You are on page 1of 8

Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychresns

Action-based cognitive remediation in bipolar disorder improved verbal


memory but had no effect on the neural response during episodic
memory encoding
Julian Macoveanu a, 1, Viktoria Damgaard a, b, 1, Caroline V. Ott a, b, Gitte M. Knudsen c, d,
Lars V Kessing a, c, Kamilla W Miskowiak a, b, *
a
Copenhagen Affective Disorder Research Centre (CADIC), Psychiatric Centre Copenhagen, Copenhagen University Hospital, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark
b
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
c
Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen,
Denmark
d
Neurobiology Research Unit and The Center for Experimental Medicine Neuropharmacology, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Verbal memory and executive function impairments are common in remitted patients with bipolar disorder (BD).
Picture encoding We recently found that Action-Based Cognitive Remediation (ABCR) may improve executive function and verbal
Verbal memory memory in BD. Here, we investigated neuronal changes associated with ABCR treatment-related memory
Fmri
improvement in a longitudinal functional MRI (fMRI) study. Forty-five patients with remitted BD (ABCR: n = 26,
control treatment: n = 19) completed a picture-encoding task during fMRI and tests of verbal memory and ex­
ecutive function outside the scanner before and after two weeks of ABCR/control treatment. The cognitive
assessment was performed again following ten weeks of treatment. Thirty-four healthy controls underwent the
same test protocol once for baseline comparisons. Patients showed a moderate improvement in a domain
composite of verbal learning and memory both after two weeks and ten weeks of ABCR treatment, which
correlated with improved executive function. At baseline, patients showed encoding-related hypoactivity in
dorsal prefrontal cortex compared to healthy controls. However, treatment was not associated with significant
task-related neuronal activity changes. Improved verbal learning and memory may have occurred through
strengthened strategic processing targeted by ABCR. However, picture-encoding paradigms may be suboptimal to
capture the neural correlates of this improvement, possibly by failing to engage strategic encoding processes.

1. Introduction BD has shown replicable and enduring pro-cognitive efficacy (Kamilla


W. Miskowiak et al., 2016a), although emerging evidence points to
Objective cognitive impairments across several domains are present beneficial effects of cognitive remediation interventions (Lewandowski
in bipolar disorder (BD), even in remitted or subsyndromal patients et al., 2017; Strawbridge et al., 2021). A study of functional remediation
(Bourne et al., 2013). Cognitive impairments in BD impede psychosocial in BD revealed a selective improvement in verbal memory, which was
function, occupational status, and quality of life (Baune and Malhi, associated with improved functioning at six months (Bonnín et al.,
2015; Brissos et al., 2008; Tse et al., 2014). Specifically, verbal learning 2016) and two years after treatment completion (Solé et al., 2020).
and memory has been identified as a robust indicator of neuropsycho­ Verbal memory thus seems particularly important for patients’ daily life
logical impairment in BD and is associated with a significant impact on functioning.
daily functioning and occupational outcome (Bonnín et al., 2012, 2010; In line with the recommendations from the International Society for
Tse et al., 2014). Currently, no treatment for cognitive impairments in Bipolar Disorder (ISBD) Cognition Task Force (Miskowiak et al., 2017),

* Corresponding author at: Neurocognition and Emotion in Affective Disorder (NEAD) Group, Copenhagen Affective Disorder Research center, Psychiatric center
Copenhagen, Copenhagen University Hospital, Rigshospitalet, Blegdamsvej 9, DK-2100 Copenhagen, and Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen,
Øster Farimagsgade 2A, DK-1353 Copenhagen, Denmark.
E-mail address: kamilla.miskowiak@regionh.dk (K.W. Miskowiak).
1
Contributed equally as first authors

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2021.111418
Received 26 May 2021; Received in revised form 24 August 2021; Accepted 16 November 2021
Available online 23 November 2021
0925-4927/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

