You are on page 1of 11

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

Social–emotional competencies
make the grade: Predicting academic
success in early adolescence
Kim Schonert-Reichl

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

Cite this paper Downloaded from Academia.edu 

Get the citation in MLA, APA, or Chicago styles

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

Teaching Adolescent s t o Become Learners CCSR


Jenny Nagaoka, Camille Farringt on

Teaching Adolescent s To Become Learners T he Role of Noncognit ive Fact ors in Shaping School Perf…
ageet h hup

Behavioral Skills Training in Port uguese Children wit h School Failure Problems
Edgar Galindo
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

Social–emotional competencies make the grade: Predicting academic


success in early adolescence☆
Eva Oberle ⁎, Kimberly A. Schonert-Reichl, Clyde Hertzman, Bruno D. Zumbo
University of British Columbia, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The goals of this study were to longitudinally examine the association between social and emotional competence
Received 23 May 2013 (SEC) and academic achievement in early adolescence, exploring the moderating role of gender in this relation.
Received in revised form 1 February 2014 Using a short-term longitudinal design, self- and teacher-reported indicators of SEC were used to predict early
Accepted 25 February 2014
adolescents' (N = 461, Mean age = 12.02 years, SD = 0.41, 47% female) achievement in math and reading on
Available online xxxx
a standardized achievement test in grade 7. As hypothesized, teacher-reported SEC in sixth grade significantly
Keywords:
predicted higher scores in math and reading on the standardized test in seventh grade. A significant
Social and emotional competence interaction between self-reported SEC and gender in predicting reading scores indicated that SEC was a
Academic success significant positive predictor for reading outcomes in boys only. Self-reported SEC was not significantly related
Early adolescence to math achievement. Results are discussed in line with the literature and practical implications of the findings
are discussed.
© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Understanding the factors that predict academic success in adoles- time away from what is perceived to be the main goal of schooling —
cence has been a central priority for parents, educators, and societal academic competence (Malecki & Elliot, 2002). This opinion has been
agencies interested in promoting positive development and deterring widely criticized by researchers in the field of education and child devel-
school failure in young people (Aronson, 2002; Caprara, Barabaranelli, opment (e.g., DiPerna & Elliott, 2000; Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki,
Pastorelli, Bandura, & Zimbardo, 2000). In fact, academic success in Taylor, & Schellinger, 2011; Elias & Haynes, 2008; Zins, Weissberg,
school is an important indicator for overall positive development in Wang, & Walberg, 2004). Instead, based on growing evidence that
early adolescence, and sets youth on a promising academic trajectory students' SEC and academic success are interrelated, researchers have
into late adolescence as well as predicting college attendance in early urged that monitoring and ultimately fostering positive social and
adulthood (Bond et al., 2007; Eccles, Vida, & Barber, 2004; Shim, Ryan, emotional development may be key to enhancing academic growth
& Anderson, 2008). Although recent research suggests that academic (see Greenberg et al., 2003; Hawkins, Kosterman, Catalano, Hill, &
success and social and emotional aspects of development are inextrica- Abbott, 2008; Jones, Brown, & Aber, 2011; Zins et al., 2004). However,
bly linked (e.g., Elias & Haynes, 2008; Elliott, Malecki, & Demaray, 2001), for this goal to be achieved, more high quality research – including
social and emotional competence (SEC) as it relates to academic longitudinal designs that investigate the role of social and emotional
development is still underrepresented in both empirical research and aspects of development in predicting academic outcomes for specific
practice (Cohen, 2001, 2006). developmental periods – needs to be conducted (Hawkins et al., 2008;
A common public perception is that investing time into fostering Malecki & Elliot, 2002; National Research Council, 2012; Welsh, Parke,
social and emotional skills in the classroom will unnecessarily take Widaman, & O'Neil, 2001).
Although a number of previous studies have found a link between
indicators of students' SEC in the classroom and academic success
☆ This research was supported by grants from the United Way of the Lower Mainland and
(e.g., Jones et al., 2011; Malecki & Elliot, 2002; Welsh et al., 2001;
the Human Early Learning Partnership (HELP) at the University of British Columbia to the
second author. The authors wish to thank Denise Buote and Angela Jaramillo for their assis- Wentzel, 1991a, 1991b), several gaps remain in the literature. First,
tance with data collection. The authors also wish to express their thanks to the numerous past research has predominately been based on single-informant
school staff and administrators who helped make this project possible; and especially to designs, highlighting students' own perceptions of their social and
the students and their teachers for their enthusiastic participation in this project. emotional skills rather than considering multiple informants (e.g.,
⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, and
Special Education, 2125 Main Mall, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4,
Gil-Olarte Marquez, Palomera Martin, & Brackett, 2006; Izard et al.,
Canada. 2001; Jones et al., 2011; Seider, Gilbert, Novick, & Gomez, 2013). Sec-
E-mail address: Oberle.eva@gmail.com (E. Oberle). ond, a large number of previous studies have used grades assigned by

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.02.004
0193-3973/© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147 139

