You are on page 1of 10

Received: 4 May 2018 Revised: 6 October 2019 Accepted: 16 October 2019

DOI: 10.1002/cb.1793

ACADEMIC PAPER

The role of storytelling in advertising: Consumer emotion,


narrative engagement level, and word-of-mouth intention

Jin-Ae Kang1 | Sookyeong Hong2 | Glenn T. Hubbard1

1
School of Communication, College of Fine
Arts and Communication, East Carolina Abstract
University, Greenville, North Carolina This experimental study with a national online sample (n = 300) tested the effects of
2
Department of Media and Advertising, Hansei
storytelling in radio advertisements on participants' positive emotional responses and
University, Gunpo-Si, Republic of Korea
intentions to share information about the product, depending on audiences' narrative
Correspondence
engagement level. Treatments included a commercial for a fictitious brand of luggage
Sookyeong Hong, Department of Media and
Advertising, Hansei University, Hansei-Ro using a story told by the founder of the company, another version of the same com-
30, Gunpo-Si, Gyunggi-Do, Republic of Korea.
mercial manipulated so the speaker was a customer of the company, and a control
Email: paris2@hansei.ac.kr
stimulus consisting only of information about the product. Results showed that narra-
tive transportation and narrative preference are positively associated with favorable
responses toward ad. Stories elicited more favorable emotional responses and had
some effect on participants' intention to share information about the product by word-
of-mouth. This was especially true among participants hearing the founder's story.
Results support previous assumptions about the power of storytelling in advertising,
including distinctions regarding the identity of the speaker (founder vs. customer).

Storytelling has become a common technique for increasing the emo- transportation, identification, and preference for narrative in one's
tional power of advertising. This is because humans tend to store thinking. We chose to compare storytelling ads with nonstorytelling
information in the form of stories (Schank, 1999), making it easy to ads to measure storytelling effectiveness. We were interested in how
remember (Lundqvist, Liljander, Gummerus, & Van Riel, 2013). Thus, advertising effects vary depending on who the storyteller is and to
much consumer literature has assumed that storytelling in advertising what extent audiences immerse themselves into stories.
would be effective. Storytelling ads stimulate consumers' emotions as The purposes of this study are to explore (a) which type of people
they mentally combine stories with their own experiences are more likely to be affected by storytelling ads, and (b) how individ-
(i.e., Escalas, 2006). However, such effects do not seem to be fully uals are differently affected by storytelling ads according to narrative
born out in research. Literature points to three reasons: experimental transportation, preference, and the identities of storytellers in the ads.
design issues, measurement problems, and questionable variables. In order to exclude previously held perceptions of a brand, this study
First, it is not easy to design experiments capturing the effects of sto- created a fictitious brand and made different types of storytelling ads
rytelling as it is done in the real world. Researchers struggle to deter- for it. Specifically, this study examined how storytelling advertising
mine which message strategies to use and how to design them, since affects consumers' emotions and word-of-mouth (WOM) intentions.
there are too many message strategies and product options. Secondly,
it is not easy to establish which measures most accurately reflect story
effectiveness, which is somehow too subjective. Lastly, there are con- 1 | LI T E RA T U R E RE V I E W
ditioning factors that influence these experiments, such as brand
image, CEO image, and consumer involvement. Researchers grapple 1.1 | Emotional responses to advertising
with how best to control these variables.
With the understanding of these issues, this study delved into the Including Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), many scholars have assumed that
question of the effectiveness of storytelling in advertising, relying on humans are rational rather than impulsive and their consumption
literature on several key elements of narrative persuasion, such as behaviors would be also made by rational decision-making processes.

J Consumer Behav. 2020;19:47–56. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cb © 2020 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 47
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
48 KANG ET AL.

