You are on page 1of 16

PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN

Vol. 52, No. 2, 1955

COMPLEX TASKS FOR USE IN HUMAN


PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH1
WILBERT S. RAY
Skill Components Research Laboratory, Air Force Personnel and
Training Research Center

Progress in any field of research lems, only the more complex have
depends on many things in addition been considered. Complexity refers
to the ideas produced by the individ- here to the amount of work the sub-
ual research men, among them the ject must do in solving the problem:
availability of measuring instruments (a) several responses are usually re-
and of standard materials and tech- quired, (b) more than one hypothesis
niques. The current high level of is available, (c) trial and error in-
activity in research in learning theory, cludes more than one or two trials,
for example, would be impossible and (d) more time than that needed
without the existence of nonsense for a single brief act is required. Al-
syllables, mazes, conditioning tech- though complexity was the principal
niques, and so on. The purpose of criterion, others were also used: (a)
the present paper is to consider the publication of a clear description of
extent of the use of such materials in the problem, (b) the presence of a
the field of human problem solving, solution which could be classified as
or, more specifically, in that part of successful, and (c) in most cases, the
the field which works with complex presence of some sort of physical
apparatus problems, by examination equipment in the task. Certain kinds
of a number of such problems which of complex problems have been
have been used in past research. omitted on what may be a purely
Development in the direction of arbitrary basis, as when mathemat-
standardized materials is already ical problems were ignored because
taking place in some sorts of human their great number discouraged any
problem-solving and reasoning in- attempt at selecting a few as sam-
vestigations: in work on concept for- ples. The same thing is true of card
mation, the Weigl and Vigotsky ma- tricks and parlor puzzles of the bent
terials and the more recent Wisconsin nail variety. An additional criterion
Card Sorting Test and Hovland's operated in the selection of problems
flowers; water jar problems, ana- in which the experimenter could
grams, etc. in research on set; short, watch the subject's solving processes
statistically evaluated problems of at work, as opposed to problems
many kinds in factor analysis stud- where he sees only the solution, the
ies. In order to keep the present en- end product.
deavor within reasonable limits, of Problem solving may be defined as
the many remaining sorts of prob- the process of changing a given situa-
tion to a specified different situation,
1
This research was carried out under the the process being new to the solver,
Air Force Personnel and Training Research and ending when the solution is
Center, Lackland AFB, San Antonio, Texas, achieved—which means that the
in support of Project 7709. Permission is
granted for reproduction, translation, publica- process is not repeated. This defini-
tion, use, and disposal in whole or in part by tion will include cases in which the
or for the United States Government. problem is solved by "pure" trial
134
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 135
and error, as in McGeoch's Problem is provided by one group which
Box (task 28). If one wished to worked on Maier's Hat Rack (task
exclude that condition he could add: 10). The experimenter required this
the situation being such that there group to watch him build the hat
is some basis for selecting one alter- rack and then asked them what pos-
native as more probable than the sible function the structure could
others. serve (21, pp. 352-353). Referring
Following Duncker (10, p. 1), back to the definition of problem
problem-solving tasks may be solving given above, "changing . . .
thought of as divided into three to a specified different situation,"
parts: (a) the given situation, (&) the it is the "specified" part which the
desired situation, and (c) the method hat rack subjects were asked to sup-
of proceeding from the one to the ply; so this example may be classified
other. The individual problem-solv- with the procedure of predicting the
ing experiments discussed here seem final situation.
to use either of two procedures; they The tasks considered in the prepa-
ask the experimental organism to dis- ration of this paper are listed and
cover the operations which will briefly described in the next section.
change the first into the final situa- They include all those which met the
tion, or they give him the first situa- criteria described in a previous para-
tion and the operations and ask him graph, and which were reported in
to predict the final situation. The articles covered in the Psychological
above definition is intended to in- Abstracts from the middle of 1948 to
clude both of these procedures. Most the middle of 1953, plus those in
of the tasks described use the first of journals too new to have been ab-
them. Szekely's Candle Lever (task stracted by July 1, 1953. Actually,
1, below) and his Hydrostatic Levers this search of Psychological Abstracts
(tasks 3 to 5) are examples of the unearthed very few problem-solving
second. Discovering the operations tasks which had not been described
sometimes includes a fair amount of previously. They and a few added
intellectual work; so one may clas- older tasks have been referred to un-
sify here the form of the Candle der the earliest references available
Lever in which the subject is shown ever, on the ground that they were not used
the first situation, the operations, as problem-solving tasks as problem solving
and the final situation, and is asked is defined here.
to explain why the operations pro- As used by Deutsche (9) the problems pre-
duced the result, i.e., is asked to pro- sent examples of several of the desirable at-
tributes listed; they provide scoring continua
vide the rational basis for the opera- since it was possible to classify the answers
tions.2 A different kind of example on an eight-point scale; and there is some evi-
dence of validity since the split-half "reliabil-
2
It is tempting to add further examples of ity" coefficient of .74 suggests that if the ex-
providing an explanation from the demonstra- perimenter has correctly identified some of
tions used by Piaget (27) and Deutsche (9), the tasks as examples of some particular psy-
such as inverting a glass jar over a burning chological process, then the others are prob-
candle and asking why it stopped burning, ably examples of the same process. On the
putting a stone into a beaker of water and ask- other hand, with regard to the "special knowl-
ing why the water level rose, or putting a edges" criterion, it seems not impossible that
penny into a box which has strings attached the answers the subjects gave to the questions
to the corners, swinging it in a circle overhead, might be better classified as knowledge rather
and then asking why the penny does not fall than as reasoning, which does not imply that
out. These descriptions are not included, how- they are not examples of how children think.
136 WILBERT S. RAY
for each, It is thought that the list ing, a container half full of water is
includes most of the tasks of the de- placed on one end of a long narrow
sired sort which have been described board (50X10 cm.) and the board is
in the psychological literature in the balanced on a fulcrum. A metal
last twenty years. weight is hung just above the water.
