You are on page 1of 22

Applied & Preventive Psychology 8:1-22 (1999). Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA.

Copyright © 1998 AAAPP 0962-1849/99 $9.50

Lonely hearts: Psychological perspectives


on loneliness

JOHN M. ERNST AND JOHN T. CACIOPPO


The Ohio State University

Abstract
Loneliness is a complex set of feelings encompassing reactions to the absence of intimate and social needs. Although
transient for some individuals, loneliness can be a chronic state for others. We review the developmental, social,
personality, clinical, and counseling psychology literatures on loneliness with an emphasis on recent empirical
findings. Chronic feelings of loneliness appear to have roots in childhood and early attachment processes. Chronically
lonely individuals are more likely to be high in negative affectivity, act in a socially withdrawn fashion, lack trust in
self and others, feel little control over success or failure, and generally be dissatisfied with their relationships compared
to nonlonely individuals. Loneliness has also been associated with a variety of individual differences including
depression, hostility, pessimism, social withdrawal, alienation, shyness, and low positive affect; loneliness is also a
concomitant of more severe disorders, such as clinical depression, borderline personality, and schizophrenia. Although
loneliness affects a large number of individuals and is associated with numerous negative outcomes, relatively few
investigations have examined the efficacy of treatments aimed at alleviating or preventing loneliness. Several
investigations raise the possibility of treating loneliness, but the absence of appropriate comparison groups casts doubt
on the efficacy of many of these treatments. Correlational studies also suggest that one close friend or romantic partner
may be sufficient to buffer those at risk for loneliness. Research on causal processes is sparse, however, and more
research is needed to delineate which factors are antecedents and which are consequences of loneliness.
Key words: Attachment, Depression, Individual differences, Loneliness, Psychopathology, Social network, Social support

Loneliness is a powerful feeling. In their seminal examina- in marital and child-bearing patterns and in the age structure
tion of loneliness, Rubenstein and Shaver (1982a) reported of U.S. society are projected to produce in the 21st century a
that by the early 1980s each month some 35 million Ameri- steady increase in the number of older people who lack
cans were affected with this devastating feeling. Socio- spouses or children. Spouses and children are the people to
demographic changes are worsening this circumstance. By whom older people must often turn for relatedness, guid-
the year 2000, the overall population of the United States is ance, assistance, and support. The prevalence and availabili-
projected to reach 270 million people, an increase of 7% ty of close social relationships and support, therefore, is also
over the preceding decade. Average household size is pro- projected to decline in the coming decades (House, Landis,
jected to decline from 2.69 in 1985 to 2.48 in 2000, and & Umberson, 1988). 1 This change in the structure of our
two-parent households are projected to decrease from 58% society is of special concern because epidemiological stud-
to 53% of households (Bantu et al., 1995). Further, changes ies have now clearly established a relationship between so-
cial support and both mental and physical health (Uchino,
Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996).
In a review of five prospective studies, for instance,
Support for this work was provided by Grant No. T32-MHI9728 from
the National Institute of Mental Health and by the Mind-Body Network of
the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation. The authors would
like to thank Michelle D. Myers for helpful suggestions to the manuscript. 1There is a complex relationship between age and loneliness. Overall,
The authors also would like to thank Eric Bearfield, Dan Church, Rob Rau, there is a positive correlation between age and loneliness. If one takes into
and Dea Riley for their assistance with the preparation of the manuscript. account, however, factors such as age-related illnesses and mobility, then the
Send correspondence and reprint requests to: John M. Ernst at Depart- relationship between age and the loneliness changes. The "pure" relation-
ment of Psychology, Illinois Wesleyan University, P.O. Box 2900, Bloom- ship between loneliness and age is such that loneliness crests during adoles-
ington, IL 61702 or John T. Cacioppo at Department of Psychology, cence and declines as age increases. Nonetheless, as a result of changes in
Ohio State University, 1885 Nell Ave. Mall, Columbus, OH 43210- marriage patterns and because many elderly individuals do suffer from
1222. E-mail may be sent via the Internet to: jernst@titan.iwu.edu or illness and a lack of mobility, the demographic trends suggest that loneliness
cacioppo. 1@osu.edu. can be expected to increase in prevalence over the next few decades.
2 Ernst and Cacioppo

House et al. (1988) found social support to be a major risk liness; that is, desire for either membership in a group of
factor for morbidity and mortality from widely varying friends or desire for an intense one-on-one relationship.
causes. This relationship was evident even after statistically What factors mitigate against the manifestation of loneli-
controlling for known biological risk factors, social status, ness for those who are at risk for loneliness? Finding the
and baseline measures of health. The negative health conse- capacity to form even one close friend or confidant seems to
quences of a lack of social support were particularly strong reduce the likelihood of loneliness in individuals at risk.
among some of the fastest growing segments of the popula- What factors influence or promote this capacity? What tech-
tion: the elderly, the poor, and minorities such as African niques for treating loneliness have been examined in the
Americans. The strength of social support as a risk factor is literature?
comparable to health risk factors such as smoking, high The literature on loneliness is now voluminous. Given the
blood pressure, obesity, and low physical activity. In their demographic changes fostering social isolation, the impor-
seminal review, House et al. (1988) concluded that tant health implications of loneliness, and the intrinsic inter-
est that loneliness holds, it perhaps should not be surprising
the mere presence of, or sense of relatedness with, anoth- that such a vast psychological literature has developed on
er organism may have relatively direct motivational, the topic of loneliness. Using the World Wide Web version
emotional, or neuroendocrinal [sic] effects that promote of PsychInfo (April 1997), a keyword search on the term
health either directly or in the face of stress or other "loneliness" over the years 1967 to 1996 yielded 1,890 jour-
health hazards but that operate independently of cognitive nal articles, dissertations, and book chapters. Extrapolating
appraisal or behavioral coping and adaptation. (p. 544) from the known years 1967 to 1969, approximately 100
articles were associated with loneliness during the 1960s
Furthermore, their review of prospective studies suggests (see Figure 1). During the 1970s this more than doubled to
that poor social relationships are in the same category as 265 articles, In the 1980s the literature nearly tripled with
smoking for predicting negative health outcomes. These 719 articles associated with loneliness. Projecting from the
studies on social support are suggestive of the potential pace of publishing from 1990 to 1996 more than 1,000
impact of loneliness on health. articles associated with loneliness can be expected during
Consider Chris, a hypothetical client, who presents with the 1990s. Thus, during each year of the 1990s the produc-
feelings of depression and anxiety, eating poorly, insomnia, tion of loneliness research has been equal to the production
and feeling ill. With further probing he reveals that he feels during the entire decade of the 1960s. In this review, which
isolation and intense loneliness. He reports that although he is necessarily selective, we focus on the antecedents, experi-
has friends, he feels as if there is no one who really under- ence, and consequences of loneliness that have emerged
stands him. That is, he is emotionally, but not socially, from this explosion of research with an emphasis on more
lonely. If Chris remains lonely, be will be at an increased recent findings.
risk for depression (Shaver & Brennan, 1991), suicide
(Kirkpatrick-Smith, Rich, & Bonner, et al., 1991-1992), a
Developmental Perspectives
host of physical disease processes, and a premature death
(House et al., 1988). What other types of information about An important factor in the development of loneliness in
the antecedents, experiences, and consequences of loneli- children is peer relations. In an elegant series of empirical
ness may be supplied to the therapist? Individuals who were investigations, Asher, Parker, and their colleagues (Asher &
temperamentally high in negative affectivity as children Wheeler, 1985; Parker & Asher, 1987, 1993; Parkhurst &
may be at increased risk for loneliness. Also, those who are Asher, 1992) found that type of peer relations are important
lonely may be experiencing either social or emotional lone predictors of loneliness. They focused on rejected and ne-

Number of
publications

1960s 1970s 1980s 1990s

Figure 1. Publication rate for research on loneliness.


Lonely Hearts 3

glected children. Rejected children are actively disliked, Social/Personality


whereas neglected children are neither liked nor disliked. In
their initial study, Asher and Wheeler (1985) found that Loneliness is associated with person factors, situation fac-
rejected but not neglected children were lonelier than higher tors, and their interaction. In an illustrative study, Shaver,
status peers were. Furthermore, among rejected children, Furman, and Buhrmester (1985) measured state and trait
children who were rejected by their peers because they were loneliness, social skills, and the friendship networks of stu-
submissive were more likely to be lonely than children who dents during the summer prior to their freshman year of
were rejected because they were aggressive (Parkhurst & college and during the fall, winter, and spring of their fresh-
Asher, 1992). Yet, not all rejected children were lonely. man year. First, they found that as students entered the fall
Those children who were in general rejected by the group of their freshman year their satisfaction with their friendship
were buffered from feeling lonely if they maintained one network decreased and their state loneliness increased. Lat-
friendship (Parker & Asher, 1993). Other factors besides er in the academic year their friendship network improved
number of friendships have also been found to be important and state loneliness declined. This pattern suggests the un-
for understanding which children feel lonely. surprising effect that a change in situation, entering college,
Not all children with poor peer relations (not even all ne- can have a large impact on state loneliness. In addition to a
glected children) or children with poor parental attachment main effect for change in situation, however, they found a
feel lonely. Among children at risk for loneliness, who actu- main effect for a person factor. Individuals high in self-rated
ally feels lonely? One possible answer may rest with work by social skills were least lonely regardless of time of year.
Fox, Davidson, and colleagues (e.g., Davidson A. Fox, There was also a person-situation interaction such that so-
1989a; Fox et al., 1995; Fox, Schmidt, Calkins, Robin, & cial skills were most predictive of state loneliness during the
Coplan, 1996). They have found consistently that resting fall when their friendship network was most disrupted but
brain asymmetry is a marker of affective temperament such not as predictive during the summer before starting college
that children with relative right-hemisphere dominance have or during the following spring. In addition, state and trait
more trait negative-affectivity than children who do not have loneliness was most linked during the summer before and
such dominance. In a representative study, Davidson mad Fox the spring after starting college when friendship networks
(1989b) found that infants with baseline relative right- were most stable. That is, there were individuals who were
hemisphere dominance were more likely to cry during mater- lonely even when their friendship networks were likely to
nal separation than were other children. Hence, children with be at their peak.
negative affectivity who suffer peer rejection may be more Where does loneliness fall within a causal sequence re-
likely to experience loneliness than those rejected children garding situation and person factors? In the study by Shaver
without negative affectivity. Two recent studies bear directly et al. (1985), the disruption in friendship networks that ac-
on this point. First, Fox et al. (1995) found that toddlers with companied the start of college increased loneliness. Some
baseline levels indicating relative right-hemisphere domi- individuals, however, were lonely regardless of time of
nance were more likely to withdraw socially from a play year. To what extent do these enduring feelings of loneli-
session with other toddlers than were toddlers without rela- ness reflect a stable situation and to what extent are person
tive right-hemisphere dominance. Second, Fox et al. (1996) factors responsible for these feelings? If the feelings are
examined internalizing problems among shy children. Chil- caused by situational forces, then interventions designed to
dren were assigned to the shyness group based on their score improve friendship networks should reduce feelings of lone-
on the Colorado Child Temperament Inventory (Rowe & liness. If, however, there is something about these individu-
Plomin, 1977) as completed by their mothers combined with als or their behavior that leave them ostracized, then inter-
their coder-rated reticence during play. Fox et al. found that ventions designed to address these factors should be more
among the shy children, children with relative right- effective. As implied by the research by Shaver et al.
hemisphere dominance were more likely to suffer internaliz- (1985), there is merit to both views, and the research re-
ing problems than were shy children who did not have such a viewed below in the following section suggests what some
right-hemisphere dominance. These data suggest that trait- of the more important factors might be.
negative affectivity may be a risk factor for loneliness. Fur-
thermore, research by Elliott, MacNair, Herrick, Yoder, and Social Cognition
B yrne (1991 ) found that participants interacting with confed- One area of fertile research has examined the relationship
erates trained to behave with stereotypically dysphoric man- between loneliness and attributions. For example, Ander-
ners rated such partners more negatively than confederates son, Miller, Riger, Dill, and Sedikides (1994) examined the
who did not behave in such a fashion. Hence, not only may relationship between loneliness attributions for success and
individuals high in negative affectivity be prone to feeling failure--specifically, behavioral and characterological attri-
lonely, they may behave in such a way as to give others a butions for success and failure (see also, Anderson & Ar-
negative impression thereby pushing away potential com- noult, 1985; Anderson, Horowitz, & French, 1983; Ander-
panions (see also Strack & Coyne, 1983). son & Riger, 1991). Although both characterological and
4 Ernst and Cacioppo