teachers as an indicator of academic achievement rather than more important indictors of students' social characteristics and functioning
objective achievement indices obtained via standardized tests (e.g., and may serve as a key to jointly promote social, emotional, and aca-
Elias & Haynes, 2008; Welsh et al., 2001; Wentzel, 1991a). Whereas demic competencies in young people (Greenberg et al., 2003).
grades are a measure of academic achievement that can be obtained
through teachers and school records fairly easily, their reliability Defining school-related SEC
may be limited given that grading criteria tend to differ among
teachers (McMillan, 2001, 2003). Third, little research is available Defining important characteristics of SEC throughout development,
to address whether SEC relates to academic outcomes for boys and we draw from theoretical and empirical research conducted by the Col-
girls in similar ways. laborative of Academic and Social and Emotional Learning (CASEL; see
Exploring the role of gender is critical because previous research has www.casel.org). CASEL is a leading collaborative of researchers and ed-
revealed overall gender differences in SEC among boys and girls. For in- ucators that advances theory, research, and practice in the field of social
stance, Elias and Haynes (2008) found that girls in third grade had sig- and emotional learning (SEL) and development. According to CASEL,
nificantly higher SEC scores as assessed with the Social Skills Rating SEL can be defined as “…the process of acquiring and effectively apply-
System (SSRS; Gresham & Elliott, 1990) than boys. Further research ing the knowledge, attitudes, and skills necessary to recognize and man-
has indicated that adolescent girls tend to score higher on measures of age emotions; developing caring and concern for others; making
social and emotional understanding in contrast to boys (e.g., Bosacki & responsible decisions; establishing positive relationships; and handling
Astington, 1999; Brackett, Mayer, & Warner, 2004; Jaffee & Hide, challenging situations adaptively” (Elias et al., 1997, p. 1). In the present
2000; Welsh et al., 2001), and respond more prosocially than boys to study, we use the term SEC to refer to social and emotional competen-
hypothetical conflict scenarios (Rose & Asher, 1999). Moreover, adoles- cies and skills that have been acquired in the past and can be assessed
cent girls have been rated by their peers as behaving in more socially in early adolescents. We do not focus on the process of learning and ac-
and emotionally competent ways compared to boys (Sandstrom & quiring SEC in the present study (for a review of studies conducted to
Cillessen, 2003). Similarly, a number of studies have found that girls evaluate SEL processes in intervention programs, see Durlak et al.,
score higher on self-report measures of perspective taking and empath- 2011). We consider SEC closely related to SEL as it describes the out-
ic concern than boys (Charbonneau & Nicol, 2002; see Eisenberg, Fabes, come of the processes that foster SEL.
& Spinrad, 2006, for a review). Finally, previous research has indicated In general, socially and emotionally competent children and adoles-
that teachers typically report higher levels of social competence for cents are commonly characterized as being able to understand, reflect
girls in contrast to boys (Ladd & Profilet, 1996). Taken together, it is crit- on, and manage their own emotions and behaviors, solve problems suc-
ical to explicate the role of gender in more detail when investigating the cessfully, and act appropriately in social situations at home, school, and
manner in which social and emotional skills are associated with aca- in the community (e.g., Durlak et al., 2011; Elias et al., 1997). In the
demic competence in order to gain further understanding as to how school context in particular, SEC relates to indicators of a positive learn-
SEC may function in similar or different ways for adolescent boys and ing context, such as cooperation with peers and social functioning in the
girls in predicting their academic achievement. classroom setting (January, Casey, & Paulson, 2011; Roseth, Johnson, &
Addressing the outlined limitations, the goal of the present re- Johnson, 2008; Wentzel, 1993).
search was to assess the relation between early adolescents' SEC In the present study, we used two indicators of school-related SEC:
and academic outcomes longitudinally, utilizing a multi-informant Students' self-reports of social responsibility goals, and teachers' reports
design that included self- as well as teacher-reports of SEC. Academic of students' social–emotional skills in the classroom. As suggested by
outcomes in two domains – reading and math, drawn from standard- Wentzel (1991a), socially responsible behavior reflects adherence to
ized achievement assessments – were included to provide an objec- rules and norms in the classroom, and helps create an environment
tive measure of academic success. Last, we explored differences conducive to learning and instruction. Given that goals are a powerful
between boys and girls to determine the extent to which gender motivator for behavior (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002), social responsibility
moderated the relation between SEC and academic achievement. goals – defined here as the degree to which students try to keep prom-
Our study focused on the developmental period of early adolescence ises and commitments made to peers and to follow classroom rules –
— a time during which a multitude of changes occur on social, cogni- have been considered an important indicator for students' intentions
tive, physiological levels (Steinberg, 2005; Stroud et al., 2009). It is to act in responsible, cooperative, and compliant ways that facilitate
during early adolescence that young people begin to shift their social social acceptance and promote academic learning (Wentzel, 1991b,
focus away from the family and toward the peer group and other 1994). According to Eccles and Midgely (1989), goals to behave appro-
contexts in which they develop, such as the school context (Eccles priately and responsibly are especially important during early ado-
& Roeser, 2009). Social and emotional skills are critical during this lescent development when teachers tend to spend much time with
time because they contribute to positive functioning in the school student behavior and classroom management challenges.
and classroom context, help young people to form healthy relation- Pursuing social responsibility goals requires self-regulatory and self-
ships with peers and teachers, and can thus foster positive develop- management abilities, such as setting, planning, and pursuing goals, and
mental pathways throughout adolescence (Graber & Brooks-Gunn, can therefore be assumed to relate to academic learning and, ultimately,
1996; McLaughlin & Clarke, 2010; Wentzel, 2009). learning outcomes (Wentzel, 1991a). Self-management and self-
Overall, although competencies in social and emotional domains regulation both have been considered critical markers of SEC
provide a critical foundation for early adolescents' life skills, these com- (Buckner, Mezzacappa, & Beardslee, 2009; Garner, 2010). Given that
petencies have traditionally received relatively little attention in re- self-management and self-regulation are involved in the regulation of
search and practice in education (Zins et al., 2004). Researchers today emotions as well as behaviors, they are important for forming positive
emphasize that SEC is a separate core domain that students need to suc- social interactions in the classroom as well as using effective learning
cessfully master in order to graduate from high school and succeed in strategies, and can thus be considered critical for academic achievement
life (Durlak et al., 2011). A lack of SEC has been found to be negatively (Buckner et al., 2009; Garner, 2010; Garner & Waajid, 2012; Trentacosta
related to several indicators of success, including lower connectedness & Shaw, 2009).
to school, less engagement, lower academic achievement, and a higher Social responsibility goals are based on students' intentions to act in
risk for school drop out (Appleton, Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; Elias socially responsible ways (Wentzel, 1994). However, students' own
& Haynes, 2008; Libbey, 2004; Wentzel, 1991b; Whitted, 2011; perspective of their SEC and their goals to be compliant and responsible
Zsolnai, 2002). Understanding the role of SEC in the school context is in the classroom context may differ from how their goals manifest in be-
paramount because it can provide researchers and educators with havior, and from how peers or teachers may perceive them. In fact, it is
140 E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147

possible that students are biased in reporting their social responsibility improvement in SEC throughout the year positively predicted academic
goals, describing intentions that exceed or do not meet their actual so- achievement in the end of the school year.
cial behavior. Among others, biases in self-reports of SEC could be due Further support for the importance of social–emotional skills to
to social norms within students' peer groups, and how students wish school success stems from intervention research with programs de-
others perceived them. Therefore, in addition to students' self-reports signed to teach SEC in the school setting. In fact, findings from a
of social responsibility goals, we included teachers' observations of large meta-analysis of 213 school-based, universal SEL programs in
students' social and emotional skills in the present study to provide a kindergartens, elementary schools, and high schools indicated that
further independent perspective of early adolescents' SEC that includes participation in SEL-programs significantly improved students' social,
critical behaviors in the classroom context (Gardner, Dishion, & Connell, emotional, and academic competencies (see Durlak et al., 2011). Specif-
2008; Hightower et al., 1986; Ryan, Jamison, Shin, & Thompson, 2012). ically, compared to controls, students who participated in SEL programs
Elementary school students spend a large portion of their school day demonstrated better social and emotional skills (e.g., social goal setting,
with the classroom-teacher, allowing teachers to obtain valuable interpersonal problem solving, conflict resolution), more prosocial atti-
insights with regard to students' social–emotional skills, behaviors, tudes (e.g., attitudes toward self, school, and social topics), and behavior
and characteristics across multiple situations both in- and outside of (e.g., getting along with peers and teachers), as well as an 11-percentile-
the classroom (e.g., Pepler & Craig, 1998; Ryan, Patrick, & Shim, 2005). point improvement in academic achievement (i.e., grades, GPA, or
Specifically, teachers have the opportunity to witness a wide range of achievement on standardized tests). Effects remained stable for up to
students' SEC that is critical for school success, including peer in- six months after SEL programs had completed. Overall Durlak et al.
teractions, cooperation in groups, interaction with adults, and self- (2011) are critical because they emphasize that social–emotional skills
regulatory abilities while working on classroom-specific academic and and academic achievement are inextricably linked, and that academic
non-academic tasks (e.g., Gardner et al., 2008; Oberle & Schonert- growth can be fostered through programs that target the promotion of
Reichl, 2013; Pepler & Craig, 1998; Ryan et al., 2012). Teachers' perspec- social and emotional development.
tives are therefore crucial and need to be considered when understand- In conclusion, recent research supports the contention that social
ing early adolescents' social and emotional functioning in the school and emotional competence is fundamental for positive academic devel-
setting, and how such functioning relates to further school outcomes opment (Taylor & Dymnicki, 2007). Support comes from studies
(Kersting, Givvin, Sotelo, & Stigler, 2010; Merrell, Bradley, & Karalyn, investigating the relation among social and academic aspects of devel-
2011). opment, as well as program evaluations of school-based interventions.
The existing evidence hence strongly suggests that the foundations of
The importance of SEC for school success positive academic growth include social and emotional aspects of devel-
opment (Caprara et al., 2000; Gil-Olarte Marquez et al., 2006; Seider
There is a general consensus among parents and educators in North et al., 2013). Still, although investigations of the connection between
America that teaching children how to read, write, and calculate is not the social–emotional and academic dimensions of development have
sufficient to become mature future workers, leaders, and citizens of increased over the past years, more high-quality longitudinal research
the world (Cohen, 2006; Public Agenda, 2002). In fact, Greenberg et al. is needed to further explicate the precise nature of the relation
(2003) have put forward that the broader mission of 21st century between SEC and academic success (National Research Council, 2012).
schools needs to be educating students to become academically knowl-
edgeable as well as responsible, caring, mature, and healthy members of Summary and hypotheses
society. Understanding the role of SEC in the school context, Elias and
Haynes (2008) have argued that being “fluent” in social and emotional Theory and research are in accordance with suggesting that social
understanding and competence helps children and adolescents to en- and emotional and academic growth are inextricably connected and
gage in positive relationships in the classroom, and to self-regulate that both need to promoted in educational settings to achieve school
their emotions and behaviors more effectively which consequently goals including academic competence (Durlak et al., 2011; Jones &
helps them to increase their focus on the academic curriculum. Accord- Bouffard, 2012; Taylor & Dymnicki, 2007; Zins et al., 2004). Based on a
ingly, Blum and Libbey (2004) suggest that early adolescents who lack number of empirical studies that have investigated the connection be-
SEC tend to experience more challenges in social interactions with tween social, emotional and academic aspects of development, there
teachers and peers, leading to decreased classroom connectedness is increasing evidence that earlier SEC predicts later academic success
that in turn can contribute to negative academic performance. (e.g., Caprara et al., 2000; Elias & Haynes, 2008; Hawkins et al., 2008;
A number of studies provide empirical support for the social and Izard et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2011). In addition, the scientific evaluation
emotional foundations of academic success and indicate that failure to of interventions that are aimed at enhancing social–emotional skills has
develop such competencies can result in a variety of personal, social, suggested that participation in programs that successfully enhance SEC
and academic difficulties (Durlak et al., 2011; Malecki & Elliot, 2002; also lead to improvements in academic skills and achievement (e.g.,
Weissberg & Greenberg, 1998; Welsh et al., 2001; Wentzel, 1991b). Spe- Durlak et al., 2011). Yet, more research is needed to investigate the pre-
cifically, Wentzel (1991a), in her classic study conducted with children dictive properties of SEC for academic competence in early adolescence
in middle school, found that socially responsible behaviors, such as (Malecki & Elliot, 2002; National Research Council, 2012; Welsh et al.,
sharing, cooperating, and helping others, were important positive corre- 2001), including multiple indicators of SEC and academic success.
lates of academic achievement assessed via end-of year school grade The present study was conducted longitudinally and included data
point averages (GPA), whereas problem behaviors, such as starting from three time points. We used fourth-grade academic achievement
fights and breaking rules were negatively related to success on academ- from standardized tests in reading and math as a baseline measure;
ic development tasks. In another study examining the link between so- self-reports and teacher-reports of social and emotional skills were
cial behaviors and academic outcomes, Malecki and Elliot (2002) found assessed at the end of grade 6; academic achievement in math and read-
that among third and fourth grade students, problem behaviors in the ing was assessed mid-way through grade 7 and used as the two out-
beginning of the school year were negatively related to concurrent come measures. Focusing on students' social, emotional, and academic
levels of academic achievement, whereas social–emotional skills were development as they move from sixth to seventh grade allows us to in-
positively predictive of both concurrent academic achievement and vestigate SEC in relation to achievement at a stage in which students are
achievement at the end of the school year. Finally, in a sample of transitioning out of middle childhood and entering early adolescence
urban and disadvantaged third grade students, Elias and Haynes (Eccles & Roeser, 2009). This transition is a critical time in development
(2008) found that SEC in the beginning of the school year as well as due to the myriad of changes that occur in self-awareness, as well as
E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147 141

