You are on page 1of 21

Received: 7 July 2020 | Revised: 11 January 2021 | Accepted: 12 January 2021

DOI: 10.1002/mar.21517

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Social good or self‐interest: Incentivizing consumer social


media engagement behaviour for health messages

Helen Siuki | Cynthia M. Webster

Department of Marketing, Macquarie


Business School, Sydney, New South Wales, Abstract
Australia
This study investigates the impact of incentive type and ad appeal on consumers'
Correspondence social media engagement in health‐related ads. The aim is to determine whether
Dr Helen Siuki, Macquarie Business School, self‐oriented incentives versus other‐oriented social incentives encourage (crowd
3 Management Drive, room 147, Macquarie
University, NSW 2109, Australia. in) or discourage (crowd out) consumers to engage in health‐related ads on social
Email: helen.siuki@mq.edu.au media. Four experimental studies examine the level of social media consumer en-
gagement under three different incentive conditions for two types of ad appeals and
two different health issues. Findings show among incentives, the social good in-
centive has a significantly greater impact on social media consumer engagement for
alcohol ads, with the social good incentive triggering people's intrinsic motivations
and encouraging social media engagement. For smoking ads, the social good in-
centive again promotes engagement, although not significant, This study finds that
overall people are significantly more likely to engage in emotional health‐related ads
on social media compared to rational ads. This study also sheds light on the mod-
erating role of gender on social media engagement for health campaigns. Finally, this
study provides insights on the interaction effect of ad appeal and incentives in
generating social media engagement.

KEYWORDS
ad appeal, consumer engagement, health promotion, incentives, motivations, social media

1 | INTRODUCTION employing methods to influence voluntary behaviour change towards


a better personal and societal wellbeing (Lowe et al., 2015). The
Using social media as an interactive platform to communicate with scope of social marketing is broad, with examples from easier be-
people about product and brand‐related information is one of the haviour changes such as wearing a seatbelt, to more complex pro-
most effective promotional strategies employed by today's success- blems such as tackling obesity, racism and drug addiction. Therefore,
ful organisations (Ashley & Tuten, 2015). Besides commercial brands, motivating e‐WOM (word‐of‐mouth) for some social marketing
increasingly, public health organisations are using social media to messages can be more challenging especially for messages that
promote healthy lifestyle changes and engage consumers, encoura- promote difficult lifestyle changes.
ging them to like, comment, tag and share health messages Promotional strategies typically used in commercial marketing
throughout their online social networks. Social marketers working to incite e‐WOM, such as the use of humour and sex appeals, may be
with governments, nonprofit organisations and nongovernmental less effective for social marketing messages as these approaches may
organisations in the areas of public health, social change and en- be interpreted as insensitive and inappropriate (Brennan &
vironmental sustainability are also taking advantage of the benefits Binney, 2010; McKay‐Nesbitt & Yoon, 2015). We argue that even
social media marketing offers (Liang & Scammon, 2011; Lim outgoing, conscientious, confident individuals who typically engage in
et al., 2017). Social marketing places behaviour change at its core, e‐WOM, may not be willing to share social marketing messages with

Psychol Mark. 2021;38:1293–1313. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar © 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC | 1293


1294 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

their online friends, especially in social networking sites such as to create online content to assist others. So, what can a marketer do
Facebook, Instagram and Twitter, where individuals use their per- to trigger consumers' self‐oriented and other‐oriented intrinsic and
sonal profiles to interact with friends and families for social purposes extrinsic motivations?
(Berger & Milkman, 2011). Moreover, along with health information, How a message is framed can intrinsically influence consumer
consumers are bombarded by a wide range of different content on engagement (Sweeney et al., 2012). Framing the message positively
social media today. News, entertainment, advertisements and per- or negatively, for instance, can lead to different levels of engagement
sonal content posted by friends flood online accounts, yet consumers (Barger et al., 2016). Emotional appeals versus rational, informational
choose to pay attention and react to only a small number of such appeals impact consumer engagement differently. Even among
messages. The important question here is what motivates consumers emotional appeals, positive emotions of warmth, inspiration and fun
to react to a post and participate in an online discussion, especially tend to be shared more (Tellis et al., 2019). Arguably, the type of
for health‐related social marketing messages? Drawing on research appeal may be even more important in the context of health. Many
conducted in commercial marketing (e.g., Ashley & Tuten, 2015; health messages are about challenging behaviour and lifestyle
Ryu & Feick, 2007), research shows designing appealing and valuable changes which may be considered as difficult, unappealing or un-
offerings (Chandon et al., 2000; Schulze et al., 2014), crafting per- pleasant by many consumers. Messages such as, 'Quite now' en-
suasive messages (Barger et al., 2016) and using appropriate in- couraging smoking cessation and 'Don't Assume' promoting
centives are key marketing strategies that may encourage e‐WOM. colorectal cancer screening can be confronting as they deal with hard
To encourage e‐WOM for social marketing messages, social lifestyle changes and sensitive issues that offer intangible paybacks
marketers need to employ effective marketing strategies (Thackeray in the distant future (Rothschild, 1999), all of which can negatively
et al., 2008). As few studies on e‐WOM focus on social marketing impact consumers' willingness to engage with the messages.
messages, we draw on research conducted in commercial marketing Incentives are one of the extrinsic means broadly used by mar-
(e.g., Ashley & Tuten, 2015; Ryu & Feick, 2007). Research shows keters to influence online engagement (Ryu & Feick, 2007). Mone-
designing appealing and valuable offerings (Chandon et al., 2000; tary incentives, such as price discounts and coupons, are used for
Schulze et al., 2014), crafting persuasive messages (Barger promoting message sharing and successful referrals in viral mar-
et al., 2016) and using appropriate incentives are key marketing keting campaigns (Hinz et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2018). Ashley and
strategies that may encourage e‐WOM. Research consistently iden- Tuten (2015) find that almost half of the brands that invite con-
tifies message appeal and incentives as critical factors influencing sumers to share content on social media offer a monetary incentive
e‐WOM (Malthouse et al., 2016; Marbach et al., 2016). Offering in- for sharing. Although marketing research reports positive outcomes
centives to consumers is common practice in marketing, yet there is from offering incentives to consumers in the context of sales pro-
relatively little research on how different types of incentives influ- motion, brand switching and online shopping (Kalra & Shi, 2010;
ence consumer motivation to engage in e‐WOM. Moreover, research Raghubir, 2004), the effectiveness of incentives depends on factors
indicates the relationship between incentives and motivation is such as incentive size, design and type (e.g., monetary vs. non-
complex with external rewards potentially having a detrimental monetary, self‐benefiting vs. other‐benefiting). Moreover, the effec-
crowding out effect (Gneezy et al., 2011) and thus reducing e‐WOM tiveness of incentives for encouraging online engagement and
likelihood (Jin & Huang, 2014). message sharing for prosocial behaviours such as health promotion is
Though some research exists on the effectiveness of product unclear.
types and message appeals in the context of social marketing Under certain situations, offering incentives actually diminishes
(Alhabash et al., 2013; Noble et al., 2014), there is need of further individuals' motivations leading to lower levels of performance and
research on the role of incentives. effort (Dubé et al., 2017; Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000; Jin &
Studies suggest people engage online in social media activities Huang, 2014). This 'crowding out effect' happens when people are
for both intrinsic and extrinsic, self‐oriented reasons. Intrinsic mo- paid to perform a behaviour which is inherently rewarding in itself.
tivations include: Hedonic pleasure (Arnold & Reynolds, 2003), en- Offering monetary incentives for prosocial behaviours may vio-
joyment (Lu & Su, 2009; Shang et al., 2005), entertainment (Azar late norms of altruism and reciprocity, creating uncertainty as to the
et al., 2016). Extrinsic motives include: Recognition and image motivations behind the behaviours resulting in a crowding out of
(Toubia & Stephen, 2013), social influence (Azar et al., 2016), intrinsic motivations by reframing the prosocial behaviour to be seen
reciprocity (Cui et al., 2014), social bonding (Berger, 2014) and self‐ as more egoistic (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000; Gneezy et al., 2011).
presentation (Smith et al., 2012) and self‐enhancement (Berger & Paying people to do something for the social good can backfire,
Milkman, 2012). Not all social media engagement is self‐interested. especially if the payment is made public (Ariely et al., 2009). Other
More altruistic reasons such as concern for others (Hennig‐Thurau research looking at participation in a recycling programme shows
et al., 2004) and genuinely wanting to help others make better that offering nonmonetary rewards benefitting a third party in-
decisions or choices (Yoo & Gretzel, 2011) also positively influence creases participants' satisfaction and that offering a mixed monetary
sharing online content (Vilnai‐Yavetz & Levina, 2018). Munzel and bundle allowing participants to reward themselves and a third party
Kunz (2014) find that other‐oriented, altruistic motives outrank so- produces the best result as this allows for both self‐interest and
cial bonding motives, with high altruistic individuals being more likely altruistic behaviours (Giebelhausen et al., 2016).
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1295

Despite the importance, research focusing on the factors en- 2 | INC E NTIVI ZING SOC IAL M E DIA
couraging online consumer engagement with health promotion is C O N S U M E R E N G A G E M E N T B E H AV I O U R
limited and underexplored. Managers need to know how different
incentives work under different situations and how to manipulate Some studies show that incentives work to stimulate engagement,
incentive characteristics to increase consumer engagement. This whereas others find incentives demotivate people. Research de-
study examines whether and under what type of ad appeal, offering monstrates that those brands that offer incentives to consumers
incentives increases or decreases people's willingness to engage with have more followers or fans, as well as a higher level of online
health‐related messages within social media networks. Specifically, consumer engagement (Ashley & Tuten, 2015; Poch & Martin, 2015).
we address the following questions: Do different types of incentives Social media engagement is defined as consumer engagement on
(monetary, social good, no incentive) encourage or discourage social media, or social media‐based engagement (Hollebeek, 2019).
health‐related engagement on social media? Do women and men Although previous research has explored four dimensions to
react similarly or differently to different incentive types? Do health‐ consumer engagement (i.e., cognitive, emotional, behavioural, social)
related messages framed with an emotional appeal generate greater (Hollebeek et al., 2016), in this study, we focus on the behavioural
online engagement compared to those framed with a rational appeal? dimension of social media‐based consumer engagement. According
Are there any interaction effects between incentives and ad appeals? to Hollebeek et al. (2014, p. 154), 'behavioural aspect of consumer
This study addresses these questions in four online experiments engagement reflects “a consumer's level of brand‐related energy,
on Facebook. Each study examines the impact of self‐oriented versus effort and time spent in brand interactions”'. Operationally, we de-
other‐oriented incentives in conjunction with emotional versus ra- fine behavioural engagement as the sum of all different types of
tional ad appeals in laboratory and online experiment recruiting real interactions a consumer may have in forms of liking, sharing, com-
FB users. This study sheds light on the role of incentives in con- menting, reposting a post.
junction with ad appeals in generating consumer engagement on Nonmonetary rewards such as promotional giveaways, product
social media for health issues. The results contribute to the literature samples and augmented products (e.g., hats, mugs and wristbands)
by providing new empirical evidence to help better understand how associated with the actual product could increase overall word‐of‐
incentives work and the conflicting impact of offering monetary, self‐ mouth, however, offering monetary incentives such as coupons and
oriented incentives for prosocial behaviours. We chose alcohol and rebates (Berger & Schwartz, 2011) does not have such impact on
smoking as two health issues to be examined in this study. online engagement. Although incentives can increase the likelihood
Alcohol is widely consumed for different purposes, including of generating referrals among existing customers (Ryu &
celebration, relaxation (Graham & Homel, 2008) and/or habit (Butt Feick, 2007), the impact of an incentivised referral on the receiver
et al., 2011). Excessive alcohol consumption can result in harm at two needs to be taken into account (Reimer & Benkenstein, 2018;
levels—micro (individual) and meso (societal) (Buyucek et al., 2018; Verlegh et al., 2013). Providing a reward for a referral may have a
Laslett et al., 2010). Moreover, the rates of alcohol consumption at negative impact on the receiver. In an unrewarded referral, the re-
times of crisis, GFC (2007–2009), Hurican Katrina and the attack on ceiver is more likely to perceive the recommendation as genuine and
world trade Centre had increased. More recently, and during the unbiased, triggered by the recommender's knowledge and/or posi-
COVID‐19 pandemic, alcohol sales has significantly increased. Ac- tive experience. However, when both the referral provider and re-
cording to Nielson USA report, in March 2020, there has been a ceiver are incentivized, the negative effects of incentives can be
240% increase in internet alcohol sales which can suggest an in- eliminated or reduced (Verlegh et al., 2013).
crease in consumption Chodkiewicz et al. (2020). Given that alcohol
consumption causes harms across levels, research attention to in-
vestigate the volume consumed and drinking patterns is warranted 2.1 | Theoretical foundation: The crowding out
to gain insights and inform future behavioural change efforts effect
Chodkiewicz et al. (2020).
Similarly, tobacco consumption is another behaviour that its health Studies in the fields of economics and business report positive re-
risks has been acknowledged in the literature (Adhikari et al., 2009. sults from offering incentives (Zhang et al., 2018), however, under
According to World Health Organization (WHO) (2013), tobacco use certain situations, offering incentives conflicts with individuals' in-
remains the single largest cause of preventable premature death and ternal motives, resulting in crowding out of their intrinsic motivations
disease (Almestahiri et al., 2017). The considerable progress that has (Dubé et al., 2017; Gneezy et al., 2011).
been made in reducing the smoking rates over the past few years, sug- Offering monetary incentives to, for example, shape a good be-
gests much can be learned by drawing on recent behaviour change at- haviour, such as reducing alcohol intake or encouraging exercise,
tempts (Almestahiri et al., 2017). Recent global pandemic has lead to an may crowd‐out individuals' motivations that are crucial for forming
increase in purchase, known as stockpiling behaviour, as many nicotine the desired behaviour (Gneezy et al., 2011). Titmuss (1970), after
consumers reported nicotine as a mechanism to cope with stress and comparing the American (mostly paid or providing other benefits)
anxiety (Yach, 2020). Thus, in this study, we focus on alcohol and tobacco and British (entirely voluntary and unpaid) systems of obtaining
consumption as two common high‐risk behaviours. blood for medical purposes, concluded that the paid system results in
1296 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