behavioral attributions are internal, they are thought to differ Relationships with Others
in their controllability. Because behavior is relatively modi-
fiable, failure outcomes attributed to behavior are thought to Loneliness has been examined in the context of a variety
be potentially avoidable in the future. In contrast, character of interpersonal relations including relationships with
is relatively nonmodifiable. Failure outcomes attributed to strangers, classmates, friends, and romantic partners. For
character, therefore, are thought to be relatively unavoidable example, prior research has shown that lonely individuals
in the future (Janoff-Bulman, 1979), Hence, Anderson et al. have fewer friends, fewer close friends, see their friends as
(1994) predicted that characterological attributions for .fail- not very similar to themselves, and are less likely to have a
ure would be positively correlated with loneliness, whereas romantic partner than nonlonely individuals (Bell, 1993).
behavioral attributions for failure would be negatively corre- Research in this area has moved beyond mere descriptions
lated with loneliness. Anderson et al. also examined circum- of loneliness and has attempted to understand these relation-
stantial attributions, which refer to denying credit for the ships from a more theoretical, process-oriented perspective.
event and placing it with external events. They reasoned that Within this area the theoretical categories of Weiss (1973)
circumstantial attributions for success suggest that the indi- dominate. He proposed two different types of loneliness
vidual making the attributions were uncertain of the source based on the type of relationship deficit: emotional loneli-
of the success and that this would be maladaptive. Hence, ness and social loneliness (which he called "social isola-
they hypothesized that circumstantial attributions for suc- tion"). Emotional loneliness results from the lack of an inti-
cess would be positively related to loneliness. mate attachment, whereas social loneliness results from lack
Participants in their study completed the UCLA Loneli- of membership in a desired group.
ness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980) and provided In an early study on the topic, Russell, Cutrona, Rose, and
explanations for imagined successes or failures. Expert rat- Yurko (1984) examined the predictors, experience, and con-
ers then coded the explanations in terms of characterologi- sequences of emotional and social loneliness. Using a group
cal, behavioral, or circumstantial attributions. As predicted, of college participants, they assessed agreement with feel-
loneliness was positively correlated with attributing failures ing lonely because one did not have an intense one-on-one
to character and negatively correlated with attributing fail- relationship (emotional loneliness) and with feeling lonely
ures to behavior. Also as predicted, loneliness was pos- because one did not have a feeling of belonging to a group
itively correlated with attributing successes to circum- for which there were shared concerns or activities (social
stances. An unexpected finding was that loneliness was also loneliness). As expected, the predictors for emotional and
positively correlated with attributing success to character. social loneliness were different. Feelings of intimacy with
They interpreted this latter finding as indicating that lonely another individual (attachment) and feelings of taking care
individuals feel that both success and failure are beyond of another (opportunity for nurturance) predicted emotional
their control. loneliness, whereas feeling that one's assets are appreciated
In addition to examining the thoughts of lonely individu- by others (reassurance of worth) predicted social loneliness.
als, investigators have examined the thoughts of others In addition to having different predictors, the items assess-
about lonely individuals. These studies have consistently ing social and emotional loneliness were related to different
revealed that lonely individuals are stigmatized by others. In patterns of items on the 20-item UCLA Loneliness Scale.
two recent studies examining this issue (Lau & Gruen, Emotional loneliness (but not social loneliness) was corre-
1992; Rotenberg & Kmill, 1992), participants read vignettes lated with three items assessing feelings regarding relation-
depicting individuals leading either a lonely lifestyle or a ships with individuals. Social loneliness (but not emotional
socially embedded lifestyle. In both studies, participants loneliness), on the other hand, was correlated with three
(perceivers) rated the lonely (targets) more negatively for items assessing feelings regarding relationships with groups
both psychosocial functioning and interpersonal attrac- of individuals. Consistent with the differentiation between
tion/acceptance than the nonlonely. Furthermore, in both the two types of loneliness, there was only a small (though
studies female perceivers rated the lonely targets as less significant) correlation between these two sets of items (r =
well psychosocially adjusted than did male perceivers. In- .17). Importantly, despite different predictors--a low cor-
terestingly, lonely perceivers were more negative toward relation between the two types, and a different pattern of
lonely targets then were nonlonely perceivers (Rotenberg correlations for 6 of the 20 items on the UCLA scale--there
& Kmill, 1992). Hence, lonely individuals are more stig- was considerable overlap in the experience of loneliness for
matized than nonlonely individuals, particularly by lonely individuals encountering the two types of loneliness. This
individuals. To the extent that lonely individuals are cogni- was evidenced by strong correlations between both types of
zant that others think poorly of them for being lonely, these loneliness and the total scale score on the UCLA scale and
individuals may try to deny these feelings or to avoid others significant correlations between both types of loneliness
rather than turning to others for help (Segrin, 1993b). In the with most of the individual items on the UCLA scale. Fur-
next section, we examine differences in the relationships thermore, both social loneliness and emotional loneliness
lonely and nonlonely individuals have with others. predicted depression. Emotional loneliness, but not social
Lonely Hearts 5

loneliness, however, was related to feelings of anxiety. Fi- sure of loneliness--the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Impor-
nally, for coping with feelings of loneliness, both types of tantly, evidence has been found suggesting that emotional
loneliness were associated with employing cognitive prob- and social loneliness are related to the source of relation-
lem solving, but only emotional loneliness was associated ship: emotional loneliness is related to one-on-one rela-
with employing behavioral problem solving. In sum, the tionships such as romantic attachments, whereas social
Russell et al. (1984) study was an early confirmation of the loneliness is related to relationships with groups of individ-
idea that social and emotional loneliness are separable con- uals.
structs. Nonetheless, the study also suggested that individu- If developing both emotional and social relationships is
als suffering from emotional loneliness share many of the important for staving off loneliness, what is the likelihood
same experiences as those suffering from social loneliness. that lonely individuals can develop such relationships with
In more recent work, DiTommaso and Spinner (1993) new acquaintances? Two recent studies have examined the
developed a multi-item assessment of social and emotional interactions of lonely individuals with strangers. In a study
loneliness termed the Social and Emotional Loneliness of social interactions among strangers, Segrin and Kinney
Scale for Adults (SELSA). The SELSA contains three sub- (1995) examined social interactions among strangers and
scales: social, romantic, and family loneliness. DiTommaso found that lonely individuals were more socially anxious
and Spinner suggest that their romantic and family scales despite not differing in self-rated or partner-rated social
tap emotional loneliness and, of course, that their social competence. In a related study, Rotenberg (1994) found that
scale taps social loneliness. They found that the three scales lonely individuals act less trusting with strangers over time.
were internally consistent and uncorrelated with one anoth- Rotenberg investigated this issue using a prisoner's dilem-
er. Nonetheless, they found their romantic and social scales ma game, in which participants played against a confederate
were highly correlated with the UCLA Loneliness Scale and for money. Before each trial, players made known to their
their family scale was moderately correlated with the UCLA opponent whether they intended to play the trial coopera-
scale. In addition, the romantic scale, but not the social tively or competitively. If one player followed through on
scale, was correlated with the number of dates over the last his or her intention to cooperate but the other did not, then a
2 weeks, suggesting discriminant validity for the two scales. "double-cross" by the latter occurred and the trusting player
Thus, the development of the SELSA lends further support lost the trial. During early trials lonely and nonlonely indi-
for the legitimacy of the idea that loneliness may be sepa- viduals were equally trusting. As play continued and occa-
rated into two theoretically distinct, but experientially simi- sional betrayals occurred, however, the lonely individuals
lar constructs--emotional and social loneliness. became much less trusting, whereas nonlonely individuals
In a study that cleverly addressed the discriminant validi- remained as trusting as they were initially. This suggests
ty of the social and emotional loneliness, Stroebe, Stroebe, that feelings of trust by lonely individuals, who are more
Abakoumkin, and Schut (1996) measured emotional loneli- anxious socially to begin with, can be undermined relatively
ness, social loneliness, perceived social support, depression, more easily than the trust of their less socially anxious,
and physical complaints of men and women who were mar- nonlonely counterparts. Close interpersonal relationships
ried or widowed following bereavement. They found that are inherently complex and treacherous so the social anxiety
marital status affected emotional loneliness but not social and vulnerability of lonely individuals' feelings of trust may
loneliness. Furthermore, they found that level of social sup- well hinder lonely individuals from developing or maintain-
port predicted feelings of social loneliness but not emotional ing meaningful relationships with others.
loneliness. They interpreted their findings from an attach- Indeed, evidence examining daily, but perhaps not very
ment perspective, suggesting that emotional loneliness intimate, encounters that lonely individuals have with class-
needs can only he met by an attachment figure such as a mates further suggests that they have a hard time developing
spouse and not by other members of one's social network. strong relationships even in situations where contact is fre-
Similarly, in work examining loneliness in elderly, rural quent. A study by Anderson and Martin (1995) indicated
adults (mean age = 83 years), Dugan and Kivett (1994) that lonely students were characterized by poorer social
found evidence that loneliness was strongly related to mea- skills when interacting with classmates than were nonlonely
sures of emotional loneliness but not as strongly related to students. For instance, lonely individuals were less likely to
social loneliness. Only one social measure was related to be responsive to their classmates during class discussions
loneliness. They found that loneliness was associated with than nonlonely individuals, and lonely students failed to
hearing, but not visual loss, suggesting that remedies such provide "appropriate and effective feedback" (Anderson &
as hearing aids may be useful in combating loneliness in this Martin, 1995, p. 121). Not surprisingly in light of these
group. findings, Nurmi, Toivonen, Salmela, and Eronen (1996) re-
In sum, research has distinguished between two types of ported that lonely individuals were ranked as less popular
loneliness: emotional and social loneliness. These types of by their peers than nonlonely individuals.
loneliness have not been found to be highly correlated with In a further test of the hypothesis that lonely individuals
one another, yet both correlate highly with an overall mea- are unable to form satisfying relationships because they lack
6 Ernst and Cacioppo

