You are on page 1of 4

Cognitive Performance in Hypertensive and

Normotensive Older Subjects


Frances Harrington, Brian K. Saxby, Ian G. McKeith, Keith Wesnes, Gary A. Ford

Abstract—Longitudinal studies suggest that hypertension in midlife is associated with cognitive impairment in later life.
Cross-sectional studies are difficult to interpret because blood pressure can change with onset of dementia and the
inclusion of subjects on treatment and with hypertensive end-organ damage can make analysis difficult. We examined
cognitive performance in hypertensive and normotensive subjects without dementia or stroke ⱖ70 years of age.
Cognitive performance was determined with the use of a computerized assessment battery in 107 untreated
hypertensives (55 women, age 76⫾4 years, blood pressure, 164⫾9/89⫾7; range, 138 to 179/68 to 99 mm Hg) and 116
normotensives (51 female, age 76⫾4 years, 131⫾10/74⫾7; 108 to 149/60 to 89 mm Hg). Older subjects with
hypertension were significantly slower in all tests (reaction time, milliseconds; simple, 346⫾100 versus 318⫾56,
P⬍0.05; memory scanning, 867⫾243 versus 789⫾159, P⬍0.01; immediate word recognition, 947⫾261 versus
886⫾192, P⬍0.05; and delayed word recognition, 937⫾230 versus 856⫾184, P⬍0.05; picture recognition, 952⫾184
versus 894⫾137, P⬍0.01; spatial memory, 1390⫾439 versus 1258⫾394, P⬍0.01; excepting choice reaction time,
510⫾75 versus 498⫾72, P⫽0.08). Accuracy was also impaired in tests of number vigilance, 99.2⫾2.5% versus
99.9⫾0.9, P⬍0.01; delayed word recognition, 83.5⫾16 versus 87.9⫾9.8, P⬍0.01; and spatial memory 64⫾32 versus
79⫾20, P⬍0.001. Hypertension in older subjects is associated with impaired cognition in a broad range of areas in the
absence of clinically evident target organ damage. (Hypertension. 2000;36:1079-1082.)
Key Words: hypertension, arterial 䡲 blood pressure 䡲 aging 䡲 dementia

C ognitive impairment and dementia are becoming increas-


ingly prevalent because of demographic changes. The
mingham7 suggested no association between BP and cognitive
performance when measured concurrently; however, when data
Downloaded from http://ahajournals.org by on December 23, 2020

prevalence of dementia doubles with each 5-year age rise, from were reanalyzed the average BP over 20 years was inversely
2.8% at 70 to 74 years to 38.6% at 90 to 95 years.1 The annual related to cognitive performance.8 Three further studies have
incidence of dementia in the age group 85 to 88 years is 9%.2 shown a link between midlife or later-life hypertension and
Dementia incidence in a population ⬎55 years of age was 10.7 subsequent cognitive impairment.9 –11 Reasons for discrepancies
per 1000 person-years, equating to a lifetime dementia risk for a between cross-sectional and longitudinal studies may include the
55-year-old woman of 0.33 and 0.16 for a man.3 tendency for BP levels to change with the onset of dementia,10
The UK population ⬎60 years of age is projected to the inclusion of individuals with established cerebrovascular
increase by a third by the year 2026; the population with disease in cross-sectional studies, and the effects of BP-lowering
cognitive impairment is projected to double by 2026.4 In this therapy on cognitive function. Except for one small study in 25
context, population risk factors for the development of older patients with severe hypertension there are no data on
dementia that are potentially modifiable are important to cognitive function in older hypertensives where the effects of
cerebrovascular disease have been excluded.12
identify. Cognitive impairment usually develops insidiously,
The hypothesis of the present study was that cognitive
eventually reaching a stage where it becomes clinically and
performance in older adults without overt cerebrovascular
functionally apparent. Studies of subjects with no or with
disease would be impaired in line with increasing BP level.
minor cognitive impairment have shown that neuropsycho-
We determined cognitive performance in untreated older
logical tests can predict who is likely to proceed to dementia.5 hypertensives and normotensives without cerebrovascular
Hypertension is common in elderly Western populations, with disease, target organ damage, or other vascular risk factors.
a prevalence of 41% for men and 54% for women 75 years of
age, defined by single blood pressure (BP) reading of Methods
⬎160 mm Hg systolic and/or ⬎95 mm Hg diastolic or any The study was performed in a community primary care population.
treatment for hypertension.6 Epidemiological data from Fra- Subjects were recruited from 10 local general practices in the

