You are on page 1of 5

Soft Neurological Signs in Learning\x=req-\

Disabled Children and Controls


Richard M. Adams, MD; Jenci J. Kocsis; Robert E. Estes, PhD

Learning-disabled fourth graders were normative data. Tactile Perception Tests of Ayres.9
compared with a control group for incidence The goal of this study was to com¬ The same author10 has recently re¬
of selected neurological signs. Factors eval- pare groups of learning-disabled, bor¬ ported constellations of sensory in¬
uated were eye-hand preference, balance, derline-disabled, and control fourth tegrative dysfunctions (including
stereognosis, graphesthesia, hand-finger im- grade children, with respect to inci¬ poor graphesthesia) in 6%- to 8%-
mobility (abnormality, choreoathetosis), fin- dence of variations in selected soft year-olds with academic disorders.
ger localization, and diadochokinesia. Color
vision and head circumference were also neurological signs. This term is Spreen11 has reported norms for
evaluated as possible correlates of learning broadly defined as those neurological stereognosis, however, no studies
variations that are equivocal or inter¬ were found in the literature relating
disability. Although two of the functions
tested (graphesthesia and diadochokinesia) mittent, and consequently may be astereognosis to poor reading or glo¬
were significantly depressed in the learning\x=req-\ overlooked during neurologic exami¬ bal school performance. The associa¬
disabled group compared with results in the nation. A review of the literature tion of finger agnosia and dyscalculia
control group, the magnitude of difference relative to the measurements eval¬ is well known,1213 though apparently
was not great enough for clinical usefulness. uated disclosed limited and often in¬ rare. Benton,14 in a study of 158 nor¬
conclusive data. In the motor realm, mal children aged 5% to 9% years, re¬
Denhoff and Siqueland1·2 have sug¬ ported that finger localization skills
gested that a positive relationship ex¬ do not attain maturity until late
some time, controversy has ists between a child's ability to per¬ childhood. He cautions that norma¬
For existed in the literature over the
relevance of soft neurological signs in
form diadochokinetic tasks and his
school performance. Grant et al3 have
tive data per se do not necessarily in¬
dicate that low scores possess patho¬
children with learning disorders. claimed that increased proficiency of logical implications, but suggests
Hundreds of publications later, we diadochokinesia is a function of age poor performance may be predictive
are scarcely nearer a resolution of the with full development at 9 to 10 of cerebral dysfunction.
controversy, primarily due to poorly years. Rutter and Birch4 and Prechtl5 Miscellaneous indexes reviewed in¬
controlled studies and insufficient have noted the association of chorei¬ cluded color vision, eye-hand prefer¬
form movements with hyperkinesis ence, and head size. At least one au¬
and learning problems, while other thor15 has suggested a correlation
Received for publication March 7, 1974; ac- authors2·6·8 have found positive corre¬ between hereditary color-vision defi¬
cepted May 5. lations between clumsiness and poor ciency and learning disabilities. The
From the Department of Pediatrics, Univer-
sity of Texas Southwestern Medical School (Drs.
school performance. majority of well-controlled studies16-20
Adams and Estes and Mr. Kocsis); and the Re- In the sensory realm, probably the indicates no relationship between
search and Evaluation Center for Learning (Drs. best norms for graphesthesia and mixed eye-hand preference and read¬
Adams and Estes), Dallas.
Reprint requests to 3700 Ross Ave, Dallas, TX stereognosis are to be found in the ing ability; however, this factor con¬
75204 (Dr. Adams). Southern California Kinesthesia and tinues to be cited as a correlate of

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Virginia Commonwealth University User on 10/03/2013


poor school performance in several (hereafter referred to as the control group) his elevated foot touched the floor, he used
school districts. In relation to head differed from the other two groups only in his arms, shifted his floor foot excessively,
circumference, there is a known cor¬ its learning quotient. In addition to the or employed wide amplitude torso gyra¬