we recently conducted a double-blinded randomized controlled study of neuroplasticity (Park and Bischof, 2013). Upon inclusion, the primary
the effects of 10 weeks of group-based biweekly Action-Based Cognitive therapist (CO) opened the randomization envelopes in a consecutive
Remediation (ABCR) compared with weekly unstructured group ses­ manner. For a power calculation for the primary outcome of the study,
sions in remitted BD patients with cognitive impairments (for full study see the full protocol (Ott et al., 2018). Randomization was carried out
protocol see Ott et al., 2018; Ott et al., 2019). Specifically, ABCR from July 2017 to April 2019, and data collection was completed in
involved a combination of strategic cognitive training and practical January 2020.
exercises to promote cognitive flexibility (Bowie et al., 2017). The study
further included functional MRI (fMRI) assessments at baseline and after 2.2. Participants
two weeks of treatment to explore early neuronal underpinnings of
treatment-related changes in cognitive function. In line with the Participants were recruited from psychiatric outpatient clinics and
emphasis on strategy use and planning to solve cognitive challenges in consultant psychiatrists in the Capital Region of Denmark. Inclusion
ABCR, we found an improvement in executive function, the secondary criteria were: an ICD-10 diagnosis of BD confirmed in a semi-structured
trial outcome, as well as on tertiary measures of verbal learning and clinical interview using the Schedules of Clinical Assessment in Neuro­
memory (Ott et al., 2020). The improvement of these two aspects of psychiatry (SCAN) (Wing et al., 1990) in full or partial remission (full
cognition - in the absence of changes in other domains - is interesting in remission: ≤7, partial remission: 8–14 on the Hamilton Depression
light of evidence for an influence of executive functions on strategic Rating Scale – 17 items (HDRS-17) (Hamilton, 1960) and the Young
aspects of verbal encoding and recall (Chang et al., 2010; Taconnat Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (Young et al., 1978), respectively), in line
et al., 2010; Tremont et al., 2000). The ABCR-related executive function with the recommendations from the International Society for Bipolar
improvements were accompanied and predicted by early increase in Disorder (ISBD) Cognition Task Force (Miskowiak et al., 2017), fluent in
dorsal prefrontal cortex (dPFC) activity during a spatial working mem­ Danish, age between 18 and 55 years old, and objective cognitive
ory (WM) fMRI paradigm (Ott et al., 2021). However, the neuronal impairment (total scores ≤74 or below the cut-offs on ≥2 subtests on the
correlates of treatment-related improvements in verbal learning and Screen for Cognitive Impairment in Psychiatry (SCIP) (Jensen et al.,
memory are unclear. There is a paucity of studies investigating the 2015)). Exclusion criteria were: daily use of benzodiazepines (≥22.5
neuronal correlates of memory impairments in mood disorders and the mg), current drug or alcohol abuse, neurological illness or previous se­
findings from these studies are conflicting (Miskowiak and Petersen, vere head trauma, schizophrenia or a schizoaffective disorder, severe
2019). Using an episodic memory encoding paradigm that probes a somatic illness, electroconvulsive treatment within the previous three
network of medial temporal, parietal and prefrontal regions, we found months, dyslexia, pregnancy, pacemaker or other internal metal objects.
that cognitively impaired vs. cognitively intact BD patients display Data from 34 healthy controls (HC) with no personal or first-degree
reduced encoding-related activity in the middle temporal lobe but no family history of psychiatric illness from the ongoing Bipolar Illness
differential dPFC or hippocampal response (Petersen et al., 2021). We Onset (BIO) study (Kessing et al., 2017) were included to assess whether
also used this encoding paradigm to investigate the neuronal un­ a treatment-related neuronal change in ABCR vs. control treatment was
derpinnings of verbal memory improvement following treatment with towards normalization (i.e. HC activation levels). Data from the same
the multifunctional growth factor erythropoietin (EPO) in patients with HC sample were included to standardize the performance of the patient
mood disorders (Miskowiak et al., 2016b). This revealed sample on neuropsychological tests of verbal learning and memory.
treatment-related dPFC and temporo-parietal activity increase, which Participants were informed about the study procedures and written
correlated with patients’ memory improvement (Miskowiak et al., consent was obtained. The study was approved by the local ethics
2016b). committee (H-16,043,480) and the Danish Data Protection Agency
In the present longitudinal fMRI study, which was part of the above- (2012–58–0004).
mentioned ABCR trial (Ott et al., 2018), we employed the same episodic
memory encoding paradigm to investigate whether this psychological 2.3. Treatment and control groups
intervention for cognitive impairments would produce similar changes
in the neurocircuitry target engagement as following EPO intervention. The ABCR program is a manual-based cognitive remediation treat­
We hypothesized that improvement in verbal memory in ABCR vs. ment that provides an action-based top-down approach to restoration of
control groups would be accompanied by early changes in neuronal cognitive impairment (Bowie et al., 2017). Treatment duration was 10
activity during episodic memory encoding after two weeks of treatment weeks with two-hour sessions twice a week and daily computer training
within (i) the medial temporal region showing aberrant at home. There were four to six patients in each group session, which
encoding-related activity in cognitively impaired BD patients (Petersen included computerized training on the Danish version of Happy Neuron
et al., 2021) and (ii) the dPFC, reflecting strengthened strategic encod­ Pro (www.happyneuronpro.com/en), cognitive strategy learning, visual
ing processes dependent on executive functions, in line with the effects and verbal working memory, attention, memory and executive functions
of ABCR on strategic aspects of cognition. with application in practical activities (e.g., preparing a meal, updating
one’s calendar etc.). Each session also included goal setting discussions
2. Methods to encourage participation in cognitively stimulating activities during
daily life. In addition to the group sessions, participants were instructed
2.1. Procedure, randomization and masking to perform the computerized training program for 30 min every day at
home. The control arm involved 10 weeks of weekly one-hour un­
Data were collected at the Psychiatric center Copenhagen, Rig­ structured group sessions in which themes relevant to patients were
shospitalet as part of the ABCR trial (Ott et al., 2018; Ott et al., 2019). An discussed but did not entail any cognitive training (for more details, see
fMRI scan and assessments of verbal memory were carried out at base­ Ott et al. 2018; Ott et al., 2019).
line and week 3 following the completion of two weeks of treatment.
Verbal memory was assessed again at treatment completion (week 10). 2.4. Neuropsychological assessment
In this outcome-assessor blind trial, participants were randomized to the
ABCR or the control treatment in a 1:1 allocation order. We used allo­ The participants performed a range of neurocognitive tests described
cation envelopes provided by Pharma Consulting Group (Uppsala, in Ott et al. (2020). Verbal learning and memory was assessed with the
Sweden), and these were kept in a locked filing cabinet to prevent Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) consisting of the following
unblinding. Randomization was stratified by gender (female or male) subtests: RAVLT total recall lists I-V, RAVLT distractor list, RAVLT im­
and age (< or ≥35 years) due to the association between age and mediate recall, RAVLT delayed recall, and RAVLT recognition (Poreh,