DESCRIPTIONS OF THE TASKS In a second form of the task, the
metal weight is replaced by a wooden
1. The Candle Lever (Szekely, 32) one of the same size.
Given and required: The central Required: If the weight is lowered
part of the task is an arrangement of until partly submerged, not touching
a lever with a fulcrum toward one the sides or bottom of the container,
end of it, the lever being balanced by what will happen to the lever?
a candle on the short end. In one
form of the task, the candle is burned, 4. The Hydrostatic Lever, Symmetrical
the lever tips up, and the subject is (Szekely, 32)
asked to explain why. In another
form, the subject is shown the equip- Given: A lever with a metal weight
ment with the candle not burning at one end and a wooden weight at
and asked what would happen if it the other large enough to balance
were lit. In the third form, the parts the metal when the lever is supported
of the apparatus are placed on the at the center. The wooden weight is
table with other objects, including a higher and wider than the metal, and
box of matches, and the subject is they are hung with bottom edges
asked to construct an arrangement level. The weights hang in a large
in which the lever will tip after a few container which contains water up
minutes. In a variant of this third to a level just below the bottom of the
form of the experiment, the objects weights.
on the table include a labeled bottle Required: Suppose we put in more
of ether or benzine. water so that both weights are im-
mersed to a depth of 2 cm. What will
2. The Inclined Plane (Szekely, 32) happen?
Given: A small cart stands half-
way up an inclined plane, a string 5. The Hydrostatic Lever, Unsym-
running from the cart over a pulley metrical (Szekely, 32)
at the top of the plane and down to a
metal weight. The weight is half Given: A lever with a metal weight
submerged in a container of water so at one end and a wooden weight of
it balances the cart. On the table are equal size at the other is balanced on
a pipette and another container a fulcrum, which would be closer to
partly full of water. In another form the metal weight. The weights hang
of the task, the metal weight is re- in a large container which is filled
placed by a block of wood. with water almost up to the bottom
Required: Think of a method by edges of the weights.
which the cart can be moved up the Required: Suppose we filled the con-
incline 1 or 2 cm. Use the pipette, tainer with water. The rising water
but do not touch the cart itself. level would simultaneously touch the
bottom of both objects. Now we let
3. Hydrostatic Lever, Single Weight more water run in, until both objects
(Szekely, 32) are immersed by about 2 cm. What
Given: With the subject watch- will happen? (32, p. 16).
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 137
6. The Hollow and Solid Spheres Maier says, "All the experience
(Szekely, 33) necessary for the solution was given
Given: After teaching the subject to the subject and did not have to be
something of moments of inertia and recalled by him" (17, p. 116).
of rotation, the experimenter says to Required: Make two pendulums,
the subject, "Imagine that we have each holding a piece of chalk, which
two metallic spheres of exactly equal will swing over the chalk marks al-
size and exactly equal weight before ready on the floor, and will mark the
us. The spheres are covered with a floor.
lacquer which makes it impossible This task was also used by Weaver
to distinguish the spheres from one and Madden (37).
another. However, there exists a
concealed difference between the two 8. Blowing Out the Candles (Maier,
spheres. One consists of a very light, 17, 19)
the other of a very heavy metal. Given: Among many objects lying
How is this possible?" (33, pp. 392- on a table there are pieces of glass
393). If the subject is not able to tell and rubber tubing, 6 to 12 in. in
that one must be hollow, the experi- length, poles several feet in length,
menter tells him so. The experi- paper clamps; lighted candles are on
menter then draws cross sections of a table some feet distant.
the spheres, with the hollow space Hint 1. Water is siphoned from
relatively small. one container to another with a tube
Required: How could we deter- constructed like the one involved
mine which of the two spheres is the here. This is to give the subject the
hollow and which the solid one? idea of the construction of the tube.
There are no physical or chemical Hint 2. A piece of cord is hooked
tools at your disposal. The spheres over a nail by the use of a pole. This
are not to be damaged, no holes is to bring the pole to the attention of
drilled, no pieces cut out, and no the subject.
paint removed. Try to think of a Required: Blow out the candles
method so simple that we can decide while standing several feet away (8
this question in this room without ft. in the later article, 10).
any apparatus. This task was also used by Saug-
7. The Pendulum Problem (Maier, stad (29).
17)
Given: The subject is presented 9. The Two String Problem (Maier,
with four poles varying in length, one 18)
table clamp (C clamp), two burette Given: Two strings dangle from
clamps, two pieces of wire, several the ceiling too far apart to permit
pieces of chalk, and several weights anyone to reach both of them at the
(pieces of lead tubing at Berlin, bolts same time, or to carry one over to
at Michigan), and there are two chalk where he can reach the other. There
marks on the floor. are many other objects in the room
One pole must be at least as long including a chair, a stick, more
as the distance between the chalk string, pliers, etc.
marks. Two of the remaining poles Hint 1. The experimenter brushed
must together equal the distance against a cord and set it swinging.
from floor to ceiling, plus enough Hint 2. The subject was handed
overlap for the C clamp to hold them. the pliers and told to use them.