relation between microcephaly and, learning quotient, each child met the fol¬ tions to maintain balance. Hopping (30
to a lesser degree, macrocephaly with lowing criteria: an IQ of 85 or above on the times on foot of subject's choice): Subject
group intelligence test; and an absence of was assessed as awkward, if hopping
low intelligence. It has been postu¬
lated that there might be a portion of significant visual or auditory acuity defi¬ was entirely flatfooted and slower than one
cits, as determined by a school screening hop per second. On both the standing and
learning-disabled children with un- examination resulting in a visual acuity of hopping tasks, the subject was permitted
diagnosed, arrested hydrocephaly. at least 20/30 OU on the Snellen chart at 20 three unsuccessful trials before being clas¬
From these investigations, it can ft and passing a five-frequency audiogram sified as awkward. If the subject was clas¬
be seen that interstudy comparisons at 25dB; no physical handicaps or major sified as awkward on either task, he was
are difficult, due to lack of uniformity motor dysfunctions were noted on routine considered to have poor balance.
of testing procedures and the hetero¬ school health examination by the child's Sensory Items.—Stereognosis.—Subject
geneity of subjects. This study was physician. In addition, no child was ac¬ was first asked to name by visual inspec¬
designed in an effort to eliminate cepted for the study whose school records tion four coins (quarter, nickle, dime, and
indicated referral for a possible emotional penny), then to identify them "by feel"
some of these shortcomings. problem. with vision occluded. Twenty identification
Clinical Population The subjects entered a quiet, unoccupied trials were administered (five for each
and Methods classroom in pairs with each child attend¬ coin) in a predetermined sequence with
ing separate stations for administration of each coin succeeding itself once; auditory
The specific neurologic measurements half the test items and exchanging sta¬ clues were avoided. No practice was given
evaluated in this study include the follow¬ tions for completion of the items. Parti¬ and the number of errors for the 20 trials
ing: (1) eye-hand preference; (2) balance; tions were employed between stations to was recorded.
(3) stereognosis; (4) graphesthesia; (5) reduce visual distraction. Examiners were Graphesthesia.—Subject was told that
hand-finger immobility (abnormality, eho- unaware of any child's learning classifica¬ numbers would be "drawn" on his hand for
reoathetosis); (6) finger localization; and (7) tion during the testing period. Individual him to identify. A ballpoint pen with the
diadochokinesia. Although not considered items were administered as follows: stylus retracted was used to draw single
"soft signs," color vision and head circum¬ Motor Items.—Diadochokinesia.—Subject digit numbers (zero through nine) on the
ference were evaluated as possible corre¬ was asked to tap a hand-controlled tally palm of the dominant hand, positioned for
lates of learning disability. These indexes counter alternately with the palm and dor- correct "visual" orientation; the eyes were
were chosen on the basis of frequency of sum of his dominant hand as fast as pos¬ occluded. One orientation trial was admin¬
occurrence in the literature, ease of admin¬ sible for ten seconds. Following a demon¬ istered and any errors made were cor¬
istration, and, in some instances, because stration and practice session, two trials rected. All digits appeared twice; no num¬
of quantifiable characteristics. were run and the number of taps regis¬ ber succeeded itself. Twenty trials were
The sample consisted of 368 fourth grad¬ tered by the counter was recorded for the administered in a predetermined sequence
ers in 18 research classrooms from the second trial. The same procedure was fol¬ and the number of errors recorded.
Dallas and Irving Independent School Dis¬ lowed for the nondominant hand. Finger Localization.—Subject was seated
tricts. The age range was 9 years, 3 Hand-Finger Immobility (abnormality, at a table with his dominant hand inserted
months to 11 years, 8 months with a mean choreoathetosis or choreiform movements).— palm down into a visual occlusion box14;
of 10 years, 2 months and a standard de¬ The subject was asked to imitate fingers were spread flat against the box
viation of 5 months. the examiner in extending the hands (6 in floor. The test consisted of the examiner
All children had been administered the above table while seated) and spreading touching two of the subject's fingers simul¬
Metropolitan Achievement Test (MAT) the fingers. He was directed "see if you taneously on the dorsum of the proximal
and a group intelligence test (the Califor¬ can keep your hands and fingers as still as phalynx at its midpoint. The examiner
nia Test of Mental Maturity for the Dallas I can for one minute." Praise was given at used the pulp portion of his distal
children and the Short Form Test of Aca¬ 30 seconds, regardless of performance. A phalynges to apply approximately one
demic Aptitudes for the Irving children). subjective assessment was made by the ex¬ gram of pressure for one second. The sub¬
Myklebust Learning Quotients21 were com¬ aminer (R.M.A.) as to whether choreo- ject was not allowed to move his fingers to
puted for all children using the total read¬ athetotic movements of the hands, fin¬ add kinesthetic clues. Ten combinations of
ing, spelling, and math computation grade- gers, or arms were constant, occasional, or the fivefingers (index-thumb, ring-middle,
placement scores from the MAT. The low¬ absent. etc) were administered twice each in a pre¬
est of these three quotients was utilized Balance. -Standing (on one foot for 30 determined order; no combination suc¬
for classification. A child whose learning seconds): The subject was asked to imitate ceeded itself. The subject was then asked
quotient was 90 or above was classified as the examiner (R.M.A.) by lifting one foot to indicateon a line drawing of a hand
normal (N), 85 to 89 as borderline (B), and (subject's choice) and placing it against the (digits numbered one to five) the two fin¬
84 or below as learning-disabled (LD), opposite knee while keeping both arms at gers touched, either by pointing or giving
yielding 130, 99, and 139 children in each his side. The subject was assessed as "awk¬ the numbers of the fingers. A practice trial
group, respectively. The normal group ward" if during the 30-second test period, of three of the combinations was given

