You are on page 1of 19

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/317539078

Personality Disorder

Chapter · May 2017


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_923-1

CITATIONS READS

0 12,538

4 authors:

Chloe Bliton Emily A. Dowgwillo


Pennsylvania State University 19 PUBLICATIONS 420 CITATIONS
12 PUBLICATIONS 133 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Sindes Dawood Aaron L. Pincus

20 PUBLICATIONS 725 CITATIONS


Pennsylvania State University
250 PUBLICATIONS 17,344 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Sindes Dawood on 29 September 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


P

Personality Disorder substance abuse, etc. In the first half of the twen-
tieth century, personality disorders were identified
Chloe F. Bliton, Emily A. Dowgwillo, and discussed in Freud’s (1914) psychoanalytic
Sindes Dawood and Aaron L. Pincus theory, Reich’s (1949) character analysis, and
Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State other early psychodynamic formulations. By the
University, University Park, PA, USA 1950s, a taxonomy of personality disorders
emerged in descriptive psychiatry (Schneider
1950) that evolved into the contemporary taxon-
Synonyms omies found in the ICD-11 and the DSM-5 and
their prior versions. Here, we focus on the DSM.
Personality impairment; Personality pathology
DSM-I
The publication of DSM-I (American Psychiatric
Definition
Association, APA 1952) provided the first widely
adopted psychiatric nomenclature in the United
Personality disorders are a form of psychopathol-
States. The personality disorders section included
ogy arising from impairments in self- and rela-
three subsections: personality pattern distur-
tional functioning resulting in identity problems
bances (paranoid, schizoid, cyclothymic, inade-
and maladaptive social behavior.
quate), personality trait disturbances (passive-
aggressive, compulsive, emotionally unstable),
and sociopathic personality disturbances
Introduction
(antisocial, dyssocial, sexual deviation, addic-
tion). This initial taxonomy was based entirely
Introduction and History
on clinical observation and opinion, and the entire
Personality disorders are a form of psychopathol-
DSM-I was problematic in terms of reliability and
ogy arising from impairments in self- and rela-
validity of its diagnoses. One particularly prob-
tional functioning. Core identity problems and
lematic aspect of DSM-I was the requirement that
maladaptive social behavior are what differentiate
the clinician choose between a primarily neurotic
personality disorders from anxiety disorders,
(symptom) disorder or a primary characterologi-
mood disorders, etc. However, personality disor-
cal (personality) disorder. This is inconsistent
der symptoms contribute to a wide variety of
with the common pan-symptomatic presentation
urgent secondary complaints including anxiety,
of personality disorders in clinical settings.
depression, disordered eating, self-harm,
# Springer International Publishing AG 2017
V. Zeigler-Hill, T.K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_923-1
2 Personality Disorder

DSM-II personality disorder) and problems with various


Although the publication of DSM-II (APA 1968) criteria were quickly identified. The revisions
was an improvement in terms of organization, it found in the DSM-III-R (APA 1987) were limited
continued to be based on clinical observation in to edits aimed at improving consistency, clarity,
the tradition of descriptive psychiatry. Addictions and conceptual accuracy of the diagnostic criteria.
and sexual deviations were moved from the per- The personality disorders included schizoid, para-
sonality disorders section, personality disorder noid, schizotypal, histrionic, borderline,
labels were revised, and new personality disorder obsessive-compulsive, dependent, passive-
diagnoses were added. The personality disorders aggressive, avoidant, narcissistic, and antisocial.
included paranoid, schizoid, cyclothymic, inade-
quate, hysterical, obsessive-compulsive, passive- DSM-IV
aggressive, explosive, aesthenic, and antisocial. The revisions found in DSM-IV (APA 1994)
Arising from the new attitudes toward mental included reducing the length and complexity of
health diagnoses based in the patient advocacy the diagnostic criteria and a more rigorous and
movements of the times, as well as continued systematic empirical evaluation of the document
problems with DSM-II diagnostic reliability, a using comprehensive research literature reviews,
major shift to highly specific diagnostic criteria reanalyses of existing data, and field trials. The
and an emphasis on diagnostic reliability were the results included deletions and revisions to most of
foundation of the major revision found in DSM- the personality disorder diagnostic criteria and the
III (Decker 2013). deletion of passive-aggressive personality
disorder.
DSM-III/DSM-III-R
The dramatic revision found in DSM-III (APA DSM-5
1980) included many innovations, the most At the time the revision process for DSM-5 began,
important being provision of highly specific and a neurobiological and genetic paradigm shift was
explicit diagnostic criteria for each disorder with taking hold in the study of psychopathology.
the aim of improving reliability of psychiatric Lofty aims for major changes in the basis for
diagnoses. Another important structural change classification and diagnosis were announced at
was the shift to a multiaxial format and the place- the outset of the revision process, which can be
ment of personality disorders on Axis II. This traced to Kupfer et al. (2002) in A Research
ensured that clinicians would consider personality Agenda for DSM-V. Eleven conferences took
disorder diagnoses for all patients, even in the place over several years in which concepts,
presence of urgent symptoms and complaints. research, and proposals were reviewed. By the
Four personality disorders were deleted time the revision was due (and needed), the sci-
(aesthenic, cyclothymic, explosive, inadequate) ence had not yet caught up to the ambitions. With
and five new personality disorder diagnoses the exceptions of moving some disorders around
were added (borderline, narcissistic, avoidant, and discontinuing the multiaxial format, the ulti-
dependent, schizotypal). These diagnostic catego- mate revisions were more modest. Nowhere is that
ries were grouped into Cluster A – odd/eccentric more evident than for the personality disorders. In
(paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal), Cluster B – dra- fact, the DSM-5 (APA 2013) contains two
matic/erratic (borderline, narcissistic, antisocial, completely separate diagnostic systems for per-
histrionic), and Cluster C – anxious (avoidant, sonality disorders. The main “Section II” diagnos-
obsessive-compulsive, dependent). The changes tic chapter simply reprints the DSM-IV diagnostic
to personality disorder diagnoses in DSM-III criteria. This is unfortunate as it ignores years of
stimulated increased interest in treatment and accumulating research suggesting that these DSM
research. However, only a subset of diagnostic personality disorder categories have limited valid-
criterion sets were based on empirical findings ity and clinical utility, and an even more abundant
(e.g., antisocial personality disorder, borderline research literature indicating that individual
Personality Disorder 3

differences in personality are best assessed and Criterion B involves assessment of pathological
understood using dimensional trait models. personality traits which are organized into 5 broad
An Alternative Model for Personality Disor- trait domains composed of 25 specific trait facets.
ders (AMPD; Skodol 2012) appears in DSM-5 Criteria C through G cover issues of pervasiveness,
Section III. Zachar et al. (2016) provide a fasci- stability, early emergence, discrimination from
nating “oral history” of the genesis of the AMPD other mental disorders, effects of substances, and
compiled from interviews with many of the par- developmental stage or sociocultural environment.
ticipants involved in revising the personality dis- The AMPD permits diagnosis of six specific
order diagnosis for the DSM-5. The Personality personality disorder categories through algorithms
and Personality Disorder Work Group (PPDWG) for combining ratings of maladaptive traits and
examined complex and competing issues and pro- specific forms of Criteria A dysfunction. These are
posals such as prototype versus dimensional diag- antisocial, avoidant, borderline, narcissistic,
nosis and the evidential value of clinical obsessive-compulsive, and schizotypal personality
experience versus empirical data. The PPDWG disorders (these were chosen for inclusion on the
concluded a hybrid approach to personality disor- bases of empirical support and clinical relevance,
der diagnosis (Krueger et al. 2007) could enhance and their diagnostic algorithms were calibrated with
acceptance by striking a balance between intro- known prevalence rates of the personality disor-
ducing dimensional elements, which have sub- ders). Maladaptive personality patterns not covered
stantial empirical support, while preserving by these algorithms (such as paranoid, schizoid,
continuity with extant DSM categories with dem- histrionic, and dependent) are diagnosed as Person-
onstrated clinical traction. For example, in the ality Disorder-Trait Specified (PD-TS). In a PD-TS
DSM-5, antisocial personality disorder, a categor- diagnosis, the specific clinically significant traits are
ical concept, can be viewed as dysfunctional stated in lieu of an overarching category (e.g.,
behavior related to a combination of pathological PD-TS with suspiciousness, restricted affectivity,
trait dimensions including manipulativeness, cal- and hostility may diagnose “paranoid” personality
lousness, deceitfulness, hostility, risk taking, disorder). This aims to correct the problem of the
impulsivity, and irresponsibility. ambiguous and ubiquitous “PD Not Otherwise
The AMPD was approved by the DSM-5 Task Specified” diagnosis by providing a scheme for
Force, but the Board of Trustees of the American clearly characterizing the numerous patients who
Psychiatric Association rejected it and retained do not fit well into available personality disorder
the traditional categorical approach (the DSM-IV categories. In a related manner, additional signifi-
personality disorders) in Section II. The AMPD is cant traits can be included with categorical diagno-
an official alternative and a recognized competitor ses when indicated, conveniently providing the
to the traditional categorical model. It legitimately clinician with information about patient personali-
complements the DSM-5 Section II personality ties that is not captured by the specific personality
disorder diagnoses and has practical relevance disorder category.
for clinicians (Waugh et al. 2017). Indeed, the
DSM-5 allows for formal coding of the AMPD
through use of the DSM-5 code Other Specified Presentation, Development, and Course
Personality Disorder (301.89).
Diagnosis with the AMPD requires fulfilling In the following sections we describe various
seven general criteria for personality disorder. aspects of the presentation, development, and
Most innovative are the first two criteria: Criterion course of personality disorders.
A (level of personality functioning) and criterion
B (maladaptive personality traits). Criterion Epidemiology
A involves assessment of disturbance in self func- Epidemiology of personality disorders aims to
tioning (identity and self-direction) and interper- identify the distribution and prevalence of person-
sonal functioning (empathy and intimacy). ality disorders in a population. Torgersen (2012)
4 Personality Disorder