This notion supports the idea that having logical arguments helps supports linkages with WOM in promotional advertising. Botha and
achieve the best effects in advertising. Thus, informational advertising Reyneke (2013) find that viewers who have positive emotional reac-
often plays an important role as a source of product knowledge for tions to audiovisual content have more intention to share it. In a
information seekers (Lin & Chen, 2006). However, decisions made by decision-making process, “the emotional connection” to content is
consumers are often nonrational. People purchase a commodity not regarded as an essential element for consumers to share (p. 168).
because they need the product but because they want it. Desires for
self-expression or socialization often justify consumers' decisions
(Holbrook & Batra, 1987; Saenger, Thomas, & Johnson, 2013) in 1.3 | Storytelling and emotion in an advertising
which consumers are emotionally attached to a product. Emotion can context
play a role as a mediator in consumer responses. Advertisers consider
the role of emotion in the decision-making process in individual con- Storytelling is regarded as an effective advertising format to deliver
sumers' minds and diversify strategies so that an ad not only delivers messages and promote communication (Padgett & Allen, 1997).
information to satisfy consumer's cognitive demands but also creates Adaval and Wyer (1998) noted that potential customers imagine “the
positive emotional responses (i.e., Taylor, 1999). This is one reason sequence of events” in which a product is used rather than calculate
why scholars have measured emotional responses to test advertising the usefulness of the product, and a narrative form in a message is
effectiveness. more easily conveyed than an informational form” (p. 208). Like a fic-
In marketing and advertising literature, emotion has been broadly tional story, storytelling advertising is designed with various types of
conceptualized as either a mood state during the viewing of commer- plots, which give a “direction or intent of meaning” (Brooks, 1992,
cials (Friestad & Thorson, 1986; Srull, 1983) or a mental state of readi- p. xi). The plot connects the story to reality and tends to “organize
ness that arises from the cognitive appraisals that people make about reality and make sense of the world unfolding within the narrative”
commercials (Bagozzi, Gopinath, & Nyer, 1999; Friestad & Thorson, (Holley & Colyar, 2012, p. 116). Characters are created in the plot of a
1986). The present study uses the term emotional response to refer to story; the main characters usually appear with opponents, and helpers
the latter definition for following reasons: Feelings that audiences to resolve the problems or the conflicts in a story (Singer & Bluck,
experience fluctuate over time in nature, and it is difficult to influence 2001). Lakoff (2008) highlights the importance of emotion in a narra-
the mood state of an individual with a 30-s commercial. In addition, tive structure, arguing “narratives and frames are not just a brain
some scholars acknowledge emotion as a part of an attitudinal dimen- structure with intellectual content” but rather “with integrated
sion (Cwalina, Falkowski, & Kaid, 2005; Heath, 2001) that reflects a intellectual-emotional content” (p. 28). If an advertisement adopts
cognitive process. such narrative components as a format of storytelling advertising,
audiences would also go through narrative processing to respond to
the ad, which elicits emotional responses.
1.2 | Emotion and WOM intention The narrative format in advertising has been actively used espe-
cially in public service announcements such as campaigns pertaining
WOM is generally defined in marketing research as “informal commu- to organ donation, antismoking, and cancer prevention (Murphy,
nications directed at other consumers about the ownership, usage, or Frank, Chatterjee, & Baezconde-Garbanati, 2013; Shen, Sheer, & Li,
characteristics of particular goods and services and/or their sellers” 2015). Stories of “people like me” increase the relevancy of narratives
(Westbrook, 1987, p. 261). With the development of social media, to audience members' lives and make people aware that the events of
WOM has a much more significant impact on consumers' purchase a story are realistic enough to happen in their lives. Story format,
decision than other forms of marketing communication because social however, has hardly been tested in a commercial advertising context.
media creates an environment where consumers act as voluntary mar- It is unknown whether people engage in a narrative in commercial
keters sharing information directly through their digital networks advertising just as they process stories in public-service messages. In
(i.e., Goh, Heng, & Lin, 2013; Kimmel & Kitchen, 2014). Where the the next section, this study reviews several key constructs in narrative
interaction occurs face-to-face or online, the essence of the WOM is persuasion literature to incorporate them into a commercial advertis-
in the notion that communication occurs among peer consumers who ing context.
do not expect to receive compensation for the communication.
Researchers studying what motivates consumers' intentions to
share content (WOM) have found that emotion is one of the most influ- 1.4 | Narrative transportation
ential factors (Heath, Bell, & Sternberg, 2001). Berger and Milkman
(2013) emphasize the importance of compelling content and advise that Scholars have produced multiple transportation theories (i.e.,
using good content is more effective than targeting opinion leaders to Bilandzic & Busselle, 2011; Busselle & Bilandzic, 2009;
diffuse WOM. In this regard, consumers are willing to share content if it Csikszentmihalyi, 1997; Escalas, 2006), all sharing a basic idea of
appeals to their emotions (Berger & Milkman, 2012). “immersion into a text (p. 702),” as mentioned in one study by Green
Meiselman (2015) notes that recent studies on positive emotions and Brock (2002). Transportation into a narrative world is described
gained more attention. It is perhaps because positive emotion as, “the process of temporarily leaving one's reality behind and
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
KANG ET AL. 49