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Virginia Commonwealth University User on 10/03/2013


and all errors corrected. The number of er¬ gated. Variation (dysfunction) was sents the percent of children, by
rors for the 20 trials was recorded. defined by two methods. One was to learning classification and sex-exhib¬
Color Visicra.-Subject was asked to establish the criteria for dysfunction iting variations from the norms, in
identify all number items on the Ishihara prior to administering the tasks; with each of the neurologic functions in¬
Color Plates (adult form, 1973). Presence each child either meeting or failing to
of normal or abnormal color vision, as de¬
vestigated.
meet the criteria. This procedure was To compare the differences be¬
fined in the test manual, was noted.
utilized forhand-finger immobility, tween the learning classification
Miscellaneous Items.—Eye-Hand Prefer-
ewce.-Subject was asked to look through a balance, eye-hand preference, and groups with respect to incidence of
child's kaleidoscope handed to him in the color vision, with the criteria for each variation in the neurological func¬
midline, at chest level. The same procedure factor as described previously. The tions, 2 tests with one degree of free¬
was then followed with a plastic tube second method was to define vari¬ dom, using Yate's correction for con¬
("telescope"). If he used the same eye for ation statistically. This was accom¬ tinuity,21 were applied with each
both, this eye was recorded as the pre¬ plished by determining the mean and measurement and are reported below.
ferred one; if not, two additional trials standard deviation for each parame¬ Motor Items.—Dysdiadochokinesia.
with each instrument were given. If at ter and defining poor function as a —A
least four of six trials were unilateral, the
significantly greater incidence
score that was 1.5-SD units or more ( 2 4.46, P<.001) was found in the
=

subject was considered to have an eye pref¬ from that mean. For head circum¬ LD boys than in the control boys
erence. In addition, the hand used by the
subject in writing his name was recorded. ference, published means and stan¬ when comparing variations with the
If the subject lacked eye preference, or if dard deviations were used to deter¬ dominant hand. No other reliable dif¬
the preferred eye and hand were not uni¬ mine the variation.22 There are no ferences were obtained when compar¬
lateral, he was considered to have mixed normative data for the procedures ing learning classifications. Within
eye-hand preference. used in testing diadochokinesia, ster- both the and LD classifications,
Head Circumference.-The frontooccipi- eognosis, graphesthesia, and finger boys had significantly greater inci¬
tal circumference was measured with a localization. For these measurements, dence than girls ( 2 4.46, P<.05;
=

steel tape measure. The largest of three the means and standard deviations of
measurements was recorded to the nearest
2 13.95, P<.001, respectively). Com¬
=

the controls, as previously defined by parisons of variations with the non-


0.2 cm.
use of the Myklebust Learning Quo¬ dominant hand indicated a signifi¬
Results
tient, were calculated for each sex cantly greater incidence ( 2 7.79, =

separately and used as normative P<.01) in the LD girls than in the


In order to compare the LD, B, and data to define variation. Table 1 pre¬ control girls. Other comparisons
control groups with respect to inci¬ sents the normative data by learning among learning classifications did not
dence of neurological variations, the classification and sex as well as the yield significant differences. Girls had
concept of variation was first defined cut-off points defining variations for a significantly greater incidence than
for each of the measurements investi- the above parameters. Table 2 pre- boys ( 2 3.88, P<.05) in the LD
=

