You are on page 1of 8

Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Sleep Medicine
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep

Original Article

Nocturnal polysomnographic sleep across the menstrual cycle


in premenstrual dysphoric disorder
Ari Shechter a,1, Paul Lespérance b, N.M.K. Ng Ying Kin c, Diane B. Boivin a,⇑
a
Centre for Study and Treatment of Circadian Rhythms, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
b
Department of Psychiatry, Centre Hospitalier de l’Université de Montréal, Université de Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
c
Clinical Research Division, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objectives: Women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) experience disturbed mood, altered
Received 3 January 2012 melatonin circadian rhythms, and frequent reports of insomnia during the luteal phase (LP) of their men-
Received in revised form 14 May 2012 strual cycle. In this study we aimed to investigate nocturnal polysomnographic (PSG) sleep across the
Accepted 15 May 2012
menstrual cycle in PMDD women and controls.
Available online 30 June 2012
Methods: Seven PMDD women who indicated insomnia during LP, and five controls, spent every third
night throughout a complete menstrual cycle sleeping in the laboratory.
Keywords:
Results: In PMDD and controls progesterone and core body temperature (BTcore) were elevated during LP
Sleep
Menstrual cycle
compared to the follicular phase (FP). Stage 2 sleep showed a significant main effect of menstrual phase
Premenstrual dysphoric disorder and was significantly increased during mid-LP compared to early-FP in both groups. Rapid eye movement
Core body temperature (REM) sleep for both groups was decreased during early-LP compared to early-FP. Slow wave sleep (SWS)
Melatonin was significantly increased, and melatonin significantly decreased, in PMDD women compared to con-
Progesterone trols.
Conclusions: PMDD women who experience insomnia during LP had decreased melatonin secretion and
increased SWS compared to controls. The sleep and melatonin findings in PMDD women may be func-
tionally linked. Results also suggest an altered homeostatic regulation of the sleep–wake cycle in PMDD,
perhaps implicating melatonin in the homeostatic process of sleep–wake regulation.
Ó 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction cause of various methodological issues including differences in


diagnostic criteria, patient group heterogeneity, and limited sam-
Premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD) is a DSM-IV classified pling frequency across the menstrual cycle. Whereas one study
menstrual cycle-related mood disorder affecting approximately 3– focusing on a group of PMDD women did not show any differences
8% of women [1]. Its occurrence is dependent on, and temporally between controls and patients [4], other studies whose patient
defined by, ovarian hormone status across the menstrual cycle. groups included a mix of women with PMDD and severe premen-
Typically, symptoms are present during the luteal phase (LP), remit strual syndrome (PMS) [5] or premenstrual depression based on
after menses onset, and are absent throughout the follicular phase DSM-III criteria [6], observed significantly lengthened rapid eye
(FP) [2]. Disturbed sleep is among the 11 symptoms that character- movement (REM) sleep onset latency [5], significantly increased
ize the disorder [2], and patients frequently report sleep com- stage 2 sleep [6], or significantly reduced REM sleep [6] in patients
plaints including insomnia, poor sleep quality, and increased compared to controls regardless of menstrual phase.
awakenings during the symptomatic phase [3]. The presence of sleep disruption in PMDD, while not a require-
Prior polysomnography (PSG)-based sleep studies focusing on ment for diagnosis, is an important clinical symptom. Insomnia or
PMDD have been limited in number and largely inconsistent be- hypersomnia is present in approximately 70% of PMDD women [7].
The DSM-IV Research Criteria for Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder
Checklist used for diagnoses does not differentiate between the
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Centre for Study and Treatment of Circadian presence of insomnia and hypersomnia [2], however, and, to our
Rhythms, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, knowledge, no study has reported the relative prevalence of each
McGill University, 6875 LaSalle Boulevard, Montreal, Quebec, Canada H4H 1R3. Tel.: within PMDD women. Some of the inconsistencies in the PMDD
+1 (514) 761 6131x2397; fax: +1 (514) 888 4099.
sleep literature may be due to studying mixed groups of PMDD wo-
E-mail address: diane.boivin@douglas.mcgill.ca (D.B. Boivin).
1
Current address: New York Obesity Nutrition Research Center, St. Luke’s/ men with insomnia and hypersomnia symptoms and PMDD wo-
Roosevelt Hospital, New York, NY, USA. men with and without sleep complaints in general. Nevertheless,

1389-9457/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.05.012
1072 A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078

