You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Child and Family Studies

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-1011-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

Parental Involvement in Primary School Education: its Relationship


with Children’s Academic Performance and Psychosocial
Competence through Engaging Children with School
Rosa Sze Man Wong1 Frederick Ka Wing Ho1 Wilfred Hing Sang Wong1 Keith Tsz Suen Tung1
● ● ● ●

Chun Bong Chow1 Nirmala Rao2 Ko Ling Chan3 Patrick Ip1


● ● ●

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
The benefits of parental involvement in children’s education have been well established but increasing evidence suggests that
overparenting may have adverse effects on children. The question of whether excessive parental involvement hinders
children’s academic and psychosocial development warrants further investigations. This study examined the associations of
1234567890();,:

parental educational involvement at home and in school with academic performance and psychological health of 507
Chinese Grade 3 schoolchildren in Hong Kong. Parents reported on their level of involvement in children’s schooling and
their children’s psychosocial issues. Children were surveyed to determine their school engagement, and their Chinese
language and mathematics attainment was assessed. We also explored the underlying mechanism by testing children’s
engagement with school as a mediator of the relationships. Our results showed that home-based parental educational
involvement was positively associated with children’s language competence and psychosocial wellbeing, and the
associations were linked through engaging children with school. However, the benefits reached a plateau at higher level of
parental involvement in children’s learning at home. School-based parental involvement had an indirect effect on children’s
prosocial behavior through school engagement. These findings highlight the significance of optimal level of parental
involvement in children’s education at home for children’s development.
Keywords Parental involvement Academic achievement Psychosocial wellbeing School engagement Child
● ● ● ●

development

Introduction children more autonomy as they mature (Locke et al.


2012). Such excessive parental control can put children at
In recent years, concerns have arisen about overparenting. higher risk for low self-esteem (Deci et al. 1991) and also
The term “helicopter parenting” has been used in the pop- various emotional and behavioral problems in adulthood
ular media (Locke et al. 2012) to refer to the practices of (Trzesniewski et al. 2006). The link between overparenting
parents who are very anxious and overinvolved in their and problems faced by emerging adults has been estab-
children’s life. Helicopter parents remain reluctant to give lished (Schiffrin et al. 2014; Segrin et al. 2015). But less is
known about the impact of helicopter parenting on young
children. Hong Kong provides a good setting to study this
topic because many Hong Kong children have helicopter
* Patrick Ip parents. These children are sent to multiple interest classes
patricip@hku.hk
at a very young age which leaves them little time for play
1
Department of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, The and other recreational activities. Excessive academic bur-
University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, Hong Kong den in turn could weaken these children’s learning moti-
2
Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, vation. It also increases learning stress and pressure which
Hong Kong are detrimental to the health and wellbeing of children
3
Department of Applied Social Sciences, The Hong Kong (Hart et al. 2013). It is therefore imperative to study whe-
Polytechnic University, Hong Kong, Hong Kong ther a dose-response relationship exists between parental
Journal of Child and Family Studies

involvement and children’s academic and psychosocial educational involvement might benefit Chinese children by
outcomes. motivating them to strive toward academic excellence.
Parental educational involvement has been considered as Parental involvement not only makes children aware of
a multidimensional construct that consists of parental parental expectations for schooling, but it also facilitates
behaviors and beliefs on children’s education at home and in children’s learning and engagement in school by sustaining
school (Englund et al. 2004). Previous studies found that students’ learning interests across contexts. There is evi-
parental support for children’s learning at home such as dence from adolescent research that parental involvement
monitoring schoolwork and visiting museums is beneficial influences student outcomes through its impact on school
for improving children’s academic performance and social engagement (Wang and Sheikh‐Khalil 2014). School
relationships (Parcel and Dufur 2001; Tam and Chan 2009). engagement, defined as “students’ involvement with
During parent-child interactions, children learn from their schooling, academics, or learning” (Bempechat and Shern-
parents’ expression of emotions and use these learned off 2012), has been found to protect against low achieve-
reactions to handle their own issues later in life (S. Denham ment, behavioral disruption, and high dropout rates
and Kochanoff 2002; S. A. Denham et al. 1997). Through (Fredricks et al. 2004; Ladd and Dinella 2009). Active
observation of parents’ behaviors, children also acquire participation in class and extracurricular activities in turn
skills to manage emotions which are important for forming can improve students’ affective states, such as increased
positive social relationships and developing prosocial positive feelings toward learning, and also lead to better
behaviors (S. A. Denham 1986). Furthermore, having good academic and behavioral outcomes (Voelkl 2012). These
emotional control is important for prevention of mis- benefits, however, occur only when students experience
behaviors at home and in school (Izard et al. 2001). intrinsic enjoyment in schoolwork (Bempechat and Shern-
On the other hand, school-based involvement such as off 2012). Research shows that children who understand
communication with teachers and attending school events parental expectations for schooling experience greater
could increase parents’ understanding of children’s perfor- intrinsic motivation to stay engaged in school-related tasks
mance in school. This enables parent-child communication (Bao and Lam 2008). These on-task behaviors are positively
on school issues which is associated with lower learning related to student resilience and ability to deal with chal-
distress and more supportive family interactions at home lenging problems (Gonzales et al. 2014; Skinner and Pitzer
(Pomerantz et al. 2007). It can also foster children’s cog- 2012). Engaging in school also indicates students having
nitive and vocabulary development by increasing children’s less time and energy available for problem behaviors on one
exposure to adult varied, complex speech (Rodriguez and hand and stronger attachment to school and bonding with
Tamis‐LeMonda 2011). teachers on the other hand (Wang and Sheikh‐Khalil 2014).
Parental involvement is also positively associated with This, in turn, may foster children’s psychosocial
children’s perceived competence and relatedness (Gonzalez- development.
DeHass et al. 2005; Grolnick et al. 1991). Relatedness Parental educational involvement research has been
represents the extent to which children feel connected to conducted in western populations. There is scant evidence
their parents. In line with the theories of attachment (Furrer on this topic in Chinese populations and school engagement
and Skinner 2003). children with high relatedness are able in childhood (Lau et al. 2011, 2012). The present study
to internalize parental values and expectations as their own examined the associations of parental involvement with
ones (Marchant et al. 2001). The process of internalization academic performance and psychosocial behaviors of Chi-
can increase children’s motivation to learn because good nese children in Hong Kong. This study also explored the
school performance is a recognized means to repay or honor mediator role of children’s school engagement in these
parental educational investment and sacrifices (Urdan and associations. We hypothesized that parental involvement in
Maehr 1995) as well as to gain relatedness and approval children’s education in school and at home were associated
from parents (Cheung and Pomerantz 2012). A previous with better academic performance and less psychosocial
study of 825 American and Chinese children in 7th grade difficulties in children. Furthermore, children’s school
found that children who perceived high parental support for engagement mediated these associations.
their education showed greater motivation to actively par-
ticipate in educational activities (Bempechat and Shernoff
2012; Cheung and Pomerantz 2012). However, this study Method
used mixed sample which limit the generalizability of the
findings to other populations including Chinese popula- Participants
tions. Chinese parents value high academic achievement,
and the culture places great emphasis on filial piety (Kim This study used the data collected in the Primary School
2013). Instead of causing harms, higher levels of parental phase of a longitudinal cohort study of Chinese children
Journal of Child and Family Studies