social, cognitive, and physiological changes (Wigfield, Byrnes, & Eccles, British Columbia, and linked to participants' survey data via Personal
2006) that likely relate to students' development of SEC. Education Number (PEN; an identifier assigned to each student by the
First, we hypothesized that self-reported social responsibility goals Ministry of Education, BC) by a data steward.
and teachers' reports of students' social and emotional skills at the
end of sixth grade would significantly and positively predict the change Measures
in standardized reading and math test scores between fourth and sev-
enth grade. Second, we explored whether gender moderated the rela- Student self-reports, teacher-reports, and measures of students'
tion between each indicator of SEC, and academic outcomes in the academic achievement were the sources of data used in the present
two domains. study. Students' self-report survey included basic demographic infor-
mation and a measure of social responsibility goals. Teachers completed
Method the Teacher Child Rating Survey (T-CRS; Hightower et al., 1986) as an
indicator for students' social–emotional skills. Achievement on the FSA
Participants test in grades 4 and 7 was used as an indicator for present and previous
academic achievement.
The current study was based on a sub-sample of sixth-grade stu-
dents who were part of a larger study on the psychological and social Student-reports
well-being of children in grades 4 to 7. A stratified random sampling
procedure was employed across seven school districts located in Demographics. A demographic questionnaire was administered to each
urban and suburban areas in Western Canada. Stratification was done student to gather information about gender, birth date, grade, first
according to the neighborhood level vulnerability rates for children's language learned, and family composition.
development, as reported by the Human Early Learning Partnership
(Kershaw, Irwin, Trafford, & Hertzman, 2005; www.earlylearning.ubc. Social responsibility goals. Social responsibility goals were assessed with
ca). The vulnerability rates were determined according to the Early De- a subscale of the Social Goals Questionnaire (Wentzel, 1993). The Social
velopment Instrument (EDI; see Janus & Offord, 2007). Schools were Goals Questionnaire is comprised of two scales measuring Prosocial
randomly selected and approached to represent “high,” “medium,” Goals and Social Responsibility Goals. In the present study, the 7-item
and “low” vulnerability rates within the eight school districts in order Social Responsibility Goals subscale was used. The subscale assesses
to obtain a diverse representation of participants. The participants in how often early adolescents try to “…keep promises you have made
the present study were recruited from 33 classrooms in 20 public to other kids?,” “…be nice to other kids when something bad has hap-
schools. Eight of the grade 6 classrooms were located in middle schools, pened to them?,” and “…be quiet when other kids are trying to
the remaining classrooms were in elementary schools. Of the 33 class- study?” Students indicate their answers on a Likert-type scale ranging
rooms, 25 were combined grades, comprised of children from fifth from 1 = Never to 5 = Always, with higher scores indicating higher so-
and/or seventh grades in addition to sixth grade students. cial responsibility goals. The reliability for the social responsibility goals
Of those early adolescents invited to participate, 82% gave assent and subscale in the present study was satisfactory (α = .75).
received parental/guardian consent to participate, resulting in a total
of 461 participants (47% female) who were on average 12.02 years old Teacher-reports
(SD = 0.41). Sixty-three percent of the students reported English as
their first language; the remaining early adolescents reported Chinese Students' social–emotional skills. Teachers rated early adolescents'
(14%), Punjabi (5%), Vietnamese (3%), or another language. Regarding social–emotional skills on a subscale of the TCR-S (Hightower et al.,
family composition, 75% of the early adolescents reported living with 1986), a measure shown to be reliable and valid with elementary
a mother and a father (this includes biological as well as reconstituted school-aged children (Hightower et al., 1986). The total social–emotion-
families with stepparents). Seven percent of adolescents reported living al skills score is a composite of four subscales assessing skills in four dif-
half time with their mother and half time with their father, and the ferent domains: 1. Frustration Tolerance, 2. Assertive Social Skills, 3. Task
remaining adolescents reported living with their mother only (7%), Orientation, and 4. Peer Interaction. Each subscale includes five items.
grandparents (3%), or other family configurations. The sub-sample of Sample items for the subscales include 1. “Accepts things not going
grade 6 students was representative of the fourth to seventh grade sam- his/her way,” 2. “Expresses ideas willingly,” 3. “Functions well even
ple regarding the background variables first language learned, family with distractions,” and 4. “Is well-liked by classmates.” Items on these
composition, gender, and vulnerability level of the schools students scales were rated on a scale of 1 = Not at all to 5 = Very well, and scores
were attending. were computed by averaging the items across subscale items. Higher
scores indicated better social–emotional skills. The reliability of the sub-
Procedure scales was satisfactory in the present study, ranging from α = .92 to
α = .96. The Cronbach's alpha of the teacher-reported social–emotional
Data in the present study were obtained from three separate points skills score was .97, also indicating good satisfactory reliability. These
in time. First, students' previous Foundational Skills Assessment (FSA) psychometric properties are comparable to the ones found in previous
scores from the BC Ministry of Education's annual provincial assessment research (e.g., Cowen et al., 1997; Hightower et al., 1986; Weissberg
in grade 4 served as a baseline measure for academic achievement. Sec- et al., 1987).
ond, students completed a self-report survey, and teachers completed
student ratings of social–emotional skills at the end of grade 6. Third, Academic achievement
students completed the annual provincial FSA mid-way through Early adolescents' scores on standardized provincial achievement
seventh grade. Regarding the survey data collection procedure, early ad- tests in fourth and seventh grade (Ministry of Education British
olescents were told that the study was a survey about students' experi- Columbia, 2011) were used as indicators for previous and present aca-
ences within and outside of school. After obtaining parent consent and demic achievement. The FSA examinations in fourth and seventh
student assent, research assistants administered the survey to the stu- grade are annual, province-wide assessments of students' foundational
dents during two 45-minute sessions. All items were read out loud to academic skills in reading, writing, and mathematics. The assessments
students. Teachers were asked to complete a teacher-report question- test cumulative skills that students are expected to have learned by a
naire for each participating student. FSA scores for fourth and seventh certain grade level, and were designed to inform schools, districts, and
grade students were obtained through the Ministry of Education in the province about students' academic learning progress at two crucial
142 E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147

