You are on page 1of 13

Jouroti of Penootlity and SocuU Piyctutcgy Copyr*u 1984 by the

19(4, Vol 47, No 4, 780-792 American Plycholopcil Aaorimon, Inc.

Some Antecedents and Consequences of


Social-Comparison Jealousy
Peter Salovey and Judith Rodin
Yale University

In the present study we explored some antecedents and consequences of social-


comparison jealousy (traditionally termed envy). Eighty college undergraduates
were randomly assigned to one of eight experimental conditions in which they
received either positive or negative feedback on a bogus personality test that was
either self-definitionaUy relevant or irrelevant, followed by feedback of successful
performance by another person in a domain that was either relevant or irrelevant
to the subject. Significantly greater jealousy of the other person was reported in
the condition in which the subject received negative feedback regarding own
performance on a self-involving characteristic, and in which the successful
performance of the other was on the same characteristic. Subjects in this condition
were more likely to disparage the rival and less likely to desire his or her
friendship. In addition, these subjects tended to feel more depressed and anxious
about interacting with the comparison person.

We envy those whose acquisitions and successful efforts We are using the terms envy and jealousy
are a reproach to us. (Aristotle, c. 340 B.C./1932, p. 128) interchangeably. Whether there is a distinction
When we compare ourselves to others and between envy and jealousy has been the cause
find that we do not measure up, we may for much philosophical debate (cf. Bryson,
experience envy and jealousy. Yet, although 1977; Gellert, 1976; Neu, 1980; Sabini &
such experiences are often implied in the Silver, 1982; Spielman, 1971; Titleman, 1982;
study of social comparison processes, they Tov-Ruach, 1980), albeit little empirical study.
rarely are explicitly investigated in that con- Semantically, a distinction has been made
text. In this study we examined some of the between jealousy and envy. Whereas jealousy
personality and situational antecedents of the refers to the belief or suspicion that a desired
experience of envy and jealousy in social relationship is in danger of being lost, envy
comparison situations as well as their cogni- is a discontent with one's own lot and a
tive and affective consequences. We designed desire for another's attributes, reputation, or
the study in order to investigate the effects of possessions (Bryson, 1977).
feedback that threatens one's self-concept on Bers and Rodin (1984), however, argue that
anxiety, mood, and evaluation of others in a envy and jealousy are words used inter-
social comparison context. From such data, changeably by the lay person, and in many
we can begin to infer the conditions that situations the emotions, cognitions, and be-
promote social-comparison jealousy as well haviors associated with envy and jealousy are
as the resultant thoughts and feelings. not clearly different. They suggest that there
is little heuristic value in distinguishing envy
and jealousy, but argue instead for the utility
Portions of this article were presented as "A Self- of studying the kinds of situations that pro-
Esteem Maintenance Model of Envy" at the Annual mote feelings labeled either envy or jealousy.
Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association in For an initial categorization, based on pilot
Philadelphia in April 1983.
We would like to thank Susan Bers, Janice Marcus,
data, they suggest two kinds ofjealousy. Their
Jefferson Singer, Abe Tessa, and especially William term social-comparison jealousy replaces the
McGuire for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of typical notion of envy, because it involves a
this article. desire for superiority on some dimension.
Requests for reprints should be sent to either Peter Social-relations jealousy involves a desire for
Salovey or Judith Rodin, Department of Psychology, Yale
University, P.Ot Box 11-A Yale Station, New Haven, exclusivity in a relationship. When the rela-
Connecticut 06520-7447. tionship is a romantic one, a specific form of
780
S O O A L P C O M P A R I S O N JEALOUSY 781

social-relations jealousy, called romantic jeal- back on self-evaluation. The major premise
ousy, can be said to exist. The present study of Tesser's theory is that individuals are mo-
focuses on social-comparison jealousy. tivated to maintain a positive self-evaluation.
(In a sense, Tesser's theory is really one of
Antecedents of Social-Comparison Jealousy self-evaluation maximization rather than self-
evaluation maintenance.) Given a situation
Valence of Information About the Self in which another person has possessions that
Relative to Others one desires or performs better than one on
Negative or esteem-diminishing informa- some task, either reflection or comparison
tion about oneself should promote feelings of can result Whereas reflection raises one's
social-comparison jealousy. According to Sil- self-evaluation, comparison lowers it When
ver and Sabini (1978), social-comparison the performance domain is relevant to one's
jealousy will result only when the possessions, self-definition, comparison is likely; reflection
attributes, and attainments of one person occurs when the domain is less relevant (cf.
diminish the status of another person. Such Cialdini et al., 1976). Individuals may actually
superiority of one person over another can try to maintain their self-evaluation, be it
be either ascribed or achieved and, as long as initially high or low, because there is a ten-
the superiority of one person threatens the dency to reject feedback showing that one
self-worth of another, jealousy may still result. has done better than he or she has expected
Silver and Sabini varied the difference in goal (see McGuire, 1966, for a discussion of some
attainment between two actors in videotaped of these "surprised by joy" studies). In the
scenarios and found that the number of present study, however, we are concerned
subjects predicting the display of envy or primarily with the situation in which one
jealousy by the unsuccessful actor varied person's successes threaten to diminish the
directly with the size of this difference. Sim- self-evaluation of another person.
ilarly, Bers and Rodin (1984) observed that There are several corollaries to Tesser's
among elementary school children, social position. First, we tend to be attracted to
comparison and resulting jealousy are com- others who perform well, so long as their
mon responses to failure situations in which superior performances are on dimensions not
another child is superior in some way or highly self-definitional. Second, we envy those
achieves something that is desired by the first who perform highly in self-definitional do-
child. mains because envy occurs when we feel
diminished (our self-evaluation is threatened)
Relevance of Information About the Self by the particularly relevant performances,
possessions, and attributes of another (recall
Although negative information about one- Silver & Sabini, 1978, discussed earlier).
self may result in social-comparison jealousy,
negative feedback that is particularly self- Relevance or Similarity of Successful Others
relevant may exacerbate feelings of jealousy.
Bers and Rodin (1984) found that sponta- Tesser's theorizing about the impact of self-
neous comparisons increased with age as the definitionally relevant social information is
children began to define domains that were tied closely to the final antecedent of social-
particularly self-relevant and increased espe- comparison jealousy examined in this study—
cially in these self-relevant domains. The the relevance of the source of that feedback
older children reported being bothered (an- to oneself. That is, how similar to me is the
gered and saddened) significantly more often successful comparison person? Traditionally,
by comparisons made in a domain that was similarity is the primary variable studied by
important to them. The younger children social comparison researchers (cf. Festinger,
were bothered by any comparison. 1954; Suls, 1977; but see Mettee & Smith,
Tesser's model of self-evaluation mainte- 1977), and it appears that comparisons are
nance (Tesser, 1980; Tesser & Campbell, 1980, most likely made with people who are similar
1982, 1983; Tesser & Smith, 1980) concerns to oneself (e.g., Goethals & Darley, 1977;
the impact of self-definitionally relevant feed- Wheeler et al., 1969). More recently, Dakin
782 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