recruited from randomly selected kindergartens in Hong Bureau (Education Bureau 2008, 2012). The LAMK has
Kong in 2012 spring. The original cohort study aimed to been validated in Rasch analysis against students’ grade
examine the differences in school readiness between chil- with excellent internal consistency (Cronbach’s alphas .88
dren from low-income families and those from high-income [Chinese language] and .91 [Mathematics]). It is used by the
families in Hong Kong. Further details of the longitudinal local education bureau to monitor the basic competencies of
cohort study can be found in previous publications (Ip et al. primary school students, identify less able students for
2015). In the year 2015 when most children in the cohort remedial treatment, and guide the allocation of education
reached 8 years of age and were studying in the first resources (Education Bureau 2012). The LAMK has 6
semester of Grade 3, the research team invited all families levels from Level A to Level F, with the Level B designed
who participated in the Kindergarten phase with agreement for students in P.3 first semester. However, to avoid a sig-
given to be followed up to take part in this study. To further nificant ceiling effect, the Level C (Mc and Cc, LAMK 2.0)
increase the sample size, upon completion of the cohort was used to assess the Chinese and mathematics skills of the
survey, a list of primary schools where our cohort children participating children in this study. Specifically, the Chinese
were attending was generated. Each listed primary school test paper consists of 2 sections (reading and writing). The
was contacted and invited to assist with the recruitment of Reading section has 12 questions about two Chinese stories;
their Grade 3 students. Families with interest and consent the Writing section has 18 questions. Each correct answer is
given to join the study were then provided with the same set worth 1 point except the last question of the Writing section
of questionnaires as were those provided to our cohort – Chinese composition which is worth 10 points. The total
families. score of the Chinese test paper is 39. On the other hand, the
mathematics test paper has total 53 questions. A point will
be given to each correct answer, with no extra point
Procedure deduction for wrong answer, thereby giving a total score of
53. Children were asked to complete the respective test
Parents who expressed an interest to join the study paper. The test paper was then graded by a trained research
received a formal consent form with an information sheet staff according to the standard answer key and scoring
about this study, a set of questionnaires, and an addressed scheme.
return envelope with postage paid. All the questionnaires
were self-administered. Specifically, children were asked Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
to complete two standard attainment tests (each for Chi-
nese language and for mathematics) at home within 90 min Psychosocial outcomes were assessed with the parent-proxy
under no assistance provided by other members in the version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
family. In addition, prior to self-administration of the tests, (SDQ) for children aged 4–17 years (R. Goodman 1997).
the child was asked to rate each statement in relation to his/ The SDQ measures five domains, namely conduct problems
her level of engagement at school. At the same time, (i.e. often has temper tantrums or hot tempers; often fights
parents/primary caregivers were surveyed with question with other children), hyperactivity/inattention (i.e. think
items on family demographics, home-based and school- things out before acting; constantly fidgeting or squirming),
based parental involvement, and children’s psychosocial emotional problems (i.e. often complaints of headaches;
issues. Completed questionnaires were then returned to nervous or clingy in new situations), peer relationship
the research team by mail using the addressed return problems (i.e. picked on or bullied by other children; gen-
envelope. erally liked by other children), and positive prosocial
behaviors (i.e. considerate of other people’s feelings; shares
readily with other children), with each subscale comprising
Measures five items. The items are therefore categorized into 5 sub-
scale scores, and the four negative behavior subscales can
Tests of Chinese language and mathematics be summed to an overall total difficulties score (A. Good-
man et al. 2010). This scale has been widely used and
Chinese language and mathematical abilities were chosen as validated in Hong Kong children population with satisfac-
the indicators of children’s academic performance in this tory validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha coefficients
study because they are the primary and core academic ranging from .45 to .82) (Lai et al. 2010). The tool appears
subjects in Hong Kong primary schools. These two out- to have sufficient reliability in the study sample (Cronbach’s
comes were assessed using the Learning Achievement alpha coefficients ranging from .53 to .87). Parents were
Measurement Kit (LAMK) which was piloted in 2006, then asked to rate each item on a scale ranging from 0 = not true
revised and standardized in 2008 by Hong Kong Education to 2 = certainly true.
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Home-based and School-based Parental Educational comprehensible and applicable for their age prior to the
Involvement Scales main study.