Table 1.—Normative Data and Cutoff Points for Neurological Measurements by Learning Classification and Sex

Cutoff Points,
Control Borderline Learning Disabled 1.5 SD from M
of controls
52 78 53 46 83 56
Neurological Measurements M F M F M F M F
Diadochokinetic rates,
taps/10 sec
Dominant hand M 30.35 29.49 29.23 29.09 28.27 28.62 26.34 23.80
SD 2.67 3.79 3.67 3.26 3.40 4.65
Nondominant hand M 26.73 27.10 26.70 26.59 25.06 25.09 21.99 21.47
SD 3.16 3.75 3.97 3.16 3.22 4.45
Stereognosis for coins,
No. of errors M 2.13 2.96 3.02 3.46 2.37 3.18 5.66 6.47
for 20 items SD 2.35 2.34 2.49 2.75 2.24 2.18
Graphesthesia for numerals,
No. of errors M 3.00 2.56 5.15 3.52 4.24 4.55 6.24 6.85
for 20 Items SD 2.16 2.86 3.55 3.36 3.19 3.61
Finger localization,
No. of errors M 3.48 4.21 4.91 4.65 3.89 4.50 7.47 8.62
for 20 items SD 2.66 2.94 3.40 3.54 2.58 2.85

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Virginia Commonwealth University User on 10/03/2013


Table 2.—Percent of Children by Learning Classification and cidence of either large or small head
Sex-Exhibiting Variations in Neurologic Functions circumference (+ and —1.5 SD from
M, respectively) were noted among
Control Borderline Learning Disabled learning classifications. In comparing
Neurological 52 78 53 46 83 56 incidence between boys and girls,
_Variation_M_F_M_F_M_F_
Motor items
small head circumference was sig¬
Dysdiadochoklnesia
nificantly more frequent in girls
Dominant hand 7.7 9.0 20.8 4.3 33.7 5.4 ( 2 6.35, P<.05; 2 13.36, P<.001;
= =