and Arrowood (1981) showed that compari- social-comparison jealousy. Bryson (1976) had
sons are particularly likely with similar others subjects choose from a list of affective states
performing at levels slightly better than one- those feelings most often experienced as a
self. We would expect that such situations consequence of jealousy. The factors that
are particularly likely to foster social-com- emerged included emotional devastation (de-
parison jealousy. pression and helplessness), reactive retribution
The relationship between the degree of (desire to get even), general arousal, desire
another's superiority and one's experience of for social support, intrapunitiveness, and an-
jealousy is, perhaps, nonmonotonic. On the ger. In a second study (Bryson, 1977), subjects
one hand, the more superior the other is, the reacted with feelings of anger, hurt, embar-
less likely he or she is to become an object rassment, rejection, frustration, and sadness
of social comparison. On the other hand, the to a laboratory simulation of a jealousy sit-
more superior the person who becomes a uation. Bers and Rodin (1984) found anger
social comparison object is, the more jealous at the other most strongly related to jealous
one feels. As a result of these opposing ten- behavior. In addition, state anxiety tends to
dencies, one is perhaps most likely to become correlate positively with self-report measures
jealous of another person who is at an inter- of jealousy (Bringle, 1981; Jaremko & Lind-
mediate level of superiority to oneself. sey, 1979).

The Present Study The Present Study


The three factors discussed earlier were In this study, then, we are interested in
examined as potential antecedents of social- two major classes of consequences of social-
comparison jealousy in the present study. We comparison jealousy: (a) cognitive conse-
hypothesized that such jealousy would be quences such as evaluations of the comparison
most intensely experienced in situations (a) person, desire for his or her friendship, and
containing negative feedback about oneself self-reported envy or jealousy, and (b) affective
(b) in a domain that is particularly self- consequences. We hypothesized that subjects
defining, (c) followed by comparison to an- would manifest social-comparison jealousy
other person who has performed well on this (in the conditions described earlier) by de-
same self-defining dimension rather than on valuing the comparison person, not desiring
another dimension. future friendship with him or her, identifying
this state as one of envy or jealousy, and
Consequences of Social-Comparison feeling anxious and depressed at the prospect
Jealousy of having to engage in any future interaction
with this person.
Degrading the Comparison Person
Silver and Sabini (1978) note that we Theoretical Framework
perceive another person as envious or jealous We can summarize two primary theoretical
when we see him or her inappropriately impetuses of the present study. The first is
attempt to demean someone in order to Silver and Sabini's (1978) notion that envy
maintain his or her own self-worth. In their (i.e., social-comparison jealousy) can be said
videotape scenario study, subjects ascribed to exist only when, following negative com-
more envy and jealousy to the unsuccessful parison, the injured party attempts to deni-
actor when he or she acted inappropriately, grate the rival. In the present experiment, we
inconsiderately, or with insufficient restraint were able to examine this process directly.
and respect toward the successful actor. It The second influence is Tesser's model of
follows that the denning quality of social- self-evaluation maintenance. We argue that
comparison jealousy should be degrading of one aspect of social-comparison jealousy de-
the comparison person. rives from the need to maintain self-esteem.
Depressed Mood and Anxiety Hence the processes that Tesser described
form an integral underpinning of social-com-
There has been little research on the affec- parison jealousy; however, we will expand on
tive consequences of situations that provoke Tesser's notions by directly examining some
SOCIAL-COMPARISON JEALOUSY 783

