).
Connolly, H. L., Lefevre, C. E., Young, A. W., & Lewis, G. J. (2019). Sex 10,000 social brains: Sex differentiation in human brain anatomy.
differences in emotion recognition: Evidence for a small overall fe- Science Advances, 6(12), eaaz1170.
male superiority on facial disgust. Emotion, 19(3), 455–­464. https:// Kimura, D. (1980). Sex differences in intrahemispheric organization of
doi.org/10.1037/emo00​0 0446 speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 240–­241.
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., Klein, S., Smolka, M. N., Wrase, J., Gruesser, S. M., Mann, K., Braus, D. F.,
& Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness &Heinz, A. (2003). The influence of gender and emotional valence of
8 | PROVERBIO

visual cues on fMRI activation in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry, 36(3), Pavlova, M. A., Scheffler, K., & Sokolov, A. N. (2015). Face-­ n-­food:
5191–­5194. Gender differences in tuning to faces. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0130363.
Kringelbach, M. L., Lehtonen, A., Squire, S., Harvey, A. G., Craske, M. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0130363
G., Holliday, I. E., Green, A. L., Aziz, T. Z., Hansen, P. C., Cornelissen, Pavlova, M. A., Sokolov, A. N., & Bidet-­Ildei, C. (2014). Sex differences in
P. L., & Stein, A. (2008). A specific and rapid neural signature for pa- the neuromagnetic cortical response to biological motion. Cerebral
rental instinct. PLoS ONE, 3, e1664. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​ Cortex, 25(10), 3468–­3 474. https://doi.org/10.1093/cerco​r/bhu175
al.pone.0001664 Peltola, M. P., Yrttiaho, S., Puura, K., Proverbio, A. M., Mononen, N.,
Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2006). What’s a mother to do? A hypothe- Lehtimäki, T., & Leppänen, J. M. (2014). Motherhood and oxytocin
sis about the division of labor and modern human origins. Current receptor genetic variation are associated with selective changes in
Anthropology, 47(6), 953–­980. https://doi.org/10.1086/507197 electrocortical responses to infant facial expressions. Emotion, 14(3),
Kurth, F., Spencer, D., Hines, M., & Luders, E. (2018). Sex differences in 469–­477. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035959
associations between spatial ability and corpus callosum morphol- Perrett, D., Lee, K., Penton-­Voak, I., Rowland, D., Yoshikawa, S., Burt,
ogy. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 96(8), 1380–­1387. https://doi. D., Henzi, S., Castles, D., & Akamatsu, S. (1998). Effects of sexual
org/10.1002/jnr.24260 dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–­887. https://
Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., & Haxby, J. V. (2004). doi.org/10.1038/29772
Mothers' neural activation in response to pictures of their children Proverbio, A. M. (2017). Sex differences in social cognition: The case of
and other children. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 225–­232. https://doi. face processing. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 2(95), 222–­234.
org/10.1016/j.biops​ych.2004.05.017 Proverbio, A. M., Adorni, R., Zani, A., & Trestianu, L. (2009). Sex dif-
Leveroni, C., & Berenbaum, S. (1998). Early androgen effects on inter- ferences in the brain response to affective scenes with or with-
est in infants: Evidence from children with congenital adrenal hy- out humans. Neuropsychologia, 47(12), 2374–­ 2388. https://doi.
perplasia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14, 321–­3 40. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.neuro​psych​ologia.2008.10.030
org/10.1080/87565​6 4980​9540714 Proverbio, A. M., Brignone, V., Matarazzo, S., Del Zotto, M., & Zani, A.
Li, G., Zhang, S., Le, T. M., Tang, X., & Li, C. R. (2020). Neural responses to (2006a). Gender and parental status affect the visual cortical re-
negative facial emotions: Sex differences in the correlates of individ- sponse to infant facial expression. Neuropsychologia, 4(14), 2987–­
ual anger and fear traits. Neuroimage, 16(221), 117–­171. https://doi. 2999. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​psych​ologia.2006.06.015
org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2020.117171 Proverbio, A. M., Brignone, V., Matarazzo, S., Del Zotto, M., & Zani, A.