Two aspects of parental involvement were assessed by Control variables


multiple-item indicators adopted from previous parent
report measures: home-based involvement and school-based Children’s gender, family income, and parental marital
involvement (Wang and Sheikh‐Khalil 2014). The items status were controlled in the analyses. These variables were
were slightly modified to better reflect the local Chinese chosen based on prior knowledge and were also found to be
primary school context, for example, “parent-teacher con- associated with parental involvement, academic perfor-
ference” in the original school-based scale was replaced mance, and psychosocial wellbeing.
with “PTA meeting” which is of similar meaning but more
commonly used in Hong Kong. For home-based scale, Data Analyses
“visiting museums or libraries” in the original scale was
replaced with “discussing about school events and matters” Descriptive statistics including mean and standard devia-
in our version because Hong Kong parents tend to have tions for all key study variables were calculated. Pearson’s
busy work schedule which give them rare opportunities to product-moment correlation coefficients among variables
visit museums or libraries with children after school. were generated to examine unadjusted bivariate associa-
The School-Based Involvement Scale includes four items tions. Multivariate linear regression analyses were per-
which assess the extent to which parents interact with formed to examine factors associated with each outcome of
school (e.g. “During the current school year, how often did interest, adjusting for confounders including child gender,
you or your spouse volunteer at school events?”) on a 5- family income and parental marital status. Explanatory
point scale, ranging from 0 = never to 4 = six times or variables with a p-value of <0.05 on correlation analyses
above each year. The Home-Based Involvement Scale were included in the regression analyses. Because the linear
includes four items which assess the extent to which parents regression analyses assume all relationships are linear,
spend time with children in educational activities outside of generalized additive models (GAMs) were used to examine
school (e.g. “During the current school year, how often did other possible forms of the relationships between the pre-
you or your spouse encourage your child to do homework dictor and outcomes (Wood 2006).
independently?” and “How often did you or your spouse Mediation analyses were used to test the hypothesized
discuss with your child on materials learned in class each relationships among study variables. Instead of the Baron
week?”). Items within this home-based scale were rated by and Kenny (1986) approach, the present study employs a
parents on a 4-point scale, ranging from 0 = never to 3 = path analysis as a general framework in analyzing the roles
five times or above each week. Home-based and School- of potential mediators (Edwards and Lambert 2007). The
based Parental Involvement Scale in this study had accep- path analysis provides a general and consistent modelling
table internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha coeffi- framework for estimating the total, direct, and indirect effect
cients of 0.71 and 0.74, respectively. between the predictor (parental involvement) and the out-
come (academic and psychosocial variables). It is essen-
Children’s School Engagement Scale tially a set of regression models but allows simultaneous
estimation so the results will be more reliable. It also allows
Children’s school engagement was assessed in two aspects, adopting the Huber robust standard errors for deriving
namely behavioral and affective dimension, using items accurate p-values (Huber 1967). Missing data were handled
adapted from the existing western and Chinese student through full-information maximum likelihood estimation
engagement scale (Fredricks et al. 2004; Lam et al. 2014). which allows for the inclusion and use of all available data
The scale has total ten items, with four items measuring to estimate the population parameter values that have the
children’s effort and participation in school and six items highest probability of producing the sample data (Baraldi
assessing children’s attitudes and enjoyment at school. All and Enders 2010). Indirect effects (i.e. mediation) were
the items were rated by child participants on a 5-point scale estimated with delta method standard errors (MacKinnon
ranging from 0 = never to 4 = always. School engagement et al. 2002). Rucker et al. (2011) raised concerns about the
was studied as a global/unidimensional construct with use of “partial” and “full” mediation labels because of the
acceptable reliability indicated by Cronbach’s alpha coeffi- differential statistical power for detecting indirect and total/
cient of 0.80 for the present study sample. direct effects which can give rise to misleading mediation
All these measures were administered in Chinese lan- conclusions (Rucker et al. 2011). Hence, the present study
guage and have been piloted in several local Grade 3 stu- did not label mediation results full or partial. Variance
dents and their parents to ensure that the items were inflation factors (VIFs) were used to examine against
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Table 1 Scale properties, mean,


Descriptive Scale properties
and standard deviation of key
statistics
variables
Variable Mean SD Number of items Cronbach’s α Score range

Academic outcomes
Chinese language 27.49 6.05 29 0.88 0–39
Mathematics 44.02 7.66 53 0.91 0–53
Psychosocial outcomes
SDQ: Emotional problems 2.37 2.08 5 0.65 0–10
SDQ: Conduct problems 3.04 1.75 5 0.63 0–10
SDQ: Hyperactivity 6.60 2.50 5 0.78 0–10
SDQ: Peer problems 4.46 1.88 5 0.54 0–10
SDQ: Prosocial behaviors 7.05 2.04 5 0.73 0–10
SDQ: Total difficulties 16.45 5.93 25 0.87 0–40
Parental involvement factors
Home-based parental involvement 1.91 0.61 4 0.71 0–3
School-based parental involvement 1.55 0.82 4 0.74 0–4
Child-level factors
Overall school engagement 2.15 0.42 10 0.80 0–4
Better performance was indicated by higher scores for all variables except the following SDQ subscales:
hyperactivity, emotional problems, conduct problems, peer problems and total difficulties