social skills, Vitkus and Horowitz (1987) asked lonely and less lonely than unmarried individuals. Within marriages,
nonlonely individuals to adopt either the role of listening to however, be found that for ages 20-49, married women
another individual describe a personal problem or the role of were lonelier than married men were. Although he was able
describing a personal problem to another. Contrary to con- to rule out several hypotheses such as women being more
ventional wisdom, both lonely and nonlonely individuals willing to report loneliness then men, he was unable to
displayed equivalent social skills regardless of role. The account for this difference. In another investigation of the
role, however, did determine the level of social skills that relationship between loneliness and marriage, Barbour
were displayed. Those in the listener role were more active (1993) found that among married clients involved in an
listeners, generated more help, and conversed longer than individual therapy practice, 20% of women and 24% of men
those in the other role. Lonely individuals nevertheless rated were lonely. These lonely individuals were more likely to
themselves as having poorer social skills than nonlonely suffer poor family functioning and marital intimacy.
individuals. The social-skills deficit of lonely individuals, In sum, lonely individuals characterized as being more
therefore, appears to have more to do with their willingness socially anxious, less trusting, and less satisfied with their
or ability to select appropriate social behaviors rather than relationships. Many loneliness researchers, led by Weiss'
their ability to adopt a given social role when explicitly (1973) early example, have been guided in their research
instructed to do so. from an attachment perspective (Bowlby, 1969; Shaver &
Wittenberg and Reis (1986) found that lonely individuals Brennan, 1991). This perspective suggests that chronically
held more negative perceptions toward their roommates lonely individuals developed insecure attachments with
than did nonlonely individuals. Duck, Pond, and Leatham their primary caregivers when they were infants. These in-
(1994) addressed why lonely individuals have a negative secure patterns lead these individuals to behave in ways that
view of their relations with others. They investigated lonely elicit peer rejection in early childhood. This rejection in turn
and nonlonely individuals' assessment of their interactions hinders the development of age-appropriate social skills and
with a friend at two points in time. In the first session each produces a pattern of cognitions in which lonely individuals
participant interacted with his or her friend. Immediately fail to trust others and perceive the self as weak agents in
after the interaction and again after viewing videotape of their environment. Although the extant data are consistent
their interaction with their friend, participants rated the with this attachment perspective, it remains unclear what
quality of the relationship and the quality of the communi- aspects of lonely individuals' cognitive, affective, and inter-
cation. In the second session, the participants again rated the personal processes are antecedents and what aspects are
quality of the interaction and the quality of the communica- consequences of loneliness.
tion simply after being reminded of their interaction during
the first session and after watching videotape of their inter- Person Factors and Individual Differences Associated
action. At all four measurement points, lonely individuals with Loneliness
rated relationship quality more negatively than did non- Recall that Shaver et al. (1985) found that a major change
lonely individuals and lonely individuals were most nega- in social environment resulted in an increase in loneliness
tive after viewing the videotape at session two. In addition, for all individuals but that individual differences also ac-
at time two, but not time one, lonely individuals rated the counted for loneliness regardless of situation. Shaver et al.'s
communication quality of the interaction more negatively (1985) findings indicate that loneliness can be temporally
than did the nonlonely individuals. Duck et al. interpreted stable. Indeed, test-retest for loneliness as measured by the
this pattern of findings as indicating that lonely individuals UCLA Loneliness Scale was found to be .73 over a 2-month
operate from a global negativity toward interpersonal inter- period and .62 over a 7-month period (Russell, 1982). This
actions. When lonely individuals rate component qualities temporal stability has fostered research on the trait proper-
of an interaction soon after the interaction, Duck reasoned, ties of loneliness and its relationship to other traits and
the global negativity has not had time to bias perception of individual differences. For example, individuals who are
the individual communication component. As time passes, lonely for long durations of time, in contrast to those who
however, the negativity bias affects the perception and are lonely for only a short time, score higher on loneliness,
memory of the individual component accounting for the possess lower self-esteem, and carry a better grade point
finding that communication was rated more negatively at average (Ginter & Dwinell, 1994).
time two than at time one. Duck et al. suggest that this Another person factor that has been examined is gender
negative impression may be caused in part by filtering their differences in loneliness. The results of research investigat-
social world through a negative lens. ing whether loneliness is related to gender depend in large
What is the relationship between loneliness and the quali- part on whether individuals are asked to respond to assess-
ty of potentially more intimate relationships such as mar- ment questionnaires that use items containing the words
riage? Torustam (1992) addressed this question in a random "lonely" and "loneliness" or to questionnaires that do not
sample of 2,795 individuals ages 15-80 in Sweden. As make specific references to loneliness. In an examination of
might be expected, he found that married individuals were this issue, for instance, Borys and Perlman (1985) tabled the
Lonely Hearts 7

results of studies using scales that make explicit references Delispanl, 1995; Jones & Kugler, 1993), low in self-esteem
to loneliness or the UCLA Loneliness Scale (which does not (Kamath & Kanekar, 1993; Riggio et al., 1993), and low in
make such references). For the UCLA Loneliness Scale, pleasure (positive affect; Mehrabian & Stefi, 1995). Lonely
gender differences were found in only 4 of 28 studies. For individuals are less likely to use humor as a method of
all four that found a gender effect, the direction was such coping (Overholser, 1992). They are also somewhat more
that males scored higher in loneliness than did females. likely than nonlonely individuals to think about missed op-
Nine of 11 studies using scales that made explicit references portunities, finances, and death, but less likely to think
to loneliness, in contrast, revealed gender differences, with about parenthood (Ben-Artzi, Mikulincer, & Glanbaman,
females reporting greater loneliness than males. In a follow- 1995-96). The pervasiveness of negative affectivity is con-
up experiment, Borys and Perlman (1985) found that per- sonant with Duck et al.'s (1994) assertion that loneliness
ceivers rated lonely male targets more negatively than lone- provides a negative lens through which the world is viewed.
ly female targets, a finding replicated by Lan and Green If the proximal cause of loneliness is a pattern of negative
(1992). Borys and Perlman interpreted this pattern of find- cognitive appraisals about relationships, then these studies
ings to indicate that in general there are no gender differ- suggest that their is plenty of ammunition for negative cog-
ences in loneliness. In the case of scales containing refer- nitive appraisals.
ences to loneliness, however, the gender effect appears to be Although the associations tend to be of small to moderate
attributable to males being less willing to self-report loneli- magnitude, lonely individuals tend to have interpersonal
ness, possibly because lonely males are more stigmatized styles that seem likely either to enhance or lead to relation-
than lonely females are. In a more recent study, Tornstam ship difficulties. The lonely individual is more likely to be
(1992), who studied over 2,000 Swedes using a loneliness shy (Booth et al., 1992; Kamath & Kanekar, 1993; Mehra-
scale that made specific reference to loneliness, also failed bian & Stefl, 1995; Schmidt & Fox, 1995) and less likely
to find a main effect for gender. It is not known at this to be extroverted (Kamath & Kanekar, 1993; Upmanyu,
juncture whether or not the absence of a gender difference Sengal, & Upmanyu, 1994) than the nonlonely individual.
in this study is a result of lonely males and females being Lonely individuals, too, are low in sociability (Schmidt &
similarly stigmatized in Sweden. Fox, 1995) and faith in people (Crandall & Cohen, 1994). In
Following the lead of Weiss (1974), Kraus and her col- addition, lonely individuals are high in conformity and low
leagues (1993) proposed a process model in which loneli- in dominance (Mehrabian & Stefl, 1995). Finally, lonely
ness is conceptualized as a direct consequence of the extent individuals tend to avoid the company of others. They pre-
to which individuals cognitively appraised their interperson- fer solitude (Burger, 1995). They are high in social avoid-
al needs as being met by their relationships or lack there- ance and low in social approach (Nurmi et al., 1996, Studies
of. This relationship fulfillment was operationalized using 1 & 2). This is consistent with the finding that they are low
Weiss' (1974) concept of social provisions (feelings of at- in popularity (but not high in unpopularity, Nurmi et al.,
tachment, social integration, reassurance of worth, nurtur- 1996, study 2). They feel alienated (Crandall & Cohen,
ance, reliable alliance, and guidance). In fact, other research 1994) and they do not trust others (Rotenberg, 1994). De-
has found that lonely individuals have a poorer social net- spite their feelings of distrust and alienation, they are high
work and have a poorer mental set (such as feelings of in social dependency (Overholser, 1992). They are also low
alienation and isolation as well as resentment and bitterness, in social skills (Buhrmester, Furman, Wittenberg, & Reis,
Damsteegt, 1992). Furthermore, Kraus et al. predicted that 1988, Riggio et al., 1993), high in social anxiety (Segrin &
relationship appraisals would be jointly determined by per- Kinney, 1995), and low in social-support quality (though
son factors, situational factors, and one's social network. not in quantity, Riggio et al., 1993). Consistent with these
Person factors and situation factors also were thought to interpersonal deficits, loneliness is negatively correlated
have direct effects on social network. Given this model, with emotional sensitivity (the ability to decode nonverbal
what person factors, both interpersonal and intrapersonal, communication); in contrast, despite this social-skills defi-
might be expected to be related to loneliness? cit, loneliness is positively correlated with social sensitivity
Lonely individuals tend to score high on negative intra- (the ability to decode verbal communication, Segrin, 1993a,
personal traits like pessimism (Davis, Hanson, Edson, & 1993b). On the face of it, this latter finding is surprising
Ziegler, 1992; Davis, Miller, et al., 1992) and low on posi- because one would expect lonely individuals to do a poor
tive intrapersonal traits like optimism (Davis, Hanson, et al., job of decoding the verbal communication of others. Segrin
1992; Davis, Miller, et al., 1992; Nurmi et al., 1996, in two (1993b) speculated on the meaning of this result by report-
separate samples). Similarly, loneliness is strongly nega- ing the findings that lonely individuals are stigmatized by
tively correlated with happiness (Booth, Bartlett, & Bo- others. Therefore, the enhanced social sensitivity of lonely
hnsack, 1992) and life satisfaction (Riggio, Watring, & individuals may prove to be a disadvantage leading to nega-
Throckmorton, 1993; Schumaker, Shea, Monfries, & Groth- tive feelings should they pick up on rejection expressed by
Marnat, 1993). In addition, lonely individuals have been others. Conversely, enhanced social sensitivity could lead to
found to be high in state and trait guilt (Baumeister, Reis, & discomfort in others. Hyper-social sensitivity in lonely indi-
8 Ernst and Cacioppo