of the affective and behavioral consequences sheet from the Minnesota Multiphase Personality Inven-
of self-esteem maintenance (such as depres- tory (MMPI). The experimenter told the subjects that he
had a few more personality scales for them to fill out
sion, anxiety, and degradation of others) not ("in order to cross-validate the RWPP") and placed in
made explicit in Tesser's model. front of them a packet containing a 4-item positive/
negative mood scale, a 12-item self-concept scale, and
the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory. Before the subjects
Method began working on these new scales, the experimenter
Subjects asked them if they would be interested in seeing some of
their results on the personality profile they had taken
Eighty-two undergraduates in introductory psychology earlier. The experimenter then produced the MMPI-like
courses who had identified themselves as having a career score report, which contained the profiles of two subjects,
interest in the medical sciences, business, or the perform- one plotted in red ink and the other in black. Subjects
ing arts participated in this experiment Forty of these were informed that the red line represented the results
subjects were men and 42 were women; 28 indicated an of their profile and that the black line reflected the scores
interest in medicine, 35 in business, and 17 in the of the subject in the next room. The experimenter then
performing arts. The data from two women were elimi- gave the subjects a short lecture on the subscales of the
nated from the analysis because the women were suspi- RWPP (e.g., medical science aptitude, business acumen,
cious of the veracity of the false feedback manipulation. performing arts sensitivity) and on the "norms" for each
Of the remaining 80 subjects, 5 men and 5 women were subscale. The experimenter then went over the subjects'
randomly assigned to each of the 8 conditions of the profiles, which were always depicted as slightly above
experiment in a 2 X 2 X 2 factorial design involving the average on all subscales but one. On this one scale,
three independent variables to be described. Subjects subjects were shown as either well below or well above
with each of the three career interests were distributed average (depending on condition). The black line, repre-
approximately equally across conditions. senting the results of the subject-next-door, was also
depicted as slightly above average on all subscales but
one. On this one subscale, the other person was always
Procedure presented as well above the average. To make certain that
Subjects arrived at the laboratory for what they thought the independent variables were properly manipulated, the
was a study of "personality characteristics and career experimenter elaborated on the meaning of the graphed
choice." They had been scheduled in pairs, but care was feedback and offered his opinion that the subject's score
taken so that subjects had no contact with each other was "surprisingly low" (or "surprisingly high") for a Yale
before the start of the experiment The two subjects were student.
greeted by the male experimenter and led into two It was at this point that the conditions of the experiment
different rooms separated by a folding wall. This arrange- were made salient to the subjects. Depending on which
ment enabled subjects to become aware of activity in the condition they had been assigned, the subjects were given
other room but to be unable to hear what was being said one from among the following eight possible kinds of
there. feedback on the RWPP results: subjects were told that
After subjects were seated comfortably in their rooms, they had scored either well above or well below average
they were presented whh a consent agreement and a on one of the RWPP's subscales. The particular subscale
short demographic questionnaire. In order to make salient was identified as one that either did or did not correspond
the career interest that formed the basis for their recruit- to the subject's stated career interest. And the profile of
ment, subjects once again indicated their chosen field of the subject next door indicated a superior performance
study on the demographic questionnaire and were asked on a scale that either corresponded or did not correspond
to write a paragraph describing more specifically their with the subjects' stated career interest.
career interest Five minutes later, when subjects had completed the
After completing these forms, subjects were reminded new sets of forms, the experimenter returned, collected
that they were participating in a study of personality and them, and informed subjects that they had just one more
career choice; specifically, the investigators were interested task to do. Another way to understand personality, subjects
in what kinds of people seem to choose which professions. were told, was to make judgments about another person.
As such, subjects were told, their first task would be to In this task, they were to read the self-descriptive essay
complete a 72-item personality test called the Robertson- written by the subject next door and then to imagine
Wagner Personality Profile (RWPP).1 what it would be like when the two of them met at the
After subjects completed the personality test, they were end of the experiment. Subjects were given the "essay," a
told that a more subjective personality assessment needed list of 25 bipolar traits on which to make judgments
to be administered as well, and so they were given a about the other person, a 20-item anxiety scale, and a
form in which they were asked to write a short self- mood checklist on which they were to indicate how they
descriptive essay. Subjects were told that they would be would feel after meeting and interacting with the other
given about 10 min to write the essay, and that while person.
they were writing, the experimenter would score a few
of the subscales on the RWPP and let them know how
1
they had done. The subjects were then left alone to work The RWPP was composed of items from Snyder's
on their essays. (1974) Self-Monitoring Scale; Rosenberg's (196S) Self-
After IS mm had elapsed, the experimenter returned, Esteem Inventory, Bringle, Roach, Andler, and Evenbeck's
carrying a new set of forms to be filled out by the subject (1979) Self-Report Jealousy Scale; and Rotter's (1966)
and a graph resembling the Profile and Case Summary Internal-External Locus of Control Scale.
784 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

The self-descriptive essay of the other person read as successful, nptimirtir/pftCTjmiaip., socially unskilled/so-
follows: cially skilled, unhealthy/healthy. The direction of these
items was randomized and balanced.
I am a freshman at Yale with many interests. I enjoy Self-esteem. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Inventory
backpacking and camping as well as going to movies was presented. The items on this measure did not differ
and plays. My main interest lies in medicine [business from those included in the RWPP.
and management; theater music, and the performing
arts in general]—I hope to be a doctor [an executive
in industry, perform professionally] someday. I'm al- Dependent Measures
ready taking some of the required courses and doing The dependent variables in this experiment were (a)
pretty well in them—really well, actually, considering evaluations of the comparison other, (b) level of friendship
my background in this area before coming to Yale. I desired with the other person, (c) mood while anticipating
study a lot, and the hard work seems to be paying off. interacting with the other, (d) self-reported envy/jealousy
Socially, things are only o.k. I have lots friends, but of the comparison other, and (e) anxiety reported in
no real romantic attachments. That, however, could be anticipating an interaction with the other person.
changing in the near future. 1 certainly hope so. Evaluations of the other Subjects evaluated the other
Academically, though, things are great I've been person on 25 bipolar traits (e.g., likable/unlikable, gen-
one of the top in all my classes—the couple of professors erous/selfish) presented on 7-point Likert scales. The
that I have talked to say I have a promising future. . . direction of these items was randomized and balanced;
I guess time will tell!! in this report, however, higher scores indicate the more
The essays were constructed so that subjects could find positive pole on each trait.2
both likable and annoying aspects of this person. Their Desired friendship Subjects indicated on a single
judgments of him or her (the person was always depicted 7-point item how much they would want the person,
as the same sex as the subject) would then reflect how whose essay they had just read, as a friend. Responses
much attention they had focused on the person's assets could range from very little to very much.
or faults. Mood. The measure of subjects' mood as they imag-
Subjects were given 1S min to complete their evaluations ined meeting the comparison person was the Depression
of the other person and their anxiety and mood about Adjective Checklist (DACL) developed by Zuckerman,
interacting with him or her. When they had finished, the Lubin, and Robins (196S). The instrument consists of a
experimenter collected their forms and debriefed them. list of 34 feeling state adjectives. Subjects are asked to
Because of the deception involved in this study, extra check all that describe their current mood state. The
care was taken during debriefing to ensure that subjects DACL is scored for depressive affect as the total number
understood the bogus nature of the feedback that had of negative items checked plus the number of positive
been given to them. During debriefing, any suspiciousness items not indicated.
regarding the procedure of the experiment was elicited. Envy/jealousy To measure self-reported envy and
jealousy in a subtle, nonreactive way, we inserted "envy"
and "jealousy" among the adjectives of the DACL. Subjects
Measures of Potential Mediating Variables were given an "envy/jealousy" score ranging from 0, if
Immediately after the false feedback manipulation was
instantiated (thereby establishing the conditions of the
2
experiment for the subject), we administered three mea- The 25-item scale measuring subjects' evaluations of
sures that might allow us to make inferences regarding the comparison person was reduced to a more manageable
the cognitive/affective process associated with receipt of set of variables. These items were subjected to a principle
such feedback. These three measures included a brief axis factor analysis with vanmax rotation and commu-
affect scale, a self-concept scale, and the Rosenberg Self- nality estimated by the squared multiple correlation
Esteem Inventory. coefficient. The analysis revealed four stable factors ac-
Positive-negative affect scale. Four bipolar items pre- counting for 85% of the variance in the total scale. The
sented on 7-point Likert scales (exhilarated/not exhila- four factors and the traits loading on to them are as
rated, sad/not sad, elated/not elated, unhappy/not un- follows: Factor 1 (understanding)—sympathetic (.73),
happy) were used to quickly assess subjects' affect im- sensitive (.66), empathic (.64), and generous (.64); Factor
mediately after they received feedback on the personality 2 (competence)—careful (.77), responsible (.74), and
test This affect scale was chosen because in previous successful (.66); Factor 3 (soriophilia)—well adjusted
studies involving mood induction, it has served as an (.77), interesting (.74), strong (.74), romantic (.71), popular
accurate mood manipulation check (Amrhein, Salovey, (.66), sociable (.57), and attractive (.55); and Factor 4
& Rosenhan, 1981; Rosenhan, Salovey, & Hargis, 1981). (friendliness)—friendly (.76), happy (.76), warm (.75),
Self-concept scale. On the self-concept measure we and nice (.59). However, the four factors correlated highly
asked subjects to assess how they felt about themselves with the full scale score (.45 < r < .89) and with each
on 12 bipolar items presented on 7-point scales that have other (F= .40). In addition subsequent analyses on the
also demonstrated their utility as a self-appraisal measure four factor scores replicated analyses on the full scale
in an earlier study (Amrhein et al., 1981). The items on scores. Consequently only the full scale data is reported
this measure were popular/unpopular, unintelligent/in- in the remainder of this article. Two items (preppie/
telligent,friendly/unfriendly,satisfied/unsatisfied with self, nonpreppie; quiet/noisy) had no distinct positive or
unattractive/attractive, unselfish/selfish, unsure of self/ negative pole and were dropped from the total scale
self-confident, trustworthy/untrustworthy, unsuccessful/ score.
S0OAIX30MPAR1S0N JEALOUSY 785

