You are on page 1of 9

Research

JAMA Surgery | Original Investigation

Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training


A Randomized Clinical Trial
Sara P. Myers, MD, PhD; Mohini Dasari, MD, MS; Joshua B. Brown, MD, MSc; Stephanie T. Lumpkin, MD;
Matthew D. Neal, MD; Kaleab Z. Abebe, PhD; Nicole Chaumont, MD; Stephanie M. Downs-Canner, MD;
Meghan R. Flanagan, MD; Kenneth K. Lee, MD; Matthew R. Rosengart, MD, MPH

Invited Commentary
IMPORTANCE Factors contributing to underrepresentation of women in surgery are page 560
incompletely understood. Pro-male bias and stereotype threat appear to contribute to Author Audio Interview
gender imbalance in surgery.
Supplemental content
OBJECTIVES To evaluate the association between pro-male gender bias and career
CME Quiz at
engagement and the effect of stereotype threat on skill performance among trainees in jamacmelookup.com and CME
academic surgery. Questions page 674
DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS A 2-phase study with a double-blind, randomized clinical
trial component was conducted in 3 academic general surgery training programs. Residents
were recruited between August 1 and August 15, 2018, and the study was completed at the
end of that academic year. In phase 1, surveys administered 5 to 6 months apart investigated
the association of gender bias with career engagement. In phase 2, residents were
randomized 1:1 using permuted-block design stratified by site, training level, and gender to
receive either a trigger of or protection against stereotype threat. Immediately after the
interventions, residents completed the Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Surgery (FLS)
assessment followed by a final survey. A total of 131 general surgery residents were recruited;
of these 96 individuals with academic career interests met eligibility criteria; 86 residents
completed phase 1. Eighty-five residents were randomized in phase 2, and 4 residents in each
arm were lost to follow-up.

INTERVENTION Residents read abstracts that either reported that women had worse
laparoscopic skill performance than men (trigger of stereotype threat [A]) or had no
difference in performance (protection against stereotype threat [B]).

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Association between perception of pro-male gender bias
and career engagement survey scores (phase 1) and stereotype threat intervention and FLS
scores (phase 2) were the outcomes. Intention-to-treat analysis was conducted.

RESULTS Seventy-seven residents (38 women [49.4%]) completed both phases of the study.
The association between pro-male gender bias and career engagement differed by gender
(interaction coefficient, −1.19; 95% CI, −1.90 to −0.49; P = .02); higher perception of bias was
associated with higher engagement among men (coefficient, 1.02; 95% CI, 0.19-2.24;
P = .04), but no significant association was observed among women (coefficient, −0.25; 95% Author Affiliations: Department of
CI, −1.59 to 1.08; P = .50). There was no evidence of a difference in FLS score between Surgery, University of Pittsburgh
School of Medicine, Pittsburgh,
interventions (mean [SD], A: 395 [150] vs B: 367 [157]; P = .51). The response to stereotype Pennsylvania (Myers, Brown, Neal,
threat activation was similar in men and women (interaction coefficient, 15.1; 95% CI, −124.5 Lee, Rosengart); Department of
to 154.7; P = .39). The association between stereotype threat activation and FLS score Surgery, University of Washington,
Seattle (Dasari, Flanagan);
differed by gender across levels of susceptibility to stereotype threat (interaction coefficient,
Department of Surgery, University of
−35.3; 95% CI, −47.0 to −23.6; P = .006). Higher susceptibility to stereotype threat was North Carolina at Chapel Hill School
associated with lower FLS scores among women who received a stereotype threat trigger of Medicine (Lumpkin, Chaumont,
(coefficient, −43.4; 95% CI, −48.0 to −38.9; P = .001). Downs-Canner); Division of General
Internal Medicine, University of
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Perception of pro-male bias and gender stereotypes may Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
(Abebe); Fred Hutchinson Cancer
influence career engagement and skill performance, respectively, among surgical trainees.
Research Center, Seattle, Washington
TRIAL REGISTRATION ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03623009 (Flanagan).
Corresponding Author: Sara P.
Myers, MD, PhD, Department of
Surgery, University of Pittsburgh
School of Medicine, 200 Lothrop St,
JAMA Surg. 2020;155(7):552-560. doi:10.1001/jamasurg.2020.1127 Pittsburgh, PA 15213
Published online May 20, 2020. (myerssp@upmc.edu).

552 (Reprinted) jamasurgery.com

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training Original Investigation Research

