You are on page 1of 4

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Research Report
BEHAVIORAL GOALS CONSTRAIN THE SELECTION OF
VISUAL INFORMATION
Paul Maruff,1,2 James Danckert,1,2 Georgina Camplin,2 and Jon Currie2,3
1
School of Psychological Science, LaTrobe University, Melbourne, Australia; 2The Mental Health Research Institute of Victoria,
Melbourne, Australia; and 3Westmead Hospital, Westmead, Australia

Abstract—Although there is agreement that attentional processes are there is disagreement about the extent to which top-down processes
limited, the necessary conditions for such limitation have not been can constrain data-driven selection (see Allport, 1993, for a review).
determined. We investigated whether behavioral goals are sufficient to For example, some authors state that the ability to ignore irrelevant
constrain the selection of visual information. In two tasks, subjects information is not absolute, and that top-down processes only modu-
were presented with targets and distractors that varied on two dimen- late data-driven selection (Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Rafal & Henik,
sions (e.g., color and letter). In separate conditions, the subjects’ goal 1994; Yantis & Johnston, 1990). However, there is also evidence to
was to identify only one dimension of the target while ignoring the sec- suggest that behavioral goals may completely constrain processes
ond dimension and ignoring the distractors. In both tasks, peripheral associated with selective attention (Allport, 1993; Baylis, Driver, &
distractors interfered with target selection only when the targets and Rafal, 1993; Kanwisher et al., 1995; Tipper, Weaver, & Houghton,
distractors differed on the goal-relevant dimension. When the goal was 1994).
changed, the pattern of interference from the same stimuli was The flanker task has been used extensively to study the ability to
reversed, so that distractors affected target selection only according to select information while simultaneously ignoring irrelevant distractors
the new goal. These results suggest that behavioral goals constrain the (C.W. Eriksen & Hoffman, 1973; C.W. Eriksen & Shultz, 1979; Flow-
selection of visual information to a greater extent than the physical ers & Wilcox, 1982; Miller, 1991). The simplest version of the flanker
characteristics of the visual information. task requires that a central target be identified with a specific manual
response. When the target (e.g., the letter E) appears, it is flanked by
distractors that are either congruent (e.g., E E E) or incongruent (e.g.,
Psychological studies of visual attention commonly seek to deter- O E O) with it. Although subjects are instructed to ignore flankers,
mine the stage in the cognitive architecture at which the selection of response times (RTs) to identify the target are faster when the flanker
information is limited by measuring the effects of distractors on the and target are congruent than when they are incongruent (C.W. Erik-
selection of targets (Allport, 1993; Broadbent, 1982; Duncan, 1980; sen & Hoffman, 1973; C.W. Eriksen & Shultz, 1979). The flanker-
B.A. Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Kanwisher, Driver, & Machado, 1995; compatibility effect (FCE) is so reliable that studies attempting to
Lavie & Cox, 1997). There is ongoing debate as to whether objects are isolate conditions under which flankers do not interfere with target
identified categorically before or after they are selected, although there identification find it difficult to eliminate the FCE completely (Miller,
is still no agreement as to the location, function, or stage of any single 1991). Manipulating the time between target and flanker onset, the
attentional “bottleneck” (Allport, 1993; Duncan, 1980; C.W. Eriksen perceptual load of the task, perceptual and response similarity between
& Hoffman, 1973; Treisman & Gelade, 1980). Neurophysiological targets and flankers, and spatial disparity between targets and flankers
studies in primates and neuroimaging studies in humans provide no alters the magnitude of the FCE but rarely eliminates it completely
evidence of any single brain area responsible for limiting the selection (C.W. Eriksen & Hoffman, 1973; C.W. Eriksen & Shultz, 1979; Lavie
of visual information. Instead, these studies suggest that attention to & Cox, 1997).
different stimulus characteristics takes place within parallel and dis- The current study investigated whether behavioral goals are suffi-
tributed neurocognitive networks that may be reconfigured according cient to constrain interference from distractors on target identification.
to task demands (Colby, 1991; Corbetta, Miezan, Dobmeyer, Shul- The task was modified so that targets and flankers varied simultane-
man, & Peterson, 1991; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Duncan, ously on two dimensions (color and letter). The same set of target-
Humphreys, & Ward, 1997; Gottlieb, Kusunoki, & Goldberg, 1998; flanker stimuli was presented in separate blocks of trials with alternate
Mesulam, 1990). Thus, no single computational function may be suf- behavioral goals. In one block, the goal was to identify target letters
ficient to encapsulate the full nature of selective processes. while ignoring colors. In the other block, the goal was to identify tar-
Utilizing both neurophysiological and psychological findings, cog- get colors while ignoring letters. In all conditions, subjects were
nitive neuroscientific models of attention propose that the ability to instructed to ignore flankers. The set of flankers was identical to the
ignore irrelevant information depends on interactions between data- set of targets, but on a given trial the flankers could be congruent or
driven selection related to the physical properties of visual information incongruent with the target on the goal-relevant or the goal-irrelevant
and top-down cognitive processes related to stimulus relevance, dimension. Examples of the different target-flanker pairings and their
expectancy, or previously learned associations (Allport, 1993; Danck- classification with respect to behavioral goals are shown in Figure 1.
ert, Maruff, Crowe, & Currie, 1998; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Dun- To ensure that any independence between the different dimensions
can et al., 1997; Lavie & Cox, 1997; Rafal & Henik, 1994). However, of the stimuli was not due to the more practiced status of letter read-
ing, we tested a second group of subjects using color and shape dimen-
sions. These dimensions were chosen because of their overlapping
Address correspondence to Paul Maruff, Neurophysiology and Neurovisu- representation in visual attentional systems and their equivalent effects
al Research Unit, Mental Health Research Institute of Victoria, Parkville, Vic- in psychophysical experiments (Corbetta et al., 1991). Examples of
toria, Australia 3052; e-mail: paul@neuro.mhri.edu.au. these stimuli are shown in Figure 2.