conducted an analysis of personality disorder disorders generally, Cluster A personality disor-


prevalence rates across numerous studies with ders, and Cluster C personality disorders. There-
varying populations and estimated that personal- fore, personality disorders are relatively evenly
ity disorders affect 10.5–12% of community distributed among age groups. Mirroring the uni-
dwelling adults. When divided into DSM-5 clus- formity in age, personality disorders do not vary
ters, Cluster A has a prevalence rate of 4%, and greatly by gender. Some specific personality disor-
Cluster B has a prevalence rate ranging from 3.5% ders may be more prevalent in men, like antisocial
to 4%. Cluster C occurs the most frequently with a personality disorder, or in women, like dependent
prevalence rate of 7–7.5% (APA 2013). It is also personality disorder, but globally speaking, person-
valuable to consider the clinical prevalence rates ality disorders occur equally in men and women. In
of diagnosed personality disorders among patients sum, personality disorders are present in commu-
receiving psychiatric care as clinical prevalence nity dwellers, with higher prevalence rates in clin-
rates are higher than those observed in the com- ical samples. Due to the nature of personality
munity. Calculated from various epidemiological disorders, poor behavioral, interpersonal, and emo-
studies, Torgersen (2012) reports a clinical popu- tional functioning paired with a reduced quality of
lation prevalence rate of about 66%. The DSM life is common among individuals diagnosed with
clusters of personality disorders mirror this personality disorders. Finally, personality disorders
increase in frequency: Cluster A (11%), Cluster are a universal issue that equally affects age groups
B (32%), and Cluster C (28%). Moreover, border- and genders. In order to gain a better understanding
line personality disorder is the most common per- of personality disorders, it is imperative to consider
sonality disorder in clinical populations. genetic and heritability factors that can give rise to
According to the DSM-5 Section II diagnostic their manifestation.
criteria, personality disorders are marked by sig-
nificant distress or impairment in interpersonal Genetic Factors
functioning, cognition, behavior, and emotional A major goal in behavioral genetic research is to
response (APA 2013). It follows that individuals parse out the influence of both genes and environ-
diagnosed with personality disorders would expe- ment on the manifestation of personality disor-
rience pervasive dysfunction and diminished ders. This manifestation takes the form of
quality of life. In a meta-analysis of studies inves- observable behaviors or characteristics and is
tigating maladjustment and outcomes of person- often referred to as a phenotype, a visible expres-
ality disorders, individuals diagnosed with Cluster sion of genes. These phenotypic expressions largely
A personality disorders, borderline personality influence how personality disorders are conceptu-
disorder, avoidant personality disorder, and alized and classified. According to a meta-analysis
dependent personality disorder were observed to conducted by South et al. (2012), both genes and
experience the most dysfunction and reduced environment play significant roles in trait expres-
quality of life, possibly from the rampant interper- sion in personality disorders. Furthermore, South
sonal issues that afflict each disorder. et al. (2012) found that the heritability of personal-
In addition to considering the prevalence and ity disorders falls between 40% and 50%. Through
consequences of personality disorders, it is crucial a behavioral genetics perspective, personality dis-
to consider their distribution across age and gen- orders can be conceptualized as resulting from
der. Personality disorders are diagnosed in indi- about equal genetic and environmental factors.
viduals who are at least 18 years old due to the Notable progress in identifying the genetic
pervasive pattern of maladaptive behavior loading of personality disorders has also occurred.
required for a diagnosis. Cluster B personality By identifying common genes that predispose an
disorders are more common in younger adults, individual to a specific dysfunction, the dimen-
especially antisocial personality disorder and bor- sionality and overlap of personality disorders is
derline personality disorder. There are no signifi- identified, providing a deeper understanding of
cant differences regarding age and personality cause and conceptualization. Comparing the
Personality Disorder 5

identical genes and dissimilar environments of adolescence because the structure of dysfunc-
twins, shared genetic risk factors for Cluster tional characteristics at that stage is similar to the
A personality disorders were identified (Kendler structure of the personality disorder in adulthood
et al. 2006). Moreover, personality disorders in (Westen et al. 2003). Similarly, each personality
Cluster B and Cluster C have common genetic disorder of the DSM-5 has a “development and
liability within clusters (Reichborn-Kjennerud course” clause to be used when pinpointing per-
et al. 2007; Torgersen et al. 2008). This common- sonality disorder features in childhood and ado-
ality among clusters illuminates the similarities of lescence. In the Children in the Community (CIC)
personality disorders within a grouping as well as Study, a group of children was assessed for per-
how genes impact observable traits and, therefore, sonality disorder features at various time points
classification. A study conducted by Kendler et al. (Cohen and Cohen 1996). Mothers and children
(2008) provides an example of how environment in two New York counties participated in the study
can shape gene expression and ultimately lead to a due to mothers expressing need for children’s ser-
specific manifestation of noticeable traits. In a vices. Beginning in adolescence, the children were
community-based twin study, a gene was identified assessed for personality disorders. Individuals
to be present in each personality disorder cluster who reported greater personality disorder traits
and have high loading onto paranoid, borderline, significantly differed in functioning than those
narcissistic, histrionic, obsessive-compulsive, and who did not (Crawford et al. 2005). This longitu-
dependent personality disorders (Kendler et al. dinal example of stable impairment suggests the
2008). Because this one gene is associated with need for early identification of personality disorder
multiple personality disorders, the gene most likely traits due to the maintained risk over time.
is indicative of a broad and general vulnerability to Because personality disorders are generally
personality disorders in which the observable man- diagnosed in adulthood, an epidemiological
ifestation of symptoms is significantly influenced study of prevalence rates of personality disorders
by environment. Progress in genetic research in adolescence does not exist; however, the prev-
regarding personality disorders paints a clearer pic- alence of personality disorders in adolescence is
ture of how and why personality disorders develop estimated to be 6–17% based off of the data from
in terms of heritability and environmental influ- the CIC Study (Johnson et al. 2006). Personality
ence. This development can begin as early as child- disorder characteristics may be present in adoles-
hood and persist into adulthood. cence, but the characteristics may not yet be stable
and enduring patterns of behavior. Furthermore, it
Childhood and Adolescent Development is generally accepted that mood and anxiety dis-
Traditionally, personality disorders are diagnosed orders in childhood are predictive of personality
in adulthood due to the rigid and pervasive mal- disorders in adulthood (De Fruyt and De Clercq
adaptive patterns of cognition, affect, and behav- 2012). In sum, early manifestation of dysfunc-
ior. During childhood and adolescence, tional traits is associated with increased genetic
personality characteristics are malleable, change loading for disorders and inability to fully recover
over the developmental course from childhood to from related impairments. This chronic and per-
adulthood, and prove difficult to distinguish vasive pattern can last into adulthood where a
between transitory and appropriate developmental personality disorder diagnosis can then be
stages and enduring, maladaptive personality applied. Just as genes can predispose an individ-
traits (APA 2013). Although personality disorders ual to choose a specific environment, experiences
are unlikely to be diagnosed before adulthood, can mitigate or exacerbate the influence of genes
pathological personality traits that are present and serve as protective or accelerating factors on
regardless of development stage throughout child- the later development of personality disorders.
hood and adolescence are critical markers of later Thus, features of dysfunction in childhood and
personality disorder diagnosis. Some assert that adolescence are indicative of greater dysfunction
personality disorders can be recognized in later in life and should be identified early.
6 Personality Disorder