2
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

2010). Alternate, matched forms of the RAVLT were used at each (Petersen et al., 2021), respectively.
assessment to minimize effects of learning. Executive functions were
assessed using the following tests: RBANS Digit Span (Randolph et al., 2.6.2. Data pre-processing and subject-level analysis
1998), Spatial Working Memory task ‘between errors’ and ‘strategy’ Functional MRI data processing was performed with the FMRI Expert
measures from the CANTAB (Goldberg, 2013), Trail Making Test B (TMT Analysis Tool (FEAT; version 6.01) part of FMRIB’s Software Library
B) (Adjutant General’s Office, 1944), verbal fluency letters “S” and “D” (FSL; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Pre-processing involved image B0 field
(Borkowski et al., 1967), and WAIS-III Letter-Number Sequencing distortion correction with acquired field map image, realignment of the
(Wechsler, 1997). acquired volumes, non-brain removal, spatial normalization to a Mon­
treal Neurologic Institute (MNI) template, and spatially smoothing
2.5. Picture encoding fMRI task (Gaussian kernel, 5 mm full-width-half-maximum). The time series in
each session were high pass-filtered (max 0.02 Hz). At subject level, we
The fMRI paradigm was programmed in e-prime 2.0 (Psychological modelled the picture encoding task using a block design, with the pic­
Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) and displayed on a screen which ture presentation (i.e., memory encoding) and fixation cross as events
participants viewed using an angled mirror. The paradigm was based on that were convolved with a double-gamma hemodynamic response
the picture encoding task from Hariri et al. (2003) and identical to the function and added temporal derivatives for improved fit to the data.
one applied in our previous fMRI study (Miskowiak et al., 2016b). Pa­
tients were instructed to classify whether pictures showed ‘indoor’ or 2.6.3. Group-level analyses
‘outdoor’ scenes and to pay attention as they would be asked to recall the At the group level, we first estimated task activations at baseline for
pictures, process involving memory encoding and executive functions. the HC group by entering the picture encoding contrast at baseline in a
This was followed by a free recall test immediately after the scan. Pic­ one-sample t-test. Secondly, we estimated group-by-time interaction
tures were matched for valence, arousal, and complexity and were effects for picture encoding responses using a two-way mixed effect
presented in a blocked paradigm to maximize sensitivity for BOLD signal repeated measures ANOVA model (group factor: ABCR vs. control, time
change (Birn et al., 2002). Six picture blocks (24 s each) were preceded factor: baseline and follow-up). The significance level for clusters was
by an instruction screen (2 s) and interleaved with a fixation cross (24 s), set at p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons using Gaussian
resulting in a task duration of 4 min and 50 s. Blocks consisted of six Random Field (GRF) theory subsequent a cluster-forming threshold of z
pictures presented serially for 3 s interleaved with a 1 s fixation cross. = 2.57 (p < 0.005). The models were estimated twice, first restricting
Each block contained an equal number of pictures representing indoor the search volume to the dPFC and secondly at whole brain level.
and outdoor scenes. Alternate, matched versions of the task were For the ROI analysis, we extracted the mean percent BOLD signal
administered at baseline and follow-up in a counterbalanced order to change from a right dlPFC, left MTG, and the hippocampi ROIs in all
minimize learning effects. participants. Using two-sample t-tests we first compared HC and BD
participants at baseline in all ROIs. We secondly estimated the longitu­
2.6. The fMRI analysis dinal effect of ABCR treatment vs. control in BD patients using a
repeated measures general linear model implemented in SPSS v25 (IBM,
2.6.1. MRI acquisition protocol Armonk, New York, United States).
MRI data were obtained at the Copenhagen University Hospital,
Rigshospitalet using a 3 Tesla Siemens Prisma scanner and a 64-channel 2.7. Statistical analyses of behavioral data
head-coil. During the performance of the picture encoding task, blood
oxygen level dependent (BOLD) fMRI was acquired using a T2*- A cognitive domain composite score of ‘verbal learning and memory’
weighted gradient echo spiral echo-planar (EPI) sequence with an were created by standardizing and averaging the following RAVLT
echo time (TE) of 30 ms, repetition time (TR) of 2 s, and flip angle of 90◦ subtests: RAVLT total recall lists I-V, RAVLT immediate recall, RAVLT
A total of 146 vol were acquired, each consisting of 32 slices with a slice delayed recall, and RAVLT recognition based on the mean and standard
thickness of 3 mm with 25% gaps in-between, and a field of view (FOV) deviations (SD) of the HC group. Verbal learning and memory composite
of 230 × 230 mm using a 64 × 64 grid. The BOLD images were regis­ scores were created for the baseline scores, two weeks of treatment
tered to T1-weighted structural images (TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.58 ms; scores, and the treatment completion scores. A similar procedure was
flip angle = 9◦ ; distance factor = 50%; FOV=230 × 230 mm; slice used for calculation of the ‘executive function’ composite score, which
thickness = 0.9 mm). A standard B0 field map sequence was also ac­ was used for a post-hoc analysis of the association between changes in
quired with the same FOV and resolution as the fMRI sequence (TR = the ‘verbal learning and memory’ and ‘executive function’ domains; for
400 ms; TE = 7.38 ms; flip angle=60◦ ) and used for geometric distor­ details see Miskowiak et al. (2021).
tions correction of the BOLD images. The image quality was ascertained To assess the effect of ABCR vs. control treatment on verbal learning
by visual inspection of all individual images. and memory, we conducted linear mixed-effect models. Factors were
time, stratum (classifying age and gender), and treatment (control
2.6.1. Regions of interest treatment as reference category). Fixed effects were time, stratum,
Based on our a priori hypothesis, we constructed a dPFC mask using time*stratum, and time*treatment. Baseline correction was applied.
FSLView 4.0.1 on a standard MNI template based on the Harvard-Oxford To assess the association between changes in ‘verbal learning and
cortical structural Atlas probabilistic maps (Desikan et al., 2006) by memory’ and ‘executive function’ (a key target for ABCR), we conducted
including bilateral superior and medial frontal gyri and the superior a multiple regression analysis with adjustments for demographic and
portions of the anterior division of the cingulate gyrus and the frontal clinical variables (age, gender, years of education, verbal IQ, and change
poles thresholded at 5%. The ventral border of the dPFC was defined by in HDRS-17 and YMRS scores, respectively).
the plane separating the dorsal from the ventral regions of medial PFC Finally, performance on the picture encoding task outside the scan­
(MNI z > 5), defied according to Veit et al. (2010). Using a similar ner (total recall and false recall) was assessed with repeated measures
procedure, we also constructed a left and right mask for the hippocampi. analyses of variance (ANOVA) with time as the within-subjects variables
To investigate the early neural mechanisms of treatment-related exec­ and group at the between-subjects factor. Behavioural data was analysed
utive function improvement, we defined two spheric (8 mm radius) re­ in SPSS at the α-level = 0.05.
gions of interest (ROIs) for the right dlPFC and left middle temporal
gyrus (MTG) centered on the peak coordinates (x = 40, y = 34, z = 29)
and (x = − 64, y = − 54, z = − 6) from Miskowiak et al. (2016b) and