shortages and a lower quality of blood supply. He also noticed that good but also may extrinsically want to comply with social norms and
the social characteristics of contributors in Britain differed from the make a good impression. Research shows offering monetary in-
characteristics of blood contributors in the United States. In Britain, centives to encourage prosocial behaviour can backfire, especially if
they were representative of the population, whereas in America, the payment is made public (Ariely et al., 2009). For example, paying
they tended to have lower income and lower education. His con- volunteers to collect charitable donations or parents to pick up their
clusion was that paying for blood destroys an altruistic motivation to children from day care on‐time violates norms of altruism and re-
contribute. ciprocity which creates uncertainties as to the motivations behind
The crowding out effect occurs especially in conditions where the behaviours (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000). The payment dilutes the
the individual feels that the monetary incentive is offered as a result voluntary normative gesture increasing the risk of appearing selfish
of mistrust or underestimating the individual's intrinsic motivation and greedy. Offering the cash reward crowds out intrinsic and ex-
(Frey, 1993). Monetary incentives, especially a large monetary re- trinsic motivations by reframing the prosocial behaviour as a
ward, may be encouraging in the short‐term, however, they may monetary transaction making it unclear whether the individual is
weaken or even destroy an individual's intrinsic motivation in the engaging in the activity because it is the right thing to do or to look
long‐term (Gneezy et al., 2011). Extending the theory on the detri- good to others (Gneezy et al., 2011, p. 11).
mental effect of monetary incentives to the context of social inter- Although nonmonetary incentives can negatively influence the
actions, Jin and Huang (2014) find the demotivating effect of outcomes of online referrals, in an incentivized campaign, a sig-
monetary incentives especially when individuals need to send posi- nificantly greater number of messages are sent and a greater pro-
tive identity signals about themselves. Situations in which individuals portion of messages are successfully received (Michalski et al., 2012).
are offered money to perform a task that has an impact on others Further research is needed to reveal whether different types of in-
such as friends and acquaintances, the detrimental impact of a centives encourage or discourage people to engage online with
monetary reward can be even higher, due to the potential con- health‐related messages. The ethical considerations of social mar-
sequences on others (Jin & Huang, 2014). keting research, crowding out a citizen's right to undertake a parti-
Several theories can be brought in to describe what exactly cular behaviour change can be discussed from different aspects such
happens and how. Attribution theory suggests that people tend to as the process, methods and manipulation (Kennedy & Santos, 2019).
attribute their behaviour to the monetary reward and thus discount Fundamentally, the right to intervene in people's lives and putting
their natural interest in the activity with this overjustification un- restrictions on their freedoms in attempts towards a better society is
dermining their intrinsic motivation (Lepper et al., 1973). Cognitive a process‐related ethical issue (Lefebvre, 2011). In particular, when
evaluation theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985) also asserts that tangible social marketers adopt a top‐down approach, aspects such as
rewards given to stimulate engagement that are contingent upon transparency, accountability equality and nondiscrimination
task performance or completion are experienced as controlling which (Szablewska and Kubacki, 2019) of ethical issues and interventions
undermines people's intrinsic need for autonomy. are of greater concern, forcing the target audience to change their
As shown in Table 1 previous research demonstrates that in- behaviour or to cope the costs (Kennedy & Santos, 2019).
centives, especially monetary rewards, reduce the performance of With the method of implementation, the targeted study groups
altruistic activities through clashing with intrinsic other‐oriented and the choice of manipulation may have the potential to violate
motivations, as well as reducing people's self‐enhancement motives individuals' rights and challenging the autonomy of the targeted in-
(Dubé et al., 2017; Cui et al., 2014). As an instance, price discounts dividual (Smith, 2001, p. 134). The method of manipulation can also
bundled with a charitable donation, negatively impact the product have negative effects on participants trust, if strategies such as de-
demand, as a result of diminishing consumers' self‐inference of al- ceptive advertising and misleading claims are adopted (Darke &
truism, as well as how consumers feel about themselves (Dubé Ritchie, 2007; Darke et al., 2010). If participants feel fooled, they will
et al., 2017). This counter interaction of incentives is not as strong if lose their trust and become motivated to response defensively as
the task is simple, straightforward, rule‐based and does not require they will see new advertisements from the same source as another
high creativity and analytical/cognitive skills (Pink, 2011). Decreasing attempt to fool them. This will create a negative bias in their eva-
the perceived social costs related to the task or enhancing the fi- luations (Darke & Ritchie, 2007), resulting in ineffective advertising
nancial benefit by offering larger monetary rewards can improve the strategies.
effectiveness of a monetary reward (Jin & Huang, 2014). Though the debate on the best approaches towards addressing
The interaction effect between incentives and social image con- ethical issues around behaviour change remains ongoing, the mar-
cerns, as an extrinsic motivation, should also be considered on per- keting bottom‐up approach deemed the reasonable approach which
forming prosocial activities (Lacetera & Macis, 2010b). The crowding is built on promise instead of top‐down stick approach (e.g., taxing
out effect of monetary incentives also occurs for publicly visible cigarettes, further disadvantaging those in lower socioeconomic
prosocial behaviours that contribute to the common good such as positions (Eagle et al., 2015; Kennedy & Santos, 2019). More im-
donating blood, recycling and volunteering (Gneezy et al., 2011). portantly, imposing penalties may neutralise the moral costs an in-
Engaging in such activities can involve both intrinsic and extrinsic dividual pays for violating a norm. Staying longer in hospital (after
motivations. People may intrinsically enjoy contributing to the social the patient treatment was completed) as a result of imposing fees
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1297

TABLE 1 Selected research on motivations, incentives and their interaction in the context of prosocial behaviour and behaviour change

Authors Incentive Type Context Findings

Fehr and Falk (2002) Monetary Performance, behaviour Intrinsic motivations (e.g., social approval, reciprocity)
change interacts with monetary incentives to shape behaviour.
Monetary incentives may reduce the performance of
individuals through weakening their intrinsic motives.

Gneezy and Monetary Prosocial behaviour, that is, Monetary rewards (vs. none) decrease performance level, in
Rustichini (2000) collecting donations general. Where monetary incentive is offered, a higher
monetary reward (vs. a lower) produces higher
performance. A fixed (noncontingent on the performance)
monetary reward may not result in lowering intrinsic
motivation.

Shang et al. (2005) Extrinsic/intrinsic Online shopping Intrinsic motivations, for example, enjoyment and interest as
motivations the main reasons for online shopping are more effective
than incentives.

Ryu and Feick (2007) Monetary and Referral reward programmes Incentives increase likelihood of generating referral,
nonmonetary however, reward size does not matter. As there may be a
ceiling effect for the effectiveness of incentives, as well
as, increases in reward size may also increase
psychological and social costs through creating feelings of
guilt regarding the inequity of the exchange.

Mellström and Self/others‐oriented Prosocial behaviour (i.e., Offering a cash incentive bundled with a charitable donation
Johannesson (2008) blood donation) option mitigates the crowding out effect but does not lead
to greater donations.

Ariely et al. (2009) Intrinsic/extrinsic and Prosocial behaviour Offering extrinsic incentives may crowd out ‘image'
image motivation motivation, which is vital for prosocial behaviour.
Importance of image motivation is based on its visibility—
in a private situation, extrinsic rewards may increase
prosocial behaviour likelihood.

Lacetera and Macis Monetary/nonmonetary Prosocial activities (i.e., blood Intrinsically motivated existing donors are not averse to any
(2010a, 2010b) donation) type of incentives, however, they're unwilling to receive
cash reward (vs. in‐kind rewards: Coupons, T‐shirts,
mugs), they're likely to judge themselves as less greedy
when accepting nonmonetary rewards, thus, the social
recognition component attached to publicly announced
symbolic nonmonetary rewards plays a significant role.

Gneezy et al. (2011) Monetary Prosocial behaviour, lifestyle Monetary incentives crowd‐out intrinsic motivation in both
habits short‐ and long‐term. Incentives' design, type and their
interaction with intrinsic and social motivations must be
taken into account—they may come into conflict with
social norm, image concern and trust.

Berger and Monetary and Word‐of‐mouth (WOM) Nonmonetary rewards such as promotional giveaways,
Schwartz (2011) nonmonetary product samples and augmented products such as hats,
mugs and wristbands could increase overall WOM;
however, there is no such an effect for monetary rewards.

Hinz et al. (2011) Extrinsic/intrinsic Online referral Both monetary and nonmonetary incentives strongly increase
motivations the likelihood of online referral.

Michalski et al. (2012) Nonmonetary Viral marketing Incentives, that is, nonmonetary rewards, can result in
negative outcomes. A greater number of messages are
successfully received and further conveyed in the
nonincentivised viral campaign.

Cheung and Lee (2012) Intrinsic motivations e‐WOM intention Consumers' e‐WOM intention is significantly related to the
motives of sense of belonging (the most impact on e‐
WOM intention) and enjoyment of helping others.

(Continues)
1298 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

TABLE 1 (Continued)

Authors Incentive Type Context Findings

Verlegh et al. (2013) Monetary and Referral reward programmes Providing a reward has a negative impact on the referral
nonmonetary receiver. Offering a combination of monetary and
nonmonetary incentives to both senders and receivers
shares the benefits and changes actions from being
perceived as purely self‐serving to more altruistic.

Gneezy and Rey‐ Monetary Survey response rate Response rate increases with the size of monetary reward;
Biel (2014) however, its effectiveness flattens out after a certain
level. Even a relatively small noncontingent monetary gift
may cause reciprocity in some individuals.

Munzel and Kunz (2014) Intrinsic motivations Online review Altruism‐related motives (positive experience) outrank social
bonding motives, the possibility of warning other
consumers through the review and the opportunity to
voice complaints (negative experience).

Schulze et al. (2014) Nonmonetary Viral marketing Offering incentives (i.e., one free month of premium
membership) for online messages that promote high‐
utilitarian products is ineffective (may not be harmful
though), companies should avoid them since they come at
a cost to the firm. However, a positive effect is seen for
low‐utilitarian products.