Received March 30, 2000; first decision April 19, 2000; revision accepted June 5, 2000.
From the Institute for the Health of the Elderly, University of Newcastle Upon Tyne (F.H., B.K.S., I.G.M., G.A.F.); and Cognitive Drug Research
(K.W.), Reading, United Kingdom.
Correspondence to Prof G.A. Ford, Wolfson Unit of Clinical Pharmacology, University of Newcastle Upon Tyne, Newcastle Upon Tyne, NE2 4HH,
United Kingdom. E-mail g.a.ford@ncl.ac.uk
© 2000 American Heart Association, Inc.
Hypertension is available at http://www.hypertensionaha.org

1079
1080 Hypertension December 2000

Tyneside area. BP readings over the last 5 years, current drug Results of Cognitive Assessment Battery in Hypertensive and
therapy, and concomitant disease from all persons in the 70- to Normotensive Groups
89-year age group were recorded from general practitioner case
records. Subjects who were not taking BP-lowering drugs, had no ms (%) Hypertensive Normotensive P
serious concomitant disease (dementia, malignancy, or advanced Simple reaction time 346 (100) 318 (56) ⬍0.05
renal failure), and had BP readings recorded in the notes were
contacted by letter and invited to attend the screening clinic; 1213 Choice reaction time 510 (75) 498 (72) 0.08
subjects attended. Hypertensive subjects were first recruited through Number vigilance accuracy 99.2 (2.5) 99.9 (0.9) ⬍0.01
screening of subjects with average recorded BP readings of ⬎160 Memory scanning reaction time 867 (243) 789 (159) ⬍0.01
and/or 90 mm Hg. Normotensive subjects were subsequently re-
cruited by screening of subjects with average BP readings of Immediate word recognition
⬍150/90 mm Hg. BP was measured with a mercury sphygmoma- Reaction time 947 (261) 886 (192) ⬍0.05
nometer 3 times on each of 3 occasions, each separated by 2 weeks.
Accuracy 88.8 (11) 89.3 (9.7) 0.68
Baseline BP was defined as the mean of the second and third
readings on the last of these 3 visits. Two cohorts of subjects were Delayed word recognition
recruited: hypertensives with systolic BP 160 to 179 mm Hg and/or Reaction time 937 (230) 856 (184) ⬍0.05
diastolic BP 90 to 99 mm Hg and normotensives with BP⬍150/
90 mm Hg. Subjects were excluded if they were receiving any Accuracy 83.5 (16) 87.9 (9.8) ⬍0.01
BP-lowering therapy, had atrial fibrillation, diabetes mellitus, previ- Picture recognition
ous stroke or transient ischemic attack (defined by clinical history or Reaction time 952 (184) 894 (137) ⬍0.01
physical examination), carotid bruits on auscultation, peripheral
vascular disease (symptomatic claudication or absent foot pulses), Accuracy 88 (13) 89.1 (10.4) 0.29
angina or previous myocardial infarction (defined by clinical history Spatial memory
or ECG changes), or dementia. Dementia was defined as mini mental Reaction time 1390 (439) 1258 (394) ⬍0.01
state examination (MMSE) ⬍24 and/or significant decline in cogni-
tive function; this was assessed by interview with a close friend or Accuracy 64 (32) 79 (20) ⬍0.0001
relative and completion of Clinical Dementia Rating13 and IQ Data are mean and SD.
Code.14 Subjects taking psychotropic medication were also excluded
because these drugs can interfere with cognitive function. Educa-
tional level was assessed by number of years at school and verbal IQ 50 women) were studied. Mean age (76⫾4 years), years in
using number of errors in the New Adult Reading Test15 (NART). education (10⫾2), and MMSE score (29⫾1) were identical in
Subjects were screened for the presence of depressive disorder by the both groups. There was no significant difference in hyperten-
30-point Geriatric Depression Scale16 (GDS). Studies were approved sive and normotensive group NART scores (19.3⫾9.2 versus
by the Newcastle Joint Ethics committee. All subjects gave in-
formed, written consent. 17.5⫾8.6) or GDS scores (5.4⫾4.6 versus 5.0⫾4.5). Ten
Downloaded from http://ahajournals.org by on December 23, 2020