viduals may lead them to overanalyze their conversations of the social interactions were not important predictors of
with others, which might result in defensiveness and nega- loneliness. There was one quantitative measure, however,
tive mood in their conversation partners. which did predict loneliness: time spent with females was
In sum, for those individuals for whom loneliness is negatively associated with loneliness. Wheeler et al.'s fine-
chronic, there appears to be a pervasive pattern of social grained analysis suggested that the key to predicting loneli-
withdrawal, isolated activities, mistrust, and negative affec- ness was whether at least one individual in the interaction
tivity. Hence, with the possible exception of greater social supplied a communal style of communication associated
sensitivity (which actually may also have negative out- with females and high femininity scale scores. That is, high-
comes), this landscape of interpersonal processes would be er participant femininity scale scores and more time spent
expected to negatively affect both one's social network and with females were important predictors of lower levels of
one's appraisals as to whether one's relationship needs (so- loneliness. Femininity also played an interesting role in
cial provisions) were being met. meaningfulness of interactions with males. For males, but
How did Kraus et al.'s (1993) model of loneliness, which not females, meaningfulness of interactions with males was
included both intra- and interpersonal factors, fair in an related to both the participant's femininity score and time
empirical test? Recall that they predicted that the relation- spent with females. Meaningfulness of interactions with
ship between personality, situation, and social network with males was an important negative predictor of loneliness for
loneliness would be mediated by an individual's cognitive both males and females. Participants, regardless of gender,
appraisals of his or her relationships (social provisions). rated interactions with females as more meaningful than
Therefore, they investigated measures of person factors, sit- interactions with males. Combined, the pattern for mean-
uation factors, social network, relationship appraisals (so- ingfulness of interaction, time spent with females, and
cial provisions), and loneliness. For person factors, they meaningfulness of interaction with males suggests that for
measured self-esteem (a measure of intrapersonal pro- males and females interactions with females reduces loneli-
cesses) and shyness (a measure of interpersonal processes). ness, and that for males having a communal interaction style
As expected, social provisions mediated most of the rela- or interacting with a male who has a communal interaction
tionships between person factors, situational factors, and style also wards off loneliness. Thus, this study is important
social network with loneliness. In contrast to their predic- for demonstrating that it is qualitative rather than quantita-
tions that relationship appraisals would mediate all relation- tive aspects of social interactions that are important for
ships with loneliness, they found a direct effect of being in a understanding loneliness. These results also fit nicely with
romantic relationship, one aspect of social network, on lone- the differences reviewed previously, suggesting that lonely
liness. They speculated that their measure of appraisals individuals are characterized by cognitive processes and
failed to capture all the necessary types of appraisals such as interpersonal traits that lead to less meaningful interactions.
social status and having a sex partner. They also failed to In another important study, Rook (1987) examined type
find the predicted effect of situation factors on social provi- of social relationship and loneliness. Whereas earlier re-
sions. This relationship was mediated entirely via social search had examined social relationships primarily from a
network. These data suggest that situation factors affect social-support framework (for examples she cites Kahn &
loneliness only indirectly via their impact on one's social Antonucci, 1980; and House, 1981), Rook contrasted the
network and relationship appraisals. These factors in turn effects of social support versus companionship on loneli-
have direct effects on loneliness. ness. She found that companionship was a more important
predictor of loneliness than social support. For example, in
Integrative Research an undergraduate sample, companionship (frequency of so-
How do the individual pieces describing lonely individu- cializing and failure to obtain socializing needs) predicted
als come together? In a review of the loneliness literature, scores on the NYU Loneliness Scale (Rubenstein & Shaver,
Marangoni and Ickes (1989) called for more research (such 1982b), whereas social support (frequency of confiding, fre-
as the Kraus et al., 1993 study just described) on the mecha- quency of help, unmet confiding needs and failure to obtain
nisms by which antecedents such as cognitive styles and needed help) failed to predict NYU Loneliness Scale scores.
social situations produce loneliness and the mechanisms by Importantly, this same pattern held for predicting friendship
which loneliness produces negative outcomes. Three studies satisfaction. Furthermore, with a community sample, fre-
of this type seem noteworthy. quency of socializing predicted a global rating of loneliness,
In the first, Wheeler, Reis, and Nezlak (1983) used a daily whereas frequency of confiding and frequency of help did
diary methodology to examine the quantitative and qualita- not predict loneliness (friendship satisfaction was not mea-
tive aspects of social interactions that are related to nontran- sured with this sample). For these samples there was one
sient loneliness. Their main finding was that an individual's exception to this pattern of results. For the undergraduate
rating of the meaningfulness of the interactions was the sample, companionship and social support were also used to
most important predictor of loneliness. Amount of time, predict scores on the UCLA loneliness scale. For this crite-
frequency of interactions, and other quantitative descriptors rion companionship again predicted loneliness, but although
Lonely Hearts 9

frequency of help and failure to obtain needed help again Clinical Dysfunctions and Psychopathologies
did not predict loneliness, frequency of confiding and fall-
ing to have someone with which to confide did predict Affective Disorders
loneliness. In an attempt to further elucidate these effects, It has long been recognized that loneliness and depression
Rook examined companionship and two forms of social are closely linked (Shaver & Brennan, 1991). Despite the
support: emotional support and instrumental support. She moderate to high correlations between the two, researchers
found that although companionship was a better predictor of have separated these two constructs (Shaver & Brennan,
loneliness than instrumental support, it was an equivalent 1991). In recent years, one attempt to sort out this relation..
predictor relative to emotional support. Thus, this study is ship has involved the dependency and self-critical factors of
important for showing that companionship in addition to the Depressive Experience Questionnaire (DEQ) (Blatt,
social support is an important concept for understanding D'Affitti, & Quinlan, 1976). Blatt and his colleagues (Blatt
loneliness. This study also is important for examining sub- et al., 1995, 1996) have examined two facets, dependence
types of social support and their unique contributions to the and relatedness, of the dependency factor of the DEQ in
understanding of loneliness. normal undergraduate and high school students. The depen-
In a third research program, de Jong-Gierveld (1987) dence facet appears to entail undifferentiated dependence on
tested a causal model that examined background variables, other, whereas the relatedness facet appears to entail feel-
living arrangements, personality characteristics, descriptive ings of loneliness with regard to specific relationships. Al-
characteristics, and subjective evaluations of social network though the potential relationship of these facets to loneliness
as predictors of loneliness. Using a semi-structured inter- is intriguing, validation against loneliness scales is needed
view she measured these variables in a sample using three to confirm these ideas.
separate communities each consisting of single, married, In a study in which the DEQ was explicitly correlated
divorced, and widowed individuals from 25 to 75 years of with a loneliness scale, Schachter and Zlotogorski (1995)
age. After specifying a model she used LISREL (J6resborg asked normal university students and high school teachers
& S6rbom, 1981) to determine goodness of fit for her mod- to complete the DEQ and the UCLA Loneliness Scale, short
el. After some small adjustments, the model accounted for form. The purpose was to examine loneliness with regard to
52.3% of the variance. Although self-concept and subjective both dependent (anaclitic) and self-critical (introjective) de-
evaluation of social relationships were among the more im- pression. They found that loneliness was common for indi-
portant predictors of loneliness, living arrangement was the viduals with both types of depression. Contrary to their
most important predictor of loneliness. That is, individuals expectations, however, individuals with self-critical depres-
living alone were lonelier than individuals living with a sion were much more likely to be lonely than were individu-
spouse or loved one. As de Jong-Gierveld noted, this fits als with dependent depression. They speculated that self-
well with the attachment perspective that only certain rela- critical individuals might be encompassed in an aloneness
tionships can serve to stave off loneliness (see also the that comes from feeling that one is the worst person alive.
Stroebe et al., 1996 findings). These data suggest that: Accordingly, feeling lonely is related to feeling self-blame
depression as well as to feeling dependency depression. In
The problems of lonely people, especially of those living sum, it appears that dependency-related loneliness is a long-
on their own and of parents without partners, cannot be ing for a secure relationship, self-critical-related loneliness
regarded as individual failures only. Characteristics of is related to feeling alone in the world, and both types of
our society, such as the prevailing norms concerning mat- loneliness are associated with dysphoria.
rimony and the nuclear family, the emphasis on individu- A further conundrum regarding the relationship between
al fulfillment, and the downgrading of the importance of depression and loneliness is the pattern of their occurrence
satisfactory and stable relationships and of the impor- when examined by gender (Koenig, Isaacs, & Schwartz,
tance of commitment to others, might also be considered 1994). Among adolescents, boys experience the same or
loneliness-provoking factors. (p. 127) greater loneliness relative to girls; depression and loneliness
are highly correlated for both boys and girls; but girls experi-
ence greater levels of depression than boys did. In order to
Clinical and Counseling Perspectives
examine this more closely, Koenig et al. (1994) administered
As reviewed previously, feeling lonely is related to feeling scales of loneliness (UCLA Loneliness Scale) and depres-
rejected by peers, negative attributions and perceptions, sion (Beck Depression Inventory) (Beck et al., 1961) to 397
poor interpersonal relations, and negative personality traits. high school males and females. After confirming that they
The clinical and counseling literatures have examined lone- had replicated the pattern of relationships found in prior
liness as it relates to other clinical dysfunctions and psycho- research, they examined two pieces of information, which
pathologies, special populations who may be at risk for combined to suggest an explanation for this puzzling rela-
loneliness, and the evaluation of treatments of loneliness tionship: (a) Highly depressed girls lonelier than mildly de-
and its associated risk factors. pressed girls but highly depressed boys were not lonelier
10 Ernst and Cacioppo

than mildly depressed boys were; (b) depression was distrib- distinct for borderline patients relative to that experienced
uted differently for boys and girls. For girls, the distribution by major depressives without borderline. More extreme
of depression was on a continuum--a significant number of loneliness and feelings of separation from attachment
girls fell within all three ranges (nondepressed, mildly de- figures marked the borderline depression. Richman and
pressed, and highly depressed). For boys, the distribution Sokolove (1992) replicated this finding. These researchers
was not along a continuum--the bulk of boys fell in the modified the UCLA Loneliness Scale by adding items to tap
nondepressed group, a fairly large number fell in the highly extreme loneliness. For example, new items referred to a
depressed group, but very few boys reported into the mildly sense of hollowness, emptiness, and inner deadness. Two
depressed group. Because of the relatively large number of factors emerged from this scale: aloneness marked by a
girls in the mild depression category the overall depression feeling of complete isolation from others and evocative
mean was higher for girls than for boys. Because boys' memories marked by an inability to recall memories of
loneliness was equal for mild and high depression, but girls' friends unless they were physically present. Borderline pa-
loneliness was lower for mild relative to high depression, the tients relative to control (nonborderline psychiatric patients)
gender effect for mean depression did not yield a concomi- were much more extreme on both aloneness and inability to
tant gender effect for mean loneliness. It appears that boys recall others. These individuals were also marked by devel-
and girls seem to report, if not experience, depression differ- opmental delays in object representation, more negative ear-
ently. Because so few boys report mild depression but do ly memories, and fewer positive memories than controls.
report high levels, it appears that depression is an all or none Finally, even after controlling for depression, loneliness still
phenomenon for boys. Because girls report depression fall- was able to discriminate between patients with a borderline
ing along a continuum, it appears that girls are able to experi- diagnosis and those without the diagnosis. Thus, loneliness
ence or at least report depression in a fine-tuned fashion. appears to be an important component of the borderline
In sum, although the loneliness and depression link is not patient's affective experience. These results also suggest
yet completely understood, it appears that different types of that extreme loneliness may be tied to an inability to recall
loneliness may be related to different types of depression. others and negative early experiences.
For example, it seems that dependency depression is linked
to desire for an intimate one-on-one relationship and self- Psychoses
critical depression is linked to feeling alone in the world Loneliness is also a component of the experience of in-
(Schachter & Zlotogorski, 1995). Because desire for a one- dividuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. DeNiro (1995)
on-one relationship is the definition of emotional loneliness examined retrospective accounts of 20 patients with
and feeling alone in the world is quite similar to social residual-type schizophrenia. She found that alienation, so-
loneliness (desire for belongingness to a group), perhaps cial isolation, and loneliness increased over the life span of
severe cases of these types of loneliness lead to differential these individuals. These individuals also expressed a desire
types of depression (dependency and self-critical, respec- for contact with individuals in the community separate from
tively). In order to confirm these links between types of hospital patients and staff. Neeleman and Power (1994) rep-
depression and loneliness more research will be necessary licated the main finding that patients suffering from chronic
where measures like the DEQ and the UCLA Loneliness schizophrenia were lonelier than controls. Furthermore,
Scale are both administered. Finally, it appears that gender schizophrenic patients reported lower actual and lower ideal
is linked to different patterns of depression and loneliness. It levels of social support relative to controls. The discrepancy
may be, as Koenig et al. (1994) suggest, that the pattern is between actual and ideal social support, however, was the
explained in large part because females are more likely to same for both groups. This suggests the possibility that
rate themselves as moderately depressed than are boys. factors other than ideal-actual discrepancies of social sup-
Whether this difference is caused by actual differences in port are important for understanding the loneliness that is
the experience of loneliness as suggested by the authors or associated with schizophrenia patients.
by differences in the ability to report mild and moderate
levels of depression awaits further research. Suicidal Ideation and Suicide
What feelings drive someone to take his or her own life?
Personality Disorders Although answers to this question are complex, feelings of
In a study by Overholser (1992), individuals with depen- loneliness appear to be an important factor. Kirkpatrick et
dent personality but without major depression experienced al. (1991-92), for example, found a positive correlation
loneliness, social-skills deficits, and mild depression. Lone- between suicidal ideation and loneliness. In a sample of
liness has also been examined in individuals diagnosed with 1,067 suicides, Heikkinen, Aro, and Lonnqvist (1994) found
borderline personality disorder with and without depression that recent life events (events within the last 3 months)
relative to individuals suffering from major depression predicted suicide. The next of kin and attending health care
without concurrent borderline symptoms (Westen et al., workers of these individuals were interviewed to determine
1992). In this study, the character of the depression was what events took place in the months preceding the suicide.
Lonely Hearts 11