times in development (Lloyd & Hertzman, 2009; Ministry of Education thermore, intercorrelations between the sub-scales of the TCRS and
British Columbia, 2011). According to the Ministry (2011), all students grade 7 math and reading scores are displayed in Table 2. As can be
in grades 4 and 7 are expected to take part in the FSA exam; however, seen, all of the four subscales were significantly and positively related
parents may withhold their children from participating in the assess- to both achievement scores. Given that the demographic variables gen-
ment, and entire schools may opt out of participation. der and ESL were significantly correlated with the outcome variables
Of the 461 participants who completed the self-reports for the they were included as control variables in subsequent analyses.
present study and for whom teacher-ratings on social–emotional skills Gender differences were found in both social and academic compe-
were obtained, 389 also completed the 7th-grade FSA numeracy assess- tence. Teachers reported significantly higher scores on SEC for girls,
ment, and 399 completed the 7th-grade FSA reading assessment in the M = 3.56, SD = 0.85, than for boys, M = 3.34, SD = 0.94, t(450) =
following year.1 Furthermore, a total of 398 had previously completed 2.12, p b .05. However, the magnitude of the effect was small (d =
the 4th-grade FSA numeracy assessment, and 406 had completed the .25). In addition, it needs to be noted that both gender groups were
4th-grade FSA reading assessment. In both grade 4 and grade 7 assess- rated above average on social and emotional skills by their teachers.
ments, students completed a computerized test version of 35–40 read- Both groups scored on average between 3 and 4 on the TCSR, meaning
ing comprehension questions, and 40 numeracy questions assessing that the average boy and girl were rated as displaying different social
math skills in a multiple-choice format (Ministry of Education British and emotional skills “sometimes” to “often”. Furthermore, girls (M =
Columbia, 2011). All data were provided to us as z-standardized raw 4.09, SD = 0.55) compared to boys (M = 3.93, SD = 0.59) rated them-
scores by the Ministry of Education (see Table 1). selves significantly higher on the social responsibility goals measure,
t(459) = 2.98, p b .01, with small to medium effect (d = .28). However,
again, both gender groups' ratings were in the range of “often” having
Results
the goal to be socially responsible. On the grade 7 math test, girls,
M = 0.03, SD = 0.90, received significantly lower scores than boys,
Data analytic procedure
M = 0.41, SD = 1.01, t(387) = 3.94, p b .001; the magnitude of the
effect was small to medium (d = .40). On the grade 4 math test, girls,
First, preliminary analyses were conducted to test whether the as-
M = .20, SD = .97, received marginally significantly lower scores than
sumptions for linear regression analysis were met, to explore intercor-
boys, M = .39, SD = .99, t(398) = 1.96, p b .1; the magnitude of the ef-
relations among all variables, and to investigate gender differences for
fect was small (d = .19). There were no significant differences for boys
the predictor and outcome variables. Cohen's d is presented as an indi-
(M = −.14, SD = .96) and girls (M = −.01, SD = .96) on the grade 7
cator for effect size where appropriate. Second, two hierarchical linear
reading test, t(397) = 1.26, ns. Similarly girls (M = .20, SD = .86) and
regression analyses were conducted to investigate whether self- and
boys (M = .04, SD = .94) did not score differently on the grade 4 reading
teacher reported indicators of SEC predict grade 7 reading comprehen-
test, t(394) = 1.72, ns.
sion scores and math scores. Control variables (ESL, gender) were en-
tered in Block 1, previous academic achievement on the grade 4
reading or math test was entered in Block 2, self-reported social respon- Predicting grade 7 reading scores
sibility goals were entered in Block 3, teacher rated social–emotional
skills were entered in Block 4, and last the interaction between social re- A hierarchical linear regression analysis was conducted to investi-
sponsibility goals and gender was entered in Block 5. The interaction gate whether perceived social responsibility goals and teacher-rated so-
was composed as the multiplicative product of the two variables. Signif- cial–emotional skills predicted reading comprehension scores in grade
icant interactions were analyzed with simple slopes analysis (Aiken & 7, controlling for grade 4 reading, gender and ESL (see Table 3). Model
West, 1991) and interpreted based on graphical illustrations. The signif- 1, including gender and ESL, was not statistically significant, adjusted
icance of the change in variance explained for each subsequent Block R2 b .001, F(2, 347) = 1.014, ns; none of the control variables were sig-
was calculated. All variables in the analysis were mean centered except nificant. Adding grade 4 reading scores, Model 2 was statistically signif-
the academic achievement scores that were z-standardized variables. icant, adjusted R2 = .369, F(3, 346) = 69.128, p b .001, and explained
Semipartial correlation coefficients are presented for the SEC variables 37.5% of the variance in grade 7 reading scores. Grade 4 reading was a
to indicate the unique contribution of a predictor to the outcome (see positive and statistically significant predictor in the Model.
Tabachnik & Fidell, 2007). Entering social responsibility goals in the next step, Model 3 ex-
plained 39% of the variance in grade 7 reading scores, adjusted R2 =
Preliminary analyses .383, F(4, 345) = 55.117, p b .001. The change in explained variance be-
tween Models 2 and 3 was statistically significant, R2 change = .015,
Preliminary analyses indicated that the assumptions for regression Fchange(1, 345) = 8.555, p b .001. In particular, results indicated that
analysis were not violated. Intercorrelations among the variables are an increase of social responsibility by 1 was associated with an increase
displayed in Table 1. Students' social responsibility goals and their SEC of reading achievement by .302. Table 3 shows that both grade 4 reading
as reported by teachers were both positively and significantly related scores and social responsibility goals significantly and positively
to grade 4 and grade 7 reading scores, and grade 7 math scores. The predicted grade 7 reading scores in Model 3. Adding teacher-rated
magnitude of the correlations was small to moderate. Furthermore, so- social–emotional skills in the next step, Model 4 explained 41% of the
cial responsibility goals and teacher-rated social–emotional skills were variance in grade 7 reading scores, adjusted R2 = .398, F(5, 344) =
significantly and positively related to gender; girls tended to score 47.240, p b .001.2 Again, the change in explained variance between
higher than boys. Gender was also significantly and negatively related Models 3 and 4 was statistically significant, R2 change = .017,
to grade 7 math scores; boys tended to have higher scores than girls. Fchange(1, 344) = 9.988, p b .001. An increase of teacher-rated so-
All the academic achievement variables in grades 4 and 7 were positive- cial–emotional skills by 1 was associated with an increase of reading
ly and significantly related. The magnitude of the relation was high. Fur-

2
Further analyses were also conducted to investigate which of the 4 TCRS subscales are
predictors of grade 7 reading. Four separate regression analyses (identical to the ones pre-
sented in this study) were conducted in which the TCRS composite was replaced with one
1
Students who participated in the FSAs were comparable to students who withheld TCRS subscale at a time. Results indicated that all 4 subscales significantly predicted read-
participation regarding the variables ESL, age, gender, family composition, and vulnerabil- ing outcomes, with Task Orientation and Frustration Tolerance being the strongest
ity level of the school attended. predictors.
E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147 143

Table 1
Means, SDs, and Pearson product–moment correlations among all variables.

Mean (SD) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7.