sleep deprivation was demonstrated to have an anti-depressive ior [13]. PMDD Subject #3 had a global seasonality score of 20 on
effect in PMDD [8], supporting the notion that sleep disturbances the SPAQ, but no diagnosis of SAD was made for this woman based
are of clinical importance in PMDD. As a growing body of research on the SPAQ and overall clinical assessment. Of interest, she did not
indicates that the menstrual cycle has an influence on both subjec- present the typical seasonal variation of sleep with increased dura-
tive and objective sleep [9], it is increasingly important to docu- tion during winter. Laboratory visits for PMDD Subjects #1, #2, and
ment sleep in these women by conducting comprehensive #3 occurred in Winter, PMDD Subject #6 in Spring, PMDD Subject
studies across a full menstrual cycle in a homogenous group of #4 in Summer and PMDD Subject #5 in Fall. Axis II disorders were
PMDD women with insomnia. ruled out by clinical evaluation but not systematically with screen-
Our aim was to document nocturnal PSG sleep across a full ing questionnaires. PMDD Subject #1 reported rodent phobia after
menstrual cycle in control women and women diagnosed with a trip to India. PMDD Subject #2 was afraid of airplanes. Age-
PMDD based on DSM-IV criteria who also indicated insomnia matched controls completed the SCID, PRISM, and VAS during
symptoms during the symptomatic phase. Changes in nocturnal screening and showed no evidence of PMDD or any other psychiat-
PSG sleep were compared between groups to determine how this ric disorder.
menstrual cycle-related mood disorder affects objective sleep, All participants were healthy and drug-free, as confirmed by
and were also considered in light of the altered sex hormone, body medical examination, blood and urine work-up, and toxicology
temperature, and nocturnal melatonin secretion profiles associated screening. All had a history of regular menstrual cycles (range:
with different menstrual cycle phases. 25–34 ± 3 days), and ovulation was confirmed via a plasma proges-
terone test scheduled on day 21 of the menstrual cycle preceding
experimental procedures. All had no history of gynecological
2. Methods pathology, were at least six months post-partum, not currently
breast feeding, and free of hormonal contraceptives. At the time
2.1. Participants of recruitment, PMDD Subject #4 had a one year old child. Her
insomnia symptoms were present selectively during LP, and were
Seven PMDD women and five controls were studied. PMDD determined to not be associated with infant sleep patterns. At
diagnoses were based on the Structured Clinical Interview for the time of recruitment, Control Subject #1 had a seven month
DSM-IV (SCID), the Prospective Record of the Impact and Severity old child but showed no indication of sleep disruption associated
of Menstrual Symptoms (PRISM) [10,11], and a Visual Analogue with infant sleep patterns based on screening period actigraphy
Scale (VAS) [10,11] completed daily for at least two menstrual cy- and sleep–wake log, as well as no indication of post-partum
cles. The reliability and validity of the VAS and PRISM as effective depression based on psychiatric evaluations during the screening
tools for the prospective tracking of menstrual cycle-related mood phase. Participants had no history of night-shift work or transme-
disturbance has been documented [10]. The 11-item VAS (100-mm ridian travel within three months prior to study. Before the exper-
bipolar scale, with 0 mm being ‘‘not at all’’ and 100 mm being ‘‘ex- imental month, participants were not allowed to nap during the
treme symptoms’’) was based on the DSM-IV criteria for PMDD day and maintained a regular schedule of 8 h sleep/darkness per
diagnoses [2] and included the measures depressed mood, tension, day for at least three weeks confirmed by sleep–wake log, calls
affective lability, irritability, decreased interest, difficulty concen- to the laboratory at bed/wake times, and wrist actigraphy for at
trating, lack of energy, change in appetite, change in sleep patterns, least two weeks (Actiwatch, Mini-Mitter, Bend, OR). Chronotype
feeling out of control, and physical symptoms. An individual mean was assessed with the Horne and Ostberg Morningness–Evening-
score for each of the four core PMDD symptoms (depressed mood, ness Questionnaire [14]. Mean morningness–eveningness scores
tension, affective lability, irritability) was calculated for days 6–10 were comparable between groups and were (mean ± SEM)
after menstruation (FP) and for the last five days of the menstrual 50.25 ± 7.73 and 57.57 ± 3.07 for controls and PMDD women,
cycle (late-LP). Eligibility criteria required the presence of at least respectively. This is within the range of ‘‘neither type,’’ i.e., not des-
five of the 11 overall symptoms during late-LP and an increase of ignated as morning- or evening chronotypes, though four patients
P200% on one, or P100% on two or more of the core symptoms and one control were ‘‘moderately morning type’’ and one control
for the mean late-LP score compared to FP. These diagnostic crite- was ‘‘definitely evening type’’ [14]. The Douglas Mental Health
ria were developed as prospective criteria to be met, which demon- University Institute Research Ethics Board approved all procedures
strate an unambiguous and clinically relevant worsening of and all participants provided informed consent.
symptoms during late-LP compared to FP, with the VAS scores
serving to illustrate prospective day-to-day objective differences 2.2. Experimental design
in the DSM-IV diagnostic symptoms. The criteria are modeled after,
but are more stringent than, those used by Steinberg et al. [12] and Participants entered the laboratory for nocturnal PSG sleep
Steiner et al. [11], which were a 50% worsening in three core symp- recordings every third night throughout a complete menstrual cy-
toms or a 100% worsening of one core symptom. All potential cle and left in the morning upon awakening (Fig. 1). The day of the
PMDD women met with a psychiatrist (P.L.) twice, at FP and late- first laboratory visit ranged from day one to three of the menstrual
LP, to clinically confirm the PMDD diagnosis. PMDD women with cycle (mean ± SD: 1.92 ± 0.51). Participants visited the lab 8–11
sleep complaints were recruited. All PMDD women included indi- times over the course of a menstrual cycle. The first night of sleep
cated insomnia symptoms selectively during the premenstrual recordings served as an adaptation and diagnostic night to rule out
phase, with no clinical evidence of subjective sleep disruption periodic leg movements in sleep and apnea/hypopnea and was ex-
during FP as assessed via clinical consultation. Of the seven PMDD cluded from subsequent analyses. Wakefulness occurred in regular
women, two indicated sleep-onset insomnia, four indicated sleep- room lighting (150 lux). Sleep episodes occurred in complete
maintenance insomnia, and one reported general insomnia (not darkness (<0.3 lux), lasted 8 h, and were based on the timing of
specified). Participants were excluded if diagnosed with another participants’ habitual sleep/wake schedule maintained during the
current Axis I disorder. From the PMDD group, Subjects #2 and screening phase. As part of a larger study, participants underwent
#7 reported one past episode of depression three to four years a 24-h period of intensive physiological monitoring under constant
prior to study. Seasonal Affective Disorder (SAD) was ruled out posture (CP) conditions during FP and LP of the menstrual cycle.
by the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire (SPAQ), a tool Throughout the CP, participants remained in constant conditions,
used for the assessment of seasonal variations in mood and behav- including a maintained semi-recumbent posture, a time-cue free
A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078 1073

