You are on page 1of 18

Sociology of Education http://soe.sagepub.

com/

Leveling the Home Advantage: Assessing the Effectiveness of Parental Involvement in Elementary
School
Thurston Domina
Sociology of Education 2005 78: 233
DOI: 10.1177/003804070507800303

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://soe.sagepub.com/content/78/3/233

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

American Sociological Association

Additional services and information for Sociology of Education can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://soe.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://soe.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://soe.sagepub.com/content/78/3/233.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Jul 1, 2005

What is This?

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage:
Assessing the Effectiveness of
Parental Involvement in Elementary
School
Thurston Domina
City University of New York

In the past two decades, a great deal of energy has been dedicated to improving children’s
education by increasing parents’ involvement in school. However, the evidence on the effec-
tiveness of parental involvement is uneven. Whereas policy makers and theorists have assumed
that parental involvement has wide-ranging positive consequences, many studies have shown
that it is negatively associated with some children’s outcomes. This article uses data from the
children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 to estimate time-lagged growth
models of the effect of several types of parental involvement on scores on elementary school
achievement tests and the Behavioral Problems Index. The findings suggest that parental
involvement does not independently improve children’s learning, but some involvement activ-
ities do prevent behavioral problems. Interaction analyses suggest that the involvement of par-
ents with low socioeconomic status may be more effective than that of parents with high
socioeconomic status.

I
n the past two decades, a great deal of been even more pronounced. A 1995–96 sur-
research and policy-making activity has vey by the National Center for Education
been dedicated to increasing the involve- Statistics showed that nearly all public ele-
ment of parents in schools. Parental-involve- mentary and middle schools in the United
ment initiatives have been a mainstay of fed- States sponsored activities that were
eral educational policy since the Reagan designed to foster parental involvement.
administration’s 1986 Goals 2000: Educate According to the survey, 97 percent of
America Act. In 1996, the Clinton administra- schools invited parents to attend an open
tion reauthorized the Elementary and house or back-to-school night, 92 percent
Secondary Education Act, adding a new pro- scheduled parent-teacher conferences, 96
vision that required the nation’s poorest percent hosted arts events, 85 percent spon-
schools to spend at least 1 percent of their sored athletic events, and 84 percent had sci-
Title I supplementary federal funds to develop ence fairs (Carey et al. 1998). Throughout the
educational “compacts” between families United States, parental-involvement initia-
and schools. Likewise, increasing parental tives have been central to state- and dis-
involvement in schools is one of the six cen- trictwide school reform efforts, most notably
tral goals of the Bush administration’s 2002 in Baltimore, Chicago, and Philadelphia
No Child Left Behind Act. (Epstein 2001; Fine 1993; Wallace and
At the state and local levels, interest and Walberg 1991). In 2003–04, New York City
activity surrounding parental involvement has schools chancellor Joel Klein appropriated

Sociology of Education 2005, Vol. 78 (July): 233–249 233

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


234 Domina

$43 million to hire parent coordinators to children’s cognitive achievement and their
arrange school events and contact parents in behavioral problems. The NLSY79’s longitudi-
each of the system’s 1,200 schools (Gootman nal design makes it possible to place parental
2003). activities in the context of children’s earlier
The spread of parental-involvement poli- achievement and behavior. Using a repeated-
cies reflects the application of key insights measures approach, I estimated the effects of
from the sociology of education to the day- parental involvement on achievement and
to-day operation of American schools. As behavior, net of earlier achievement and
Epstein (1986, 1987a, 1987b) argued, behavior. Finally, I used interaction terms to
parental-involvement efforts acknowledge determine whether the effects of parental-
the crucial role that families and communities involvement activities vary by parents’ socioe-
play in children’s education. They attempt to conomic status (SES).
moderate upper- and middle-class students’
home advantage by bringing all families,
regardless of social class or race, into the daily PREVIOUS RESEARCH
life of the school. Parental-involvement poli-
cies seek to redistribute cultural and social Given the popularity of parental-involvement
capital, boosting the resources that are avail- initiatives as a tool for school reform, it is sur-
able to disadvantaged children. They are prising to note that research on the link
thought to foster social closure by creating between involvement and school success has
opportunities for parents, teachers, and been inconclusive. A few national studies
administrators to network and share informa- have associated high levels of parental
tion with one another. involvement with improved educational out-
But does parental involvement deserve the comes for children (Fehrmann, Keith, and
faith that the public has invested in it? Reimers 1987; Stevenson and Baker 1987;
Researchers have generally agreed that Useem 1992), but others have reported that
parental-involvement activities are associated parental involvement in education is nega-
with stronger educational outcomes, but it is tively related to children’s educational out-
not clear that these activities cause educa- comes (Fan 2001; Milne et al. 1986). Even
tional success (A. Baker and Soden 1998; within studies, the results have often been
Downey 2002). Indeed, evidence regarding mixed, with the observed effects of parental
the effectiveness of parental involvement has involvement depending on which aspects of
been mixed and largely discouraging. Many involvement and which educational out-
studies have suggested that the parental- comes have been considered (Crosnoe
involvement activities that are most frequent- 2001b; Ho and Willms 1996; Keith 1991;
ly targeted by schools have little or no direct Miedel and Reynolds 1999; Muller 1993;
influence on children’s educational outcomes; Singh et al. 1995).
others have indicated that the effectiveness of Indeed, a review of multivariate studies of
parental involvement may be conditional on the effectiveness of parental involvement yield-
parents’ race and class. However, these find- ed no single parental-involvement activity that
ings may be misleading. Past researchers on was consistently linked to favorable children’s
parental involvement have focused on cogni- outcomes. For example, whereas Catsambis
tive and academic outcomes, neglecting the (1998), Muller (1993) and Ho and Willms
effect of involvement on children’s behavior. (1996) reported positive effects of parents’ at-
Furthermore, most have studied middle home educational supervision, Desimone
school and high school students, rather than (2001) reported negative effects, and Fan
elementary school students. (2001) and McNeal (1999) reported nonsignif-
I used data on the elementary school-aged icant findings. Even within studies, the estimat-
children of the National Longitudinal Survey ed effects of particular parental-involvement
of Youth 1979 cohort (NLSY79) to investigate activities have varied according to the outcome
the relationship between six forms of parental measured. For example, Desimone (2001) and
involvement and two distinct outcomes— McNeal (1999) found that attendance at

