You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, YOI1KSTON,BEUKELMAN,Volume 46, 398-404, November 1981

ATAXIC DYSARTHRIA: TREATMENT SEQUENCES BASED ON


INTELLIGIBILITY AND PROSODIC CONSIDERATIONS

ICa,TttRYN M. YORKSTON DAVID R. BEUKELMAN


Unicersity vf Washington1, Seattle

Treabnent programs of fimr improving ataxic dysarthric speakers are reviewed. Treatment sequences were based on two
overall measures of speech pertormance--intelligibility and prosody. Increases in intelligibility were initially achieved by
control of speaking rate. A hierarch~ of rate control strategies, ranging from a rigid imposition of rate through rhythmic cueing to
selfmunitored rate control is discussed. As speakers improved their monitoring skills, a compromise was made between intel-
ligibilit3, and rate. Normal prosodic patterns were not achieved by the ataxic speakers due to difficulty in precisely coordinating
the subtle flmdamental frequency, loudness and timing adjustments needed to signal stress. Three of the four subjects were
taught to use only durational adjustments to signal stress. In this way, they were able to achieve stress on targeted words
consistently and minimize bizarreness which resulted from sweeping changes in fllndameutal frequency and bursts of loudness.
The need tor further clinically oriented research is discussed.

Researchers have studied ataxic dysarthria from a vari- tribute the prosodic abnormalities of ataxic speakers to
ety of perspectives. Perceptual speech characteristics of specific points along the vocal tract. Also, treatment of
ataxic dysarthria include articulatory inaccuracy often the complex, inconsistent and interrelating patterns of
accompanied by irregular artieulatory breakdowns and artieulatory breakdown, laryngeal control, and respira-
prosodic abnormalities such as slow rate, excess and tory support necessitates measurement of overall per-
equal stress, monoloudness, monopiteh and prolonged formance in which coordination of all speech mechanism
syllables (Brown, Darley, & Aronson, 1970). Physiologi- components is involved. The purpose of this paper is to
cal characteristics of ataxie dysarthria include slow ar- review the treatment of four improving ataxic dysarthric
ticulatory movements, errors of direction and range of ar- speakers. Attention is given to the use of overall meas-
ticulatory movements, and reduced overall articulatory ures of intelligibility and prosody as a means of making
mobility (Kent & Netsell, 1975). Acoustic characteristics decisions that guide treatment sequencing.
of ataxie dysarthria indicate timing control problems in
prolongation of segments and a tendency to equalize
segment durations (Kent, Netsell, & Abbs, 1979). Al- SUBJECTS
though durational measures are usually outside normal
limits, formant frequencies for vowels in CVC syllables The four subjects described in this paper exhibited
are normal. Kent and his colleagues concluded "when sudden, adult-onset of ataxie dysarthria (see Table 1).
sufficient time was available, the ataxie speakers were Subjects ranged in age from 23 to 55 years. Etiology in
capable of reaching appropriate vowel targets," (p. 648). two eases was anoxie eneephalopathy and in two eases
Research has led to some understanding of the unique was closed head injury. Although these etiologies do not
constellation of perceptual, physiological, and acoustic rule out involvement of areas other than the cerebellum,
characteristics of ataxie dysarthria. However, this infor- the predominant neurological symptoms including gait
mation has not b e e n a p p l i e d to the selection and disturbances and dysrhythmia were indicative of an
s e q u e n c i n g of t r e a t m e n t tasks for ataxie speakers. ataxic disorder. None of the subjects had degenerative
Rosenbek and LaPointe (1978) state that to their knowl- eerebellar disease. All experienced a period of coma
edge no specific treatment programs exist for the dif- ranging from less than one week for Subject 1 to four
ferent types of dysarthria. Bather, they suggest a "point- weeks for Subject 4. For all subjects, coma was followed
place" approach that is based on the speech physiology by a period of confusion, distractibility, and lack of atten-
work of Netsell (1973). Using the point-place model, the tion lasting as long as six weeks. Speech treatment was
speaker is evaluated systematically b y determining initiated from two to six weeks post onset and continued
speech mechanism performance at points along the vocal until eight to ten months post onset. When speech treat-
tract. Netsell and Daniel (1979) describe the treatment of ment was initiated, all subjects were wheelchair bound
a flaccid dysarthric individual with whom the point- because of severely ataxie gait patterns. At the time of
place approach was used successfully. However, due to discharge from speeeh treatment, all were independent
the inability to coordinate and regulate the activities of ambulators except Subject i who continued to require
several speech components simultaneously, ataxic dysar- standby guarding for safety.
thrie individuals are not easily treated using the point- Initially, speech intelligibility scores of all subjects
place approach alone. For example, it is difficult to at- were less than 30% and speaking rates were in excess of