Fig. 1. Illustration of laboratory experimental protocol. Participants entered the laboratory for a full night of nocturnal sleep recording once every third night for the duration
of a full menstrual cycle. Polysomnographic sleep and core body temperature (BTcore) were recorded during each of the full sleep episodes, illustrated as black bars. Upon
awakening, a urine sample was collected and assayed for progesterone content. The gray bars illustrate a 24-h period of intensive physiological monitoring under constant
conditions, occurring during the follicular phase and luteal phase, the results of which are the topic of another manuscript. ME, menses; EF, early follicular; MF, mid-follicular;
LF, late follicular; OV, ovulation; EL, early luteal; ML, mid-luteal; LL, late luteal.

environment, iso-caloric snacks served 1x/h, and dim ambient light were also assayed for their content of 6-sulfatoxy-melatonin, the
levels (<10 lux) throughout the waking period before and after the main melatonin metabolite. 6-Sulfatoxy-melatonin concentration
sleep nocturnal sleep episode. Results from the 24-h CP visits will was measured in duplicate using commercially-available radioim-
be reported elsewhere and include all five controls and six of seven munoassay kit (Stockgrand Ltd., Surrey, UK). The sensitivity of the
PMDD women included in the current report. assay was 0.05 ng/ml, with a coefficient of variation ranging from
11.3% to 12.4%.
For all sleep episodes, PSG recordings, including central and
2.3. Measures
occipital electroencephalogram submental electromyogram, and
electrooculogram, were made on a computerized system (Harmonie,
The variation of mood across the menstrual cycle was measured
Stellate Systems, Montreal, QC) at a sampling rate of 250 Hz and
during the screening phase with the 11-item VAS described above.
high- and low-pass filtered at 0.3 Hz and 35 Hz, respectively.
After mean scores were calculated across days 6–10 after menstru-
Periodic limb movements in sleep were screened for by recording
ation and also across the last five days of the menstrual cycle for
electromyograms of the left and right anterior tibialis and scored
each of the four core PMDD symptoms individually (depressed
using established criteria [15]. Respiratory parameters were
mood, tension, affective lability, and irritability), the four core
monitored with bucconasal thermistance and airflow pressure
measures were averaged together to yield a mean score represent-
transducer, and all participants had an apnea/hypopnea index below
ing overall mood symptoms called the VAS-core [11,12].
five. Sleep was visually scored in 30-s epochs according to standard
Core body temperature (BTcore) was continuously monitored
criteria [16]. The amount of each sleep stage was determined and
(4x/min) throughout all sleep episodes via a thermistor (Steri-
expressed as a percent of the sleep period time. Total sleep time
Probe, Cincinatti Sub-Zero Products, Inc., Cincinnati, OH) inserted
was the sum of sleep stages 1–4 plus REM sleep. Sleep efficiency
10 cm into the rectum and connected to an in-house data acquisi-
was total sleep time divided by the time from lights-off to lights-
tion system. Data were inspected visually and by an ad hoc pro-
on multiplied by 100. Sleep onset latency was the time from
gram for probe malfunctions or ‘‘slips,’’ which were discarded. To
lights-off to the first appearance of at least two epochs of stage 1
investigate the variation of nocturnal BTcore across the menstrual
sleep, or the first appearance of any deeper stage. Non-REM sleep
cycle, a sleep-episode average (mean 8-h BTcore, from lights-off to
was the sum of sleep stages 2–4. REM sleep onset latency was the
lights-on) was calculated.
time from sleep onset to the first appearance of REM sleep.
Urine samples were collected each morning upon awakening
and assayed for progesterone concentration. Assays were per-
formed on the Beckman Coulter DxI 800 system using Beckman re- 2.4. Menstrual cycle delineation
agents for chemiluminescence immunoassays (Beckman Coulter
Inc., Brea, CA; coefficient of variation: 6.8%). Morning urine sam- Menstrual cycle length throughout the experimental proto-
ples, to reflect melatonin metabolite excretion during the night, col for PMDD and controls ranged from 24–29 days
1074 A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078