Three of the seven predictors for suicide involved relation- sies followed on the heels of conflicts involving loneliness,
ship difficulties (family discord, separation, and death and humiliation, anger, and feelings of inadequacy and rejec-
illness in family). In addition, the specific complaint of tion. Loneliness was listed most frequently as the result of
loneliness was high for female suicides, but not male sui- these conflicts. For pedophiles, however, deviant sexual
cides. Given the gender differences reviewed previously, fantasies were related only to negative mood.
which indicate that males are less likely to report loneliness In sum, loneliness has been associated with a variety of
than are females, this suggests that family members and clinical dysfunctions and psychopathologies. Specifically,
mental health care cannot rely on self-reported loneliness as loneliness is related to depression and other psychiatric dis-
a marker for suicide intent in males. Rather, the other con- orders, eating disorder, substance abuse, self-destructive
comitants of loneliness reviewed previously may need to be behaviors, and deviant behaviors. Because of the close
probed. It is also possible, though we believe less likely, relationship between loneliness and psychopathologies,
that loneliness is not an important determinant of suicide in loneliness has been used to predict individuals at risk for
males. depression and use of psychiatric services (Kent & Yellow-
lees, 1995). As with personality factors, however, the causal
Self-Regulatory Behavior relationship between loneliness and psychopathology is not
One self-regulatory behavior that has been recently ex- yet clear and awaits resolution in future research.
amined for its relationship with loneliness is bulimia ner-
vosa. Coric and Murstein (1993) examined the prevalence Special Populations
of bulimia nervosa and its psychological correlates in a One area of research in loneliness that has received exten-
college sample. Of the participants matching the criteria for sive coverage has been the evaluation of loneliness in popu-
bulimia nervosa, they were higher in loneliness relative to lations that are at risk for social alienation, isolation, and
participants not suffering from bulimia. separation. These groups include the bereaved, individuals
Substance abuse, another form of self-regulatory behav- with physical diseases and disabilities, individuals with
ior, has also been examined for its association with loneli- mental retardation or developmental disabilities, divorced
ness (for a review of alcohol abuse and loneliness see individuals, gay men, and combat veterans.
Akerlind & Hornquist, 1992). Loneliness has been found to
be a risk factor associated with substance abuse. Three re- Physical Disability or Disease
cent findings confirm this relationship. In the first, Sadava Over the last 5 years the bulk of the research on special
and Pak (1994) examined relationship factors associated populations has been on individuals suffering from a physi-
with problem drinking among adults ages 20-29. They cal disability or disease. Among those with a physical dis-
found that unattached individuals were much more likely to ease, loneliness is more common to individuals with dia-
be problem drinkers than were attached individuals. Fur- betes (Coclami & Bor, 1993; Hanestad, 1992), men with
thermore, these unattached individuals displayed higher lev- AIDS (Cherry & Smith, 1993), and individuals suffering a
els of loneliness and depression and less satisfaction with brain injury (Harrick et al., 1994). Among those with a
social support than did attached individuals. Consistent with physical disability, loneliness tends to be elevated. Consis-
this finding, for elderly adults, Jensen et al. (1994) found tent with the Dugan and Kivett (1994) finding that loneli-
that loneliness was one of five factors (the other four were ness was related to hearing impairment in elderly adults,
frequency of doctor visits, subjective health, smoking, and King and Stephens (1992) found that individuals suffering
objective health) that when combined predicted 70% of in- from an auditory disability with normal hearing were lone-
dividuals who were using drugs. Further evidence for this lier than age, gender, and socioeconomically-matched nor-
link was provided by Stacy, Newcoms, and Bentler (1995), mals. In a related study, Hart, Zasloff, and Benfatto (1996)
who found a positive correlation between loneliness and found that hearing dog owners relative to prospective hear-
cocaine abuse. ing dog owners were less lonely. Not only did the dog itself
provide companionship, but the dog also helped improve
Deviant behaviors communication with family, the hearing community, and
Regarding deviant Behaviors, two recent studies have neighbors. Foxall et al. (1992) and Barron et al. (1992)
demonstrated that increased deviancy among sexual of- examined loneliness in low-vision adults and elderly adults.
fenders is related to feeling lonely. In the first study (Seid- In general, for these groups they found that loneliness was
man, Marshall, Hudson, & Robertson, 1994), rapists and not a problem, suggesting that vision problems are not as
child molesters were lonelier than were exhibitionists detrimental to relationships as hearing problems. Low-
(and other prisoners, including both violent and nonviolent vision individuals who were not satisfied with their social
nonsex-related offenders, and community control popula- support were at risk for loneliness, however. Consistent
tions). A second study (McKibben, Proulx, & Lusignan, with the idea that not all individuals within at-risk groups
1994) examined the fantasies of rapists and pedophiles in actually experience loneliness, research has demonstrated
prison. For rapists, but not pedophiles, deviant sexual fanta- substantial differences in loneliness within these groups.
12 Ernst and Cacioppo

For example, Hanestad (1992) divided diabetic patients into depression and anger as well. The authors speculated that
those who were high versus low in perceiving their disease Black men might seek less material support from friends
as having an impact on their daily life. Those high in such and family for help with their condition for fear of being
perceptions were lonelier than individuals who were low. It labeled as a homosexual. The authors further speculated that
appears that a positive mental attitude or some structural status as a gay is more stigmatizing for Blacks than for
support (such as a hearing dog) can buffer the loneliness Whites.
effects of being in certain isolated or alienated groups.
Immigrants and International Students
Be reavement Loneliness has been examined in Korean immigrants and
A second group of individuals who suffer from loneliness Asian students studying in the United States. Two studies
are those who are recently bereaved. As described previ- have examined Korean immigrants. In the first, Nab (1993)
ously, Stroebe et al. (1996) found that emotional loneliness conducted interviews with 90 Korean families. Among
for widows and widowers could not be buffered via one's these individuals, loneliness along with alienation, relation-
social-support network. There was no substitute for the pri- ship difficulties, and language problems were listed as com-
mary attachment figure. This is consistent with Wortman mon problems. In the second, Pang (1995) examined major
and colleagues' work on bereavement (Wortman, Silver, & depression in Korean immigrants. Regardless of whether
Kessler, 1993). In a prospective study they found that indi- they were classified as depressed or nondepressed, 50% of
vidual's with the best marriages faced the longest recovery individuals in both classifications reported loneliness as an
from bereavement. It seems likely that it is these individuals important daily problem. Finally, Yang and Clum (1994)
who relied on their spouses to fulfill their emotional examined depression, suicidal ideation, and hopelessness in
loneliness-related needs. The loneliness that individuals feel Asian students living in the United States. They found that
in response to the loss of a loved one does not seem to loneliness was correlated with depression, hopelessness,
depend on the cause of death. For example, McIntosh and and suicidal ideation. They suggested that loneliness was a
Kelly (1992) examined grief reactions to the loss of a close partial mediator for the relationship between life stress and
friend or family member who died of suicide, accident, or hopelessness. Furthermore, loneliness was not directly re-
natural causes. For all three groups of survivors, loneliness lated to suicidal ideation, but depression and hopelessness
was high and equally strong. Similarly, extreme loneliness mediated its effect on suicidal ideation. The extant data
was reported by both family members and partners who clearly suggest that individuals living in a foreign land suf-
survived the AIDS-related death of a loved one. Hence, fer from elevated levels of loneliness. The inclusion of more
loneliness appears to be a gripping but normal response to comparison groups, such as matched foreign students living
the death of a loved one whether the loved one is a friend or in their homeland and American students living abroad or in
family member and regardless of the cause of death. the United States would be informative regarding the speci-
ficity of and possible factors underlying this result.
Stigmatized Groups
Recent research in this area has focused on the experience Developmentally and Learning-disabled Individuals
of being a gay man. Berger and Mallon (1993) recruited Margalit and her colleagues have examined loneliness in
gay men through gay community groups and social net- both developmentally and learning-disabled children. In one
works. They found that individuals in this group had well- study, Margalit and Ronen (1993) compared the self-
developed social-support networks. Using a single-item reported loneliness of preadolescents with mild retardation
face-valid measure, they found that loneliness was higher versus adolescents with mild retardation. They found that
for individuals living alone relative to individuals living the adolescents were less lonely than the preadolescents.
with others and it was higher for individuals who were not Furthermore, the pattern of loneliness in relation to other
in a committed relationship relative to those in a committed variables was different for the two groups. For adolescents,
relationship. Gant and Ostrow (1995) examined loneliness, loneliness was related to a complex set of variables (em-
psychosocial functioning, and social support in White and pathy, assertion, and cooperative skills), whereas for the
Black gay men with HIV. They measured four facets of younger group loneliness was related to only one variable
social support: social conflict, objective social integration, (assertion skills). In a second study, Margalit and Ben-Dov
material support from family, and material support from (1995) examined the relationship of learning-disabled status
friends. For White men, all four indicators were related to and environment with loneliness. They found a main effect
loneliness--as social support decreased, loneliness in- for learning-disabled status such that learning-disabled
creased. For Black men, in contrast, only social support and students were lonelier and less socially competent than
objective social integration were related to loneliness; mate- nonlearning-disabled students. In addition, although kibbutz
rial support from friends and family were not related to learning-disabled students versus city learning-disabled stu-
loneliness. This differentiation of White and Black gay men dents reported equivalent feelings of loneliness, teachers
held for other measures of psychosocial functioning such as reported the kibbutz students had fewer maladaptive behav-
Lonely Hearts 113