© 1981, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 398 0022-4685/81/4604-0398501.00/0


Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
YORKSTON, BEUKELMAN: Ataxic Dysarthria 399

TABLE 1. Subject characteristics.

Pretreatment Post Treatment


Months
Subject Age Etiology Coma Intell. (%) Rate (wpm) InteU. (%) Rate (wpm) Post Onset
1 55 Anoxic
Encephalopathy
2° to Cardiac Arrest < 1 wk 20 120 98 64 9
2 39 Closed Head Injury
2° Motor Vehicle
Accident 1 wk 19 132 99 132 8
3 24 Anoxic
Encephalopathy
2° Drug Overdose 2 wk 10 110 68 74 9
4 23 Closed Head Injury
2° to Motor Vehicle
Accident 3-4 wk 25 t15 98 60 10

100 wpm for oral reading tasks. Analysis of articulatory was the pacing board described by Helm (1979). Helm
performance revealed correct production of all sounds in described the use of this system with a Parkinsonian pa-
isolation, CV, or VC contexts. However, during oral read- tient who was thought to be demented, but actually was
ing, articulatory targets were only occasionally approxi- severely palilalic. Verbal suggestions to reduce rate and
mated and speech was judged to be excessively hyper- external rate control devices such as a m e t r o n o m e
nasal. Therefore, speech intelligibility was severely proved ineffective for Helm's speaker. However, use of a
reduced and the initial phases of treatment were de- pacing board was thought to impose the necessary
signed to develop functionally intelligible speech. Pat- "stop-go control" and to bring what had been automatic
terns of recovery and treatment strategies for all subjects motor acts under voluntary control. Although our sub-
were so similar that the overall treatment approach will jects were not palilalic, during the early stages of recov-
be presented with occasional notes about individual dif- ery, they experienced confusion and reduced ability to
ferences. monitor their own rates voluntarily. Both the alphabet
board and the pacing board techniques may be effective
when other rate control strategies fail. However, such
ACHIEVING INTELLIGIBLE techniques result in prosodic disruption. Often t h e s e
SPEECH highly structured techniques appear to encourage the
relative lengthening of pause time rather than lengthen-
Rate Control ing of all durational aspects of speech. As less rigid rate
control techniques became effective, they were incorpo-
Control of speaking rate as a means of maximizing rated into treatment,
speech intelligibility was an early goal for all subjeets. A Rhythmic Cueing. A technique used with all subjects
number of rate control techniques were ordered into a as a transition between the rigid control of rate'and the
hierarchy ranging from rigidly controlled tasks that lim- self-monitoring of rate was called "rhythmic cueing."
ited speaking to essentially a "one-word-at-a-time" style Using this technique, the clinician paced the reading of
to techniques that required subjects to control their own a passage by imposing on the speaker a slow rate with
speaking rates voluntarily. appropriate pausing and phrasing. This technique results
Rigid Imposition of Rate. As comas cleared, all four in a more natural prosody than the "one-word-at-a-time"
subjects were unable to monitor the adequacy of their quality of metronomic pacing or other types of pacing
speaking rate and thus compensate for their motor in- that give equal time allotments to all syllables and rela-
coordination. Therefore, it was necessary to impose a tively large interword pause times. The clinician indi-
strict rate reduction program. For example, with Subject cated the pace with which words should be read by
4, a system similar to the one described by Beukelman pointing to them. Stressed syllables were cued slowly,
and Yorkston (1977) was used. This system requires and u n s t r e s s e d syllables w e r e c u e d m o r e quickly.
speakers to point to the first letter of each word on an Speakers were told to follow the rhythm and were per-
alphabet board as they speak that word. It not only slows mitted to lag behind but not "get ahead" of the pacer. As
a speaker's rate, it also gives the communication partners the speakers learned to control speaking rate success-
extra information in the form of the initial letter of each fully with obvious cueing, the cueing gestures were
word. Using the a l p h a b e t board, speakers r e s o l v e d faded by gradually diminishing and then eliminating
communication breakdowns by spelling out the entire them.
word. Oscilloscopic Feedback. Another method of rate con-
Another technique for rigid imposition of rate control trol that does not inherently disrupt prosody is oscillo-