M
edicine trails other fields in gender diversity.1,2 Gen-
der disparity is particularly pronounced within aca- Key Points
demic surgery; women represent less than 10% of full
Question Do the associations between pro-male bias and career
professors and only 22 department chairs in the US and engagement, as well as stereotype threat and skill performance,
Canada.3 Evidence indicates that pro-male bias and negative differ by gender?
stereotypes depress women's performance 4 in male-
Findings In this multicenter randomized clinical trial that included
dominated professions.5,6 If and how this bias differentially af-
86 general surgery trainees interested in pursuing academic
fects health care professionals remains to be characterized. careers, pro-male gender bias was significantly associated with
Lack of information is a substantial impediment to develop- higher career engagement among men; no significant association
ing valid interventions that address challenges to the promo- was observed in women. Women with higher susceptibility to
tion and retention of female surgeons.7 We studied how tech- stereotype threat scored lower on Fundamentals of Laparoscopic
nical skill performance and engagement—2 crucial components Surgery assessment after receiving a stereotype threat trigger.
of career advancement—are influenced by pro-male bias and Meaning Pro-male gender bias may increase career engagement
negative stereotypes against women during residency, which among men, and stereotype threat may negatively influence
is a phase of profound personal and professional develop- women surgical trainees’ skill performance in a way that can be
ment. detrimental to professional development.
Psychosocial constructs, ie, attitudes or behaviors that
arise from interactions between an individual and their
environment,8 forecast and reinforce professional success. domized clinical trial exploring the effect of stereotype threat
These constructs have been investigated in models of educa- activation on technical skill performance. Institutional re-
tional achievement, making them especially relevant to resi- view board approval was obtained at each institution. The trial
dency, which is a period of intensive training. Previous inves- protocol appears in Supplement 1. Participants provided writ-
tigations have emphasized learner engagement as integral to ten informed consent (eAppendix 3 in Supplement 2). We as-
professional advancement.9 Engagement is defined as involve- signed each resident a unique alphanumeric code for deiden-
ment with social or academic activities and is associated with tification of serially collected data, the purpose of which was
affective aspects, such as sense of belonging10; cognitive com- to blind the individuals conducting the study. No compensa-
ponents, such as resilience and self-regulation11; and ability to tion was provided for study participation. This study fol-
relate to one’s profession (ie, domain identification).12 Nega- lowed the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials
tive stereotypes have been shown to undermine engagement (CONSORT) reporting guideline for randomized clinical trials.
and associated behaviors among members of stigmatized so-
cial groups.13 The risk of confirming negative stereotypes, Participants
known as stereotype threat, is especially germane to women General surgery residents at University of Pittsburgh Medical
employed in medicine, as they are frequently reminded of gen- Center, the University of North Carolina–Chapel Hill, and the
der stereotypes in their environments.5 Stereotype threat University of Washington were recruited (eAppendix 1 in
contributes to measurable differences in technical skill Supplement 2) and completed an electronic pre-enrollment
performance14 by depleting executive function and siphon- survey to determine eligibility (eAppendix 2 in Supple-
ing attention away from the task at hand.15 ment 2). Residents who completed undergraduate medical
This multicenter study, conducted in 2 phases, first sought education in the US and were interested in pursuing teaching
to evaluate the association between gender bias and research- or research as a principal component of their career (ie, aca-
related career engagement among trainees, and second, to in- demic surgery)16 were eligible for inclusion. Those who were
vestigate the interaction between gender and stereotype threat in noncategorical positions and/or not interested in pursuing
on laparoscopic skill performance. Our hypothesis for phase 1 a career in academic surgery, did not identify as their biologic
was that the association between perceiving pro-male bias in sex, or had entered general surgery training after completing
the training environment and career engagement would dif- any training in another specialty were excluded. Residents were
fer by gender. For phase 2, we hypothesized that stereotype asked to participate in a 2-part study over an academic year
threat activation would affect technical skill performance and exploring how training experiences influenced psychosocial
that this association would differ between men and women. determinants of success.

Study Instruments and Interventions


During phase 1, 2 identical surveys (eAppendix 4 in Supple-
Methods ment 2) evaluating psychosocial constructs germane to pro-
Study Setting and Design fessional achievement were sequentially administered (eFig-
We conducted a 2-phase study of general surgery residents at ure 1 in Supplement 2). Existing studies on burnout have
3 diverse academic campuses: University of North Carolina– suggested that trainees’ experiences can alter motivation, job
Chapel Hill; University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, Pitts- satisfaction, and quality of life over time.17 Based on feasibil-
burgh, Pennsylvania; and University of Washington, Seattle ity and previous studies investigating the association be-
(Figure 1). The study included a survey-based investigation into tween behavioral interventions and resident attitudes and
gender bias and career engagement and a multicenter ran- perceptions,18,19 we chose the predetermined time frame of

jamasurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 553

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Research Original Investigation Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training

Figure 1. Trial Profile

147 Recruitment emails sent


45 University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill
52 University of Washington
50 UPMC

16 Excluded
16 Did not respond

131 Expressed interest; request sent for further


eligibility information

35 Excluded
22 Noncategorical
13 Categorical; not interested in pursuing
academic career

10 Excluded
10 Did not complete surveys 1 and 2

86 Completed surveys 1 and 2

1 Excluded
1 Withdrew before randomization

85 Randomized
17 University of
North Carolina-Chapel Hill
30 University of Washington
38 UPMC

42 Assigned to receive stereotype 43 Assigned to receive protection Diagram of participants in phases 1


threat trigger against stereotype threat and 2. Of the 85 participants
randomized, 17 were from the
4 Excluded 4 Excluded
University of North Carolina–Chapel
4 Did not complete FLS task 4 Did not complete FLS task Hill, 30 from University of
Washington, and 38 from UPMC.
FLS indicates Fundamentals of
38 Completed the study (20 men, 39 Completed the study (19 men, Laparoscopic Surgery;
18 women) 20 women)
UPMC, University of Pittsburgh
Medical Center.

5 to 6 months between survey administrations. Rationale for was modified to interrogate research-related engagement as
psychosocial constructs assessed and associated scales or sub- scholarly pursuits are a distinguishing feature of academic vs
scales is provided in eAppendix 5 in Supplement 2. nonacademic surgeons.16 Engagement is a complex concept
Residents had 2 weeks to complete each survey after it was influenced both by environmental cues and characteristics in-
delivered via a software-generated link (Qualtrics). The sur- trinsic to the individual.9 These characteristics and the tools
vey instrument comprised 55 questions derived from vali- used in their evaluation include sense of belonging (7-item
dated scales assessing equity and fairness in the work envi- Sense of Belonging Index-Psychological State subscale),23 iden-
ronment, susceptibility to stereotype threat, sense of belonging, tification with one’s professional domain (7-item Domain
resilience, career engagement, and identification with the do- Identification Measure),24 and resilience (10-item Connor-
main of surgery, and 5 questions that assessed demographic Davidson Resilience Scale).25 Subscale items were each graded
data. Eight items adapted from the Employee Environment on a 5-point Likert scale.
Diagnostic Survey (EEDS)20 were used to measure residents’ In phase 2, residents who completed phase 1 were ran-
perceptions of pro-male gender bias in their environment. Nine domized 1:1 to receive either a stereotype threat trigger (A) or
items from the Social Identity and Attitudes Scale (SIAS)21 as- protection against stereotype threat (B) in a permuted-block
sessed susceptibility to stereotype threat based on the degree design stratified by site, postgraduate year, and gender. In an
to which an individual identifies with their gender (ie, gen- effort to enhance validity and promote spontaneous behav-
der identity) and their level of gender stigma consciousness. ior, detailed information regarding the study’s purpose of
Nine items adapted from the Career Engagement Scale (CES)22 investigating the effect of stereotype threat on skills perfor-
were used to assess engagement, which has been suggested mance was not proffered26; instead, residents were told that
to be important for achieving professional success.9 The CES they would be randomly assigned to receive an intervention