Jin and Huang (2014) Monetary/in‐kind Referral reward programmes Offering incentives diminishes individuals' extrinsic and
rewards intrinsic motives, resulting in a crowding out effect.
Demotivating effect of monetary incentives is higher
when individuals need to send positive identity‐signals
about themselves. However, the effectiveness of a
monetary reward could be improved for example by
enhancing the financial benefit (offering larger monetary
rewards) or decreasing the perceived social costs related
to it.

Ashley and Tuten (2015) Monetary and User‐generated Brands that offer an incentive for participation have more
nonmonetary content (UGC) fans/followers and higher engagement scores. Intrinsic
motivations such as the sense of being entertained are
stronger motivator of engagement than informativeness
of the message (Luo, 2002).

Poch and Martin (2015) Extrinsic/intrinsic UGC Extrinsic rewards (economic incentives) result in more
positive intentions to create UGC than intrinsic
motivations. However, an effect for altruism is also
evident revealing that high altruism consumers are more
likely to create positive UGC.

Giebelhausen Self/others‐oriented Voluntary green programme Offering others‐oriented (vs. self‐oriented) incentives
et al. (2016) (e.g., recycling) increases satisfaction of volunteers in green programmes.
A 'mixed incentive bundle' including both types of 'self
and others‐oriented rewards is the most effective
incentive structure, giving participants a choice of
benefitting themselves or a third party.

Woolley and Extrinsic/intrinsic Exercise, different tasks Both extrinsic (earing money, impressing others) and intrinsic
Fishbach (2016) (having fun and enjoyment) incentives encourage long‐
term engagement. People value intrinsic incentive more
during an activity (vs. before or after) as they are
associated with an immediate delivery, whereas extrinsic
incentives will be valued similarly before, during and after
the activity.

Reimer and Extrinsic/intrinsic e‐WOM Monetary incentives include potential negative consequences
Benkenstein (2016) such as credibility loss through consumer uncertainty and
decreases the intrinsic motivations by causing a crowding
out effect, so extrinsic‐oriented incentives work at the
cost of intrinsic motivation.
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1299

TABLE 1 (Continued)

Authors Incentive Type Context Findings

Dubé et al. (2017) Monetary/nonmonetary Prosocial behaviour Offering incentives, especially large and moderate monetary
discounts, negatively affects consumer motivation and
reduces consumer engagement. Bundling a company's
charitable donations with the purchase of price
discounted movie tickets crowds out consumers' altruistic
behaviours.

Chu et al. (2018) Intrinsic motivations e‐WOM intention The need for 'self‐enhancement' positively influences social
media engagement and to some extent e‐WOM intention,
the sense of belonging, however, does not have a
significant impact on consumer engagement.

Vilnai‐Yavetz and Extrinsic/intrinsic e‐Commerce sharing Intrinsic motivations (e.g., altruism) are more important than
Levina (2018) extrinsic motives (e.g., financial rewards) for e‐commerce
sharing. Financial incentives (e.g., a discount) result in
crowding out effect, shifting individuals' motives from
intrinsic reasons (e.g., enjoyment) toward extrinsic ones.

Reimer and Monetary/Altruistic Online recommendation Monetary incentives negatively influence credibility of
Benkenstein (2018) incentives recommendations and the company image for review
reader, whereas altruistic incentives make online reviews
more trustworthy and lower scepticism, as well as
improving perceptions of the company and increase
purchase intentions.

Fletcher‐Brown Corporate social media Vulnerable consumer Corporate social media can facilitate resources for vulnerable
et al. (2020) content engagement consumers. Through providing resources and platforms
for sharing experiences, showing renewed strength,
supporting Destigmatization and providing a support
network.

(Holmas et al., 2010), or leaving children in childcare longer and Therefore, it is important to adopt and implement means of
picking them up late after the childcare centre introduced penalties behaviour change that are not discriminatory towards vulnerable
for longer stay kids (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000) are examples of population. Thus, in this study, we adopt a bottom‐up approach and
crowding out effects of imposing monetary penalties. On the con- offer prosocial intrinsic motivations as an alternative to monetary
trary, offering monetary incentives for behaviour change also has the incentives.
risk of making the behaviour change subjective to such extrinsic Built on the current literature on the influence of incentives on
incentives (Mortimer et al., 2013). online consumer engagement, we argue that offering a monetary
Monetary‐based strategies have been used in the past in both incentive discourages individuals through diminishing their motiva-
the top‐down social marketing and bottom‐up approaches towards tion and as a result decreases the level of online consumer engage-
behaviour change. In either approach, the vulnerable population with ment. The role of consumer motivation is especially critical for
low socioeconomic backgrounds that often the social marketing engaging in or promoting prosocial behaviours. Thus, we expect that
messages are targeted towards are at risk of being disadvantaged or offering an other‐oriented incentive generates a higher level of on-
manipulated. In the top‐down approach, monetary‐based means of line consumer engagement for health‐related messages compared to
behaviour change can be seen as punishment for those already offering a self‐oriented incentive. Therefore, we propose the fol-
classified as vulnerable, mostly those in lower socioeconomic posi- lowing hypotheses:
tions, further disadvantaging them (Eagle et al., 2015; Kennedy & Self‐oriented incentive:
Santos, 2019). On the contrary, in the bottom‐up approach, offering H1: Offering a self‐oriented, monetary incentive (vs. no incentive)
monetary incentives may work (changing the risky behaviour, but reduces (crowds out) online consumer engagement behaviour for health
only temporarily and until the incentive is available. Relying solely on ads on social media.
monetary incentives may again be resulted in a biased approach in Other‐oriented incentive:
behaviour change, as vulnerable and low‐income target audience are H2: Offering an other‐oriented, 'social good' incentive (vs. no in-
generally more susceptible towards financial incentives (Kennedy & centive) increases (crowds in) online consumer engagement for health
Santos, 2019). ads on social media.
1300 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

2.2 | Ad appeal and social media consumer participants to engage in the desired behaviour change (Brennan &
engagement behaviour Binney, 2010).
Several studies have investigated the impact of fear appeal in
Online messages are framed differently by different marketers. Po- behaviour change, across a wide range of communication settings
sitive versus negative and rational versus emotional framed mes- such as antidrinking (Bécheur et al., 2007), AIDS prevention
sages are used to grab people's attention (Sweeney et al., 2012). (Bennett, 1996; Marchand & Filiatrault, 2002), antismoking (Arthur &
Research shows that emotional (Berger, 2014; Dobele et al., 2007), Quester, 2004) and road safety (Rossiter & Thornton, 2004). The im-
positive (de Angelis et al., 2012; Previte et al., 2015; Tellis pact of fear appeal has been summarised in several meta analysis
et al., 2019), arousing (Berger & Milkman, 2012) messages with a studies, built around four main variables: Severity and susceptibility,
moderate level of controversy (Chen & Berger, 2013) are more likely response and self efficacy. Ruiter et al. (2014) reclassifying the severity
to be shared. Berger (2014) suggests that emotion regulation drives and susceptibility dimensions into threat and response and self efficacy
people to share more emotional content either to increase the va- into efficacy, found a significant interaction between the two major
lence of the content they share, or to share more psychologically categories. Threat was found effective only when the efficacy was high.
arousing content. Arousal remains important even when the message Similarly, efficacy was effective only under high threat condition.
content is surprising, interesting or practical as people are more Moreover, previous research has found that if perceived efficacy is low
active when in a state of excitement (Brooks & Schweitzer, 2011). (feeling helpless what to do), but the seriousness of the threat is ac-
Messages that activate emotions such as anger and fear are more knowledged by the individual, there is a chance of a negative effect on
likely to go viral compared to messages conveying deactivating persuasion. That is people may become defensive and start engaging in
emotions such as sadness (Berger & Milkman, 2012). Furthermore, practicing the risky behaviour even more intensively (Goldenberg &
research shows that messages framed with certain characteristics Arndt, 2008; Ruiter et al., 2014).
such as imagination, fun, intrigue have a greater potential to go viral Previte et al. (2015) highlight the importance of emotions and
(Tellis et al., 2019; Woerndl et al., 2008). emotional appeals in positive responses to moderate‐drinking mes-
Ad appeal plays a critical role in developing effective health sages. Alhabash et al. (2013) report the effects of emotional appeal
promotion campaigns (Devlin et al., 2007; Michaelidou et al., 2008). on persuasiveness and virality of online messages on Facebook.
Emotional appeals are commonly used in health communication to Therefore, we propose the following hypothesis regarding message
grab attention and provoke action (Previte et al., 2015). Fear‐related appeal and online engagement:
emotions, for instance, are used to convey fearful and alarming in- H3: Emotional appeals (vs. rational appeals) increase online con-
formation about negative health consequences (Brennan & sumer engagement for health ads on social media.
Binney, 2010; Devlin et al., 2007). Existing studies examine the Previous research shows that almost half of online brand‐
success of scare tactics in social and public health messages and supported incentives are monetary (Ashley & Tuten, 2015), which
report varying results. Some argue that the application of scare are generally effective; however, when incentives are offered for
tactics is ineffective in particular for influencing younger people prosocial online behaviours, a crowding out effect is expected. This
(Duke et al., 1993; Hastings et al., 2004), whereas other studies find means that offering incentives may reduce individuals' motivations
positive outcomes (Biener et al., 2000; Witte & Allen, 2000). In the leading to lower levels of engagement (Dubé et al., 2017). However,
context of health/social behaviour change, many studies demon- such crowding out effect may be neutralised by other factors and
strate the critical role of emotion message framing. Noble et al. triggers that are used to encourage and motivate engagement.
(2014) examine rational versus emotional appeals for public health Neutralising the crowding out effect of offering monetary incentives
ads and find emotional appeals are more effective than rational for pro‐social engagement is expected if the incentive is offered in
appeals. conjunction with message appeals that activate human behaviour.
Fear appeal that is defined as “a persuasive communication that Berger (2014) suggests that emotion is a major trigger for people to
attempts to arouse fear to promote precautionary motivation and share more content. Activating emotions such as anger and fear may
self‐protective action” (Rogers & Deckner, 1975), offers information generate greater engagement than messages conveying deactivating
mainly in two forms. First, exposing the susceptible participant to a emotions such as sadness (Berger & Milkman, 2012). Such emotional
threat that is believed to arouse fear (e.g., presenting a smoker with a messages, therefore, are likely to positively moderate the effect of
message that talks about the risks of smoking associated with can- incentives, specifically, softening the crowding out effect of in-
cer). Second, fear appeal can be presented as an opportunity to centives. Moreover, the altruism and egoism (prosocial and self‐care)
minimise and neutralise the threat (Ruiter et al., 2014). In other motivations can also be used to distinguish emotions
words, offering solutions to prevent and take a health preventive (Doenyas, 2017). For instance people with other‐oriented motiva-
action. The impact of negative emotional appeals on persuading tions tend to share emotions of sensitive and sympathy or shame,
behaviour change has been studied extensively in the literature facilitating cooperative social behaviour (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
(Agrawal & Duhachek, 2010; Arthur & Quester, 2004; Bécheur On the contrary, the emotions of self‐oriented individuals with ego‐
et al., 2007; Brennan & Binney, 2010; Duhachek et al., 2012). focused emotions are associated with satisfaction or frustration of
Creating a feeling of discomfort, negative appeals encourage individual's internal attributes such as their own needs, goals, desires
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1301