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
400 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46 398-404 November 1981

scopic feedback of intensity by time tracings. Berry and I00-


D
Goshorn (1979) described a technique in which their
speaker was asked to "fill up" the five-seeond window of
90- ..........
an oscilloscopic display or to match slow productions of
a target sentence. This technique is advantageous be-
cause speakers receive immediate feedback about the 80-
success of their rate control attempts.
No specific instructions to control pause time or articu- 70-
lation time were given to Berry and Goshorn's speaker.
However, d e p e n d i n g on the individual speaker, the 60-
specific modification of either pause time or artieulatory
time may be taught via oscilloscopic feedback. This
~- 50-
point can be illustrated with Subject 3 who was taught to
.-.."
lengthen pause time selectively. By increasing certain
pause times, the number of irregular articulatory break- 40-
downs decreased markedly, and, as a consequence, intel-
ligibility was improved. For this subject, increasing the 30-
inter-word pause time seemed to reduce the demands of
articulatory coordination of eomplex sequences of sylla- 20-
bles. On the other hand, Subject 4, during the later
stages of treatment when intelligibility had improved,
was encouraged to reduce pause time and to increase ar- P0-
ticulation time in an effort to normalize prosody. At this
point in his treatment program, long inter-word pauses
that tended to disrupt prosody could be reduced without 2's -/s ,do go
Rate (WPM)
markedly affecting intelligibility.
Establishing Optimum Rate and Intelligibility Rela- FIGURE 1. Sentence intelligibility and speaking rates obtained
tionships. Perhaps the least rigidly imposed rate reduc- from Subject 1 as he read different passages. Instructions to
Subject 1 were as follows (A) "Speak at your normal rate," (B)
tion technique used with our subjects was a process of "Use a rate at which the clinician can understand 90% of the
rate monitoring with feedback of intelligibility. Speakers passage," (C) "Slow the rate an additional 25% as compared to
were recorded as they read passages of a known length Condition B," and (D) "Slow your rate as much as you can."
that were unfamiliar to the clinician. They were given
instructions to reduce speaking rate to the point at which
their speeeh was at least 95% intelligible to the clinician. ily, as the patient's optimum rate. Treatment was focused
The clinician transeribed the recorded passage, deter- on achieving this rate on both reading and conversa-
mined the level of intelligibility, and computed speaking tional tasks. Obviously, the optimum speaking rate for a
rate. While maintaining intelligibility within the target recovering dysarthric speaker changes over time. At each
range, the subject was encouraged to maximize speaking point during the course of recovery, intelligibility can be
rate. used to estimate the most efficient speaking rate.
The following examples illustrate the use of this tech- Data obtained from Subject 2 also illustrates the com-
nique to estimate the optimum speaking rate. Figure 1 promise between intelligibility and rate. Over the course
contains rate and intelligibility data obtained approxi- of eight months, Subject 2 achieved the most normal
mately one month post onset when Subject 1 was able to speaking rate of the four subjects. Figure 2 illustrates
maintain a slowed rate without rhythmic cueing. During rate and intelligibility data obtained at three points dur-
Condition A, the subject was instructed to speak at a ing his improvement. During Session i, one month post
normal rate. At this rate (120 wpm), his intelligibility onset, Subject 2 read orally at a rate of 137 wpm. Al-
score was 37%. During Condition B, he was asked to though this rate was somewhat reduced from a normal
achieve a rate at which the clinieian could understand at oral reading rate of 160-170 wpm, it was too rapid to
least 90% of what was read. During this condition, he permit intelligible speech. At this time his speech was
slowed his rate to 90 wpm with an accompanying in- characterized by little articulatory movement. However,
crease in intelligibility to 82%. During Condition C, an oceasional word could be recognized if a pause pre-
Subject 1 was asked to slow his rate by an additional ceded it. Subject 2 seemed unable to modify his speak-
25% as compared to Condition B. At this slowed rate, 61 ing rate. Therefore, rhythmic cueing was employed to
wpm, his intelligibility score was within the target control the rate. After four weeks of treatment, two
range, 95%. Slowing the rate still further to 50 wpm months post onset, Subject 2 was able to maintain a
(Condition D) resulted in only a small increase in intel- slowed rate of 80 wpm when he read a passage. This
ligibility, to 98%. At 50 wpm, the subject appeared to slowed rate enabled him to achieve articulatory targets
increase pause time rather than articulation time. At the adequately. As Subject 2 continued to improve, the
slowest of the four rates, prosody patterns were severely speaking rate at which he was able to achieve target in-
disrupted. Therefore, 60-65 wpm was adopted, temporar- telligibility increased. At eight months post onset, Ses-