1. Gendera –
2. ESL −.043 –
3. Social responsibility goals 4.00 (.58) .138⁎⁎ −.018 –
4. Teacher-rated social–emotional skills 3.44 (.90) .117⁎ −.039 .246⁎⁎⁎ –
5. Gr. 4 math −.30 (.98) −.098 −.023 .088 .265⁎⁎⁎ –
6. Gr. 4 reading −.11 (.90) .086 −.126⁎ .177⁎⁎⁎ .348⁎⁎⁎ .655⁎⁎⁎ –
7. Gr. 7 math −.24 (.98) −.197⁎⁎⁎ .198⁎⁎⁎ .116⁎ .231⁎⁎⁎ .614⁎⁎⁎ .392⁎⁎⁎ –
8. Gr. 7 reading −.08 (.96) .063 .003 .224⁎⁎⁎ .374⁎⁎⁎ .526⁎⁎⁎ .603⁎⁎⁎ .571⁎⁎⁎
a
0 = English, 1= ESL; b 0 = male, 1 = female.
⁎ p b .05; ⁎⁎ p b .01; ⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

Table 2 p b .001). For girls, there was no significant association between social
Pearson product–moment correlations between TCRS subscales and grade 7 reading and responsibility goals and reading comprehension (slope lines was b =
math achievement.
−.023, t = −.23, ns).
TCRS subscale Grade 7 reading Grade 7 math

Frustration tolerance .305⁎⁎ .202⁎⁎


Assertive social skills .320⁎⁎ .172⁎ Predicting grade 7 math scores
Task orientation .405⁎⁎ .302⁎⁎
Peer social skills .248⁎⁎ .108⁎ Similarly, to understand whether perceived social responsibility
⁎ p b .05. goals and teacher-rated social–emotional skills predicted math scores
⁎⁎ p b .01. in grade 7, a hierarchical linear regression analysis was conducted
using the same predictive model as reported for reading comprehen-
sion. Model 1, including gender and ESL, was statistically significant,ad-
achievement by .152.3 Last, entering the interaction between gender justed R2 = .062, F(2, 339) = 12.249, p b .001; the model explained 6%
and social responsibility goals in the classroom, the full Model explained of the variance in grade 7 math scores. Gender was statistically signifi-
42% of the variance in grade 7 reading scores adjusted R2 = .407, F(6, cant with boys achieving higher math scores than girls on average,
343) = 40.857, p b .001. The interaction term significantly added to and ESL was statistically significant with students whose first language
the explained variance between Models 4 and 5, R2 change = .010, was not English achieving higher scores than native speakers on
Fchange(1, 343) = 5.170, p b .001.4 The interaction term was negatively average in grade 7. Adding grade 4 math scores in the next step,
statistically significant. Grade 4 reading scores, social responsibility Model 2 was statistically significant, adjusted R2 = .260, F(3, 338) =
goals, and teacher-rated social–emotional skills also remained as signif- 41.001, p b .001, and explained 27% of the variance in grade 7 math
icant and positive predictors for grade 7 reading scores in the full Model. scores. Grade 4 reading was a positive statistically significant predictor
The semipartial correlation coefficients indicated the unique contribu- in the Model, and the predictive pattern of the control variables
tion of social responsibility to reading outcomes was 2% (sr = .132), remained the same as in the previous model. Adding grade 4 math
and the unique contribution of social–emotional skills was 2% (sr = scores led to a significant change in the explained variance, R2 change
.129) in the full model. Follow-up analyses were conducted next to un- = .199, Fchange(1, 338) = 91.943, p b .001.
derstand the nature of the interaction term. Entering social responsibility goals in the next step, the explained var-
iance in grade 7 math scores did not change significantly, R2 change =
.003, Fchange(1, 337) = 1.396, ns. Adding teacher-rated social–
Simple slopes analysis
emotional skills in the next step, Model 4 explained 28% of the variance
Post-hoc analyses of the significant interaction term were conducted
in grade 7 math scores, adjusted R2 = .268, F(5, 336) = 17.188, p b .001.
according to the guidelines of Aiken and West (1991) to further decom-
The change in explained variance between Models 3 and 4 was statisti-
pose the significant interaction between gender and social responsibili-
cally significant, R2 change = .009, Fchange(1, 336) = 4.197, p b .05.5
ty goals. First, reading comprehension at high and low levels of social
Teacher-rated social–emotional skills significantly and positively
responsibility goals was plotted separately for boys and girls. Next, sim-
predicted grade 7 math scores.6 In particular, an increase in social–
ple slopes analyses were conducted to determine whether the
emotional skills by 1 was associated with an increase in math
slopes of the plotted regression lines were significantly different from
achievement scores by .113. Grade 4 math scores and the control
zero (Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006). Fig. 1 and Table 3 indicate that
variables remained statistically significant with the same predictive
the association between social responsibility goals in grade 6 and read-
pattern as in the previous models.7 The semipartial correlation
ing comprehension in grade 7 differed for boys and girls. Specifically, a
positive significant relation between grade 6 social responsibility goals
5
and reading comprehension in grade 7 was found for boys (slope line Similarly, if teacher-rated social–emotional skills were entered in Block 3 in-
stead of Block 4 (i.e., before controlling for social-responsibility goals), they added
was statistically significantly different from zero; b = .302, t = 3.376,
a significant change in explained variance of 1.1% in grade 7 math scores,
Fchange(1, 337) = 5.116, p b .05. The overall pattern of the results remained the
3
Similarly, if teacher-rated social–emotional skills were entered in Block 3 instead of same when subsequently entering social responsibility goals in Block 4; social re-
Block 4 (i.e., before controlling for social-responsibility goals), they added a significant sponsibility goals did not contribute significantly to the variance explained in
change in explained variance of 2.4% in grade 7 reading scores, Fchange(1, 345) = grade 7 math scores.
6
13.572, p b .001. The overall pattern of the results remained the same when subsequently Four further analyses were conducted substituting the overall TCRS composite with
entering social responsibility goals in Block 4; social responsibility goals contribute a sig- each of the four TCRS subscales. Results indicated that Task Orientation followed by Frus-
nificant amount of 1% to the variance explained in grade 7 reading scores, Fchange(1, tration Tolerance were significant predictors of math outcomes among the four subscales.
7
344) = 5.035, p b .05. Neither the interaction term between gender and social responsibility nor the interac-
4
The interaction term between gender and teacher-rated social–emotional skills was tion term between gender and teacher-rated social–emotional skills were statistically sig-
not statistically significant and were therefore not included in the model. nificant and were therefore not included in the model.
144 E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147

Table 3
Hierarchical linear regression predicting grade 7 reading comprehension and math scores.

Block Variable name Grade 7 reading Grade 7 math

B SE β t B SE β t

1 ESLa
.044 .107 .022 .413 .313 .105 .156 2.972⁎⁎
Gender .139 .101 .074 1.377 −.387 .100 −.203 −3.878⁎⁎⁎
2 ESL .196 .086 .098 2.288⁎ .435 .094 .216 4.600⁎⁎⁎
Gender .030 .081 .016 .376 −.487 .090 −.256 −5.424⁎⁎⁎
Gr. 4 reading/math .671 .047 .615 14.289⁎⁎⁎ .508 .053 .453 9.588⁎⁎⁎
3 ESL .193 .085 .096 2.374⁎ .431 .094 .215 4.563⁎⁎⁎
Gender .003 .080 .001 .033 −.487 .090 −.256 −5.424⁎⁎⁎
Gr. 4 reading/math .645 .047 .592 13.666⁎⁎⁎ .495 .054 .442 9.162⁎⁎⁎
Self-reported social responsibility goals .204 .070 .126 2.925⁎⁎ .093 .078 .057 1.182
4 ESL .191 .084 .096 2.285⁎ .431 .094 .214 4.583⁎⁎⁎
Gender −.023 .080 −.012 −.290 −.505 .090 −.265 −5.624⁎⁎⁎
Gr. 4 reading/math .597 .049 .547 12.152⁎⁎⁎ .462 .056 .412 8.212⁎⁎⁎
Self-reported social responsibility goals .158 .070 .098 2.244⁎ .056 .080 .034 .705
Teacher-rated social–emotional skills .155 .049 .145 3.160⁎⁎ .113 .055 .104 2.049⁎
5 ESL .199 .083 .099 2.391⁎
Gender −.016 .079 −.008 −.198
Gr. 4 reading/math .590 .049 .541 12.068⁎⁎⁎
Self-reported social responsibility goals .302 .092 .187 3.272⁎⁎
Teacher-rated social–emotional skills .152 .049 .142 3.131⁎⁎
Gender × self-reported social responsibility goals −.325 .136 −.132 −2.390⁎
a
0 = English, 1 = ESL; b 0 = male, 1 = female; c In analyses where grade 7 reading comprehension was the outcome, grade 4 reading comprehension scores were entered as a control
variable; in analyses where grade 7 math achievement scores were the outcome, grade 4 math scores were entered as a control variable.
⁎ p b .05. ⁎⁎ p b .01. ⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.