522 Copyright © 1999 American Psychological Society VOL. 10, NO. 6, NOVEMBER 1999
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

P. Maruff et al.

Fig. 1. Color-letter task. Group mean response times are plotted separately for the two behavioral goals (identify color and iden-
tify letter) as a function of target-flanker congruency: Targets and flankers were congruent on both the goal-relevant and goal-
irrelevant dimensions (CR/CI), congruent on the goal-relevant dimension and incongruent on the goal-irrelevant dimension (CR/II),
incongruent on the goal-relevant dimension and congruent on the goal-irrelevant dimension (IR/CI), and incongruent on both the
goal-relevant and goal-irrelevant dimensions (IR/II). Below the graph, examples of the corresponding target-flanker pairs are
shown. Filled stimuli represent red letters, and unfilled stimuli represent green letters.

For both tasks, we hypothesized that if behavioral goals were suf- stimuli varied randomly and independently. Examples of the four pos-
ficient to constrain data-driven selection, then flankers would interfere sible target and flanker pairs for a red E target are shown in Figure 1.
with target identification only when they differed on the goal-relevant For the color-shape task, targets and flankers were filled circles or
dimension (Allport, 1993). Response-competition models typically squares colored red or green. Examples of the four possible target and
invoked to explain the FCE suggest that both dimensions of flanker flanker pairs for a red square target are shown in Figure 2. For both
stimuli may activate their associated response codes (Miller, 1991). tasks, the horizontal edge-to-edge distance between targets and
Therefore, these models predicted that interference from flankers on flankers subtended 0.63°.
target identification would occur irrespective of subjects’ behavioral All stimuli remained visible until a response was made, and the
goal. subsequent trial began 1,500 ms afterward. An incorrect response was
signaled by a tone, and the trial was rescheduled to the end of the
block.
METHOD For each task, there were two behavioral goals. For the color-letter
task, one goal was to identify the color of the target while ignoring its
Twenty-four subjects with normal visual acuity and normal color form (i.e., respond either “red” or “green”). The second goal was to
vision were tested after giving informed consent. Twelve subjects identify the target letter while ignoring its color (i.e., respond either
were assigned to each task (color-letter task: mean age = 22.9 years, “E” or “O”). For the color-shape task, the first goal was to identify the
SD = 2.5; color-shape task: mean age = 24.3 years, SD = 1.7). color of the target while ignoring its shape, and the second goal was
The Microelectronic Laboratory (Schneider, 1988) software con- to identify the shape of the target while ignoring its color (i.e.,
trolled all stimulus presentation and data collection. Subjects sat 113 respond either “circle” or “square”). A training session of 16 trials
cm from the monitor with their head stabilized on a chin rest. Three occurred prior to each block of trials. During training, each exemplar
white horizontal bars, 0.35° in length, remained visible throughout from the goal-relevant dimension was mapped to a specific finger
each block of trials, with targets appearing above the central bar. (e.g., either “red” or “E” mapped to the index finger and either
Flankers appeared above the left or right bar with equal probability. “green” or “O” mapped to the middle finger for the color-letter task).
For the color-letter task, targets and flankers consisted of the upper- For each subject, the response mappings were randomized between
case letters E and O colored red or green. The letter and color of the blocks.