emerging from the experience somehow different from the person 1.6 | Identification
one was before entering the milieu of the narrative” (Green, Brock, &
Kaufman, 2004, p. 315). Identification has long been understood as factor in the potential impact
Scholars have paid attention to the fact that narrative trans- of a mediated message (i.e., Bandura, 2004). Social cognitive theory posits
portation proves persuasive through reducing negative cognitive that individuals tend to mimic behaviors that they have seen modeled
response and eliciting strong affective responses (Green & Brock, more than behaviors that have been recommended. People more readily
2002). In the narrative persuasion process, people are more likely accept messages demonstrated by models they like or want to be like.
to be persuaded by emotions rather than by strong arguments Thus, identification has been often conceptualized as perceived similarity
from the information provided in the story. Such a tendency is or wishful identification (Moyer-Gusé, 2008; Murphy et al., 2013). The
observed in both noncommercial and commercial messages. concepts of perceived similarity and likability are compatible with the
McQueen, Kreuter, Kalesan, and Alcaraz (2011) found among Afri- credibility of the endorser in advertising literature. Advertising has
can American women that an individual's level of transportation involved diverse spokespersons such as celebrities, experts, CEOs, and
was associated both with decreased counterarguing and increased customers. Likability and expertise of the spokesperson play vital roles in
interpersonal discussion about the breast cancer survivor stories. influencing consumers' purchase decisions (Till & Busler, 2000), which is
Escalas (2006) discovered that the more participants were trans- why celebrities and experts are recognized as desirable speakers in a tradi-
ported by their narrative self-referencing, the better they liked the tional ad. However, a new format of ad, storytelling advertising, employs
fictitious shoe brand in her experiment. Weak arguments per- CEOs and customers because the audience is more likely to accept them
taining to the brand message also did not negatively affect persua- as characters in a story. For example, some CEOs have adventurous
sion when people were led to the narrative thought process. stories of how they started their businesses. Ideally, consumers would
Therefore, it is logical to suggest that the effect of transportation wish to be like the CEOs in such stories (wishful identification).
is associated with positive emotional responses and behavioral On the other hand, customers share their experiences of products
outcomes such as a desire to share information about the ad by in a story format. Consumers perceive peer customers to be like them-
WOM. Thus, we propose the following hypotheses based on the selves (perceived similarity) because the peers are believed to care for
literature: consumers' interest, not for corporate profits. A spokesperson would be
H1a. Narrative transportation has a positive impact on participants' identified as a character in a storytelling ad. Thus, with both a CEO and
emotional responses toward advertising content. a customer character, there is a theoretical basis on which to ascertain a
H1b. Narrative transportation has a positive impact on WOM relationship between likability and listener favorability toward an ad's
intention. content, but there are notable differences between the two. It is for this
H1c. There is a mediation effect of emotional responses toward reason that we chose both a CEO and a customer as characters in the
advertising content between narrative transportation and WOM design of advertising stimuli, with the idea of testing the effectiveness
intention. of both and gaining at least preliminary insights into which is more
effective. Previous research has found that identification with narrative
characters showed a positive association with affective and cognitive
1.5 | Narrative preference involvement, and thus facilitated interpersonal discussion (Sood, 2002).
Thus, this study develops the following hypotheses.
Scholars have acknowledged that people use both analytical and nar- H3a. Emotional responses toward the ad content differ on the story-
rative thought processes when they interpret information by relating teller types in the ad that participant hears.
it to personal experience (i.e., Bruner, 1986; Escalas, 2006). A tradi- H3b. WOM intention differs depending on the storyteller types that
tional elaboration-based persuasion model emphasized the impor- the participant hears.
tance of logical thinking. However, persuasion is not always made H4. There is an interaction effect between storyteller types and nar-
solely based on fact-based arguments. Slater and Rouner (2002) noted rative transportation on (a) emotional responses toward the ad content
that sensation-seeking personality traits elicit preference for story- and (b) WOM intention.
based messages. High sensation-seekers are more responsive to mes- H5. There is an interaction effect between storyteller type and narra-
sages that are high in narrative elements such as drama, emotion, and tive preference on (a) emotional responses toward the ad content
event (Everett & Palmgreen, 1995). Although this study does not (b) WOM intention.
mainly explore differences in personal traits in narrative processing,
the research team decided to test one variable, narrative preference,
exploring its impact on emotional responses and WOM intention. 2 | METHODS
H2a. Narrative preference has a positive impact on participants'
emotional responses toward advertising content. 2.1 | Participants
H2b. Narrative preference has a positive impact on WOM intention.
H2c. There is a mediation effect of emotional responses toward We conducted a web-based experiment using the platform of
advertising content between narrative preference and WOM intention. Qualtrics. Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) online panel was used
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
50 KANG ET AL.