coefficient indicated that the unique contribution of social–emotion- Jaffee & Hide, 2000; Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2003), a further aim of the
al skills was 1% (sr = .094). present study was to explore whether differences exist for boys and
girls when predicting academic success from SEC. In the following par-
Discussion agraphs, the findings will be discussed in line with the existing litera-
ture as well as considering their practical relevance. Given that the
Past theory and research have suggested that social–emotional and findings in the present study differed for the two academic subjects of
academic competencies are inextricably interconnected (e.g., Durlak investigation, we will first discuss the findings for predicting reading
et al., 2011; Greenberg et al., 2003). Given that the research examining outcomes, followed by math outcomes. Last, we will consider limita-
links between social and emotional learning and academic achievement tions of the study, and provide suggestions for future research.
is still in a nascent stage, researchers have posited a need for more large-
scale empirical research to better understand the relation between so- SEC and reading comprehension
cial and emotional skills and academic success (e.g., Hawkins et al.,
2008; Malecki & Elliot, 2002; National Research Council, 2012). The As hypothesized and in accordance with previous findings (e.g., Elias
main goal of the present study was to investigate whether self- and & Haynes, 2008; Malecki & Elliot, 2002; Welsh et al., 2001), the present
teacher-reported SECs in early adolescence play a significant role in fu- study revealed that reading achievement in grade 7 could be indepen-
ture academic achievement in the domains of math and reading. More- dently predicted from early adolescents' perceptions of their own social
over, given that previous research has indicated gender differences in responsibility goals, and from teachers' reports of students' SEC at the
social and emotional, as well as the academic domains (e.g., Bosacki & end of grade 6, controlling for prior reading scores in grade 4. Although
Astington, 1999; Brackett et al., 2004; Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990; our analyses revealed that previous achievement in reading accounted
for the largest amount of variance in grade 7 reading scores, we found
that teacher and student reported SEC jointly explained an additional
1
4% of variability in reading scores. This is important because it adds to
Reading Comprehension Grade 7

the emerging evidence that has established positive connections be-


0.5 tween social–emotional competence and academic success (Caprara
et al., 2000; Hawkins et al., 2008; Izard et al., 2001; Seider et al.,
2013). Further analyses that took into account the role of gender, indi-
0
cated a significant interaction between social responsibility goals and
Boys
gender when predicting change in reading scores. Specifically, social re-
-0.5 Girls sponsibility goals significantly predicted an increase in reading achieve-
ment in boys, but not in girls.
Differences in the way social responsibility goals predicted reading
-1
outcomes for boys and girls in our study can be understood by consider-
ing gender differences in overall social competencies. In fact, previous
-1.5 research with early adolescents and young adults has indicated that,
Low High on average, girls tend to score higher on measures of social and emo-
Social Responsibility Goals tional understanding, and they also tend to report a stronger orientation
toward behaving in prosocial ways than boys (Bosacki & Astington,
Fig. 1. Interaction effect of gender × social responsibility goals on grade 7 reading. 1999; Brackett et al., 2004; Jaffee & Hide, 2000; Rose & Asher, 1999). A
E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147 145

similar finding emerged in the present study, with girls reporting signif- ways. Hence, the self-report measure reflects early adolescents' percep-
icantly higher social responsibility goals on average than boys, and the tions of their own social goals and intentions, rather than observed be-
variability in those scores was lower for girls than for boys. Despite haviors in the school setting. It is possible that students' goals and
this statistically significant difference, it needs to be noted – with re- intentions to be socially responsible only partially translate into actual
spect to practical importance – that for both genders, social responsibil- behaviors, and that social responsibility goals are therefore an overall
ity scores were similarly in the higher range of socially responsibility weaker predictor for math achievement than teacher-rated social–emo-
goals. It is possible that the higher cohesion in girls' social responsibility tional skills which presents an aggregation of several indicators and re-
goals and greater variability in boys' social responsibility goals contrib- flects a range of social–emotional skills (Merrell et al., 2011; Ryan et al.,
uted to finding a positive relation between social responsibility goals 2005). Last, the teacher-rated social–emotional skills measure captured
and reading achievement in boys only. skills that are more learning-related than the social responsibility goals
In contrast to the predictive pattern for student's social responsibil- measure. It has been argued that learning-related social–emotional
ity goals, the significant role of teacher-rated social–emotional skills did skills, acting responsibly in group work and staying on task are especial-
not differ for boys and girls. In accordance with previous research in the ly conducive to learning and may therefore present a stronger predictor
field, this finding indicates that teachers' reports of students' social– for academic outcomes (Cooper & Farran, 1991; McCleland, Morrison, &
emotional skills, and behaviors in the school context are positively relat- Holmes, 2000).
ed to indicators of positive adjustment and achievement in the class- The present study also revealed overall gender differences in math
room (Gardner et al., 2008; Kersting et al., 2010; Merrell et al., 2011; achievement, with boys performing significantly better than girls on av-
Ryan et al., 2005, 2012). This finding is important because it indicates erage on the grade 7 standardized math test. This finding aligns with
that academic success in reading can be predicted from teachers' obser- previous research, indicating small differences in math achievement fa-
vations of a broad range of skills and behaviors in classroom and school- voring boys with the onset of adolescence, typically found to emerge in
specific social and emotional domains (Hightower et al., 1986). the late elementary or early high-school years (Guiso, Monte, Sapienza,
Finally, the two assessments of SEC in the present study captured & Zingales, 2008; Hyde et al., 1990). Similar to the studies reported by
critical perspectives (i.e., student, teacher) as well as different domains Hyde et al. (1990) in their meta-analytic review, the size of the gender
(i.e., goals/intentions, observed behaviors/skills) representing social difference effect for math achievement was small to medium in the
and emotional development in early adolescents. Combing measures present study. It is also noteworthy to mention that in contrast to
that assess students' perspectives in addition to teachers' perspectives math achievement in grade 7, the gender differences for math achieve-
is important because they reflect multiple critical voices when reporting ment in grade 4 were only marginal in the present study, with the same
relevant indicators for SEC in early adolescence. Furthermore, the multi- trend toward favoring boys. Again, this finding is in line with previous
informant assessment used in the present study also provided an oppor- research indicating that differences in math achievement seem to be
tunity to reflect students' intentions in addition to teachers' observed less apparent in the earlier elementary school years and become larger
actions reflecting students' SEC, and to investigate how far they are con- throughout the later elementary and early high school years (Hyde
sistent in predicting academic reading achievement. et al., 1990). This developmental trend is thought to occur due to a con-
tinuous decline in girls' competence-beliefs and values in math
SEC and math achievement throughout elementary and high school compared to boys (Jacobs,
Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002; Skaalvik & Skaalvik, 2004);
Similar to the findings for reading outcomes, and in line with previ- this decline in turn has been argued to be a result of gender-role stereo-
ous research, SEC in grade 6 played a significant role when predicting typic socialization (Jacobs, 1991; Ruble & Martin, 1998).
math achievement in grade 7, after controlling for previous achieve-
ment (e.g., Hawkins et al., 2008; Malecki & Elliot, 2002; Welsh et al., Practical contribution
2001). Previous achievement scores on the grade 4 math test explained
the largest portion of variance in grade 7 math achievement. SEC signif- The present study provides further evidence for the importance of
icantly predicted math achievement by explaining an additional 1% in SEC in predicting academic achievement in the two domains of reading
variance in math scores. In contrast to our findings for reading out- and math (Caprara et al., 2000; DiPerna & Elliott, 2000; Durlak et al.,
comes, only teacher-ratings of students' social–emotional skills 2011; Elias & Haynes, 2008; Izard et al., 2001; Malecki & Elliot, 2002;
emerged as a significant predictor when forecasting changes in math Welsh et al., 2001). Specifically, they indicate that both social goals in
scores; social responsibility goals were not significantly related to the classroom as well as behaviors that indicate SEC need to be consid-
changes in math achievement. Gender significantly predicted math ered when examining academic success in reading and math. This find-
achievement in grade 7, with boys scoring higher on the math test ing can inform the design of classroom interventions in that it identifies
than girls; there were no interactions with gender when predicting areas of social and emotional development that can be targeted in SEL
math from SEC indicators. Both findings will be discussed in the order programs, and that significantly predict academic outcomes. Further-
they are presented here. more, in accordance with previous recommendations (see National
Considering explanations for why teacher-ratings of students' so- Research Council, 2012), the different relational patterns for social re-
cial–emotional skills, but not students' social responsibility goals signif- sponsibility goals compared to teacher-rated social–emotional skills in
icantly predicted math achievement, the nature of the two SEC predicting academic achievement suggest distinguishing among indi-
measures used in this study needs to be discussed. The teacher measure vidual indicators of SEC, and taking into account multiple perspectives
(see Hightower et al., 1986) captured four different domains of SEC (i.e., when assessing and promoting social–emotional skills in educational
assertive social skills, frustration tolerance, peer interactions, task orien- contexts.
tation). All four domains have been significantly and positively related The benefits of SEL promotion have been established in previous
to academic achievement on standardized tests in the past (Trickett, meta-analytic research. Durlak et al. (2011) found that promoting SEL
McBride-Chang, & Putman, 1994). Reporting on four different SEC do- through universal interventions boosts academic outcomes by 11 per-
mains, it can be reasoned that this measure reflects a broad range of so- centile points, increases motivation to learn and cooperative behavior
cial–emotional skills and behaviors observed by teachers in and outside in the classroom, decreases disruptive behavior and non-compliance,
the classroom setting that are relevant to positive learning processes and decreases emotional distress. Hence, SEL promotion benefits not
and academic success in all students. In comparison, the self-reported only academic achievement, but also the well-being and positive devel-
social responsibility goals measure (Wentzel, 1993) only captured one opment of “the whole student.” Considering the cost–benefit ratio of
SEC domain — the intention to act in socially responsible and compliant SEL promotion, Zins et al. (2004) note that in effective SEL programs,
146 E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147