VOL. 10, NO. 6, NOVEMBER 1999 523


PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Selection of Visual Information

Fig. 2. Color-shape task. Group mean response times are plotted separately for the two behavioral goals (identify color and iden-
tify shape) as a function of target-flanker congruency: Targets and flankers were congruent on both the goal-relevant and goal-
irrelevant dimensions (CR/CI), congruent on the goal-relevant dimension and incongruent on the goal-irrelevant dimension
(CR/II), incongruent on the goal-relevant dimension and congruent on the goal-irrelevant dimension (IR/CI), and incongruent on
both the goal-relevant and goal-irrelevant dimensions (IR/II). Below the graph, examples of the corresponding target-flanker pairs
are shown. Filled stimuli represent red shapes, and unfilled stimuli represent green shapes.

Experimental conditions were defined according to the congru- N = 12] = 3.12, p > .05, for shape). Error trials were excluded from
ence between targets and flankers on both dimensions with reference further analysis.
to the behavioral goal, yielding four conditions within each block. For each task, subjects’ mean RTs were submitted to a 2 (behav-
Targets and flankers could be congruent on both the goal-relevant ioral goal: identify color vs. identify letter or shape) × 4 (target-flanker
and goal-irrelevant dimensions (CR/CI), congruent on the goal-rele- congruency: CR/CI vs. CR/II vs. IR/CI vs. IR/II) profile analysis with
vant dimension and incongruent on the goal-irrelevant dimension data collapsed across visual fields. The mean RT for each condition in
(CR/II), incongruent on the goal-relevant dimension and congruent the color-letter task is shown in Figure 1. There was no significant
on the goal-irrelevant dimension (IR/CI), and incongruent on both deviation from parallelism, Wilks Λ = 0.88, F(3, 9) = 0.48, p > .05, and
the goal-relevant and goal-irrelevant dimensions (IR/II). (See Figs. 1 there was no significant effect of levels, F(1, 11) = 0.64, p > .05. How-
and 2.) ever, the test of flatness was significant, Wilks Λ = 0.36, F(3, 9) = 6.4,
The different combinations of target and flanker colors and letters p < .01. Investigation of the main effect of flatness indicated no sig-
(shapes) and spatial positioning gave 32 possible stimulus displays nificant difference between the CR/CI and CR/II conditions (t < 1), a
that were equiprobable in each block of trials. Each subject complet- significant difference between the CR/II and IR/CI conditions (t[23] =
ed four blocks of 40 trials for each behavioral goal in each task (i.e., –4.42, p < .01, mean difference = 22.48 ms), and no significant differ-
color-letter or color-shape), with blocks randomized between subjects. ence between the IR/CI and IR/II conditions (t < 1).
Subjects were tested in two testing sessions with rest periods of at least The mean RT for each condition in the color-shape task is shown
2 min between blocks. in Figure 2. There was no significant deviation from parallelism, Wilks
Λ = 0.88, F(3, 11) = 0.48, p > .05. However, the test of levels showed
a significant effect, F(1, 11) = 21.65, p < .01, η2 = 0.66 (mean differ-
RESULTS ence = 19.27), indicating that in general RTs to identify color were
On each task, errors occurred on less than 1% of all trials. There faster than RTs to identify shape. There was a significant deviation
was no relationship between condition and the frequency of errors from flatness, Wilks Λ = 0.12, F(1, 11) = 22.57, p < .001. Investiga-
under either behavioral goal for the color-letter task (χ2[3, N = 12] = tion of the main effect of flatness indicated no significant difference
2.1, p > .05, for color and χ2[3, N = 12] = 1.3, p > .05, for letter) or for between the CR/CI and CR/II conditions (t[23] = –1.02, p > .05), a sig-
the color-shape task (χ2[3, N = 12] = 3.96, p > .05, for color and χ2[3, nificant difference between the CR/II and IR/CI conditions (t[23] =