554 JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 (Reprinted) jamasurgery.com

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training Original Investigation Research

meant to direct their focus to or away from a surgery-related distributed data were logarithmically transformed. All statis-
subject. After the investigation was complete, participants tical tests were clustered by site, as data within an institution
were debriefed with regard to study intent, procedures, and may not be independent. We modeled the association be-
results via email communication and open-forum presenta- tween survey 2 EEDS and CES scores using linear regression
tion. We adapted a validated and previously used method of incorporating an interaction term for gender. In phase 1 sec-
activating or protecting against stereotype threat.27 Inter- ondary analyses, we adjusted for the remaining 4 psychoso-
vention A triggered stereotype threat by asking participants cial constructs to determine whether these results modified
to read the abstracts of 2 articles that reported women per- the association between EEDS and CES.
formed worse with regard to laparoscopic skills than men. The second part of this study explored the effects of ste-
Intervention B was intended to protect against stereotype reotype threat, a concept related to but separate from gender
threat by asking participants to read 2 abstracts that reported bias, on technical skill performance. Differences in perfor-
no gender-based differences in laparoscopic skills perfor- mance may manifest independent of bias within the work en-
mance. According to the randomization assignment, articles vironment owing to gender stereotypes from a pervasive cul-
were placed in a sealed envelope labeled with the partici- tural context. We hypothesized that the FLS scores in the
pant’s study code and provided to each participant just prior intervention arms would differ and that gender-based differ-
to administration of the Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Sur- ences in performance would vary by whether stereotype threat
gery (FLS) assessment (task portion). Immediately after the was activated or protected against.
interventions, participants completed the FLS, a standard- A minimum sample size of 84 residents was calculated to
ized, validated simulation-based assessment of laparoscopic detect a 20-point difference in FLS score at 80% power and
ability,28-31 followed by a third iteration of the survey, which α = .05, and accounting for 10% attrition.35,36 Analyses were
included additional questions evaluating residents’ percep- based on intention-to-treat. Gender-based differences in sur-
tion of their performance. The FLS exams were administered vey 3 responses were assessed using the Wilcoxon Mann-
by 2 proctors certified by the Society of American Gastro- Whitney test. Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank tests were
intestinal and Endoscopic Surgeons, who were blinded to used to compare survey 2 and 3 subscale responses. Differ-
the participants’ intervention arm. Exams were sent to the ences in FLS scores between intervention arms and by gen-
Society of American Gastrointestinal and Endoscopic Sur- der were evaluated using 2-tailed, unpaired t tests. Post hoc
geons for blind scoring under deidentified participant code. analysis assessed whether susceptibility to stereotype threat
Residents understood that their score could not be used for modified the effect of intervention across genders on FLS
formal FLS certification. Proctors and Society of American scores. Stata, version 15 (StataCorp), was used for all analy-
Gastrointestinal and Endoscopic Surgeons were compen- ses. A P value ≤.05 was considered significant.
sated for exam administration and scoring.

Statistical Analysis
Survey subscale items were assigned a score based on the
Results
5-point Likert scale and then summated to yield a total sub- General surgery residents at University of North Carolina–
scale score. Internal consistency of each subscale was as- Chapel Hill, University of Washington, and University of Pitts-
sessed using Cronbach α, with values between 0.7 and 0.9 con- burgh Medical Center were recruited between August 1 and
sidered acceptable.32 The α values from unmodified original August 15, 2018. A total of 131 residents were assessed for eli-
subscales were as follows: EEDS, 0.96; CES, 0.87; Social Iden- gibility. Of 96 eligible residents (Figure 1), 86 individuals (90%)
tity and Attitudes Scale gender identity, 0.81, Social Identity completed phase 1 and were enrolled in phase 2. One resident
and Attitudes Scale gender stigma consciousness, 0.87; Sense withdrew before randomization. Of 85 residents randomized
of Belonging Index-Psychological State subscale, 0.93; (Table 1), 42 were assigned to intervention A and 43 were as-
Domain Identification Measure, 0.75; and Connor-Davidson signed to intervention B. Four residents in each arm dropped
Resilience Scale, 0.85.20-25 out; 77 residents (91%) completed the study.
Our primary hypothesis in phase 1 was that the associa-
tion between perception of pro-male bias as measured by EEDS Phase 1
score and career engagement as measured by CES score dif- The association between perception of pro-male gender bias
fered by gender. To avoid sampling bias and accurately re- in the environment and career engagement differed signifi-
flect resident population demographics, we used a poststrati- cantly by gender (interaction coefficient, −1.19; 95% CI, −1.90
fication weighting adjustment33 for survey data based on the to −0.49; P = .02); men who had a higher perception of pro-
participant’s gender and race using national data.34 Wilcoxon male bias had higher career engagement scores (coefficient,
Mann-Whitney tests evaluated gender-based differences in 1.02; 95% CI, 0.19-2.24; P = .04) (Figure 2). Although the di-
subscale scores. Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank tests as- rection of the data was opposite compared with that of men,
sessed differences between survey 1 and 2 scores. Since sur- no significant association was noted between perception of pro-
vey 1 was administered early in the academic year, first-year male bias and career engagement scores among women (co-
postgraduate residents may not have acclimated to their train- efficient, −0.25; 95% CI, −1.59 to 1.08; P = .50). After adjust-
ing environment; analyses for phase 1 were performed using ing for the other psychosocial constructs, the moderating effect
survey 2 responses. Before regression analyses, nonnormally of gender on the association between perception of pro-male

jamasurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 555

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Research Original Investigation Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training