subjects in each group were taking aspirin. Results of the


Cognitive Assessment cognitive assessment battery in hypertensive and normoten-
Subjects were administered the Cognitive Drug Research Comput- sive groups are shown in the Table. The hypertensive group
erized Assessment Battery.17,18 The subject sits in front of a laptop was significantly slower in all tests except for choice reaction
computer; a series of words, pictures, and numbers appear on the
screen and the subject responds by pressing “yes” or “no” on a button
time, in which the slower response was of borderline signif-
box in front of them. The battery comprises 8 tests and takes 20 icance. Accuracy of response in hypertensive subjects was
minutes to administer. also impaired for number vigilance, delayed word recogni-
Simple reaction time is a test of attention, alertness, and power of tion, and spatial memory.
concentration. Choice reaction time is similar to simple reaction time
with an element of stimulus discrimination and response. Immediate
and delayed word recognition time test the ability to store and Discussion
retrieve verbal information and discriminate novel from previously We have shown by using a cognitive assessment instrument
presented words. Picture recognition time tests the ability to store that has been well validated in the elderly17,18 that older
and retrieve pictorial information and discriminate novel from subjects with hypertension but without clinical evidence of
previously presented pictures. Number vigilance is a test of attention,
vascular disease have impaired cognition in a broad range of
requiring intensive vigilance and the ability to ignore distraction.
Spatial memory tests the ability to store and retrieve visuospatial tests of attention and short- and long-term memory. Hyper-
information. Memory scanning tests the ability to store information tensives were on average ⬇10% slower in psychomotor tests
in the working memory and rapidly retrieve it, testing subvocal compared with normotensive peers. The decrement seen in
rehearsal of digit sequences. the hypertensive subjects is one-third to one-half that seen in
The computerized battery was administered once as a training
session before baseline. Preliminary studies with a subgroup estab- mild dementia. A study using the CDR battery in subjects
lished that 1 training session was sufficient to familiarize the subjects with mild Alzheimers dementia (MMSE 24) and healthy
with the test system. age-matched control subjects without dementia (MMSE 29)
found the dementia group to be slower by 13% to 44% in the
Statistical Analysis tests.19
Data were analyzed with SPSS for Windows. Statistical significance
These differences may not be sufficient to interfere with
was set at the 5% level. Means were analyzed by means of the
independent samples t test. activities of daily living and would not be recognized in
routine clinical practice because of wide interindividual
Results variability. However, many studies have observed that pre-
One hundred seven untreated hypertensives (164⫾9/89⫾7; morbid cognitive function levels are associated with in-
range, 138 to 179/68 –99 mm Hg, 55 women) and 116 creased risk of developing dementia.5,20,21 The public health
normotensives (131⫾10/74⫾7; 108 to 149/60 – 89 mm Hg, implications of relatively small individual declines in cogni-
Harrington et al Cognitive Performance in Hypertension 1081

tion are large when applied to a population. The presence of or placebo (n⫽5). These observations require confirmation,
vascular risk factors in some population subgroups was but such reversibility would suggest that psychomotor im-
associated with a left shift of the normal distribution of pairment is a direct consequence of hypertension, most likely
MMSE scores and a large increase in numbers of individuals mediated through effects on cerebral blood flow or
falling below the cutoff score for probable dementia.22 metabolism.
The majority of studies examining the effect of BP on Genotypic influences that contribute to hypertension could
cognition have looked at BP levels very much higher than influence cognition through common or separate mecha-
those in the present study. We have found important impair- nisms. A potential genetic link factor between hypertension
ments in cognitive function in a group of older hypertensives and cognitive impairment is the apolipoprotein E allele,
with only moderately increased BP. This has implications for which has been linked to vascular risk factors,40 coronary
the large numbers of elderly people with only minimally artery disease,41,42 and stroke,43 as well as Alzheimer’s
raised BP. Results from this study agree with those from a dementia.44,45 In postmortem studies,46,47 an increased ⑀4
previous smaller study12 of more severe hypertensives (mean allele frequency has been reported in subjects with hyperten-
age, 70.6 years; mean BP, 192/112) and normotensives (mean sion or critical coronary artery disease and an increased
age, 70.4 years; mean BP, 143/80). The hypertensives were number of senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the
rigorously screened to exclude those with end-organ damage, brains of apo⑀4-positive hypertensives who were not de-
although the majority of hypertensives had been on treatment mented. Further work is needed to explore the link between
and were tested off treatment. This study found the hyperten- apo⑀4 and hypertension and cognitive impairment in older
sives to be significantly slower in tests of attention and people.
psychomotor speed. The present study confirms these obser- Our observations do not necessarily imply that treatment of
vations and extends the findings to older subjects with lesser hypertension will reduce cognitive decline. However, the
degrees of BP elevation. Interpretation of cross-sectional SystEur Vascular Dementia project48 reported treatment of
studies is difficult for a number of reasons, including use of hypertension in the elderly with the calcium channel blocker
wide age ranges,23–25 use of tests not validated in the elderly, nitrendipine reduced incidence of dementia by 50%. A recent
use of combinations of treated and untreated subjects,25 longitudinal observational study also suggests that treatment
subjects with likely end organ damage,26 inclusion of subjects of hypertension in older individuals may reduce cognitive
with probable dementia,27 and too few BP readings to decline.49 Hypertension was associated with cognitive decline
adequately define BP status and level.28,29 However, repeated during 4 years of follow-up, with the highest risk in untreated
BP measurements in this study to clearly characterize partic- patients. If treating hypertension reduces the rate of cognitive
Downloaded from http://ahajournals.org by on December 23, 2020