the benefits have been found to outweigh the costs, with benefits in- Brackett, M. A., Mayer, J. D., & Warner, R. M. (2004). Emotional intelligence and its relation
to everyday behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1387–1402.
cluding improved educational outcomes, lower substance use, reduced Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. R. (2009). Self-regulation and its relations
crime, and decreased mental health problems. Overall, identifying im- to adaptive functioning in low income youths. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79,
portant indicators of social success that predict academic achievement 19–30.
Caprara, C. V., Barabaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Bandura, A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2000).
in early adolescence, the findings in the present study inform the devel- Prosocial foundations of children's academic achievement. Psychological Science, 11,
opment of potential intervention programs aimed to foster social goals 302–306.
and skills that may in turn enhance positive academic growth. Charbonneau, D., & Nicol, A. A. M. (2002). Emotional intelligence and leadership in ado-
lescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1101–1113.
Cohen, J. (2001). Social emotional education: Core principles and practices. In J. Cohen
(Ed.), Caring classrooms/intelligent schools: The social emotional education of young
Limitations and future directions children (pp. 3–29). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Cohen, J. (2006). Social, emotional, ethical, and academic education: Creating a climate for
A number of limitations need to be considered when interpreting the learning, participation in democracy, and well-being. Harvard Educational Review, 76,
201–237.
results of the present study. First, although the present study predicted Cooper, D. H., & Farran, D. C. (1991). The Cooper–Farran Behavioral Rating Scales. Brandon,
academic achievement longitudinally, self-reports and teachers' reports VT: Clinical Psychology Publishing Co.
of social–emotional skills were only provided at one time point — at the Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A., Work, W. C., Kim, J. Y., Fagen, D. B., & Magnus, K. B. (1997).
Follow-up study of young stress-affected and stress-resilient urban children.
end of the school year in grade 6. As discussed by Welsh et al. (2001), Development and Psychopathology, 9, 565–577.
studies investigating the relation between social, emotional, and aca- DiPerna, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (2000). The academic competence evaluation scales. San
demic success should ideally obtain indicators of SEC and academic Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The
achievement at several points in the longitudinal data collection in impact of enhancing students' social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of
order to test a circular model in which present SEC may predict future school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82, 405–432.
academic achievement and present academic achievement may also Eccles, J. S., & Midgely, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit: Developmentally appropriate
classrooms for young adolescents. In C. Ames, & R. Ames (Eds.), Research on
predict future SEC. Second, the present study was conducted with
motivation in education, Vol. 3. (pp. 139–186). New York, NY: Academic Press.
early adolescents from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds. More re- Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2009). Schools, academic motivation, and stage-environment
search is needed to understand whether the present findings replicate in fit. In R. M. Lerner, & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp.
specific sub-groups of the population, for instance affluent or socioeco- 404–434) (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Eccles, J. S., Vida, M. N., & Barber, B. (2004). The relation of early adolescents' college plans
nomically disadvantaged early adolescents in particular. Future research and both academic ability and task-value beliefs to subsequent college enrollment.
also needs to investigate the role of variables that may influence SEC at a Journal of Early Adolescence, 24, 63–77.
school or classroom level, such as teachers' experiences with and atti- Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 109–132.
tudes toward SEC promotion, teacher mobility, and size of the school. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. (2006). Prosocial development. In N. Eisenberg
Furthermore, the study of SEC predicting academic success also needs (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development
to be conducted across several stages of development to allow identify- (pp. 646–718). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Elias, M. J., & Haynes, M. N. (2008). Social competence, social support, and academic
ing trajectories in social, emotional, and academic development. Last, fu- achievement in minority, low-income, urban elementary school children. School
ture research needs to be conducted to further replicate and further Psychology Quarterly, 23, 474–495.
investigate gender differences in SEC related to academic achievement. Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Frey, K. S., Greenberg, M. T., Haynes, N. M., et al.
(1997). Promoting social and emotional learning: Guidelines for educators. Alexandria,
In conclusion, the present study provides an important contribution VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
to the growing field of developmental science that connects social, emo- Elliott, S. N., Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2001). New directions in social skills assess-
tional, and academic growth. Specifically, the present findings provide ment and intervention for elementary and middle school students. Exceptionality, 9,
19–32.
empirical evidence that social and emotional development plays a sig-
Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. (2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resil-
nificant role in predicting academic success, suggesting that advances ience: Resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. Journal of
in SEC may be a way to promote academic growth. These findings are Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 273–284.
important because they highlight the contention that the focus of Garner, P. W. (2010). Emotional competence and its influences on teaching and learning.
Educational Psychology Review, 22, 297–321.
schooling needs to remain on “the whole child” rather than exclusively Garner, P. W., & Waajid, B. (2012). Emotion knowledge and self-regulation as predictors
on teaching academic subjects (DiPerna & Elliott, 2000; Elias & Haynes, of preschoolers' cognitive ability, classroom behavior, and social competence. Journal
2008). Both research and practice need to focus on integrating academic of Psychoeducational Assessment, 30, 330–343.
Gil-Olarte Marquez, P., Palomera Martin, R., & Brackett, M. A. (2006). Relating emotional
and social–emotional development rather than viewing them as two intelligence to social competence and academic achievement in high school students.
separate domains. Adding to the scientific evidence that views SEC Psychothema, 18, 118–223.
and academic achievement as closely interrelated, the present study Graber, J. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Transitions and turning points: Navigating the pas-
sage from childhood through adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 768–776.
suggests that SEC needs to be a focus of early adolescent development Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., O'Brien, M. U., Zins, J. E., Fredericks, L., Resnik, H., et al.
in both research and practice, and that fostering and promoting social (2003). Enhancing school-based prevention and youth development through coordi-
and emotional growth may be a crucial way to raise responsible and nated social, emotional, and academic learning. American Psychologist, 58, 466–474.
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System manual. Circle Pines, MN:
competent learners and citizens in society (Greenberg et al., 2003). American Guidance Service.
Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science,
320, 1164–1165.
References Hawkins, J. D., Kosterman, R., Catalano, R. F., Hill, K. G., & Abbott, R. D. (2008). Effects of
social development intervention in childhood 15 years later. Archives of Pediatrics
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. and Adolescent Medicine, 162, 1133–1141.
Newbury Park, London, England: Sage. Hightower, A. D., Work, W. C., Cowen, E. L., Lotyczewski, B. S., Spinell, A. P., Guare, J. C.,
Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with school: et al. (1986). The teacher–child rating scale: A brief objective measure of elementary
Critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology in the children's school problem behaviors and competencies. School Psychology Review, 16,
Schools, 45, 369–386. 239–255.
Aronson, J. (2002). Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on ed- Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics perfor-
ucation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. mance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155.
Blum, R. W., & Libbey, H. P. (2004). School connectedness—strengthening health and ed- Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Youngstorm, E. (2001). Emotion
ucation outcomes for teenagers. Journal of School Health, 74, 229–299. knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at
Bond, L., Butler, H., Thomas, L., Carli, J., Glover, S., Bowes, G., et al. (2007). Social and school risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18–23.
connectedness in early secondary school as predictor of late teenage substance use, Jacobs, J. E. (1991). The influence of gender stereotypes on parent and child math attitudes:
mental health, and academic outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, Differences across grade−levels. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 518–527.
357e9–357e18. Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in
Bosacki, S., & Astington, J. W. (1999). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations be- children's self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades
tween social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8, 237–255. one through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509–527.
E. Oberle et al. / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 35 (2014) 138–147 147