multicollinearity. All the statistical analyses were performed involvement (r = .68, p < .001). The correlations between
using the R statistical package (version 3.20) with package school engagement and different indicators of academic and
lavaan (version 0.5–18). psychosocial development were of medium strength (r
= .27, p < .001, for Chinese language; r = .22, p < .001, for
mathematics; r = .32, p < .001, for prosocial behaviors; r =
Results −.28, p < .001, for conduct problems; r = −.33, p < .001,
for hyperactivity; r = −.30, p < .001, for total psychosocial
This study recruited total 507 children (age range: 8–9 years difficulties).
old) in their first semester of Grade 3. 244 (48%) were There was a large size of correlation between perfor-
males and 20 (3.9%) had single parent status. 28% had mance in Chinese and mathematics test (r = .67, p < .001).
monthly household income of HKD 50,000 (USD 6,410) or Only hyperactivity and conduct problems were correlated
above. Children received an average of 27.49 (SD = 6.05) with children’s Chinese language test performance (r =
out of 39 points in the Chinese language test and 44.02 (SD −.30, p < .001, for hyperactivity; r = −.20, p < .001, for
= 7.66) out of 53 points in the mathematics test. The mean conduct problems) and mathematics test performance (r =
of their overall school engagement was 2.15 ± 0.42 on a 5- −.27, p < .001, for hyperactivity; r = −.17, p < .01, for
point scale. With a maximum score of 10 points, parents’ conduct problems). For parental involvement, home-based
ratings of their children’s prosocial behaviors, hyperactivity, and school-based involvement were correlated (r = .37, p
emotional and conduct problems were 7.05 (SD = 2.04), < .001). However, involvement in these two contexts
6.60 (SD = 2.50), 2.37 (SD = 2.08) and 3.04 (SD = 1.75), showed different patterns of correlations with children’s
respectively. In terms of home-based parental involvement, outcomes. Home-based parental involvement was related to
an average of 1.91 (SD = .61) on a 4-point scale reflects that children’s Chinese language (r = .16, p < .05), mathematics
parents discussed and did school-related activities with the (r = .15, p < .05), hyperactivity (r = −.25, p < .001), peer
child at home 3–4 times each week. For school-based problems (r = −.26, p < .001), prosocial behaviors (r = .31,
involvement, an average of 1.55 (SD = .82) on a 5-point p < .001), and total psychosocial difficulties (r = −.25, p
scale reflected that parents engaged with school 1–3 times < .001). On the other hand, school-based parental involve-
yearly.Table 1. ment was correlated with children’s prosocial behaviors
Table 2 shows the inter-correlations among all variables. only (r = .19, p < .05).
Based on Cohen’s effect size classification (Cohen 1992), Gender, monthly household income, and single parent
children’s school engagement was highly correlated with status were correlates of several variables, suggesting that
school-based (r = .70, p < .001) and home-based parental the subsequent analyses should adjust for their possible
Journal of Child and Family Studies

confounding effects on the relationship between parental

−0.20**
0.20***

0.19**
−0.11
−0.13

−0.06

−0.16
−0.03
−0.12
−0.06
0.17*
involvement and children’s outcomes.

0.11

0.05
14
To further examine the relationships among the vari-
ables, adjusted regressions were conducted using children’s

−0.24***
−0.20**
−0.20**

0.28***

0.24***
academic and psychosocial outcomes as dependent vari-
0.19**
−0.13

−0.15
0.17*

0.09

0.05

0.00
ables and home-based and school-based parental involve-
13

ment and children’s school engagement as independent

−0.16*
−0.04

−0.08

−0.09

−0.10
variables. As showed in Table 3, after adjusting for gender,
0.02

0.06
0.10
0.06

0.05

0.05
12

monthly household income, and single parent, children’s


school engagement was associated with parental home (B
−0.28***
−0.33***

−0.30*** = .47, p < .001) and school involvement (B = .37, p


0.27***
0.22***

0.32***

0.66***
0.70***
−0.17*
−0.07

< .001). More home-based parental involvement was also


11

associated with higher scores on Chinese language test (B


= 1.32, p < .01) and less behavioral problems (B = −1.79,
0.37***
0.19**
−0.09
−0.09
−0.04

−0.08

p < .001). All VIFs were smaller than 5, indicating a small


0.07
0.05
0.00
10

risk of multicollinearity.
−0.25***
−0.27***

−0.25***

Furthermore, GAMs (see Fig. 1a, b) revealed significant


0.31***
−0.04

−0.11

relationships between home-based parental involvement


0.16*
0.15*

and children’s outcomes but there were ceiling effects on


9

children’s academic performance and psychosocial beha-


−0.27***
−0.24***

−0.36***
0.68***
0.73***
0.78***
0.68***

viors. Figure 1c shows that increasing levels of school-


based parental involvement was associated with more
8

prosocial behaviors and less hyperactivity in children.


−0.33***
−0.32***
−0.31***

Mediation analyses were performed to test whether


−0.10

children’s school engagement mediated the association of


0.09
0.03

parental involvement with children’s academic and psy-


7

chosocial outcomes. Results showed that children’s school


0.41***
0.28***
0.31***
−0.13
−0.10

engagement mediated 74% of the association between


home-based parental involvement and children’s Chinese
6

language performance (Fig. 2a) as well as 71% of the


−0.30***
−0.27***
0.27***
0.57***

association between home-based parental involvement and


children’s psychosocial difficulties (Fig. 2b). In terms of
5

positive psychosocial outcomes, home-based parental


−0.20***
−0.17**

involvement had direct effect on children’s prosocial


0.29***

behaviors and indirect effect through children’s school


engagement Fig. 3a). School-based parental involvement
4

had indirect effect on prosocial behaviors only through


−0.14
−0.10
Table 2 Inter-correlations among all variables (N = 507)

children’s engagement with school (Fig. 3b).