138 WILBERT S. RAY
Required: Tie the two pieces of 12. The Box Problem (Duncker, 10;
string together. and Adamson, 1)
This task was also used by Bat- Given: On a table, among many
tersby, Teuber, and Bender (2), Birch other objects, lie a few tacks; three
and Rabinowitz (4), Cofer (7), Gofer little pasteboard boxes (more or less
and Gelfand (8), and Guetzkow (12). the size of match boxes), apparently
10. The Hat Rack (Maier, 19, 21) not identical in size, shape, color,
position; and three candles. In one
Given: The subject is presented experimental situation, the candles
with two poles, neither of which will are placed in one box, the tacks in
reach the ceiling, but each more than another, and matches in the third.
half the required length, and a C Required: Put the candles side by
clamp. side on the door of the room at eye
Hint 1. The two string problem height ("for visual experiments").
is presented first, each string hung
from an inverted U-shaped construc- 13. The Gimlet Problem (Duncker,
tion which utilizes the principle of the 10; and Adamson, 1)
hat rack for each support pole. This Given: Among other objects on a
is built by the experimenter, and table lie three cords, two screw hooks,
left standing during the solving of the and a gimlet. In one experimental
hat rack task. condition, there are three holes in a
Hint 2. The structure of Hint 1 is board on the wall, in the other the
built and then removed before the holes have not been bored.
hat rack task is presented. Required: Hang the three cords
Hint3. The subjects who had been from the board.
in the group in which the two string
structure had been removed were 14. ThePaper ClipProUem (Duncker,
asked to draw it. 10; and Adamson, 1)
(These hints were used in the later Given: Many objects lie on a
experiment, 21.) table, among them a piece of white
Required: Build a hat rack sturdy cardboard, four smaller pieces of
enough to hold a hat and a heavy black cardboard, paper clips, glue or
coat. thumb tacks. A screw eye is fixed in
This task was also used by Cofer the ceiling. In one condition of the
and Gelfand (8). experiment the black pieces are fas-
11. The Children's Swastika Pathway tened to the white one with paper
(Maier, 20) clips, in the other they are glued on
(Adamson had them tacked on).
Given: The child learns his way Required: Hang the cardboard ar-
around a swastika-shaped path, with rangement from the ceiling screw eye.
a booth at each end, the booths being
different in size, shape, and furniture. 15. The Cut Pyramid (Katona, 13,
The child then is shown a toy in one Duncker, 10; and Reid, 28)
booth, the toy requiring a penny to Given: An orthogonal tetrahedron
make it work. (a four-sided pyramid, the sides being
Required: The child is given a triangles of equal size), cut into two
penny somewhere outside the ap- pieces along a plane defined by a line
paratus, introduced into a booth running between the midpoints of
other than the one with the toy, and the sides of a triangle and parallel
expected to find his way to the toy. to its base, and a similar line parallel
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 139
to the same edge of the tetrahedron 75-cc. flask, a penstaff, pencils, a file,
and in another triangle. were presented to the subject. In
Aid 1, What does this problem, to group experiments the experimenter
form a solid having only triangle sur- put the balloon into the flask and
faces, really mean? blew into the balloon.
Aid 2. Doesn't it mean forming Required: To blow up the balloon
large triangular surfaces, or at least inside the bottle.
one for a start?
Aid 3. Which two of all these sur- 18. Weighing the Coins (Simmel, 31)
faces when seen as extending to- Given: The subject is to imagine
gether in the same plane, i.e., to- he has eight coins, which may or may
gether forming one plane, ought to not include one counterfeit which
make that plane a large triangular would be lighter than the others. He
surface, just the kind we are looking- also has an imaginary balance.
for? Required: In two weighings, iden-
(These aids were used by Reid, 28.) tify the lighter coin or prove that
Required: Put these two pieces to- there is none.
gether to make a pyramid. (Duncker Simmel also used nine coins, one
did not permit actual manipulation certainly counterfeit, in two weigh-
of the pieces, but had the subject ings; twenty-five coins, one certainly
tell him how to do it.) counterfeit, in three weighings.
16. Breaking the Beads (Bulbrook, 5) 19. The Shooting of the Captain and
Given: The subject is presented His Son (Burack, 6)
with a string of beads arranged to Given: The subject is presented
alternate one yellow with two white, with a printed problem referring to
but with five white beads in the two men shot to death at close range,
center. Other objects (pliers, needle, the gun being under one body, no
saw, etc.) lie on the table, footprints except their own, and so
Required: Instruction 1. Make a on.
single regularly repeated pattern Required: The subject is asked to
without either unstringing or re- tell whether each man was killed by
stringing the beads, and without either accident, suicide, murder, and later
knotting or breaking the thread. Pro- is asked more specific questions such
ceed aloud. as, "Was there information which
Instruction 2 was like the above would tell which man died first?" etc.
except that, "Use any of the means
supplied on the table" was inserted 20. The Cylinder in the Can (Saug-
between the two sentences. stad, 29)
Note: The task following the one Given: A wooden cylinder is
above in Bulbrook's article was simi- placed upright in a can into which it
lar to it. The string was made of un- fits loosely. Apparently the top of
painted wooden beads and ink stained the cylinder should be a bit below
wooden beads and the subject had the top of the can, and the fit should
to stain the extra beads with ink be close enough that one cannot put
from a bottle on the table. his fingers in between the can and the
cylinder. On the table beside the can
17. The Balloon in the Bottle (Bul- are a penholder, pliers, thread, screw
brook, 5) driver, pitcher of water, drinking
Given: A deflated toy balloon, a glasses.
140 WILBERT S. RAY
Required; Get the cylinder out of with two discs, then three, and so on
the can without tipping the can. to seven.
This task was also used by Shaw
21. Twenty Questions (Lindley, 16; (30).
Taylor and Faust, 34)
Given; The subject is told that the 24. Making the Last Draw (]. C.
experimenter is thinking of an object Peterson, 26)
and whether it is in the animal, vege- Given: The subject is presented
table, or mineral kingdom. The sub- with a set of seven beads on a string
ject is to ask questions which narrow (or seven matches or pennies or
the field of possibilities, each ques- other objects lying on a table). He
tion being answered by "Yes," "No," is told that he and the experimenter
"Partly," "Sometimes," or "Not in will draw from the set alternately,
the usual sense of the word." A each taking one or two beads as he
question which cannot be so answered wishes. The subject gets the first
must be restated. This description turn and is to try to draw the last
applies to Taylor and Faust's pro- bead.
cedure. Required: To learn to win the
Required: The subject is to be able game every time with different num-
to name the object with thirty ques- bers of beads; to find a general prin-
tions or less (Taylor and Faust). ciple for winning (a) with draws of
one or two beads, (b) with draws of
22. The Incorrect Square Root (Marks, other numbers.
22) This task was also used by Waters
Given: The subject is taught a (36).
method of finding square roots using
a calculating machine and a table of 25. The Electromaze (Kruglak, 15)
factors. By means of incorrect en- Given: The apparatus presents
tries in the table, the subject gets the subject with four push buttons
wrong answers to two out of four arranged in a square, with a fifth but-
problems. ton in the middle. On top of the box
Required: Ascertain the source of are a red light and a green light. The
the error. center one of the five buttons is an
"erase" button.