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 235

Parent-Teacher Association (PTA) meetings is middle- and high school students. This focus
associated with positive values on some chil- may have caused researchers to underesti-
dren’s outcomes and negative values on others. mate the effects of parental involvement.
The one area in which recent studies have Parents’ involvement in school tends to
seemed to agree is one in which involvement decline as children age (Catsambis and
has a counterintuitive negative effect on chil- Garland 1997; Crosnoe 2001a), as does its
dren’s educational outcomes; Catsambis effectiveness (Catsambis and Suazo-Garcia
(1998), Desimone (2001), Fan (2001), Muller 2000; Muller 1998; Singh et al. 1995). The
(1993), and Ho and Willms (1996) all reported only relatively recent study that analyzed the
a significant, negative association between par- involvement of parents of elementary school
ents’ educational contacts with schools and students using quantitative methods (Miedel
children’s educational outcomes. and Reynolds 1999) found that parental
Studies that have used other methodolo- involvement improves the reading scores of
gies have yielded similarly ambiguous find- kindergarteners but has no effect on the read-
ings. Among qualitative studies, Comer ing scores of high school students.
(1980), Comer and Haynes (1991), Epstein Furthermore, most parental-involvement pol-
(2001), and Lareau (1989) reported positive icy-making activity has focused on increasing
effects of parental involvement. Fine (1993) the involvement of children in elementary
and Reay (1998), however, argued that school (Chen and Chandler 2001). It is possi-
observed associations between parental par- ble that these initiatives could be effective,
ticipation and children’s educational perfor- even if the effects of parental involvement on
mance are really artifacts of the class and high school-aged children are negligible. This
racial advantages that involved parents bring article addresses this oversight in the litera-
to the table. The evidence from evaluations of
ture by estimating the implications of
parental-involvement programs has been no
parental-involvement activities for a sample of
more encouraging. Mattingly et al. (2002)
elementary school students.
reviewed the evaluations of 41 school- and
district-level parental-involvement programs
and concluded that there is little empirical Conceptualizing Parental
evidence to support the claim that schools’ Involvement and Its Effects
efforts to improve parental involvement ulti-
Influenced by Epstein’s (1992) typology of
mately improve students’ outcomes. Only
parental involvement—a typology that
half the parental-involvement studies that
includes basic parental roles like keeping chil-
Mattingly et al. considered to be adequately
dren safe, as well as higher-level involvement
designed found positive effects of parental-
involvement programs. activities, such as collaborating with commu-
nity organizations—researchers on parental
involvement have considered a variety of
activities as examples of parental involve-
EXPLANATIONS FOR THE ment. By contrast, analyses of the implica-
UNEVEN EFFECTS tions of involvement have been narrowly
focused on children’s cognitive and academic
Given the resources that have been dedicated achievement. As McNeal (1999) pointed out,
to parental involvement, these findings are dis- this tight analytic focus may come at a cost.
couraging. However, previous research on McNeal argued that the conceptual ties
parental involvement may have underestimat- between most forms of parental involvement
ed its effect by neglecting the following four and children’s learning are weak compared to
considerations. the ties between parental involvement and
children’s behavior.
Differential Effects by Students’ Ages Involvement may be expected to influence
children’s outcomes via three mechanisms:
Nearly all past research on the effectiveness of
parental involvement has been conducted on • Parental involvement socializes. When, for exam-

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


236 Domina

ple, parents supervise their children’s home- Separating the Causes and Effects
work, they convey the importance of school-
ing.
of Involvement

• Second, parental involvement generates social


Researchers have often explained negative
control. Parents who attend PTA meetings and and nonsignificant associations between par-
volunteer in school develop relationships with ents’ involvement in school and children’s
their children’s teachers and the parents of outcomes by arguing that the direction of
their children’s classmates. These relationships causal effects between involvement and edu-
make it easier for parents to monitor children’s cational outcomes is ambiguous. According
behavior and teachers’ practices. to this argument, when children are experi-
encing difficulties in school, their parents are
• Parental involvement gives parents access to
insider information. When children have prob- more likely to schedule meetings with teach-
lems at school, involved parents learn about ers and to become involved, but when chil-
these problems earlier and know more about dren are succeeding in school, their parents
available solutions. tend to relax their involvement in school
(Crosnoe 2001a; McNeal 1999; Muller 1998;
McNeal argued that since the mechanisms Sanders 1998).This explanation suggests that
vary, different types of parental involvement cross-sectional estimates of the effect of
will have different effects on children’s cogni- involvement are biased, since they neglect
tive and behavioral outcomes. Socialization the cross-cutting influence of prior perfor-
primarily affects children’s behavior and mance.1
engagement. Similarly, the social-control A handful of studies have adopted a longi-
aspects of parental involvement tend to curb tudinal survey design to address causal direc-
children’s problem behaviors, but do relative- tion. Singh et al.’s (1995) study tested the
ly little to influence children’s learning. Only effect of parental involvement on students’
the insider-information element of parental eighth-grade academic achievement with a
involvement, McNeal contended, directly control for prior grades. Muller (1998) and
influences both children’s cognitive and Fan (2001) used a more-rigorous, repeated-
behavioral outcomes. measures approach. Their analyses took
Researchers who have focused on the cog- advantage of the repeated measures available
nitive effects of parental involvement may in the National Education Longitudinal Study
have overlooked more dramatic implications (NELS) of 1988 to model the effect of
of involvement for children’s behavior. This parental involvement on high school stu-
article models the effect of six parental- dents’ achievement, net of prior achievement
involvement activities on children’s behav- scores. These studies controlled for potential
ioral and cognitive development. Following reverse-causality bias, but their estimates of
McNeal (1999), I argue that the six parental- the effect of parental involvement were no
involvement activities influence children’s less ambiguous than were those of earlier
outcomes in different ways: Two of the studies. Muller found that children who dis-
parental-involvement variables that are cuss school with their parents experience rel-
included in my analyses measure the extent atively rapid growth in math achievement,
to which parents are engaged in students’ but otherwise, both Muller and Fan found
homework, an activity that primarily affects that most parental-involvement activities
students’ outcomes via socialization. Three of have, at best, no effect on the cognitive
the involvement measures—attending PTA development of high school students.
meetings and volunteering inside and outside This article also adopts a repeated-mea-
the classroom—tap involvement in activities sures approach, using NLSY data for elemen-
that allow parents to exercise social control tary school students. The approach tests the
over their children. In addition, my analysis hypothesis that parental involvement is both
includes a measure of attendance at parent- the cause and the effect of children’s elemen-
teacher conferences, an activity that exposes tary school performance by examining the
parents to insider information. effects of parental involvement net of chil-