3

0.67***

Discussion
2

School-based parental involvement


Home-based parental involvement

This study explored how parental involvement in children’s


*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

education can be related to psychosocial and academic


Overall student engagement
SDQ: Prosocial behaviours

outcomes among Chinese primary school students. In the


current study, we investigated the associations between two
SDQ: Total difficulty
SDQ: Peer problems
SDQ: Hyperactivity

aspects of parental educational involvement (i.e., home-


SDQ: Emotional

based and school-based involvement) and young children’s


Family income
SDQ: Conduct

Single parent
Mathematics

academic (i.e. Chinese language and Mathematics test


Chinese

scores) and psychosocial outcomes (i.e., behaviors) using


Gender

both objective and subjective measures. We found that


home-based parental educational involvement was asso-
10
11
12
13
14

ciated with children’s language competence and


1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Table 3 Adjusted association analysis for the relationship between parental involvement, children’s school engagement and academic/
psychosocial outcomes (n = 507)
Potential predictor/mediator
Home-based parental involvement School-based parental involvement Student engagement
B (95% CI) p B (95% CI) p B (95% CI) p

Academic outcomes
Chinese language 1.32 (0.33, 2.32) 0.01 ** 0.51 (−0.19, 1.22) 0.15 3.18 (1.90, 4.46) <0.001 ***
Mathematics 1.10 (−0.16, 2.35) 0.09 0.53 (−0.36, 1.42) 0.25 3.39 (1.76, 5.02) <0.001 ***
Psychosocial outcomes
Emotional problems −0.11 (−0.45, 0.22) 0.50 0.04 (−0.19, 0.28) 0.72 −0.35 (−0.83, 0.13) 0.16
Conduct problems −0.53 (−0.81, −0.26) <0.001 *** −0.22 (−0.42, −0.03) 0.03 * −0.91 (−1.30, −0.52) <0.001 ***
Hyperactivity −0.90 (−1.29, −0.52) <0.001 *** −0.34 (−0.62, −0.06) 0.02 * −1.73 (−2.28, −1.18) <0.001 ***
Peer problems −0.17 (−0.47, 0.13) 0.26 −0.07 (−0.28, 0.15) 0.54 −0.53 (−0.96, −0.10) 0.02 *
Prosocial behaviours 0.95 (0.64, 1.26) <0.001 *** 0.47 (0.25, 0.70) <0.001 *** 1.36 (0.91, 1.80) <0.001 ***
Total difficulties −1.73 (−2.74, −0.85) <0.001 *** −0.58 (−1.26, 0.10) 0.10 −3.56 (−4.90, −2.22) <0.001 ***
Potential mediator
Student engagement 0.47 (0.41, 0.52) <0.001 *** 0.37 (0.33, 0.40) <0.001 *** – –
All associations adjusted for gender, family income, and parental marital status
B: unstandardized regression coefficient
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

psychosocial wellbeing. The benefits reached a plateau education in school. Moreover, perceived lack of compe-
when parents were overinvolved in children’s learning tence, energy, skills and knowledge to develop school
outside of school. This finding is not counterintuitive policies may also contribute to parental reluctance to get
because academic pressure caused by excessive parental involved in school (Hoover-Dempsey and Sandler 1997;
educational involvement can discourage children’s learning Wang and Sheikh‐Khalil 2014). In particular, Chinese
interest and motivation to make scholarly pursuit. More- parents have high respect for teachers and believe that their
over, overly involved parents tend to grant insufficient teaching methods and materials are adequate for children to
autonomy to their children. Autonomy, which refers to self- acquire important life skills and knowledge (Chen 2005).
governance or rule by the self (Ryan and Deci 2006), is This expectation for teachers as the main and primary
essential for personal achievement and behavioral adjust- contributor to children’s learning may further hinder par-
ment (Inguglia et al. 2015). When parents behave in ents’ motivation to become involved in school. Moreover,
autonomy restrictive manners, children are less likely to compared with home-based parental involvement, involve-
explore and develop their genuine interest and more likely ment in school was found to have weaker effects on chil-
to exhibit behavioral problems (Lansford et al. 2014). dren’s psychosocial outcomes, possibly because the
Although exposure to various activities can be devel- effectiveness of parental involvement in school is limited
opmentally enhancing, engaging children in activities they with minimal interactions between parents and children at
intrinsically like and enjoy is fundamental to the develop- school. In Hong Kong, schools usually invite parents to take
ment of positive emotions and learning motivation that are part in mainly administrative meetings and occasionally
essential for achievement and wellbeing (Perry 2011; Seipp extracurricular activities (Ho and Kwong 2013) which do
1991). In addition to quantity, the quality of interactions not provide as many one-on-one and direct parent-child
between parents and children during school-related activ- interactions as home activities can offer. To overcome these
ities are also influential to children’s academic and health difficulties, schools should take initiatives in getting parents
outcomes which warrant further research. involved in their children’s school lives such as organizing
Consistent with previous findings on Hong Kong chil- family events in school and inviting parents to join these
dren in kindergartens and secondary schools (Ho 2003; Lau events. Teachers should also regularly speak to parents
et al. 2011), parents of primary schoolchildren also reported about their children’s classroom performance (Epstein and
higher level of home-based than school-based involvement Salinas 2004) in order to maximize children’s academic
in children’s education. Long work hours and inflexible potential and improve their wellbeing.
schedule, which are common among Hong Kong parents, Interestingly, while the correlations between home-based
are key barriers to parental involvement in children’s parental involvement and children’s conduct problems,
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Fig. 1 a Relationships between


home-based parental
involvement and academic
outcomes. b Relationships
between home-based parental
involvement and psychosocial
outcomes. c Relationships
between school-based parental
involvement and psychosocial
outcomes

hyperactivity and prosocial behaviors were significant, this is that compared to externalizing behavioral problems
children’s emotional and peer problems were not related to such as hyperactivity and conduct problems, children’s
either type of parental involvement. A possible reason for internalizing problems such as sadness and fears are more
Journal of Child and Family Studies

a b
-0.05 Children’s
-0.07 Children’s Home-based parental psychosocial
Home-based parental
involvement Chinese language involvement difficulties

Children’s school -0.20**


Children’s school 0.29*** 0.64*** engagement
0.64***
engagement

Total effect: 0.12** Total effect: -0.18***


Indirect effect: 0.18 *** Indirect effect: -0.13***
Percentage mediated: 74% Percentage mediated: 71%