neither item were checked, to 2 if both were t*t**t4 A Table 1


1 was assigned to subjects who ducked either jealousy Correlations Among Dependent Variables
or envy.
Anxiety Anxiety about the prospect of meeting the Variable 1
other person was assessed by the State Anxiety Inventory,
from Spielberger, Gorsuch, and Lushene's (1970) State- 1. Other-evaluation — .78 -.44 -.37 -.35
Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). The STAI is a widely 2. Desired friendship — -.43 -.41 -29
used instrument composed of 20 items by which the 3. Envy/jealousy — .33 .34
level of transitory anxiety experienced in performance- 4. Depression — .56
demand situations is measured. The essential qualities 5. Anxiety —
evaluated are feelings of apprehension, tension, nervous-
ness, and worry. Spielberger et al. (1970) reported the Note. For all correlations; if \r\ > .21, then p < .05, and
psychometric properties of this scale. if fH > .28, then p< .01.

Results3
Independent Variables' Relation to means for all of the dependent variables are
Dependent Variables4 presented in Table 2. (Table 2 also shows the
results of univariate analyses.) A MANOVA on
Multivariate analysis of variance. Table 1 the cognitive variables revealed (a) significant
illustrates the correlational relations among effects for all three main effects [Wilks's
the dependent variables. As this table indi- lambda = 0.77, J^3, 68) = 6.74, p < .0005
cates, they tended to be intercorrelated. Be- for valence; Wilks's lambda = 0.86, P(3t
cause of such associations as well as the 68) = 3.66, p < .02 for relevance; and Wilks's
number of dependent variables, we performed lambda = 0.86, PQ, 68) = 3.58, p < .02 for
a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) similarity], (b) significant effects for all three
on the combination of all dependent variables two-way interactions [Wilks's lambda = 0.86,
to protect against Type 1 error. The MANOVA F(3, 68) = 3.78, p < .01 for Va-
revealed significant main effects for feedback lence X Relevance; Wilks's lambda = 0.86,
valence [Wilks's lambda = 0.73, F{5, 66) = F(3, 68)= 3.81, p<.0l for Va-
4.79, p < .0009], and for feedback relevance lence X Similarity; and Wilks's lambda =
[Wilks's lambda = 0.83, F(Sy 66) = 2.73, p < 0.85, F(3, 68) = 3.93, p < .01 for Rele-
.03], and marginally for similarity of the vance X Similarity], and (c) a nearly signifi-
comparison person [Wilks's lambda = 0.85, cant three-way interaction [Wilks's lambda =
7=1(5, 66) = 2.24, p < .06]. There were signif- 0.88, F(3, 68) = 2.52, p < .06 for Valence X
icant effects for all two-way interactions: Relevance X Similarity].
Wilks's lambda = 0.85, f\5, 66) = 2.28, p < In order to directly test the major hypoth-
.05 for Valence X Relevance; Wilks's esis of this study (that social-comparison
lambda = 0.84, F{5, 66) = 2.44, p < .04
for Relevance X Similarity; and Wilks's
lambda = 0.84, F\5, 66) = 2.50, p < .04 for 3
Valence X Similarity. The three-way interac- Correlation and multiple regression analyses that
included the personality scales that made up the RWPP
tion, Valence X Relevance X Similarity, did were conducted, and revealed little that was theoretically
not quite reach significance: Wilks's interesting. With the exception of the Self-Report Jealousy
lambda = 0.88, F(5, 66) = 1.67, p < .15. Scale, none of the measures was associated with the
dependent variables of this study. The Self-Report Jealousy
Because of the largely significant MANOVA, Scale, though significantly related to some of the dependent
we analyzed the dependent variables individ- variables, accounted for very little variance in them.
ually. First, however, we reexamined the de- Results of these analyses are available from the authors
pendent variables together, but in two groups on request.
*ANOVAS revealed no occupational group differences
because two distinct types of measures had on any of the dependent measures. Women gave more
been planned for this study. These were cog- positive evaluations to the comparison person on com-
nitive variables—evaluations of the compar- petence, F{1, 77) = 5.64, p < .02, and on sociophiua,
ison person, desired friendship with this per- f[l, 77) = 6.40, p < .02. Data have been pooled over
son, and self-perceived envy/jealousy—and gender and occupational group in the remaining analyses.
Because of the small n that would have resulted, ANOVAS
affective variables—depression and anxiety. were not performed with sex and occupational group as
Cognitive dependent variables. The cell independent variables with the manipulated variables.
786 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

