You are on page 1of 5

Frequency of Usage and the Perception of Words

Author(s): Mark R. Rosenzweig and Leo Postman


Source: Science , Feb. 7, 1958, New Series, Vol. 127, No. 3293 (Feb. 7, 1958), pp. 263-266
Published by: American Association for the Advancement of Science

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/1756116

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/1756116?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

American Association for the Advancement of Science is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Science

This content downloaded from


164.73.236.7 on Fri, 11 Aug 2023 19:38:49 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
7 }'et)ruary 1958) Volurne 127, Nurnbet 3293 SCIENCE

It had been pointed out earlier that


words employed in a list will be more
intelligible if they cannot readily be con-
fused with one another.

Frequency of Usage and t} e


Measurement of Intelligibility 7

Perception of Worz ds All the studies from which these re-


sults come have used the same basic
technique to measure the intelligibility
A{ark R. Rosenzweig and Leo Postv nan of words: The words are presented one
by one in the presence of a masking
noise, and listeners attempt to identify
each word. The relative intensity of
the noise to the words is varied systemati-
Telephoning in London with a poor only in audition but i in other sensory cally S that is, the sound intensity of the
connection, Mr. Sedgwick spells his modalities as well. In f Fact, some of the words is held constant, but the intensity
name, identifying each letter with a same investigators have also studied the of the noise is changed from one part of
word: "S as in 'sugar,' E as in 'Edward,' visual recognition of v vords, and com- the experiment to another. (In some ex-
D as in 'David,' G as in 'George,' W as parison of rcsults in au dition and vision periments, the intensity of the noise is
in 'William,' I as in 'Isaac,' C as in 'Char- will help to show what is general in the held constant, and the intensity of the
lie," K as in 'king.' " (The words he perception of words an Ld what is pecul- speech is varied.) The relative intensity
uses are suggested in "Aids toward clear- iar to one sensory moc lality. Studies of of the noise at which listeners can just
ness of speech" in the front of the Lon- the perception of words have broad theo- hear a word gives a measure of intelligi-
don telephone directory. ) retical as well as pracl tical interest be- bility, or a recognition threshold for that
Monsieur Dumartin, telephoning un- cause they illustrate- so me of the ways word. To mask some words may require
der similar conditions in Paris, ldentifies in which past experienc e influences per- 20 decibels more noise than to mask
* * * S

the letters of his name in this way: "D ceptual c lscrlmlnatlon. others. The importance of this difference
comme 'Desire,' U comme 'Ursule,' M Since 1950, three m rin findings have may be gauged by noting that the noise
comme 'Marcel,' A comme 'Anatole,' been reported. We sha 11 list them here of a pneumatic drill (or of a noisy auto)
R comme 'Raoul,' T comme 'Therese,' and then consider hov v they were ob- is about 20 decibels more intense than
I comme 'Irma,' N comme 'Nicolas'." tained and what they imply: the level of ordinary conversation; these
(These alphabetic equivalents are in the 1 ) A listener can ide ntify words more two levels are respectively about 80 and
list given in the Paris telephone direc- accurately if he know zs the list from 60 decibels more intense than those of
tory. ) which the words will be chosen. The the weakest sounds that normal ears can
In both cases, the procedure works shorter the list, the mo] re intelligible the -hear. The fact that one word can be
and the name is recognized. The alpha- words will be ( 1 ) . Evil dently, then, any heard over the noise of a pneumatic
betic equivalents are more intelligible list of alphabetic equis zalents would be drill while another word is just intelli-
than the surnames or the letters. Such an aid to intelligibilit: y, provided only gible over a background of conversation
examples have intrigued psychologist:s that the speaker and lis stener agree upon does not mean that the two words differ
who are interested in perception. What them. (How much of a n aid an arbitrar- greatly in intelligibility in the quiet. The
makes some words more intelligible thall ily chosen list would be, , we will consider measure tells only how well a word can
others? How should groups of srords be later) . overcome the effects of masking noise.
chosen for maximum intelligibility? 2) The greater the r elative frequency It is of course precisely where there is
Within the past few years, several psy- of usage of a word in t the language, the noise in the office, on the street, over
chologists (most of them alumni of the more intelligible it tendc s to be (2). Thus, the telephone, in the presence of other
Psycho-Acoustic Laboratory at Harvard in writing and speaking we use the word conversations that we are concerned
University ) have conducted studies to age about a hundred t times more often about intelligibility, so the measure of
find what factors determine intelligibil- than the word cud, anl d tests show that resistance to masking noise is a useful
* .