Course personality disorder also meet the criteria neces-


Personality disorders are characterized as being sary for a diagnosis of multiple personality disor-
enduring and stable patterns of behavior with an ders (Zimmerman et al. 2005). Trull et al. (2012)
onset in late adolescence or early adulthood (APA analyzed the prevalence rates of co-occurring per-
2013). However, recent findings suggest that per- sonality disorders in clinical samples in order to
sonality disorders may not be as untreatable and discern correlations between personality disor-
unmalleable as previously thought. Data from the ders. Results indicate that narcissistic personality
CIC Study, a longitudinal study collecting infor- disorder was the most likely to occur alongside
mation regarding personality disorders from other disorders while antisocial personality disor-
childhood to adulthood, were used to estimate a der was the least likely, and personality disorders
point-prevalence rate (i.e., an estimate of preva- within Cluster A and Cluster B were highly cor-
lence based on a single time point) ranging from related. The comorbidity among personality dis-
about 13% to 15% and a lifetime prevalence rate orders suggests marked overlap in diagnostic
(i.e., cumulative assessment across all time criteria as well as severity of symptomatology.
points) of about 28% (Johnson et al. 2008). Personality disorders are also commonly comor-
Because the prevalence rates differ, there is evi- bid with other forms of psychological disorders.
dence that the course of personality disorders is The widespread comorbidity of personality disor-
not completely stable across the lifespan. Simi- ders with other psychological disorders signifies
larly, the data from the Longitudinal Study of the complexity of interactions between mecha-
Personality Disorders, comprised of 250 college nisms driving dysfunction as well as the shared
students assessing the stability of personality dis- symptomatology criteria of various disorders.
orders over time, found variable improving and
declining trajectories of personality dysfunction
Cultural Considerations
across participants (Wright et al. 2011). The
Considering the cultural aspects of personality
McLean Study of Adult Development, consisting
disorders proves to be a critical yet difficult task.
of 290 in-patient participants diagnosed with bor-
Cultural considerations of personality disorders
derline personality disorder illustrating that severe
are lacking an overarching, cohesive theory and
personality disorders could remit, and a reduction
research. One central issue rests in how culture is
in severity and number of symptoms could occur
defined as the chosen definition of culture directly
(Zanarini et al. 2003). Finally, 85% of patients
influences how cultural aspects relate to personal-
assessed in the Collaborative Longitudinal Per-
ity disorders. Without a consensus, there is vari-
sonality Disorder Study remitted after a 10-year
ability in piecemeal findings. Additionally,
period (Gunderson et al. 2011). These examples,
research regarding culture and personality disor-
paired with identifiable dysfunctional traits in
ders is lacking; thus, the findings to consider and
childhood and adolescence, illustrate that person-
compare are sparse. The majority of personality
ality disorders may not be as stable and consistent
disorders and culture research uses the distinction
as originally thought supporting the need for fur-
between individualism and collectivism (Mulder
ther research into the variable development,
2012). It is clearly important to consider differ-
course, and treatment outcomes for personality
ences in societies that endorse personal unique-
disorders.
ness and those that promote cooperativeness,
interpersonal relationships, and social roles. Addi-
Comorbidity
tionally, cross-culturally reliable personality
Co-occurring psychological disorders are often
traits, as well as culturally unique characteristics
the rule rather than the exception, and personality
that constitute dysfunctional behavior should be
disorders are no different. Personality disorders
identified when forming classification standards
are extensively comorbid with each other as well
for and diagnosis of personality disorders (Mulder
as other psychopathology. It is estimated that 50%
2012; Ryder et al. 2015).
of individuals who have one diagnosed
Personality Disorder 7

Conceptualization learning that may contribute to the interpersonal


impairments characteristic of the diagnosis
Major approaches to conceptualizing the nature of (Mitropoulou et al. 2002). Deficient prepulse inhi-
personality disorders are reviewed in the follow- bition and impaired P50 wave suppression suggest
ing sections. that these deficits may result from an inability to
filter out irrelevant information early on in infor-
Neurobiological Conceptualization mation processing (Cadenhead et al. 2000; Kumari
Neurobiological approaches to personality disor- et al. 2005). Although schizotypal personality dis-
ders have primarily focused on identifying endo- order is more generally associated with increases in
phenotypes (observable characteristics with clear dopamine activity, decreases in dopamine activity
genetic connections that are associated with a in the prefrontal cortex may explain deficits in
larger disorder without themselves being direct working memory, executive functioning, and to a
symptoms of the disorder) associated with mal- lesser degree, sustained attention and verbal learn-
adaptive personality dimensions and traits. Person- ing (Roussos and Siever 2012). Dopamine agonists
ality disorders then are conceptualized as complex have been found to improve performance in these
combinations of these endophenotypes. Thus far, domains (Rosell et al. 2015). Additionally,
endophenotypes associated with a number of per- although findings are inconsistent, qualitative def-
sonality dimensions have been examined (Depue icits in smooth pursuit eye movement measures
and Lenzenweger 2006; Roussos and Siever have been proposed as a candidate endophenotype
2012). These dimensions include but are not lim- for schizotypal personality disorder.
ited to psychotic-like perceptual distortions and Cluster B. Research on possible endo-
cognitive impairment (commonly associated with phenotypes associated with Cluster B personality
DSM-5 Cluster A), affective instability, aggres- disorders has focused on the dimensions of affec-
sion, and impulsivity (commonly associated with tive instability, aggression, and impulsivity, par-
DSM-5 Cluster B), and anxiety and compulsivity ticularly in borderline personality disorder
(commonly associated with DSM-5 Cluster C). populations. Research on the limbic system, an
Cluster A. Most of the research on possible area of the brain associated with encoding,
endophenotypes associated with Cluster A has inhibiting, and regulating emotions, has also
focused on schizotypal personality disorder. As been examined. Although results across studies
part of the schizophrenia spectrum, schizotypal are inconsistent, a meta-analysis on limbic system
personality disorder is characterized by structures in borderline patients suggests that
psychotic-like perceptual distortions and cognitive these patients have reduced bilateral hippocampal
and social impairments. Higher dopamine neuro- and amygdala volumes compared to healthy con-
transmission is consistently associated with per- trols (Nunes et al. 2009). Functionally, borderline
ceptual distortions and is often found in patients have shown increased amygdala activity
individuals with schizophrenia. Although they when viewing aversive stimuli (Koenigsberg et al.
have consistently less dopaminergic activity than 2009) and similar amygdala responses across
acutely schizophrenic patients, individuals with aversive and neutral stimuli (Schnell et al. 2007).
schizotypal personality disorder seem to exhibit This suggests a possible neural explanation for the
modestly increased dopaminergic activity (Siever heightened emotional responsiveness characteris-
and Davis 2004). It is possible that reduced striatal tic of borderline personality disorder.
and caudate volumes and elevated relative glucose Research on the neurobiological correlates of
metabolic rate in the striatum (Levitt et al. 2004; impulsive aggression has found evidence of
Shihabuddin et al. 2001) may protect schizotypal increased limbic reactivity paired with a
patients from more apparent psychosis. decreased ability to control aggressive impulses
Unlike other personality disorders, schizotypal and behaviors. Among borderline patients,
personality disorder is associated with deficits in aggression has been associated with a heightened
sustained attention, working memory, and verbal relative glucose metabolic rate in limbic
8 Personality Disorder