Table 2
Means and Analyses of Dependent Variables
Condition Other ratings Desired Friendship Envy/Jealousy Depression Anxiety
Positive valence
Irrelevant
Dissimilar
M 122.30. 4.50. 0.30. 6.30. 37.50.
SD 13.33 1.65 0.67 2.06 9.22
Similar
M 124.70. 4.80. 0.20. 7.90. 34.10.
SD 15.56 1.03 0.63 3.35 7.82
Relevant
Dissimilar
M 125.10. 5.20. 0.00. 5.20. 32.80.
SD 11.93 0.92 0.00 2.44 4.89
Similar
M 122.30, 5.00. 0.00. 5.90. 34.50.
SD 19.88 1.70 000 4.41 6.43
Negative valence
Irrelevant
Dissimilar
M 120.60. 3.60.h 0.30. 8.90. 34.20.
SD 17.68 1.84 0.67 5.57 8.94
Similar
M 109.78. 3.70.h 0.10. 8.50. 33.90.
SD 10.11 1.49 0.32 3.78 8.24
Relevant
Dissimilar
M 119.56. 4.90. 0.10. 6.00. 31.30.
SD 23.71 1.45 0.32 3.89 6.22
Similar
M 84.00* 2.30,, 1.30* 9.80. 39.70.
SD 21.42 0.82 0.95 5 14 10.97
Three-way A NOVA results
Contrast
F 39.19"* 21.85"" 38.87"** 4.43** 4.32"
df 1,70 1,72 1,72 1,72 1,72
Residual
F 0.97 2.04* 0.54 1.32 0.55
df 6, 70 6,72 6,72 6,72 6,72
•Valence
F 15.58*" 15.71"" 7.01"* 4.89" 0.00
df 1.70 1,72 1,72 1, 72 1,72
Relevance
F 3.23* 0.40 1.04 1.73 0.04
df 1,70 1, 72 1,72 1, 72 1,72
Similarity
F 8.46*" 3.62* 3.36* 2.55 0.79
df 1,70 1,72 1,72 1,72 1,72
V XR
F 3.59* 0.63 9.33*" 0.18 1.00
df 1,70 1,72 1,72 1,72 1.72
v Fxs 8.62"* 4.25" 5.02" 0.09 1.85
df 1,70 1, 72 1,72 1,72 1,72
R XS
F 3.53* 6.44" 9.33*" 0.85 3.67*
df 1,70 1,72 1,72 1, 72 1,72
VXRXS
F 1.56 3.04» 7.01"* 2.04 0.25
df 1,70 1,72 1,72 1,72 1,72
Note. V = valence; R = relevance, S = similarity. Subscripts refer to results of Tukey's Multiple Comparison Procedure.
Comparisons should be made vertically, and means that do not differ according to this criterion have the same subscript.
* p < .10. " p < .05. " • p < .01. " " p < .001.
SOCIAL-COMPARISON JEALOUSY 787

jealousy would be felt most strongly under other person. F tests on the residual variance
conditions of negative self-relevant feedback following the contrast are reported in Table
and the presence of a similar, successful other 2 and are all nonsignificant, indicating that
person), means for this cell on the evaluation there is no systematic variance left to explain
measure, desired friendship, and envy/jeal- following the removal of the theoretically
ousy were compared with means in the re- interesting variance. Results from the three-
maining seven cells via an a priori weighted way ANOVAS, showing strong effects only for
contrast. The results of this contrast are valence, are presented in Table 2 as well.
provided in Table 2. On other-evaluation,
desired friendship, and envy/jealousy, this Independent Variables' Relation to
cell (termed the jealousy cell) was significantly Mediational Measures5
different from the remaining seven cells. In
Cell means for the four mediational vari-
addition, Ftest on the residual variance (after
ables are presented in Table 3. Because these
the contrast variance was removed) were not
four variables tended to be intercorrelated,
significant, indicating that there was little
we performed a MANOVA for the combina-
systematic variance left following that ex-
tion of these variables. With regard to the
plained by the a priori contrast. Table 2 also
apparently weak mean differences depicted
presents the results from two other analyses,
in Table 3, it is not surprising that there
the three-way univariate analyses of variance
were no significant main effects [Wilks's
(ANOVAS) and Tukey's Multiple Comparison
lambda = 0.95, F\4, 68) = 0.93, p = .45 for
Procedure. Tukey's multiple comparisons
feedback valence; Wilks's lambda = 0.95,
verified that the jealousy cell was different
F\4, 68) = 0.88, p = .48, for feedback
from the remaining seven cells, for the most
relevance; Wilks's lambda = 0.94, ^ 4 , 68) =
part, and that there were few differences
1.12,/? = .35 for similarity of the comparison
among the seven nonjealousy cells.
person], nor any significant two-way interac-
Affective dependent variables. The cell tions [Wilks's lambda = 0.92, 7=1(4, 68) = 1.55,
means for the two affective variables (depres- p = .20 for Valence X Relevance; Wilks's
sion and anxiety) are also presented in Table lambda = 0.99, F{4, 68) = 0.07, p = .99 for
2. A MANOVA on the combination of the two Valence X Similarity; and Wilks's lambda =
affective variables revealed only a significant 0.95, 7=1(4, 68) = 0.97, p = .43 for Rele-
main effect for feedback valence [Wilks's vance X Similarity], nor a significant Va-
lambda = 0.91, ^ 2 , 71) = 3.52, p < .03]. lence X Relevance X Similarity three-way
No other main effects nor any interactions interaction [Wilks's lambda = 0.99, 7=1(4,
were significant: Wilks's lambda = 0.97, f\2, 68) = 0.23, p = .95]. Because of the lack of
71) = 1.08, p = .35 for feedback relevance; significant results on the MANOVA, no further
Wilks's lambda = 0.96, F\2, 71) = 1.26, p = analyses of these variables (e.g., univariate
.29 for similarity of the other person; Wilks's ANOVAS) were undertaken.
lambda = 0.98, F(2, 71) = 0.51, p = .60 for
Valence X Relevance; Wilks's lambda = 0.97,
F(2, 71) = 1 . 0 7 , p = .35 for Va- Discussion
lence X Similarity; Wilks's lambda = 0.95,
F{2, 71)= 1.83, p = .17 for Relevance X As hypothesized, individuals experienced
Similarity; and Wilks's lambda = 0.97, F(2, social-comparison jealousy only when they
71) = 1.08, p = .35 for Valence X Rele- received self-relevant negative feedback and
vance X Similarity. subsequently had to associate with a similar
(self-relevant), successful other person. The
As indicated in Table 2, the multiple com-
parison procedure revealed no differences
among the eight cells in anxiety or depression. 5
ANOVAS revealed no significant main effects of gender
However, the planned weighted contrast of or occupational group on mediational measures. Data
the jealousy cell, as compared with the re- have been pooled over gender and occupational group in
maining seven cells, suggests that subjects the remaining analyses, ANOVAS were not performed with
sex and occupational group as independent variables
were indeed more anxious and depressed with the manipulated variables to identify interaction
when they received negative relevant feedback effects because of the small n that would have been
and were compared with a similar, successful represented in each cell.
788 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