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
YORKSTON, BEUKELMAN:Ataxic Dgsarthria 401

I00- -150 Subject 1 was unable to achieve rates higher than 75-80
wpm even when asked to speak as rapidly as possible.
90- -135 There appeared to be a new rate "ceiling" for this
i sJSJ' Iq speaker. The subject's rate was slow enough to allow for
80- -120 achievement of the articulatory targets. The establish-
I
I
ment of this new rate may have been achieved through
--, 70- -105 careful treatment and continued drill to habituate this
i !~, ~'isJs
I
~- 60-
new pattern. However, it is possible that as improve-
ment occurred, this speaker regained the ability to take
advantage of feedback, that was not available earlier.
50 ~ Session 2 75 Specific treatment strategies designed to improve
self-monitoring skills varied from subject to subject. Of-
ten, subjects were asked to predict, immediately follow-
4o- 60 .~
ing a recorded trial, the percentage of their speech that
was understood by the clinician. These estimates were
30- 45~ compared to the clinician's transcription accuracy. Re-
cording tasks ranged in difficulty from single words, to
20- 30 sentences, and, eventually, to paragraph material.
Because the subject's treatment was carried out on an
I0 15 outpatient basis, a home practice program was devel-
oped for Subject 3. Sentence generation grids, similar to
the one shown in Table 2, were used to create practice
Months post onset nonsense sentences. These grids contained the frame
sentences, for example, "The (adjective) (subject) are
FIGURE 2. Intelligibility scores and speaking rates obtained as (verbed) by the (agent)." As he practiced, Subject 3
Subject 2 read passages at one month post-onset (Session 1), two
and a half months post-onset (Session 2), and 8 months post- created sentences by randomly selecting one of approx-
onset (Session 3). imately 50 words from each column. He then recorded
his verbal production for the sentence. On the following
day, he transcribed the tape, either by typing or verbal
sion 3, speaking rate had increased to 134 wpm with an dictation to a family member. The results of the tran-
intelligibility score of over 99%. Throughout the course scription were compared to a script which had been
of treatment, a compromise was derived between intel- created at the time of recording and intelligibility scores
ligibility scores within the target range and the most were computed.
rapid rate possible.
Developing Self-monitoring Skills. As improvement
progressed, an increasing ability to achieve articulatory TABLE 2. Example of a sentence generation grid developed for
targets was noted in all subjects. Early in their recovery, a home practice program.
articulatory targets were almost never achieved in con-
nected speech. However, all subjects were able to Sentence Frame
achieve articulatory targets in isolation and in some short (Adjective) (Subject) were (Verbed) by the (Agent)
words. All appeared unable to monitor obvious failure to
reach articulatory postures in connected speech, and all some oranges thrown boys
continued to speak at estimated premorbid rates. During those peas tossed men
few walnuts cooked neighbor
the course of treatment, ability to monitor rate improved all tables sold clerks
substantially. The monitoring process involved a series free balls packed ladies
of stages. Initially, there appeared to be little awareness your cakes taken farmer
of the consequences of rates which were too rapid to hidden telephones left family
permit target intelligibility. During this time, rate con-
trol had to be imposed by the clinician. The next stage
appeared to be voluntary conscious control as subjects
began to recognize excessively rapid rates during treat- etc. etc. etc. etc.
ment related speaking activities. Finally, control of
speaking rate began to generalize to other speaking situ-
ations. By the time of discharge, the new, slowed rates Specific Point-place Assessment and Treatment. Al-
became so habitual that it was impossible for some of the though speech intelligibility, an overall indicator of
subjects to increase their rate beyond the point at which speech performance, was used to guide many treatment
they could maintain target intelligibility. For example, decisions, the speech characteristics of these subjects
Subject 1 was asked to re-record a series of passages at were also assessed using the point-place model (Rosen-
rates similar to those produced earlier. At the time of bek & LaPointe, 1978). As consistent specific deficits
discharge eight months following the initial recordings, were identified, treatment was undertaken to correct