structures, including the amygdala and this information, individuals develop schemas or
orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and a lower glucose cognitive frameworks that help the individual
metabolic rate in more dorsal brain regions asso- organize and interpret incoming information,
ciated with cognitive control (Roussos and Siever decide what information to attend to, and antici-
2012). Additionally, amygdala and OFC activa- pate and navigate similar situations in the future.
tion is only weakly correlated in patients with Ideally, an individual will accurately perceive and
Cluster B personality disorders (New et al. 2007) correctly interpret the situation using their
suggesting decreased functional connectedness schemas and as a result have adaptive and appro-
between emotion related and inhibitory regions priate cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
of the brain, making it difficult to modulate responses. However, schemas can also bias the
aggressive impulses. way an individual processes information by
Evidence of a diminished capacity of the sero- influencing three important aspects of cognition:
tonergic system, which plays an important role in automatic thoughts (an immediate and spontane-
the top-down regulation of aggression, was asso- ous appraisal of the situation), interpersonal strat-
ciated with intense anger, impulsivity, and self- egies (beliefs and assumptions about how to
damaging behavior (but not affective instability) respond when certain automatic thoughts occur),
in borderline patients (Coccaro et al. 1989). These and cognitive distortions (systematic errors in rea-
abnormalities may be due to a number of poly- soning that can contribute to misinterpretations of
morphisms in genes related to the synthesis, events). When an individual’s perception or inter-
metabolism, and receptor responsiveness of the pretation is biased, their cognitive, emotional, and
serotonergic system that have been implicated in behavioral responses will be inappropriate to
aggression. In addition to the diminished activa- some extent. Given the ambiguity of many inter-
tion in areas of the frontal cortex involved in personal interactions and the complexity of daily
impulse control and diminished capacity of the life, isolated misperceptions are inevitable and
serotonergic system, impulsivity more generally can be addressed by corrective feedback and sub-
has also been associated with variations in dopa- sequent experience. Psychopathology, and in par-
minergic genes. ticular, personality pathology, occurs when an
Cluster C. Very few neurobiological studies individual experiences the world in a chronically
have examined Cluster C personality disorders. distorted and systematically biased way that
The limited work has identified altered dopamine results in large discrepancies between their sub-
and serotonin activity in social anxiety disorders jective experience and objective reality. Once this
more generally, which are highly comorbid with occurs, an individual’s schemas tend to bias their
avoidant personality disorder (Mathew et al. perception of events, including feedback, such
2001), as well as links between serotonin deficits that counterevidence is overlooked, discounted,
and impulsive aggressive symptoms in obsessive or misinterpreted. Additionally, consistently
compulsive personality disorder (Stein et al. distorted responses may create a self-fulfilling
1996). Allelic variants of the dopamine receptors prophecy, where others begin to act in ways con-
D3 and D4 have also been associated with sistent with the original distortion.
increased rates of avoidant and obsessive compul- Thus from a cognitive perspective, the term
sive personality disorder symptomatology (Joyce personality disorder applies to individuals with
et al. 2003). pervasive, self-perpetuating, cognitive interper-
sonal cycles that cause significant distress and
Cognitive Conceptualization are resistant to change (Pretzer and Beck 2005).
A cognitive theory of personality disorders Personality disorders can be differentiated from
assumes that humans are constantly and continu- one another based on differences in the content of
ously perceiving, interpreting, recalling, and the individual’s commonly activated schemas,
learning from information in their environment beliefs, assumptions, and distortions. Core mal-
(Pretzer and Beck 2005). To facilitate processing adaptive beliefs and schemas associated with
Personality Disorder 9

different types of personality disorders have been characterized by identity diffusion. When identity
systematized (Beck et al. 2015; Young diffusion occurs, an individual’s sense of self and
et al. 2003). other is loosely organized, lacks depth and sub-
tlety, is polarized (very good or very bad with few
Psychodynamic Conceptualization shades of gray in between), and is unstable across
A psychodynamic perspective is inherently broad time and situations. Individuals with diffuse iden-
and encapsulates a number of different tities tend to have inconsistent tastes, opinions,
approaches and theories. The current discussion and values that are often adopted from those
will focus predominately on an object-relations around them and have trouble accurately under-
theory perspective on personality pathology standing other people and responding appropri-
(Yeomans et al. 2015), which predominates in ately to social cues. Personality rigidity, a second
contemporary discussions and most directly component of a structural formulation, refers to
relates to the dynamic treatment approaches pre- the automatic, repeated, and inflexible activation
sented below. More information on alternative of particular behaviors across situations regard-
psychodynamic conceptualizations can be found less of whether they are appropriate to the situa-
elsewhere (e.g., McWilliams 2011). tion. Generally, greater rigidity is associated with
From an object-relations perspective, a greater personality pathology.
description of personality disorders begins with Additionally, the structural formulation is
the symptoms or descriptive features of the disor- determined by how an individual responds to dis-
der, including an individual’s presenting prob- tress and can the person accurately decide what is
lems, maladaptive personality traits, and the real and what is not. Defenses are the strategies a
quality of their interpersonal relationships. This person uses to minimize distress. Individuals that
understanding then deepens to include a struc- use predominately mature defenses, like humor or
tural formulation (i.e., persistent ways the indi- anticipating and preparing for potentially stressful
vidual organizes experience of self and other) that situations, tend to be higher functioning. Mature
underlies those symptoms. The structural formu- defenses involve minimal reality distortion and do
lation provides information about the severity of not keep distressing information out of awareness.
the individual’s personality pathology (ranging Neurotic defenses avoid unpleasant feelings by
from healthy and neurotic levels of functioning repressing or keeping distressing parts of experi-
to borderline and psychotic levels of functioning). ence from awareness, distorting internal but not
Because the structural formation is integral in external reality. Primitive defenses make use of
determining whether personality pathology is pre- dissociation or splitting to avoid emotional dis-
sent and if so, its severity, the structural formula- tress. Splitting refers to a process in which two
tion is described in greater detail below. Finally, a aspects of experience that are in conflict (often the
comprehensive description of personality pathol- positive, idealized aspects of experience and neg-
ogy includes developing a theory about the indi- ative, persecutory aspects of experience) are com-
vidual, including what might be motivating the partmentalized. That is, both are allowed to
individual’s behavior without their awareness and emerge into awareness but not at the same time,
what internal conflicts might be present, to give distorting a person’s external reality and often
meaning to the descriptive and structural features. leading to affectively charged, rapid, and chaotic
The structural formulation begins by examin- shifts in experience.
ing how an individual views themselves and other Thus, according to object-relations theory,
people. When identity is integrated, individuals healthy individuals are characterized by an inte-
have the capacity to pull together different aspects grated identity, flexible adaptation, mature
(both good and bad) of the self and/or other to defenses, and intact and stable reality testing. At
comprise a coherent, complex, and whole picture the neurotic level of personality organization,
of a person that is consistent over time and across individuals are characterized by maladaptive per-
experiences. Lower levels of functioning are sonality rigidity in the presence of normal identity,
10 Personality Disorder