Fig. 2 Mediation results illustrating the relationships among home- family income and parental marital status (n = 507). The numbers are
based parental involvement, children’s school engagement and lan- standardized coefficient estimates. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p <
guage and psychosocial outcomes after adjusting for child gender, 0.001

difficult to be observed by parents and caregivers (Cicchetti A. Denham et al. 2000). In addition to shaping good
and Toth 2014) and be accurately evaluated through a single behaviors, socialization also increases children’s exposure
informant method (Gilliom and Shaw 2004). As a result, to adult’s complex speech which could strengthen children’s
this may have masked the true effect of parental involve- cognitive and vocabulary skills (Rodriguez and Tamis‐
ment on these outcomes. Some researchers also suggest that LeMonda 2011). This may explain why home-based par-
children’s internalizing psychopathology involves a com- ental involvement had stronger association with children’s
plex mechanism that cannot be easily changed through language skills than mathematical skills in this study.
parental influences (Chen and Liu 2012). Our measure of We also found that children’s school engagement medi-
parental involvement assessed only the frequency of aca- ated the association of home-based parental involvement
demically based involvement strategies which encourage with children’s language skills and overall behavioral pro-
learning motivation and behaviors but may not be able to blems. It also mediated the associations between school-
modify inner maladaptive thoughts and feelings. Children’s based and home-based parental involvement and children’s
internalizing issues should be tackled through non- prosocial behaviors. School engagement is a board concept
academically based involvement strategies such as parent- concerning children’s behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
child play activities which involve more emotional support experiences in school. Consistent with previous studies
for children to develop positive emotions (Gilliom and (Voelkl 2012), this study found that student engagement was
Shaw 2004). associated with better academic performance and fewer
Another interesting result is that home-based parental behavioral problems. Student engagement reflects good
involvement was associated with children’s overall beha- student qualities such as following school rules, good self-
vioral problems and language competence but not mathe- regulation, and being interested in school work (Fredricks
matic skills, whereas school-based parental involvement et al. 2004), all of which are important for academic
was associated with only a few problem behavior domains excellence. When children stay on task, they are less likely
and not with any academic outcomes. Such findings perhaps to act in disruptive ways. On the other hand, disengaged
are due to the design of our questionnaire, as our ques- students may have a higher chance of failure to develop
tionnaire items on home-based involvement measured the positive learning attitudes and behaviors. They also tend to
frequency of interactions between parents and children at make ties with other disengaged students which may further
home, but the items on school-based involvement con- increase their educational risks such as retention and drop-
cerned parents’ participation in schools and their interac- ping out. Hence school engagement is an important factor
tions with teachers only. Accordingly, the significant that can protect students from academic failure and problem
associations between home-based parental involvement and behaviors (Finn and Zimmer 2012). To promote student
better behavioral and language outcomes in children may be engagement, parents should get involved in children’s edu-
attributed to the socialization experiences involved in home cation at home and in school. Our study showed that both
activities. Socialization is a process in which a child is types of parental involvement had positive links with student
taught to develop appropriate behaviors needed for inter- engagement. Parental educational involvement at home such
action with other individuals (Maccoby 2007). Previous as parental guidance on homework and communication with
research has documented the benefits of parental socializa- children on school issues can increase children’s awareness
tion to promotion of positive behaviors (Pettygrove et al. of parental expectations for schooling which is important for
2013) and prevention of problem behaviors in children (S. learning motivation and engagement (Bao and Lam 2008). It
Journal of Child and Family Studies

a b
0.17** Children’s prosocial School-based parental -0.03 Children’s
Home-based parental
behaviors involvement prosocial behaviors
involvement

Children’s school 0.19*** 0.70*** Children’s school 0.32***


0.64*** engagement
engagement

Total effect: 0.29*** Total effect: 0.19***


Indirect effect: 0.12** Indirect effect: 0.22***
Percentage mediated: 41% Percentage mediated: 88%

Fig. 3 Mediation results illustrating the relationships among parental status (n = 507). The numbers are standardized coefficient estimates.
involvement, children’s school engagement and prosocial behaviors Note. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
after adjusting for child gender, family income and parental marital

also promotes children’s perceived support and approval given that children completed the attainment tests at home,
from parents so children may have stronger desire to use although we had asked parents not to provide assistance, we
academic achievement as a way to repay parental educa- cannot ascertain how much help children received in com-
tional investment and sacrifices (Urdan and Maehr 1995). pleting the tests. Another limitation is that the present study
On the other hand, school-based parental involvement such used cross-sectional data on parental involvement, chil-
as regular communication with teachers can increase tea- dren’s school engagement, and children’s academic and
chers’ understanding of children’s background and needs so psychosocial issues and thus cannot establish casual rela-
the teachers can provide students with better learning sup- tionships. It is therefore important to note that the mediation
port. Evidence shows that students experiencing greater models examined in this study are only able to demonstrate
teacher support exhibit more engagement behaviors (Klem suggestive temporal order among variables of interest. More
and Connell 2004). Future research that explores the extent advanced research designs such as longitudinal and trial
to which social and demographic factors such as parental studies are required to reduce bias and demonstrate causal
education affect parental educational involvement, student relationships (Maxwell and Cole 2007).
engagement, and ultimately, student performance and well-
being will help identify at-risk students who need more Acknowledgements This manuscript is a revision of RMSW MPhil
thesis which was accepted at the University of Hong Kong in
learning support and evidence-based programs to improve
November 2015. We are grateful to the primary schools, the children
behavior and academic outcomes. and their parents who participated in this study.