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
402 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46 398-404 November 1981

those deficits. For example, Subject i distorted most live. Rhythmic cueing for rate control was employed to
v o w e l p r o d u c t i o n s that i m m e d i a t e l y p r e c e d e d the diminish excess and equal stressing and to encourage at
phoneme /1/. Contrast drills including the words hoe, least primary stress patterns.
hill, heal, hell, who'll, Hal and hall, were developed and Normal stress patterns are thought to be achieved by a
e m p l o y e d in a t r a i n i n g p r o g r a m i n c l u d i n g words, combination of fundamental frequency shifts, loudness
phrases, and sentences until the vowel production was variations, and durational adjustments. A normal speaker
correct at least 95% of the time in sentence context. may use any or all of these strategies, probably relying
Early in treatment, Subject 4 exhibited hypernasal most heavily on fundamental frequency adjustments
vowel quality and confusion of the nasality feature dur- (Lehiste, 1970). A review of the characteristics of ataxie
ing consonant production (/m/ for /b/ and /n/ for /d/). dysarthrie speech reveals that many of these strategies
Measures of volume velocity of nasal airflow and intra- are not readily available to ataxie speakers because of the
oral air pressure were completed (Netsell & Daniel, precise coordination that is demanded. This point can be
1979). This evaluation demonstrated incomplete velo- illustrated with data o b t a i n e d from Subject 1. The
pharyngeal closure during production o f / p / a n d / b / d u r - speaker was audio recorded while producing the target
ing rapid speech. However, when rate was reduced and sentence, "Show Sam some snow)' In response to the
Subject 4 was instructed to articulate carefully, adequate examiner's questions, "To whom should I show the
velopharyngeal closure was achieved. It appeared that snow?" The relative level of stress for each word was
inadequate velopharyngeal closure did not stem from rated according to a system developed by Chueng, Hol-
weakness or lack of ability to achieve closure, but rather den and Minifie (1973). We rated the relative stress of
from problems in timing and overall coordination of the each word by placing marks on a five point continuous
speech mechanism. This problem was treated as an ar- scale from "most possible" to "least possible" stress.
tieulatory problem, and no palatal lift was recommended. From these judgments, the primarily stressed syllable in
Subsequent aerodynamic studies revealed only occa- the sentence was identified. Although Subject 1 did not
sional instances of velopharyngeal incompetency as this always achieve stress on the targeted word, in this in-
subject's articulation became more preeise. stance, he was judged to be achieving stress successfully
Early in treatment, Subject 3 consistently substituted on the word, "Sam.'" Sentences with targeted stress on
/p/ for /f/ in conversational speech. Since each of these the word "Sam" produced by Subject 1 and a normal
sounds could be correctly produced in isolation, a treat- speaker were analyzed by computer using a fundamental
ment program was developed in which these sounds frequency and intensity extraction program (Yorkston,
were contrastedin words, phrases, sentences and finally Beukehnan & Minifie, 1979). Results of this analysis are