a predominance of higher level, repression based closely matches the unfolding proximal situation
defenses, and intact reality testing. The more and are, therefore, able to accurately respond to
severe, borderline organization is associated with the agentic and communal bids offered by others.
clinically significant identity pathology, a pre- Personality pathology occurs when an individual
dominance of lower level splitting based defenses, chronically distorts the agentic and communal
and reality testing that deteriorates in affectively behavior of self and others resulting in maladap-
intense situations. tive interpersonal functioning. Because they often
distort reality, individual with personality disor-
Interpersonal Conceptualization ders may react chaotically, self-protectively, or
According to Contemporary Integrative Interper- rigidly pull for complementary responses, but
sonal Theory (CIIT; Dawood et al. in press; have difficulty responding to others in comple-
Pincus 2005), the most important expressions of mentary ways. This reduces the likelihood that
personality and personality pathology occur in the agentic and communal needs of both people
situations involving more than one person. will be satisfied in the interpersonal situation and
These interpersonal situations can occur in the create disturbed interpersonal relations (Hopwood
external world via observable, in-person et al. 2013; Pincus and Hopwood 2012).
(proximal) interactions, or within an individual
via memories of past experiences, fantasies, or Integrative Conceptualizations
expectations of future experiences. According to According to an integrative perspective, a person-
CIIT, both proximal and internal interactions can ality disorder develops when personality traits
be described and conceptualized in terms of become extreme and begin to interfere with occu-
agency and communion. Agency refers to an indi- pational and interpersonal functioning. This com-
vidual’s ability to differentiate themselves and is plex process cannot be fully explained from a
manifested in strivings for autonomy, power, and purely biological, psychological, or social per-
achievement. Communion refers to an individ- spective. Thus, an integrative perspective tends
ual’s ability to integrate themselves into a larger to follow a biopsychosocial model that attributes
social entity and is manifested in strivings for the development of personality disorders to the
intimacy, union, and solidarity. interaction between biological (temperament),
Within an interaction, behavior is not random. psychological, and social factors (Paris 2012).
Rather, perceived behaviors, which vary in their Temperament is composed of the in-born ten-
level of distortion, tend to pull, elicit, invite, or dencies that underlie individual differences in
evoke particular responses. One pattern of behav- traits related to reactivity and self-regulation,
ior that has been well articulated and can be con- including emotionality (in how a person experi-
sidered a common baseline for healthy ences and expresses negative emotions), activity
socialization is interpersonal complementarity. level, sociability, and shyness. While tempera-
In interpersonal complementarity, dominant ment itself is not sufficient for the development
behavior pulls for submissive behavior on the of a personality disorder, the genetic vulnerabil-
agency dimension and friendliness pulls for ities associated with temperament do appear to
friendliness while hostility pulls for hostility on link personality traits and psychopathology
the communal dimension. Other patterns of (vulnerability model). There is also evidence that
behavior are unique to the individual and develop a person’s temperament may influence the sever-
based on past experience and learning history. ity of personality pathology when present
These patterns can be adaptive and help the indi- (pathoplasiticity model).
vidual navigate their social world or can be prob- Although there are a number of psychological
lematic and disrupt an individual’s interpersonal factors that can influence the development of a
functioning. personality disorder, childhood adversity has
CIIT proposes that healthy individuals gener- received particular attention in the literature.
ally have an undistorted internal situation that Research on the effects of childhood physical
Personality Disorder 11

and sexual abuse, early separations or losses, bul- and well-validated treatments, including symp-
lying and violence in school, and abnormal par- tom-oriented psychopharmacology, dialectical
enting behavior has identified these variables as behavioral therapy, schema-focused therapy,
possible risk factors. However, given that many mentalization-based therapy, transference-focused
children who experience these risk factors never psychotherapy, and interpersonal psychotherapy.
develop a personality disorder, the etiological sig-
nificance of any one risk factor remains unclear. Psychopharmacology
Rather, the cumulative experience of adversity in Although there are no medications specifically
childhood seems to best explain the lower thresh- approved by the United States Food and Drug
olds for psychopathology in this population Administration for the treatment of personality
(Rutter et al. 2006). disorders, medications are regularly used in their
In addition to biological and psychological treatments. Randomized controlled trials for per-
considerations, personality disorders develop in a sonality disorders, of which most focus on bor-
sociocultural context and appear to be socially derline personality disorder (BPD), show that
sensitive. Although the role of social factors has mood stabilizers and anticonvulsants are useful
not been widely studied, a number of theoretical in treating depression and affective instability in
models have been proposed. One hypothesis sug- BPD as well as specific symptoms in personality
gests that modernity and social change have disorders more generally, particularly aggression
increased the risk for impulsive personality disor- and impulsivity, characteristics of both BPD and
ders (Paris 2004) and is supported by cross- antisocial personality disorder (see Paris 2011;
cultural studies that have found lower rates of Ripoll et al. 2011 for reviews). Antidepressants
antisocial personality disorder in more traditional are also efficacious in reducing anger, aggression,
societies (Hwu et al. 1989). Researchers also and impulsivity in personality disorders; antipsy-
hypothesize that cohort effects (changes in person- chotics are efficacious for decreasing psychotic-
ality disorder prevalence rates over brief periods of like symptoms in both BPD and schizotypal per-
time) could be due to difficulty adapting to a sonality disorder; and monoamine oxidase inhib-
changing, less supportive society and propose itors for reducing social anxiety in avoidant
that acting impulsively may be one way to deal personality disorder. Overall, medications may
with emotional dysregulation (Linehan 1993). be useful in treating specific symptoms of person-
An integrative perspective then suggests that ality disorders, but they should be considered an
the biological, psychological, and social risk fac- adjunctive rather than as a stand-alone interven-
tors for personality disorders be integrated within a tion, as psychotherapy is considered the gold stan-
single interactive and integrative model. Heritable dard for treatment of personality disorders.
factors influence individual variability in tempera-
ment and trait dimensions. However, this variabil- Dialectical Behavior Therapy
ity usually only becomes maladaptive when Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT) is a specific
amplified by cultural context and cumulative life type of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT)
stressors. Thus, according to the integrative model, developed in the late 1980s by Linehan (1993),
temperamentally predisposed individuals who initially to treat chronically suicidal, multi-
experience multiple risk factors would be most problem patients (e.g., BPD), but has since been
likely to develop a personality disorder later in life. extended to patients with traditionally difficult-to-
treat disorders (e.g., eating disorders, substance
dependence; Lungu and Linehan 2016). DBT is
Empirically Supported Treatment based on the biosocial theory of the etiology of
BPD, and it draws on principles of Zen and dia-
Over the past several decades, evidence-based lectical philosophies, blending cognitive-
treatments have emerged for treating personality behavioral methods with more acceptance-based
disorders. We briefly describe the most researched strategies. This treatment consists of four main
12 Personality Disorder

components: weekly individual therapy sessions, these coping strategies may mitigate intense neg-
weekly skills training groups, weekly therapist ative emotions, they inadvertently reinforce the
consultation team meetings, and between-session underlying schema and cause problems in self
telephone coaching is offered to patients in and interpersonal functioning. SFT focuses on
moments of crisis. helping patients identify their schemas and
One of the most importance aspects of DBT is replace them with more adaptive ones. To accom-
the “dialectics” of acceptance and change. That is, plish this, SFT uses four core mechanisms: (1) lim-
therapists accept the patient as they are (in the ited reparenting, (2) experiential imagery and
context of emotional validation), but also actively chair dialogue work, (3) education and cognitive
encourage the patient’s need for behavioral restructuring, and (4) behavioral pattern breaking.
change. The goals of treatment are to reduce A major component of SFT is the therapeutic
acute life-threatening behaviors (suicide, self- relationship, which is conceptualized as limited
harm), reduce behaviors that interfere with ther- reparenting. That is, the therapist creates a good,
apy (noncompliance, lateness), reduce behaviors healthy parent-like relationship with the patient
that interfere with quality of life (mental illness, that is characterized by empathy, warmth, care,
psychosocial factors), and increase behavioral protection, limit setting, and careful self-
skills (mindfulness, emotional regulation, inter- disclosure (Young et al. 2003). The corrective
personal effectiveness, distress tolerance). The therapeutic experience works to disconfirm and
patient uses the new learned skills to deal with reshape the patient’s maladaptive schemas and
problem behaviors and adversity and to facilitate enhance the patient’s capacity to relate to others
achievement of personal goals. beyond the therapist. Throughout different points
in the therapy, the therapist will use techniques
Schema-Focused Therapy such as role-playing, letter writing, assertiveness
Schema-focused therapy (SFT) also derives training, anger management, guided imagery,
mainly from CBT and was originally developed relaxation exercises, gradual exposure to
in the early 1990s by Young (1994) for patients anxiety-provoking situations, and challenge neg-
who did not respond to standard CBT, most often ative thoughts and beliefs about the self, others,
those with personality disorders and character- and the world.
ological problems. SFT is more integrative than
standard CBT, combining principles and tech- Mentalization-Based Therapy
niques from CBT with psychodynamic, attach- Mentalization-based therapy (MBT) is a specific
ment, gestalt, and emotion-focused therapies. type of psychodynamically oriented therapy,
Although first developed for BPD, SFT has since developed and manualized by Bateman and
been elaborated for most personality disorders. Fonagy (2012). They argue that the effectiveness
SFT is based on the idea that aversive childhood of all treatments involves increasing the patient’s
experiences and unmet basic childhood needs capacity to mentalize. Mentalization refers to
(e.g., safety, autonomy, love, acceptance, respect) one’s ability to think about the mental states (i.e.,
lead to the development of early maladaptive beliefs, desires, needs, feelings, intentions) of
schemas, which are negative thoughts and feel- one’s own and others. According to Bateman
ings individuals have about themselves and their and Fonagy, a key problem for individuals with
relations with others. Young described 18 mal- BPD is a deficit in mentalization, especially
adaptive schemas; examples of schemas in per- within interpersonal contexts, which leaves them
sonality disorders include abandonment, mistrust/ vulnerable to emotional dysregulation and impul-
abuse, shame/defectiveness, and unrelenting stan- sivity. Thus, the focus of MBT is on stabilizing
dards (Young et al. 2003). If a schema becomes emotional expression, promoting the exploration
triggered, associated negative emotions result and of one’s internal world and that of others, and
a maladaptive coping response arises (e.g., avoid- strengthening the patient’s sense of self. Specifi-
ance, surrender, overcompensation). Though cally, the therapist follows a series of steps to
Personality Disorder 13