ipants BP status may have had the disadvantage of “labeling”


decline, the benefits of treating hypertension would extend
subjects, which may have led subjects who were aware that
beyond the prevention of stroke and myocardial infarction to
they were hypertensive to perform less well. However, it is
primary prevention of dementia. The present findings support
also possible that low levels of anxiety, generated by “label-
undertaking further studies to elucidate the mechanisms
ing” of subjects as hypertensive, may have enhanced some
through which hypertension is associated with cognitive
aspects of cognitive performance.
impairment and intervention studies to determine whether
The differences seen in our study are likely to be due to a
treatment of hypertension prevents dementia.
direct effect of hypertension. The groups are well matched for
other factors known to influence cognitive function: age,
educational level, depressive disorder, and psychotropic med-
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by the Astra Foundation, United Kingdom.
ication. There are a number of possible mechanisms through We thank Helene Poppleton for assistance with cognitive testing.
which hypertension might directly impair cognitive function.
Ischemic stroke increases the risk of dementia.30 –32 The References
hypertensive subjects in our study had no clinical evidence of 1. Jorm A, Korten A, Henderson A. The prevalence of dementia: a quanti-
cerebrovascular disease or other target organ damage from tative integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1987;76:
465– 479.
hypertension. However, an increased prevalence of asymp-
2. Asvarsson O, Skoog I. A population-based study on the incidence of
tomatic cerebrovascular disease may have been present in the dementia disorders between 85 and 88 years of age. J Am Geriatr Soc.
hypertensive subjects. Multiple small infarcts can lead to 1996;44:1455–1460.
dementia, depending on volume of brain affected,33,34 loca- 3. Ott A, Breteler M, van Harskamp F, Stijnen T, Hofman A. Incidence, and
risk of dementia the Rotterdam study. Am Heart J. 1998;147:574 –580.
tion of infarcts, and bilateralality. White matter lesions that
4. Melzer D, Ely M, Brayne C. Cognitive impairment in elderly people:
consist of areas of demyelination and narrowing of small population based estimate of the future in England, Scotland, and Wales.
arteriolar lumen size have been associated both with hyper- BMJ. 1997;315:462.
tension and with cognitive dysfunction. The presence of brain 5. Flicker C, Ferris S, Reisberg B. Mild cognitive impairment in the elderly:
predictors of dementia. Neurology. 1991;41:1006 –1009.
MRI white matter lesions is associated with impaired cogni-
6. Vokonas P, Kannel W, Cupples L. Epidemiology and risk of hypertension
tive functioning in subjects with and without dementia.35,36 in the elderly: the Framingham Study. J Hypertens. 1988;6(suppl
Other possible mechanisms are changes in cerebral auto- 1):S3–S9.
regulation37 and cerebral blood flow38 associated with hyper- 7. Farmer M, White L, Abbott R, Kittner S, Kaplan E, Wolz M, Brody J,
Wolf P. Blood pressure and cognitive performance: the Framingham
tension. Kalra et al39 reported improvements in tests of Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1987;126:1103–1114.
attention and psychomotor speed when 25 elderly hyperten- 8. Farmer ME, Kittner SJ, Abbott RD, Wolz MM, Wolf PA, White LR.
sive subjects were treated with BP-lowering therapy (n⫽20) Longitudinally measured blood pressure, antihypertensive medication
1082 Hypertension December 2000