(mean ± SD: 26.42 ± 1.68 days). Menstrual cycles were normalized 3.2. Urinary progesterone
to a standard 28-day cycle based on criteria modeled after Driver
and colleagues [17]. Data obtained during each visit were allocated Two-way ANOVA did not show a significant group  menstrual
into one of eight menstrual phases, including the menses (ME), phase interaction or main effect of group for progesterone (Fig. 2).
early follicular (EF), mid-follicular (MF), late follicular (LF), ovula- A significant main effect of menstrual phase was observed, with
tion (OV), early luteal (EL), mid-luteal (ML), and late luteal (LL) progesterone significantly increased during LP compared to FP in
phases. Menstrual cycle delineation was based on anchor points both groups (Table 2 and Fig. 2).
at the first day of menstruation, the periovulatory period, as deter-
mined by a surge in luteinizing hormone (LH), and the day of men- 3.3. 6-Sulfatoxy-melatonin
ses for the subsequent menstrual cycle. The LH surge was
determined by a commercially available urine ovulation test Two-way ANOVA did not show a significant group  menstrual
(Church & Dwight Co., Inc., Princeton, NJ), and the periovulatory phase interaction or main effect of menstrual phase for 6-sulfat-
period was defined as a three-day window centered on the LH oxy-melatonin (Fig. 2). A significant main effect of group and large
surge. Days 1–4 after menstruation were assigned to the ME bin. between-group effect size was observed, with significantly
The three nights before OV were designated as LF, the three nights decreased 6-sulfatoxy-melatonin in PMDD compared to controls
occurring before LF were designated as MF, and EF was defined as (Table 2 and Fig. 2).
the days between ME and MF. The four nights after OV were des-
ignated as EL, the four nights following EL were designated as 3.4. Nocturnal core body temperature (BTcore)
ML, and LL was defined as occurring nine nights after OV to the
night before the onset of menses for the subsequent cycle (Tables Two-way ANOVA did not show a significant group  menstrual
S1 and S2). Data from ME were not included in the present results. phase interaction or main effect of group for BTcore (Fig. 2). A signif-
icant main effect of menstrual phase was observed, with BTcore sig-
2.5. Statistical analyses nificantly increased during LP compared to FP in both groups
(Table 2 and Fig 2).
Unpaired samples t-tests were used to compare participant
characteristics including age, body mass index, and menstrual cy- 3.5. Polysomnographic (PSG) sleep
cle length between groups. Two-way ANOVA for repeated mea-
sures (factors: group  menstrual phase) was used to analyze Two-way ANOVA did not reveal a significant main effect of
VAS-core, BTcore, progesterone, 6-sulfatoxy-melatonin, and sleep menstrual phase or a significant group  menstrual phase interac-
parameters across the menstrual cycle. A specific and directional tion for slow wave sleep (SWS). A significant main effect of group
change (i.e., a reduction) in REM sleep during the luteal phase and a large between-group effect size was observed, with signifi-
was predicted based on our prior findings [9]. This a priori predic- cantly higher SWS in PMDD compared to controls (Table 2 and
tion later formed the basis of a series of paired samples t-tests Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA did not reveal a significant main effect
comparing REM sleep during the follicular and luteal phases (see of group or a significant group  menstrual phase interaction for
Section 3). Effect sizes for between-group differences were calcu- stage 2 sleep. A significant main effect of menstrual phase was ob-
lated using Cohen’s d, with pooled standard deviation (PMDD served (Table 2). Tukey’s HSD post-hoc comparisons revealed sig-
and control). Values of d P 0.8 were considered a large effect size. nificantly increased stage 2 sleep during ML compared to EF
(Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA did not reveal significant main effects
3. Results of group or menstrual phase, or a significant group  menstrual
phase interaction for REM sleep. A paired-samples t-test on pooled
3.1. Participant characteristics data from controls and PMDD, which was conducted in order to
test the validity of an a priori prediction of reduced REM sleep dur-
Controls and PMDD women did not differ in age, body mass ing LP compared to FP [9], confirmed significantly decreased REM
index, or menstrual cycle length (Table 1). Two-way ANOVA sleep at EL compared to EF (t11 = 2.40, p = 0.04; Fig. 2). Two-way
indicated a significant group  menstrual phase interaction for ANOVA did not reveal a significant main effect of group or a signif-
VAS-core score, with significantly increased late-LP values icant group  menstrual phase interaction for sleep onset latency.
observed for PMDD compared to their own FP and to controls A significant main effect of menstrual phase was observed, though
(Table 1). Controls demonstrated no menstrual phase variation Tukey’s HSD post-hoc comparisons did not reveal significant differ-
for VAS-core. ences between menstrual phases (Table 2 and Fig. 2).

Table 1
Participant characteristics.

Characteristic Control PMDD Control vs. PMDD, p-value


Sample size 5 7
Age, years 30.4 ± 8.20 32.0 ± 5.72 p = NS
BMI, kg/cm2 23.5 ± 1.3 22.8 ± 2.1 p = NS
Menstrual cycle length, days 26.4 ± 2.3 26.6 ± 1.5 p = NS
Mean VAS-core, mm
FPa 3.12 ± 3.87 3.06 ± 3.38 p = NS
LLPa 5.57 ± 4.32 48.81 ± 16.17b p < 0.001

PMDD, premenstrual dysphoric disorder; BMI, body mass index; FP, follicular phase; LLP, late luteal phase; VAS-core, mean score of visual analogue scale measures for
depression, tension, affective lability, and irritability; NS, not significant.
Values are mean ± SD.
a
Indicates significant group  menstrual phase interaction for VAS-core, by two-way ANOVA; F1,10 = 37.70, p < 0.001.
b
Indicates significant menstrual phase and group differences for VAS-core, by simple main effects tests; p < 0.001.
A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078 1075

Two-way ANOVA did not reveal significant main effects of


group or menstrual phase, or a significant group  menstrual
phase interaction for wakefulness after sleep onset (Table 2).
Two-way ANOVA did not reveal significant main effects of group
or menstrual phase, or a significant group  menstrual phase inter-
action for non-REM sleep (Table 2 and Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA did
not reveal significant main effects for group or menstrual phase, or
a significant group  menstrual phase interaction for sleep effi-
ciency (Table 2 and Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA did not reveal signif-
icant main effects of group or menstrual phase, or a significant
group  menstrual phase interaction for REM sleep onset latency
(Table 2 and Fig. 2).