to recruit the research participants because online recruiting and test- or story elements and could not be embellished without adding infor-
ing are regarded as a valid procedure. Casler, Bickel, and Hackett mation not indicated in the story versions. Also, in the customer's
(2013) found that the MTurk online samples are more socioeconomi- story, the speaker introduces himself as “here to talk to you about…”
cally and ethnically diverse compared to other types of samples such instead of “the founder of…,” which accounts for the word count dif-
as in-lab participants and social media recruits. The researchers also ference between those two stimuli.
noted that the test results across the three samples were indistin-
guishable, which is consistent with prior research (i.e., Buhmester,
Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). In recruiting the MTurk panel, we allowed 2.3 | Measures
participation only by those who currently live in the US and whose
MTurk “approval rate” is 97% or above. A total of 300 participants 2.3.1 | Narrative transportation
were recruited in February 2018, of whom 149 (49.7%) were male,
and 151 were female (50.3%). The average age of the participants was Seven items were used to measure narrative transportation based
38.8, ranging from 18 to 73. on previous studies (Green & Brock, 2002; Brechman, 2010). How-
ever, we edited the wording according to the purpose of our study.
The items were used as follows: (a) While I was listening to the ad, I
2.2 | Experimental design and stimuli could easily picture what was described, (b) While I was listening to
the ad, I could picture myself experiencing what was described, (c) I
We used a posttest-only control design with two treatment groups was mentally involved in the ad while listening to it; (d) I had a hard
(storyteller types in the ad: a founder's story and a customer's story), time keeping my mind in listening to the ad (reversed), (e) My atten-
and one control group (a traditional informational ad). Regarding the tion was focused on the ad, (f ) It was easy to follow the action and
stimuli design, we created a fictitious travel luggage brand that has events taking place in the ad, and (g) I could easily image myself in a
never been on the market because a virtual product does not have similar situation to what is described in the ad: M = 4.16, SD = .65,
any prior reputation among consumers. To choose a product type and Cronbach's alpha = .863.
message strategy, we consulted the Six-Segment Strategy Wheel,
developed by Taylor (1999), which establishes six types of purchasing
decisions and places them according to their level of consumer 2.3.2 | Narrative preference
involvement and the extent to which they are associated with infor-
mational or transformational messages. Since our control stimulus had One item was newly created to measure narrative preference: In gen-
to be purely informational, it made sense to create messages on the eral, I prefer to receive information in the form of a story: M = 3.47,
informational side of the model. In terms of involvement, our research SD = 1.01, Cronbach's alpha = n/a.
team determined that we should choose a product category for which
a purchasing decision is important enough that a consumer would be
inclined to seek detailed information, but not such high involvement 2.3.3 | Types of storytellers in the ad
as to make it unrealistic that the person would make a purchase based
on a 30-s commercial. Therefore, we chose the acute need segment of This study tests three different types of advertising by storyteller
the strategy wheel and luggage as the product category. It was plausi- (Hong & Cho, 2016): (a) founder (CEO's story), (b) consumer's story,
ble for a character to tell a story of an acute need involving luggage and (c) nonstory informative advertising.
and for a narrator in a nonstory form to describe product characteris-
tics that would meet such a need.
In the design of story plot for the ad, we included one main char- 2.3.4 | Emotional response toward the advertising
acter and one event, which is the minimum element of a story content
(Genette & Levonas, 1976; Hong & Cho, 2016). Then, three different
versions of the ad were created. The appendix presents the full scripts The current study addresses the impact of positive emotion as an
of the stimuli. A male professional narrator recorded the three stimuli, advertising effect. While both positive and negative responses
and the same background music was used. All three stimuli were care- have potential impact on ad effectiveness, the particular types of
fully edited so only the manipulated variables were different among ads tested in this study focus on efforts to manipulate listener
the three versions of the commercial. Each ad included the fictitious responses to become more favorable toward a product. Thus, all
brand name three times. The word counts of the ads were 102 for the eight items in emotional response reflect positive aspects of emo-
founder's story, 107 for the customer's story, and 87 for the informa- tion. The items asked if the contents of the ad are (a) interesting,
tional ad, but the running time of all three ads was identical because (b) creative, (c) impressive, (d) different, (e) touching, (f ) meaningful,
the same length of background music was used, controlling a poten- (g) believable and (h) convincing. Cronbach's alpha was .914
tially confounding factor of different ad lengths. The reason for the (M = 3.29, SD = .87; range 1 [strongly disagree]–5 [strongly
varied word counts was that the informational ad did not include plot agree]).
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
KANG ET AL. 51

2.3.5 | WOM intention 3 | RE SU LT S

The WOM intention construct used three items as follows: (a) I would Our research team used Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Hypoth-
like to talk to people about the product, (b) I would recommend the lug- eses 1 and 2, analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Hypothesis 3, and mul-
gage to my friends, and (c) I would like to tell the story of the ad to people tivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) for Hypotheses 4 and
around me. Cronbach's alpha was for the reliability check was .911 5. First, Hypotheses 1a and 1b examine the impact of narrative trans-
(M = 2.61; SD = 1.07; range: 1–5). portation on participants' positive emotional responses toward the ad
content and WOM intention. Hierarchical regression analysis showed
that both narrative transportation (H1a: β = .495, p < .001) and narra-
2.4 | Procedure tive preference (H2a: β = .336, p < .001) lead to stronger emotional
response and WOM intention respectively at the statistically signifi-
2.4.1 | Pretest cant level (Table 1).
Second, Hypotheses 2a and 2b tested the impact of narrative
A pretest was conducted in two steps. The first one was conducted preference on emotional responses and WOM intention. Hierar-
among college students (n = 25) to correct inappropriate wording or chical regression analysis demonstrated that narrative preference
ambiguous terms. The second was launched with a small set of has a positive impact on participants' emotional responses (H2a:
MTurk panelists (n = 30) to double-check any possible issues in the β = .274, p < .001) and WOM intention (H2b: β = .251, p < .001),
flow of the online questionnaire through MTurk recruitment. After respectively.
the second pretest, we inserted two safeguards to make sure partici- H1c and H2c examined mediation effects of emotional responses
pants did not skip listening to one of the stimuli before answering toward the advertising content between (H1c) narrative transporta-
the questions, and whether the participants carefully read the ques- tion and (H2c) narrative preference, and WOM intention. Two multi-
tionnaire. The administration of the pretest took approximately ple regressions were conducted to see the mediation effect of
4.5 minutes. Those who could not listen to the audio advertising emotional response, and gender and age effects were controlled. The
stimuli, or who failed to answer the validity check question correctly, first analysis showed that both narrative transportation (β = .495,
were screened out and not allowed to complete the rest of the p < .001) and narrative preference (β = .274, p < .001) have a positive
questions. The responses of 11 people out of 311 were excluded effect on participants' positive emotional responses toward the ad
from the analysis. (R2 = .39, p < .000). However, the effect of narrative transportation
and narrative preference became insignificant when the multiple
regression was conducted with both narrative factors and emotional
2.4.2 | Main procedure response level on WOM intention. Instead, only the effect of emo-
tional response toward the ad content was found to be significant
The invitation to our study was sent only to qualified MTurk regarding WOM intention (βnarrative transportation = .06, p = n.s.; βnarrative
panelists. Once the panel agreed to participate in our study, the preference = 1.36, p = n.s.; βemotional responses = .671, p < .001; R2 = .473).
participants were randomly assigned to one of the three stimuli. Therefore, H1c and H2c found that emotion toward the ad content
Then, they continued to answer our questions. The random has a mediation effect between narrative engagement and WOM
block design function in Qualtrics was used to set a random intention. In sum, all H1s and H2s were confirmed.
assignment. Hypothesis 3s examined whether the types of storytellers in the
advertisement would yield different levels of positive emotional
responses toward the advertising content and WOM intention.
2.4.3 | Manipulation check ANOVA test results showed that there was a significant difference in
terms of emotional responses (H3a: F = 3.56, df = 2, p < .05) and
The manipulation check demonstrated our story design was per- based on different storytellers. Post-hoc results demonstrated that
2
ceived in the way we intended (χ = 461.341, df = 4, p = .00), with the founder's story elicited positive emotional responses more than
95.1% (n = 98 out of 103) of the respondents who were exposed to the nonstory (informational) advertisement (Mfounder's story = 3.47,
the founder's story correctly perceiving the story as the one with Mnon-story = 3.15, p < .05). An Additional ANOVA test led us to
the founder speaking. Also, 83% (82 out of 99) who listened to the observe how the different types of storytellers in the ads would elicit
customer's story correctly answered that the story was a customer's different emotions. The results show that the founder's story was per-
testimonial. Finally, 97% (95 out of 98) of the respondents who lis- ceived as more creative (F = 8.452, df = 2, p < .001), touching
tened to the informational ad responded that the story only con- (F = 10.394, df = 2, p < .001), and meaningful (F = 3.653, df = 2,
veyed information. In the main analysis, we only used the data of p = .027 < .05) compared to the informational ad (control group). The
the respondents who correctly perceived the storyteller in the founder's story was also perceived as a more believable story com-
advertisement. Therefore, the total number of valid responses was pared to the customer story (F = 3.525, df = 2, p = <.05). Therefore,
reduced to 275 from 300. Hypothesis 3a is confirmed.
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
52 KANG ET AL.