ity. The results have general significance age 1S reCOgNlZeC . CO: nsiderably more olae.
for our understanding of perception, not readily than cud. To select words with regard to their
3) The greater the l length of a word, frequency of usage in English, the Lorge
the more intelligible it t
The authors are on the staff of the department tends to be, when magazine count (4) has generally been
of psychology, University of California, Berkeley. frequency of usage is h
eld constant (3). used. This is a tabulation of 4.5 million
7 FEBRUARY 1958
263

This content downloaded from


164.73.236.7 on Fri, 11 Aug 2023 19:38:49 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
> o-. . o
words from 19291938 issues of five words-pronounced correctly are near of any group of alphabetic equivalents
popular American magazines. No large- zero for English listeners. Similarly, brings the length of the list to 26. Esti-
scale count of oral usage in English is French listeners would perform poorly mates indicate that, with no list avail-
available, but the magazine-count fre- on the English list. We have even found able, listeners respond in terms of a set
quencies correlate fairly highly ( about that American university students whose of alternatives of the order of 30,000
0.75) with estimates of oral usage of col- maternal language was not English score words. Thus an arbitrary selection of al-
lege students. In French, a count of oral significantly lower on our intelligibility phabetic equivalents reduces the alter-
usage has recently been completed (5), test than do students whose original lan- natives by 1000-fold and can be expected,
and we have used it in a study of intel- guage was English. Mr. Sedgwick and on the average, to improve intelligibility
ligibility as a function of frequency in M. Dumartin clearly require separate by about 12 decibels.
French. lists of alphabetic equivalents while tele- Since the selection of an arbitrary
phoning within their own countries. For group of alphabetic equivalents would
international telephonic communication be expected to improve intelligibility, it
Frequency of Usage
a special list of alphabetic equivalents may be asked why the names of the let-
has been worked out, a list consisting ot ters themselves are not satisfactory
Now let us consider the results ob- common geographical terms whose pro- enough. In favor of the letter names, it
tained when intelligibility is measured as nunciations are reasonably similar in could be added that many of them are
a function of frequency of usage. The Western European languages. homonyms of frequently used words such
masked thresholds show that, for each I -i,
as a, be, and see. Letter names have one
tenfold increase in frequency of usage,
Fet .d

distinct disadvantage, however: Many of


about 4 decibels more of masking noise Restriction of Alternatives
them are readily confused with each
can be overcome. Figure 1 shows this other. The confusions occur because
relationship for monosyllabic English The effects of changing the length of many of the letter names have the same
words. The most frequent words used in the list of test words have also been de- vowel sound, the largest group sharing
the experiment, shown at the right of the termined experimentally. For a tenfold one vowel sound being the nine letters
graph, can be heard over about 12 deci- increase in list length, intelligibility of B, C, D, E, G, P, T, V, and Z. The sound
bels more noise than can the least fre- the words falls about 4 decibels. This power of a word is concentrated chiefly
quent words used. A similar experiment indicates that a word does not have an in the vowels, while the consonants are
was performed with French words and absolute intelligibility. The listener can relatively weak. Consequently, it is diE-
French listeners, and similar results were recognize a word more readily when it cult in the presence of noise to distin-
obtained; thus the effect of frequency on is one of two alternatives than when it guish words that differ in their consonant
intelligibility seems to be a general one. is one of twenty. Thus the word is per- sounds but not in their vowels.
English listeners would of course be ceived in terms of the alternatives avail-
expected to do poorly on the French list, able, as the mathematical theory of in-
since the frequencies of usage of French formation would predict. The selection
Length of Word