4. Discussion

We examined sleep across the menstrual cycle in PMDD women


with insomnia symptoms during LP and in controls.
Ovulation during the experimental month was confirmed in
participants via increased progesterone during LP compared to
FP. In agreement with others [5,18–22], we found similar proges-
terone levels between groups, which implies that decreased abso-
lute levels of circulating progesterone are unlikely to play a
causative role in the development of PMDD. A positive relationship
was observed between progesterone and BTcore across the men-
strual cycle. Both groups showed significantly increased nocturnal
BTcore throughout LP compared to FP. Progesterone is thermogenic
and likely raises BTcore by inducing an upward shift in the hypotha-
lamic set-point of temperature regulation [23].
Mean nocturnal BTcore was not different between groups. A lim-
ited number of studies have compared nocturnal BTcore between
controls and PMS/PMDD women. In the earliest [24], mean in-
bed rectal temperature was significantly elevated in patients com-
pared to controls throughout EF, LF, ML, and LL. It should be noted
that BTcore recordings in that study were obtained while partici-
pants were at home under ambulatory conditions, and that time-
in-bed was determined from actigraphy and sleep–wake logs.
These experimental conditions contrast with our laboratory-con-
trolled setting in which sleep was polysomnographically docu-
mented. A later study [6] showed no difference between patients
and controls for nocturnal BTcore minimum during sleep episodes
which occurred in the laboratory at EF, LF, EL, and LL. A subsequent
investigation [25] also reported no difference between controls
and PMDD for nocturnal BTcore recorded during in-lab sleep epi-
sodes at MF and LL. Our results indicate that PMDD status does
not alter nocturnal BTcore, and that patients and controls show sim-
ilar variations in BTcore during sleep across menstrual phases.
Elevated nocturnal BTcore during LP in healthy and PMDD wo-
men may contribute to subjective and objective changes in sleep
[9] since sleep propensity [26] and structure [27] vary with circa-
dian phase coincidental with BTcore variations. When evaluating
PSG sleep across the menstrual cycle in healthy and PMDD women,
slight changes were noted for both groups. We observed a men-
strual phase variation for sleep onset latency, though post-hoc
Fig. 2. The variation of sleep, nocturnal core body temperature (BTcore), urinary comparisons did not reveal significant differences between men-
6-sulfatoxy-melatonin (aMt6s), and urinary progesterone across the menstrual strual phases. Upon closer inspection, the decreases in sleep onset
cycle in PMDD women and controls. BTcore values are the mean temperatures
latency at two time points may be due to the scheduling of the two
recorded during the nocturnal sleep episode, from lights-out to lights-on. Proges-
terone and aMt6s were assayed from urinary excretion during the sleep period and 24-h CP visits which occurred (mean ± SEM) 6.63 ± 0.47 days after
collected after awakening. Select sleep measures include sleep efficiency (SE), sleep menses (range of days after menses onset: 5–11) and
onset latency (SOL), rapid eye movement sleep onset latency (ROL), stage 2 sleep 21.1 ± 0.76 days after menses (range of days after menses onset:
(St. 2), slow wave sleep (SWS), non-rapid eye movement sleep (NREM), and rapid 18–25), respectively, in our participants (Fig. 1). Specifically, the
eye movement sleep (REM). Significant main effects of menstrual phase were
observed for SOL, St. 2, BTcore, and progesterone. REM sleep was reduced during EL
dim lighting (<10 lux), reduced physical activity, constant environ-
compared to EF. Significant main effects of group were observed for SWS and aMt6s, mental conditions, and maintained semi-recumbent posture dur-
with increased SWS and decreased aMt6s in PMDD compared to controls. Values ing the daytime preceding the nocturnal sleep episode may have
are mean + SEM. EF, early follicular; MF, mid-follicular; LF, late follicular; OV, encouraged more rapid sleep initiation at these menstrual phases.
ovulation; EL, early luteal; ML, mid-luteal; LL, late luteal.
We also observed a significant menstrual phase variation for stage
1076 A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078

Table 2
ANOVA results.

Variable Main effect of group Main effect of menstrual phase Group  menstrual phase interaction
a
Progesterone, nmol/l p = NS; d = 0.01 p < 0.001 p = NS
aMt6s, ng/ml p = 0.04b; d = 1.01 p = NS p = 0.06c
Tcore, °C p = NS; d = 0.47 p < 0.001d p = NS
SE, % p = NS; d = 0.03 p = NS p = NS
WASO, % p = NS; d = 0.74 p = NS p = NS
SOL, min p = NS; d = 0.50 p = 0.04e p = NS
ROL, min p = NS; d = 0.39 p = NS p = NS
St. 2, % p = NS; d = 0.61 p < 0.04f p = NS
SWS, % p < 0.004g; d = 1.74 p = NS p = NS
NREM, % p = NS; d = 0.22 p = NS p = NS
REM, % p = NS; d = 0.38 p = NS p = NS

aMt6s, 6-sulfatoxy-melatonin; BTcore, core body temperature; SE, sleep efficiency; WASO, wakefulness after sleep onset; SOL, sleep onset latency;
ROL, rapid eye movement sleep onset latency; St.2, stage 2 sleep; SWS, slow wave sleep; NREM, non-rapid eye movement sleep; REM, rapid eye
movement sleep.
p-Values are for two-way between-subjects ANOVA for repeated measures.
d-Values are for between-group Cohen’s d. d P 0.8 was considered a large effect size.
a
F6,60 = 7.43.
b
F6,60 = 2.15.
c
F1,10 = 5.40.
d
F6,60 = 16.65.
e
F6,60 = 2.44.
f
F6,60 = 2.40.
g
F1,10 = 13.35.