TABLE 1 Hierarchical regression analysis of narrative transportation and narrative preference on emotional response and word-of-mouth

DV: Emotional response toward ad content DV: Word-of-mouth (WOM)

Independent variables β R change


2
β R2 change
Step 1 1. Age .146* 3.7** .061 .00
2. Gender .145* .061
Step 2 Narrative transportation .495*** 39.0*** .336*** 21.7***
Narrative preference .274*** .251***
Step 3 Emotional response toward ad content N/A .671*** 25.6***
Total R2 (%) 42.7*** 47.3***

Notes: n = 275; gender (0 = female; 1 = male).


*p < .05.
**p < .01.
***p < .001.

Regarding Hypothesis 3b, an ANOVA test did not find different Mcustomer's story = 3.602, Mnon-story = 3.194). However, nonstory format
WOM intention resulting from different storyteller types in the adver- advertising was found to be emotionally appealing to the experiment
tising (F = 2.735, df = 2, p = n.s.). Post Hoc test results, however, dem- participants (M = 3.096) more than the founder's story (M = 2.958)
onstrated that participants who were exposed to the founder's story and customer's story (M = 2.770). Overall, the results indicated that
showed the highest level of WOM intention (M = 2.81) compared to the founder's story was the most appealing to those who like story
the customer's story (M = 2.49, p < .05) and the nonstory format formats for information. These findings are consistent with the
advertising (M = 2.50, p < .05). Thus, Hypothesis 3b is partially results of H3a. From Hypotheses 3s and 5, we would conclude that
supported. the founder's story elicits the most favorable results in terms of
Hypothesis 4 tested interaction effects between storyteller emotional responses.
types and narrative transportation on (a) emotional responses
toward the ad content and (b) WOM intention. Since the results
of our hypotheses H1c and H2c indicated that emotional 4 | DI SCU SSION AND CO NCLUSIO N
responses toward the ad content and WOM intention were posi-
tively associated with each other, we concluded that a two-way The current study explores in which context storytelling works best in
MANCOVA serves best to investigate an interaction effect terms of advertising effectiveness. We verify that key concepts in
between storyteller type and narrative transportation. This test processing a narrative such as transportation and identification still
demonstrated an interaction effect between narrative transporta- determine the storytelling advertising effect in a commercial context
tion and storyteller type on participants' positive emotional just as they worked in a narrative-based public service announcement
responses toward the advertising content and WOM intention (McQueen et al., 2011). At the same time, we studied the role of sto-
(F[4, 532] = 2.86, p < .05; Wilks' Λ = .958). However, tests of ryteller in advertising effectiveness, specifically whether it makes a
between-subject effects did not specify the interaction effect of difference that the storyteller portrayed in an ad is the owner of a
storyteller types and transportation on each of the emotional company or its customer. This study particularly enhances our under-
responses and WOM in a separate way. standing of how emotion plays a role in the effectiveness of storytell-
Hypothesis 5 examined interaction effects between storyteller ing advertising, as part of the framework of narrative persuasion. In
types and narrative preference on (a) emotional responses toward the this sense, the current study adds empirical evidence to the claim that
ad content and (b) WOM intention, respectively. Another MANCOVA narrative in storytelling advertising improves branding and promo-
test demonstrated an interaction effect between storyteller type and tional efforts (Padgett & Allen, 1997).
narrative preference on participants' positive emotional responses We found that research participants differently interpreted
toward the ad content and WOM intention (F[4, 532] = 3.12, p < .01; stories in advertising. The more they imagined themselves in the
Wilks' Λ = .955). Tests of between-subject effects provided detailed scene of the story (transportation), the more likely they were to per-
snapshots of the interactions. In particular, an interaction effect ceive the ad as creative, touching, and meaningful. In this case, people
between narrative preference and storyteller type was found regarding could easily feel empathy toward the main characters in the situation
the emotional responses (F[2, 267] = 6.246, p < .01, μ2 = .045), not on of the plot. The emotions that the main characters carried in the ad
the WOM intentions. These findings are somehow consistent with the were transferred to the listeners. The main character's experiences
results of H2s. Overall, the storyteller type of ad was found to be effec- were perceived as something that the listeners could experience in
tive in eliciting emotional responses, when the participants preferred to their lives, which would be explained by the concept of perceived simi-
receive information in a story format (Figure 1: Mfounders story = 3.776, larity in identification.
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
KANG ET AL. 53