and abilities, such as anger (Doenyas, 2017; Roth, 2008). We argue 3 | OVE RVIEW O F ST UD IES
that people tend to participate and engage on social media with
health messages for altruistic reasons to assist others as well as for Studies 1a, 1b, 2a and 2b investigate the effectiveness of self‐
social recognition motivations, both of which are emotionally oriented versus other‐oriented incentives in generating online en-
charged. Offering other‐oriented, social good incentives is consistent gagement for health‐related ads, under two different ad appeal
with an emotional appeal, whereas offering a self‐oriented, monetary conditions: Emotional appeal and rational appeal. Facebook was
incentive triggers more of a rational, cost benefit analysis. Therefore, chosen as the experiment platform for several reasons. Facebook is
we expect an interaction effect to occur and propose the following still the most popular social media platform (Statista, 2021). Also,
hypothesis: because participants needed to join groups to be exposed to the ads,
H4: There is an interaction effect between incentive type and ad incentive messages and so forth, Facebook was a better option as it
appeal with an other‐oriented, social good incentive boosting online would allow us to create groups, where such a feature was not
consumer engagement for health messages with emotional appeals. available on other platforms like Instagram. Moreover, to control for
According to schema theory, and spreading activation theory, platform inconsistencies, we only used Facebook across all four ex-
males and females respond differently to stimuli, that aligns better periments. In studies 1a & 2a (lab experiments) participants were
with their gender schema. The semnatic network structures in our instructed to join the study's Facebook page—a dedicated Facebook
brain known as schemas enable us to process information and cues, page which was set up for the purpose of this project. Once parti-
receiving, encoding, registering and decoding (Anderson, 1988). The cipants joined (became a Facebook friend), they then were randomly
gender‐related schemas which start forming in early childhood, af- assigned to one of the three Facebook groups that were set up for
fects how we communicate, evaluate cues and respond. According to the three incentive conditions. Group 1: Monetary incentive
spreading activation theory, once a node is activated by external (a monetary incentive has an explicit monetary value; in addition to
cues and stimuli, it spreads through highly connected nodes facil- cold, hard cash, monetary rewards can take the form of: Gift cards,
itating the process of encoding and decoding the cues. The greater bonuses, profit sharing, etc). Group 2: Social good incentive and
the degree of connection between the external cue and the existing Group 3: Control group. In each group, participants were simulta-
concepts and nodes, the higher the chance of stronger response, due neously exposed to two ads, one framed with emotional appeal and
to easier and more enjoyable stimuli comprehension (Bower, 1983). one framed with rational appeal (the two ads were identical across
Previous research for instance has studied that moderating role of three groups). Further details regarding incentive conditions and
gender in comedic violence advertising perception. Yoon and Kim emotional/rational ads are provided in the following sections, dis-
(2014) argue that highly masculine individuals tend to have strong cussing each study.
nodes and concepts associated with aggressiveness, dominance and Participants were able to contribute to the project (if they wished)
forcefulness (Bem, 1974), thus they're expected to respond differ- by liking, commenting, sharing the ads. Though some participants only
ently to comedic violence compared to less masculine, individuals. viewed the ads with no engagement, some other participants got en-
Feminine individual on the contrary, tend to have existing nodes gaged in multiple ways, for example, by liking the ads, commenting on the
highly connected to values and concepts like warmth, compassion ads, liking/replying to others' comments, sharing and so forth. The de-
and sympathy (Bem, 1974; Yoon & Kim, 2014). Thus, stimuli like pendent variable: 'Total Engagement' was calculated on the ad level—as
violence won't be generally be supported by feminine individuals, the sum of all the activities (e.g., like, comment, share) on each ad. For
resulting in less favourable responses, both attitudinal and beha- example, if emotional ad in condition 1 received 35 likes, 15 comments, 4
vioural. Putrevu et al. (2004) associate the differing response to shares, the total engagement for this ad was equal to 54 and the average
sensory stimuli among men and women to the differences in their activity was 1.450, which was calculated as the ratio of total engagement
psychological orientation, in particular, Agency and Communion. to the number of participants in each group. The minimum sample size of
Agency as a characteristic associated with men denotes a single fo- 154 volunteer participants, in study 1a (health issue alcohol), allowed us
cused orientation, which women with Communion characteristic to assign 51–52 participants to each incentive condition which is a de-
tend to be both self‐focused and other‐focused. Previous research cent sample size for experiments. Study 2a, which was a replication of
suggest that such characters are also associated with traditional study 1a with a different health issue (smoking), had a sample size of 218
societal roles, men having more dominant roles, versus women oc- of volunteer participants, which allowed us to assign 70–72 participants
cupying more accommodating roles (Hall, 1984; Putrevu et al., 2004). to each incentive condition which is a decent sample size for experi-
Men and women are also different in responding to verbal and visual mental studies. Study 1b and 2b which were real‐life online experiments,
communication cues, where due to their sensory capabilities, women on Facebook, attracted more than 1000 Facebook users, overall:
are likely to accurately process the cues (Chamblee et al., 1993). Due
to established gender differences in responding to external stimuli, in 1. Study 1a (Alcohol): 154 university students volunteered
this study, we examine the role of gender in individuals' responses to 2. Study 1b (Alcohol): 588 self‐selected Facebook users participated
health message. by reacting to the 'sponsored' ads (e.g., liking, commenting,
sharing)
H5: Men and women respond differently to health‐related messages. 3. Study 2a (Smoking): 218 university students volunteered
1302 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

4. Study 2b (Smoking): 499 self‐selected Facebook users partici- and ad appeal (emotional vs. rational) as the within‐subjects factor,
pated by reacting to the 'sponsored' ads (e.g., liking, commenting, while controlling for a health issue (alcohol). The monetary condition
sharing) offered a chance to win a $50 gift card and the social good condition
encouraged participants with the statement: 'Participate and con-
Manipulation Checks. As manipulation checks, participants were tribute towards the social good', whereas the no incentive control
asked to rate their feelings regarding each ad on a 5‐point likert scale condition simply stated: 'Participate in the project'.
(1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree), expressing their feel- Participants in each group were simultaneously exposed to
ings about the extent to which each ad was emotional versus two messages on the effects of alcohol. Both messages featured a
rational. black and white, side facing head shot of a young adult female
Our manipulation check for ad appeal indicated that subjects drinking a glass of champagne. The rational appeal stated, 'Having
perceived the ads as intended. Though the emotional alcohol ad was more than two alcoholic drinks a day raises your stroke risk by
perceived as emotional versus rational, emotional ad for alcohol: 50%' and showed the standard image. The emotional appeal stated,
Memotional = 3.90 versus Mrational = 2.88; t(80) = 5.954, p < 0.000; the 'Heavy drinking damages your brain' and showed the standard
rational alcohol ad was perceived as rational versus emotional, ra- image with stylised shattered glass and a bullet leaving the back of
tional ad for alcohol: Mrational = 4.09 versus Memotioal = 2.69; t her head.
(79) = 8.659, p < 0.000. Similar results also achieved for the smoking Participants were asked to take part in the online discussion by
ads. Emotional smoking ad was perceived as emotional versus ra- sharing, commenting and liking the two posts on their Facebook
tional, emotional ad for smoking: Memotional = 4.04 versus Mrational = group page, with no moderation or intervention from the re-
2.99; t(79) = 6.603, p < 0.000, whereas the rational smoking ad was searchers. During a 2‐week period, participants' level of engagement
perceived as more rational than emotional, rational ad for smoking: (the number of likes, comments, shares and other activities such as
Mrational = 4.20 versus Memotioal = 2.83; t(79) = 9.149, p < 0.000. liking and replying to others' comments and mentioning others),
were recorded for each message posted in each group. Total level of
engagement was the dependent variable and calculated as the sum of
3.1 | Study 1a: Laboratory experiment, health all Facebook engagement metrics, each given an equal weighting
issue: Alcohol (Lim et al., 2017).

Study 1a uses a laboratory experiment on Facebook to test all four


hypotheses for the health issue of alcohol. According to H1 and H2, 3.1.2 | Results and discussion of study 1a
we anticipate a lower level of engagement under the self‐oriented
monetary condition and a higher level of engagement under the As shown in Table 2 and Figure 1, a repeated‐measures analysis of
other‐oriented social good condition, compared to the no incentive variance (ANOVA) reveals a significant main effect for incentives, as
control condition. This study also compares two different alcohol a between‐subjects factor, on the level of engagement (Fengage-
messages, one message framed with an emotional appeal and one ment_incentive = 32.37, p = 0.000). To probe the interaction, we ran
with a rational appeal. On the basis of H3, we expect a greater level spotlight analysis across the levels of incentive as our categorical
of engagement with the emotional appeal compared to the rational independent variable using model 1 in PROCESS V3 by Hayes
appeal. Finally, according to H4, we anticipate a boost in online en- (2013). This model allows for an interaction between a categorical
gagement as a result of the interaction between the other‐oriented variable (i.e., incentives) and another categorical variable (i.e., ad
social good incentive and emotional ad appeal. appeal). As predicted, overall the self‐oriented monetary incentive
generates the lowest level of online engagement, a spotlight analysis
shows although the difference between the monetary and the no
3.1.1 | Method incentive conditions is not significant overall, it is marginally sig-
nificant for the rational ad appeal (at 0.10 level). Therefore, H1 is
University students aged over 18 years were invited to participate in only marginally supported for the rational ad appeal (MDrational_NI‐MI =
the study for course credit. In total, 154 students volunteered to 0.448, p = 0.061), as opposed to the emotional ad appeal
take part in study 1a. Participants were randomly assigned to one of (MDemotional_NI‐MI = 0.168, p = 0.701).
three Facebook groups (monetary, social good and no incentive). The social good condition generates the highest level of total online
Although Facebook groups were public groups where member ac- engagement behaviour compared to the monetary incentive (MDsocial
tivities were visible to the participants' Facebook friends outside the good>monetary = 2.147, p = 0.000) and compared to no incentive (MDsocial
experiment, the group privacy settings were set up to prevent people good>no incentive = 1.838, p = 0.000), providing full support for H2. In sup-

outside the experiments joining the groups. A 3 × 2 mixed experi- port of H3 the main effect of ad appeal is significant (Fengagement_ad ap-

mental design conducted on Facebook manipulated three incentive peal = 35.527, p = 0.000), with participants significantly more engaged
conditions: Self‐oriented (monetary), other‐oriented (social good) with the emotional message compared to the rational message (MDe-
and a control condition (no incentive), as the between‐subjects factor motional>rational = 0.944), regardless of incentive type.
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1303

T A B L E 2 Results of the repeated‐


Experimental condition Post‐hoc F (p Value)
measure analysis of variance (study 1a)
Incentive • SG (Mean = 6.575, SD = 5.182) 32.37 (0.000)
• MI (Mean = 2.281, SD = 1.883)
• NI (Mean = 2.898, SD = 2.223)

Ad appeal • Emotional (Mean = 2.431, SD = 3.342) 35.527 (0.000)


• Rational (Mean = 1.487, SD = 1.512)

Incentive*ad appeal interactions • SG*EA (Mean = 4.534, SD = 4.024) 24.476 (0.000)


• SG*RA (Mean = 2.041, SD = 1.593)
• MI*EA (Mean = 1.296, SD = 1.313)
• MI*RA (Mean = 0.986, SD = 0.992)
• NI*EA (Mean = 1.464, SD = 1.312)
• NI*RA (Mean = 1.435, SD = 1.557)

Abbreviations: EA, emotional ad apeal; MI, monetary incentive; NI, no incentive; RA, rational ad apeal;
SG, social good.

ad along with an other‐oriented social good incentive. Next, we fol-


low with an online experiment recruiting real FB users to determine
whether these results hold.