The influence of word length on in-


telligibility can be interpreted as a spe-
10
cial case of the eSects of length of list.
When frequency is held constant, intel-
ligibility increases steadily with word

5 length. This beneficial effect of length


is found even up to the longest words
tested-words of 21 letters. In inter-
- preting this length eSect, Howes (3) hqs
suggested that the listener can discrimi-
nate the length of a worzl even if he
Apt
.O V / does not recognize the word correctly
The population from which the test
c -5 Pew- \ *Nrord could come would thus be reducecl

* a/ * - to words of the discriminated length. If


this is so, then we have once more a
Dram / . case where the number of alternative;
has been restricted. Since there .'.F:9 many
I (. . * -10
j\,. * < short words but fewer long ones, the re-
striction of alternatives would be greater
for, say, an 11-letter erord than for a
-15 1 * -15
3-letter word. 0n thc basis of relative
1 1() 100 1,000 10,,000
frequencies of words of each length irl
Frequency of Usoge
the Lorge magazine count, Howes has
Fig. 1. Intelligibility of monosyllabic English words increases as a function of their £re-
calculated the degree to which nword
quency of usage. The intelligibility scale indicates the difference in decibels between the
length might be expected to aid intelli-
intensity of the masking noise and the intensity of the word when the word is just intel
ligible Each dot represents one of the words used in the experiment, and four typical gibility, and his calculated values fit his
words are identified. The figure is modified from Rosenzweig (2). data wellO

264 SCIENCEs VOL. 127

This content downloaded from


164.73.236.7 on Fri, 11 Aug 2023 19:38:49 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
.|> o'
words masked by noise or words flashed
briefly on the screen. The thresholds for
correct responses in the two sensory mo-
dalities could not, of course, be com-
pared since one cannot measure masking
noise and flash duration in the same
terms. Taking only incorrect responses,
we found that subj ects tended to give
complete, three-letter responses in the
auditory case but that they tended to
give incomplete one- or two-letter re-

::S f 0 020 0
sponses in the visual case. The responses
to auditory stimuli tended to be made
in units of a word or syllable; the re-
sponses to visual stimuli tended to be
made in terms of letters or pairs of let-
ters. If a perceived fragment of an audi-
tory stimulus tends to be completed in
the response, we would expect that, the
longer the word, the greater the chance
that the listener will perceive at least
part of it and then complete the rest. In
vision, however, the longer the word, the
greater the chance that part of it will not
be perceived and that the report will
then be incomplete. It should also be
Fig. 2. A three-dimensional representation of visibility of words as a function of both
noted that length is penalized by the
frequency and word length. The sloping upper surface of the figure represents mean
thresholds, greater height indicating greater visibility. The greater the frequency of the fixed time of tachistoscopic presentation
words, the less long they need be exposed to be seen, and thus the greater their visibility. in a way that does not occur in auditory
Visibility decreases with word length, except for the most frequent words. (The values presentation. In tachistoscopic presenta-
on the vertical scale equal 100 minus exposure duration in milliseconds.) The figure was
tion, words of different length are com-
prepared from data of Postman and Adis-Castro (9).
pared at the same durations of exposure,
and there is not time to shift the fixation
of the eye as in ordinary reading. The
At this point a comparison of auditory quency, greater length impairs ease of greater the number of units to be dis-
and visual experiments is instructive. perception, as shown by the upLvard criminated during a single exposure, the
Experiments similar to the auditory ones slope from back to front at the left of more rapidly must each be discrimi-
have been performed with tachistoscopic the figure. Figure 3 shows the effects of nated. In auditory presentation, each
perception of words. In this case the frequency and word length in audition. word is pronounced at a normal rate,
w-ords are flashed on a screen for expo- Here, too, a higher level indicates greater
sures of perhaps 1/100 second and the ease of perception. The frequency effect
observer attempts to perceive and record is similar to that in vision the threshold
them. Thresholds are determined by surface slopes upward from left to right
varying either the duration or the in- as frequency increases. But, contrary to
tensity of the flash exposure. Here, too, the case in vision, intelligibility increases
there is a word-frequency effect; in fact, with word length-the surface slopes
it was discovered earlier in vision than doznLvard from back to front-for both
it was in audition. The more frequently frequent and infrequent words.
a word is used in the language, the more The fact that word length has oppo-
readily it can be seen (6). But in vision, site effects in audition and vision requires
contrary to the case in audition, the that we seek for differences between the
longer the word is, the less chance there auditory and visual cases. (Howes' for-
is that the observer will report it cor- mulation, as stated, would seem to be
rectly (7). The combined effects of fre- just as applicable to vision as to audi-
Fig. 3. Intelligibility as a function of fre-
quency and word length on the visual tion, but it clearly would not predict quency of usage and word length. The
threshold are shown in Fig. 2. The correctly for visual perception.) The di- sloping upper surface represents mean
threshold is indicated by the level of the vergent effects of word length in audi- thresholds. With increases in both fre-
sloping surface: The higher the level, quency of usage and word length, words
tion and vision may be explained partly
can be heard over greater intensities of
the more readily the items can be seen. in terms of differences in tendencies to masking noise. (The values on the vertical
The fact that greater frequency favors give complete responses to auditory and scale equal 20 plus intensity of masking
perception is shown by the upward slope visual stimuli. In two experiments, one noise in decibels minus intensity of the
of the surface from left to right. Length words in decibels. ) The numbered loca-
auditory and the other visual, we em-
tions on the surface are discussed in the
has negligible effects when word fre- ployed the same list of three-letter words
final section of this article. The figure was
quency is high. With words of lower fre- ( 8 ) . The subj ects had to write down prepared from data of EIowes (3).