23. The Disc Transfer (]. Peterson Required: The subject is to solve
and Lanier, 25) a series of seven problems, most of
Given: The subject is given three which are sequences of button pushes.
stationary circles (e.g., painted on a The red light indicates an error in the
board) and several discs varying in sequence and the subject then pushes
diameter, stacked on one circle in the "erase" button and starts again
order of size, the smallest at the top. on a new trial. The green light ap-
(If one were going to make himself a pears only at the end of a correct se-
set of discs, it would be easier to quence,
make squares.)
Required: The subject is to trans- 26. Water Jars Problems (Terman,
fer the stack to another circle, mov- 35)
ing one disc at a time, using the third Given: Typically, the subject is
circle as a way station, never putting told to imagine that he has two or
a disc on a smaller one. He starts three containers of specified sizes,
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 141

although sometimes actual contain- out, on each side. Ten weights, all of
ers are used. the same diameter and of different
Required: To tell the experimenter lengths, are provided, again plainly
how to use the containers to secure numbered 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 12.
a given amount of water, no estima- Required: The experimenter says,
tions (as of "half") being permitted. for example, "If you had a 1 ounce
This sort of task has been used by weight on 4 (he puts it there), where
many people, lately especially in in- would you put a 2 ounce weight to
vestigations of set. balance it?" The subject gives his
answer in writing and the weight is
27. The Multiple-Choice Apparatus not actually hung on the lever to
(Yerkes, 39; and Beasley, 3) show that it does or does not balance.
Given: In the form of the equip- The subject receives no knowledge of
ment used for humans, the subject results, neither by actual trial, nor
sits before an apparatus from which by any statement from the experi-
any one or any set of twelve keys menter. Instead, each problem is
can be protruded toward him. Re- followed by a demonstration of a dif-
peated problems can be presented by ferent problem. There are twenty
different sets of three keys each, or problems and twenty demonstrations.
by successive sets of keys which vary The subject is now asked to state the
in number except that the number is principle involved.
always odd, and so on.
Required: To find the key which ATTRIBUTES WHICH MAY BE
will cause a buzzer to sound; to find USEFUL IN PROBLEM-
a rule by which to identify the correct SOLVING TASKS
key in future sets. It is possible to offer a set of at-
Note: This task has a correlative tributes which may be found useful
form which can be used with animals, in problem-solving tasks, although
and it is ordinarily assumed that the none of the tasks considered in the
problem is the same for human and paper meet the complete list. In
animal subjects. fact, it may be doubted that any one
task ever will exhibit all of them, and
28. McGeoch's Problem Box (Erick- some of them are certainly unsuited
sen, 11) to certain kinds of investigations,
Given: The subject is given a box, as for example, "only one way of
8 X 8 X 1 2 in,, with three knobs pro- solving the problem" is certainly not
truding from each of two opposite desired in most of the current experi-
sides, and a seventh knob on one ments on set. The attributes are
end. The knobs can be pushed, pulled, presented as suggestions to be con-
turned, or ignored, making a com- sidered in choosing or designing
plex trial and error problem. tasks, and in the hope that they will
Required: Remove the end con- serve as a base from which improved
taining the knob. lists of criteria can be constructed.
Two important criteria for prob-
29. Moments of Levers (G. M. Peter- lem-solving tasks are that the task
son, 24) should provide a scoring continuum
Given: A lever is set on a table with longer and more useful than the two-
hooks at two-inch intervals out from step continuum of pass-fail scores
the fulcrum, the hooks being plainly which have been frequent in prob-
numbered 1, 2, 3, 4 from the center lem-solving tasks, and that it should
142 WILBERT S. RAY
provide the experimenter with a same lever, but with differing weights
maximum amount.of knowledge of and distances, or in the Multiple-
what the subject is doing. Hypothe- Choice Apparatus (task 27); (c) the
ses must be regarded as intervening number of steps on which the sub-
variables, definable on the basis of ject succeeded (see paragraph be-
the task presented and of the overt low) ; and (d) the amount of assist-
responses produced. The experi- ance provided before solution as was
menter's knowledge of what the sub- done by Maier with his Pendulum
ject is doing is provided by his knowl- Problem (task 7) and by Reid (28) in
edge of the task and by these overt an experiment with Katona's Cut
responses. Other more detailed char- Pyramid (task 15). Deutsche (9)
acteristics of tasks may be listed un- turns a qualitative answer into a
der those headings, some of them quantitative one by providing an
falling under both. eight-point scale against which to
compare it (see footnote 2). Other
Score Continuum possibilities for providing continua
A continuum of scores from a task are given below under Types of
will be useful in differentiating among Scores.
subjects and among experimental Time to solution always provides
groups, thus increasing the possibil- a possible scoring continuum and
ity of finding functional relationships has been used frequently, but it is
with independent variables. The possible that time is not always a de-
scores should all lie along the same sirable measure of problem-solving
dimension—a matter of knowing performance, because, for example,
what the subject is doing, or, in the events which occupy that time
other terms, of factorial purity and of may not all lie on the same behavior
validity (see below). dimension. Consider also Maier's
A continuum may be found in (a) statement that' 'some subj ects worked
the number of unsuccessful trials as long as three hours, those who got
preceding solution as in Kruglak's the solutions worked five or ten
Electromaze (task 25) where the sub- minutes" (17, p. 121).