3.2 | Study 1b: Online experiment, health issue:


Alcohol

Study 1b uses an online experiment on Facebook to test the same


incentive and ad appeal experimental conditions as study 1a. Ac-
cording to H1, we expect the self‐oriented monetary condition to
create lower levels of engagement compared to the no incentive
F I G U R E 1 Total engagement behaviour by ad appeal and condition and for H2, we expect the social good condition to gen-
incentive condition—Alcohol: Laboratory setting (study 1a) erate higher levels of engagement compared to the no incentive
condition. Additionally, for H3, we anticipate that emotional ads to
generate a greater level of engagement compared to rational ads,
and for H4, we predict an interaction effect between incentive and
Moreover, in support of H4, the interaction effect between in- ad appeal. In this study, we perform further analyses and control for
centives and ad appeal is significant for engagement level (Fincentive*ad participants' gender, to find out whether gender moderates the re-
appeal = 24.476, p = 0.000). The interaction effect between incentive sults and plays a role in stimulating individuals' reactions to alcohol
condition and ad appeal suggests that the other‐oriented social good messages (H5).
incentive has the greatest impact on the level of engagement under
the emotional alcohol message (Msocial good*emotional = 4.534), com-
pared to the rational alcohol message (Msocial good*rational = 2.041). On 3.2.1 | Method
the other end of the spectrum, the combination of monetary in-
centive and rational appeal (Mmonetary incentive*rational = 0.986) and the Study 1b is a 3 × 2 online experiment recruiting real FB users, ma-
combination of no incentive with rational appeal (Mno incentive*ra- nipulating three between‐subject incentive conditions (self‐oriented
tional = 1.435) generate the lowest levels of engagement. monetary $50 gift card vs. other‐oriented social good encourage-
A follow‐up spotlight analysis shows that the interaction of so- ment vs. no incentive) and two between‐subject ad appeal conditions
cial good incentive and emotional ad appeal generates more en- (emotional vs. rational), controlling for the health issue alcohol. A
gagement compared to social good and rational appeal. Moreover, separate Facebook page was set up for each incentive condition
the interaction of monetary incentive and emotional appeal is mar- where the study's two ads were posted. Instead of randomly as-
ginally greater than the rational appeal. signing participants to separate Facebook groups, the two messages
Overall, the findings of study 1a support hypotheses H2, H3 and were promoted to potential participants through Facebook adver-
H4 and partially support H1. These results from the online Facebook tising, featured as 'sponsored' ads by Facebook. All incentive con-
experiment suggest the most effective way to stimulate consumer ditions were allocated the same budget, running for the same
engagement for an alcohol‐related health issue is to use an emotional duration of time (2 weeks). The study was promoted to Facebook
1304 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

users over the age of 18, living in Australia, United States, United
Kingdom and New Zealand and was conducted over a period of 2
weeks. Similar to study 1a, the number of likes, comments, shares
and other Facebook activities such as liking and replying to others'
comments and mentioning friends were recorded for each partici-
pant's engagement. Total engagement behaviour, computed as the
summation of all Facebook activities, with each given an equal
weighting, was used as the dependant variable for both the rational
and emotional messages. In study 2b, 588 self‐selected Facebook
users who were exposed to the promoted ads chose to react to
the ads.

3.2.2 | Results and discussion F I G U R E 2 Total engagement behaviour by ad appeal and


incentive condition—Alcohol: Online experiment setting (study 1b)

ANOVA results in Table 3 and Figure 2 show a significant main effect for
incentives (Fengagement_incentive = 8.708, p = 0.000). Consistent with study
1a results, even though the average engagement for the monetary in- The main effect of ad appeal is also significant (Fengagement_ad
centive condition is greater than no incentive, there is no significant appeal = 16.848, p = 0.000). In line with study 1a results, the emotional
difference between the two providing no support for H1. The social good appeal ad for alcohol generates greater online engagement com-
incentive again is the most effective in generating online engagement for pared to the rational ad (MDemotional>rational = 0.248, p = 0.000). These
the alcohol health ads. As expected, the average engagement level with results provide further support for H3. Looking at the interaction
the other‐oriented social good incentive is significantly greater than the between incentive type and ad appeal (Fincentive*ad appeal = 2.860,
self‐oriented monetary incentive (MDsocial good>monetary = 0.136, p = 0.000) p = 0.058), results suggest some marginal support for H4 (at 0.10
and also significantly greater than the no incentive condition (MDsocial level). Again, the combination of an emotional appeal with a social
good>no incentive = 0.258, p = 0.002). These results again support hypoth- good incentive generates the highest level of engagement for the
esis H2. alcohol message (Msocial good*emotional = 1.546). Similar to study 1a, the

TABLE 3 Results of univariate analysis of variance (study 1b)

Experimental condition Post‐hoc F (p Value)

Incentive • SG (Mean = 1.24, SD = 0.766) 5.989 (0.000)


• MI (Mean = 1.09, SD = 0.469)
• NI (Mean = 1.06, SD = 0.251)

Ad appeal • Emotional (Mean = 1.21, SD = 0.687), 16.848 (0.000)


• Rational (Mean = 1.04, SD = 0.265),

Incentive*ad appeal interactions • SG*EA (Mean = 1.546, SD = 0.961) 2.860 (0.058)


• SG*RA (Mean = 1.095, SD = 0.412)
• MI*EA (Mean = 1.220, SD = 0.658)
• MI*RA (Mean = 1.030, SD = 0.159)
• NI*EA (Mean = 1.114, SD = 0.285)
• NI*RA (Mean = 1.011, SD = 0.204)

Gender*ad appeal*incentive • SG*Female*EA (Mean = 1.893, SD = 0.119) 5.757 (0.003)


• SG*Female*RA (Mean = 1.094, SD = 0.112)
• SG*Male*EA (Mean = 1.200, SD = 0.07)
• SG*Male*RA (Mean = 1.097, SD = 0.084)
• MI*Female*EA (Mean = 1.143, SD = 0.138)
• MI*Female*RA (Mean = 1.025, SD = 0.100)
• MI*Male*EA (Mean = 1.297, SD = 0.077)
• MI*Male*RA (Mean = 1.035, SD = 0.062)
• NI*Female*EA (Mean = 1.143, SD = 0.138)
• NI*Female*RA (Mean = 0.972, SD = 0.105)
• NI*Male*EA (Mean = 1.086, SD = 0.056)
• NI*Male*RA (Mean = 1.050, SD = 0.060)

Abbreviations: EA, emotional ad apeal; MI, monetary incentive; NI, no incentive; RA, rational ad apeal; SG, social good.
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1305

combination of no incentive with rational appeal (Mno incentive*ra- three incentive groups. In each group, participants were simulta-
tional = 1.011) and the combination of monetary incentive and rational neously exposed to the same two messages on the effects of
appeal (Mmonetary incentive*rational = 1.030) generate the lowest levels of smoking, one message framed with a rational appeal and one with an
engagement. Overall, regardless of different experimental groups emotional appeal. Both messages showed a black and white head
and participant characteristics of age group and geographical loca- shot of a young adult male smoking a cigarette. The rational message
tion, the results of study 1b are consistent and confirm study 1a stated: 'Smokers are four times more likely to have a stroke'. The
results. emotional message stated: 'Smoking Kills' and showed a stylised
Though prior research shows mixed results, there is some evi- image of a gun made of cigarette smoke aimed at the young man's
dence indicating women respond more positively to social incentives head. Participants were instructed to engage with the messages in
(Croson and Gneezy, 2009) and males to monetary incentives (Ariely forms of sharing, commenting and liking posts on their Facebook
et al., 2009). Taking gender into account shows a significant three‐ group page, without any intervention from researchers. Participants'
way interaction (Table 2 and Figure 3), supporting H5. Both women total level of engagement was recorded for each ad posted in each
and men engage most with emotional appeals, but women engage group. The sum of all engagement types was used as study's de-
the most when the other‐oriented social good incentive is offered pendent variable (Lim et al., 2017).
(Mfemale*social good*emotional = 1.893), whereas men engage most when
the self‐oriented monetary incentive is offered (Mmen*monetary*emo-
tional = 1.297). Offering no incentive in conjunction with a rational 3.3.2 | Results and discussion of study 2a
message is the least engaging for women, whereas for men offering a
monetary incentive with a rational message is less effective (Mfema- Repeated‐measures ANOVA results in Table 4 and Figure 4 indicate
le*no incentive*rational = 0.970, Mmale*monetary*rational = 1.035). Spotlight no main effect for incentives (Fengagement_incentive = 1.33, p = 0.267)
analysis shows females are significantly more engaged than males as the between‐subject factor. Follow‐up spotlight analysis in-
(MDsocial good*gender = 0.345, p = 0.014), but only for the social good dicates that the monetary incentive condition (Mno incentive = 1.465)
incentive. generates greater engagement compared to no incentive (Mno in-

centive = 1.760), which is opposite to H1. The social good incentive


does produce the highest level of total online engagement
3.3 | Study 2a: Laboratory experiment replication,
health issue: Smoking

Study 2a replicates study 1a for a different health issue: Smoking. As


in study 1a, this study uses the same experimental design, method
and approach, but for smoking. The same three incentive conditions
(monetary, social good, vs. no incentive) and two ad appeals (emo-
tional vs. rational) examine participants' total level of social media
engagement (likes, comments, shares, replies to comments). Similar
to study 1a, we expect to see the self‐oriented monetary incentive
condition having a lower level of engagement (H1) and the other‐
oriented social good incentive having a higher level of engagement
(H2) compared to the no incentive condition. Furthermore, regarding
the ad appeal (H3), we expect to see more engagement with the
emotional ad compared to the rational ad. Finally, for H4 we expect
to see an interaction effect between different types of incentives and
ad appeals on social media engagement.

3.3.1 | Method

This study uses the same 3 × 2 mixed experimental design on Face-


book as in study 1a. Also, the three incentive conditions (self‐
oriented monetary vs. other‐oriented social good vs. no incentive)
are between‐subject factors and the two ad appeal conditions
(emotional vs. rational) are within‐subject factors, controlling for a F I G U R E 3 (a) Female and (b) male engagement behaviour by ad
health issue (smoking). In total, 218 university students participated appeal and incentive condition—Alcohol: Online experiment setting
for course credit. Participants were randomly assigned to one of (study 1b)
1306 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

behaviour (Mengagement_social good incentive = 1.849) which is consistent


with H2; however, the differences across the three incentive con-
ditions are not significant and provide no support for H1 or H2.
These results for incentives regarding engagement with the smok-
ing ads differ compared to alcohol ads.
Taking ad appeal into account, the results show a significant impact
on the level of engagement (Fad appeal = 11.36, p = 0.001). Similar to stu-
dies 1a and 1b, the emotional appeal generates significantly higher levels
of engagement compared to the rational appeal (Memotional = 1.86 and
Mrational = 1.52, MD = 0.34, p = 0.001), providing support for H3. The
overall interaction effect between incentives and ad appeal is insignif-
icant for social media engagement (Fincentive*ad appeal = 0.610, p = 0.554).
Further analysis shows that the interaction of the social good incentive
and emotional appeal generates significantly greater engagement com- F I G U R E 4 Total engagement behaviour by ad appeal and
incentive condition—Smoking: Laboratory setting (study 2a)
pared to social good and rational ad appeal (MDsocial good_rational mes-

sage = 0.493, p = 0.008), which is partially consistent with H4.

subject ad appeals, for alcohol. Study 2b's emotional and rational


3.4 | Study 2b: Online experiment replication, smoking messages were posted on separate Facebook pages
health issue: Smoking designed for each incentive condition and promoted through
Facebook sponsored ads. Total engagement was calculated the same
Study 2b is an online experiment recruiting real FB users, replicating the as in the previous studies. Study 2b was promoted to Facebook users
experimental conditions and recruitment approach in study 1b, but using over the age of 18, living in Australia, United States, United Kingdom
the same smoking ads used in study 2a. This study again tests whether a and New Zealand. A total of 499 self‐selected Facebook users
self‐oriented monetary incentive discourages engagement (H1), an other‐ participated over a period of 2 weeks.
oriented social good incentive encourages engagement (H2) compared to
no incentive condition, whether emotional appeals encourage higher le-
vels of engagement than rational appeals (H3) and whether an interac- 3.4.2 | Results and discussion
tion effect exists between different types of incentives and ad appeals on
social media engagement (H4). Finally, we examine whether gender in- Findings in Table 5 and Figure 5 show Facebook users who engaged
fluences the interaction between ad appeal and incentive (H5). reacted differently to different incentives and ad appeals. One‐way
ANOVA results reveal a significant main effect for incentives (Fen-
gagement_incentive = 14.005, p = 0.000) and a significant main effect for
3.4.1 | Method ad appeal (Fengagement_ad appeal = 10.053, p = 0.002). Post‐hoc tests
indicate those Facebook users attracted to the smoking ad under the
Study 2b uses the same 3 × 2 experimental design as in study 1b, monetary incentive are significantly more engaged compared to the
with three between‐subject incentive conditions and two between‐ social good incentive (MDmonetary>social good = 0.164, p = 0.000) and no