7 FEBRUARY 1958 265

This content downloaded from


164.73.236.7 on Fri, 11 Aug 2023 19:38:49 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
and
andlonger
longerwords
words
conseqtlently
conseqtlently are English
are giverl giverltelephorle list (Andrew, Benja- 19 of the changes substituted items of
proportionately
proportionately longer
longer
presentations. The min,
presentations. The(:harlie, David, and so on) have greater length for the original words.
rate
rateofof
discrimination
discrimination
then need
thennot
need an average
varynot vary length of 5.7 letters and an This improved list, which was nevcr
with the length of the word. average frequency of 254 on the Lorge adopted, had an average length of 6 4
magazine count. These values determine letters and an average frequency of 56.
the location labelled 1 near the lower Length was gained, though with some
Habits and Performance Factors right corner of the surface. Words of loss in frequency.
in the Perception of Words the joint U.S.-British radiotelephone Now that the effects of frequency and
list (the familiar Able, Baker, Charlie, of length on intelligibility have been
Vision and audition share the lreneS- Dog, and so on) have an average length made explicit, it should be possible to
cial effects of frequency of past usage of 4.1 letters and an average frequency choose items from a pool of words that
and of restriction of alternatives upon of 355; this group centers around the 2 are both frequent and long. The dia
ease of perception of words. Frequency on the surface. (In both cases the indi- gram, of course, presents only average
of past usage may be considered to dew vidual words scatter rather widely thresholds. Within any region, some
velop habits of responding to a particu- arourtd the average values, considerably etords are more intelligible and some are
lar stimulus with a particular response. more widely than the cross-hatching in- less intelligible than would be predicted
The more frequently a word has been dicates.) Now these lists were not delib- on the basis of their frequency and
used in the past, the more readily it is erately selected to be on the lower right length. Empirical tests will therefore be
recognizedO The restriction of alterna corner of the diagram, because the studo required to determine whether the most
tives in a particular situation operates ies on effects of frequency of usage and intelligible items from the pool of longer
differently. It does not change the per- word length had not yet been done when words are clearly superior to the most
ceiver's habits, which are based on his the lists were prepared. The main cri° intelligible of the shorter words. If they
history of past usage. Rather it is a terion employed was that the words be are, we may find Mr. Sedgwick spelling
"performance factor" which influences common, familiar ones. In the case of his name on a future occasion in this
the use made of already formed habits. the U.S.-British list, empirical tests were way: ';S as in Cstudent,) E as in Cexami-
The smaller the number of alternatives used to choose highly intelligible words nation,) D as in 'department,' G as in
included in the set of words expected by from a pool of familiar items. The use sgrandmother,' W as in 'welcome,' I as
the perceiver, the more ready he can be of the criterion of familiarity leads to in Cindustry,' C as in 'companion K as
to use each of the corresponding habits. words of high frequency and low length, in 6kindness.' "