ject has to discover a sequence by
trial and error and one can count Subgoals
the number of errors; (b) the num- One possible method of providing
ber of problems from a series on a continuum of scores is to use a
which the subject was successful, as, problem which includes several steps,
again, in the Electromaze where several subgoals on the way to the
several sequences are to be discov- final solution. This is to be differ-
ered, or as in Water Jars problems entiated from a series of problems
(task 26) where one can use as many as that phrase was used in the pre-
problems as he wishes, or in the ceding section, in that in a series of
Children's Swastika Pathway (task problems each is independent of the
11) where the child is asked to find others, whereas in a series of steps,
the goal from different points within what the subject discovers on one
the swastika, or in Burack's Shooting step is, ideally, used on the next step.
of the Captain and His Son (task 19) A nice example of a problem con-
where there is a series of questions taining several steps is provided by
to be answered, or in G. M. Peter- Making the Last Draw (task 24),
son's Moments of Levers (task 29) where the subject learns to solve a
where 20 problems are given with the problem with successively larger num-
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 143

bers of manipulanda and where he the number of problems in a series


typically makes his generalizations since the number of hypotheses in-
about the method more and more in- cludes the possible inapposite hypoth-
clusive as he goes along. Another eses which the subject may propose.
example is given in Simrnel's Weigh- Maier's Blowing Out the Candles
ing the Coins (task 18), where the (task 8) requires two steps in the
subject has to identify the odd coin construction of the tube, the joining
from among eight, then from among of the pieces and the stiffening of the
nine, then from twenty-five. Peter- construction, each with its accom-
son and Lanier's Disc Transfer (task panying hypothesis, and an addi-
23) is another series of steps in which tional step and hypothesis in blowing
increasing numbers of pieces can be through the tube. Maier also, how-
used. Increasingly complex tasks can ever, lists additional inapposite hy-
be presented with Yerkes' Multiple- potheses such as pouring water down
Choice Apparatus (task 27). the tube; so the number of hypotheses
It is probably much too early in the is greater than the number of steps.
history of research in problem solving The opposite sort of example, of an
to ask for a set of steps such that if a indeterminate number of hypothe-
subject fails on one step we can be ses, is provided by Twenty Questions
sure that he will fail all succeeding (task 21) and by Yerkes' Multiple-
steps, but this would be an advan- Choice Apparatus (task 27). Inso-
tageous feature of any complex task, far as most of the tasks being consid-
especially if the task were ever to be ered here are one-apposite-hypothe-
used in measuring problem-solving sis kinds of problems, it is easy to list
abilities or aptitudes. If the steps of their apposite hypotheses as in Dunck-
the problem cannot be arranged in er's Box Problem (task 12), where
any such scale as that implied in the the only hypothesis needed is that
previous sentence, then the task the box can be fastened to the wall
must provide every subject with an to make a shelf, or in Bulbrook's
opportunity to try every step. An Breaking the Beads (task 16), where
example of a task where the latter the idea of smashing the beads is the
does not happen is Maier's Pendulum only idea needed. It is rare, how-
Problem (task 7), where the subject ever, that an investigator attempts
must construct a pendulum which to list all of both the apposite and
will make marks across particular inapposite hypotheses, although one
spots on the floor and where he can man does so when Marks' descrip-
hardly begin on the marking step of tion of the Incorrect Square Root
his problem until he has constructed Problem (task 22) says that the dif-
the pendulum. ficulty may lie in the operator, the
calculating machine, the method, or in
Few Hypotheses the table of factors. Even here, how-
It will assist the experimenter in ever, one may wonder whether sub-
knowing what the subject is doing if jects cannot produce further hypoth-
the possibilities for action are limited, eses, as, for instance, in producing
which is to say that the task should several about the particular fault
permit only a small number of hypoth- which may be occurring in the oper-
eses, including both apposite and in- ator.
apposite hypotheses. The number of
possible hypotheses is different from One Solution
the number of steps in a problem or Having only one solution, and
144 WILBERT S. RA Y
only one method of arriving at that viously be of advantage that as much
solution for a problem, or for the as possible of the behavior be overt,
steps or parts of the problem when with any covert behavior being
there are any, will contribute further treated as intervening variables.
to the experimenter's knowledge of Such overt responses could be verbal
exactly what the subject is doing. if they are parts of, rather than re-
These attributes will also contribute ports of, the solving. Equipment
to factorial purity and to reliability. problems are more likely to produce
Maier's Two String Problem (task overt behavior than are other types,
9) may be mentioned here to empha- and ideally each possible apposite
size the statement made in the in- or inapposite hypothesis would be
troduction to this section of this expressed in some manipulation of
paper where it was pointed out that the equipment, different hypotheses
not all of these attributes would be producing different manipulations.
desirable in all problems. The Two There are tasks in which all of the
String Problem has four or five pos- apposite, and perhaps even all of the
sible solutions 3 and that feature of inapposite, hypotheses are expressed
the task is an essential feature of, for overtly. Bulbn,ok's Balloon in the
example, one c-f Guetzkow's experi- Bottle (task 17) is a case in point.
ments (12). The subject must inflate the balloon
inside the bottle, and to do so must
Multiple Scoring insert into the neck something to pre-
More than one method of scoring vent the balloon from completely fill-
the subject's behavior may some- ing it and thus preventing escape of
times bring surprises, as when two the air. Inserting a pencil or a glass
methods do not agree in their ratings tube into the bottle neck would seem
of the subjects' performances. Such a fairly good indication that the sub-
disagreements may lead to a finding ject is using the air escape hypothesis.