3
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

3. Results interquartile range = 2; control: Mdn = 21.0, interquartile range = 2, U


= 207, p ≥ 0.34). The demographic and clinical data in the ABCR and
3.1. Participants control groups were well-balanced, although more patients in the con­
trol group took antidepressants (p = 0.02) (Table 1).
Among the patients included in the ABCR trial (ABCR: n = 32, con­ Patients in the trial had more subsyndromal mood symptoms and
trol: n = 29), fMRI data were complete and analyzed for n = 45 patients; fewer years of education compared to the HC sample included for
n = 26 (81%) in the ABCR group and n = 19 (66%) in the control group baseline verbal memory and fMRI comparisons (ps ≤ 0.01) (Table 2).
(see Fig. 1 for CONSORT flow diagram). There were no significant
clinical or demographic differences between patients with complete
fMRI data (n = 45) and patients without fMRI data (n = 16) in age, 3.2. Behavioral results
gender, years of education, verbal IQ, mood symptoms, mood episodes,
illness duration, hospitalizations, and BD type (ps ≥ 0.23). There was no Remitted BD patients showed deficits in the domain of verbal
significant between-group difference in the time from the fMRI scanning learning and memory at baseline with a large effect size (t(77) = − 5.6,
at baseline to after two weeks of treatment (ABCR: Mdn = 21.5 days, 95% CI [ − 2.68, − 1.28], p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.32). There was a
significant effect of ACBR vs. control treatment on the verbal learning

Fig. 1. CONSORT flow diagram.

4
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

Table 1 3.3. fMRI results


ABCR vs. Control group demographic and clinical characteristics at baseline.
ABCR (N = Control (N = p- In HC at baseline, episodic memory encoding activated a fronto-
26) 19) value occipital network (Fig. 3, peak effect paracingulate gyrus at x = 10, y
Gender (F/M%) 77/23 79/21 0.87a = 18, z = 40, p < 0.048, z-max = 3.85, cluster size=256 voxels; and
Age in years, median (IQR) 36 (23) 38 (22) 0.77 temporal occipital fusiform cortex, peak effect at x = 28, y = − 48, z = −
Educational years, mean (SD) 13 (3) 14 (3) 0.35 8, p < 0.0001, z-max = 7.54, cluster size = 17,152 voxels).
Verbal IQ, mean (SD) 111 (6) 111 (8) 0.92 The voxel-wise analyses showed no significant differences between
BD type (I/II) 35/65 32/68 0.83
HDRS-17, mean (SD) 7 (5) 6 (4) 0.46
the HC and BD groups at baseline within the dPFC mask and at whole
YMRS, median (IQR) 1 (3) 2 (5) 0.50 brain. However, the ROI analysis showed dPFC hypoactivity in BD
Illness duration in years, median 15 (16) 14 (10) 0.75 compared to HC in the dlPFC ROI (F(1,77)= 5.695, p = 0.019, ηp2 =
(IQR) 0.069) and the paracingulate cluster activated in the HC group (F(1,77)=
Depressive episodes, median (IQR) 5 (7) 6 (11) 0.50
8.030, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.094). No significant group difference was
Manic episodes, median (IQR) 0 (1) 0 (2) 0.95
Hypomanic episodes, median (IQR) 3 (8) 2 (8) 0.66 observed in the hippocampi or the MTG ROIs (p > 0.195).
Mixed state episodes, median (IQR) 0 (1) 0 (1) 0.91 There were no significant differential longitudinal changes in
Total number of episodes, median 9 (14) 15 (12) 0.20 neuronal response to episodic memory encoding (group-by-time inter­
(IQR) action effects) between the ABCR and control BD patients within the
Hospitalizations, median (IQR) 1 (2) 1 (3) 0.85
Medication:
dPFC mask or at whole brain. Similarly, the ROI analyses of the
Antidepressants, no. (%) 2 (8) 7 (37) 0.02a,* extracted BOLD signal change showed no group-by-time interaction
Antipsychotics, no. (%) 12 (4) 9 (47) 0.95 effects in any of the tested ROIs (p > 0.54, Fig. 3).
Anticonvulsants, no. (%) 15 (58) 13 (68) 0.46
Lithium, no. (%) 16 (62) 10 (53) 0.56
4. Discussion
No medication, no. (%) 1 (4) 1 (5) 0.82a
Number of medications, median 2 (1) 2 (1) 0.11
(IQR) This is the first randomized controlled fMRI study to investigate the
neuronal correlates of improved verbal learning and memory in
Chi-square for binary variables, Mann-Whitney for non-parametric data (median
(IQR)), independent t-tests for normally distributed data (mean, (SD)). remitted BD patients following ABCR treatment. In this subgroup of 45
Abbreviations: ABCR: Action-based cognitive remediation, BD: Bipolar disorder, patients with complete longitudinal fMRI data, we confirmed our pre­
HDRS-17: Hamilton Depression Rating Scale 17-items, IQR: Interquartile range, vious preliminary report on treatment-related improvements in verbal