Thus both frequency of usage and re- since familiarity implies high frequency
References
striction of alternatives make it easier and since, in turn, frequent words tend
1. G. A. Miller, G. A. Heise, D. Lichten, J.
to give a complete response to a stimuhls to be shortO It should be noted, however, Exptl. Psychol, 41, 329 (1951)
word on the basis of fragmentary dis- that although frequently used words tend 2. M. R. Rosenzweig, Cahiers d>Etudes Radio-
Television 12 283 (1956)
to be short, there are many frequent
. . .

crlmlnatlons.
3* D. Howes, J. Acoust. Socg Am. 29, 296 (1957)
words of considerable length. The figure 4e E. L. Thorndike and I. Lorge, The Teacher's
indicates that by selecting longer words Word BooAc of 30,000 Words (Teachers Collegeg
Columbia Univ., New York, 1944)*
A Practical Suggestion
toward the upper right corner of the sur- 5e G. Gougenheim, P. Rivenc, R. Michea A.
face, the region labelled 3, a gain of over Sauvageot, L'elaboration du fran f ais e'letmen-
taire (Didier, Paris, 1956)
Our survey of the factors governing 6 decibels in resistance to noise might 6. D. Howes and R. L. Solomon, J. Exptl. Psy-
intelligibility suggests a way in which be achievede In fact, wartime tests of in- chol. 41 401 (1951)e f
7 E. McGitlnies, P. B. Comer) 0. L. Lacey, ibid.
more highly intelligible alphabetic equiv- telligibility demonstrated that many of 44n 65 (1952)
alents might be selected. First let us see the items of the U.S.-British list could 8. L. Postman and M. R. Rosenzweig, Am. J.
Psychol. 69, 209 ( 1956 ) *
where the current alphabetic equivalents be improved. Twenty-two changes were 94 L. Postman and G. Adis-Castro, Science 125
are located on Fig. 3. The words of the suggested on the basis of empirical tests; 193 (1957)

over a thousand analyses of human bone


and interprets these data in terms of
present concentrations of strontium-90 in
the various critical phases of the gco-
sphere and biosphere. The new data per-
Strontium-90 in Man II mit a closer definition of the average
concentration of strontium-90 in a large
part of the human race, the geograph-
Walter R. Eckelmann, J. Laurence Kulp, Arthur R. Schulert ical and dietary variation, the increase
irl the concentration with time) and the
distribution in urban populations. From
the existing data) an attempt is made to
ne contlnuatlon ot nuc. .ear tests first data on a world-wide analysis of
T. . P

predict future levels under spec;Sed con-


makes a thorough understanding of the strontium-90 in human tissue were pre- ditions.
movement and uptake of strontium-90 a sented. The study of the geochemical
necessity while the levels in man and his distribution of strontium-90, of its trans- Dr. Kulp and Dr. Schulert are OI1 the staff of
the Lamont Geological Observatory (Columbia
environment are still relatively small fer through the food chain, and of its University), Palisades, N.Y. Dr. Eckelmaml, for-
compared with natural background radi- variation in human populations has con- merly a member of the staff of the Lamont Ge-
ological Observatory, is now at the Carter Re-
aticon. Irl a previous publication (1) the tinued (2). This article (3) summarizes search Laboratories, Tulsa, Oklac

266 SCIENCE, VOL. 127

This content downloaded from


164.73.236.7 on Fri, 11 Aug 2023 19:38:49 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like