2 sleep, which increased during LP compared to FP. This is consis- SWS and SWA by acting on GABAA-receptors in a manner reminis-
tent with the findings of Driver and colleagues [17], who con- cent of benzodiazepines [33]. Since slow waves are generated by
ducted the first systematic study of sleep across the full interactions between cortical and thalamocortical neuronal net-
menstrual cycle in healthy women. A reduction in REM sleep dur- works [34], the added presence of melatonin receptors at the tha-
ing LP compared to FP has also been reported by us and others, lamic reticular nucleus [35] and the cerebral cortex [36] may
who observed this variation in healthy women [4,9,17,23,28] and indicate a potential site of action. Interestingly, decreased plasma
PMDD patients [4]. GABA concentrations [37] and reduced GABAA-receptor sensitivity
The main group difference observed was a significant increase [38] were also reported in PMDD patients compared to controls. It
in SWS in PMDD compared to controls across the menstrual cycle. is possible that in PMDD women the slow wave-generating mech-
This seems to be at the expense of stage 2 sleep, which is decreased anisms in thalamocortical networks may be allowed to function at
in PMDD, though group-differences were not significant, possibly a higher baseline level, since the attenuating effects involving the
due to the small sample size. Previous PSG-based studies compar- melatonin or GABA systems may be compromised.
ing PMDD and controls are limited in number and suffer from dif- In the current study, PMDD women with insomnia have para-
ferences in experimental design, menstrual phase of study, and doxically increased levels of SWS and unaffected sleep efficiency
PMS/PMDD diagnostic criteria, and are therefore inconsistent. A during nocturnal sleep episodes compared to controls with no
study of eight women with premenstrual depression and eight sleep complaints. In another report, women with severe PMS expe-
controls examined sleep at EF, LF, EL, and LL and noted significantly rienced poorer subjective sleep quality, but no PSG-determined
increased stage 2 sleep and decreased REM sleep in patients com- sleep disturbance, compared to controls [39]. Interestingly, in
pared to controls across the menstrual cycle [6]. In a follow-up those women with severe PMS, poor subjective sleep quality dur-
study, including 14 women with PMDD and nine controls who ing the luteal phase was found to be associated with higher levels
were studied at MF and LL, no significant between-group differ- of state-anxiety. This association between mood symptoms and
ences were observed in sleep parameters [4]. In a group of nine perceived poor sleep may underlie the dissociation of subjective
women with either PMDD or extreme PMS and 12 controls, who and objective sleep measures in these women [39]. Moreover, this
were studied during FP and LL, patients demonstrated significantly dissociation between subjective and objective sleep characteristics
lengthened ROL and a trend for reduced sleep efficiency at both in PMDD women and controls is reminiscent of comparisons made
menstrual phases [5]. between men and women, which determined that, while women
The finding of increased SWS in our PMDD women is interesting report worsened subjective sleep, PSG-based estimates actually
to consider in light of the reduced nocturnal melatonin secretion indicate increased sleep efficiency and fewer awakenings in wo-
that characterizes this patient population [18,29]. Indeed, the re- men [3]. It is also probable that adequately assessing sleep quality
duced 6-sulfatoxy-melatonin levels observed here in PMDD com- requires more complex metrics than just quantifying sleep effi-
pared to controls may be functionally related to the alterations in ciency based on PSG. A 40-h sleep deprivation study suggests that
SWS in these women. Melatonin has soporific properties and can there are sex-based differences in sleep regulation processes [40].
alter specific aspects of sleep architecture. Exogenous melatonin Specifically, women responded to sleep deprivation with a larger
administration during the daytime when endogenous levels are homeostatic response, as illustrated by significantly increased
low induced significant reductions in SWS [30] as well as reduc- SWA power and amplitude during recovery sleep, compared to
tions in stage 3 sleep during the first half of a 16-h sleep opportu- men [40]. A further alteration in homeostatic sleep mechanisms
nity from 16:00–08:00 [31], and during the first 100 min of a appears to exist in PMDD, which indicates that sleep changes in
nocturnal sleep episode [32]. Furthermore, exogenous melatonin these women, like those with major depression, may be the result
also alters the quantitative sleep EEG during a daytime sleep epi- of a faulty homeostatic process of sleep regulation [41,42]. As sta-
sode by reducing slow wave activity (SWA) [33]. Authors have ted earlier, PMDD women have a perception of poor/insufficient
indicated that melatonin may exert its specific effects of reducing sleep. However, these women seem to also experience abnormally
A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078 1077