Liu, S., Seidlitz, J., Blumenthal, J. D., Clasen, L. S., & Raznahan, A. (2020). (2006b). Gender differences in hemispheric asymmetry for face pro-
Integrative structural, functional, and transcriptomic analyses of sex-­ cessing. BMC Neuroscience, 7, 44.
biased brain organization in humans. Proceedings of National Academy Proverbio, A. M., De Gabriele, V., Manfredi, M., & Adorni, R. (2011). No
of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(31), 18788–­18798. race effect (ORE) in the automatic orienting toward baby faces: When
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.19190​91117 ethnic group does not matter. Psychology, 2(9), 931–­935. https://doi.
Luders, E., & Toga, A. W. (2010). Sex differences in brain anatomy. org/10.4236/psych.2011.29140
Progress in Brain Research, 186, 3–­12. Proverbio, A. M., & Galli, J. (2016). Women are better at seeing faces
Luo, L. Z., Li, H., & Lee, K. (2011). Are children’s faces really more ap- where there are none: An ERP study of face pareidolia. Social
pealing than those of adults? Testing the baby schema hypothesis Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(9), 1501–­1512. https://doi.
beyond infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 115–­ org/10.1093/scan/nsw064
124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.002 Proverbio, A. M., Matarazzo, S., Brignone, V., Del Zotto, M., & Zani, A.
McCarthy, M. M., Arnold, A. P., Ball, G. F., Blaustein, J. D., & De Vries, (2007). Processing valence and intensity of infant expressions: The
G. J. (2012). Sex differences in the brain: The not so inconve- roles of expertise and gender. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
nient truth. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(7), 2241–­2247. https://doi. 48(6), 477–­485. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-­9450.2007.00616.x
org/10.1523/JNEUR​OSCI.5372-­11.2012 Proverbio, A. M., Mazzara, R., Riva, F., & Manfredi, M. (2012). Sex differ-
McDuff, D., Kodra, E., Kaliouby, R. E., & LaFrance, M. (2017). A large-­scale ences in callosal transfer and hemispheric specialization for face cod-
analysis of sex differences in facial expressions. PLoS ONE, 12(4), ing. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2325–­ 2332. https://doi.org/10.1016/​
e0173942. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0173942 j.neuro​psych​ologia.2012.05.036
Menzler, K., Belke, M., Wehrmann, E., Krakow, K., Lengler, U., Jansen, A., Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., Martin, E., & Zani, A. (2010a). Neural markers
Hamer, H. M., Oertel, W. H., Rosenow, F., & Knake, S. (2011). Men of opposite-­sex bias in face processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 18(1),
and women are different: Diffusion tensor imaging reveals sexual di- 169. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00169
morphism in the microstructure of the thalamus, corpus callosum and Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., Martin, E., & Zani, A. (2010b). Face coding
cingulum. Neuroimage, 54(4), 2557–­2562. https://doi.org/10.1016/​ is bilateral in the female brain. PLoS ONE, 5, e11242. https://doi.
j.neuro​image.2010.11.029 org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0011242
Nitschke, J. B., Nelson, E. E., Rusch, B. D., Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R., & Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., Paganelli, L., Cappa, S. F., Canessa, N., Perani, D., &
Davidson, R. J. (2004). Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in Zani, A. (2011). Neural coding of cooperative vs. affective human inter-
mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. NeuroImage, 21, actions: 150 ms to code the action's purpose. PLoS ONE, 6(7), e22026.
583–­592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​image.2003.10.005 Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., & Zani, A. (2010). When neurons do not mir-
Orozco, S., & Ehlers, C. L. (1998). Gender differences in electrophysiolog- ror the agent’s intentions: Sex differences in neural coding of goal-­
ical responses to facial stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 44(4), 281–­289. directed actions. Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1454–­ 1463. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006​-­3223(97)00487​-­3 org/10.1016/j.neuro​psych​ologia.2010.01.015
Parsons, C. E., Stark, E. A., Young, K. S., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., Zani, A., & Martin, E. (2011). Is it a baby?
(2013). Understanding the human parental brain: A critical role of Perceived age affects brain processing of faces differently in women
the orbitofrontal cortex. Social Neuroscience, 8, 525–­543. https://doi. and men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3197–­3208.
org/10.1080/17470​919.2013.842610 https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00041
Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Kumari, N., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2011). Proverbio, A. M., Zani, A., & Adorni, R. (2008). Neural markers of a
The motivational salience of infant faces is similar for men and women. greater female responsiveness to social stimuli. BMC Neuroscience,
PLoS ONE, 6(5), e20632. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0020632 30(9), 56. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-­2202-­9-­56
PROVERBIO | 9

Rossion, B., Schiltz, C., & Crommelinck, M. (2003). The functionally de- Thompson, A. E., & Voyer, D. (2014). Sex differences in the ability to rec-
fined right occipital and fusiform "face areas" discriminate novel ognize non-­verbal displays of emotion: A meta-­analysis. Cognition
from visually familiar faces. Neuroimage, 19(3), 877–­883. https://doi. and Emotion, 28(7), 1164–­1195.
org/10.1016/S1053​-­8119(03)00105​-­8 Tiedt, H. O., Weber, J. E., Pauls, A., Beier, K. M., & Lueschow, A. (2013).