engage the patient in the process of mentalizing: recognizing and addressing threats to the treat-
(1) empathize and support the patient’s present ment, and identifying and recapitulating the
subjective state; (2) clarify, elaborate, and chal- patient’s predominant relational patterns as they
lenge; (3) focus on intense affect states and the are expressed and experienced outside the con-
expression of emotions; and (4) mentalize the sulting room and within the transference. The
therapist-patient relationship. Individual gains therapist repeatedly uses the techniques of clarifi-
from MBT include more gratifying relationships, cation, confrontation, and interpretation to pro-
greater tolerance of distress and negative emo- vide the patient with the opportunity to integrate
tions, and a reduction in impulsive behaviors. split off and disorganized motives, thoughts, and
MBT can be delivered in individual, group, or feelings, thereby leading to changes in both symp-
family settings. toms and personality structure over time.

Transference-Focused Psychotherapy Interpersonal Psychotherapy


Transference-focused-psychotherapy (TFP) is a There is no single interpersonal psychotherapy,
highly structured, once to twice weekly, psycho- but rather interpersonal therapy encompasses a
dynamic treatment designed for patients with variety of therapeutic approaches that stem from
BPD and other severe personality disorders the shared view that interpersonal processes are
(Yeomans et al. 2015). TFP, based on Kernberg’s fundamental to the development and maintenance
(1984) object relation’s model of identity diffu- of pathology and to the patient’s experience of
sion in borderline personality organization, con- distress (Pincus and Cain 2008). The major aim
ceptualizes the disorder in terms of of interpersonal psychotherapy is to clarify and
undifferentiated and unintegrated representations increase the personality disorder patient’s under-
of self and others and their affective valence. The standing of how he or she views their maladaptive
aim of TFP is to help patients “integrate all aspects communication patterns with others. This new
of their internal world. . .in order to experience insight brings about new interpersonal awareness
themselves and others in a coherent and balanced and social learning; thereby, leading to improved
way” (Yeomans et al. 2015, p. 42). As the patient relational capacity and a reduction in symptoms.
develops the capacity to think more flexibly, real- Kiesler’s (1988) interpersonal meta-
istically, and benevolently about their own mental communication (IM) is an exemplar of an inter-
states and those of important others, he or she personal approach to psychotherapy. IM focuses
tends to experience a reduction in self-defeating on exploring the unfolding relationship between
and self-destructive behaviors, increased emotion the patient and therapist, as the therapeutic rela-
regulation and behavioral control, increased tionship is thought to contain important informa-
coherence of identity, greater capacity for inti- tion about how the patient views his or her self and
macy, and general improvement in symptoms their relationships with others. In the initial phase
(e.g., depression, anxiety) and functioning. of IM, the therapist becomes “hooked” and reacts
TFP begins with establishing a treatment frame to the patient in ways that others do. The therapist
or contract, which outlines the responsibilities and must become aware of this pattern and should
roles of the patient and therapist, the method of question his or her experience of the patient by
treatment, and the conditions of the therapy, asking questions such as “What is this patient
including the management of self-harming and doing to me?” and/or “What am I feeling when
suicidal urges and behavior. As treatment pro- I’m with this patient?” (Pincus and Cain 2008).
gresses, the focus shifts to affect-laden themes Once attuned to the patient’s maladaptive inter-
that emerge in the “here-and-now” relationship personal behavior and communication, the thera-
between the patient and therapist (i.e., transfer- pist should disrupt it by disengaging or choosing
ence). During the first year of treatment, TFP to respond in an asocial manner (e.g., silence,
focuses on the following hierarchy of goals: reflection of content or feeling, delayed
containing suicidal and self-destructive behaviors, responses). Doing so provides the patient with a
14 Personality Disorder

corrective emotional experience and models more remains entrenched. We believe that the future of
adaptive ways of communicating with others. personality disorder classification and diagnosis
Other interpersonal psychotherapies share similar will require a further shift toward dimensional
goals and features to IM but also differ by specific models and away from categorical
intervention techniques (see Anchin and Pincus conceptualizations.
2010; Cain and Pincus 2016). The second major challenge for the field is the
development of more effective treatments for per-
sonality disorders. It is notable that virtually all
Conclusion major therapeutic approaches began as treatments
for BPD and the majority of empirical support for
Future Directions these treatments involves studies of patients with
The diagnosis and classification of personality BPD. Although the treatments have been
disorders is in flux, as reflected in the two distinct extended to other personality disorders, there has
diagnostic alternatives found in the DSM-5. The been little research on their efficacy and effective-
two most important and interrelated challenges ness for such patients. It remains problematic that
facing the field are transitioning to a dimensional there are no empirically supported treatments
model of personality disorder and developing developed specifically for categories of personal-
effective treatments for these challenging patients. ity disorder other than BPD, limiting the treatment
Over the last century, based on accumulating implications of these diagnoses. However, this is
empirical evidence, the study of personality and likely due to the lack of validity of the current
individual differences has generally abandoned categories. As conceptualizations shift toward
typological approaches in favor of hierarchically dimensional models, new treatments will need to
arranged dimensional models of personality traits focus on the core impairments of personality dis-
such as the Five-Factor Model (Digman 1990). In order common across all patients with modifica-
contrast, the classification of personality disorders tions directed at working with individual
in the psychiatric nomenclature has remained differences in prominent pathological traits and
steadfast in its categorical description of discrete their maladaptive behavioral expressions. This
personality disorder diagnoses (APA 2013). can be conceived of as distinguishing the genus
Although a dimensional representation of person- (personality disorder) and the species (prominent
ality disorders is possible (Widiger and Simonsen traits) of individual patients (Pincus 2011). Fortu-
2005), objections have been raised regarding its nately, the existing treatments for BPD actually
clinical utility and the loss of important clinical target many manifestations of the personality dis-
conceptualizations that have a long history in the order genus and can likely be further modified and
field. Although long-standing categorical person- extended to address problems associated with a
ality disorder conceptualizations are more familiar consensus dimensional model describing the indi-
to practitioners, research demonstrates that vidual differences of various species that present
dimensional alternatives are actually easier to for treatment (Pincus et al. 2016). As the field
use and favored by a majority of clinicians shifts its perspectives on the conceptualization
(Morey et al. 2014). Of the current categorical and nature of personality disorders, intervention
diagnoses, the research base on BPD and antiso- models will need to shift in concurrence as well.
cial personality disorder far exceeds that of the
remaining diagnoses, perhaps because of the
social, medical, economic, and legal costs of Cross-References
these disorders. Thus the empirical support for
these other categorical diagnoses is weaker and ▶ Antisocial Personality Disorder
empirically supported treatments for them are lim- ▶ Avoidant Personality Disorder
ited. As reflected in the DSM-5 AMPD, the tide is ▶ Borderline Personality Disorder
now shifting (Krueger 2013), but resistance ▶ Dependent Personality Disorder
Personality Disorder 15