iors and peers rated them as having more friends. In a third time. It may be that social loneliness would recede soon in
study (Margalit & Efrati, 1996), learning-disabled and low- divorced men if they attend to and address their concerns
achieving children were lonelier and less accepted by peers about the reactions of friends and family to their divorce. As
than average-achieving children. These three studies sug- the current review suggests, however, social and emotional
gest that learning at a slower pace than others may foster loneliness is differentiable. Friends and family may do more
feelings of loneliness at least in school settings. One ques- to ease the feelings of social than emotional loneliness for
tion that remains open is why learning-disabled students in these men. The loss of their attachment bond and its associ-
the kibbutz were not buffered by their relative increase in ated emotional loneliness may require other resources such
friends against loneliness. Self-perceptions of relationship as the passage of time or a new attachment figure.
quality in these groups may prove informative. In sum, loneliness has been associated with a variety of
special groups. Among these groups one division for sorting
Veterans of War out the lonely might entail separating individuals for whom
Two recent studies have examined loneliness in Vietnam loneliness is caused by membership in an alienated group or
War veterans and World War II veterans. Using a qualita- by a change in attachment status. Bereaved and divorced
tive analysis of first-person accounts, Vallenga and Chris- individuals are examples of the latter set; the physically ill
tenson (1995) examined the mental status of 12 Vietnam and disabled, the developmentally ill and disabled, immi-
veterans in the wake of Operation Desert Storm. They found grants, veterans of war, and stigmatized individuals are
that loneliness and isolation were two recurrent themes examples of the former set. It may prove worthwhile to
among veterans. Macleod (1994) examined the clinical in- examine loneliness from a process orientation involving
terviews of elderly World War II veterans experiencing re- measures of the person factors, situation factors, social net-
currence of posttraumatic stress disorder. Among the factors work, relationship appraisals, and emotional versus social
implicated in recurrence were feelings of loneliness, retire- loneliness when comparing members of these two sets. The
ment, wedding anniversaries, and service reunions. Al- Stroebe et al. (1996) work suggests that attachment change
though it appears that loneliness is a factor in the lives of will primarily affect emotional loneliness, not social loneli-
war veterans, it is not clear whether loneliness is higher in ness. Thus, the predictors of loneliness for the two groups
this group relative to age-matched controls because control may differ.
individuals were not included in these studies.
Evaluation of Treatments
Miscellaneous Groups Loneliness, as reviewed previously, is related to a com-
Two other recent, but unrelated, studies examining the plex set of individual differences, situational factors, social
adult children of alcoholics and divorced men are of note. network, and relationship variables. In addition, there are
Martin (1995) examined loneliness patterns in adults who many different patterns of these variables that can lead to a
are children of alcoholics (ACAs) versus adults who were lonely state. Loneliness itself is a complex feeling encom-
not children of alcoholics (ACNAs). Although loneliness passing both social and emotional components. Given these
levels were the same for the two groups, there was a much elaborate, highly differentiated patterns, a variety of treat-
larger negative cox-relation between loneliness and intimacy ments have been applied to loneliness.
for ACNAs than for ACAs. In addition, intimacy was lower Rook (1984) proposed a categorization scheme in which
overall for ACAs relative to ACNAs. This pattern of results she outlined both goals and approaches to the treatment of
suggests that a family history of alcohol abuse may be asso- loneliness. She suggested three goals for the treatment of
ciated with attachment problems or with intimacy needs loneliness: (a) the promotion of social bonding as a way to
being devalued. reduce loneliness, (b) the enhancement of coping with lone-
For divorced men, Mitchell-Flynn and Hutchinson (1993) liness, and (c) the prevention of loneliness. For these treat-
performed a factor analysis on problems and concerns ment goals there exist individual, group, and environmental
shared by this group. They found five factors: loneliness; approaches. In addition, although some programs are aimed
interacting with their former spouse; worry about the judg- specifically at loneliness, there are other programs aimed at
ments of their friends, co-workers, and family; control; and relevant issues whose treatment also may relieve loneliness.
competence. These men reported significantly less loneli- In the period 1992-1996 our Psychlnfo search revealed 16
ness at 1 year following divorce relative to 1 month follow- protocols relevant to the treatment of loneliness. At least
ing divorce. Concomitant with this decrease in loneliness one study fell into each of Rook's categories.
over the year following divorce the participants became less
concerned about being judged negatively by their friends Promote Social Bonding
and family because of the divorce. These concerns may Rook's (1984) first category of treatments for loneliness
foster feelings of alienation among recently divorced men. includes approaches aimed at helping lonely individuals de-
Longitudinal research is needed to determine whether lone- velop social skills and adopt healthier social cognitions (see
liness precedes or follows the decline in these concerns over McWhirter, 1990 for an earlier review of social-skills train-
14 Ernst and Cacioppo

ing and loneliness). Several recent studies have used such who self-identified as adult children of alcoholics. The goal
approaches. Brough (1994) used a 10-week Adlerian-based of the treatment was to teach ACAs about the codependency
group treatment for lonely individuals but, as with many of profile (over or undercontrol, hypervigilence, lack of trust,
studies in this area, the absence of a control group makes the excessive feelings of responsibility, emotional numbness,
results equivocal. Brough's treatment focused on using edu- reluctance to express anger, and inability to express needs
cation and analysis of past and current relationships to for intimacy) that fits these individuals and to help the par-
change feelings of inferiority, help group members develop ticipants develop individual identities that are separate from
interests in the interests of other people, and encourage the their family life with their parents. Group sessions consisted
members to make changes in their lifestyle such as chal- of the group leader educating the participants about why it
lenging their faulty perceptions. Posttreatment scores for may be difficult for members to openly discuss their par-
group members relative to pretreatment scores indicated a ent's alcoholism thereby violating family secrecy, denial or
decrease in loneliness and a concomitant increase in social personal problems, and codependency. Group leaders also
interaction. In another study for which there was no control employed process techniques with which they challenged
group, Rodway (1992) conducted a group treatment for participant behaviors within the meeting that were related to
loneliness that was directed specifically at self-examination. control, codependency, or invalidation of other's feelings.
Nine participants who complained of chronic loneliness Although no control group was examined, the loneliness
were induced to examine their expectations for their rela- scores and other criterion variables were compared to nor-
tionships as well as their perceptions of what they bring to a med data. The pretreatment scores for the participants were
relationship by completing the Heimler Scale of Social not different from the normed data. This result is consistent
Functioning (Heimler, 1975). This 55-item scale assesses an with the study reviewed earlier (Martin, 1995) finding that
individual's perceptions of his or her social relationships. loneliness scores did not differ overall for ACAs versus
After completing the scale in the first session, participants ACNAs. Over the course of treatment, the participants de-
discussed their answers during the remaining 10 sessions. clined from average loneliness to below average loneliness.
Group members then generated solutions to relationship Whether this effect reflected the effect of being in the thera-
problems that were revealed by the Heimler Scale. Al- py group or if it had a broader and lasting impact on social
though changes in loneliness were not directly evaluated, and emotional loneliness is not known.
participants reported decreased depression, increased hope- The goal of another such study was to increase and im-
fulness and feelings of control, and the acquisition of strate- prove the social-support networks of parents at risk for child
gies for dealing with loneliness. Unfortunately, regression maltreatment (Lovell & Richey, 1995). An experimental
to the mean would predict the changes found in these stud- group received training in the stages of relationships, friend-
ies. ship formation, communication and social-support skills,
As detailed previously, loneliness has been related to and accessing community resources. Hence, the training
child molestation. As part of a group treatment for child focused on skill practice and conceptual knowledge for rela-
molesters, Marshall, Bryce, Hudson, Ward, and Moth tionships as well as the availability of community groups
(1996) emphasized the reduction of loneliness and the en- such as government support agencies. In support of the
hancement of intimacy. The treatment protocol employed in training efficacy, relative to the control group, the experi-
this study is noteworthy for developing treatments based on mental group showed an increase in the size of the support
the theoretical antecedents and experience of loneliness. For network. No changes in loneliness as measured by a single-
instance, because loneliness has been related to attachment item index were seen for the experimental versus the control
style, participants assessed their own attachment style using group, however. Although single-measure assessments are
an adult measure of attachment (Bartholomew, 1993) and nonoptimal, this result is consistent with research reviewed
were then given feedback regarding the costs and benefits of previously showing that loneliness is not simply the absence
their attachment style relative to other styles. In addition, of a social network.
the participants were taught about the difference between In another study (De Luca, Hazen, & Curler, 1993), girls
social and emotional loneliness and the concomitant need aged 10-11 years who had been the victims of incest partic-
for different types of relationships. Furthermore, this treat- ipated in a 10-week group therapy (there was no control
ment attempted to train the participants to tolerate being group). The themes that were the focus of this therapy were
alone without making negative attributions regarding their the effects of disclosing abuse, self-esteem, and the legacy
worthiness as individuals. Individuals in the group were of abuse for dating and sexuality. One of the outcome mea-
lower in loneliness and higher in intimacy posttreatment sures was the feeling of loneliness. Each treatment session
relative to pretreatment. consisted of a 15-minute discussion about activities of the
Four studies have examined group treatments in which past week; a 45-minute discussion of the theme for the week
loneliness was not the primary target but nonetheless was (e.g., the effects of disclosing abuse); a 30-minute diary
measured as an outcome variable. Cooper and McCormack completion during which participants wrote and drew pic-
(1992) examined a short-term treatment for college students tures expressing their feelings aroused during the theme
Lonely Hearts 15

discussion. Group leaders interacted with the participants social loneliness than in emotional loneliness). The social
during all three periods. Although individuals in this group group, however, showed equally large decreases in both
reduced externalizing and internalizing behaviors and im- social and emotional loneliness. It appeared that the skills
proved self-esteem and anxiety levels, no changes in loneli- learned by this group generalized to helping both types of
ness were reported. The authors speculated that because relationships. The emotional group, however, failed to show
sexual abuse ruptures the child's attachment system with a decline in emotional or social loneliness. The author's
their parents, focusing solely on the child is too narrow. speculated that the emotional treatment raised expectations
They advised that a multimodal treatment including work too high for romantic relationships preventing participants
with the family might be necessary to reduce loneliness in in this group from being satisfied with their relationships.
this population. Margalit (1995a, 1995b) has developed a computer-
Finally, in yet another treatment study with no compari- assisted protocol for teaching social skills to students with
son control group, Kostyk et al. (1994) examined an age- learning and mental disabilities. The computer program pre-
specific group treatment for elderly individuals with a sents the user with a scenario in which a student with a
substance-abuse problem. The goal was to increase life sat- learning or mental disability is confronted with an unpleas-
isfaction and the ability to deal with problems specific to ant situation such as being teased for being in a class for
aging. Each group was team led by a professional social students requiring extra assistance. After each scenario, the
worker and a peer counselor in recovery from abuse. There student is asked to choose among multiple options for how
were three treatment tools: (a) The program used the recov- to deal with the situation. The student loses points if he or
ered peer counselor as a role-model for recovery; (b) the she chooses aggressive strategies such as hitting someone or
group leaders encouraged the participants to engage in self- passive strategies such as crying, and the student is awarded
disclosure; and (c) the program used a three-phase cycle: points for more appropriate responses such as assertion and
elimination of chemical dependency, improving relation- negotiation. In the first study (Margalit, 1995a), students
ship problems such as showing affection to a grandchild, with learning disabilities and behavior disorders used the
and improved lifestyle changes aimed at stress management program and took part in teacher-led groups in which they
and relapse prevention. Solutions to lifestyle problems were discussed the day's lessons and personal experiences related
generated through group discussion. Among the solutions to the lesson. Following training both individuals with
discussed were ideas for improving sleep, increasing activ- learning disabilities and behavior disorders who were ini.-
ity by going to the mall, and gaining new perspectives on tially high in loneliness reported decreased loneliness. The
family conflicts. The clinical evaluation of the group leaders contributions of the computer program relative to the group
was that loneliness was a pervasive issue for the partici- discussion that followed in affecting this change were not
pants. Following treatment, the participants attributed a de- assessed. In the second study (Margalit, 1995b)participants
crease in depression and an increase in social activities to with a mild intellectual disability again used the program
their membership in the group. and took part in teacher-led group discussions. In contrast to
In addition to group-treatment approaches, several stud- the first study, participants in this study did not evidence
ies have examined individual-treatment approaches to the any decrease in their loneliness relative to a control group,
diminishment of loneliness. In a rigorous experiment Participants, however, did show improvements in behavior
employing multiple treatment comparisons that included a (cooperation, self-control, and assertion) and peer accep-
control group, the investigators tested treatments specific- tance. Teachers who led group activities in this study ob-
ally aimed at emotional versus social loneliness (McWhir- served that although the program was excellent in training
ter & Horan, 1996). Participants engaged in one of four appropriate social skills, it failed to explicitly offer sugges-
cognitive-behavioral interventions: emotion focused, social tions to the participants about how to deal with feelings of
focused, combined, or control. The emotion-focused treat- loneliness.
ment was aimed at improving cognitions, affect, and behav- Although several studies have examined group and indi-
ior related to developing and maintaining intimate one-on- vidual approaches promoting social bonding, only one study
one relationships (e.g., role-playing self-disclosure during a has investigated an environmental approach to easing lone-
date). The social-focused treatment was aimed at improving liness. Mullins, Cook, Mushel, Machin, and Georgas (1993)
cognitions, affect, and behavior related to developing and examined the characteristics of a senior citizens nutrition
maintaining group affiliations (e.g., initiating conversations and activities program. This program provides in-home
and lowering stress in small groups). The combined treat- meals to some participants and group meals and activities
ment emphasized helping both types of relationships and the for a larger group of participants. Loneliness and social
control treatment emphasized developing self-help strate- provisions were both measured. Participants in the group
gies during group discussion for which the group leader program were less lonely than those in the in-home pro-
specified no specific topic. It was hypothesized that treat- gram. In addition, group participants reported less need for
ments would have fairly specific effects on loneliness (e.g., support (guidance), less need to provide nurturance to oth-
the social-treatment group would show larger decreases in ers (nurturance), and less need for social integration, but no
16 Ernst and Cacioppo