that different dimensions or aspects If Simmel (task 18) had provided
of the behavior are being measured, actual instead of imaginary balances
or, of course, to the conclusion that and coins, she might have had an
one or all of the scores are unreliable. overt expression of an inapposite
On the other hand, if the scoring hypothesis when the subjects tried
methods do agree one will be justi- to solve the problem by dividing the
fied in thinking them the more reli- coins equally between the two pans
able. Several methods of scoring are of the balance. Saugstad's Cylinder
presented below in the section on in the Can (task 20) offers opportuni-
scoring. We have a fine example of ties to watch the subject manipulate
ingenuity in the invention of scoring a water pitcher as the result of a
methods when Burack uses ten (task hypothesis about floating the cylinder
19). high enough to permit grasping it
and removing it from the can. Saug-
Overt Behavior stad also provides an opportunity for
If the experimenter is to get a inapposite hypotheses to exhibit
maximum amount of information themselves by overt, manipulatory
about subject behavior, it will ob- behavior as when an attempt is
3 made to loop a thread around the
"Four or five" because although Guetzkow
and Maier list four, Duncker (10, p. 23) lists
cylinder, to grasp it with pliers, to
a fifth, which, however, does not seem to me insert one's fingers into the can, etc.
to be a solution. It may be, however (and the present
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 145

writer has not seen the equipment), Granting that there is available
that these hypotheses could be re- little knowledge of intervening varia-
jected on the basis of visual cues, bles in problem solving, or of the
which produces the same practical operations which will discriminate
results as if they were rejected on a among them, it is still possible to rule
completely covert basis. Kruglak's out patently "extra" abilities such as
Electromaze (task 25) seems to offer the arithmetic ability needed in a
a fine opportunity to see subjects water jar problem which includes
adopt hypotheses although Kruglak such numbers as 163, 29, 11; although
makes no reports of such aspects of it may be permissible to include them
their work. These examples suggest in cases where it is fairly certain that
that it may be possible to design every member of the experimental
tasks that will do excellent jobs of population has the ability in a high
turning internal, symbolic behavior, degree, as'when Duncker and Adam-
into external, observable behavior. son expect all their subjects to be able
to bore holes with a gimlet (task 13)
Unidimensionality and to bend paper clips (task 14), or
Another characteristic of problem- when Marks expects his subjects to
solving tasks which would increase have enough arithmetic ability to
the experimenter's knowledge of what learn to find square roots (task 22).
the subject is doing would be to have Examples of the other kind of task,
him doing only one kind of thing at that which is multidimensional, are
a time; for example, using only one apparently provided by the Hollow
hypothesis at any one moment, or, and Solid Spheres (task 6) and by the
in other cases, using only one type of Shooting of the Captain and His Son
process (as deduction, induction, per- (task 19).
ceptual reorganization). This is a
request for what the factor analysts Special Knowledges
call factorial purity and what the ex-
perimentalists call unidimensionality. As in the case of extra abilities, so
It implies more knowledge than is extra and special knowledges should
presently available about such inter- be avoided or should be identified and
when necessary, provided. Examples
vening variables as hypotheses and
processes, and about tasks demand- of this latter are Szekely's tasks
(tasks 1 to 6) in connection with
ing overt responses which will dif-
ferentiate one intervening variable which he sometimes asked the sub-
jects for the required information,
from another. On such bases we
sometimes provided it, etc. In
could choose tasks which included
only one dimension, or, when de- the present realm of discourse. Cornell, in an
sired, tasks including more than one unpublished Twelfth Quarterly Report on an
dimension, but in which the dimen- Air Force contract, uses a "Tab Test" which
sions are known and differentiated is an analogue of a radar set, and is a printed
by different kinds of responses. Work test in which a trouble shooter can look for
symptoms, make checks of components, etc.
on this sort of thing is proceeding He finds that the behavior can be classified
apace among the factor analysts, but tentatively into such categories as efficien-
there is very little of it in the more cy, misinterpretation, precipitancy, caution.
complex apparatus task studies.* Whether or not these categories are eventually
included in the final set, they are the general
4
It is possible to find an example of an at- sort of thing that dimensions are—and they
tempt to identify psychological dimensions provide four different methods of scoring a
in a problem-solving task by going outside task.
146 WILBERT S. RA Y
Marks' problem of the Incorrect and with five discs; (b) in G. M. Pe-
Square Root (task 22), the subject terson's reported split-half reliability
has to be taught a special method for of .95 with twenty highly similar tasks
rinding square roots before being able all involving the moments of a lever
to work on the task at all. In the (task 29); and (c) in Deutsche's work
Pendulum Problem (task 7), Maier (footnote 2). Unreliability of tasks
provided all the necessary "experi- will presumably not lead to unwar-
ence" and asked the subject to as- ranted generalizations, but may re-
semble it properly. Twenty Ques- sult in failure to find relationships
tions (task 21), on the other hand, is which would otherwise appear.
a case where the knowledge needed
could never be specified. The Cylin- Validity
der in the Can (task 20) gives an in- If by validity one refers to the
stance of information (that wood question as to whether or not any one
floats) which can be safely assumed task really demands problem-solving
to be within the repertoire of all of behavior, the answer will lie in expert
the members of many different judgment and in definitions which
groups of subjects. It has already can be described as expert judgment
been suggested (footnote 2) that per- again. Identification of types of proc-
haps some of Piaget's tasks (27) are esses which occur in what experts
tests of special knowledge rather than agree to be problem solving will, in
of reasoning. turn, add to the assurance of validity
in future selection of tasks. As far as
Reliability the individual experimenter is con-
Problem-solving tasks, like any cerned, he can frequently avoid the
other experimental tasks, should be question of validity by refusing to
reliable in the usual statistical, test- label his task as problem solving, de-
construction meaning of that word, scribing it completely enough that
which is to say that the variance be- anyone interested in the matter can
tween subjects should be produced by make his own judgment. A more im-
features of the task and/or of the in- portant question than the above is
dependent variable under examina- whether the behavior in question has
tion, rather than by chance factors any relation to any other kind of be-
such as events during the period im- havior, which brings us to the other
mediately preceding entrance to the definition of validity: the relation-
experimental room. This matter of ship of performance on one task to
reliability is one which has never been performance on another, for instance,
solved very satisfactorily for experi- the relation between performance on
mental tasks, although the need has a test and performance on an equip-
been apparent for many years (see ment trouble-shooting job. The split-
Melton, 23), and is an exceptionally half reliability of a series of tasks is
baffling question in problem solving this sort of validity (task 29 and
where the subject frequently meets a footnote 2).