YMRS: Young Mania Rating Scale. learning and memory for ABCR vs. control treatment, which emerged
*
Significant at the 0.05-level. already after two weeks of treatment. Specifically, ABCR-treated pa­
a
Likelihood ratio 2-sided due to violation of Chi-squared assumption of ex­ tients showed improvement in a domain composite score of verbal
pected count. learning and memory, which correlated directly with increase in exec­
utive functions. At a neural level, patients showed encoding-related
dlPFC hypoactivity relative to HC. However, in contrast with the hy­
Table 2
potheses, we observed no early encoding-related neuronal activity
Demographic characteristics at baseline for the bipolar patients and healthy
change in the dPFC, hippocampus or MTL after ABCR vs. control
controls.
treatment.
BD (N = 61) HC (N = 34) p-value
The absence of differential neuronal activity changes between ABCR
Gender (F/M%) 74/26 65/35 0.35 and control treatment contrasts with our previous findings of increased
Age in years, mean (SD) 38 (11) 34 (10) 0.13 ABCR-related dlPFC activity during working memory in the same sam­
Educational years, mean (SD) 14 (3) 16 (2)
ple of patients (Ott et al., 2021). This change in prefrontal activity was
<0.01
Verbal IQ, mean (SD) 112 (6) 113 (5) 0.15
interpreted as a treatment-related modulation of dorsal neuronal re­
Abbreviations: BD: Bipolar disorder, HC: Healthy controls. sources during executive performance. Given our preliminary findings
of potential effects of ABCR on executive functions and the importance
and memory composite score, which was present after two weeks of of executive functions in strategic aspects of verbal memory, we had
treatment (treatment effect = 0.73, 95% CI [0.09, 1.37], p = 0.03, hypothesized that we would observe similar activity in the dorsal pre­
Cohen’s d = 0.40) and prevailed at end of treatment (treatment effect = frontal area in addition to change in MTL activity using the picture
1.01, 95% CI [0.32, 1.71], p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.54). Change in encoding task. The absence of such neuronal changes was therefore
verbal learning and memory correlated with change in executive func­ surprising and could suggest that patients did not employ strategic
tion from baseline to end of treatment after controlling for demographic learning in the applied picture encoding paradigm as expected. Indeed,
and clinical variables (F(6,37) = 2.705, p = 0.028), with executive examining the task-related activations at baseline did not show expected
function increase as the strongest predictor of verbal learning and prefrontal engagement in our patient cohort. While the HC displayed
memory improvements (B = 0.85, p = 0.02). encoding-related dorsomedial PFC activation (Fig. 3), patients showed
The post-scan picture recall test showed a lower performance at hypoactivity in this prefrontal region. This absence of encoding-related
baseline for the BD patients compared to the HC participants with a prefrontal activity in patients suggests that they failed to implement
lower total recall (F(1,71)= 4.973, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.70), but also lower strategic encoding strategies, which may also explain their poorer recall
false recall rate (F(1,71 )= 5.378, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.70). There was no performance compared to HC after the scan. Our findings also contrast
difference between the ABCR and the control BD patients on the picture with the previous study of erythropoietin (EPO) in which we found
encoding performance at baseline nor after two weeks of treatment treatment-related increase in dPFC and temporo-parietal regions during
(total recall: p=.93, false recall: p=.75). However, in both treatment picture encoding, which correlated with improvement in post-scan
groups there was a general improvement in the number of recalled picture recall (Miskowiak et al., 2016b). These differences in the
pictures after two weeks of treatment compared to baseline (F(1,43)= neuronal activity after EPO and ABCR interventions could reflect the
25.55, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.37, Fig. 2). distinct mechanisms of these treatments. Specifically, neuroplasticity
increase with EPO treatment may account for the more global changes in
cognition-relevant cortical networks, including the dPFC, parietal and
temporal regions as well as hippocampal volume and function