paragraphs. Eventually, Subject 3 eliminated this sub- presented in Figure 3. Examination of the figure reveals
stitution error. that the dysarthric sample was twice the duration of the
normal sample. Further the dysarthric sample tended to
contain larger fundamental frequency shifts than did the
MAXIMIZING NORMAL PROSODY normal sample. Finally, Subject 1 produced near-peak
intensities on all of the syllables, while the normal
Beeause of the elose relationship between intelligibil- speaker achieved peak intensity only on the primarily
ity and the ability of a speaker to funetion in a com- stressed syllable. So, both in terms of loudness and fun-
municative setting, intelligibility is undeniably of pri- damental frequency shifts, the prosodic patterns pro-
mary eoncern to the clinician treating the improving duced by this dysarthric speaker were abnormal. Dura-
ataxie dysarthric speaker. However, prosodic patterns tional data were extracted from these samples so that a
which are often markedly abnormal and eontribute to comparison of relative durational patterns for the normal
bizarreness of dysarthric speakers, can not be ignored. and dysarthric samples could be made. Figure 4 illus-
Beeause normal prosody requires precise eoordination of trates the duration (in reset.) for each word in each of the
all of the speech processes, ataxic dysarthrie speakers are samples. It is apparent that all of the dysarthric speaker's
particularly susceptible to deficits in this area. These words are longer than the normal speakers; but the rela-
a b n o r m a l i t i e s are well d o c u m e n t e d by Kent et al., tive patterns are similar.
(1979). Treatment, primarily designed to improve prosody,
Although work on prosody typically comes late in was initiated after speech intelligibility had reached the
t r e a t m e n t programs, we concur with R o s e n b e k and target level consistently. Clinical observations confirmed
LaPointe (1978) who feel that early emphasis on appro- what the acoustic data had suggested. Control of funda-
priate stress patterns has a variety of beneficial effects. mental frequency shifts and loudness levels were dif-
Forcing marginally intelligible speakers into specific ficult for Subject 1. When adjustments in fimdamental
stress patterns tend to increase intelligibility and reduce frequency and intensity were used to signal stress, the
bizarreness. Several techniques which attempt to pre- result was an increase in bizarreness with sweeping
serve and encourage appropriate prosody have b e e n changes in fundamental frequency and extreme bursts of
mentioned previously in the discussion of maximizing intensity. Durational adjustments seemed to be the only
intelligibility. For example, the time spent rigidly im- stress strategy available to this ataxic speaker. Further,
posing rate control was minimized or eliminated al- his durational adjustments were essentially undirec-
together when other rate control strategies beeame effec- tional. Subject 1 was t a u g h t to l e n g t h e n s y l l a b l e s

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
YORKSTON, BEUKELMAN:Ataxic Dysarthria 403

NORMAL SPEAKER
300 I ~DYSARTHRIC

.... Fundamental frequency (Hz) ~NORMAL


- - Relative intensity
250 l

O m: ~ O A
3= ,¢ O Z o 20O
co t,D (D 09 o
AN 200
-•1oo E
t-
o~c- 17s ~8o O
O 150
C
60 ¢~
.E a
, ;
m.
e, 125 40 ~. £
Ig
G o 100
E
..~ 100 20 ¢
e,,

14.
75
300 600 50
Time (msec)