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 237

dren’s prior cognitive achievement and children’s cognitive and behavioral develop-
behavior. ment. The NLSY79 began in 1979, collecting
data on the family background, labor market
Heterogeneity of Causal Effects experience, and educational history of a sam-
ple of 12,686 American children and young
Recent theoretical and empirical work has adults aged 14–22. Annual follow-up surveys
pointed to the possibility that the efficacy of tracked the school-to-work transition of this
particular parental-involvement activities original cohort, and in 1986, the NLSY79 ini-
varies according to parents’ race, ethnicity, tiated a mother-child sample, focusing on the
and class background (Desimone 2001; children of the NSLY79 respondents. In addi-
Lareau and Horvat 1999; McNeal 1999). tion to surveying mothers and children about
Taken as a whole, this line of inquiry suggests their families, friends, and schooling, the
that the involvement efforts of middle-class NLSY79 has administered a battery of cogni-
and white parents may meet with greater tive and socioemotional assessments to the
educational rewards than may the involve- children biannually (Baker et al. 1993; U.S.
ment efforts of poor and minority parents. Department of Labor 2001).
Potential heterogeneity in the causal Researchers have relied heavily on NLSY79
effects of parental involvement has profound data to analyze the influence of parents’
implications for the effectiveness of parental socioeconomic characteristics on children’s
involvement as a school reform strategy. If educational development (see, e.g., Duncan
Desimone (2001), Lareau and Horvat (1999), and Brooks-Gunn 1997; Mayer 1997; Parcel
and McNeal (1999) were correct that the and Menaghan 1994). However, I know of
positive effects of parental involvement are only one study that used NLSY79 mother-
the strongest for the most-advantaged fami- child data to assess the influence of parental
lies, a general increase in parental-involve- involvement, and that study was limited to a
ment activities may actually widen gaps in small subsample of respondent children who
educational achievement, rather than amelio- were enrolled in the Head Start program
rate them. (Cohen 2002). The NLSY79 is uniquely suited
To address these issues, I used time-lagged to research on parental involvement, since it
models to investigate the possibility that the provides rich data on parental involvement
effects of parental involvement vary by par- and multiple children’s educational and
ents’ socioeconomic background by testing behavioral outcome measures. In addition, it
for interactions between the effects of provides repeated observations of children
involvement activities and parents’ SES.2 If while they are still in elementary school,
these analyses show greater returns to the which distinguishes it from the NELS, the
involvement of high-SES parents, the progno- data set that has most often been used in
sis for parental-involvement programs as a recent research on parental involvement.
tool for generating educational equity is grim. I studied 1,445 children of NLSY79 respon-
If, on the other hand, they show that the dents. These children were enrolled in ele-
involvement of disadvantaged parents has a mentary school in 1996 and completed the
stronger effect than the involvement of rela- NLSY79’s Peabody Individual Achievement
tively affluent parents, even modest effects of Test (PIAT) and the Behavior Problems Index
parental involvement could have major con- (BPI) in 1996 and 2000.4 All the children in
sequences for improving educational equity. this sample were in the fourth or lower grades
in 1996, and the median student was
enrolled in the second grade. I followed these
DATA AND METHODS children through three survey rounds, look-
ing at the effect of parents’ school-involve-
I used longitudinal data from the mother- ment activities in 1996 on the children’s 2000
child sample of the NLSY793 to sort out the PIAT and BPI scores. Since the NLSY purpose-
relationship between several parental school- ly oversampled the economically disadvan-
involvement activities and elementary school taged, African Americans, and Hispanics in

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


238 Domina

the construction of the initial 1979 cohort et al. 2001; King et al. 2001).6 Table 1 pre-
and since response rates in the screening sents weighted descriptive statistics for each
process and in subsequent interviews varied variable that was used in the analyses.
by race and other characteristics, I used the
NLSY’s 2000 child weights to correct for Dependent Variables
potential biases.5 Although rates of nonre-
sponse were low, listwise deletion would dis- Using ordinary least-squares (OLS) regression
card nearly one third of the cases in my sam- models, I assessed the effect of the six
ple. As King et al. (2001) demonstrated, the parental school-involvement activities on two
result would be the loss of valuable informa- outcomes: the PIAT and the BPI. The PIAT
tion and could cause severe selection bias. To measures children’s academic achievement
avoid these problems, I filled missing data (Dunn and Markwardt 1970). This composite
using Amelia multiple imputation, a tech- score is the mean of the child’s age-standard-
nique that has been demonstrated to pro- ized percentile scores on subtests in mathe-
duce more reliable data than other tech- matics, reading recognition, and reading
niques for dealing with missing data (Honaker comprehension, each of which consists of 84

Table 1. Variables Used in the Analyses: Descriptive Statistics of NLSY79 Elementary School
Children

Variables Range Mean SD

Dependent Variables
2000 Composite PIAT percentile score 0–100 57.88 24.84
2000 BPI percentile score 0–100 56.83 28.03

Parental Involvement Variables


Parents attended PTA meeting, 1996 0–1 .550 .498
Parents attended one-on-one meeting with
teacher or school official, 1996 0–1 .937 .242
Parents volunteered in classroom, 1996 0–1 .608 .488
Parents volunteered outside the classroom, 1996 0–1 .641 .480
How often parents helped with homework, 1996 0–5 3.11 1.72
How often parents checked homework, 1996 0–5 3.88 1.65

Background Control Variables


Black 0–1 .140 .347
Hispanic 0–1 .071 .258
Other 0–1 .073 .260
Male 0–1 .518 .500
Dummy, child attended public school, 1996 0–1 .855 .353
Dummy, child lived with mother and her
spouse/partner, 1996 0–1 .791 .407
Child’s grade in 1996 0–4 1.82 1.38
Family SES, 1996 -2.26–2.46 0 .69

Prior Performance Control Variables


1996 Composite PIAT percentile score 0–100 51.66 18.11
1996 BPI percentile score 0–100 57.79 27.43
Child repeated a grade before 1996 0–1 .052 .222