524 VOL. 10, NO. 6, NOVEMBER 1999


PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

P. Maruff et al.

–6.34, p < .001, mean difference = 15.37 ms), and no significant dif- whether there are spatial limits to this goal-relevant selection of dis-
ference between the IR/CI and IR/II conditions (t < 1). tracting information.
Two previous studies using different methodologies also found that
behavioral goals can constrain attentional processing in normal sub-
DISCUSSION jects. Tipper et al. (1994) found that only the goal-relevant dimension
of multidimensional distractors gave rise to negative priming. Similar-
The results indicate that interference on target identification from
ly, Kanwisher et al. (1995) found that repetition blindness occurred
peripheral flankers occurs only for the physical dimension of those tar-
only for the attended dimension of targets that could vary on color and
gets and flankers that is relevant to the subjects’ current behavioral
letter. Taken together, the results of these two previous studies and
goal (Figs. 1 and 2). For example, in the color-letter task, RTs to iden-
those of the current study suggest that the data-driven selection of visu-
tify the target letter were approximately 25 ms faster when there was
al information, based on the physical properties of visual stimuli, can
congruency between the letter dimension of targets and flankers than
be constrained completely by the individual’s current behavioral goal.
when targets and flankers were incongruent on the letter dimension.
The congruency of the goal-irrelevant dimension (i.e., color) did not
affect this pattern of RTs. Thus, there was no additional benefit for REFERENCES
RTs to identify targets when flankers were congruent on both the goal-
Allport, A. (1993). Attention and control: Have we been asking the wrong questions? A
relevant and goal-irrelevant dimensions. Similarly, there was no addi- critical review of twenty-five years. In D.E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention
tional cost for RTs to identify targets when flankers were incongruent and performance XIV (pp. 183–218). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baylis, G.C., Driver, J., & Rafal, R.D. (1993). Visual extinction and stimulus repetition.
on both the goal-relevant and goal-irrelevant dimensions. When the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 453–466.
behavioral goal was reversed so that subjects were required to identi- Broadbent, D.E. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information. Acta Psy-
fy the target color, the pattern of interference from flankers also was chologia, 50, 253–290.
Cohen, A., & Shoup, R.E. (1997). Perceptual dimensional constraints in selection process-
reversed. In this case, responses to targets depended only on the con- es. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 128–181.
gruency of the color of targets and flankers, irrespective of the con- Colby, C.L. (1991). The neuroanatomy of and neurophysiology of attention. Journal of
gruency of letters. The magnitude of the difference in RTs to identify Child Neurology, 6, S90–S118.
Corbetta, M., Miezan, F.M., Dobmeyer, S., Shulman, G.L., & Peterson, S.E. (1991). Selec-
the colors when flankers were congruent and incongruent with targets tive and divided attention during visual discrimination of shape, colour and speed:
on the color dimension was also approximately 25 ms. These results Functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. Journal of Neuroscience, 11,
2383–2402.
indicate that interference from flankers on target identification was Danckert, J., Maruff, P., Crowe, S., & Currie, J. (1998). Inhibitory processes in covert ori-
reversed when the behavioral goal of the task was altered, even though enting in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 12, 225–241.
the physical dimensions of targets and flankers did not change. There- Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.
fore, the physical properties of the targets and flankers were not, by Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli.
themselves, sufficient for the interference from flankers to occur. Psychological Review, 87, 272–300.
The results for the color-shape task were quantitatively and quali- Duncan, J., Humphreys, G., & Ward, R. (1997). Competitive brain activity in visual atten-
tion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 255–261.
tatively similar to the results for the color-letter task. Although RTs to Eriksen, B.A., & Eriksen, C.W. (1974). Effects of noise shapes upon the identification of