differences were seen in attachment, reliable alliance, and of coping-skills training for 237 students randomly assigned
enhancement of worth than in-home participants. Partici- to an experimental or a no-treatment control condition. Par-
pants in the group program were also younger and healthier ticipants and controls took part in 12 weekly sessions. Con-
than participants in the in-home program. Participants in trols took part in an unrelated psychological study, whereas
both programs reported that their involvement helped re- participants in the treatment group received training in three
duce loneliness. Although the authors noted that the nature phases. In phase one, participants received information on
of their evaluation did not allow for a test of effectiveness of the nature of distress (e.g., hopelessness and depression,
the programs for reducing loneliness, they suggested that self-destructive behavior and suicide, etc). In phase two,
their work demonstrated that the program was effective in participants were taught coping skills--how to reduce
reaching an elderly population and that their work could stress, how to be empathic toward others, and how to help
serve as a baseline for a follow-up investigation. friends who are in trouble. In phase three, which was con-
ducted concurrently with phase two, participants rehearsed
Enhance Coping with Loneliness their new coping skills in groups and as part of take-home
A second approach to dealing with loneliness is to help assignments. Group therapy was modeled on rational-
individuals cope with their loneliness. Rook (1984) sug- emotive education and cognitive-modification strategies.
gested that these treatments could be aimed at widows and Relative to the control condition, participants showed an
widowers coping with the loss of a partner and helping increase in empathy but no change in loneliness. Partici-
individuals find more enjoyment in solitary activities. With pants were most satisfied with the unit on helping friends
the exception of work by Marshall et al. (1996) reviewed who were suicidal but least satisfied with the unit on loneli-
earlier, we are unaware of any recent studies attempting to ness. The authors suggested that the loneliness unit needed
help individuals feel at ease during solitary activities, but to be revamped to be more relevant to students. They also
Nash (1992) examined the effect of a 6-week group- suggested that they needed to use a longer measurement
treatment program aimed at helping widowed persons. The period to capture changes in trait loneliness.
program explained the stages of grief, stress management, In sum, studies during the early 1990s that attempted to
finances, and goal setting. At the end of the program the alleviate loneliness through promoting social bonding fo-
participants reported diminished loneliness and a larger cused on social-skills training. Group approaches with this
friendship network. focus have been successful at diminishing loneliness. Three
In addition to group treatment, two crisis telephone lines innovative programs have met with limited success. Marga-
have been examined, one aimed at lonely elders ( Loring, lit (1995a, 1995b) has developed a computer-assisted
Smith, & Thomas, 1994) and the other more generally program for training social skills in students with mild
aimed at troubled citizens in a Croatian city during wartime disorders. McWhirter et al. (1996) attempted to develop
(Stumfol, 1992). The lonely elder hotline was a crisis line differential treatments for social versus emotional loneli-
set up for use during the holiday season. The primary reason ness. This type of differentiation makes sense in light of
for calling given by the elders was loneliness, grief, and theoretical insights and empirical investigations suggesting
bereavement. These individuals were then referred to other that these types of loneliness differ in their antecedents. The
agencies. Follow-ups with the callers revealed that 86% of social treatment in the McWhirter et al. study did diminish
those aged 55-74 contacted the referral agency and 36% of loneliness; however, it failed to affect emotion and social
those 75 and older contacted the referral agencies. Those loneliness differentially. Marshall et al. (1996) were suc-
who did not, felt that they were too immobile for the agency cessful in alleviating loneliness in a group of inmates incar-
to help. Those who did not contact the referral agency, were cerated for child molestation. Their treatment not only fo-
contacted by the referral agency. A more general-purpose cused on the antecedents of loneliness, attachment style, but
hotline was set up for civilian crises during wartime in also educated participants about the different needs required
Croatia. The largest number of calls was from panicked to satisfy emotional and social loneliness, and worked with
individuals, but 8% of the calls were from individuals seek- participants to better tolerate states of being alone. Unfor-
ing relief from feelings of loneliness. These studies suggest tunately, it is difficult to judge whether this program was
that crisis lines may offer a way for lonely individuals to successful as a result of the special population it treated.
reach out to others. Unfortunately, research on efficacy of Nonetheless, given this initial, limited success, the develop-
the interventions and referrals offered to the callers of these ment of treatment programs that focus on elaborated theo-
hot lines is still needed. ries of loneliness such as the distinction between emotional
and social may improve the outcomes for individuals suffer-
Prevention of Loneliness ing from loneliness and are worth pursuing. Although fo-
Our survey of the published literature over the past 5 cusing on building social bonds does seem to facilitate an
years uncovered only one primary prevention study in improvement in social networks, unfortunately a concomi-
which loneliness served as a criterion measure. In this study, tant decrease in loneliness is not always reported. This
Klingman and Hochdorf (1993) evaluated the effectiveness seeming treatment failure may simply be a failure to wait
Lonely Hearts 17

long enough to assess loneliness following treatment. It guilt and they blame their loneliness on inadequacies in
takes time for social networks to develop and initiate a themselves. The human costs of loneliness range from risks
decrease in loneliness. Alternatively, it may not be sufficient of affective disorders, marital and familial problems, and
to train social skills, even those aimed at both emotion and unfulfilled potential to susceptibility, morbidity, and mortal-
social types of loneliness. Consistent with this possibility, ity from widely varying causes.
Lovell and Richey (1995) increased the size of the social Given the evidence regarding the personality and behav-
network of the participants in their study, but failed to show ior of the chronically lonely individual it would seem pos-
concomitant decline in loneliness. In line with basic re- sible to conclude that lonely individuals are lonely for just
search on loneliness such as that conducted by Kraus et al. cause. Who wants to be friends with a gloomy and untrust-
(1993) treatments directed at the cognitive appraisals of ing individual who appears to lack social skills? Because of
relationships such as social provisions is also needed. Re- the correlational nature of much of the extant research on
latedly, more research is needed to delineate which factors loneliness, however, the direction of causation has not been
are antecedents and which are consequences of loneliness. clearly determined. Does negativity lead to poor early at-
Finally, the absence of appropriate comparison groups re- tachments, poor peer relations, and negative cognitive and
mains a problem when interpreting many studies in this personality patterns leading to loneliness? Or, does loneli-
area. ness result in many of the relationships with cognition and
Despite the rapid growth of the literature on loneliness, personality reviewed previously? Recall the study by Vitkus
research focused on helping those suffering from or at risk and Horowitz (1987) in which lonely and nonlonely partici-
for loneliness is still sparse. Although several studies have pants were assigned roles in a conversation. This study
examined small group and organizational (e.g., school- demonstrated that lonely individuals know how to use ap-
based) approaches to loneliness, interventions based on in- propriate social skills and can be induced to use those skills.
dividual therapy is less common. Because many aspects of Unfortunately, they appear to fail to do so in their everyday
loneliness have developmental roots and because relation- lives. Why do they fail to use skills that they already pos-
ships involve one-on-one interactions, individual therapy sess, which may lead them to just the types of relationships
may be important to consider. Second, more work on coping that could alleviate their loneliness? If lonely individuals are
with loneliness (as suggested by the teachers who led aware of their stigmatized status, then this awareness may
groups in one study; Margalit, 1995b) and enjoying solitary cause them to withdraw and behave passively when inter-
activities appears worthwhile. For example, one of the most acting with others. This behavior, in turn, helps maintain
effective treatments for showing declines in loneliness in- their lonely status.
cluded a component about living with aloneness (Marshall A transactional model in which environmental factors
et al., 1996). Thus, the negative affect associated with lone- leads to loneliness and loneliness operates on the environ-
liness can be reduced if lonely individuals learn to enjoy ment in such a way that loneliness is maintained seems to
solitary time rather than longing for others during these offer the best description of loneliness. This type of model
times. Third, much of the work in this area has not included fits with Coyne's (Coyne, Burchill, & Stiles, 1991; Strack &
appropriate comparison conditions. This is not simply a Coyne, 1983) theorizing regarding depression. Although de-
methodological nicety as one may not be able to distinguish pression and loneliness are separable constructs, their over-
between an effective intervention and regression to the lap allows us to suspect that similar principles might oper-
mean. ate. That is, in order to understand loneliness we must not
only consider the personality of the lonely individuals and
the ways in which he or she operates on the environment,
CONCLUSIONS
but also the ways in which the social environment operates
The portrait of a lonely person that is offered by recent on the lonely individual. For example, when considering
research suggests that the lonely person lacks trust in others whether an individual's social provisions are being met it is
and feels that success and failure is beyond his or her con- not only important to consider how biases in the individu-
trol. The lonely individual is dissatisfied with his or her al's mental processes might lead him or her to misinterpret
relationships and has an interpersonal style that seems at whether his or her social provisions are being met, but also
odds with developing more satisfying relationships. This whether the environment is providing adequate resources to
person is withdrawn and high in negative affect. This nega- meet the social provisions. When the environment is not
tive affect seems to be one key to understanding the lonely fulfilling social provisions, it is easy to imagine a normal
individual. Studies of early childhood indicate that those individual developing the negative traits and behaviors
with high negative affect are likely to be socially with- that characterize the lonely individual. Recall de Jong-
drawn. Even among shy children, negative affectivity pre- Gierveld' s (1987) admonishment that "characteristics of our
dicts a decrease in social interactions. The adult lonely s o c i e t y . . , might also be considered loneliness-provoking
person seems to be awash in negative affectivity such as factors" (p. 127). Thus, although research suggests that
depression and cynicism. These people are wracked with there are stable individual differences in loneliness, the an-
18 Ernst and Cacioppo