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 239

multiple-choice items.7 These three subtests parental-involvement activities on children’s


have high reliability and correlate closely with outcomes: how often the parents helped the
a variety of other cognitive measures.8 children with homework and how often the
The BPI measures the frequency, range, parents checked the children’s homework.
and type of childhood behavioral problems. These measures of home involvement are
This measure is derived from mothers’ scale variables, ranging from 0 (for children
responses to 28 questions about their chil- whose parents never checked or helped with
dren’s behavior in the three months before homework) to 5 (for children whose parents
they were surveyed. Among the behaviors checked or helped with homework every
included in the BPI are cheating and lying, day). Although these two variables correlate
argumentativeness, difficulty concentrating, with one another at .52, they do not correlate
bullying, disobedience at home and at closely with the school-involvement variables,
school, trouble getting along with other chil- and post-hoc multicollinearity tests suggested
dren, trouble getting along with teachers, that they do not create multicollinearity issues
and impulsiveness.9 The BPI is a negative in the regression models.11
measure; a high score on the BPI indicates It should be noted that these measures of
many behavioral problems. parental involvement are less precise than
one may like.12 The four measures of parental
Measures of Parental Involvement participation in school activities are blunt; as
dichotomous measures, they blur the distinc-
I used six NLSY variables to measure aspects tion between parents who regularly partici-
of parental involvement. Four of these vari- pate in school activities and those who do so
ables measure parental participation in infrequently. Likewise, by focusing exclusively
school-based parental-involvement activities, on help with homework, the measures of at-
and two measure at-home parental involve- home parental involvement miss more-
ment in the educational process. The NLSY nuanced forms of parental involvement, such
includes several more-general questions as discussing school lessons and engaging in
about parent-child relations, but these six informal academic coaching. These data limi-
questions most directly tap the involvement tations likely have a conservative effect on the
of parents in children’s education. reported findings; since the data imperfectly
The four school-involvement measures are capture parental involvement, estimates of
attendance at parent-teacher conferences, the effectiveness of involvement could be
participation in the PTA, volunteering in the understated.
classroom, and volunteering outside the
classroom (such as supervising lunch and Control Variables
chaperoning field trips). These items are all
dichotomous, and each is based on inter- Socioeconomic Background I took several
views with the mothers. Mothers who said characteristics of children’s social, economic,
that they or their husbands or partners par- and family backgrounds into account to con-
ticipated in the school activity in question trol for possible spurious correlations between
were coded 1, and those who said that they parental school-involvement activities and
did not were coded 0. To eliminate simul- children’s outcomes. Each of these character-
taneity and time-order issues, I used data on istics has been shown elsewhere to be related
parents’ school involvement from 1996 to to both parents’ school involvement and chil-
predict children’s outcomes in 2000. While dren’s educational and behavioral outcomes.
the four items are correlated,10 multicollinear- The control variables are child’s race and gen-
ity diagnostics on the OLS regression models der (measured in a series of dummy vari-
suggested that including all four types of ables), child’s grade, type of child’s school,
involvement in one regression model does two-parent family, and family SES (created by
not introduce collinearity problems. taking the mean of the standardized logged
In addition, I used two child-reported vari- family income-to-needs ratio, mother’s and
ables to measure the effect of at-home father’s highest grade completed, and moth-

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


240 Domina

er’s and father’s occupational prestige level). the child’s prior score on the dependent vari-
Each of these controls was measured in 1996, able.
when the parental-involvement measures The base PIAT model indicates that several
were taken. parental-involvement activities are significant-
ly related to high academic achievement.
Prior Performance To assess the possible Attending parent-teacher conferences and
reciprocal causal relationship between PTA meetings, volunteering both in and out
parental involvement and children’s out- of the classroom, and checking homework
comes, I added measures to control for chil- are all positively associated with subsequent
dren’s prior performance on relevant assess- scores on academic achievement tests.
ments. With the addition of these prior per- Furthermore, these effects are not insubstan-
formance variables, the PIAT and BPI analyses tial: Children whose parents participated in
became time-lagged growth analyses. While each of these five involvement activities
the earlier models predict the effect of scored an average of 15.35 percentage points
parental involvement on children’s 2000 PIAT higher on the 2000 standardized composite
and BPI assessment values, these time-lagged PIAT examination. Homework help is nega-
models predict the change in assessment val- tively associated with academic achievement
ues over these four years. Controlling for prior in this model. The six parental involvement
values in the PIAT and BPI models reduces the variables explain less than 5 percent of the
model’s variability considerably; the students’ variance in PIAT scores. Nonetheless, this
1996 PIAT scores correlate with their 2000 model clearly shows that children with
scores at .72, and the 1996 BPI scores corre- involved parents tend to have higher acade-
late with the 2000 BPI scores at .65. mic achievement.
The second PIAT model suggests that
Interaction Effects some of this positive association between
parental involvement and academic achieve-
The final step of the analysis was to investi- ment is spurious. Attending PTA meetings,
gate heterogeneity in the effects of parental volunteering outside the classroom, and
involvement by parent’s SES, using interac- checking on homework are all significantly
tion terms created by multiplying the SES and positively associated with academic
scale by each of the parental-involvement achievement, even after race, family back-
activities. The two outcome variables were ground, and school sector are controlled.
then regressed on each of these interaction However, the coefficient for attendance at
terms, along with controls for SES and the parent-teacher conferences becomes nega-
other background and prior performance tive in this model, the coefficient for class-
controls. For clarity’s sake, each of these inter- room volunteering becomes statistically
action analyses was run separately. insignificant, and the positive and statistically
significant coefficients for attending PTA
meetings and volunteering outside the class-
FINDINGS room shrink considerably. Controlling for a
child’s background characteristics reveals that
Table 2 presents the effects of parental- at least some of the positive association
involvement activities in 1996 on children’s between each parental-involvement measure
academic achievement and behavioral prob- and academic achievement that was
lems in 2000. Three models are reported for observed in the base model is an artifact of
each dependent variable: The base model the mutual dependence of parental involve-
looks at the effects of parental involvement ment and academic achievement on back-
without any controls, the second addresses ground characteristics. Like much of the
the issue of spurious causality by controlling research on the link between parental
for the child’s socioeconomic background, involvement and academic achievement, the
and the third addresses the issue of time overall picture that emerges from the second
order and causal direction by controlling for PIAT model is ambiguous. Net of race and

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Table 2. Unstandardized OLS Coefficients: Effect of 1996 Parental Involvement on 2000 PIAT and BPI Scores

2000 PIAT 2000 BPI

Variables 1 2 3 1 2 3

Parents attended one-on-one meeting with teacher, 1996 2.94* -3.99*** -1.21 1.81 2.78* 1.61
Parents attended PTA meeting, 1996 3.32*** 1.92*** .47 -2.38** -.56 .36
Parents volunteered in classroom, 1996 1.71* -.48 .37 -2.76*** -2.08** -.14
Parents volunteered outside the classroom, 1996 6.56*** 2.67*** .27 -5.34*** -3.58*** -1.82**
How often parents checked homework, 1996 .82*** 1.02*** -.34* -6.51** -.93*** -.39*
Leveling the Home Advantage

How often parents helped with homework, 1996 -2.16*** -1.61*** -.33* 2.79 .22 -.43*
Black 11.80*** -5.81*** -5.67*** -3.08***
Hispanic -5.02*** -1.00 -3.19* .57
Other -3.61*** -2.05** 1.36 2.17*
Male .59 .71 4.05*** .58
Dummy, child attended public school, 1996 -3.26*** -1.14* 6.57*** 4.91***
Dummy, child lived with mother and her spouse/partner, 1996 .80 .85 -5.29*** -2.69***
Child’s grade in 1996 -.15 -2.78*** .41 -.44*
Family SES, 1996 15.76*** 6.91*** -7.75*** -2.86***
PIAT, 1996 .90*** —
BPI, 1996 — .63***
Constant 51.38*** 62.78*** 19.61*** 63.17*** 58.42 21.86

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Adjusted R2 .049 .294 .601 .019 .083 .435

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.