F I G U R E 1 The interaction effect between


narrative preference and storyteller types on
positive emotional responses toward the ad
content [Colour figure can be viewed at
wileyonlinelibrary.com]

However, our study also demonstrated that persuasive messages


in a narrative format might be effective with certain types of people.
Not surprisingly, the results of our study reported that storytelling
advertising is more effective for those who prefer to receive informa-
tion in a story format. Our study, however, did not explore which
types of individuals prefer story format information compared to non-
story format information such as rational arguments and reasoning. In
fact, such analytical and narrative thinking processes are regarded as a FIGURE 2 Proposed model of storytelling advertising effect
different “route” of information processing (Bruner, 1986), but it [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
would not necessarily mean that those thought processes are mutu-
ally exclusive. Therefore, future studies could address how the analyti- tendency to prefer a special character like a founder in the story, rather
cal and narrative thought processes work together or separately in than a customer character just like oneself (perceived similarity). Therefore,
the interpretation of story based advertising messages. Future studies the results provide practical implications that a company, whose CEO has
also could explore the characteristics or personalities of those who an interesting story could strategically use the story in branding and pro-
prefer story format information. motion. However, the current study did not address which characteristics
Second, our study found that emotion played a crucial role in in the founder or customer of the adelicited different emotional out-
motivating individuals to talk about the ads (WOM), which is consis- comes. It will be meaningful if future studies explore how a storyteller's
tent with previous research findings in narrative persuasion literature perceived traits such as credibility and attractiveness of the storyteller
(Green & Brock, 2002). Consumers are more likely to be persuaded by produce different outcomes in ad effects.
affective outcomes rather than by strong arguments from the infor- Lastly, our additional statistical analysis indicated that women
mation provided in an ad (Escalas, 2006). The mediation effect were more likely to have positive emotions toward the founder's story
observed at H1c and H2c suggested that the storytelling ad did not compared to the customer's story or the informational advertising.
yield a behavioral outcome intention without creating emotional reac- Although gender was one of the control variables in this study, litera-
tions among the advertising listeners. Such a finding could be note- ture notes that women are more likely to be open-minded in
worthy to advertising producers who want to use a story format in expressing their emotion (Simon & Nath, 2004). Nevertheless, the
their ads. Thus, we propose a storytelling advertising effect model findings among the female audiences should be considered in the con-
(Figure 2), which provides a comprehensive framework in which story- text that our study used a male voice for the advertising stimuli.
telling formats work with narrative levels and emotion. Since testing Therefore, the effect of gender differences among audience members,
the model goes beyond the scope of the current study, we suggest story characters, and narrators in storytelling advertising needs to be
future studies elaborate and test the model. tested with a comprehensive approach within theoretical frameworks.
Third, our study also brought an interesting finding that the founder's
story is more likely to create positive emotions compared to the cus-
tomer's story or purely informational ad. Our literature review supported 4.1 | Limitations
the idea that recognition of the CEO in the ad would be related to the
concept of wishful identification and the customer in the ad would appeal Although our study has several contributions to the body of knowl-
to perceived similarity. Our findings seem to support the audiences' edge on storytelling effects in advertising, there are limitations worthy
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
54 KANG ET AL.