Table 3
Means of Mediational Variables by Condition
Positive Negative

Irrelevant Relevant Irrelevant Relevant

Variable Dissim. Sim. Dissim. Sim. Dissim. Sim. Disam. Sim.

Positive/negative affect
M 21.00 18.50 20.20 19.30 19.50 17.50 18.30 18.60
SD 3.16 4.45 4.21 5.12 5.40 4.43 5.42 4.06
Self-concept
M 67.20 67.30 69.50 67.40 63.30 64.20 68.00 63.70
SD 4.71 7.11 6.93 5.44 14.00 9.75 5.98 9.75
Self-esteem
M 59.60 59.70 56.20 54.30 56.80 56.50 60.30 56.70
SD 5.76 6.98 6.66 6.29 11.27 11.70 4.24 8.01
Self-esteem change
M -0.30 0.90 0.40 1.20 -0.10 3.00 0.70 -O.30
SD 2.83 4.23 4.40 3.82 3.28 5.83 2.45 6.09

Note. Dissim. = dissimilar other, Sim. = similar other. There are 10 subjects in each cell.

experience of social-comparison jealousy was The Necessity of Three


manifested by individuals' disparaging of the Situational Conditions
comparison person under these conditions.
In addition, subjects identified this situation Are all three conditions—negative feed-
as one in which they experienced envy or back, self-relevant feedback, and a similar,
jealousy, and there was some evidence that it successfulrival—necessaryfor the experience
engendered feelings of depression and anxiety of social-comparison jealousy? Without doubt,
as well. mere inspection of condition means indicates
The state that we label social-comparison that jealousy is most manifest (asrivaldepre-
jealousy is similar to that state described by cation, negative mood, anxiety, and self-report
Tesser and Campbell (1983) as "the pain of of envy/jealousy) when all three conditions
comparison" (p. 6). Tesser sees this painful are present. We used the a priori contrast in
state as resulting from unfavorable compari- the analyses of this experiment to test the
sons with others on self-relevant dimensions, hypothesis that all three conditions are nec-
and so do we. Both Tesser and we note the essary for social-comparison jealousy to be
necessity of self-relevant comparison objects experienced. For the case in which any one
(i.e., we require that the other person must of these conditions was absent, no jealousy
be similar to us; Tesser states that the other was predicted. Of course, the "low" condition
person should be in a close relationship with of any of the three independent variables is
us). In the present study we have extended really not equivalent to the absence of that
Tesser's "pain of comparison" notion by dis- variable. As such it is not correct to speak of
covering that such pain is composed of de- an "all-or-none" effect of the three indepen-
pressed affect and anxiety. In addition, dent variables on jealousy. Thus although a
whereas Tesser sees the results of such com- multiplicative relation among these variables
parisons taking three potential forms—(a) a may exist, the implications of this model are
decreased closeness with the other, (b) an not addressed by this study. Certainly, the
interference with the performance of the other, planned contrast positing all of the impact
or (c) an altering of one's self-definition—we of the three variables in just one of the eight
have identified a fourth consequence of com- cells fits the data very well for all of the
parison, the degradation of the character (and cognitive and affective dependent variables.
not just the performance) of the rival. We argue, then, that social-comparison jeal-
SOCIAL-COMPARISON JEALOUSY 789

ousy is most manifest during the conjoint An explanation for subjects' behavior that
action of these three situational factors. When focuses on motivation to maintain positive
one or more of them is reduced, there is little self-evaluation (cf. Tesser & Campbell, 1980)
degradation of the rival, depression, or anxi- may be more fruitful. Subjects may disparage
ety, and almost no self-report of envy or a potential rival (who outperformed them) in
jealousy. order to decrease their similarity to him or
her, that is, they undercut his or her relevance
Alternative Explanations and as an object of comparison. One possible
Mediational Variables explanation for our results, not suggested by
An alternative explanation for the main Tesser's work, is that anxiety and sadness at
findings of this study is that negative feedback the prospect of having to interact with this
(particularly when it is self-relevant and in person may arise as a consequence of the act
the implied presence of a similar, successful of disparaging an "innocent" person, rather
rival) causes a negative affective state in its than from this person's performance per se.
recipient. This depressed mood, then, primes However, we can also integrate our work with
the availability of negatively toned material Tesser's in the following way. Antecedents
in memory (cf. Bower, 1981; Isen, Shalker, such as unflattering comparisons in self-rel-
Clark, & Karp, 1978) and promotes selective evant domains, which provoke self-esteem
attention to affectively negative external stim- maintenance processes, should also lead to
uli. As a result, the recipient of such feedback the experience of jealousy. It is the feeling of
is likely to evaluate other people negatively jealousy that in return increases depression,
and do the same for his or her own thoughts anxiety, and behaviors that serve to degrade
and beliefs. Negative feedback thus leads to the other.
a negative mood state, causing one to attend Speculating further, it seems that when we
to and recall only the negative aspects of attack a similar other, we attack part of
other people, the "feel-bad-see-others-as-bad" ourselves, because our self-definition was ob-
hypothesis. tained through contextuaUy anchored pro-
The best evidence for evaluating this hy- cesses (i.e., social comparison) in the first
pothesis comes from the mediational mea- place. Thus our deprecation of rivals may,
sures, administered immediately after the bo- rather than bolstering a diminished self-es-
gus feedback was conveyed to the subject. teem, actually serve in some cases to diminish
On all four of these measures—affect, reactive it even further, thus exacerbating social-com-
self-concept, self-esteem, and self-esteem parison jealousy. Of course, the positing of
change—no significant differences were noted such sequences of events is only speculative
among the eight feedback conditions of this in the context of the present study. There is
experiment. According to the "feel-bad-see- really no opportunity here to distinguish
others-as-bad" hypothesis, at least, negative among alternative sequence models. Perhaps
feedback produces negative affective ratings future researchers who use structural equation
and, perhaps, negative views of oneself. This designs and time series data will be able to
was not the case. It seems, then, that the further illuminate these issues.
negative feedback was processed (and mani-
fested itself) in a more subtle way. Rather
than causing an immediate mood and self- The Processing of Self-Definitional
concept deflation, it led to increased dispar- Information
aging of a potential rival (along with some The effects of relatively negative versus
anxiety and depression) at a later point in positive feedback in the presence of similar
time. Perhaps such feedback only becomes or dissimilar others has had a somewhat long
salient when the recipient thinks that he or history in the social comparison literature
she is going to interact with the rival. Subjects (cf. Darley, 1966; Festinger, 1954; Singer,
labeled such a state as envy or jealousy more 1980; Suls & Miller, 1977). The processing
consistently than they termed it depression of self-relevant information, however, has only
or anxiety. recently become an area of particularly in-
790 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