Table 1. Demographics of Participants Who Underwent Randomization

No. (%)
Trigger of Protection against
stereotype threat stereotype threat
Variable All participants (intervention A) (intervention B)
No. 85 42 43
Postgraduate training level, y
1 15 (17.6) 7 (16.7) 8 (18.6)
2 18 (21.2) 9 (21.4) 9 (20.9)
3 11 (12.9) 6 (14.3) 5 (11.6)
4 11 (12.9) 5 (11.9) 6 (14.0)
5 10 (11.8) 5 (11.9) 5 (11.6)
Lab 20 (23.5) 10 (23.8) 10 (23.3)
Sex
Male 43 (50.6) 21 (50.0) 22 (51.2)
Female 42 (49.4) 21 (50.0) 21 (48.8)
Racea
a
No participants identified as
White 57 (67.1) 28 (66.7) 29 (67.4)
American Indian or Alaska Native or
Black/African American 6 (7.1) 2 (4.8) 4 (9.3) as Native Hawaiian or Pacific
Asian 16 (18.8) 10 (23.8) 6 (14.0) Islander.
b
Other 6 (7.1) 2 (4.8) 4 (9.3) No participants identified as
widowed. Divorced and separated
Marital statusb
represented separate response
Married 40 (47.1) 23 (54.8) 17 (39.5) categories on the survey instrument
Divorced/separated 2 (2.4) 0 2 (4.7) but have been collapsed within
these data for presentation
Never married 43 (50.6) 19 (45.2) 24 (55.8)
purposes.

Figure 2. Association Between Perception of Gender Bias


Phase 2
and Career Engagement Across Genders There was little difference among men and women in FLS score
between interventions (mean [SD], A: 395 [150] vs B: 367 [157];
50 P = .51). Although the mean (SD) FLS score was lower for
women compared with men in both intervention A (367 [152]
40 vs 423 [148]; P = .12) and intervention B (328 [168] vs 400 [142];
Men
P = .08), these differences were not statistically significant. The
Career engagement

30 response to stereotype threat activation was similar in men and


women (interaction coefficient, 15.1; 95% CI, −124.5 to 154.7;
20
P = .39). However, association between stereotype threat ac-
Women tivation and FLS score differed by gender across levels of sus-
ceptibility to stereotype threat (interaction coefficient, −35.3;
10
95% CI, −47.0 to −23.6; P = .006). Among women with higher
susceptibility to stereotype threat, a trigger of stereotype threat
0
5 10 15 20 25 30 was associated with lower FLS scores (coefficient, −43.4; 95%
Perception of pro-male bias CI, −48.0 to −38.9; P = .001), but protection against stereo-
type threat was associated with higher FLS scores (coeffi-
The association between Employee Environment Diagnostic Survey score and cient, 12.0; 95% CI, 2.0-21.9; P = .04) (Figure 3). Among men
Career Engagement Scale score is plotted for men and women participants.
with a higher SIAS score, evoking negative stereotypes about
bias and career engagement scores persisted (interaction co- women trended toward a nonsignificant increase in FLS score
efficient, −1.15; 95% CI, −1.72 to 0.58; P = .01). (coefficient, 5.2; 95% CI, −7.6 to 18.0; P = .22), but those ex-
Men had significantly higher susceptibility to stereotype posed to protection against stereotype threat did (coeffi-
threat scores than women on survey 1 (median, 21.0; inter- cient, −8.2; 95% CI, −15.3 to −1.03; P = .04).
quartile range [IQR], 17.5-26.5 vs 17.5; IQR, 15.0-21.0; P < .001) Significant differences in subscale responses were noted
and survey 2 (median, 20.0; IQR, 16.5-24.0 vs 17.5; IQR, 16.0- on survey 3 (eTable 2 in Supplement 2). Among participants
20.0; P = .005) (Table 2; eTable 1, eFigure 3 and eFigure 4 in who received intervention A, women had higher perception
Supplement 2). Women had higher resilience scores than men of pro-male bias scores than men (median, 16.0; IQR, 13.0-
on survey 1 (median, 17.0; IQR, 14.0-20.5 vs 16; IQR, 12-20; 19.0 vs 13.5; IQR, 11.0-17.5; P = .04). In both study arms, women
P = .009) and survey 2 (median, 19.0; IQR, 14.0-21.5 vs 15.0; had higher resilience scores than men (A: median, 18.0; IQR,
IQR, 12.0-20.0; P = .02). Women had higher resilience scores 14.0-20.0 vs 14.0; IQR, 10.0-19.0; P = .05; B: median, 19.0; IQR,
on survey 2 compared with survey 1 (P = .004). 16.0-22.0 vs 13.5; IQR, 11.0-18.0; P = .03). When compared with

556 JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 (Reprinted) jamasurgery.com

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training Original Investigation Research

Table 2. Survey 1 and 2 Responses

Median (IQR) P value for


between-gender
Subscale Women Men difference Cronbach α
Career Engagement Scale
Survey 1 23.5 (13.5-29.0) 21.5 (16.5-29.0) .46 0.87
Survey 2 22.5 (15.5-28.5) 21 (17.0-29.0) .97 0.85
P value for between-survey difference .15 .75
Employee Environment Diagnostic Survey
Survey 1 14 (12.0-17.5) 15 (13.0-17.5) .69 0.83
Survey 2 15 (11.0-19) 15 (13.0-18.0) .69 0.82
P value for between-survey difference .36 .40
Social Identity and Attitudes Scale
Survey 1 17.5 (15.0-21.0) 21.0 (17.5-26.5) <.001 0.85
Survey 2 17.5 (16.0-20.0) 20.0 (16.5-24.0) .005 0.86
P value for between-survey difference .32 .14
Sense of Belonging Index
Survey 1 19.5 (17.0-23.5) 20.5 (17.0-23.5) .91 0.87
Survey 2 20 (16.5-23.0) 21 (17.0-23.5) .52 0.85
P value for between-survey difference .57 .24
Domain Identification Measure
Survey 1 12 (9.0-14.0) 10 (9.0-15.5) .21 0.80
Survey 2 13 (10.0-14.5) 11 (9.0-14.5) .75 0.82
P value for between-survey difference .38 .99
Connor-Davidson resilience scale
Survey 1 17 (14.0-20.5) 16 (12.0-20.0) .009 0.86
Survey 2 19 (14.0-21.5) 15 (12.0-20.0) .02 0.86
Abbreviation: IQR, interquartile
P value for between-survey difference .004 .87
range.