of consideration in future studies. First, our experiment tested adver- Escalas, J. E. (2006). Self-referencing and persuasion: Narrative transporta-
tising for only one product. Also, the message was created within the tion versus analytical elaboration. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(4),
421–429.
category of acute need in Taylor's (1999) advertising message strategy
Everett, M., & Palmgreen, P. (1995). Influences of sensation seeking, mes-
wheel, so it is possible other types of ads or other types of purchasing sage sensation value, and program context on effectiveness of anti-
decisions might yield different results. Lastly, we only focused on cocaine public service announcements. Health Communication, 7,
WOM intention as a behavioral outcome in this study and did not 225–248.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An
observe purchase intention as an advertising effect. WOM is regarded
introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
as one of the most credible indicators of purchase behavior in con- Pub. Co.
sumer research (Sundaram, Mitra, & Webster, 1998), but further study Friestad, M., & Thorson, E. (1986). Emotion-eliciting advertising: Effects on
is needed in order to contextualize the results of the present study. long term memory and judgment. ACR North American Advances, 7,
111–116.
Future research can take a more comprehensive approach to test the
Genette, G., & Levonas, A. (1976). Boundaries of narrative. New Literary
effect of storytelling advertising on both communication and behav- History, 8(1), 1–13.
ioral levels. Nevertheless, our study is a step toward improving under- Goh, K. Y., Heng, C. S., & Lin, Z. (2013). Social media brand community and
standing of how storytelling in advertising leads to affective and consumer behavior: Quantifying the relative impact of user-and
marketer-generated content. Information Systems Research, 24(1),
behavioral outcomes.
88–107.
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind's eye: Transportation-
ORCID imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, &
Jin-Ae Kang https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2483-4598 T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations
Sookyeong Hong https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7620-1667 (pp. 315–341). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understanding media
enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative worlds. Communi-
RE FE R ENC E S cation Theory, 14(4), 311–327.
Adaval, R., & Wyer, R. S. (1998). The role of narratives in consumer infor- Heath, C., Bell, C., & Sternberg, E. (2001). Emotional selection in memes:
mation processing. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7(3), 207–245. The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Bagozzi, R. P., Gopinath, M., & Nyer, P. U. (1999). The role of emotions in 81(6), 1028–1041.
marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 184–206. Heath, R. (2001), The hidden power of advertising: How low involvement
Bandura, A. (2004). Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health processing influences in way we choose brands (Admap Monograph;
Education & Behavior, 31(2), 143–164. No. 7). Henley-on-Thames: World Advertising Research Centre (WARC).
Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Holbrook, M. B., & Batra, R. (1987). Assessing the role of emotions as
Media Psychology, 12(4), 321–347. mediators of consumer responses to advertising. Journal of Consumer
Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2012). What makes online content viral? Jour- Research, 14(3), 404–420.
nal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192–205. Holley, K., & Colyar, J. (2012). Under construction: How narrative ele-
Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2013). Emotion and virality: What makes ments shape qualitative research. Theory Into Practice, 51(2), 114–121.
online content go viral? GfK Marketing Intelligence Review, 5(1), 18–23. Hong, S. Y., & Cho, S. H. (2016). The effect of brand storytelling based on
Bilandzic, H., & Busselle, R. W. (2011). Enjoyment of films as a function of the subject of story. The Journal of the Korea Contents Association, 16
narrative experience, perceived realism and transportability. Communi- (7), 112–121.
cations, 36(1), 29–50. Kimmel, A. J., & Kitchen, P. J. (2014). WOM and social media: Presaging
Botha, E., & Reyneke, M. (2013). To share or not to share: The role of con- future directions for research and practice. Journal of Marketing Com-
tent and emotion in viral marketing. Journal of Public Affairs, 13(2), munications, 20(1–2), 5–20.
160–171. Lakoff, G. (2008). The political mind: Why you can't understand 21st-century
Brechman, J. M. (2010). Narrative “flow”: A model of narrative processing politics with an 18th-century brain. New York, NY: Penguin.
and its impact on information processing, knowledge acquisition, and Lin, L. Y., & Chen, C. S. (2006). The influence of the country-of-origin
persuasion. (Unpublished Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from image, product knowledge and product involvement on consumer pur-
http://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/204 chase decisions: An empirical study of insurance and catering services
Brooks, P. (1992). Reading for the plot: Design and intention in narrative. in Taiwan. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 23(5), 248–265.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Lundqvist, A., Liljander, V., Gummerus, J., & Van Riel, A. (2013). The impact
Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard of storytelling on the consumer brand experience: The case of a firm-
University Press. originated story. Journal of Brand Management, 20(4), 283–297.
Buhmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical McQueen, A., Kreuter, M. W., Kalesan, B., & Alcaraz, K. I. (2011). Under-
Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives standing narrative effects: The impact of breast cancer survivor stories
on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. on message processing, attitudes, and beliefs among African American
Casler, K., Bickel, L., & Hackett, E. (2013). Separate but equal? A compari- women. Health Psychology, 30(6), 674–682.
son of participants and data gathered via Amazon's MTurk, social Meiselman, H. L. (2015). A review of the current state of emotion research
media, and face-to-face behavioral testing. Computers in Human Behav- in product development. Food Research International, 76, 192–199.
ior, 29(6), 2156–2160. Moyer-Gusé, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion:
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement Explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education mes-
with everyday life. New York, NY: Basic Books. sages. Communication Theory, 18(3), 407–425.
Cwalina, W., Falkowski, A., & Kaid, L. L. (2005). Advertising and the image Murphy, S. T., Frank, L. B., Chatterjee, J. S., & Baezconde-Garbanati, L.
of politicians in evolving and established democracies: Comparative (2013). Narrative versus nonnarrative: The role of identification, trans-
study of the Polish and the US presidential elections in 2000. Journal portation, and emotion in reducing health disparities. Journal of Com-
of Political Marketing, 4(2–3), 19–44. munication, 63(1), 116–137.
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
KANG ET AL. 55