DYSARTHRIC SPEAKER
SHOW SAM SOME SNOW SHOW SAM SOME SNOW
A 200 - 100
FIGURE 4. Duration (in msec.) of each word in the sentence
~ 175 -
o I a
so "Show SAM some snow." produced by a dysarthric speaker
u)
(Subject 1) and a normal speaker.
C
'150 60 •
._=
~ 125 40 .-> encouraging more appropriate stress patterns for the
three subjects receiving specific stress training.
E 100 20 ¢ Obviously, it is not intended that durational adjust-
I ments be considered the primary strategy for training
"= 7S I I stress in all ataxic dysarthric speakers. Rather, it is
1000 2000 suggested that stress techniques need to be assessed sys-
Time (msec) tematically and a variety of issues need to be addressed.
FIGURE 3. Fundamental frequency, relative intensity, and dura- For example, is the speaker achieving stress on words
tion for the sentence, "'Show Sam some snow" produced by a that are targeted for primary stress? Are there different
normal and a dysarthric speaker in response to the question, levels of secondary stress or is one word primarily
"'To whom should I show the snow?" stressed and all of the other words essentially equal? If
there is primary stressing, how is the speaker achieving
it? Are any of the stressing strategies used by a speaker
targeted for stress and to increase the length of pauses increasing the bizarreness of the prosody pattern? From
prior to stressed words. He was unable to decrease the answers to questions such as these, programs designed
duration of unstressed words without detrimental effects to improve prosody can be developed systematically.
on articulatory preeisions. Although these strategies for
achieving stress did not result in normal prosody, their
use allowed Subject 1 to achieve stress on targeted DISCUSSION
words consistently and to reduce bizarreness caused by
bursts of loudness and sweeping changes in fundamental The treatment programs for four reeovering ataxie
frequency. dysarthric speakers have been reviewed. The sequenc-
Three of our four subjects were trained to modify dura- ing of treatment using two overall measures of speeeh
tion as a primary means of signalling stress. Subject 3, performance, intelligibility and prosody, have been illus-
the most severely involved speaker, was not taught to trated. The treatment approach outlined here was neces-
make durational adjustments because they were so dif- sitated by the unique pattern of ataxic speakers whose
ficult for him. Rather, he was taught simply to decrease chief deficit appears to be coordination of speech proc-
the frequency of loudness bursts which contributed to ess components rather than deficits specific to certain
the bizarreness of his prosodic pattern. This training was points along the vocal tract. This overall approach must,
carried out with feedback from a VU meter. The "con- of course, be s u p p l e m e n t e d by c o m p o n e n t consid-
trastive stress drill" similar to those described by Rosen- erations when consistent, specific problem areas can be
bek and LaPointe (1978) was the primary technique for identified. With dysarthrias other than the ataxic type,

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
404 Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46 398-404 N o v e m b e r 1981