▶ Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Serotonergic studies in patients with affective and per-
Disorders (DSM-5) sonality disorders: Correlates with suicidal and impul-
sive aggressive behavior. Archives of General
▶ Histrionic Personality Disorder Psychiatry, 46(7), 587–599. doi: 10.1001/
▶ Narcissistic Personality Disorder archpsyc.1989.01810070013002
▶ Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1996). Life values and adolescent
▶ Schizoid Personality Disorder mental health. Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Crawford, T. N., Cohen, P., Johnson, J. G., Kasen, S., First,
▶ Schizotypal Personality Disorder M. B., Gordon, K., & Brook, J. S. (2005). Self-reported
personality disorder in the children in the community
sample: Convergent and prospective validity in late
adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Personality Dis-
References orders, 19(1), 30–52. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.1.30.62179
Dawood, S., Dowgwillo, E. A., Wu, L. Z., & Pincus, A. L.
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and (in press). Contemporary integrative interpersonal the-
statistical manual of mental disorders (1st ed.). ory of personality. In V. Ziegler-Hill & T. K.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and and individual differences. New York: Guilford.
statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). De Fruyt, F., & De Clercq, B. (2012). Childhood anteced-
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. ents of personality disorder. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.),
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and Oxford handbook of personality disorders
statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Arling- (pp. 166–185). New York: Oxford University Press.
ton: American Psychiatric Association. Decker, H. S. (2013). The making of DSM-III: A diagnostic
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). DSM-III-R: manual’s conquest of American psychiatry. New York:
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders Oxford University Press.
(3rd ed., rev.) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2006).
Association. A multidimensional neurobehavioral model of person-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and ality disturbance. In R. F. Kreuger & J. L. Tackett (Eds.),
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Personality and psychopathology (pp. 210–261).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. New York: Guildford.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of
statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Wash- the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41,
ington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. 417–440. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221.
Anchin, J. C., & Pincus, A. L. (2010). Evidence-based Freud, S. (1914). On narcissism. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
interpersonal psychotherapy with personality disor- Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psycho-
ders: Theory, components, and strategies. In J. J. logical works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. XIV,
Magnavita (Ed.), Evidence-based treatment of person- pp. 66–102). London: Hogarth Press.
ality dysfunction: Principles, methods, and processes Gunderson, J. G., Stout, R. L., McGlashan, T. H., Shea,
(pp. 113–166). Washington, DC: American Psycholog- M. T., Morey, L. C., Grilo, C. M., . . . Skodol, A. E.
ical Association. (2011). Ten-year course of borderline personality dis-
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of order: Psychopathology and function from the collab-
mentalizing in mental health practice. Arlington: orative longitudinal personality disorders study.
American Psychiatric Publishing. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(8), 827–837. doi:
Beck, A. T., Davis, D. D., & Freeman, A. (Eds.). (2015). 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.37
Cognitive therapy of personality disorders (3rd ed.). Hopwood, C. J., Wright, A. G., Ansell, E. B., & Pincus,
New York: Guilford. A. L. (2013). The interpersonal core of personality
Cadenhead, K. S., Swerdlow, N. R., Shafer, K. M., Diaz, pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27(3),
M., & Braff, D. L. (2000). Modulation of the startle 270–295. doi:10.1521/pedi.2013.27.3.270.
response and startle laterality in relatives of schizo- Hwu, H. G., Yeh, E. K., & Chang, L. Y. (1989). Prevalence
phrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal per- of psychiatric disorders in Taiwan defined by the Chi-
sonality disorder: Evidence of inhibitory deficits. nese Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Acta Psychiatrica
American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 1660–1668. Scandinavica, 79(2), 136–147. doi:10.1111/j.1600-
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1660. 0447.1989.tb08581.x.
Cain, N. M., & Pincus, A. L. (2016). Treating maladaptive Johnson, J. G., Bromley, E., Bornstein, R. F., & Sneed,
interpersonal signatures. In W. J. Livesley, G. S. J. R. (2006). Adolescent personality disorders. In D. A.
Dimaggio, & J. F. Clarkin (Eds.), Integrated treatment Wolfe & E. J. Mash (Eds.), Behavioral and emotional
of personality disorder (pp. 305–324). New York: disorders in children and adolescents: Nature, assess-
Guilford. ment, and treatment (pp. 463–484). New York:
Coccaro, E. F., Siever, L. J., Klar, H. M., Maurer, G., Guildford.
Cochrane, K., Cooper, T. B., . . . Davis, K. L. (1989).
16 Personality Disorder

Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., Skodol, A. E., & Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral treatment of
Oldham, J. M. (2008). Cumulative prevalence of per- borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford.
sonality disorders between adolescence and adulthood. Lungu, A., & Linehan, M. M. (2016). Dialectical behav-
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 118(5), 410–413. ioral therapy: A comprehensive multi-and trans-
doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01231.x. diagnostic intervention. In C. Maguth Nezu & A. M.
Joyce, P. R., Rogers, G. R., Miller, A. L., Mulder, R. T., Nezu (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cognitive and behav-
Luty, S. E., & Kennedy, M. A. (2003). Polymorphisms ioral therapies (pp. 200–214). New York: Oxford Uni-
of DRD4 and DRD3 and risk of avoidant and obsessive versity Press.
personality traits and disorders. Psychiatry Research, Mathew, S. J., Coplan, J. D., & Gorman, J. M. (2001).
119(1-2), 1–10. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00124-0. Neurobiological mechanisms of social anxiety disor-
Kendler, K. S., Czajkowski, N., Tambs, K., Torgersen, S., der. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10),
Aggen, S. H., Neale, M. C., & Reichborn-Kjennerud, 1558–1567. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1558.
T. (2006). Dimensional representations of DSM-IV McWilliams, N. (2011). Psychoanalytic diagnosis: Under-
cluster A personality disorders in a population-based standing personality structure in the clinical process
sample of Norwegian twins: A multivariate study. Psy- (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
chological Medicine, 36(11), 1583–1591. doi:10.1017/ Mitropoulou, V., Harvey, P. D., Maldari, L. A., Moriarty,
S0033291706008609. P. J., New, A. S., Silverman, J. M., & Siever, L. J.
Kendler, K. S., Aggen, S. H., Czajkowski, N., Røysamb, (2002). Neuropsychological performance in
E., Tambs, K., Torgersen, S., . . . Reichborn-Kjennerud, schizotypal personality disorder: Evidence regarding
T. (2008). The structure of genetic and environmental diagnostic specificity. Biological Psychiatry, 52(12),
risk factors for DSM-IV personality disorders: 1175–1182. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01426-9.
A multivariate twin study. Archives of General Psychi- Morey, L. C., Skodol, A. E., & Oldham, J. M. (2014).
atry, 65(12), 1438–1446. doi: 10.1001/ Clinician judgments of clinical utility: A comparison
archpsyc.65.12.1438 of DSM-IV-TR personality disorders and the alterna-
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders: Psy- tive model for DSM-5 personality disorders. Journal of
chotherapeutic strategies. New Haven: Yale University Abnormal Psychology, 123(2), 398–405. doi:10.1037/
Press. a0036481.
Kiesler, D. J. (1988). Therapeutic metacommunication: Mulder, R. T. (2012). Cultural aspects of personality dis-
Therapist impact disclosure in psychotherapy. Palo order. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Oxford handbook of
Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. personality disorders (pp. 260–274). New York:
Koenigsberg, H. W., Siever, L. J., Lee, H., Pizzarello, S., Oxford University Press.
New, A. S., Goodman, M., . . . Prohovnik, I. (2009). New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Goodman,
Neural correlates of emotion processing in borderline M., Mitelman, S. A., Newmark, R., . . . Siever, L. J.
personality disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimag- (2007). Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in border-
ing, 172(3), 192–199. doi: 10.1016/j. line personality disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology,
pscychresns.2008.07.010 32(7), 1629–1640. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301283.
Krueger, R. F. (2013). Personality disorders are the van- Nunes, P. M., Wenzel, A., Borges, K. T., Porto, C. R.,
guard of the post-DSM-5.0 era. Personality Disorders: Caminha, R. M., & Reis, D. O. (2009). Volumes of
Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4(4), 355–362. the hippocampus and amygdala in patients with bor-
doi:10.1037/per0000028. derline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal
Krueger, R. F., Skodol, A. E., Livesley, W. J., Shrout, P. E., of Personality Disorders, 23(4), 333–345. doi:10.1521/
& Huang, Y. (2007). Synthesizing dimensional and pedi.2009.23.4.333.
categorical approaches to personality disorders: Refin- Paris, J. (2004). Sociocultural factors in treatment of per-
ing the research agenda for DSM-V Axis sonality disorders. In J. Magnavita (Ed.), Handbook of
II. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric personality disorders: Theory and practice
Research, 16(S1), 65–73. doi:10.1002/mpr.212. (pp. 135–147). New York: Wiley.
Kumari, V., Das, M., Zachariah, E., Ettinger, U., & Sharma, Paris, J. (2011). Pharmacological treatments for personality
T. (2005). Reduced prepulse inhibition in unaffected disorders. International Review of Psychiatry, 23,
siblings of schizophrenia patients. Psychophysiology, 303–309. doi:10.3109/09540261.2011.586993.
42(5), 588–594. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00346.x. Paris, J. (2012). Pathology of personality disorder: An
Kupfer, D. J., First, M. B., & Regier, D. A. (Eds.). (2002). A integrative conceptualization. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.),
research agenda for DSM-V. Washington, DC: Ameri- Oxford handbook of personality disorders
can Psychiatric Association. (pp. 399–406). New York: Oxford University Press.
Levitt, J. Westin, C., Nestor, P., Estepar, R., Dickey, C., Pincus, A. L. (2005). A contemporary integrative interper-
Voglmaier, M., . . . Shenton, M. E. (2004). Shape of the sonal theory of personality disorders. In M. F.
caudate nucleus and its cognitive correlates in Lenzenweger & J. F. Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of
neuroleptic-naïve schizotypal personality disorder. Bio- personality disorder (2nd ed., pp. 282–331).
logical Psychiatry, 55(2), 177–184. doi: 10.1016/j. New York: Guilford.
biopsych.2003.08.005.
Personality Disorder 17