high sleep need. PMDD women could therefore experience a ‘‘rel- tion between homeostatic and circadian processes, which may be
ative deficit’’ in sleep, such that despite achieving normal sleep further modulated by the menstrual cycle, plays a role in PMDD-
efficiency, sleep duration, and increased SWS, they may suffer a associated sleep characteristics.
relative sleep debt. This is consistent with the observation of con-
comitant increases in SWS and sleep complaints in PMDD women Funding/support
and implies that an increased sleep opportunity may be useful in
alleviating subjective sleep complaints in PMDD. This study was supported by an operating Grant from the Cana-
The strengths of this investigation include studying all partici- dian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). D.B.B. was supported by
pants across their complete menstrual cycle. Furthermore, ovula- scholarships from the Fonds de la recherche en santé du Québec
tion was confirmed in all participants. Importantly, as opposed to (FRSQ), CIHR, and Standard Life Foundation.
prior studies, which included a heterogeneous patient group com-
prising both PMS and PMDD, all the women in the PMDD group Disclosure statement
met the strict criteria for PMDD diagnoses. All PMDD women in-
cluded in the study reported insomnia symptoms. While we be- D.B.B. has participated in speaking and advisory engagements
lieve this to be a strength of the study, insomnia in these women for Servier and Cephalon and is Founder/CEO of Alpha Logik Con-
may contribute to the observed changes in SWS and serve as the sultants, Inc. The remaining authors report no potential conflicts
basis of the proposed altered sleep pressure and hypothesized ‘‘rel- of interest or financial disclosures related to this manuscript.
ative sleep deficit.’’ As such, these findings would be limited in A.S.’s current affiliation is with the New York Obesity Nutrition Re-
their generalizability, and would be applicable to PMDD women search Center, St. Luke’s/Roosevelt Hospital, New York, NY, USA. He
with insomnia symptoms and not necessarily those without sleep worked on this project during the course of his graduate studies at
complaints. An interesting avenue of future research may be to the Centre for Study and Treatment of Circadian Rhythms, Douglas
investigate PSG sleep changes in PMDD women with hypersomnia. Mental Health University Institute.
Some limitations were present in the current study. Sample size
for both groups is small, thus increasing the potential of a type II Conflict of interest
error occurring. Nevertheless, in the intensive study design, each
woman acted as her own control by being studied across a full The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of
menstrual cycle, which we believe adds to the statistical control Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on
of the study. The nature of these laboratory-based experiments the following link: doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2012.05.012.
may have affected the results in participants by disrupting their
Acknowledgements
usual sleeping environment. As both groups might react to this po-
tential stressor differently, we cannot totally exclude the possibil-
We wish to thank the research participants and the staff and
ity that environmental factors have contributed to the observed
students of the Centre for Study and Treatment of Circadian
between-group differences in PSG sleep. Conversely, everyday
Rhythms for their contributions to this investigation. We also
stressors and responsibilities could have also been reduced in par-
thank Dr. Sylvie Rheaume and Abdelmadjid Azzoug R.N. for medi-
ticipants because large portions of the menstrual cycle, including
cal supervision, Francine Duquette for dietary information, Véro-
the symptomatic phase in PMDD women, were spent in the labo-
nique Pagé for statistical advice, and Dr. Julie Carrier, Manon
ratory in a controlled environment. Environmental factors could
Robert and Sonia Frenette for their advice/assistance with the sleep
therefore have influenced mood, as well as subjective and even
analysis, and Zia Choudhry for the sleep recordings.
objective sleep characteristics. Nevertheless, we believe this poten-
tial bias to be minimal as sleep efficiency was comparable between
groups during both laboratory visits. Along these lines, the con- Appendix A. Supplementary data
stant conditions employed during the CP visits may have created
a relaxing environment, fostering a more rapid sleep onset. Never- Supplementary data associated with this article can be found,
theless, these conditions were applied uniformly for all partici- in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2012.05.
pants during both FP and LP, thus limiting its potential as a 012.
contributor to observed group or menstrual phase PSG differences.
Future comprehensive sleep studies based on ambulatory sleep References
recordings could clarify these issues.
[1] Steiner M. Premenstrual syndrome and premenstrual dysphoric disorder:
One PMDD woman included had a high score on the SPAQ, guidelines for management. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2000;25(5):459–68.
though a diagnosis of SAD was not made. Moreover, her reported [2] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
values did not differ from that of the remainder of the PMDD disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC; 1994.
[3] Baker FC, O’Brien LM, Armitage R. Sex differences and menstrual-related
group. Finally, some results presented here were interpreted to changes in sleep and circadian rhythms. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC,
indicate a possible disruption in homeostatic sleep regulatory editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. St. Louis: Elsevier; 2011.
mechanisms within PMDD women. However, in addition to the [4] Parry BL, Mostofi N, LeVeau B, Nahum HC, Golshan S, Laughlin GA, et al. Sleep
EEG studies during early and late partial sleep deprivation in premenstrual
description of baseline SWS included in the current report, to dysphoric disorder and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res
objectively and precisely document an altered homeostatic sleep 1999;85(2):127–43.
system in these women, it is necessary to examine the dynamics [5] Baker FC, Kahan TL, Trinder J, Colrain IM. Sleep quality and the sleep
electroencephalogram in women with severe premenstrual syndrome. Sleep
of SWA decay across the night under baseline conditions and after 2007;30(10):1283–91. Epub 2007/11/01.
increasing homeostatic drive via sleep restriction challenge. [6] Parry BL, Mendelson WB, Duncan WC, Sack DA, Wehr TA. Longitudinal sleep
Considering our current findings in light of prior reports under- EEG, temperature, and activity measurements across the menstrual cycle in
patients with premenstrual depression and in age-matched controls.
scores the preliminary nature of our present SWS results and indi-
Psychiatry Res 1989;30(3):285.
cates the need for more refined investigations of quantitative sleep [7] Hurt SW, Schnurr PP, Severino SK, Freeman EW, Gise LH, Rivera-Tovar A, et al.
EEG in PMDD women. Nonetheless, the large effect size observed Late luteal phase dysphoric disorder in 670 women evaluated for premenstrual
for SWS in the present study points toward an altered homeostatic complaints. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149(4):525–30. Epub 1992/04/01.
[8] Parry BL, Wehr TA. Therapeutic effect of sleep deprivation in patients with
regulation of sleep in PMDD that might be related to their observed premenstrual syndrome. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144(6):808–10. Epub 1987/
abnormal melatonin secretion. This would imply that an interac- 06/01.
1078 A. Shechter et al. / Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) 1071–1078