Padgett, D., & Allen, D. (1997). Communicating experiences: A narrative


approach to creating service brand image. Journal of Advertising, 26(4), community and social capital, community branding and environ-
49–62.
mental communication. PR ethics and its role as an internal/exter-
Saenger, C., Thomas, V. L., & Johnson, J. W. (2013). Consumption-focused
self-expression word of mouth: A new scale and its role in consumer nal change agent. Dr Kang has published in PR Review, Journal of
research. Psychology & Marketing, 30(11), 959–970. Communication Management, Journalism & Mass Communication
Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic memory revisited. Cambridge: Cambridge Educator and Journal of Disaster Studies among others. She is a
University Press.
public relations concentration coordinator at the ECU school of
Shen, F., Sheer, V. C., & Li, R. (2015). Impact of narratives on persuasion in
health communication: A meta-analysis. Journal of Advertising, 44(2), communication.
105–113.
Sookyeong Hong received the doctor's degree in the School of
Simon, R. W., & Nath, L. E. (2004). Gender and emotion in the United States:
Do men and women differ in self-reports of feelings and expressive Communication & Information from University of Paris 2, France.
behavior? American Journal of Sociology, 109(5), 1137–1176. She is an associate professor at Hansei University, Korea and cur-
Singer, J. A., & Bluck, S. (2001). New perspectives on autobiographical rently a visiting scholar at East Carolina University. Her research
memory: The integration of narrative processing and autobiographical
field is a multicultural society, media content analysis, risk commu-
reasoning. Review of General Psychology, 5(2), 91–99.
Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (2002). Entertainment—education and elabora- nication, and storytelling. She published over 40 research articles
tion likelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. and several books. She was a chair of the Department of Journal-
Communication Theory, 12(2), 173–191. ism and Broadcasting at Hansei University and a board member
Sood, S. (2002). Audience involvement and entertainment—education.
of the National Election Broadcasting Debate Commission
Communication Theory, 12(2), 153–172.
Srull, T. K. (1983). Affect and memory: The impact of affective reactions in (2008–2017) and the Korea Press Arbitration Commission
advertising on the representation of product information in memory. (2011–2014). Also, she is a media critic and a writer.
ACR North American Advances., 10, 520–525.
Sundaram, D. S., Mitra, K., & Webster, C. (1998). Word-of-mouth commu- Glenn T. Hubbard is an associate professor at East Carolina Uni-
nications: A motivational analysis. ACR North American Advances., 25, versity in Greenville, North Carolina. He has published research
527–531. on the effects of production elements and locality of origination
Taylor, R. E. (1999). A six-segment message strategy wheel. Journal of
and ownership on listener responses to radio and audio media.
Advertising Research, 39(6), 7–17.
Till, B. D., & Busler, M. (2000). The match-up hypothesis: Physical attrac- Also, he has published on the subject of convergent journalism
tiveness, expertise, and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase education and mass media pedagogy. Before entering academe,
intent and brand beliefs. Journal of Advertising, 29(3), 1–13. Hubbard was a large-market radio news anchor and reporter. He
Westbrook, R. A. (1987). Product/consumption-based affective responses
also has a background as a producer and engineer in music, with
and post-purchase processes. Journal of Marketing Research, 24
several albums to his credit. He teaches television production and
(August), 258–270.
performance, as well as journalism history and media law.

AUTHOR BIOGRAPHI ES

How to cite this article: Kang J-A, Hong S, Hubbard GT. The
Jin-Ae Kang is an associate professor of Communication at East
role of storytelling in advertising: Consumer emotion, narrative
Carolina University. Her research has focused on the impact of
engagement level, and word-of-mouth intention. J Consumer
emerging media on PR strategies, and health-related crisis man-
Behav. 2020;19:47–56. https://doi.org/10.1002/cb.1793
agement. Her research areas also include the role of PR in building
14791838, 2020, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.1793 by University Of Southampton, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
56 KANG ET AL.

APPENDIX A.

Stimuli: Audio advertising scripts

Founder story: Treatment 1

Traveling is hard enough without your luggage letting you down. I'm Dan, the founder of Dura. I learned the importance of good luggage on my
honeymoon trip to Hawaii. I had to pack for 2 weeks and could barely fit everything in my suitcase. The zipper broke in the airport and my stuff fell
all over the floor. Then, I almost fell down an escalator because a wheel jammed. I needed something different—and that's why I started a luggage
company…. Dura. Reliable, spacious, and built to last. I travel with confidence now, and so can you. For more information go to Dura.com. (102
words)
Customer story: Treatment 2

Traveling is hard enough without your luggage letting you down. I'm Dan, here to talk to you about Dura. I learned the importance of good luggage on
my honeymoon trip to Hawaii. I had to pack for 2 weeks and could barely fit everything in my suitcase. The zipper broke in the airport and my stuff
fell all over the floor. Then, I almost fell down an escalator because a wheel jammed. I needed something different—and that's when I found out
about Dura. Reliable, spacious, and built to last. I travel with confidence now, and so can you. For more information go to Dura.com. (107 words)
Non-story (informational) advertising: Control group

Traveling is hard enough without your luggage letting you down. The solution? Dura. Luggage that's spacious and flexible enough to fit everything in
for a long trip. Sturdy materials? It has them. Wheels that don't jam or break? Definitely. It even has a metal chain zipper—with teeth that interlock
perfectly—so it's extremely durable—and the inline skate polyurethane wheels are solid, resilient and flexible. You can travel with confidence now,
with Dura. Reliable, spacious, and built to last. For more information, go to Dura.com. (87 words)

You might also like