other d e c i s i o n - m a k i n g processes m a y g u i d e treatment. intelligibility of cerebral palsied speech to the average lis-
F o r example, an a s s e s s m e n t of flaccid dysarthric speak- tener. Folia Phoniatrica, 1977, 29, 292-301.
BE~tRY, W. R., & GOSHO~N, E. L. Oscilloscopic feedback in the
ers f o c u s i n g on s p e e c h m e c h a n i s m c o m p o n e n t s m a y treatment of ataxic dysarthria. A paper presented at ASHA
h e l p to m a k e decisions about w h i c h c o m p o n e n t s should Convention, Atlanta, November, 1979.
be m a n a g e d p r o s t h e t i c a l l y and w h i c h s h o u l d be man- BEUKELMAN, D. R., ~ YORKSTON, K. M. A communication sys-
a g e d through training. Progress in such t r e a t m e n t can b e tem for the severely dysarthric speaker with an intact lan-
guage system. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
a s s e s s e d with m e a s u r e s of c h a n g e in specific compo-
1977, 42, 265-270.
n e n t s , p e r h a p s s u p p l e m e n t e d b y m e a s u r e m e n t s de- BEUKELMAN, D. R., & YORKSTON, K. M. Influence of passage
s i g n e d to assess the i m p a c t of c o m p o n e n t changes on familiarity on intelligibility estimates of dysarthric speech.
overall s p e e c h performance. Journal of Communication Disorders, 1980, 13, 33-41.
Although m a n y p r i n c i p l e s w e have o u t l i n e d are d r a w n BROWN, J. R., DARLEY, F. L., & ARONSON, A. E. Ataxie dysar-
thria. International Journal of Neurology, 1970, 7 (2-4), 302-
from c l i n i c a l e x p e r i e n c e a n d have b e e n successful at 318.
least to some extent for the subjects d e s c r i b e d , we be- CHUENG, J. Y., HOLDEN, A. D., & MINIFIE, F. D. Computer es-
lieve r e s e a r c h is n e c e s s a r y to verify and refine clinical timation and modeling of linguistic stress patterns in speech.
practices. F o r example, rate control strategies have re- Technical Report #108, Department of Electrical Engineer-
c e i v e d little attention. Clinical j u d g e m e n t s , preferring ing, University of Washington, 1973, p. 150.
HELM, N. A. Management of palilalia with a pacing board.
one rate control strategy over another, are m a d e w i t h o u t Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1979, 44, 350-353.
a clear understanding of the consequences of these KENT, R., ~ NETSELL, R. A case study of an ataxic dysarthric:
strategies. As clinicians, it is important to a n s w e r such Cineradiographic and spectrographic observations. Journal of
q u e s t i o n s as: Are t h e r e s p e a k e r s w h o n e e d to a d j u s t Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1975, 40, 115-134.
KENT, R. D., NETSELL, R., ~: ABBS, J. H. Acoustic characteris-
pause t i m e or articulation time s e l e c t i v e l y rather than
tics of dysarthria associated with cerebellum disease. Journal
r e d u c e overall s p e a k i n g rate, and, w h a t are the conse- of Speech and Hearing Research, 1979, 22, 627-648.
q u e n c e s of l e n g t h e n i n g pause t i m e and/or articulation LEHISTE, I. Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press,
time on i n t e l l i g i b i l i t y and p r o s o d y ? A n o t h e r area that 1970.
warrants attention is the a s s e s s m e n t of overall s p e e c h NETSELL, R. Speech Physiology. In F. D. Minifie, T. J. Hixon,
& F. Williams (Eds.), Normal Aspects of Speech, Hearing and
performance. T e c h n i q u e s to m e a s u r e i n t e l l i g i b i l i t y are Language. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973.
r e c e i v i n g i n c r e a s i n g attention (Andrews, Platt & Young, NETSELL, R., & DANIEL, B. Dysarthria in adults: Physiologic
1977; B e u k e l m a n & Yorkston, 1980; Yorkston & Beu- approach to rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine and
kelman, 1978, 1980). D e v e l o p m e n t a n d evaluation tech- Rehabilitation, 1979, 60, 502-508.
ROSENBEK, J. C., & LA POINTE, L. L. The dysarthrias: Descrip-
n i q u e s to m e a s u r e p r o s o d y are also necessary. tion, diagnosis and treatment. In D. F. Johns (Ed.), Clinical
Management of Neurogenic Communication Disorders, Bos-
ton: Little, Brown & Company, 1978.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS YOtlKSTON, K. M., & BEUKELMAN, D. R. A comparison of tech-
niques for measuring intelligibility of dysarthric speech.
Journal of Communication Disorders, 1978, 11, 499-512.
The preparation of this article was supported in part by Insti- YORKSTON,K. M., BEUKELMAN,D. R., & MINIFIE, F. D. Com-
tute of Handicapped Research Grant #16-P-56818-17. The au- puter analysis of some acoustic parameters of ataxie dysarthrie
thors wish to express their thanks to Patricia Mitsuda for her speech. A paper presented at ASHA Convention, Atlanta,
clinical assistance, to William Berry for personally discussing November, 1979.
certain aspects of this paper and to Dave Hooks for serving as
our normal speaker.
Received April 16, 1980
Accepted October 23, 1980

REFERENCES Requests for reprints should be sent to Kathryn M. Yorkston,


Ph.D., Dept. of Rehabilitation Medicine, RJ-30, University of
ANDREWS, G., PLATT, L. J., & YOUNG,M. Factors affecting the Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195.

Downloaded From: http://jshd.pubs.asha.org/ by a Western Michigan University User on 02/09/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like