Nondomlnanthand 3.8 6.4 7.5 8.7 10.8 25.0 2 15.07, P<.001 for the control, B,
=

Choreoathetosis and LD groups, respectively). None of


Occasional 19.2 10.3 28.3 10.9 30.1 17.9
Constant 17.3 0.0 15.1 2.2 6.0 0.0 the differences was reliable with re¬
Poor balance_32.7 11.5 35.8 10.9 44.6 16.1 spect to large head circumference.
Sensory items Diadochokinesia, stereognosis,
Poor stereognosis 9.6 9.0 13.2 15.2 12.0 10.7
Poor graphesthesia 9.6 11.5 35.8 19.6 22.9 26.8 graphesthesia, finger localization, and
Poor finger localization 7.7 9.0 22.6 13.0 9.6 7.1 head circumference were amenable to
Color deficient
Miscellaneous Items
vision_3J3_CL0_5^7_0_ _8^4_0.0 evaluation by analysis of variance
Mixed laterally 23.1 46.2 28.3 52.2 36.1 42.9 techniques. Results of the analysis in¬
Head circumference dicated that only two of the factors,
-1.5 SD from M 1.9 17.9 0.0 28.3 4.8 30.4 diadochokinesia and graphesthesia,
+1.5 SD from M 7.7 1.3 11.3 0.0 4.8 1.8
produced reliable differences among
learning classifications. The diadocho-
kinetic rate for the dominant hand
classification but no sex differences than control girls ( 2 4.17, P<.05),
=
was significantly faster (P<.005) in
were found in either the control or while the incidence in the boys was the control as compared to the LD
groups. significantly greater than that in the group, with no significant differences
Choreoathetosis.—No significant dif¬ control boys ( 2 8.81, <.01). The re¬
=
noted between the and either the
ferences in incidence were found maining differences among learning control or LD groups. The rate for the
among any of the learning classifica¬ classifications and those between nondominant hand was significantly
tions for either occasional or constant sexes were not reliable. faster (P<.01) for both the control
choreoathetotic movements. A signif¬ Poor Finger Localization.-differ¬ and as compared to the LD groups,
icant sex difference was obtained ences in incidence were not reliable with no reliable differences indicated
( 2 5.20, P<.05) that indicated that
=
either when comparing by learning between the rates of the control and
the boys had a greater incidence of classifications or by sex. children. The number of errors
occasional choreoathetosis than the Poor Color Vision.—'No significant made in graphesthesia was signifi¬
girls. Boys also had a greater inci¬ differences in incidence were ob¬ cantly less (P<.001) for the control
dence than girls ( 2 =11.94, P<.001) tained among learning classifications group when compared to both the
of constant choreoathetotic move¬ or between sexes. When the incidence and LD groups, with no reliable dif¬
ment in the control group. figures in the present study are com¬ ferences indicated between the rates
Poor Balance.-There were no sig¬ pared to those for color blindness in of the latter two groups.
nificant differences in incidence the general population, the LD chil¬
among learning classifications for ei¬ dren are quite comparable to pub¬ Comment
ther boys or girls. Within all three lished norms.24 A significantly greater incidence of
classifications, however, the boys had Miscellaneous Items.—Mixed Eye- dysdiadochokinesia was found in the
a significantly greater incidence than Hand Preference.-There were no re¬ LD boys than in the control boys
the girls ( 2 7.45, P<.01; 2 =7.06,
=
liable differences with respect to inci¬ when comparing variations with the
P<.01; 2 11.01, P<.001 for the con¬
= dence among learning classifications. dominant hand. Comparisons of vari¬
trol, B, and LD classifications, respec¬ For both the control and classifica¬ ations with the nondominant hand
tively). tions, girls had a significantly greater disclosed a significantly greater inci¬
Sensory Items.—Poor Stereognosis.— incidence than boys ( 2 6.18, P<.05;
= dence in the LD girls than in the con¬
Differences among learning classifi¬ 2 4.92, P<.05, respectively). This
=
trol girls. No significant differences in
cations and between sexes disclosed difference was not noted with the LD incidence were found among any of
no reliable differences in incidence. children. the learning classifications for poor
Poor Graphesthesia.—The LD girls Variation in Head Circum¬ stereognosis, choreoathetotic move¬
had a significantly greater incidence ference—No reliable differences in in- ments, poor balance, mixed eye-hand

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Virginia Commonwealth University User on 10/03/2013


preference, color blindness, or poor There were 34 (9.2%) additional sub¬ extrapolated to younger children, as
finger localization. Neither were sig¬ jects with finger agnosia only. the incidence of soft neurological
nificant differences found among It is apparent that 9- and 10-year- signs is an age-related phenomenon.
learning classifications for head size, old learning-disabled children cannot However, as early identification and
which deviated 1.5 SD or more from be reliably distinguished from nor¬ educational intervention improve
the mean. Although two of the func¬ mally achieving children on the basis prognosis, the authors plan to repli¬
tions tested (diadochokinesia and of the soft neurological signs outlined cate this study with first and second
graphesthesia) were significantly dif¬ in this study. Thus, classroom obser¬ grade children. It is hoped that other
ferent in the LD group as compared vation and selected psychoeducation- investigators will employ uniform
to the control group, the magnitude al testing would appear the logical testing procedures to facilitate in-
of the differences was not great approach to identification of learning terstudy comparisons not currently
enough to make it helpful in differ¬ disabilities, at least for older elemen¬ possible.
entiating the two groups clinically. tary children. For the present, gen¬
The combination of both conditions eral physical examination and vision This investigation was supported in part by a
showed only 13.7% of the LD group and hearing screening should consti¬ grant from the Jonsson Foundation and the Edu¬
and included none of the control tute adequate initial medical eval¬ cation Service Center, Region X to the Research
and Evaluation Center for Learning through the
children. uation for the large majority of school Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas
In looking for possible cases of de¬ children with learning disorders. Ob¬ Southwestern Medical School at Dallas.
John Nelson, MD, and Michael Blaw, MD,
velopmental Gerstmann syndrome,12 vious exceptions would be suspicion of made helpful suggestions during preparation of
seven subjects (1.9%) were identified seizure disorder or hyperkinesis that the manuscript.
with both poor finger localization would merit special pediatrie or neu¬ Consent for use of subjects in this study was
obtained from the parents or guardians after the
(finger agnosia) and math-learning rologic consultation. It must be cau¬ nature of the procedures had been fully ex¬
quotients below 85 (dyscalculia). tioned that these results should not be plained.