tense research in social cognition. Research during the debriefing that although they did
in this area may help us to understand why not want to believe the negative feedback,
feedback must be on a self-definitional di- they could not put it out of their minds. No
mension in order for it to have jealousy- matter how much they tried to convince
inducing consequences. themselves that perhaps a mistake had been
For feedback to be thoroughly attended to, made, they still began to doubt their abilities
it must be consistent with one's self-schema. in the feedback domain. As one subject stated,
Markus (1977) defines a self-schema as a
I tried to ignore the results on the graph because they
collection of "cognitive generalizations about seemed so far from the truth. But no matter how much
the self, deriving from past experience, that I tried to get on with the task, my mind kept returning
organize and guide social experience" (p. 63). to that below-average profile. Soon, I was feeling a bit
The self-schema organizes personally relevant worried and very sensitive about my own abilities. Then,
experiences, serving as a template against when I read the other guy's story, I couldn't help but
think, "If he's such a not-shot premed and does so well
which individuals perceive and encode infor- in his classes, I bet he's really just a nerd; I bet he's one
mation about themselves. Self-schema con- of those unfriendly, antisocial weenies that hang out in
gruent information is better remembered than the library 20 hours a day; he probably couldn't have an
incongruent material (Markus & Sentis, 1982) interesting conversation with anyone." I hated him and
envied him at the same time because he seemed so good
and, within certain bounds, is processed more at his work, while I was beginning to doubt my ability
efficiently and in a more prototypic way to ever become a doctor, (emphasis added)
(Kuiper & Derry, 1980; Kuiper & Rogers,
1979; Rogers, Rogers, & Kuiper, 1979). Such doubting of one's abilities in a self-
The negative feedback that led to jealousy definitional domain seems to provoke social
in this study was certainly not congruent comparison, perhaps in a determined effort
with a subject's self-schema. Yet the infor- to reaffirm one's sense of competence. This
mation was still highly relevant. On the one leads to sensitivity regarding the successful
hand, individuals are inclined to ignore self- performances of others in this domain and
schema incongruent information, but there is ultimately to disparaging these people, as one
a simultaneous tendency for self-relevant in- searches for factors that might mitigate their
formation to be especially affectively powerful. apparent success. By degrading successful
As Higgins, Kuiper, and Olson (1981) note, rivals or their performances, jealous individ-
"a recurring theme in the 'self literature is uals believe that they can regain their dimin-
that increases in the perceived relevance of ished status. Such attempts are often unsuc-
an event to the self lead to heightened affect cessful in the long term and may make matters
or emotionality (Cooley, 1902; Epstein, 1973; worse by publicly revealing one's jealous feel-
James, 1890; Rogers, 1951; Snygg & Coombs, ings.
1959)" (p. 413). In the present study, the In summary, the present study served to
same information could have been both self- systematically vary the antecedents of social-
relevant and self-schema incongruent. For comparison jealousy and examine their role
example, a premedical student might have in producing cognitive, behavioral, and affec-
received false feedback that he or she had tive consequences, enabling us to identify
below-average medical science aptitude. Neg- many of the component processes involved
ative feedback on a dimension that is partic- in social-comparison jealousy. Further work
ularly important to oneself, then, may be is needed, however, to fully understand the
impossible to ignore because of its affective integration of cognitive, affective, and physi-
consequences. The tension between the affec- ological processes in such complex emotional
tive power of self-relevant information and states.
the predilection to ignore incongruent infor-
mation may underlie social-comparison jeal-
ousy. References
This tension was often exhibited by subjects Amrhein, J., Salovey, P., & Rosenhan, D. L. (1981). Joy
in the jealousy-inducing conditions of the and sadness generate aitnbutional vulnerability in
present study. Such subjects often reported men Unpublished manuscript, Stanford University.
SOCIAL-COMPARISON JEALOUSY 791