intervention B, intervention A was associated with higher ca- Figure 3. Gender-Based Differences in the Association Between
reer engagement (median, 23.0; IQR, 17.0-30.0 vs median 17.5; Stereotype Threat and Susceptibility to Stereotype Threat and
IQR, 13.5-25.5; P = .02), susceptibility to stereotype threat (me- Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Surgery (FLS) Skills Assessment Score
dian, 20.0; IQR, 15.0-22.0 vs 17.0; IQR, 14.5-23.5; P = .05), and
1000
sense of belonging (median, 18.0; IQR, 15.0-21.0 vs 16.0; IQR,
Men, protection against stereotype threat arm
15.0-20.0; P = .03) scores among men, but lower sense of be-
Men, stereotype threat trigger arm
longing scores among women (median, 18.0; IQR, 14.0-22.0 vs 800
Women, protection against stereotype threat arm
20.0; IQR, 16.0-25.0; P = .03) on survey 3. No adverse out- Women, stereotype threat trigger arm

comes were reported. 600


FLS score

400

Discussion
200
Factors associated with the underrepresentation of women in
academic surgery1,2 are complex. This study explores how af-
0
fective interactions between an individual and their resi- 0 5 10 15 20 25 30

dency environment are associated with engagement and tech- Susceptibility to stereotype threat score

nical skill performance, which are key factors in professional


The association between susceptibility to stereotype threat score and FLS score
success (eFigure 2 in Supplement 2).9 The association of pro- is depicted by participant gender and intervention arm.
male bias with engagement differed across genders. Al-
though the association between intervention arm and FLS score
did not differ by gender, higher susceptibility to stereotype male bias is associated with increased career engagement
threat among women who received a trigger of stereotype among men. Although the data had an opposite direction
threat was associated with lower FLS scores. These findings among women, no significant association was observed. Oth-
suggest that negative stereotypes might be detrimental to the ers have proposed that reduced engagement may contribute
operative performance of stigmatized individuals. to reduced academic performance,9 burnout, and attrition
Gender bias may influence women’s underrepresenta- among women in academic medicine.37 While further inves-
tion in surgery practice.3 We found that experiencing pro- tigations are necessary to establish the mechanisms by which

jamasurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 557

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Research Original Investigation Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training

bias challenges professional efficacy and engagement, one pos- efforts to reduce the effect of negative gender stereotypes are
sibility is that bias may result in reduced motivation among stig- needed for the retention of women in academic surgery.
matized individuals.38 Engagement appears to be malleable and This study was conceptualized within a social cognitive
therefore exists as a potential for targeted interventions aimed theory framework60; behaviors that facilitate or impede learn-
at cultivating professional development.39 Gender-based dif- ing are dynamic and develop as a result of the interaction be-
ferences in the affective constructs associated with engage- tween a person and their surroundings. Psychosocial deter-
ment suggest that individual factors, such as resilience,40,41 minants of success may fluctuate in response to environmental
also may be candidates for intervention.42 triggers (eg, stereotypes or bias). In particular, we found that
Prior research indicates that gender stereotypes depress triggering stereotype threat was associated with a lower sense
the professional performance of women. 43,44 In male- of belonging among women. Belonging is important for inten-
dominated professions, role congruity theory posits that pro- tions to persist in academic endeavors.43 Strategies such as val-
male bias can seem to be more pronounced5,6; being female ues affirmation, ie, affirming the importance of a task for
and a surgeon is perceived as incompatible with the gen- achieving goals, can ameliorate the consequences of reduced
dered stereotype of the surgeon as male. While previous stud- belonging61 and may be incorporated into surgical curricula as
ies have illustrated the consequences of gender stereotypes interventions to promote self-worth and motivation.
on promotion 4 5 - 4 8 and opportunities (eg, operative
autonomy),49,50 a causal link between bias and stereotype Limitations
threat activation has not been established. Herein, we showed This study has limitations. It was underpowered to study ef-
that after a trigger of stereotype threat, women had a higher fect modification or the intersectional effects associated with
perception of pro-male bias. However, differences in perfor- membership in multiple underrepresented groups (ie,
mance may manifest independent of whether bias exists in the intersectionality).62 The sample size restricted our ability to
work environment owing to the valences of gender stereo- perform factor analyses that would confirm the validity of the
types from a pervasive cultural context. For surgeons, a domi- modified survey and its subscales. Contamination63 that re-
nant metric of professional performance is technical ability, and sulted from residents recognizing the purpose of activating ste-
this has become an important factor by which surgeon com- reotype threat (ie, failure of planned deception) and develop-
petence and achievement are judged.51-53 The American Board ing a reactive, rather than vulnerability, response64 may have
of Surgery31 requires that surgeons achieve a passing score on biased our results toward the null. As there are no metrics with
the FLS examination. Although previous studies have been which to evaluate the degree to which training environments
unable to identify gender-based differences in FLS scores54 or perpetuate negative stereotypes about women, we could not
other laparoscopic skills metrics,55 we now show that remind- account for how ambient cues and organizational policies may
ing women who exhibit higher susceptibility to stereotype have influenced intervention effect.65
threat of negative gender stereotypes reduces their perfor-
mance on the FLS examination. This result corresponds with
data previously published indicating that individuals with un-
derrepresented demographics express increased anxiety about
Conclusions
their abilities56,57 in a manner that may reduce performance. Negative stereotypes about women and pro-male gender bias
Among men with higher susceptibility to stereotype threat, affect career engagement and technical performance among
there was a nonsignificant positive (ie, upward) directional- individuals pursuing careers in academic surgery. Mitigating
ity in FLS scores when presented with negative stereotypes the effect of negative stereotypes is requisite for the profes-
about women. This phenomenon, referred to as stereotype sional development of surgeons-in-training, particularly
lift,58 theoretically occurs if men perceive an advantage when women. Cultivating inclusivity is needed to protect our in-
women are negatively labeled.59 Given that technical skill per- vestment in future generations of surgeons and to advance
formance is requisite to ascertaining surgical competence, medicine.