241
242 Domina

family background, some parental-involve- background are controlled in the second


ment activities are positively related to acade- model. Net of controls, parents’ volunteering
mic achievement; others are negatively or in school (both inside and outside the class-
nonsignificantly related. room) and homework checking are strongly
But like most research on parental involve- associated with lower levels of childhood
ment, this second PIAT model fails to account behavioral problems. As was the case in the
for reciprocal causality between parental PIAT models, some of the association
involvement and achievement. In the third between involvement and BPI that was
and final model, I allowed for the possibility observed in the base model is spurious; net of
that parents will adjust their involvement background characteristics, the three signifi-
according to their children’s needs by adding cant parental-involvement variables are asso-
a control for a child’s 1996 PIAT score. The ciated with a decline in BPI of just 6.6 per-
coefficients reported in this model can be centage points. Nonetheless, the second BPI
understood as the effect of parental involve- model suggests that the independent influ-
ment on the rate of children’s improvement ence of parental-involvement activities on
in academic achievement between 1996 and children’s behavior is much clearer and
2000. In this model, the only parental- stronger than is the influence of parental
involvement practices that are significantly involvement on academic achievement.
associated with academic achievement— The third model confirms this point, show-
attending parent-teacher conferences, check- ing that parents’ volunteering outside school,
ing homework, and helping with home- homework checking, and homework help all
work—are negatively associated. This finding reduced children’s 2000 behavioral problems,
suggests that once the effect of prior acade- even after background variables and 1996 BPI
mic achievement on parents’ educational- scores were controlled. It is worth noting that
involvement activities is accounted for, the the effect of homework help on BPI has
positive association between involvement reversed to become statistically significant
and achievement melts away. It lends sup- and negative in this third model. In other
ports to McNeal’s (1999) contention that the words, although the first two models showed
link between parental involvement and chil- no relationship between homework help and
dren’s cognitive outcomes is tenuous. BPI, the third model shows that homework
Furthermore, it substantially challenges the help prevents children’s behavioral problems.
notion that parental involvement boosts chil- In sum, the observed effect of parental
dren’s academic achievement. Unless involvement on children’s behavioral prob-
parental-involvement programs improve the lems is relatively modest; together, the three
effectiveness of parental involvement, this significant parental-involvement variables
finding should raise serious questions about account for a change of 2.6 percentage
these initiatives’ efficacy as a strategy to points in the standardized BPI scale. Despite
improve students’ learning. the weak causal link between parental
The analysis of the effect of parental involvement and children’s cognitive devel-
involvement on children’s behavioral prob- opment, these analyses show that parental-
lems yields more encouraging results. In the involvement activities can be effective in pre-
base model, four of the six parental-involve- venting children’s problem behaviors. The
ment activities are associated with fewer effect of parental involvement on children’s
behavioral problems. This model demon- behavior seems to come primarily through
strates that children whose parents attend socialization and the exercise of social con-
PTA meetings, volunteer inside and outside trol.
the classroom, and check their homework The last question to be answered is
score 17 percentage points lower on the stan- whether the effects of parental involvement
dardized BPI distribution. vary on the basis of the parents’ socioeco-
Three of the four desirable parental- nomic background. Table 3 uses multiplica-
involvement effects remain significant after tive interaction terms to address this ques-
child’s school, family, and socioeconomic tion. It reports a series of two-step regressions

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 243

Table 3. OLS and Logistic Regression Coefficients: Heterogeneity in the Causal Effects of Parental
Involvement, by Parental SES (standard errors in parentheses)

2000 PIAT 2000 BPI

1 2 1 2

Parents Attended One-on-One Meeting, 1996 -1.19 -1.64 .53 1.29


(.77) (.84) (1.03) (1.12)
SES 7.00*** 8.70*** -2.91*** -5.84**
(.33) (1.29) (.42) (1.72)
Interaction — -1.78 — 3.06
(1.30) (1.74)
R-square .604 .604 .432 .432

Parents Attended PTA Meeting, 1996 .35 0.37 -.18 -.25


(.38) (.38) (.50) (.51)
SES 6.90*** 7.02*** -2.87*** -3.43***
(.33) (.47) (.42) (.62)
Interaction — -0.20 — .94
(.56) (.75)
R-square .604 .604 .432 .432

Parents Volunteered in the Classroom, 1996 .42 0.38 -.71 -.70


(.39) (.39) (.52) (.52)
SES 6.90*** 6.38*** -2.83*** -2.75***
(.33) (.50) (.42) (.64)
Interaction — 0.80 — -1.30
(.57) (.77)
R-square .604 .604 .432 .432

Parents Volunteered Outside the Classroom, 1996 .40 0.44 -1.66** -1.77**
(.40) (.40) (.53) (.53)
SES 6.91*** 7.70*** -2.77*** -5.00***
(.33) (.49) (.42) (.63)
Interaction — -1.25* — 3.59***
(.57) (.76)
R-square .604 .604 .433 .435

How Often Parents Checked Homework, 1996 -.52*** -0.48*** -.59*** -.56***
(.12) (.12) (.15) (.16)
SES 6.89*** 7.92*** -2.90*** -1.96*
(.33) (.72) (.42) (.95)
Interaction — -0.27 — -.24
(.17) (.22)
R-square .605 .605 .433 .433

How Often Parents Helped with Homework, 1996 -.50*** -0.52*** -.61*** -.57***
(.11) (.11) (.15) (.15)
SES 6.98*** 6.49*** -2.87*** -1.59*
(.33) (.57) (.42) (.75)
Interaction — .16 — -.42*
(.15) (.20)
R-square .605 .605 .434 .434