Jaffee, S., & Hide, S. J. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-analysis. (Eds.), Peer relationships and adjustment at school (pp. 165–186). Charlotte, NC: Infor-
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 703–726. mation Age Publishing.
January, A. M., Casey, R. J., & Paulson, D. (2011). A meta-analysis of classroom-wide inter- Ryan, A. M., Patrick, H., & Shim, S. O. (2005). Differential profiles of students identified by
ventions to build social skills: Do they work? School Psychology Review, 40, 242–256. their teacher as having avoidant, appropriate, or dependent help-seeking tendencies
Janus, M., & Offord, D. (2007). Development and psychometric properties of the early de- in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 275–285.
velopment instrument (EDI): A measure of children's school readiness. Canadian Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2003). Sociometric status and children's peer expe-
Journal of Behavioural Science, 39, 1–22. riences: Use of the daily diary method. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 427–452.
Jones, S. M., & Bouffard, S. M. (2012). Social and emotional learning in schools: From pro- Seider, S., Gilbert, J., Novick, S., & Gomez, J. (2013). The role of moral and performance
grams to strategies. Social Policy Report, 4, 1–33. character strengths in predicting achievement and conduct among urban middle
Jones, S. M., Brown, J. L., & Aber, J. L. (2011). Two-year impacts of a universal school-based school students. Teachers College Record, 115, 1.18.
social–emotional and literacy intervention: An experiment in translational develop- Shim, S. S., Ryan, A. M., & Anderson, C. J. (2008). Achievement goals and achievement dur-
mental research. Child Development, 82, 533–554. ing early adolescence: Examining time-varying predictor and outcome variables in
Kershaw, P., Irwin, L., Trafford, K., & Hertzman, C. (2005). (1st ed.)The British Columbia atlas growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 655–671.
of child development, Vol. 40, Vancouver, BC: Human Early Learning Partnership/ Skaalvik, S., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2004). Gender differences in math and verbal self-concept,
Western Geographical Press. performance expectations, and motivation. Sex Roles, 50, 241–252.
Kersting, N. B., Givvin, K. B., Sotelo, F. L., & Stigler, J. W. (2010). Teachers' analyses of class- Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in
room video predict student learning of mathematics: Further explorations of a novel Cognitive Science, 9, 69–74.
measure of teacher knowledge. Journal of Teacher Education, 61, 172–181. Stroud, L. R., Foster, E., Papandonatos, G. D., Handwerger, K., Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T.,
Ladd, G. W., & Profilet, S. M. (1996). The child behavior scale: A teacher-report measure of et al. (2009). Stress response and the adolescent transition: Performance versus peer
young children's aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental rejection stressors. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 47–68.
Psychology, 32, 1008–1024. Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA:
Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to school: Attachment, bonding, Pearson Education.
connectedness, and engagement. Journal of School Health, 74, 274–283. Taylor, R. D., & Dymnicki, A. B. (2007). Empirical evidence of social and emotional learn-
Lloyd, J. E. V., & Hertzman, C. (2009). From kindergarten readiness to fourth-grade assess- ing’s influence on school success: A commentary on “Building Academic Success on
ment: Longitudinal analysis linked with population data. Social Sciences and Medicine, Social and Emotional Learning: What does the Research say“. Journal of Educational
68, 111–123. and Psychological Consultation, 17, 2–3.
Malecki, C. K., & Elliot, S. N. (2002). Children's social behaviors as predictors of academic Trentacosta, C. J., & Shaw, D. S. (2009). Emotional self-regulation, peer rejection, and an-
achievement: A longitudinal analysis. School Psychology Quarterly, 17, 1–23. tisocial behavior: Development associations from early childhood to early adoles-
McClelland, M. M., Morrison, F. J., & Holmes, D. L. (2000). Children at risk for early aca- cence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30, 356–365.
demic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early Childhood Research Trickett, P. K., McBride-Chang, C., & Putman, F. W. (1994). The classroom performance
Quarterly, 15, 307–329. and behavior of sexually abused females. Development and Psychopathology, 6,
McLaughlin, C., & Clarke, B. (2010). Relational matters: A review of the impact of school 183–194.
experience on mental health in early adolescence. Educational and Child Psychology, Weissberg, R. P., Cowen, E. L., Loyyczewski, B. S., Boike, M. F., Orara, N. A., Stalonas, P., et al.
27, 91–103. (1987). Teacher ratings of children's problem and competence behaviors: Normative
McMillan, J. H. (2001). Secondary teachers' classroom assessment and grading practices. and parametric characteristics. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15,
Educational measurement: Issues and practice, 20, 20–32. 387–401.
McMillan, J. H. (2003). Understanding and improving teachers' classroom assessment de- Weissberg, R. P., & Greenberg, M. T. (1998). School and community competence—
cision making: Implications for theory and practice. Educational measurement: Issues enhancement and prevention programs. In I. E. Siegel, & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Hand-
and practice, 22, 34–43. book of child psychology (5th ed.)Child psychology in practice, Vol. 4. (pp. 877–954).
Merrell, K. W., Bradley, C. P., & Karalyn, T. M. (2011). Development and validation of a New York, NY: Wiley.
teacher report measure for assessing social–emotional strengths of children and ad- Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Widaman, K., & O'Neil, R. (2001). Linkages between children's so-
olescents. School Psychology Review, 40, 226–241. cial and academic development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of School Psychology,
Ministry of Education British Columbia (2011). Foundational Skills Assessment (FSA). Re- 39, 463–481.
trieved from. http://www.bced.gov.bc.ca/assessment/fsa/ Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Relations between social competence and academic achievement
National Research Council (2012). Education for life and work: Developing transferable in early adolescence. Child Development, 62, 1066–1078.
knowledge and skills in the 21st century. In James W. Pellegrino, & Margaret L. Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Social competence at school: Relations between social responsibil-
Hilton (Eds.), Committee on defining deeper learning and 21st century skills, board on ity and academic achievement. Review of Educational Research, 61, 1–24.
testing and assessment and board on science education, division of behavioral and social Wentzel, K. R. (1993). Motivation and achievement in early adolescence: The role of mul-
sciences and education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. tiple classroom goals. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13, 4–20.
Oberle, E., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2013). Relations among peer acceptance, inhibitory Wentzel, K. R. (1994). Relations of social goal pursuit to social acceptance, classroom be-
control, and math grade in early adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental havior, and perceived social support. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 173–182.
Psychology, 45–51. Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Peers and academic functioning at school. In K. H. Rubin, W. M.
Pepler, D. J., & Craig, M. W. (1998). Assessing children's peer relationships. Child Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups
Psychology & Psychiatry Review, 3, 176–182. (pp. 531–547). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interac- Whitted, K. S. (2011). Understanding how social and emotional skill deficits contribute to
tions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. school failure. Preventing school failure: Alternative education for children and youth,
Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 21, 437–448. 55, 10–16.
Public, Agenda (2002). A lot easier said than done: Parents talk about raising children in Wigfield, A., Byrnes, J. P., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Development during early and middle ad-
today's America. New York, NY: Author. olescence. In P. A. Alexander, & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychol-
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (1999). Children's goals and strategies in response to conflicts ogy (pp. 87–113). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
within a friendship. Developmental Psychology, 35, 69–79. Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Wang, M. C., & Walberg, H. J. (Eds.). (2004). Building academic
Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2008). Promoting early adolescents' achieve- success on social and emotional learning: What does the research say?. New York, NY:
ment and peer relationships: The effects of cooperative, competitive, and individual- Teachers College Press.
istic goal structures. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 223–246. Zsolnai, A. (2002). Relationship between children's social competence, learning motiva-
Ruble, D., & Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender. In & N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psy- tion, and school achievement. Educational Psychology, 22, 317–329.
chology (5th ed.)In & W. Damon (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality
development, Vol. 3. (pp. 993–1016). New York, NY: Wiley.
Ryan, A. M., Jamison, R. S., Shin, H., & Thompson, G. N. (2012). Social achievement goals
and adjustment at school during early adolescence. In A. M. Ryan, & G. W. Ladd

You might also like