5
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

Fig. 2. Behavioral performance for the picture encoding post-scan recall in the Action-Based Cognitive Remediation (ABCR) and Control bipolar patient groups, and
healthy controls (HC). Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Data from 5 HC is missing.

Fig. 3. Neuronal response to picture encoding in the Action-Based Cognitive Remediation (ABCR) and Control bipolar patient groups, and healthy controls (HC). Top
panel – statistical map showing task activations at baseline in HC together with investigated regions-of-interest (green). Bottom panels - bar charts displaying
neuronal response extracted from the regions-of-interest (ROIs) in ABCR (red), control treatment (blue), and HC (green) participants. Error bars represent standard
error of the mean.

6
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

(Miskowiak et al., 2016b; Miskowiak et al., 2015). In contrast, ABCR picture encoding was not affected by ABCR. We suggest that picture
seems to primarily increase executive functions and associated dPFC encoding paradigms that do not explicitly tap into strategic cognitive
activity (Ott et al., 2021), which was a key focus in the interventions and processes may be suboptimal for capturing the neuronal underpinnings
is likely to mediate the observed improvement in verbal memory. of memory improvement following cognitive remediation interventions
Indeed, we observed a direct correlation between changes in verbal in BD.
memory and executive functions in our participants after controlling for
demographic and clinical variables. This association is in line with ev­ Contributors
idence for an impact of executive functions on strategic aspects of verbal
learning and memory, especially in encoding and recall tasks (Chang KWM, VD and CVO planned and conducted the behavioral analyses.
et al., 2010; Hill et al., 2012). JM conducted the fMRI analyses. VD and KWM created the first draft of
This present report has implications for future cognition trials in BD. the manuscript, which was then revised by JM. GMK and LVK were
Based on the current findings, an episodic memory encoding fMRI involved in the original trial design together with KWM. All authors
paradigm may not be sensitive to capture treatment related neuronal reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.
activity following ABCR in BD patients. This is in line with our previous
report in which we observed no underlying activity difference in Funding
cognitively impaired vs. intact BD patients using the same picture
encoding task (Petersen et al., 2021). In addition, the encoding para­ The study was funded by the Lundbeck Foundation Fellowship grant
digm did not show sensitivity towards change in activity due to the awarded to KWM (grant no. R215–2015–4121). The funding source had
occurrence of mood episodes. The present findings add to this emerging no involvement in study design; in the collection, analysis and inter­
evidence indicating that this fMRI paradigm does not capture the pretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to
neuronal correlates of episodic memory improvements. In contrast, WM submit the article for publication.
tasks seem more sensitive to detect underlying neuronal changes of
treatment related improvement in cognition following ABCR (Ott et al., Declaration of Competing Interest
2021). We therefore recommend that future cognition trials in mood
disorders implement fMRI paradigms that are more focused on the The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
cognitive domain that shows treatment-related improvement to examine this work. KWM declare to have received consultancy fees from Lund­
neurocircuitry target engagement. beck and Janssen-Cilag in the past three years.
Strengths of the present study were that it followed the recommen­
dations from the ISBD Cognition Task Force regarding inclusion of Acknowledgements
remitted patients with objective cognitive impairments and investiga­
tion of underlying neuronal activity in relation to the patient’s outside We would like to acknowledge the Copenhagen Affective Disorder
scanner cognitive performances (Miskowiak et al., 2017). Since signifi­ Clinic, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark for assistance with
cant levels of subsyndromal symptoms were allowed in patients within recruitment, and specialist in psychotherapy Nanna Tuxen for her
our trial, the findings may not be representative of patients in full involvement in the ABCR treatment. The computer software used in the
remission. However, ABCR and control groups showed no differential ABCR group is provided free of charge by HappyNeuron Pro (www.
change in subsyndromal symptoms over time (Ott et al., 2020), sug­ happyneuronpro.com).
gesting that the present findings were not confounded by unspecific
effects of mood. A limitation of the current study was that we used an
References
fMRI paradigm that probes visual memory for investigating the neuronal
correlates of verbal memory improvement. Nevertheless, both the verbal Adjutant General’s Office, 1944. Army Individual Test Battery. Manual of Directions and
learning and memory (RAVLT) and picture encoding tests probe Scoring. War Dep, Washington, DC.
Baune, B.T., Malhi, G.S., 2015. A review on the impact of cognitive dysfunction on social,
episodic memory processes that involve strategic processes and the
occupational, and general functional outcomes in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord
integrity of overlapping fronto-temporal regions (Kim, 2011). In addi­ 17, 41–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12341.
tion, almost all participants in the patient group received psychopha­ Birn, R.M., Cox, R.W., Bandettini, P.A., 2002. Detection versus estimation in event-
rmacological treatment which may have influenced their neuronal related fMRI: choosing the optimal stimulus timing. Neuroimage 15, 252–264.
https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0964.
response (e.g., in the hippocampus). However, this does not explain the Bonnín, C.M., Martínez-Arán, A., Torrent, C., Pacchiarotti, I., Rosa, A.R., Franco, C.,
absence of changes in encoding related neuronal activity given the Murru, A., Sanchez-Moreno, J., Vieta, E., 2010. Clinical and neurocognitive
previous observation of dorsal prefrontal activity change following predictors of functional outcome in bipolar euthymic patients: a long-term, follow-
up study. J. Affect. Disord. 121, 156–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ABCR treatment in the same cohort (Ott et al., 2021). Another limitation jad.2009.05.014.
was the significant increase in post-scan picture recall from baseline to Bonnín, C.M., Reinares, M., Martínez-Arán, A., Balanzá-Martínez, V., Sole, B.,
follow-up in both patient groups which suggests a learning effect which Torrent, C., Tabarés-Seisdedos, R., García-Portilla, M.P., Ibáñez, A., Amann, B.L.,
Arango, C., Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., Crespo, J.M., González-Pinto, A., Colom, F.,
may have precluded ABCR-related treatment effects. Lastly, verbal Vieta, E., Alegría, A., Al-Halabi, S., Alonso-Lana, S., Anaya, C., Barbeito, S., Bobes, J.,
learning and memory was only a tertiary outcome in the original ACBR Calvo, A., Cerrillo, E., Correa, P., Custal, N., Fernández, P., Fernández, M., Fuentes-
trial (Ott et al., 2020) and the findings should therefore be considered Durá, I., Galván, G., Isella, S., Jiménez, E., Landín-Romero, R., Mayoral, M., Manuel
Menchón, J., Merchan-Naranjo, J., Ortiz-Gil, J., Pacchiarotti, I., Reyes, R., Rosa, A.
exploratory. Nevertheless, our calculation of an overall domain composite
R., Rapado-Castro, M., Rodao, J.M., Saiz, P.A., Sánchez-Moreno, J., Salamero, M.,
score for verbal learning and memory in the present report revealed Segura, B., Selva-Vera, G., Saiz-Ruiz, J., Soria, V., Subirá, M., Ugarte, A., Valle, J.,
robust treatment-related change in this measure, which emerged already Valls, E., Vega, P., 2016. Effects of functional remediation on neurocognitively
impaired bipolar patients: enhancement of verbal memory. Psychol. Med. 46,
after two weeks of treatment and had a large effect size after treatment
291–301. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001713.
completion. Bonnín, C.M., Sánchez-Moreno, J., Martínez-Arán, A., Solé, B., Reinares, M., Rosa, A.R.,
In the present fMRI study, we investigated the neuronal correlates of Goikolea, J.M., Benabarre, A., Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., Ferrer, M., Vieta, E., Torrent, C.,
verbal learning and memory improvement following 10 weeks of ABCR 2012. Subthreshold symptoms in bipolar disorder: impact on neurocognition, quality
of life and disability. J. Affect. Disord. 136, 650–659. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
in remitted patients with BD. We showed ABCR-related improvement in jad.2011.10.012.
an overall measure of verbal learning and memory after two and 10 Borkowski, J.G., Benton, A.L., Spreen, O., 1967. Word fluency and brain damage.
weeks of treatment. This effect correlated directly with improved ex­ Neuropsychologia 5, 135–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(67)90015-2.
Bourne, C., Aydemir, O., Balanzá-Martínez, V., Bora, E., Brissos, S., Cavanagh, J.T.O.,
ecutive functions and was likely mediated by strengthened strategic Clark, L., Cubukcuoglu, Z., Dias, V.V., Dittmann, S., Ferrier, I.N., Fleck, D.E.,
processing – a key focus in ABCR. Nevertheless, neuronal activity during Frangou, S., Gallagher, P., Jones, L., Kieseppä, T., Martínez-Aran, A., Melle, I.,