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


244 Domina

predicting students’ 2000 PIAT and BPI scores—the statistically significant interaction
scores. In the first set of each of these models, terms suggest that the involvement activities
the dependent variable is regressed on a of low-SES parents have a more favorable
parental-involvement activity, SES, student’s influence on their children’s outcomes than
race and gender, school sector, family status, do the activities of high-SES parents.
grade, and score on the 1996 administration Table 4 illustrates this finding by calculating
of the outcome. In the second set of these the predicted outcome scores for each of the
models, an interaction term, created by mul- regressions in Table 3 that indicated significant
tiplying the parental-involvement variable interaction effects. This table compares the pre-
times family SES, is added. (For ease of inter- dicted effects of parental-involvement activities
pretation, only the involvement, SES, and for a hypothetical student whose parents’ SES is
interaction coefficients are reported here.) one standard deviation above the mean value
This table provides limited evidence that with the predicted parental-involvement effects
the effectiveness of parental involvement is for another hypothetical student whose par-
conditional on parental SES. Three of the 12 ents’ SES is one standard deviation below the
models reported here show statistically signif- mean. It shows that the PIAT return to parental
icant interactions between parental-involve- volunteering is nearly twice as high for low-SES
ment activities and SES. Two of these signifi- children as it is for high-SES children. Even
cant interactions point in a surprising direc- more striking is the effect of parental volunteer-
tion, in light of the evidence presented in ing on students’ BPI; while volunteering at
Desimone (2001), Lareau and Horvat (1999), school is associated with a small increase in
and McNeal (1999). With one exception— behavioral problems among affluent children; it
the significant negative interaction between substantially reduces behavioral problems
homework help and SES on children’s BPI among poor children.

Table 4. Predicted Values on Children’s Outcomes for Parental Involvementa

Parent Parent Did Not


Parental Involvement Participated Participate Difference

Outcome: 2000 PIAT


Volunteer outside the classroom
High SES 58.17 57.58 0.58
Low SES 49.66 48.50 1.16

Outcome: 2000 BPI


Volunteer outside of classroom
High SES 55.14 54.52 .62
Low SES 56.98 60.94 -3.96
Homework help
High SES 57.18 58.18 -1.00
Low SES 60.56 61.33 -.77

a These values were calculated by substituting values into the regression equations summarized
in Table 4. Predicted values were calculated for four hypothetical students: two students whose
values on the SES scale were one standard deviation above the mean, one whose parent partici-
pated in the parental-involvement activity and one whose parent did not, and two students whose
values on the SES scale were one standard deviation below the mean, one whose parent partici-
pated in the parental-involvement activity and one whose parent did not. For each of the remain-
ing predictors, sample means were substituted in the regression equation.

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 245

These findings are directly contrary to ed as a panacea—policy makers bill parental-


Lareau and Horvat’s (1999) qualitative analy- involvement initiatives as a tool to reform fail-
ses and McNeal’s (1999) quantitative analy- ing schools, improve students’ learning, and
ses, both of which suggested that the involve- reduce class- and race-based gaps in skills.
ment efforts of high-SES parents were more This study has suggested that some—but
effective than those of low-SES parents. In not all—of this faith may have been mis-
light of earlier research, the interactions placed. Attendance at parent-teacher confer-
reported here, which suggest that the ences, PTA membership, volunteering at
involvement of low-SES parents may pay school, homework checking, and homework
greater cognitive and behavioral dividends help are, indeed, associated with high scores
than the involvement of high-SES parents, on achievement tests and a low incidence of
should be regarded with caution. One impor- behavioral problems for elementary school
tant difference between my analyses and the children. However, after school and family
previous analyses may explain this difference: background and child’s prior academic
My analyses were time lagged to control for achievement are controlled, the effect of each
students’ prior achievement and therefore of these involvement activities on children’s aca-
separate the effect of socioeconomic back- demic achievement is negative or nonsignifi-
ground on the effectiveness of parental inter- cant. Rather than advance children’s cogni-
ventions from the confounding effect of its tive development, some forms of parental
academic and behavioral context. Thus, it is involvement may actually hurt. Although this
possible that earlier findings that showed finding is discouraging and difficult to recon-
greater parental-involvement returns for afflu- cile with the theoretical work that associated
ent students may have been biased by the involvement with academic advantages, it is
correlation between students’ skills and consistent with much of the previous research
behavior and their families’ SES and that on the link between parental involvement
these interactions show the true relationship and children’s cognitive outcomes.
between family SES and the effectiveness of By contrast, this study found a clear causal
parental-involvement activities. On the other link between parental involvement and chil-
hand, it is also possible that the interactions dren’s behavioral problems. Even after I
reported in Tables 3 and 4 simple reflect a applied stringent controls for children’s fami-
ceiling effect. If the children of highly ly and school backgrounds and scores on an
involved high-SES parents began the study earlier measure of behavioral problems, the
with optimal PIAT and BPI scores, they may analyses demonstrated that parents prevent
have experienced little change over the children’s behavioral problems when they vol-
course of the study, regardless of their par- unteer at school, help their children with their
ents’ school-involvement activities. homework, and check their children’s home-
work.
McNeal’s (1999) treatment of parental
DISCUSSION involvement as a form of social capital helps
to make sense of the contrast between the
The idea that parents can change their chil- demonstrated effectiveness of parental
dren’s educational trajectories by engaging involvement in the domain of children’s
with their children’s schooling has inspired a behavior and its relative ineffectiveness in the
generation of school reform policies. In the area of cognition. McNeal argued that
two decades since the Reagan administra- involved parents influence their children in
tion’s Goals 2000, parental involvement has three ways: First, they socialize their children
become a catchphrase in educational policy by demonstrating their interest in their chil-
making, and school outreach efforts, such as dren’s education (as they do when they pay
open houses, PTAs, and parent coordinators, close attention to their children’s homework).
have become increasingly common in U.S. Second, they exercise social control by spend-
elementary schools. In the public discourse, ing time at school, getting to know teachers
parental involvement has come to be regard- and other parents, and using these ties to