Pincus, A. L. (2011). Some comments on nomology, diag- Schneider, K. (1950). Systematic psychiatry. The Ameri-
nostic process, and narcissistic personality disorder in can Journal of Psychiatry, 107, 334–335. doi:10.1176/
the DSM-5 proposal for personality and personality ajp.107.5.334.
disorders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, Schnell, K., Dietrich, T., Schnitker, R., Daumann, J., &
and Treatment, 2(1), 41–53. doi:10.1037/a0021191. Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Processing of autobiographical
Pincus, A. L., & Cain, N. M. (2008). Interpersonal psycho- memory retrieval cues in borderline personality disor-
therapy. In D. C. S. Richard & S. K. Huprich (Eds.), der. Journal of Affective Disorders, 97(1), 253–259.
Clinical psychology: Assessment, treatment, and doi:10.1016/j.jad.2006.05.035.
research (pp. 213–245). San Diego: Academic. Shihabuddin, L., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A.,
Pincus, A. L., & Hopwood, C. J. (2012). A contemporary Silverman, J., New, A., Brickman, A. M., Mitropoulou,
interpersonal model of personality pathology and per- V., Nunn, M., Fleischman, M. B., Tang, C., & Siever,
sonality disorder. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Oxford hand- L. J. (2001). Striatal size and relative glucose metabolic
book of personality disorders (pp. 372–398). rate in schizotypal personality disorder and schizophre-
New York: Oxford University Press. nia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(9), 877–884.
Pincus, A. L., Dowgwillo, E. A., & Greenberg, L. S. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.9.877.
(2016). Three cases of narcissistic personality disorder Siever, L. J., & Davis, K. L. (2004). The pathophysiology
through the lens of the DSM-5 alternative model for of schizophrenia disorders: Perspectives from the spec-
personality disorders. Practice Innovations, 1(3), trum. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(3),
164–177. doi:10.1037/pri0000025. 398–413. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.398.
Pretzer, J. L., & Beck, A. T. (2005). A cognitive theory of Skodol, A. E. (2012). Personality disorders in DSM-5.
personality disorders. In M. F. Lenzenweger & J. F. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 317–344.
Clarkin (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorder doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143131.
(2nd ed., pp. 43–113). New York: Guilford. South, S. C., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Eaton, N. R., &
Reich, W. (1949). Character-analysis (3rd ed.). Oxford: Krueger, R. F. (2012). Behavior and molecular genetics
Orgone Institute Press. of personality disorders. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Oxford
Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Czajkowski, N., Torgersen, S., handbook of personality disorders (pp. 143–165).
Neale, M. C., Ørstavik, R. E., Tambs, K., & Kendler, New York: Oxford University Press.
K. S. (2007). The relationship between avoidant per- Stein, D. J., Trestman, R. L., Mitropoulou, V., Coccaro,
sonality disorder and social phobia: A population- E. F., Hollander, E., & Siever, L. J. (1996). Impulsivity
based twin study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, and serotonergic function in compulsive personality
164(11), 1722–1728. doi:10.1176/appi. disorder. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
ajp.2007.06101764. Neurosciences, 8(4), 393–398. doi:10.1176/
Ripoll, L. H., Triebwasser, J., & Siever, L. J. (2011). jnp.8.4.393.
Evidence-based pharmacotherapy for personality dis- Torgersen, S. (2012). Epidemiology. In T. A. Widiger
orders. International Journal of Neuropsychophar- (Ed.), Oxford handbook of personality disorders
macology, 14, 1257–1288. doi:10.1017/ (pp. 186–205). New York: Oxford University Press.
S1461145711000071. Torgersen, S., Czajkowski, N., Jacobson, K., Reichborn-
Rosell, D. R., Zaluda, L. C., McClure, M. M., Perez- Kjennerud, T., Røysamb, E., Neale, M. C., & Kendler,
Rodriguez, M., Strike, K. S., Barch, D. M., . . . Siever, K. S. (2008). Dimensional representations of DSM-IV
L. J. (2015). Effects of the D1 dopamine receptor ago- cluster B personality disorders in a population-based
nist dihydrexidine (DAR-0100A) on working memory sample of norwegian twins: A multivariate study. Psy-
in schizotypal personality disorder. Neuropsychophar- chological Medicine, 38(11), 1617–1625. doi:10.1017/
macology, 40(2), 446–453. doi: 10.1038/ S0033291708002924.
npp.2014.192. Trull, T. J., Scheiderer, E. M., & Tomko, R. L. (2012). Axis
Roussos, P., & Siever, L. J. (2012). Neurobiological con- II comorbidity. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Oxford hand-
tributions. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), Oxford handbook of book of personality disorders (pp. 219–236).
personality disorders (pp. 299–324). New York: New York: Oxford University Press.
Oxford University Press. Waugh, M., Hopwood, C. J., Krueger, R. F., Morey, L. C.,
Rutter, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Capsi, A. (2006). Gene- Pincus, A. L., & Wright, A. G. C. (2017). Psychological
environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple assessment with the DSM-5 alternative model for per-
varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychiatry sonality disorders: Tradition and innovation. Professional
and Psychology, 47, 226–261. doi:10.1111/j.1469- Psychology: Research and Practice, 48(2), 79–89.
7610.2005.01557.x. doi:10.1037/pro0000071.
Ryder, A. G., Sunohara, M., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2015). Westen, D., Shedler, J., Durrett, C., Glass, S., & Martens,
Culture and personality disorder: From a fragmented A. (2003). Personality diagnoses in adolescence: DSM-
literature to a contextually grounded alternative. Cur- IV axis II diagnoses and an empirically derived alter-
rent Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(1), 40–45. doi:10.1097/ native. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(5),
YCO.0000000000000120. 952–966. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.952.
18 Personality Disorder

Widiger, T. A., & Simonsen, E. (2005). Alternative dimen- Young, J. E., Klosko, S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003).
sional models of personality disorder: Finding a com- Schema therapy: A practitioner’s guide. New York:
mon ground. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19(2), Guildford.
110–130. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.2.110.62628. Zachar, P., Krueger, R. F., & Kendler, K. S. (2016). Per-
Wright, A. G. C., Pincus, A. L., & Lenzenweger, M. F. sonality disorder in DSM-5: An oral history. Psycho-
(2011). Development of personality and the remission logical Medicine, 46(1), 1–10. doi:10.1017/
and onset of personality pathology. Journal of Person- S0033291715001543.
ality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1351–1358. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Hennen, J., & Silk,
doi:10.1037/a0025557. K. R. (2003). The longitudinal course of borderline
Yeomans, F. E., Clarkin, J. F., & Kernberg, O. F. (2015). psychopathology: 6-year prospective follow-up of the
Transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline phenomenology of borderline personality disorder.
personality disorder: A clinical guide. Washington, American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(2), 274–283.
DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.2.274.
Young, J. E. (1994). Cognitive therapy for personality Zimmerman, M., Rothschild, L., & Chelminski, I. (2005).
disorders: A schema focused approach (rev. ed.). Sar- The prevalence of DSM-IV personality disorders in
asota: Professional Resource Press. psychiatric outpatients. The American Journal of Psy-
chiatry, 162(10), 1911–1918. doi:10.1176/appi.
ajp.162.10.1911.

View publication stats

You might also like