Author Contributions RSMW: conceived the study concept, designed


and executed the study, and wrote the manuscript. FKWH: analyzed
Limitations and interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript. WHSW: executed
the study, supervised data analyses, and wrote the manuscript. KTST:
Several limitations of this study should be noted. First, executed the study and wrote the manuscript. CBC: conceived the
parents’ self-ratings of their own involvement strategies are study concept, designed and executed the study, and revised the
manuscript. NR: conceived the study concept and revised the manu-
subject to response bias. They may have given a more script. KLC: conceived the study concept and revised the manuscript.
favorable rating to each involvement scale item. This can PI: conceived the study concept, designed and executed the study, and
inflate or deflate the strength of the associations reported in revised and proofread the manuscript.
this study. However, compared with previous findings on
Funding This study was supported by a grant from the Research
parental involvement in Chinese populations (e.g. Lau et al. Grants Council of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region,
2011), our study revealed similar patterns of involvement at China (Project No. 743413).
home and in school, suggesting that our parents’ report of
involvement should be generally valid. Similar concerns are Compliance with Ethical Standards
also applied to parents’ report of child psychosocial issues
and child self-report of school engagement but our study Conflicts of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
found significant associations between school engagement
and psychosocial well-being which are consistent with Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving
existing theoretical and empirical claims. Therefore, the human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
parent-proxy and self-reported data should not have sub- the Institutional Review Board of the University of Hong Kong/Hos-
pital Authority Hong Kong West Cluster.
stantially influenced the validity of the findings. Second,
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual effects of parental involvement, expectations, and quality of
participants included in the study. assistance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 723–730.
Epstein, J. L., & Salinas, K. C. (2004). Partnering with families and
communities. Educational Leadership, 61(8), 12–19.
Finn, J. D., & Zimmer, K. S. (2012). Student engagement: What is it?
References Why does it matter? Handbook of Research on Student
Engagement (pp. 97–131). New York, NY: Springer.
Bao, X. H., & Lam, S. F. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School
the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese children’s engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence.
motivation. Child Development, 79(2), 269–283. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59–109.
Baraldi, A. N., & Enders, C. K. (2010). An introduction to modern Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in
missing data analyses. Journal of School Psychology, 48(1), children’s academic engagement and performance. Journal of
5–37. Educational Psychology, 95(1), 148–162.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. S. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing
variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and
strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality Psychopathology, 16(02), 313–333.
and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. Gonzales, N. A., Wong, J. J., Toomey, R. B., Millsap, R., Dumka, L.
Bempechat, J., & Shernoff, D. J. (2012). Parental influences on E., & Mauricio, A. M. (2014). School engagement mediates long-
achievement motivation and student engagement. Handbook of term prevention effects for Mexican American adolescents. Pre-
Research on Student Engagement (pp. 315–342). New York, NY: vention Science, 15(6), 929–939.
Springer. Gonzalez-DeHass, A. R., Willems, P. P., & Holbein, M. F. D. (2005).
Chen, J. J. L. (2005). Relation of academic support from parents, Examining the relationship between parental involvement and
teachers, and peers to Hong Kong adolescents’ academic student motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 17(2),
achievement: The mediating role of academic engagement. 99–123.
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 131(2), Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to
77–127. use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of
Chen, J. J. L., & Liu, X. (2012). The mediating role of perceived the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties
parental warmth and parental punishment in the psychological Questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and
well-being of children in rural china. Social Indicators Research, children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8),
107(3), 483–508. 1179–1191.
Cheung, C. S. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2012). Why does parents’ Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a
involvement enhance children’s achievement? The role of parent- research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38
oriented motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), (5), 581–586.
820–832. Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner resources
Cicchetti, D, Toth, S. L. (2014). Internalizing and externalizing for school achievement: Motivational mediators of children’s
expressions of dysfunction (Vol.2). New York, NY: Psychology perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Press. 83(4), 508.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), Hart, S. L., Hodgkinson, S. C., Belcher, H. M. E., Hyman, C., &
155–159. Cooley-Strickland, M. (2013). Somatic symptoms, peer and
Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). school stress, and family and community violence exposure
Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. among urban elementary school children. Journal of Behavioral
Educational Psychologist, 26(3-4), 325–346. Medicine, 36(5), 454–465. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-
Denham, S., & Kochanoff, A. T. (2002). Parental contributions to 9440-2.
preschoolers’ understanding of emotion. Marriage & Family Ho, E. S. C. (2003). Students’ self-esteem in an Asian educational
Review, 34(3-4), 311–343. system: The contribution of parental involvement and parental
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and investment. School Community Journal, 13(1), 65–84.
emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Develop- Ho, E. S. C., & Kwong, W. M. (2013). Parental involvement on
ment, 57(1), 194–201. children’s education: What works in Hong Kong. Singapore:
Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Springer.
Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emo- Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., & Sandler, H. M. (1997). Why do parents
tional competence: direct and indirect effects. Motivation and become involved in their children’s education? Review of Edu-
Emotion, 21(1), 65–86. cational Research, 67(1), 3–42.
Denham, S. A., Workman, E., Cole, P. M., Weissbrod, C., Kendziora, Huber, P. J. (1967). The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates
K. T., & ZAHN–WAXLER, C. (2000). Prediction of externa- under nonstandard conditions. Proceedings of the fifth Berkeley
lizing behavior problems from early to middle childhood: the role symposium on mathematical statistics and probability (Vol. 1, pp.
of parental socialization and emotion expression. Development 221–233). CA: Berkeley.
and Psychopathology, 12(1), 23–45. Inguglia, C., Ingoglia, S., Liga, F., Lo Coco, A., & Lo Cricchio, M. G.
Education Bureau. (2008). Learning achievement measurement Kit (2015). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescence and emerging
2.0: user’s guide. Hong Kong: Education Bureau. adulthood: relationships with parental support and psychological
Education Bureau. (2012). The EDB circular (EDBC012/2012) on distress. Journal of Adult Development, 22(1), 1–13. https://doi.
learning support. Hong Kong: Education Bureau. org/10.1007/s10804-014-9196-8.
Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for integrating Ip, P., Rao, N., Bacon-Shone, J., Li, S. L., Ho, F. K.-w, Chow, C.-b, &
moderation and mediation: a general analytical framework using Jiang, F. (2015). Socioeconomic gradients in school readiness of
moderated path analysis. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 1–22. Chinese preschool children: the mediating role of family pro-
Englund, M. M., Luckner, A. E., Whaley, G. J., & Egeland, B. (2004). cesses and kindergarten quality. Early Childhood Research
Children’s achievement in early elementary school: Longitudinal Quarterly, 35(2), 111–123.
Journal of Child and Family Studies

Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., & Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 13–36).
Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of Amsterdam: Academic Press.
social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Pettygrove, D. M., Hammond, S. I., Karahuta, E. L., Waugh, W. E., &
Psychological Science, 12(1), 18–23. Brownell, C. A. (2013). From cleaning up to helping out: Parental
Kim, S. Y. (2013). Defining tiger parenting in Chinese Americans. socialization and children’s early prosocial behavior. Infant
Human Development, 56(4), 217–222. Behavior and Development, 36(4), 843–846.
Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships matter: Linking Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D. (2007). The
teacher support to student engagement and achievement. Journal how, whom, and why of parents’ involvement in children’s aca-
of School Health, 74(7), 262–273. demic lives: More is not always better. Review of Educational
Ladd, G. W., & Dinella, L. M. (2009). Continuity and change in early Research, 77(3), 373–410.
school engagement: Predictive of children’s achievement trajec- Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the
tories from first to eighth grade? Journal of Educational Psy- home learning environment across the first 5 years: Associations
chology, 101(1), 190–206. with children’s vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten.
Lai, K. Y., Luk, E. S., Leung, P. W., Wong, A. S., Law, L., & Ho, K. Child Development, 82(4), 1058–1075.
(2010). Validation of the Chinese version of the strengths and Rucker, D. D., Preacher, K. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2011).
difficulties questionnaire in Hong Kong. Social Psychiatry and Mediation analysis in social psychology: Current practices and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(12), 1179–1186. new recommendations. Social and Personality Psychology
Lam, S. F., Jimerson, S., Wong, B. P., Kikas, E., Shin, H., Veiga, F. Compass, 5(6), 359–371.
H., & Negovan, V. (2014). Understanding and measuring student Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self‐regulation and the problem of
engagement in school: The results of an international study from human autonomy: does psychology need choice, self‐determina-
12 countries. School Psychology Quarterly, 29(2), 213–232. tion, and will? Journal of Personality, 74(6), 1557–1586.
Lansford, J. E., Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. Schiffrin, H. H., Liss, M., Miles-McLean, H., Geary, K. A., Erchull,
(2014). Mothers’ and Fathers’ Autonomy-Relevant Parenting: M. J., & Tashner, T. (2014). Helping or hovering? The effects of
Longitudinal Links with Adolescents’ Externalizing and Inter- helicopter parenting on college students’ well-being. Journal of
nalizing Behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(11), Child and Family Studies, 23(3), 548–557.
1877–1889. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-0079-2. Segrin, C., Givertz, M., Swaitkowski, P., & Montgomery, N. (2015).
Lau, E. Y. H., Li, H., & Rao, N. (2011). Parental involvement and Overparenting is associated with child problems and a critical
children’s readiness for school in China. Educational Research, family environment. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(2),
53(1), 95–113. 470–479.
Lau, E. Y. H., Li, H., & Rao, N. (2012). Exploring parental invol- Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis
vement in early years education in China: development and of findings. Anxiety Research, 4(1), 27–41.
validation of the Chinese Early Parental Involvement Scale Skinner, E. A., & Pitzer, J. R. (2012). Developmental dynamics of
(CEPIS). International Journal of Early Years Education, 20(4), student engagement, coping, and everyday resilience. Handbook
405–421. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669760.2012.743099. of research on student engagement (pp. 21–44). New York, NY:
Locke, J. Y., Campbell, M. A., & Kavanagh, D. (2012). Can a parent Springer.
do too much for their child? An examination by parenting pro- Tam, V. C., & Chan, R. M. (2009). Parental Involvement in Primary
fessionals of the concept of overparenting. Australian Journal of Children’s Homework in Hong Kong. School Community Jour-
Guidance and Counselling, 22(02), 249–265. nal, 19(2), 81–100.
Maccoby, E. E. (2007). Historical overview of socialization research Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Robins, R. W.,
and theory. In J. E. Grusec & H. P. D (Eds.), Handbook of Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during ado-
socialization: Theory and research (pp. 13–41). New York, NY: lescence predicts poor health, criminal behavior, and limited
Guilford Press. economic prospects during adulthood. Developmental Psychol-
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & ogy, 42(2), 381–390.
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and Urdan, T. C., & Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two-goal theory of
other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7(1), motivation and achievement: A case for social goals. Review of
83–104. Educational Research, 65(3), 213–243.
Marchant, G. J., Paulson, S. E., & Rothlisberg, B. A. (2001). Relations Voelkl, K. E. (2012). School identification. Handbook of research on
of middle school students’ perceptions of family and school student engagement (pp. 193–218). New York, NY: Springer.
contexts with academic achievement. Psychology in the Schools, Wang, M. T., & Sheikh‐Khalil, S. (2014). Does parental involvement
38(6), 505–519. matter for student achievement and mental health in high school?
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses Child Development, 85(2), 610–625.
of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 23–44. Wood, S. (2006). Generalized additive models: an introduction with
Parcel, T. L., & Dufur, M. J. (2001). Capital at home and at school: R. Boca Raton, FL: CRC press.
Effects on student achievement. Social Forces, 79(3), 881–911.
Perry, R. P. (2011). The control-value theory of achievement emo-
tions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. A.

You might also like