T A B L E 4 Results of the repeated‐


Experimental condition Post‐hoc F (p Value)
measure analysis of variance (Study 2a)
Incentive • SG (Mean = 1.849, SD = 1.371) 1.33 (0.267)
• MI (Mean = 1.760, SD = 1.867)
• NI (Mean = 1.465, SD = 1.104)

Ad appeal • Emotional (Mean = 1.86, SD = 1.803) 11.36 (0.001)


• Rational (Mean = 1.52, SD = 1.503)

Incentive*ad appeal interactions • SG*EA (Mean = 2.096, SD = 1.757) 0.610 (0.554)


• SG*RA (Mean = 1.603, SD = 1.371)
• MI*EA (Mean = 1.904, SD = 2.148)
• MI*RA (Mean = 1.616, SD = 1.890)
• NI*EA (Mean = 1.583, SD = 1.411)
• NI*RA (Mean = 1.347, SD = 1.152)

Abbreviations: EA, emotional ad apeal; MI, monetary incentive; NI, no incentive; RA, rational ad apeal;
SG, social good.
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1307

incentive (MDmonetary>no incentive = 0.212, p = 0.000) conditions. These


results do not support H1 or H2, as against our expectations the
monetary incentive generated greater engagement than both the
social good and no incentive conditions. Ad appeal also has a dif-
ferent effect on engagement compared to the results in studies 1a
and 1b. Unlike studies 1a, 1b and 2a, the rational ad about smoking in
this study is more effective in generating online engagement com-
pared to the emotional message (MDrational>emotional = 0.101,
p = 0.002), therefore, H3 is not supported in this study.
The interaction of ad appeal and incentives is also significant
(Fincentive*ad_appeal = 18.291, p = 0.000). The combination of a mone-
tary incentive with a rational message is the most effective (Mmone-
tary*rational = 1.447), followed by the combination of a social good
incentive with an emotional appeal as the second most effective F I G U R E 5 Total engagement behaviour by ad appeal and
incentive condition—Smoking: Online experiment setting (study 2b)
(Msocial good*emotional = 1.109). Offering no incentive while using a ra-
tional ad appeal has the least impact on online engagement for the
health issue of smoking (Mno incentive*rational = 1.010). Spotlight ana-
lysis reveals that the rational appeal is significantly more effective self‐oriented monetary incentive compared to the emotional appeal
than the emotional appeal when a monetary incentive is offered (MD = 0.455, p = 0.001). This suggests a monetary incentive stimu-
(MD = 0.404, p = 0.000). However, there is no a significant difference lates males' online engagement but only for rational messages, thus,
for the social good or no incentive conditions. H5 is marginally supported.
As in study 1b, we further examine the effect of gender in A possible explanation for these different results may be due to
generating online engagement, testing H5. Figure 6 shows that for self‐selection related to the topic or to the ad. A significantly higher
females, similar to the other studies, offering the other‐oriented number of males (n = 265) than female (n = 167) reacted to smoking
social good incentive through an emotional appeal is the most ef- ads, compared to a significantly higher number of females (n = 300)
fective combination. However, unlike other studies, males engage than males (n = 199) who reacted to alcohol ads in study 1b. Due to
significantly more with the rational appeal if they are offered the the settings of our online experiment recruiting real FB users, the

TABLE 5 Results of univariate analysis of variance (study 2b)

Experimental condition Post‐hoc F (p Value)

Incentive • SG (Mean = 1.08, SD = 0.299) 14.005 (0.000)


• MI (Mean = 1.19, SD = 0.565)
• NI (Mean = 1.04, SD = 0.209)

Ad appeal • Rational (Mean = 1.170, SD = 0.424) 10.053 (0.002)


• Emotional (Mean = 1.069, SD = 0.281)

Incentive*ad appeal interactions • SG*EA (Mean = 1.109 (SD = 0.035) 18.291 (0.000)
• SG*RA (Mean = 1.053 (SD = 0.038)
• MI*RA (Mean = 1.447 (SD = 0.054)
• MI*EA (Mean = 1.043 (SD = 0.043)
• NI*EA (Mean = 1.056 (SD = 0.030)
• NI*RA (Mean = 1.010 (SD = 0.034)

Gender*ad appeal*incentive • SG*Female*EA (Mean = 1.155, SD = 0.037) 2.53 (0.081)


• SG*Female*RA (Mean = 1.074, SD = 0.047)
• SG*Male*RA (Mean = 1.095, SD = 0.089)
• SG*Male*EA (Mean = 1.091, SD = 0.071)
• MI*Female*RA (Mean = 1.00, SD = 0.11)
• MI*Female*EA (Mean = 1.00, SD = 0.055)
• MI*Male*RA (Mean = 1.515, SD = 0.071)
• MI*Male*EA (Mean = 1.060, SD = 0.058)
• NI*Female*RA (Mean = 1.00, SD = 0.036)
• NI*Female*EA (Mean = 1.00, SD = 0.053)
• NI*Male*EA (Mean = 1.079, SD = 0.043)
• NI*Male*RA (Mean = 1.026, SD = 0.066)

Abbreviations: EA, emotional ad apeal; MI, monetary incentive; NI, no incentive; RA, rational ad apeal; SG, social good.
1308 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

social media platform. The results of the experiments reveal that


depending on the incentive and ad appeal, people engage differently
with smoking and alcohol messages. Offering a socially good in-
centive significantly encourages online engagement, especially if the
health‐related message is framed as an emotional ad. Studies 1b and
2b provide further understanding of how people react and get en-
gaged with social messages in real life and what are the most ef-
fective combinations of incentive and message appeal for males and
females. Our findings provide evidence that for women other‐
oriented incentives with emotional ad appeals is the most effective
combination regardless of the health issue, whereas men show a
greater level of engagement when they are offered self‐oriented
monetary incentives in combination with emotional ad appeals for
alcohol but rational appeals for smoking. When offering no incentive,
rational and emotional ad appeals generate similar amounts of online
engagement for males regardless of the health issue. For females
offering no incentive combined with a rational ad appeal generates
the least online engagement only with the alcohol ads.
From a theoretical perspective, this study contributes to-
wards a better understanding of the crowding out effect and the
conflicting impact of offering a monetary, self‐oriented incentive
for a prosocial behaviour. This study sheds light on the interac-
tion of different incentives and ad appeals in generating online
social media engagement for health issues, where people tend to
F I G U R E 6 (a) Female and (b) male engagement behaviour by ad
have other‐oriented, social recognition motivations to partici-
appeal and incentive condition—Smoking: Online experiment setting
pate and engage with a message. Therefore, this study con-
(study 2b)
tributes to the literature on incentives by capturing the impact
incentives may have in neutralising or diminishing personal mo-
ads were advertised to people in their Facebook newsfeed as tivations if used without taking into account the context, or
sponsored ads, thus participation in the study was completely self‐ without considering the interactions of incentives with other
selected based on individuals' interest in the ad or involvement with elements of an ad campaign such as ad appeal. Overall, this study
the topic. All the ads were black and white head shots, but the al- does not find strong support for 'the crowding out theory' about
cohol ads featured a female model and the smoking ads featured the detrimental effects of monetary incentives, which is most
a male. likely due to the nature of the task participants were asked to do
in the experiments. Monetary incentives typically have negative
effects when the task is challenging, intrinsically interesting in-
4 | C O N C L U S I O N , RE C O M M E N D A T I O N S volving creativity and effort beyond rudimentary abilities (Pink,
A N D F U T U R E RE S E A R C H 2011), requires long‐term commitment where the incentive re-
cipient is seen as greedy thus damaging their credibility, social
Exploring the factors encouraging engagement with public health status, or trust (Gneezy & Rustichini, 2000; Gneezy et al., 2011;
concerns is the focus of this paper. The current paper demonstrates Reimer & Benkenstein, 2016, 2018). Monetary incentives work
the impact of different types of incentives and ad appeals in gen- best as a short‐term strategy for activities that are routine, dull
erating online engagement for four health‐related ads about smoking and involve little social risk (Pink, 2011). For example, Hennig‐
and alcohol. The reason for selecting smoking and alcohol is to test Thurau et al.'s (2004) incentivized internet survey of over 2000
the hypotheses across two different health issues related to lifestyle consumers and Hinz et al.'s (2011) 19‐day online information
choices, with alcohol arguably considered as a more socially accep- sharing experiment both show that monetary incentives sig-
table behaviour than smoking at least across the four countries nificantly increase e‐WOM. Many researchers report the detri-
studied. mental effect of monetary incentives especially for activities that
This paper uses a series of laboratory and online experiment are intrinsically interesting or fun (Deci et al., 1999; Osterloh and
recruiting real FB users to empirically examine the impact of self‐ Fery, 2000). Monetary incentives are thought to be effective in
oriented monetary versus other‐oriented social good incentives un- these situations as they resemble low risk, transient business
der emotional versus rational ad appeal conditions in generating transactions compensating individuals for their efforts. Taking
online engagement for public health messages within the Facebook into account these factors, this study suggests providing
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1309

monetary incentives to encourage social media engagement is

(Silver zone) This zone is mainly recommended for targeting


(Neutral zone) The combination of social good and rational

better alternatives, we call this zone neutral. Regardless


ad to get a better results in terms of E‐WOM. Given the

adopt Zone 2 combination, offering monetary incentive


isn't recommended as the best zone to generate online

good with monetary, or the rational ad with emotional

mixed with a rational ad message to generate greater


Managers focusing on smoking, are recommended to
not detrimental and, in some circumstances, may actually in-

emergent as managers can either replace the social

make participant when the health issue is smoking.


crease engagement especially for men.

of the gender or the health issue, adopting this

online engagement, especial among men.


combination won't be the most effective
4.1 | Managerial implications

This study offers several managerial implications for those working


in public health and health promotion sector. The research findings
can assist marketers in public health to develop better social media
campaigns by offering appropriate incentives and appealing ads. For
behaviours that are rewarding by themselves, managers may offer
other‐oriented social incentives, especially to engage people in ads
promoting prosocial behaviours. Moreover, managers need to take

Zone 4
into account the moderating role of gender in designing social media
health campaigns. Overall, women are most likely to engage more
actively with a message if an other‐oriented incentive is offered,
whereas men are more engaged when a monetary incentive is of-

should see this as the golden combination, that


fered. Also, considering the interaction effect of incentive and ad

emotional ad appeal, in generating e‐WOM. In


(Golden zone) Practitioners and social marketers

matter whether the health issue is alcohol or

prioritise the combination of social good and

particular, this combination if effective when


triggers the highest level of engagement. No
appeal, the combination of an other‐oriented social good incentive

smoking, managers are recommended to


with an emotional appeal for women and a self‐oriented monetary
incentive with an emotional appeal for men is recommended. In si-
tuations where no incentive is desired, an emotional ad appeal is
recommended as opposed to a rational appeal for optimal social
media engagement, especially for alcohol ads and for females.
Table 6 provides tailored managerial recommendations for each

targeting women.
combination of ad appeal and incentive. Identifying four zones and
labelling them golden, silver and neutral, we discuss each zone and
provide recommendations depending on the health issue.
Zone 3

4.2 | Future research

This study suggests further research is needed to examine the im-


appeal for best results in terms of online engagement
(e‐WOM). This zone is mainly male dominant, as such

portance of product involvement, as in the online experiment re-


recommended to combine it with an emotional ad
combination of offering monetary incentive, are

cruiting real FB users where the health messages were advertised to


(Silver zone) Managers choosing to go with the

a broader range of people the results indicate people reacted very


differently to the alcohol versus smoking ads under different in-
centive and ad appeal conditions. Moreover, future research can
combination works better for men.

focus on the role of consumers' psychological characteristics, such as


Managerial implications summary

personality traits, regulatory focus as well as product involvement on


their level of engagement should also be examined. For instance,
some research demonstrates that personality is strongly associated
with social media behaviour (Acar and Polonsky, 2007; Seidman,
2013). Although many research studies consider consumer char-
acteristics, motivation and behaviour within social networks (e.g.,
Niederhoffer et al., 2007), fewer studies examine the influence of
Zone 2

individual factors on e‐WOM and online consumer engagement,


especially for social marketing messages.
Moreover, with regard to incentives, this paper concentrates on
TABLE 6