use, and cognitive performance: the Framingham Study. J Clin Epi- prevalence, incidence, risk factors, and computed tomographic findings.
demiol. 1990;43:475– 480. Stroke. 1990;21:858 – 866.
9. Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovich H, Foley D, Havlik RJ. The association 31. Tatemichi TK, Desmond DW, Mayeux R, Paik M, Stern Y, Sano M,
between blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function: the Remien RH, Williams JB, Mohr JP, Hauser WA. Dementia after stroke:
Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA. 1995;274:1846 –1851. baseline frequency, risks, and clinical features in a hospitalized cohort.
10. Skoog I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, Palmertz B, Andreasson LA, Nilsson L, Neurology. 1992;42:1185–1193.
Persson G. Fifteen-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and 32. Loeb C, Gandolfo C, Croce R, Conti M. Dementia associated with
dementia. Lancet. 1996;347:1141–1145. lacunar infarction. Stroke. 1992;23:1225–1229.
11. Kilander L, Nyman H, Boberg M, Hansson L, Lithell H. Hypertension is 33. Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, Roth M. Observations on the brains of
related to cognitive impairment: a 20-year follow-up of 999 men. Hyper- demented old people. J Neurol Sci. 1970;11:205–242.
tension. 1998;31:780 –786. 34. Erkinjuntti T, Haltia M, Palo J, Sulkava R, Paetau A. Accuracy of the
12. Kalra L, Jackson S, Swift C. Psychomotor performance in elderly hyper- clinical diagnosis of vascular dementia: a prospective clinical and post-
tensive patients. J Hum Hypertens. 1993;7:279 –284. mortem neuropathological study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1988;
13. Morris J. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and 51:1037–1044.
scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412. 35. Skoog I, Johansson B, Palmertz B, Andreasson L. The influence of white
14. Jorm A, Scott R, Cullen J, McKinnon A. Performance of the Informant matter lesions on neuropsychological functioning in demented and non-
Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE) as a demented 85-year-olds. Acta Neurol Scand. 1996;93:142–148.
screening test for dementia. Psychol Med. 1991;21:785–790. 36. van Swieten J, Geyskes G, Derix M, Peeck B, Ramos L, van Latum J, van
15. Nelson H, O’Connell A. Dementia: the estimation of premorbid intel- Gijn J. Hypertension in the elderly is associated with white matter lesions
ligence levels using the New Adult Reading Test. Cortex. 1978;14: and cognitive decline. Ann Neurol. 1991;30:825– 830.
234 –244. 37. Strandgaard S. Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow in hypertensive
16. Yesavage J, Brink T, Rose T, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, Leirer V. patients. Circulation. 1976;53:720 –727.
Development, and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a 38. Nobili F, Rodriguez G, Marenco S, Carli FD, Gambaro M, Castello C.
preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37– 49. Regional cerebral blood flow in chronic hypertension. Stroke. 1993;24:
17. Simpson P, Surmon D, Wesnes K, Wilcock G. The cognitive drug 1148 –1153.
research computerized assessment system for demented patients: a vali- 39. Kalra L, Jackson S, Swift C. Effect of antihypertensive treatment on
dation study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1991;6:95–102.
psychomotor performance in the elderly. J Hum Hypertens. 1993;7:
18. Nicholl C, Lynch S, Kelly C, White L, Simpson P, Wesnes K, Pitt B. The
285–290.
cognitive drug research assessment system in the evaluation of early
40. Vauhkonen I, Niskanen L, Ryynanen M, Voutilainen R, Partanen J, Toyry
dementia: is speed of the essence? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1995;10:
J, Mercuri M, Rauramaa R, Uusitupa M. Divergent association of apoli-
199 –206.
poprotein E polymorphism with vascular disease in patients with NIDDM
19. Mohr E, Walker D, Randolph C, Simpson M, Mendis T. Utility of clinical
and control subjects. Diabet Med. 1997;14:748 –756.
trial batteries in the measurement of Alzheimer’s and Huntingdon’s
41. Wilson PW, Myers RH, Larson MG, Ordovas JM, Wolf PA, Schaefer EJ.
dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 1996;8:397– 411.
Apolipoprotein E alleles, dyslipidemia, and coronary heart disease: the
20. Johansson B, Zarit SH, Berg S. Changes in cognitive functioning of the
Framingham Offspring Study. JAMA. 1994;272:1666 –1671.
oldest old. J Gerontol. 1992;47:75– 80.
42. van der Cammen T, Verschoor C, van Loon C, van Harskamp F, de
21. Brayne C, Best N, Muir M, Richards SJ, Gill C. Five-year incidence and
Koning I, Schudel W. Risk of left ventricular dysfunction in patients with
Downloaded from http://ahajournals.org by on December 23, 2020