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


246 Domina

monitor their children’s schooling. Finally, may translate into cognitive advantages in
they gain access to insider information by the long run. As students progress through
meeting and sharing information with teach- school, their attitudes and behavior can have
ers, administrators, and other parents. For the important implications for their academic
first and second of these mechanisms, in par- engagement and success. For example,
ticular, the link between involvement and McLeod and Kaiser (2004) demonstrated that
children’s behavior is more direct than the young children’s behavior (as measured by
link between involvement and children’s cog- the BPI) influences their odds of later com-
nition. My findings are consistent with this pleting high school and enrolling in college.
analysis, showing that socializing activities Further research is needed to understand how
like homework help and checking and social parental-involvement activities that occur
control activities like school volunteering pre- when students are in elementary school pay
vent children’s behavioral problems, even off as students move into middle school, high
though they have little direct influence on school, and beyond.
academic achievement.
The findings in this article undermine the
traditional case for increasing parental involve- NOTES
ment in schools, suggesting that involvement
is ultimately unrelated to students’ academic 1. However, this is not the only way in
performance. It should be noted, however, which reciprocal causality can bias estima-
that the article did not directly assess the tions of the effect of parental involvement on
effectiveness of parental-involvement policies. children’s outcomes. Crosnoe’s (2001b) study
Although many of the parental-involvement of California high school students showed
activities that I studied likely took place in the that classroom success tends to stimulate the
context of schools’ parental-involvement ini- late high school involvement of parents
tiatives, many others took place in the whose children are enrolled in the noncollege
absence of programmatic intervention. This preparatory tracks.
distinction could have important implications 2. Because of the small sample size, I was
for the effectiveness of parental-involvement unable to replicate Desimone’s (2001) analy-
activities and should be examined more care- sis by race using the NLSY elementary school
fully in future research. On the one hand, children.
parental-involvement initiatives may improve 3. The NLSY79 is sponsored by the U.S.
the effectiveness of involvement by teaching Department of Labor’s Bureau of Labor
parents how to intervene successfully in their Statistics and administered in collaboration
children’s schooling and by sensitizing school with the Center for Human Resource
personnel to parents’ needs. On the other Research, Ohio State University, and the
hand, if school-involvement programs flood National Opinion Research Center, University
schools with eager parents, they could dilute of Chicago.
the effectiveness of individual involvement 4. I used NLSY data only from 1996 to
activities. Unless school policies make 2000 because these are the only years for
parental-involvement activities more effective, which data on parents’ school involvement
the findings of this study suggest that they will are available.
do little to improve elementary school chil- 5. It should be noted, however, that even
dren’s learning. when weighted, this NLSY subsample should
However, the findings suggest a new, not be considered representative of all
more circumscribed, rationale for supporting American elementary school students.
parental-involvement programs: Involvement Instead, the children I analyzed are represen-
may do little to encourage students’ learning tative of the elementary school-aged children
in the short run, but it effectively prevents of all American women aged 35–42 in 2000.
students’ misbehavior. This is no small However, there is no evidence of any system-
achievement. The behavioral improvements atic difference between these children and all
that are associated with parental involvement elementary school children.

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 247

6. For each case that is missing, Amelia REFERENCES


generates five estimated values using data
from values on nonmissing variables and the Baker, Amy J. L., and Laura M. Soden. 1998. “The
EMis expectation maximization algorithm. Challenges of Parent Involvement Research”
The result is five separate data sets, in which (ERIC/CUE Digest No.134). Available on-line
nonmissing values remain unchanged (and at http://www.ericdigests.org/1998-3/par-
ent.html
are identical across data sets) and previously
Baker, Paula C., Canada K. Keck, Frank L. Mott, and
missing variables are filled.
Stephen V. Quinlan. 1993. NLSY Child
7. In models that I do not report here, I Handbook: A Guide to the 1986–1990 National
analyzed the three PIAT subtests separately; Longitudinal Survey of Youth Child Data.
the findings are substantively similar to the Columbus: Center for Human Resource
summary findings reported here. Research, Ohio State University.
8. One-month test-retest reliabilities vary Carey, Nancy, Laurie Lewis, Elizabeth Farris, and
between the three PIAT subtests, ranging from Shelley K. Burns. 1998. Parent Involvement in
.64 for the reading comprehension subtest to Children’s Education: Efforts by Public
.74 for the mathematics subtest and .89 for Elementary Schools (NCES 98-032).
Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
the reading recognition subtest (Baker et al.
Office.
1993; Dunn and Markwardt 1970). The three
Catsambis, Sophia. 1998. “Expanding Knowledge
subtests scale well with one another—for the of Parental Involvement in Secondary
2000 subtests, Cronbach’s alpha = .83. Education: Effects on High School Academic
9. Although individual BPI items have low Success” (Report 27). Baltimore, MD: Center
test-retest reliability, scaled values have for Research on the Education of Students
proved to be more stable, with a one-month Placed at Risk, Johns Hopkins University.
test-retest reliability of .63 (Baker et al. 1993). Catsambis, Sophia, and Janet E. Garland. 1997.
The Cronbach’s alpha for the 28 2000 BPI “Parental Involvement in Students’ Education
items is .88. During Middle and High School” (Report 18).
Baltimore, MD: Center for Research on the
10. Volunteering in the classroom and vol-
Education of Students Placed at Risk, Johns
unteering outside the classroom are correlat- Hopkins University.
ed at .46. This is the highest correlation Catsambis, Sophia, and Belkis Suazo-Garcia. 2000.
between the four school-involvement vari- “Parents Still Matter: Parental Influence on
ables and the only correlation above the con- High School Seniors’ School-Related
ventional .4 threshold. Motivation, Future Plans, and Expectations.”
11. Variance inflation factors (VIF) were run Unpublished manuscript, Queens College,
on all the reported regression models and are City University of New York.
available on request. According to the rules of Chatterjee, Samprit, Ali S, Hadi, and Bertran Price.
thumb presented by Chatterjee, Hadi, and 2000. Regression Analysis by Example. New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
Price (2000), collinearity is an issue if any VIF
Chen, Xianglei, and Kathryn A. Chandler. 2001.
is greater than 10 and if the mean of all VIFs Efforts by Public K–8 Schools to Involve Parents
is considerably larger than 1. No VIF in the in Children’s Education: Do School and Parent
models reported here was greater than 2, and Reports Agree? (NCES 2001-076). Washington:
the mean VIFs for these models ranged from DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
1.28 to 1.30. Cohen, Jennifer Lynn. 2002. “The Effect of Parental
12. Adequately measuring parental Involvement on Academic Achievement in
involvement is a problem that is repeatedly Children Who Attended Head Start.”
encountered in the quantitative literature on Unpublished MSW thesis, California State
the consequences of parental involvement. University, Long Beach.
Comer, James P. 1980. School Power: Implications of
An ideal data set may provide measures of
an Intervention Project. New York: Free Press.
parental involvement from parents, students, Comer, James P., and Norris M. Haynes. 1991.
and teachers, as well as measures of the “Parent Involvement in Schools: An Ecological
intensity of involvement and information Approach.” Elementary School Journal
about the context of and motivations for 91:271–77.
parental-involvement activities. Crosnoe, Robert. 2001a. “Academic Orientation