identify color were generally faster than RTs to identify shape, inter- a target shape in a non-search task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.
Eriksen, C.W., & Hoffman, J.E. (1973). The extent of processing noise elements during
ference from flankers occurred only for the goal-relevant dimension. selective encoding from visual displays. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 155–160.
This replication suggests that the independence of selection was not Eriksen, C.W., & Shultz, D.W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous
due to any specific physical property of stimuli used in the color-letter flow conception and experimental results. Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 249–263.
Flowers, J.H., & Wilcox, N. (1982). The effect of flanking context on visual classification:
task. Instead, the limits to selection were determined by the relation- The joint contribution of interactions at different processing levels. Perception &
ship between targets and flankers on the dimension that was relevant Psychophysics, 32, 331–342.
to the behavioral goal of the subject. Gottlieb, J.P., Kusunoki, M., & Goldberg, M.E. (1998). The representation of visual
salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature, 391, 481–484.
Previously, the FCE was eliminated by defining targets and Kanwisher, N., Driver, J., & Machado, L. (1995). Spatial repetition blindness is modulat-
flankers according to different semantic categories (i.e., letters and ed by selective attention to color or shape. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 303–337.
Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: Efficient visu-
digits) with no probabilistic relationship between them (Paquet & al search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8, 395–398.
Craig, 1997). In addition, no FCE was observed when flankers and tar- Mesulam, M.M. (1990). Large scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing
gets belonging to the same response category differed on perceptual for attention, language and memory. Annals of Neurology, 28, 587–613.
Miller, J. (1991). The flanker compatibility effect as a function of visual angle, attention-
dimensions (Cohen & Shoup, 1997). Similarly, in an experiment com- al focus, visual transients, and perceptual load: A search for boundary conditions.
bining the flanker task with a visual search paradigm, the effect of dis- Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 270–288.
tractors on the speed of visual search was eliminated when targets and Paquet, L., & Craig, G. (1997). Evidence for selective target processing with a low per-
ceptual load flankers task. Memory & Cognition, 25, 182–189.
distractors were perceptually similar or when the number of distrac- Rafal, R.D., & Henik, A. (1994). The neurology of inhibition: Integrating controlled and
tors was increased. This suggested that under conditions of low per- automatic processes. In D. Dagenbach & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in
attention, memory and language (pp. 1–50). San Diego: Academic Press.
ceptual load, it becomes more difficult for individuals to ignore Schneider, W. (1988). Microelectronic Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM-PC
distractors (Lavie & Cox, 1997). In the current study, we reversed compatibles. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 20, 206–217.
completely the effect of distractors on targets without manipulating Tipper, S.P., Weaver, B., & Houghton, G. (1994). Behavioural goals determine inhibitory
mechanisms of selective attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol-
semantic category, probability, perceptual distinctiveness, or the per- ogy, 47A, 809–840.
ceptual load of the task itself. Therefore, this study suggests behav- Treisman, A.M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive
ioral goals are sufficient to constrain the effect of distracting Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Yantis, S., & Johnston, J. (1990). On the locus of visual selection: Evidence from focussed
information on selection. As flanker effects are reduced when the spa- attention tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Per-
tial separation between targets and flankers is increased (B.A. Eriksen formance, 16, 135–149.
& Eriksen, 1974; Miller, 1991), it will be important to determine (RECEIVED 9/14/98; ACCEPTED 12/22/98)

VOL. 10, NO. 6, NOVEMBER 1999 525

You might also like