Smoking

the effects of incentives on e‐WOM sender; however, we encourage


Alcohol
Zone 1

future research to investigate the influence of incentivised e‐WOM


on the message recipient. Research demonstrates that providing a
1310 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

reward has a negative impact on the referral receiver (Reimer & typology of consumer–brand interactions. Journal of Brand
Benkenstein, 2018; Verlegh et al., 2013). In an unrewarded referral, Management, 23(2), 153–178.
Barger, V., Peltier, J. W., & Schultz, D. E. (2016). Social media and
the receiver is more likely to perceive the recommendation as being
consumer engagement: A review and research agenda. Journal of
genuine and unbiased, triggered by the recommender's knowledge Research in Interactive Marketing, 10(4), 268–287.
and positive experience. Offering incentives to both referral provider Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal
and receiver may eliminate this negative effect. Moreover, the ne- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155–162.
Bennett, R. (1996). Effects of horrific fear appeals on public attitudes
gative impact of an incentivised referral could be reduced if it is
towards AIDS. International Journal of Advertising, 15(3), 183–202.
solicited by the receiver (Verlegh et al., 2013). Berger, J. (2014). Word of mouth and interpersonal communication: A
review and directions for future research. Journal of Consumer
DATA AV AILA BILITY STATEMENT Psychology, 24(4), 586–607. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2014.
05.002
The data that support the findings of this study are available from
Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2011). Social transmission, emotion, and the
the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
virality of online content. Journal of Marketing Research, 49,
192–205.
ORCID Berger, J., & Milkman, K. L. (2012). What makes online content viral?
Helen Siuki https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4414-1174 Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 192–205. https://doi.org/10.
1509/jmr.10.0353
Cynthia M. Webster https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6697-2676
Berger, J., & Schwartz, E. M. (2011). What drives immediate and ongoing
word of mouth? Journal of Marketing Research, 48(5), 869–880.
R EF E RE N C E S https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.5.869
Acar, A.S., & Polonsky, M. (2007). Online social networks and insights Bécheur, I., Dib, H., Merunka, D., & Valette‐Florence, P. (2007). Emotions
into marketing communications. Journal of Internet Commerce, 6(4), of fear, guilt or shame in anti‐alcohol messages: Measuring direct
55–72. effects on persuasion and the moderating role of sensation seeking.
Adhikari, B., Kahende, J., Malarcher, A., Pechacek, T., & Tong, V. (2009). In S. Borghini, M. A. McGrath, & C. Otnes (Eds.), E‐European advances
Smoking‐attributable mortality, years of potential life lost, and in consumer research (Vol. 8, pp. 99–106). Association for Consumer
productivity losses. Oncology Times, 31, 40–42. Research.
Agrawal, N., & Duhachek, A. (2010). Emotional compatibility and the Biener, L., McCallum‐Keeler, G., & Nyman, A. L. (2000). Adults' response
effectiveness of antidrinking messages: A defensive processing to Massachusetts anti‐tobacco television advertisements: Impact of
perspective on shame and guilt. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), viewer and advertisement characteristics. Tobacco Control, 9(4),
263–273. 401–407. https://doi.org/10.1136/tc.9.4.401
Alhabash, S., McAlister, A. R., Hagerstrom, A., Quilliam, E. T., Rifon, N. J., & Bower, G. H. (1983). Affect and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the
Richards, J. I. (2013). Between likes and shares: Effects of emotional Royal Society, 302(B), 387–403.
appeal and virality on the persuasiveness of anticyberbullying Brennan, L., & Binney, W. (2010). Fear, guilt, and shame appeals in social
messages on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behaviour, and Social marketing. Journal of Business Research, 63(2), 140–146. https://doi.
Networking, 16(3), 175–182. org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2009.02.006
Almestahiri, R. D., Rundle‐Thiele, S., Parkinson, J., & Arli, D. (2017). The Brooks, A. W., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2011). Can nervous nelly negotiate?
use of the major components of social marketing: A systematic How anxiety causes negotiators to make low first offers, exit early,
review of tobacco cessation programs. Social Marketing Quarterly, and earn less profit. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
23(3), 232–248. Processes, 115(1), 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.
Anderson, N. H. (1988). A functional approach to person cognition. In T. K. 01.008
Srull, & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), A dual process model of impression Butt, P., Beirness, D., Gliksman, L., Paradis, C., & Stockwell, T. (2011). Alcohol
formation (pp. 37–51). Erlbaum. and health in Canada: A summary of evidence and guidelines for low‐
de Angelis, M., Bonezzi, A., Alessandro, M., Peluso, A. M., Rucker, D. D., & risk drinking. Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. www.ccsa.ca/
Costabile, M. (2012). On braggarts and gossips: A self‐enhancement Resource%20Library/2011-Summary-of-Evidence-and-Guidelines-for-
account of word‐of‐mouth generation and transmission. Journal of Low-Risk%20Drinking-en.pdf
Marketing Research, 49(4), 551–563. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr. Buyucek, N., Knox, K., & Rundle‐Thiele, S. (2018). Observing alcohol
11.0136 drinking in licensed premises: A formative social marketing study.
Ariely, D., Bracha, A., & Meier, S. (2009). Doing good or doing well? Image Journal of Social Marketing, 8, 40–57.
motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. Chamblee, R., Gilmore, R., Thomas, G., & Soldow, G. (1993). When copy
American Economic Review, 99(1), 544–555. https://doi.org/10. complexity can help ad readership. Journal of Advertising Research,
1257/aer.99.1.544 33(May/June), 23–28.
Arnold, M. J., & Reynolds, K. E. (2003). Hedonic shopping motivations. Chandon, P., Wansink, B., & Laurent, G. (2000). A Benefit Congruency
Journal of Retailing, 79, 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022- Framework of Sales Promotion Effectiveness. Journal of Marketing,
4359(03)00007-1 64(4), 65–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.64.4.65.18071
Arthur, D., & Quester, P. (2004). Who's afraid of that ad? Applying Chen, Z., & Berger, J. (2013). When, why, and how controversy causes
segmentation to the protection motivation model. Psychology & conversation. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(3), 580–593. https://
Marketing, 21(9), 671–696. doi.org/10.1086/671465
Ashley, C., & Tuten, T. (2015). Creative strategies in social media Cheung, C. M., & Lee, M. K. (2012). What drives consumers to spread
marketing: An exploratory study of branded social content and electronic word of mouth in online consumer‐opinion platforms.
consumer engagement. Psychology and Marketing, 32(1), 15–27. Decision Support Systems, 53(1), 218–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20761 dss.2012.01.015
Azar, S. L., Machado, J. C., Vacas‐de‐Carvalho, L., & Mendes, A. (2016). Chodkiewicz, J., Talarowska, M., Miniszewska, J., Nawrocka, N., &
Motivations to Interact with brands on Facebook—Towards a Bilinski, P. (2020). Alcohol consumption reported during the
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1311

COVID‐19 pandemic: The initial stage. International Journal of Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don't pay at all.
Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(13), 4677. Quarterly Journal of Economcis, 115(2), 791–810.
Chu, S.C., Lien, C.H., & Cao, Y. (2019). Electronic word‐of‐mouth (eWOM) Gneezy, U., & Rey‐Biel, P. (2014). On the relative efficiency of
on WeChat: Examining the influence of sense of belonging, need for performance pay and noncontingent incentives. Journal of the
self‐enhancement, and consumer engagement on Chinese travellers’ European Economic Association, 12, 62–72.
eWOM. International Journal of Advertising, 38(1), 26–49. Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). The implications of death for health:
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. A terror management health model for behavioral health promotion.
Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 448–474. https://doi.org/10. Psychological Review, 115, 1032–1053.
1257/jel.47.2.448 Graham, K., & Homel, R. (2008). Raising the bar: Preventing aggression in
Cui, J., Wang, L., Wang, L., & Teng, Y. (2014). Empirical study of the and around bars, clubs and pubs. Willian Publishing.
motivations of e‐wom spreading on online feedback system in china. Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Coinmunication acctrracy and
PACIS 2014 Proceedings, 251. expressive style. John Hopkins University Press.
Darke, P. R., Ashworth, L., & Main, K. J. (2010). Great expectations and Hastings, G., Stead, M., & Webb, J. (2004). Fear appeals in social
broken promises: Misleading claims, product failure, expectancy marketing: Strategic and ethical reasons for concern. Psychology and
disconfirmation and consumer distrust. Journal of the Academy of Marketing, 21(11), 961–986. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20043
Marketing Science, 38(3), 347–362. Hayes, J. R. (2013). The complete problem solver, Routledge.
Darke, P. R., & Ritchie, R. B. (2007). The defensive consumer: Advertising Hennig‐Thurau, T., Gwinner, K. P., Walsh, G., & Gremler, D. D. (2004).
deception, defensive processing, and distrust. Journal of Marketing Electronic word‐of‐mouth via consumer opinion platforms: What
Research, 44, 114–127. motivates consumers to articulate themselves on the Internet.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta‐analytic review of Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(1), 38–52. https://doi.org/10.
experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic 1002/dir.10073
motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–668. Hinz, O., Skiera, B., Barrot, C., & Becker, J. U. (2011). Seeding strategies
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self‐determination in for viral marketing: An empirical comparison. Journal of Marketing,
human behavior. Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1- 75, 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1509/jm.10.0088
4899-2271-7 Hollebeek, L., Glynn, M., & Brodie, R. (2014). Consumer brand
Devlin, E., Eadie, D., Stead, M., & Evans, K. (2007). Comparative Study of engagement in social media: Conceptualization, scale development
Young People's response to anti‐smoking messages. International and validation. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 28(2), 149–165.
Journal of Advertising, 26, 99–128. Hollebeek, L., Srivastava, R., & Chen, T. (2016). S‐D logic‐informed
Dobele, A., Lindgreen, A., Beverland, M., Vanhamme, J., & van Wijk, R. customer engagement: Integrative framework, revised fundamental
(2007). Why pass on viral messages? Because they connect propositions, and application to CRM. Journal of the Academy of
emotionally. Business Horizons, 50(4), 291–304. https://doi.org/10. Marketing Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-016-0494-5
1016/j.bushor.2007.01.004 Hollebeek, L. D. (2019). Developing business customer engagement
Doenyas, C. (2017). Self versus other oriented social motivation, not lack through social media engagement‐platforms: An integrative SD
of empathic or moral ability, explains behavioral outcomes in logic/RBV‐informed model. Industrial Marketing Management, 81,
children with high theory of mind abilities. Motivation and Emotion, 89–98.
41, 683–697. Holmås, T.H., Kjerstad, E., Lurås, H., & Straume, O. R. (2010). Does
Dubé, J. P., Luo, X., & Fang, Z. (2017). Self‐signaling and prosocial monetary punishment crowd out pro‐social motivation? A natural
behavior: A cause marketing experiment. Marketing Science, 36(2), experiment on hospital length of stay. Journal of Economic Behavior &
161–186. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.2016.1012 Organization, 75(2), 261–267.
Duhachek, A., Agrawal, N., & Han, D. (2012). Guilt versus shame: Coping, Jin, L., & Huang, Y. (2014). When giving money does not work: The
fluency, and framing in the effectiveness of responsible drinking differential effects of monetary versus in‐kind rewards in referral
messages. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(6), 928–941. reward programs. International Journal of Research in Marketing,
Duke, C. R., Pickett, L. C., & Grove, S. J. (1993). A method for evaluating 31(1), 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2013.08.005
the ethics of fear appeals. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 12(1), Kalra, A., & Shi, M. (2010). Consumer value‐maximizing sweepstakes and
120–129. https://doi.org/10.1177/074391569501200112 contests. Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 287–300. https://doi.
Eagle, L., Dahl, S., & Low, D. R. (2015). Ethics in social marketing. In L. Eagle, & org/10.1509/jmkr.47.2.287
S. Dahl (Eds.), Marketing ethics and society. Sage Publications. Kennedy, A. M., & Santos, N. (2019). Social fairness and social marketing.
Fehr, E., & Falk, A. (2002). Psychological foundations of incentives. European Journal of Social Marketing, 9, 522–539.
Economic Review, 46(4), 687–724. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0014- Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010a). Do all material incentives for pro‐social
2921(01)00208-2 activities backfire? The response to cash and non‐cash incentives for
Fletcher‐Brown, J., Turnbull, S., Viglia, G., Chen, T., & Pereira, V. (2020). blood donations. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(4), 738–748.
Vulnerable consumer engagement: How corporate social media can https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2010.05.007
facilitate the replenishment of depleted resources. International Journal Lacetera, N., & Macis, M. (2010b). Social image concerns and prosocial
of Research in Marketing. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2020.06.002 behavior: Field evidence from a nonlinear incentive scheme. Journal
Frey, B. S. (1993). Motivation as a limit to pricing. Journal of Economic of Economic Behavior and Organization, 76(2), 225–237.
Psychology, 14, 635–664. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(93) Laslett, A. M., Catalano, P., Chikritzhs, T., Dale, C., Doran, C., Ferris, J.,
90014-C Jainullabudeen, T., Livingston, M., Matthews, S., Mugavin, J.,
Giebelhausen, M., Chun, H. H., & Cronin, J. J., Jr., Hult, G. T. M. (2016). Room, R., Schlotterlein, M., & Wilkinson, C. (2010). The range and
Adjusting the warm‐glow thermostat: How incentivizing participation in magnitude of alcohol's harm to others. Foundation for Alcohol
voluntary green programs moderates their impact on service Research and Education.
satisfaction. Journal of Marketing, 80(4), 56–71. https://doi.org/10. Lefebvre, C. R. (2011). An integrative model for social marketing. Journal
1509/jm.14.0497 of Social Marketing, 1(1), 54–72.
Gneezy, U., Meier, S., & Rey‐Biel, P. (2011). When and why incentives Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s
(don't) work to modify behavior. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25, intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the ‘overjustification’
191–210. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.25.4.191 hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.
1312 | SIUKI AND WEBSTER