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


248 Domina

and Parental Involvement in Education During Gootman, Elissa, 2003, August 30. “In Gamble,
High School.” Sociology of Education New York Schools Pay to Get Parents
74:210–30. Involved.” New York Times, p. A1.
—-. 2001b. “Parental Involvement in Adolescent Ho, Esther Sui-Chu and J. Douglas Willms. 1996.
Education: The Interplay of the School, “Effects of Parental Involvement on Eighth-
Community, and Ethnicity.” Sociological Focus Grade Achievement.” Sociology of Education
34:417–34. 69:126–41.
Desimone, Laura. 2001. “Linking Parent Honaker, James, Anne Joseph, Gary King, Kenneth
Involvement with Student Achievement: Do Scheve, and Naunihal Singh. 2001. Amelia: A
Race and Income Matter?” Journal of Program for Missing Data (Windows Version).
Educational Research 93:11–30. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Available
Downey, Douglas B. 2002. “Parental and Family on-line at http://Gking.Harvard.edu
Involvement in Education.” Pp. 6.1–6.27 in Keith, Timothy Z. 1991. “Parent Involvement and
School Reform Proposals: The Research Evidence, Achievement in High School.” Advances in
edited by Alex Molnar. Tempe: Education Reading/Language Research 5:125–41.
Policy Studies Laboratory, Arizona State King, Gary, James Honaker, Anne Joseph, and
University. Kenneth Scheve. 2001. “Analyzing
Duncan, Greg J., and Jeanne Brooks-Gunn. 1997. Incomplete Political Science Data: An
Consequences of Growing Up Poor. New York: Alternative Algorithm for Multiple
Russell Sage Foundation. Imputation.” American Political Science Review
Dunn, Lloyd M., and Frederick C. Markwardt, Jr. 95:49–69.
1970. Peabody Individual Achievement Test Lareau, Annette. 1989. Home Advantage: Social
Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Class and Parental Intervention in Elementary
Service. Education. London: Falmer.
Epstein, Joyce L. 1986. “Parents’ Reactions to Lareau, Annette, and Erin McNamara Horvat.
Teacher Practices of Parental Involvement.” 1999. “Moments of Social Inclusion and
Elementary School Journal 56:277–94. Exclusion: Race, Class, and Cultural Capital in
—-. 1987a. “Effects on Student Achievement of Family-School Relationships.” Sociology of
Teachers’ Practices of Parental Involvement.” Education 72:37–53.
Pp. 261-276 in Literacy Through Family, Mattingly, Doreen J., Radmila Prislin, Thomas L.
Community, and School Interaction, edited by McKenzie, James L. Rodriguez, and Brenda
Steven Silvern. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Kayzar. 2002. “Evaluating Evaluations: The
—-. 1987b. “Parent Involvement: What Research Case of Parent Involvement Programs.” Review
Says to Administrators.” Education and Urban of Educational Research 72:549–76.
Society 19:119–36. Mayer, Susan E. 1997. What Money Can’t Buy:
—-. 1992. “School and Family Partnerships.” Pp. Family Income and Children’s Life Chances.
1139–51 in Encyclopedia of Educational Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Research, edited by Marvin C. Alkin. New York: McLeod, Jane D., and Karen Kaiser. 2004.
Macmillan. “Childhood Emotional and Behavioral
—-. 2001. School, Family, and Community Problems and Educational Attainment.”
Partnerships: Preparing Educators and American Sociological Review 69:636–58.
Improving Schools. Boulder, CO: Westview McNeal, Ralph B., Jr. 1999. “Parental Involvement
Press. as Social Capital: Differential Effectiveness on
Fan, Xitao. 2001. “Parental Involvement and Science Achievement, Truancy, and Dropping
Students’ Academic Achievement: A Growth Out.” Social Forces 78:117–44.
Modeling Analysis.” Journal of Experimental Miedel, Wendy T., and Arthur J. Reynolds. 1999.
Education 70:27–61. “Parent Involvement in Early Intervention for
Fehrmann, Paul G., Timothy Z. Keith, and Thomas Disadvantaged Children: Does It Matter?”
Reimers. 1987. “Home Influence on School Journal of School Psychology 37:379–402.
Learning: Direct and Indirect Effects of Milne, Ann M., David E. Myers, Alvin S. Rosenthal,
Parental Involvement on High School and Alan Ginsburg. 1986. “Single Parents,
Grades.” Journal of Educational Research Working Mothers, and the Educational
80:330–37. Achievement of School Children.” Sociology of
Fine, Michelle. 1993. “[Ap]parent Involvement: Education 59:125–39.
Reflections on Parents, Power, and Urban Muller, Chandra. 1993. “Parent Involvement and
Public Schools.” Teachers College Record Academic Achievement: An Analysis of Family
94:682–710. Resources Available to the Child.” Pp. 77–114

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014


Leveling the Home Advantage 249

in Parents, Their Children, and Schools, edited Components of Parental Involvement on


by Barbara Schneider and James S. Coleman. Eight-Grade Student Achievement: Structural
Boulder: Westview Press. Analysis of NELS-88 Data.” School Psychology
—-. 1998. “Gender Differences in Parental Review 24:299–317.
Involvement and Adolescents’ Mathematics Stevenson, David L., and David P. Baker. 1987.
Achievement.” Sociology of Education “The Family-School Relation and the Child’s
71:336–56. School Performance.” Child Development
Parcel, Toby, and Elizabeth G. Menaghan. 1994. 58:1348–57.
Parents’ Jobs and Children’s Lives. New York: U.S. Department of Labor, Bureau of Labor
Walter de Gruyter. Statistics. 2001. The National Longitudinal
Reay, Diane. 1998. Class Work: Mothers’ Surveys Handbook 2001. Washington, DC:
Involvement in their Children’s Primary Author.
Schooling. London: UCL Press. Useem, Elizabeth. 1992. “Middle Schools and
Sanders, Mavis. 1998. “The Effects of School, Math Groups: Parents’ Involvement in
Family, and Community Support on the Children’s Placement.” Sociology of Education
Academic Achievement of African American 65:263–79.
Adolescents.” Urban Education 33:384–409. Wallace, Trudy, and Herbert J. Walberg. 1991.
Singh, Kusum, Patricia G. Bickley, Paul Trivette, “Parental Partnerships for Learning.”
Timothy Z. Keith, Patricia B. Keith, and Eileen International Journal of Educational Research
Anderson. 1995. “The Effects of Four 15:131–45.

Thurston Domina is a Ph.D. candidate in sociology at the Graduate School and University Center,
City University of New York. His dissertation investigates the causes and consequences of recent
increases in residential segregation between college graduates and people with lower levels of edu-
cation.

The author is grateful to Paul Attewell, David Lavin, and Julia Wrigley for their helpful comments on
earlier versions of this article. This research was supported by a Social Justice and Social Development
in Educational Studies training grant, codirected by Colette Daiute and Michelle Fine and funded by
the Spencer Foundation. Address correspondence to Thurston Domina, Ph.D. Program in Sociology,
CUNY Graduate Center, 365 Fifth Avenue, 6109, New York, NY 10016; e-mail: tdomina@gc.cuny.edu.

Downloaded from soe.sagepub.com at CARLETON UNIV on November 28, 2014

You might also like