References
1. Denhoff E: The measurement of psychoneuro- mental Gerstmann's syndrome. Arch Neurol 8:490\x=req-\
logical factors contributing to learning efficiency. J 501, 1963.
Learn Dis 1:636-644, 1968. 13. Slade PD: Developmental dyscalculia: A brief
2. Denhoff E, Siqueland M: Developmental and report on four cases. Psychol Med 1:292-298, 1971.
predictive characteristics of items from the Meeting 14. Benton A: Development of finger-localization
Street School screening test. Dev Med Child Neurol capacity in school children. Child Dev 26:225-230,
10:220-234, 1968. 1955.
3. Grant WW, Boelsche A, Zin D: Developmental 15. Espinda SD: Color vision deficiency: A learning
patterns of two motor functions. Dev Med Child disability? J Learn Dis 6:163-166, 1973.
Neurol 15:171-177, 1973. 16. Holveston EM: Controlling eye-dominant hem-
4. Rutter M, Birch H: Interrelations between the isphere relationships as factor in reading ability. Am
choreiform syndrome, reading disability and psy- J Ophthalmol 70:96-199, 1970.
chiatric disorder in children 8-11 years. Dev Med 17. Towen BDL: Laterality and dominance. Dev
Child Neurol 8:149-159, 1966. Med Child Neurol 14:747-755, 1972.
5. Prechtl HF: The choreiform syndrome in chil- 18. Spitzer RL: The relationship between mixed
dren. Dev Med Child Neurol 4:119-127, 1962. dominance and reading disabilities. Pediatr 54:76\x=req-\
J

6. Illingworth RS: The clumsy child, in Minimal 80, 1959.


Cerebral Dysfunction: Little Club Clinics in Devel- 19. Belmont L, Birch H: Lateral dominance, lat-
opmental Medicine. London, William Heinemann eral awareness, and reading disability. Child Dev
Medical Books Ltd, 1963, vol 10, p 26. 36:57-71, 1965.
7. Reuben RN, Bakwin H: Developmental clumsi- 20. Witty PA, Kopel D: Sinistral and mixed man-
ness. Pediatr Clin North Am 15:601-610, 1968. ual-ocular behavior in reading disability. J Ed Psy-
8. Gubbay SS: Clumsy children: A study of apraxic chol 27:119-134, 1936.
and agnostic defects in 21 children. Brain 88:295-312, 21. Myklebust HR: Learning disabilities: Defini-
1965. tion and overview, in Progress in Learning Dis-
9. Ayres J: Southern California Kinesthesia and abilities. New York, Grune & Stratton Inc, 1968, vol
Tactile Perception Test Manual. Beverly Hills, 1, pp 4-9.
Western Psychological Services, 1966. 22. Cooke RE: The Biologic Basis of Pediatric
10. Ayres J: Types of sensory integrative dysfunc- Practice. New York, McGraw-Hill Book Co Inc, 1968,
tion among disabled learners. Am J Occup Ther vol 2, pp 1698-1699.
26:13-18, 1972. 23. Guilford JP: Fundamental Statistics in Psy-
11. Spreen O: Developmental norms for 15 neu- chology and Education, ed 4. New York, McGraw\x=req-\
ropsychological tests, age 6 to 15. Cortex 5:170-191, Hill Book Co Inc, 1965, pp 237-238.
1969. 24. Wright WD: Researches on Normal and Defec-
12. Kinsbourne M, Warrington GK: The develop- tive Colour Vision. London, Kimpton, 1946.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Virginia Commonwealth University User on 10/03/2013

You might also like