[9] Shechter A, Varin F, Boivin DB. Circadian variation of sleep during the follicular deprivation in premenstrual dysphoric disorder and normal comparison
and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. Sleep 2010;33(5):647–56. subjects. J Biol Rhythms 1997;12(1):34–46.
[10] Steiner M, Steinberg S, Stewart D, Carter D, Berger C, Reid R, et al. Fluoxetine [26] Lack LC, Lushington K. The rhythms of human sleep propensity and core body
in the treatment of premenstrual dysphoria. N Engl J Med 1995;332(23): temperature. J Sleep Res 1996;5(1):1–11.
1529–34. [27] Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep
[11] Steiner M, Streiner DL, Steinberg S, Stewart D, Carter D, Berger C, et al. The homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow
measurement of premenstrual mood symptoms. J Affect Disord waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J Neurosci 1995;15(5 Pt.
1999;53(3):269–73. 1):3526–38.
[12] Steinberg S, Annable L, Young SN, Liyanage N. A placebo-controlled clinical [28] Baker FC, Driver HS, Rogers GG, Paiker J, Mitchell D. High nocturnal body
trial of L-tryptophan in premenstrual dysphoria. Biol Psychiatry temperatures and disturbed sleep in women with primary dysmenorrhea. Am
1999;45(3):313–20. J Physiol 1999;277(6 Pt 1):E1013–21.
[13] Hardin TA, Wehr TA, Brewerton T, Kasper S, Berrettini W, Rabkin J, et al. [29] Parry BL, Berga SL, Mostofi N, Klauber MR, Resnick A. Plasma melatonin
Evaluation of seasonality in six clinical populations and two normal circadian rhythms during the menstrual cycle and after light therapy in
populations. J Psychiatr Res 1991;25(3):75–87. Epub 1991/01/01. premenstrual dysphoric disorder and normal control subjects. J Biol Rhythms
[14] Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine 1997;12(1):47–64.
morningness–eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol [30] Hughes RJ, Badia P. Sleep-promoting and hypothermic effects of daytime
1976;4(2):97–110. melatonin administration in humans. Sleep 1997;20(2):124–31.
[15] Coleman RM. Periodic movements in sleep (nocturnal myoclonus) and restless [31] Rajaratnam SM, Middleton B, Stone BM, Arendt J, Dijk DJ. Melatonin advances
legs syndrome. In: Guilleminault C, editor. Sleeping and waking disorders: the circadian timing of EEG sleep and directly facilitates sleep without altering
indications and techniques. Menlo-Park: Addison-Wesley; 1982. p. 265–95. its duration in extended sleep opportunities in humans. J Physiol 2004;561(Pt.
[16] Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques 1):339–51.
and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects. Los Angeles: Brain [32] Stone BM, Turner C, Mills SL, Nicholson AN. Hypnotic activity of melatonin.
Information Service, Brain Research Institute, UCLA; 1968. Sleep 2000;23(5):663–9.
[17] Driver HS, Dijk DJ, Werth E, Biedermann K, Borbely AA. Sleep and the sleep [33] Aeschbach D, Lockyer BJ, Dijk DJ, Lockley SW, Nuwayser ES, Nichols LD, et al.
electroencephalogram across the menstrual cycle in young healthy women. J Use of transdermal melatonin delivery to improve sleep maintenance during
Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996;81(2):728–35. daytime. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2009;86(4):378–82.
[18] Parry BL, Berga SL, Kripke DF, Klauber MR, Laughlin GA, Yen SS, et al. Altered [34] Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ. Thalamocortical oscillations in the
waveform of plasma nocturnal melatonin secretion in premenstrual sleeping and aroused brain. Science 1993;262(5134):679–85.
depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990;47(12):1139–46. [35] Lindroos OF, Leinonen LM, Laakso ML. Melatonin binding to the anteroventral
[19] Parry BL, Hauger R, LeVeau B, Mostofi N, Cover H, Clopton P, et al. Circadian and anterodorsal thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neurosci Lett 1992;143(1–
rhythms of prolactin and thyroid-stimulating hormone during the menstrual 2):219–22. Epub 1992/08/31.
cycle and early versus late sleep deprivation in premenstrual dysphoric [36] Mazzucchelli C, Pannacci M, Nonno R, Lucini V, Fraschini F, Stankov BM. The
disorder. Psychiatry Res 1996;62(2):147–60. melatonin receptor in the human brain: cloning experiments and distribution
[20] Parry BL, Meliska CJ, Sorenson DL, Lopez A, Martinez LF, Hauger RL, et al. studies. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 1996;39(1–2):117–26. Epub 1996/07/01.
Increased sensitivity to light-induced melatonin suppression in premenstrual [37] Halbreich U, Petty F, Yonkers K, Kramer GL, Rush AJ, Bibi KW. Low plasma
dysphoric disorder. Chronobiol Int 2010;27(7):1438–53. Epub 2010/08/28. gamma-aminobutyric acid levels during the late luteal phase of women with
[21] Rapkin AJ, Morgan M, Goldman L, Brann DW, Simone D, Mahesh VB. premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1996;153(5):718–20.
Progesterone metabolite allopregnanolone in women with premenstrual [38] Sundstrom I, Backstrom T. Patients with premenstrual syndrome have
syndrome. Obstet Gynecol 1997;90(5):709–14. decreased saccadic eye velocity compared to control subjects. Biol Psychiatry
[22] Schmidt PJ, Purdy RH, Moore Jr PH, Paul SM, Rubinow DR. Circulating levels of 1998;44(8):755–64.
anxiolytic steroids in the luteal phase in women with premenstrual syndrome [39] Baker FC, Sassoon SA, Kahan T, Palaniappan L, Nicholas CL, Trinder J, et al.
and in control subjects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1994;79(5):1256–60. Epub Perceived poor sleep quality in the absence of polysomnographic sleep
1994/11/01. disturbance in women with severe premenstrual syndrome. J Sleep Res
[23] Baker FC, Driver HS, Paiker J, Rogers GG, Mitchell D. Acetaminophen does not 2012. Epub 2012/03/16.
affect 24-h body temperature or sleep in the luteal phase of the menstrual [40] Armitage R, Smith C, Thompson S, Hoffmann R. Sex differences in slow-wave
cycle. J Appl Physiol 2002;92(4):1684–91. activity in response to sleep deprivation. Sleep Res Online 2001;4(1):33–41.
[24] Severino SK, Wagner DR, Moline ML, Hurt SW, Pollak CP, Zendell S. High [41] Boivin DB. Influence of sleep-wake and circadian rhythm disturbances in
nocturnal body temperature in premenstrual syndrome and late luteal phase psychiatric disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2000;25(5):446–58.
dysphoric disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1991;148(10):1329–35. [42] Borbely AA, Wirz-Justice A. Sleep, sleep deprivation and depression. A
[25] Parry BL, LeVeau B, Mostofi N, Naham HC, Loving R, Clopton P, et al. hypothesis derived from a model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol
Temperature circadian rhythms during the menstrual cycle and sleep 1982;1(3):205–10.

You might also like