ARTICLE INFORMATION Statistical analysis: Myers, Brown, Lumpkin, Abebe, fees from CSL Behring outside the submitted work.
Accepted for Publication: February 22, 2020. Rosengart. No other disclosures were reported.
Obtained funding: Myers. Funding/Support: Dr Myers was supported in part
Published Online: May 20, 2020. Administrative, technical, or material support:
doi:10.1001/jamasurg.2020.1127 by National Center for Advancing Translational
Myers, Dasari, Neal, Chaumont, Downs-Canner. Sciences training grant 5TL1TR001858-02.
Author Contributions: Drs Myers and Rosengart Supervision: Myers, Neal, Chaumont,
had full access to all of the data in the study and Downs-Canner, Lee, Rosengart. Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The National Center
take responsibility for the integrity of the data and for Advancing Translational Sciences contributed to
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Lumpkin the conduct of the study by supporting
the accuracy of the data analysis. reported receiving grants from the Agency of
Concept and design: Myers, Lumpkin, Neal, Lee, administration of the Fundamentals of
Healthcare Quality and Research during the Laparoscopic Surgery examinations at participating
Rosengart. conduct of the study. Dr Neal reported receiving
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: sites and reimbursement to the Society of American
grants and personal fees from Janssen Gastrointestinal and Endoscopic Surgeons for
Myers, Dasari, Brown, Lumpkin, Abebe, Chaumont, Pharmaceuticals, personal fees and nonfinancial
Downs-Canner, Flanagan, Lee, Rosengart. scoring of these examinations. The funding did not
support from Haemonetics, grants from Instrument include a role in study design, analysis or
Drafting of the manuscript: Myers, Rosengart. Laboratories, financial support from Haima
Critical revision of the manuscript for important interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or
Therapeutics, grants from Noveome, and personal
intellectual content: All authors.

558 JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 (Reprinted) jamasurgery.com

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training Original Investigation Research

approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit 11. Pintrich PR, DeGroot E. Motivational and 26. Pascual-Leone A, Singh T, Scoboria A. Using
the manuscript for publication. self-regulated learning components of classroom deception ethically: practical research guidelines for
Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3. academic performance. J Educ Psychol. 1990;82(1): researchers and reviewers. Can Psychol. 2010;51
33-40. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 (4):241-248. doi:10.1037/a0021119
Additional Contributions: We acknowledge the
contribution of the residents who participated in 12. Scalone G. “I’m a Consumer, I’m Not a Scientist”: 27. Milam LA, Cohen GL, Mueller C, Salles A.
this study as well as the faculty and staff of the Cultivating Student Domain Identification, Agency, Stereotype threat and working memory among
University of North Carolina–Chapel Hill, UPMC, and and Affect Through Engagement in Scientific Practices. surgical residents. Am J Surg. 2018;216(4):824-829.
University of Washington departments of general University of Washington; 2016. doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2018.07.064
surgery. Melina R. Kibbe, MD, Michael O. Meyers, 13. Aronson J, Fried CB, Good C. Reducing the 28. Society of American Gastrointestinal and
MD (both University of North Carolina–Chapel Hill), effects of stereotype threat on African-American Endoscopic Surgeons. Fundamentals of
and Karen D. Horvath, MD (University of college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Laparoscopic Surgery. Published 2019. Accessed
Washington), provided encouragement and J Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;38(2):113-125. September 10, 2019. https://www.flsprogram.org
support; Kathie Patterson (University of North doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1491 29. Derossis AM, Fried GM, Abrahamowicz M,
Carolina–Chapel Hill), Maggie Mrozinski, BS (UPMC) 14. Steele CM, Aronson J. Stereotype threat and Sigman HH, Barkun JS, Meakins JL. Development of
, and Inga Brissman (Society of American the intellectual test performance of African a model for training and evaluation of laparoscopic
Gastrointestinal and Endoscopic Surgeons) assisted Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):797-811. skills. Am J Surg. 1998;175(6):482-487. doi:10.1016/
in the coordination and administration of doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 S0002-9610(98)00080-4
Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Surgery
examinations; and Scott D. Rothenberger, PhD 15. Ramsbottom-Lucier M, Johnson MM, Elam CL. 30. Fried GM, Feldman LS, Vassiliou MC, et al.
(University of Pittsburgh), assisted with statistical Age and gender differences in students’ Proving the value of simulation in laparoscopic
analysis. In addition, we recognize the faculty and preadmission qualifications and medical school surgery. Ann Surg. 2004;240(3):518-525.
staff of the University of Pittsburgh’s Clinical and performances. Acad Med. 1995;70(3):236-239. doi:10.1097/01.sla.0000136941.46529.56
Translational Science Institute and the Pittsburgh doi:10.1097/00001888-199503000-00016 31. Soper NJ, Fried GM. The fundamentals of
Surgical Outcomes Research Center for their 16. Rosengart TK, Mason MC, LeMaire SA, et al. The laparoscopic surgery: its time has come. Bull Am
support of this study. No financial compensation seven attributes of the academic surgeon: critical Coll Surg. 2008;93(9):30-32.
outside of salary was provided. aspects of the archetype and contributions to the 32. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of
surgical community. Am J Surg. 2017;214(2):165-179. Cronbach’s alpha. Int J Med Educ. 2011;2:53-55.
REFERENCES doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2017.02.003 doi:10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd
1. Valantine HA, Collins FS. National Institutes of 17. Hutter MM, Kellogg KC, Ferguson CM, Abbott 33. Little RJA. Post-stratification: a modeler’s
Health addresses the science of diversity. Proc Natl WM, Warshaw AL. The impact of the 80-hour perspective. J Am Stat Assoc. 1993;88(423):1001-
Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(40):12240-12242. resident workweek on surgical residents and 1012. doi:10.1080/01621459.1993.10476368
doi:10.1073/pnas.1515612112 attending surgeons. Ann Surg. 2006;243(6):864-
871. doi:10.1097/01.sla.0000220042.48310.66 34. Wong RL, Sullivan MC, Yeo HL, Roman SA, Bell
2. Murphy B. AMA to aim for more diverse, better RH Jr, Sosa JA. Race and surgical residency: results
prepared physician workforce. Published June 14, 18. Greenbaum A, Lawrence E, Auyang ED, Russell from a national survey of 4339 US general surgery
2017. Accessed October 10, 2019. https://www. JC, Paul JS. The mandatory participation in a residents. Ann Surg. 2013;257(4):782-787.
ama-assn.org/education/medical-school-diversity/ wellness program: the general surgery resident’s doi:10.1097/SLA.0b013e318269d2d0
ama-aim-more-diverse-better-prepared- perspective. J Am Coll Surg. 2017;225(4):S178-S179.
physician-workforce doi:10.1016/j.jamcollsurg.2017.07.406 35. Flyckt RL, White EE, Goodman LR, Mohr C,
Dutta S, Zanotti KM. The use of laparoscopy
3. Agrawal S. Diversity & inclusion in surgery. 19. Yeung F, Yuan C, Jackson DS, Chun MBJ. Gone, simulation to explore gender differences in resident
Published April 18, 2018. Accessed October 10, but not forgotten? survey of resident attitudes surgical confidence. Obstet Gynecol Int. 2017;2017:
2019. http://www.aasurg.org/blog/diversity- toward a cultural standardized patient examination 1945801. doi:10.1155/2017/1945801
inclusion-in-surgery for a general surgery residency program. Health
Equity. 2017;1(1):150-155. doi:10.1089/heq.2017.0016 36. Swanstrom LL, Fried GM, Hoffman KI, Soper
4. Ulloa JG, Viramontes O, Ryan G, Wells K, NJ. Beta test results of a new system assessing
Maggard-Gibbons M, Moreno G. Perceptual and 20. Munro PA. The Development and Evaluation of competence in laparoscopic surgery. J Am Coll Surg.
structural facilitators and barriers to becoming a the Employee Environment Diagnostic Survey. 2006;202(1):62-69. doi:10.1016/j.jamcollsurg.2005.
surgeon: a qualitative study of African-American ProQuest Information & Learning; September 2002. 09.024
and Latino surgeons. Acad Med. 2018;93(9):1326- 21. Picho K, Brown SW. Can stereotype threat be
1334. doi:10.1097/ACM.0000000000002282 37. Grisso JA, Sammel MD, Rubenstein AH, et al.
measured? a validation of the Social Identity and A randomized controlled trial to improve the
5. Eagly AH, Karau SJ. Role congruity theory of Attitudes Scale (SIAS). J Adv Academics. 2011;22(3): success of women assistant professors. J Womens
prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol Rev. 374-411. doi:10.1177/1932202X1102200302 Health (Larchmt). 2017;26(5):571-579. doi:10.1089/
2002;109(3):573-598. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109. 22. Hirschi A, Freund PA, Herrmann A. The Career jwh.2016.6025
3.573 Engagement Scale: development and validation of a 38. Inzlicht M, Schmader T, eds. ST: Theory,
6. Wood W, Eagly AH, Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey measure of proactive career behaviors. J Career Process, and Applications. Oxford University Press;
G, eds. Handbook of Social Psychology. Wiley; 2010. Assess. 2014;22(4):575-594. doi:10.1177/ 2012.
7. Wong NZ, Abelson JS, Symer M, Yeo HL. Gender 1069072713514813
39. Christenson SL, Reschly AL, Wylie C, eds.
disparities in retention and promotion of academic 23. Hagerty BMK, Patusky K. Developing a Handbook of Research on Student Engagement.
surgeons: a prospective national cohort [abstract]. measure of sense of belonging. Nurs Res. 1995;44 Springer; 2012. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7
Accessed September 10, 2019. https://www.asc- (1):9-13. doi:10.1097/00006199-199501000-00003
abstracts.org/auth2018/wong-natalie/ 40. Watkins NL. Disarming Microaggressions: How
24. Smith JL, White PH. Development of the Black College Students Self-regulate Racial Stressors
8. Aiken L. Attitudes and Related Psychosocial Domain Identification Measure: a tool for Within Predominately White Institutions.
Constructs: Theories, Assessment, and Research. Sage; investigating stereotype threat effects. Educ Dissertation. Columbia University; 2012.
2002. Psychol Meas. 2001;61(6):1040-1057. doi:10.1177/ doi:10.7916/D84T6RFR
9. Marks HM. Student engagement in instructional 00131640121971635
41. Hemmings A. Conflicting images? being black
activity: patterns in the elementary, middle, and 25. Campbell-Sills L, Stein MB. Psychometric and a model high school student. Anthropol Educ Q.
high school years. Am Educ Res J. 2000;37:153-184. analysis and refinement of the Connor-Davidson 1996;27(1):20-50. doi:10.1525/aeq.1996.27.1.
doi:10.3102/00028312037001153 Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): validation of a 10-item 04x0640p
10. Finn JD. Withdrawing from school. Rev Educ Res. measure of resilience. J Trauma Stress. 2007;20(6):
1019-1028. doi:10.1002/jts.20271 42. Forbes CE, Schmader T, Allen JJB. The role of
1989;59(2):117-142. doi:10.3102/ devaluing and discounting in performance
00346543059002117 monitoring: a neurophysiological study of

jamasurgery.com (Reprinted) JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 559

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024
Research Original Investigation Effects of Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training