7
J. Macoveanu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 319 (2022) 111418

Moore, P.B., Mur, M., Pfennig, A., Raust, A., Senturk, V., Simonsen, C., Smith, D.J., Miskowiak, K.W., Petersen, C.S., 2019. Neuronal underpinnings of cognitive impairment
Bio, D.S., Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G., Stoddart, S.D.R., Sundet, K., Szöke, A., and - improvement in mood disorders. CNS Spectr 24, 30–53. https://doi.org/
Thompson, J.M., Torrent, C., Zalla, T., Craddock, N., Andreassen, O.A., Leboyer, M., 10.1017/S1092852918001062.
Vieta, E., Bauer, M., Worhunsky, P.D., Tzagarakis, C., Rogers, R.D., Geddes, J.R., Miskowiak, K.W., Vinberg, M., Macoveanu, J., Ehrenreich, H., Køster, N., Inkster, B.,
Goodwin, G.M., 2013. Neuropsychological testing of cognitive impairment in Paulson, O.B., Kessing, L.V., Skimminge, A., Siebner, H.R., 2015. Effects of
euthymic bipolar disorder: an individual patient data meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. erythropoietin on hippocampal volume and memory in mood disorders. Biol.
Scand. 128, 149–162. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12133. Psychiatry 78, 270–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.013.
Bowie, C.R., Grossman, M., Gupta, M., Holshausen, K., Best, M.W., 2017. Action-based Ott, C.V., Macoveanu, J., Bowie, C.R., Fisher, P.M., Knudsen, G.M., Kessing, L.V.,
cognitive remediation for individuals with serious mental illnesses: effects of real- Miskowiak, K.W., 2021. Change in prefrontal activity and executive functions after
world simulations and goal setting on functional and vocational outcomes. Psychiatr. action-based cognitive remediation in bipolar disorder: a randomized controlled
Rehabil. J. 40, 53–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000189. trial. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 1113–1121. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-
Brissos, S., Dias, V.V., Kapczinski, F., 2008. Cognitive performance and quality of life in 020-00901-7.
bipolar disorder: https://doi.org/10.1177/070674370805300806 53, 517–524. Ott, C.V., Vinberg, M., Bowie, C.R., Christensen, E.M., Knudsen, G.M., Kessing, L.V.,
10.1177/070674370805300806. Miskowiak, K.W., 2018. Effect of action-based cognitive remediation on cognition
Chang, Y.L., Jacobson, M.W., Fennema-Notestine, C., Hagler, D.J., Jennings, R.G., and neural activity in bipolar disorder: study protocol for a randomized controlled
Dale, A.M., McEvoy, L.K., 2010. Level of executive function influences verbal trial. Trials 19, 487. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-018-2860-8.
memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and predicts prefrontal and Ott, C.V., Vinberg, M., Bowie, C.R., Christensen, E.M., Knudsen, G.M., Kessing, L.V.,
posterior cingulate thickness. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1305–1313. https://doi.org/ Miskowiak, K.W., 2019. Correction to: effect of action-based cognitive remediation
10.1093/cercor/bhp192. on cognition and neural activity in bipolar disorder: study protocol for a randomized
Desikan, R.S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B.T., Dickerson, B.C., Blacker, D., controlled trial (Trials (2018) 19 (487) 10.1186/s13063-018-2860-8). Trials.
Buckner, R.L., Dale, A.M., Maguire, R.P., Hyman, B.T., Albert, M.S., Killiany, R.J., https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-019-3289-4.
2006. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on Ott, C.V., Vinberg, M., Kessing, L.V., Bowie, C.R., Forman, J.L., Miskowiak, K.W., 2020.
MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980. https://doi. Effect of action-based cognitive remediation on cognitive impairment in patients
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. with remitted bipolar disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Bipolar Disord 23,
Goldberg, M.C., 2013. CANTAB, In: Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. 487–499. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.13021.
Springer, New York, New York, NY, pp. 519–522. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1- Park, D.C., Bischof, G.N., 2013. The aging mind: neuroplasticity in response to cognitive
4419-1698-3_1915. training. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 109–119. https://doi.org/10.31887/
Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, dcns.2013.15.1/dpark.
56–62. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. Petersen, J., Macoveanu, J., Kjærstad, H.L., Knudsen, G.M., Kessing, L.V., Miskowiak, K.
Hariri, A.R., Goldberg, T.E., Mattay, V.S., Kolachana, B.S., Callicott, J.H., Egan, M.F., W., 2021. Assessment of the neuronal underpinnings of cognitive impairment in
Weinberger, D.R., 2003. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism bipolar disorder with a picture encoding paradigm and methodological lessons
affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory learnt. J. Psychopharmacol. 35, 983–991. https://doi.org/10.1177/
performance. J. Neurosci. 23, 6690–6694. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.23-17- 02698811211008569.
06690.2003. Poreh, A., 2010. Rey auditory verbal learning test. The Corsini Encyclopedia of
Hill, B.D., Alosco, M., Bauer, L., Tremont, G., 2012. The relation of executive functioning Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, NJ, USA https://doi.org/10.1002/
to CVLT-II learning, memory, and process indexes. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 19, 9780470479216.corpsy0796.
198–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2011.643960. Randolph, C., Tierney, M.C., Mohr, E., Chase, T.N., 1998. The repeatable battery for the