Sabatinelli, D., Flaisch, T., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., & Lang, P. J. Sex-­differences of face coding: Evidence from larger right hemi-
(2004). Affective picture perception: Gender differences in visual cor- spheric M170 in men and dipole source modelling. PLoS ONE, 8(7),
tex? NeuroReport, 15(7), 1109–­1112. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001​ e69107. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.0069107
756-­20040​5190-­0 0005 Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differ-
Sander, K., Frome, Y., & Scheich, H. (2007). FMRI activations of amygdala, cin- ences in spatial abilities: A meta-­analysis and consideration of crit-
gulate cortex, and auditory cortex by infant laughing and crying. Human ical variables. Psychological Bullettin, 117(2), 250–­270. https://doi.
Brain Mapping, 28(10), 1007–­1022. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20333 org/10.1037/​0 033-­2909.117.2.250
Sawada, R., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., & Williams, L. M., Mathersul, D., Palmer, D. M., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., &
Toichi, M. (2014). Sex differences in the rapid detection of emotional Gordon, E. (2009). Explicit identification and implicit recognition of
facial expressions. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e94747. https://doi.org/10.1371/ facial emotions: I. Age effects in males and females across 10 de-
journ​al.pone.0094747 cades. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(3), 257–­
Schulte-­Ruether, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, 277. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803​39080​2255635
M. (2008). Gender differences in brain networks supporting empa- Wingenbach, T. S. H., Ashwin, C., & Brosnan, M. (2018). Sex differences in
thy. Neuroimage, 42(1), 393–­4 03. facial emotion recognition across varying expression intensity levels
Seifritz, E., Esposito, F., Neuhoff, J. G., Lüthi, A., Mustovic, H., Dammann, from videos. PLoS ONE, 13(1), e0190634. https://doi.org/10.1371/
G., von Bardeleben, U., Radue, E. W., Cirillo, S., Tedeschi, G., & Di journ​al.pone.0190634
Salle, F. (2003). Differential sex-­independent amygdala response to Zaidi, Z. F. (2010). Gender differences in human brain: A review. Open
infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological Anatomy Journal, 2, 37–­55. https://doi.org/10.2174/18776​09401​
Psychiatry, 54(12), 1367–­ 1375. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006​ 00201​0 037
-­3223(03)00697​-­8 Zhou, L., & Meng, M. (2020). Do you see the “face”? Individual differ-
Sergent, J., Ohta, S., & MacDonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanat- ences in face pareidolia. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 14, E2.
omy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography https://doi.org/10.1017/prp.2019.27
study. Brain, 115(1), 15–­36. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/​115.1.15
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Constable, R. T., Skudlarski,
P., Fulbright, R. K., Bronen, R. A., Fletcher, J. M., Shankweiler, D. P.,
Katz, L., & Gore, J. C. (1995). Sex differences in the functional organi-
S U P P O R T I N G I N FO R M AT I O N
zation of the brain for language. Nature, 373(6515), 607–­609. Additional Supporting Information may be found online in the
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, Supporting Information section.
C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sen- Transparent Peer Review Report
sory components of pain. Science, 303(5661), 1157–­1162.
Sokołowski, A., Tyburski, E., Sołtys, A., & Karabanowicz, E. (2020). Sex dif-
ferences in verbal fluency among young adults. Advances in Cognitive
Psychology, 16(2), 92–­102. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-­0288-­1 How to cite this article: Proverbio AM. Sex differences in the
Stevens, J. S., & Hamann, S. (2012). Sex differences in brain activa- social brain and in social cognition. J Neurosci Res. 2021;00:​
tion to emotional stimuli: A meta-­analysis of neuroimaging stud- 1–­9. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24787
ies. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1578–­ 1593. https://doi.org/10.1016/​
j.neuro​psych​ologia.2012.03.011

You might also like