Liang, B., & Scammon, D. L. (2011). E‐word‐of‐mouth on health social Previte, J., Russell‐Bennett, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). Shaping
networking sites: An opportunity for tailored health communication. safe drinking cultures: Evoking positive emotion to promote
Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 10(6), 322–331. moderate‐drinking behaviour. International Journal of Consumer
Lim, M. S., Wright, C. J., Carrotte, E. R., & Pedrana, A. E. (2017). Reach, Studies, 39(1), 12–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijcs.12146
engagement, and effectiveness: A systematic review of evaluation Putrevu, S., Tan, J., & Lord, K. R. (2004). Consumer responses to complex
methodologies used in health promotion via social networking sites. advertisements: The moderating role of need for cognition,
Health Promotion Journal of Australia, 27(3), 187–197. https://doi. knowledge, and gender. Journal of Current Issues & Research in
org/10.1071/HE16057 Advertising, 26(1), 9–24.
Lowe, B., Lynch, D., & Lowe, J. (2015). Reducing household water Raghubir, P. (2004). Coupons in context: Discounting prices or decreasing
consumption: A social marketing approach. Journal of Marketing profits? Journal of Retailing, 80, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Management, 31(3–4), 378–408. jretai.2004.01.001
Lu, H.‐P., & Su, P. Y.‐J. (2009). Factors affecting purchase intention on Reimer, T., & Benkenstein, M. (2016). Altruistic e‐WOM marketing: More
mobile shopping web sites. Internet Research, 19, 442–458. https:// than an alternative to monetary incentives. Journal of Retailing and
doi.org/10.1108/10662240910981399 Consumer Services, 31, 323–333.
Malthouse, E. C., Calder, B.J., Kim, S.J., & Vandenbosch, M. (2016). Evidence Reimer, T., & Benkenstein, M. (2018). Not just for the recommender: How
that user‐generated content that produces engagement increases e‐WOM incentives influence the recommendation audience. Journal
purchase behaviours. Journal of Marketing Management, 32(5‐6), of Business Research, 86, 11–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.
427–444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0267257x.2016.1148066 2018.01.041
Marbach, J., Lages, C. R., & Nunan, D. (2016). Who are you and what do Rogers, R. W., & Deckner, C. W. (1975). Effects of fear appeals and
you value? Investigating the role of personality traits and customer‐ physiological arousal upon emotion, attitudes, and cigarette
perceived value in online customer engagement. Journal of Marketing smoking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 222–230.
Management, 32(5–6), 502–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0267257x. Rossiter, J. R., & Thornton, J. (2004). Fear‐pattern analysis supports the
2015.1128472 fear‐drive model for antispeeding road‐safety TV ads. Psychology &
Marchand, J., & Filiatrault, P. (2002). AIDS prevention advertising: Marketing, 21(11), 945–960.
Different message strategies for different communication Roth, G. (2008). Perceived parental conditional regard and autonomy
objectives. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector support as predictors of young adults' self‐ versus other‐oriented
Marketing, 7(3), 271–287. prosocial tendencies. Journal of Personality, 76, 513–534.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for Rothschild, M. L. (1999). Carrots, sticks, and promises: A conceptual
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, framework for the management of public health and social issue
224–253. behaviors. Journal of Marketing, 63, 24–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/
McKay‐Nesbitt, J., & Yoon, S. (2015). Social marketing communication 002224299906300404
messages: How congruence between source and content influences Ruiter, R. A., Kessels, L. T., Peters, G. J. Y., & Kok, G. (2014). Sixty years of
physical activity attitudes. Journal of Social Marketing, 5(1), 40–55. fear appeal research: Current state of the evidence. International
Mellström, C., & Johannesson, M. (2008). Crowding out in blood donation: Journal of Psychology, 49(2), 63–70.
Was titmuss right? Journal of the European Economic Association, Ryu, G., & Feick, L. (2007). A penny for your thoughts: Referral reward
6(4), 845–863. programs and referral likelihood. Journal of Marketing, 71(1), 84–94.
Michaelidou, N., Dibb, S., & Ali, H. (2008). The Effect of Health, https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.71.1.084
Cosmetic and Social Antismoking Information Themes on Schulze, C., Scholer, L., & Skiera, B. (2014). Not all fun and games: Viral
Adolescents' Beliefs About Smoking. International Journal of marketing for utilitarian products. Journal of Marketing, 78, 1–19.
Advertising, 27(2), 235–250. https://doi.org/10.1509/jm.11.0528
Michalski, R., Jankowski, J., & Kazienko, P. S. C. A. (2012), Negative effects Seidman, G. (2013). Self‐Presentation and belonging on Facebook: How
of incentivised viral campaigns for activity in social networks, In Social personality influences social media use and motivations. Personality
Computing and its Applications 2012 Proceedings of the 2nd and Individual Differences, 54(3), 402–407.
International Conference, Xiangtan, China. Shang, R.‐A., Chen, Y.‐C., & Shen, L. (2005). Extrinsic versus intrinsic
Mortimer, D., Ghijben, P., Harris, A., & Hollingsworth, B. (2013). Incentive‐ motivations for consumers to shop on‐line. Information and
based and non‐incentive‐based interventions for increasing blood Management, 42, 401–413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2004.
donation. Cochrane Database System Review, 2013(1), CD010295. 01.009
Munzel, A., & Kunz, H. W. (2014). Creators, multipliers, and lurkers: Who Smith, A. N., Fischer, E., & Yongjian, C. (2012). How does brand‐related
contributes and who benefits at online review sites. Journal of user‐generated content differ across Youtube, Facebook, and
Service Management, 25(1), 49–74. https://doi.org/10.1108/JOSM- Twitter? Journal of Interactive Marketing, 26(2), 102–113. https://
04-2013-0115 doi.org/10.1016/j.intmar.2012.01.002
Niederhoffer, K., Mooth, R., Wiesenfeld, D., & Gordon, J. (2007). The Smith, C. N. (2001). Social marketing and social contracts: applying
origin and impact of CPG new‐product buzz: Emerging trends and integrative social contracts theory to ethical issues in social
implications. Journal of Advertising Research, 47(4), 420–426. marketing. In A. Andreasen (Ed.), Ethics in social marketing.
Noble, G., Pomering, A., & Johnson, L. W. (2014). Gender and message Georgetown University Press.
appeal: Their influence in a pro‐environmental social advertising Statista. (2021). Most used social media 2021. Statista [online]. Available
context. Journal of Social Marketing, 4(1), 4–21. https://doi.org/10. at: <https://www.statista.com/statistics/272014/global-social-networks-
1108/JSOCM-12-2012-0049 ranked-by-number-of-users/> [Accessed 24 May 2021].
Osterloh, M., & Frey, B. S. (2000). Motivation, knowledge transfer, and Sweeney, J. C., Soutar, G. N., & Mazzarol, T. (2012). Word of mouth:
organizational forms. Organization Science, 11(5), 538–550. Measuring the power of individual messages. European Journal of
Pink, D. H. (2011). Drive: The Surprising Truth about What Motivates Us, Marketing, 4 6(1 /2) , 2 37 –2 57 . h ttps://doi.org /10.1108/
Penguin. 03090561211189310
Poch, R., & Martin, B. (2015). Effects of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation on Szablewska, N., & Kubacki, K. (2019). A human rights‐based approach to
user‐generated content. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 23(4), 305–317. the social good in social marketing. Journal of Business Ethics, 155(3),
https://doi.org/10.1080/0965254X.2014.926966 871–888.
SIUKI AND WEBSTER | 1313

Tellis, G. J., MacInnis, D. J., Tirunillai, S., & Zhang, Y. (2019). What drives Woerndl, M., Papagiannidis, S., Bourlakis, M., & Li, F. (2008). Internet‐
virality (sharing) of online digital content? The critical role of induced marketing techniques: Critical factors in viral marketing
information, emotion, and brand prominence. Journal of Marketing, campaigns. Journal of Business Science and Applied Management, 3(1),
83(4), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022242919841034 35–45.
Titmuss, R.M. (1970). The Gift Relationship: From Human Blood to Social World Health Organization (WHO). (2013). WHO report on the global
Policy, London, U.K.: Allen & Unwin. tobacco epidemic, 2013: Enforcing bans on tobacco advertising,
Thackeray, R., Neiger, B. L., Hanson, C. L., & McKenzie, J. F. (2008). promotion and sponsorship: Executive summary. http://apps.who.int/
Enhancing promotional strategies within social marketing programs: iris/bitstream/10665/85380/1/9789241505871_eng.pdf?ua¼1
Use of Web 2.0 social media. Health Promotion Practice, 9(4), Yach, D. (2020). Tobacco use patterns in five countries during the COVID‐19
338–343. lockdown. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 22, 1671–1672.
Toubia, O., & Stephen, A. T. (2013). Intrinsic vs. image‐related utility in Yoo, K. H., & Gretzel, U. (2011). Influence of personality on travel‐related
social media: Why do people contribute content To Twitter? consumer‐generated media creation. Computers in Human Behavior,
Marketing Science, 32(3), 368–392. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc. 27(2), 609–621. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.002
2013.0773 Yoon, H. J., & Kim, Y. (2014). The moderating role of gender identity in
Verlegh, P. W., Ryu, G., Tuk, M. A., & Feick, L. (2013). Receiver responses responses to comedic violence advertising. Journal of Advertising, 43(4),
to rewarded referrals: The motive inferences framework. Journal of 382–396.
the Academy of Marketing Science, 41(6), 669–682. https://doi.org/ Zhang, T., Lu, C., Torres, E., & Chen, P. J. (2018). Engaging customers in value
10.1007/s11747-013-0327-8 co‐creation or co‐destruction online. Journal of Services Marketing, 32(1),
Vilnai‐Yavetz, I., & Levina, O. (2018). Motivating social sharing of e‐ 57–69. https://doi.org/10.1108/JSM-01-2017-0027
business content: Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, or
crowding‐out effect? Computers in Human Behavior, 79, 181–191.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.034 How to cite this article: Siuki, H., & Webster, C. M. (2021).
Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta‐analysis of fear appeals:
Social good or self‐interest: Incentivizing consumer social
Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health
Education and Behavior, 27(5), 591–615. https://doi.org/10. media engagement behaviour for health messages. Psychol
1177/109019810002700506 Mark, 38, 1293–1313. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21517

You might also like