minorities under threat. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 51. Allen M, Pierce O. Making the cut: why choosing women’s performance in mathematics. J Exp Educ.
2008;3(3):253-261. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn012 the right surgeon matters even more than you 2012;80(2):137-149. doi:10.1080/00220973.2011.
43. Schmader T, Johns M, Forbes C. An integrated know. ProPublica. Published July 13, 2015. Accessed 567312
process model of stereotype threat effects on July 22, 2019. https://www.propublica.org/article/ 59. Walton G, Cohen GL. Stereotype lift. J Exp Soc
performance. Psychol Rev. 2008;115(2):336-356. surgery-risks-patient-safety-surgeon-matters Psychol. 2003;39:456-467. doi:10.1016/S0022-
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336 52. Veillard J, Champagne F, Klazinga N, Kazandjian 1031(03)00019-2
44. Spencer SJ, Steele CM, Quinn DM. ST and V, Arah OA, Guisset AL. A performance assessment 60. Diperna JC, Elliott SN. Promoting academic
women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. framework for hospitals: the WHO regional office enablers to improve student achievement:
1999;35:4-28. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 for Europe PATH project. Int J Qual Health Care. introduction to a miniseries. School Psychol Rev.
2005;17(6):487-496. doi:10.1093/intqhc/mzi072 2002;31(3):293-297.
45. Davison HK, Burke MJ. Sex discrimination in
simulated employment contexts: a meta-analytic 53. Burger I, Schill K, Goodman S. Disclosure of 61. Layous K, Davis EM, Garcia J, Purdie-Vaugns V,
investigation. J Vocat Behav. 2000;56(2):225-248. individual surgeon’s performance rates during Cook JE, Cohen GL. Feeling left out, but affirmed:
doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1711 informed consent: ethical and epistemological protecting against the negative effects of low
considerations. Ann Surg. 2007;245(4):507-513. belonging in college. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;69:
46. Olian JD, Schwab DP, Haberfeld Y. The impact doi:10.1097/01.sla.0000242713.82125.d1
of applicant gender compared to qualifications on 227-231. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.008
hiring recommendations: a meta-analysis of 54. Kolozsvari NO, Andalib A, Kaneva P, et al. Sex is 62. O’Brien LT, Blodorn A, Adams G, Garcia DM,
experimental studies. Organ Behav Hum Decis not everything: the role of gender in early Hammer E. Ethnic variation in gender-STEM
Process. 1988;41(2):180-195. doi:10.1016/0749-5978 performance of a fundamental laparoscopic skill. stereotypes and STEM participation: an
(88)90025-8 Surg Endosc. 2011;25(4):1037-1042. doi:10.1007/ intersectional approach. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor
s00464-010-1311-8 Psychol. 2015;21(2):169-180. doi:10.1037/a0037944
47. Tosi HL, Einbender SW. The effects of the type
and amount of information in sex discrimination 55. Ali A, Subhi Y, Ringsted C, Konge L. Gender 63. Torgerson DJ. Contamination in trials: is cluster
research: a meta-analysis. Acad Manage J. 1985;28 differences in the acquisition of surgical skills: randomisation the answer? BMJ. 2001;322(7282):
(3):712-723. a systematic review. Surg Endosc. 2015;29(11): 355-357. doi:10.1136/bmj.322.7282.355
3065-3073. doi:10.1007/s00464-015-4092-2
48. Lyons NB, Bernardi K, Huang L, et al. Gender 64. Eagly AH, Carli LL. The female leadership
disparity in surgery: an evaluation of surgical 56. Myers SP, Hill KA, Nicholson KJ, et al. advantage: an evaluation of the evidence. Leadersh
societies. Surg Infect (Larchmt). 2019;20(5):406-410. A qualitative study of gender differences in the Q. 2003;14(6):807-834. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.
doi:10.1089/sur.2018.220 experiences of general surgery trainees. J Surg Res. 09.004
2018;228:127-134. doi:10.1016/j.jss.2018.02.043
49. Hoops H, Heston A, Dewey E, Spight D, Brasel 65. Walton GM, Murphy MC, Ryan AM. Stereotype
K, Kiraly L. Resident autonomy in the operating 57. Fonseca AL, Reddy V, Longo WE, Udelsman R, threat in organizations: implications for equity and
room: does gender matter? Am J Surg. 2019;217(2): Gusberg RJ. Operative confidence of graduating performance. Annu Rev Organ Psychol Organ Behav.
301-305. doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2018.12.023 surgery residents: a training challenge in a changing 2015;2:523-550. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-
environment. Am J Surg. 2014;207(5):797-805. 032414-111322
50. Meyerson SL, Sternbach JM, Zwischenberger doi:10.1016/j.amjsurg.2013.09.033
JB, Bender EM. The effect of gender on resident
autonomy in the operating room. J Surg Educ. 2017; 58. Johnson HJ, Barnard-Brak L, Saxon TF,
74(6):e111-e118. doi:10.1016/j.jsurg.2017.06.014 Johnson MK. An experimental study of the effects
of stereotype threat and stereotype lift on men and

Invited Commentary

Gender Bias and Stereotypes in Surgical Training


Is It Really Women Residents We Need to Worry About?
Caprice C. Greenberg, MD, MPH; Jacob A. Greenberg, MD, EdM

It is uncomfortable to acknowledge that our interpersonal in- (Fundamentals of Laparoscopic Surgery [FLS] score) as mea-
teractions and experiences in the workplace are affected by sures of professional success. This study is richly nuanced and
gender. Traditional mantra tells us that gender (or skin color packed with insightful observations about this important is-
or religion or any other char- sue. There are many conclusions that can be drawn from this
acteristic) should not influ- well-designed study, and we are not convinced the authors fo-
Related article page 552 ence our expectations, our as- cus on the most salient ones. The authors’ main conclusions
sessment, or our treatment of are: (1) negative stereotypes about women and pro-male gen-
any individual as a surgeon. This one-size-fits-all approach has der bias affect career engagement and technical perfor-
existed, especially in surgical training, for a very long time. The mance; and (2) mitigating the effect of negative stereotypes
problem with this, of course, is that the one size is not agnos- is requisite for the professional development of trainees, par-
tic but rather reflects the traditional majority, and the rest of ticularly women.
us have been expected to conform. To think otherwise re- The study included a randomized clinical trial, with the
quires an acknowledgement that interpersonal interactions and primary finding that triggering stereotype threat did not in-
experiences vary by gender and as a result may require differ- fluence FLS score overall or when stratified by gender. This is
ent approaches to training.1 strongly encouraging and suggests that negative gender ste-
In this issue of JAMA Surgery, Myers et al 2 present a reotypes do not affect technical performance either for men
thought-provoking study investigating gender bias and ste- or women. While this may be an issue of power, it should be
reotypes in surgical training. The study explores how affec- considered a negative trial.
tive interactions between an individual and the residency en- The second primary aim was to examine the effect of pro-
vironment affect engagement and technical skill performance male gender bias on professional engagement. First and most

560 JAMA Surgery July 2020 Volume 155, Number 7 (Reprinted) jamasurgery.com

© 2020 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.


Downloaded from jamanetwork.com by University of California - Irvine user on 01/07/2024

You might also like