Jensen, J.H., Støttrup, M.M., Nayberg, E., Knorr, U., Ullum, H., Purdon, S.E., Kessing, L. assessment of neuropsychological status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity.
V., Miskowiak, K.W., 2015. Optimising screening for cognitive dysfunction in bipolar J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 20, 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1076/
disorder: validation and evaluation of objective and subjective tools. J. Affect. jcen.20.3.310.823.
Disord. 187, 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.07.039. Solé, B., Bonnín, C.M., Radua, J., Montejo, L., Hogg, B., Jimenez, E., Reinares, M.,
Kessing, L.V., Munkholm, K., Faurholt-Jepsen, M., Miskowiak, K.W., Nielsen, L.B., Valls, E., Varo, C., Pacchiarotti, I., Valentí, M., Garriga, M., Torres, I., Martínez-
Frikke-Schmidt, R., Ekstrøm, C., Winther, O., Pedersen, B.K., Poulsen, H.E., Arán, A., Vieta, E., Torrent, C., 2020. Long-term outcome predictors after functional
McIntyre, R.S., Kapczinski, F., Gattaz, W.F., Bardram, J., Frost, M., Mayora, O., remediation in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychol. Med. 1–9. https://doi.org/
Knudsen, G.M., Phillips, M., Vinberg, M., 2017. The bipolar illness onset study: 10.1017/S0033291720001968.
research protocol for the BIO cohort study. BMJ Open 7, e015462. https://doi.org/ Strawbridge, R., Tsapekos, D., Hodsoll, J., Mantingh, T., Yalin, N., McCrone, P.,
10.1136/bmjopen-2016-015462. Boadu, J., Macritchie, K., Cella, M., Reeder, C., Fish, J., Wykes, T., Young, A.H.,
Kim, H., 2011. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta- 2021. Cognitive remediation therapy for patients with bipolar disorder: a
analysis of 74 fMRI studies. Neuroimage 54, 2446–2461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. randomised proof-of-concept trial. Bipolar Disord 23, 196–208. https://doi.org/
neuroimage.2010.09.045. 10.1111/bdi.12968.
Lewandowski, K.E., Sperry, S., Cohen, B., Norris, L., Fitzmaurice, G., Ongur, D., Taconnat, L., Baudouin, A., Fay, S., Raz, N., Bouazzaoui, B., El-Hage, W., Isingrini, M.,
Keshavan, M., 2017. Treatment to enhance cognition in bipolar disorder (TREC-BD): Ergis, A.M., 2010. Episodic memory and organizational strategy in free recall in
efficacy of a randomized controlled trial of cognitive remediation versus active unipolar depression: the role of cognitive support and executive functions. J. Clin.
control. J. Clin. Psychiatry 78, e1242–e1249. https://doi.org/10.4088/ Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 719–727. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390903512645.
JCP.17m11476. Tremont, G., Halpert, S., Javorsky, D.J., Stern, R.A., 2000. Differential impact of
Miskowiak, K.W., Burdick, K.E., Martinez-Aran, A., Bonnin, C.M., Bowie, C.R., executive dysfunction on verbal list learning and story recall. Clin. Neuropsychol.
Carvalho, A.F., Gallagher, P., Lafer, B., López-Jaramillo, C., Sumiyoshi, T., 14, 295–302. https://doi.org/10.1076/1385-4046(200008)14:3;1-P;FT295.
McIntyre, R.S., Schaffer, A., Porter, R.J., Torres, I.J., Yatham, L.N., Young, A.H., Tse, S., Chan, S., Ng, K.L., Yatham, L.N., 2014. Meta-analysis of predictors of favorable
Kessing, L.V., Vieta, E., 2017. Methodological recommendations for cognition trials employment outcomes among individuals with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord 16,
in bipolar disorder by the international society for bipolar disorders targeting 217–229. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12148.
cognition task force. Bipolar Disord 19, 614–626. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Veit, R., Lotze, M., Sewing, S., Missenhardt, H., Gaber, T., Birbaumer, N., 2010. Aberrant
bdi.12534. social and cerebral responding in a competitive reaction time paradigm in criminal
Miskowiak, Kamilla W., Carvalho, A.F., Vieta, E., Kessing, L.V., 2016a. Cognitive psychopaths. Neuroimage 49, 3365–3372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
enhancement treatments for bipolar disorder: a systematic review and neuroimage.2009.11.040.
methodological recommendations. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. https://doi.org/ Wechsler, D., 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition. (WAIS-III), San
10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.08.011. Antonio.
Miskowiak, K.W., Macoveanu, J., Vinberg, M., Assentoft, E., Randers, L., Harmer, C.J., Wing, J.K., Babor, T., Brugha, T., Burke, J., Cooper, J.E., Giel, R., Jablenski, A.,
Ehrenreich, H., Paulson, O.B., Knudsen, G.M., Siebner, H.R., Kessing, L.V., 2016b. Regier, D., Sartorius, N., 1990. SCAN. Schedules for clinical assessment in
Effects of erythropoietin on memory-relevant neurocircuitry activity and recall in neuropsychiatry. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47, 589–593. https://doi.org/10.1001/
mood disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 134. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12597. archpsyc.1990.01810180089012.
Miskowiak, K.W., Møller, A.B., Ott, C.V., 2021. Neuronal and cognitive predictors of Young, R.C., Biggs, J.T., Ziegler, V.E., Meyer, D.A., 1978. A rating scale for mania:
improved executive function following action-based cognitive remediation in reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry 133, 429–435. https://doi.org/
patients with bipolar disorder. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 47, 1–10. https://doi. 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429.
org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2021.02.013.

You might also like