task only once. Signs of an interest The reader is again referred to
in reliability among the tasks de- Melton (23) for a more extended dis-
scribed above appear (a) in connec- cussion of this topic.
tion with the Disc Transfer Problem
(task 23), where correlations in the TYPES OF SCORES USABLE IN
region of .10 are reported between PROBLEM-SOLVING TASKS
scores on the problem with four discs It is possible to make a list of vari-
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 147

ous ways of scoring problem-solving the problem clearly formulated by


tasks, developing the list from the the subject?" (task 19). Deutsche's
methods used in conjunction with (footnote 2) use of a scale against
the tasks themselves, from remarks which to compare written answers is
made by other persons in other an example of quantifying the ele-
places, and from reflection on these gance of "solutions."
sources. The simpler kinds of scores
require no examples and some of the Qualitative
more complex ones have none. 1. Type of psychological process
used in the solving; as, deduction,
Quantitative perceptual reorganization.
Easily quantifiable 2. Content of the process; what de-
1. Success or failure duction or what hypothesis does the
a. On the problem as a whole subject use? Simmel does this in her
b. On steps in a problem experiment on Weighing the Coins
c. On each of a series of prob- (task 18) when she looks to see
lems whether the subject is reacting on the
2. Error scores basis of symmetry (having the same
a. Omits correct procedures, number of coins on each pan of the
hypotheses, etc. balance), totality (weighing all the
b. Uses correct procedures in coins simultaneously), or divisibility
the wrong places (dividing the coins into groups which
c. Repeats correct procedures will leave no extra, nongrouped
unnecessarily coins),
d. Adds incorrect procedures 3. Elegance of the solution process.
3. Time scores Compare Wertheimer (38, p. 18) et
al. on sensible procedures which
Not as easily quantifiable evince some understanding of what is
1. Assistance scores. This is a to be done, and blind procedures such
question of how many aids or hints as trying to apply a formula without
were required to produce the solu- first deciding whether the formula
tion and is therefore a sort of success fits.
or failure score. This sort of score was 4. Deficiency scores. What neces-
used by Maier, for instance with the sary steps were omitted? There is a
Two String Problem (task 9) and use of this sort of thing when Burack
with the Hat Rack (task 10), and was (The Shooting of the Captain and
used by Reid (28) with Katona's Cut His Son, task 19) remarks (6, p. 12)
Pyramid (task 15). "Her solution would be satisfactory
2. Deficiency scores. How close to except for ignoring. . . "
solution did the subject come? How 5. The nature of the error. This is
many and how large were the steps the opposite of elegance of the solu-
yet to be taken? tion process. Compare Duncker's
3. Elegance and neatness of the (10, p. 6) meaningful and nonsensi-
solution. If there are two methods of cal errors and Kohler's (14, p. 203)
solving the task does the subject take good, uninformed, and stupid errors.
the shorter or easier or the one with 6. Content of the error; what spe-
the higher probability of success? cific errors appeared? There are
This is the sort of scoring done in many examples of these errors in the
Twenty Questions (task 21) and is articles describing our tasks as when
suggested in Burack's asking "Was one subject wanted to put Bulbrook's
148 WILBERT S. RAY
(task 17) balloon clear into the bottle twice and validity never by the writers
and blow it up with a glass tube— of the reports. Only seldom are all
except that there was no tube avail- the cogitable apposite hypotheses
able. This would be a "good" error listed, and cogitated inapposite hy-
in the preceding category. potheses are sometimes stated, but a
complete list of them for any one task
DISCUSSION has apparently never been offered.
As one reads through the experi- There is a reiterated plea, espe-
mental literature of complex problem cialty in articles and chapters which
solving as denned by the sort of tasks summarize the state of the field of
included here, he is struck with the problem solving and reasoning, for
fact that many of these tasks have more theory—a plea which has pro-
been of the parlor puzzle or game duced no great results. Perhaps a
variety rather than being tasks con- more modest sounding plea may be
structed for experimental use, which made. What this field needs are di-
is even more true in the problems mensionable independent variables,
which have been omitted than in predictions of their effects on dimen-
those included. Such problems are sionable dependent variables, and
frequently either too simple in that tasks especially designed to measure
the solving process is a single, rapid those effects. Once this is achieved,
response (and often a covert re- we can start to work on hypothetical
sponse), or too complex in that there constructs, perhaps using specific
are so many possible responses that hypotheses as first order intervening-
the experimenter has little chance of variables and processes such as hy-
identifying them. This, however, is pothesis formation by induction or
as often true in the tasks designed for deduction as second order variables.
experiments as it is in those taken From one point of view, research
over from the parlor. The choice of in problem solving is simpler than
such problems is presumably due to research in learning, in that in learn-
the many investigations made in this ing the experimenter predicts changes
field which have been observational in responses, whereas in problem
rather than experimental in their aim, solving all he has to predict is the
although this is not so true today as probability of occurrence of the re-
it was in the past. sponse. From another point of view,
Few of the tasks described herein however, it must be admitted that
permit more than a two-category research in problem solving may be
scoring system, success or failure, more difficult because a greater num-
although some investigators have ber of more complex, intervening
shown considerable ingenuity here. variables may be needed here than in
Reliability has been mentioned only learning research.