prediction of dementia and cognitive decline in a population sample of


women aged 70 –79 at baseline. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;12: probable Alzheimer’s disease with APOE4 Allele. J Am Geriatr Soc.
1107–1118. 1998;46:962–967.
22. Breteler M, Claus J, Grobbee D, Hofman A. Cardiovascular disease and 43. Kessler C, Spitzer C, Stauske D, Mende S, Stadlmuller J, Walther R,
distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam study Rettig R. The apolipoprotein E and B-fibrinogen G/A-455 gene poly-
BMJ. 1994;308:1604 –1608. morphisms are associated with ischemic stroke involving large-vessel
23. Light K. Slowing of response time in young and middle aged hyper- disease. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1997;17:2880 –2884.
tensive patients. Exp Aging Res. 1975;1:209 –227. 44. Strittmatter WJ, Saunders AM, Schmechel D, Pericak-Vance M, Enghild
24. Schultz NR, Dineen JT, Elias MF, Pentz CA, Wood WG. WAIS perfor- J, Salvesen GS, Roses AD. Apolipoprotein E: high-avidity binding to
mance for different age groups of hypertensive and control subjects beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset
during the administration of a diuretic. J Gerontology. 1979;34:246 –253. familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90:
25. van Boxtel M, Gaillard C, Houx P, Buntinx F, Leeuw P, Jolles J. Can the 1977–1981.
blood pressure predict cognitive task performance in a healthy population 45. Houlden H, Crook R, Hardy J, Roques P, Collinge J, Rossor M. Confir-
sample? J Hypertens. 1997;15:1069 –1076. mation that familial clustering and age of onset in late onset Alzheimer’s
26. Boller F, Vrtunski B, Mack J, Kim Y. Neuropsychological correlates of disease are determined at the apolipoprotein E locus. Neurosci Lett.
hypertension. Arch Neurol. 1977;34:701–705. 1994;174:222–224.
27. Cacciatore F, Abete P, Ferrara N, Paolisso G, Amato L, Canonico S, 46. Sparks DL, Scheff SW, Liu H, Landers TM, Coyne CM, Hunsaker JC III.
Maggi S, Varricchio M, Rengo F. The role of blood pressure in cognitive Increased incidence of neurofibrillary tangles (NFT) in non-demented
impairment in and elderly population. J Hypertens. 1997;15:135–142. individuals with hypertension. J Neurol Sci. 1995;131:162–169.
28. Battersby C, Hartley K, Fletcher A, Markowe H, Brown R, Styles W, 47. Sparks DL. Coronary artery disease, hypertension, ApoE, and cholesterol:
Carne S, Jamieson T, Koppel I, Fraser S, Sapper H, Bulpitt C. Cognitive a link to Alzheimer’s disease? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997;826:128 –146.
function in hypertension: a community based study. J Hum Hypertens. 48. Forette F, Seux M, Staessen J, Thijs L, Birkenhager W. Prevention of
1993;7:117–123. dementia in randomised double-blind placebo-controlled Systolic Hyper-
29. Scherr P, Hebert L, Smith L, Evans D. Relation of blood pressure to tension in Europe (Syst-Eur) trial. Lancet. 1998;352:1347–1351.
cognitive function in the elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;134:1303–1315. 49. Tzourio C, Dufouil C, Ducimetiere P, Alperovitch A. Cognitive decline in
30. Tatemichi TK, Foulkes MA, Mohr JP, Hewitt JR, Hier DB, Price TR, individuals with high blood pressure: a longitudinal study in the elderly.
Wolf PA. Dementia in stroke survivors in the Stroke Data Bank cohort: Neurology. 1999;53:1948 –1952.

You might also like