Aristotle (1932). Rhetoric. In L. Cooper (Ed.), The Kuiper, N. A., & Rogers, T. B. (1979). The encoding of
rhetoric of Aristotle. An expanded translation with personal information: Self-other differences. Journal of
supplementary examples for students of composition Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 499-514.
and public speaking (Book 2, Chap. 10). Englewood Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing infor-
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. (Original work published c. mation about the self. Journal of Personality and
340 B.c.) Social Psychology. 36, 1278-1290.
Bers, S A., & Rodin, J. (1984). Social-comparison jeal- Markus, H., & Sentis, K. (1982). The self in social
ousy: A developmental and motivational study. Journal information processing. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological
of Personality and Social Psychology, 47. 766-779. perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 41-70). Hillsdale,
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychologist, 36, 129-148. McGuire, W. J. (1966). Attitudes and opinions. Annual
Bringle, R. G. (1981). Conceptualizing jealousy as a Review of Psychology. 17. 475-514.
disposition. Alternative Lifestyles, 4. 274-290. Mettee, D. R., & Smith, G. (1977). Social comparison
Bringle, R. G., Roach, S., Andler, C , & Evenbeck, S. and interpersonal attraction: The case for dissimilarity.
(1979). Measuring the intensity of jealous reactions. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison
Journal Supplement Abstract Service, 9, 23-24. processes Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp.
Bryson, J. B. (1976). The nature of sexual jealousy: An 69-101). Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing.
exploratory study. Paper presented at the annual meeting Neu, J. (1980). Jealous thoughts. In A. O. Rorty (Ed.),
of the American Psychological Association, Washington, Explaining emotions (pp. 425-463). Los Angeles: Uni-
D.C. versity of California Press.
Bryson, J. B. (1977). The siluatwnal determinants of the Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. New York:
expression ofjealousy. Paper presented at the annual Houghton Mifflin.
meeting of the American Psychological Association, Rogers, T. B.. Rogers, P. J., & Kuiper, N. A. (1979).
San Francisco. Evidence for the self as a cognitive prototype: The
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thome, A., Walker, M. "false alarm effect." Personality and Social Psychology
R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Basking in Bulletin. 5. 53-56.
reflected glory: Three (football) field studies. Journal Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366-375. image Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. Rosenhan, D. L., Salovey, P., & Hargis, K. (1981). The
New York: Scribner. joys of helping: Focus of attention mediates the impact
Dakin, S., & Arrowood, A. J. (1981). The social com- of positive affect on altruism. Journal of Personality
parison of ability. Human Relations, 34, 89-109. and Social Psychology. 40, 899-905.
Darley, J. (1966). Studies in social comparison. Journal Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal
of Experimental Social Psychology, Supplement 1. versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological
Epstein, S. (1973). The self concept revisited: Or a theory Monographs, SO (1, Whole No. 609).
of a theory. American Psychologist, 28, 404-416. Sabini, J., & Silver, M. (1982). Envy. In J. Sabini & M.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison Silver (Eds.), Moralities of everyday life (pp. 15-33).
processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140. New York: Oxford University Press.
Gellert, S. (1976). Mixed emotions. Transactional Analysis Silver, M., & Sabini, J. (1978). The perception of envy.
Journal. 6, 129-130. Social Psychology, 41, 105-117.
Goethals, G. R., & Darley, J. M. (1977). Social comparison Singer, J. E. (1980). Social comparison: The process of
theory: An attributional approach. In J. M. Suls & self evaluation. In L. Festinger (Ed.), Retrospections
R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison processes- Theo- on social psychology (pp. 158-179). New York; Oxford
retical and empirical perspectives (pp. 259-278). University Press.
Washington, D. C.: Hemisphere Publishing. Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring processes of expressive
Higgins, E. T, Kuiper, N. A., & Olson, J. M. (1981). behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Social cognition: A need to get personal. In E. T. 30. 526-537.
Higgins, C. P. Herman & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social Snygg, D., & Coombs, A. W. (1959). Individual behavior:
cognition: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 395- A perceptual approach to behavior (rev. ed.). New
420). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. York: Harper.
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Spielberger, C , Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1970).
Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behav- Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto,
ior A cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Psychology, 36, 1-12.
Spielman, P. M. (1971). Envy and jealousy: An attempt
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New
at clarification. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 40, 59-82.
York Holt.
Jaremko, M. E., & Iindsey, R. (1979). Stress-coping Suls, J. M. (1977). Social comparison theory and research.
abilities of individuals high and low in jealousy. Psy- In J. M. Suls & R. M. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison
chological Reports, 44, 547-553. processes' Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp.
Kuiper, N. A., & Derry, P. A. (1980). The self as a 1-19). Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing.
cognitive prototype: An application to person perception Suls, J. M., & Miller, R. L. (1977). Social comparison
and depression. In N. Cantor & J. Kihlstrom (Eds.), processes. Theoretical and empirical perspectives.
Cognition, social interaction, and personality {pp. 215- Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing.
232). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tesser, A. (1980). Self-esteem maintenance in family
792 PETER SALOVEY AND JUDITH RODIN

dynamics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology between envy and jealousy. Journal of Pkenomenologkal
31, 77-91. Psychology. 12, 189-204.
Tessa; A., & Campbell, J. (1980). Self-definition: The Tov-Ruach, L. (1980). Jealousy, attention, & loss. In
impact of the relative performance and similarity of A. Q Rorty (Ed.), Explaining emotions (pp. 465-488).
others. Social Psychology Quarterly. 43, 341-347. Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Tessa; A., & Campbell, J. (1982). Self-evaluation mainte- Wheela; L., Shaver, K. G., Jones, R. A., Goethals,
nance and the perception of friends and strangers. G. A., Cooper, J., Robinson, J. E., Gruda; C. L., &
Journal of Personality, 50, 261-279. Butzine, K. W. (1969). Factors determining the choice
Tessa; A., & Campbell, J. (1983). Self-definition and of comparison other. Journal of Experimental Social
self-evaluation maintenance. In J. Suls & A. GreenwaM Psychology, 5, 219-232.
(Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self Zuckerman, M., Lubin, B., & Robins, S. (1965). Validation
(Vol. 2, pp. 1-31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. of the multiple affect adjective checklist in clinical
Tessa; A., & Smith, J. (1980). Some effects of task situations. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29, 594.
relevance and friendship on helping: You don't always
help the one you like. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 16, 582-590. Received August 18, 1983
Titleman, P. (1982). A phenomenotogical comparison Revision received February 3, 1984 •

Editor for Psychological Bulletin Named: Search for New Editor Continues
David Zeaman, editor of Psychological Bulletin, died on July 19, 1984. Betty J. House,
Zeaman's colleague at the University of Connecticut, and one of the journal's associate editors,
will complete David Zeaman's term and serve as editor through 1986. Effective immediately,
authors should submit manuscripts to:

Betty J. House, Editor


Psychological Bulletin
Department of Psychology U-20, Rm #107
25 Cross Campus Road
Storrs, Connecticut 06268

APA's Publications and Communications Board is continuing its recently opened search for a
new editor. Candidates for the journal editorship must be members of A PA and should be
available to start receiving manuscripts in early 1986 to prepare for issues published in 1987.
The term of editorship is from 1987 through 1992. To nominate candidates, prepare a
statement of one page or less in support of each nomination. Submit nominations no later
than February 1, 1985 to the chair of the search committee:

Barbara Strudler Wallston


Box512Peabody
Vanderbilt University
Nashville, Tennessee 37203

The other members of the search committee are Elizabeth Loftus, Wilbert McKeachie, Paul
Mussen, Lyman Porter, and Lee Sechrest.

You might also like