REFERENCES
1. ADAMSON, R. E. Functional fixedness as Yerkes' multiple-choice apparatus,
related to problem solving: a repetition Amer. J. PsyclwL, 1931, 43, 497-498.
of three experiments. /. exp. Psychol., 4. BIRCH, H. G., & RABINOWITZ, H. S. The
1952, 44, 288-291. negative effect of previous experience
2. BATTERSBY, W. S., TEUBER, H. L., & on productive thinking. J. exp.
BENDER, M. B. Problem solving be- Psychol., 1951, 41, 121-125.
havior in men with frontal or occipital 5, BULBROOK, MARY E. An experimental
brain injuries. /. Psychol,, 1953, 35, inquiry into the existence and nature
329-351. of "insight." Amer. J. Psychol,, 1932,
3. BEASLEY, W. A miniature model of 44, 409-453.
TASKS FOR HUMAN PROBLEM-SOLVING RESEARCH 149
6. BURACK, B. The nature and efficacy of 23. MELTON, A. W. The methodology of ex-
methods of attack on reasoning prob- perimental studies of human learning
lems. Psycliol, Monogr., 1950, 64, No. and retention: I. The functions of a
7 (Whole No. 313). methodology and the available criteria
7. GOFER, C. N. Verbal behavior in relation for evaluating different experimental
to reasoning and values. In H. Guetz- methods. PsychoL Bull., 1936, 33, 305-
kow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and men. 394.
Pittsburgh: Carnegie, 1951. 24. PETERSON, G. M. An empirical study of
8. GOFER, C. N., & GELFAND, S. The role the ability to generalize. 7. gen. Psy-
of increased associative strength in choL, 1932,6,90-114.
reasoning: .further report. Twelfth 25. PETERSON, J., & LANIER, L. H. Studies
Technical Report under Contract N7- in the comparative abilities of whites
onr-397, T. 0. Ill, Univer. of Maryland, and negroes. Ment. Measmt Monogr.,
1952. Duplicated. 1929, Serial No. 5.
9. DEUTSCHE, JEAN M. The development of 26. PETERSON, J. C. The higher mental pro-
children's concepts of causal relations. cesses in learning. PsychoL Monogr.,
Minneapolis: Univer, of Minnesota 1920, 28, No. 7 (Whole No. 129).
Press, 1937. 27. PIAGET, J. The child's conception of
10. DUNCKER, K. On problem-solving. physical causality. New York: Har-
Psycliol. Monogr., 1945, 58, No, 5 court, Brace, 1930.
(Whole No. 270).
11. ERICKSEN, S. C. Variability of attack in 28. REID, J. W. An experimental study of
massed and distributed practice. J. "analysis of the goal" in problem solv-
exp. PsychoL, 1942, 31, 339-345. ing. J, gen. PsychoL, 1951, 44, 51-69.
12. GUETZKOW, H. An analysis of the opera- 29. SAUGSTAD, P. Incidental memory and
tion of set in problem solving behavior. problem solving. PsychoL Rev., 1952,
J. gen. PsychoL, 1951, 45, 219-244. 59, 221-226.
13. KATONA, G. Eine kleine Anschauungs- 30. SHAW, MARJORIE E. A comparison of
aufgabe. PsychoL Forsch,, 1927, 9, individuals and small groups in the ra-
159-162, tional solution of complex problems.
14. KOHLER, W. Mentality of apes. New Amer. J. PsychoL, 1932, 44, 491-504.
York: Harcourt, Brace, 1925. 31. SIMMEL, MARIANNE L. The coin prob-
15. KRUGLAK, H., SCHENSTED, C. E., &SELF, lem: a study in thinking. Amer. J,
H. C. An electric multiple choice maze. PsychoL, 1953, 66, 229-241.
Technical Report Number 2 under 32. SZ&KELY, L, Knowledge and thinking.
Contract NSonr-66213, Project NR192- Ada PsychoL, 1950,7, 1-24.
041, Univer. of Minnesota, 1952. Du- 33. SzfeELY, L. Productive processes in
plicated. learning and thinking. Ada PsychoL,
16. LINDLEY, E. H. A study of puzzles with 1950, 7, 388-407.
special reference to the psychology of 34. TAYLOR, D. W., & FAUST, W. L. Twenty
mental adaptation. Amer. J. PsychoL, questions: efficiency in problem solving
1897, 8, 431-493. as a function of size of group. J. exp.
17. MAIER, N, R. F. Reasoning in humans: PsychoL, 1952, 44, 360-368.
I. On direction. J. comp. PsychoL, 35. TERMAN, L. M. Genius and stupidity.
1930, 10, 115-143. Fed. Sent., 1906, 13, 307-373.
18. MAIER, N, R. F. Reasoning in humans: 36. WATERS, R. H. The influence of tuition
II. The solution of a problem and its upon ideational learning. /. gen.
appearance in consciousness. J, comp. PsychoL, 1928, 1, 534-549.
PsychoL, 1931, 12, 181-194. 37. WEAVER, H, E., & MADDEN, E. H. "Di-
19. MAIER, N. R. F. An aspect of human rection" in problem solving. /. PsychoL,
reasoning. Brit. J. Psycliol., 1933, 24, 1949, 27, 331-345.
144-155. 38. WERTHEIMER, M. Productive thinking.
20. MAIER, N. R. F. Reasoning in children.
J. comp. PsychoL, 1936, 21, 357-366. New York: Harper, 1945.
21. MAIER, N. R. F. Reasoning in humans; 39. YERKES, R. M. A new method of study-
III. The mechanisms of equivalent ing the ideational behavior of mentally
stimuli and of reasoning. /. exp. defective and deranged as compared
PsychoL, 1945, 35, 349-360. with normal individuals. J. comp.
22. MARKS, M. R. Problem solving as a func- PsychoL, 1921, 1, 369-394.
tion of the situation. /. exp. PsychoL,
1951, 41, 74-80. Received March 14, 1954.

You might also like