tecedents and consequences of these individual differences other psychopathologics have been tested. Yet loneliness
are not well understood. The extant research nevertheless has been implicated in borderline patients and sexual of-
offers guidance to the health professional. For example, fenders. Other individuals suffering from pathologies such
because loneliness is more stigmatizing for males, males as schizophrenia also suffer from loneliness. To what extent
appear to be less likely to self-report loneliness. Mental treatments for loneliness diminish unpleasant affect or aid in
health professionals may need to probe more subtly for treatment of the accompanying psychopathology is un-
feelings of loneliness in male than female populations. known.
Given the high incidence of loneliness and the volumi- Following in the tradition set forth by pioneers in this
nous literature on loneliness, it is noteworthy that few proto- area, process-oriented and intervention research on loneli-
cols for the treatment and prevention of loneliness have ness promise to provide a fine-grained understanding of the
been tested. Many of the tests that do exist failed to provide antecedents, consequences, and concomitants of loneliness.
control groups for a more complete assessment of the treat- For instance, experience-sampling or daily-interaction data
ment's effectiveness. More research with appropriate con- may provide valuable information about how personality
trols would be valuable, especially considering how difficult and situational factors affect the selection, execution, and
such research is to conduct. It seems likely that focusing experience of social interactions. Standardized protocols for
treatments based on what we know about the components of assessing loneliness or for studying the effects of loneliness
loneliness (e.g., McWhirter & Horan, 1996) and related on cognitions, affect, physiology, and behavior may also
concepts such as social provisions will be most successful in facilitate cross-lab comparisons. Brief psychological
treating loneliness. It also seems clear that while social- stressors that can be implemented in the laboratory (e.g.,
skills treatments are effective in improving interactions with speech stressors) may hold some promise in this regard.
others, they are not sufficient to provide relief from lonely Finally, intervention studies represent an important means
feelings. Hence, treatments that help individuals understand of examining experimentally the role of theoretical factors
their feelings of loneliness and understand them as resulting in the experience of loneliness. As research on loneliness
from both person and situation factors may be useful. Sim- unveils the secrets of the lonely hearts, prevention and inter-
ilarly, no recent research has focused on treatments for help- vention programs can be better designed. Given the preva-
ing individuals better enjoy solitary activities. In addition, lence and human cost of loneliness, this outcome cannot
few interventions for treating loneliness in association with occur too soon.

REFERENCES

Akerlind, I., & Hornquist, J. O. (1992). Loneliness and alcohol riage. Journal of Group Psychotherapy, Psychodrama and Socio-
abuse: A review of evidences of an interplay. Social, Science, metry, 46, 70-72.
and Medicine, 34, 405-414. Barton, C. R., Foxall, M. J., von Dollen, K., Shull, L. A., & Jones,
Anderson, C. A., & Arnoult, L. H. (1985). Attributional models of P. A. (1992). Loneliness in low-vision older women. Issues in
depression, loneliness, and shyness. In J. Harvey & G. Weary Mental Health Nursing, 13, 387-401.
(Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (pp. 235- Bartholomew, K. (1993). From childhood to adult relationships:
279). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Attachment theory and research. In S. Duck (Ed.), Learning
Anderson, C. A., Horowitz, L. M., & French, R. (1983). Attribu- about relationships (pp. 30-62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
tional style of lonely and depressed people. Journal of Person- Baum, A., Cacioppo, J. T., Melamed, B. G., Gallant, S. J., &
ality and Social Psychology, 45, 127-136. Travis, C. (1995). Doing the right thing: A research plan for
Anderson, C. A., Miller, R. S., Riger, A. L., Dill, J. C., & Se- healthy living. Human Capital Initiative Strategy Report.
dikides, C. (1994). Behavioral and characterological attribu- Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
tional styles as predictors of depression and loneliness: Review, Baumeister, R. F., Reis, H. T., & Delespaul, P. A. E. G. (1995).
refinement, and test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Subjective and experiential correlates of guilt in daily life. Per-
ogy, 66, 549-558. sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1256-1268.
Anderson, C. A., & Riger, A. L. (1991). A controllability attribu- Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mack, J., & Erbaugh, J.
tional model of problems in living: Dimensional and situational (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of
interactions in the prediction of depression and loneliness. So- General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
cial Cognition, 9, 149-181. Bell, B. (1993). Emotional loneliness and the perceived similarity
Anderson, C. M., & Martin, M. M. (1995). The effects of commu- of one's ideas and interests. Journal of Social Behavior and
nication motives, interaction involvement, and loneliness on Personality, 8, 273-280.
satisfaction: A model of small groups. Small Group Research, Ben-Artzi, E., Mikulincer, M., & Glaubman, H. (1995-96). The
26, 118-137. multifaceted nature of self-consciousness: Conceptualization,
Asher, S., & Wheeler, V. A. (1985). Children's loneliness: A com- measurement, and consequences. Imagination, Cognition and
parison of rejected and neglected peer status. Journal of Con- Personality, 15, 17-43.
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 500-505. Berger, R. M., & Mallon, D. (1993). Social support networks of gay
Barbour, A. (1993). Research report: Dyadic loneliness in mar- men. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 20, 155-174.
Lonely Hearts 19

Blatt, S. J., D'Affitti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976). Experiences loneliness, and death anxiety. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Soci-
of depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal ety, 30, 135-136.
Psychology, 85, 383-389. de Jong-Gierveld, J. (1987). Developing and testing a model of
Blatt, S. J., Zohar, A. H., Quinlan, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., & Hart, B. loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
(1995). Subscales within the dependency factor of the Depres- 119-128.
sive Experiences Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assess- De Luca, R. V., Hazen, A. & Cutler, J. (1993). Evaluation of a
ment, 64, 319-339. group counseling program for preadolescent female victims of
Blatt, S. J., Zohar, A., Quinlan, D. M., Luthar, S., et al., (1996). incest. Elementary School Guidance and Counseling, 28, 104-
Levels of relatedness within the dependency factor of the De- 114.
pression Experiences Questionnaire for Adolescents. Journal of DeNiro, D. A. (1995). Perceived alienation in individuals with
Personality Assessment, 67, 52-71. residual-type schizophrenia. Issues in Mental Health Nursing,
Booth, R., Bartlett, D., & Bohnsack, J. (1992). An examination of 16, 185-200.
the relationship between happiness, loneliness, and shyness in DiTommaso, E., & Spinner, B. (1993). The development and ini-
college students. Journal of College Student Development, 33, tial validation of the social and emotional loneliness scale for
157-162. adults (SELSA). Personality and Individual Differences, 14,
Borys, S., & Perlman, D. (1985). Gender differences in loneliness. 127-134.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 63-74. Duck, S., Pond, K., & Leatham, G. (1994). Loneliness and the
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume 1. Attachment. evaluation of relational events. Journal of Social and Personal
New York: Penguin. Relationships, 11, 253-276.
Brough, M. F. (1994). Alleviation of loneliness: Evaluation of an Dugan, E., & Kivett, V. R. (1994). The importance of emotional
Adlerian-based group therapy program. Individual Psychology: and social isolation to loneliness among very old rural adults.
Journal of Adlerian Theory, Research & Practice, 50, 40-51. Gerontologist, 34, 340-346.
Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, H. R. Elliott, T. R., MacNair, R. R., Herrick, S. M., Yoder, B., & Byrne,
(1988). Five domains of interpersonal competence in peer rela- C. A. (1991). Interpersonal reactions to depression and physical
tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, disability in dyadic interactions. Journal of Applied Social Psy-
991-1008. chology, 21, 1293-1302.
Burger, J. M. (1995). Individual differences in preference for soli- Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., Marshall, T. R. Coplan,
tude. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 85-108. R. J., Porges, S. W., Long, J. M., & Stewart, S. (1995). Frontal
Cherry, K., & Smith, D. H. (1993). Sometimes I cry: The experi- activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of
ence of loneliness for men with AIDS. Health Communication, age. ChiM Development, 66, 1770-1784.
5, 181-208. Fox, N. A., Schmidt, L. A., Calkins, S. D., Rubin, K. H., & Coplan,
Coclami, T., & Bor, R. (1993). Family relationships in Greek R. J. (1996). The role of frontal activation in the regulation and
insulin-dependent diabetics. Counselling Psychology Quar- dysregulation of social behavior during the preschool years.
terly, 6, 267-279. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 89-102
Cooper, A., & McCormack, W. A. (1992). Short-term group treat- Foxall, M. J., Barron, C. R., yon Dollen, K., Jones, P. A., & Shull,
ment for adult children of alcoholics. Journal of Counseling K. A. (1992). Predictors of loneliness in low vision adults.
Psychology, 39, 350-355. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 14, 86-99.
Coric, D., & Murstein, B. I. (1993). Bulimia nervosa: Prevalence Gant, L. M., & Ostrow, D. G. (1995). Perceptions of social support
and psychological correlates in a college community. Eating and psychological adaptation to sexually acquired HIV among
Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 1, 39-51. White and African American men. Social Work, 40, 215-224.
Coyne, J. C., Burchill, S. A. L., & Stiles, W. B. (1991). An interac- Ginter, E. J., & Dwinell, P. L. (1994). The importance of perceived
tional perspective on depression. In C. R. Snyder & D. R. Forsyth duration: Loneliness and its relationship to self-esteem and aca-
(Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health demic performance. Journal of College Student Development,
perspective (pp. 317-349). New York: Pergamon Press. 35, 456-460.
Crandall, C. S., & Cohen, C. (1994). The personality of the stig- Hanestad, B. R. (1992). Self-reported impact of insulin-dependent
matizer: Cultural world view, conventionalism, and self- diabetes mellitus (IDDM) on daily life and quality of life expe-
esteem. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 461-480. rience. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 6, 229-240.
Damsteegt, D. (1992). Loneliness, social provisions and attitude. Harrick, L., Krefting, L., Johnston, J., Carlson, P., & Minnes, P.
College Student Journal, 26, 135-139. (1994). Stability of functional outcomes following transitional
Davidson, R. J., Fox, N. A. (1989a). Asymmetrical brain activity living program participation: 3-year follow-up. Brain Injury, 8,
discriminates between positive and negative affective stimuli in 439-447.
human infants. Science, 218,1235-1237. Hart, L. A., Zasloff, R. L., & Benfatto, A. M. (1996). The socializ-
Davidson, R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1989b). Frontal brain asymmetry ing role of hearing dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science,
predicts infants' response to maternal separation. Journal of 47, 7-15.
Abnormal Psychology, 98, 127- l 31. Heikkinen, M. E., Aro, H. M., & Lonnqvist, J. K. (1994). Recent
Davis, S. F., Hanson, H., Edson, R., & Ziegler, C. (1992). The life events, social support and suicide. Yrjo Jahnsson Founda-
relationship between optimism-pessimism, loneliness, and level tion VIII Medical Symposium: Depression: Preventive and risk
of self-esteem in college students. College Student Journal, 26, factors (1992, Porvoo, Finland). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinav-
244-247. ica, 89, 65-72.
Davis, S. F., Miller, K. M., Johnson, D., McAuley, K., & Dinges, Heimler, E. (1975). Survival in society. London: Weidenfeld and
D. (1992). The relationship between optimism-pessimism, Nicolson.
20 Ernst and Cacioppo

Hojat, M., & Crandall, R. (1987). Loneliness: Theory, research and Macleod, A. D. (1994). The reactivation of post-traumatic stress
applications. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2. disorder in later life. Australian and New Zealand Journal of
House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Psychiatry, 28, 625-634.
Addison-Wesley. Marangoni, C., & Ickes, W. (1989). Loneliness: A theoretical re-
House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social rela- view with implications for measurement. Journal of Social and
tionships and health. Science, 241, 540-545. Personal Relationships, 6, 93-128.
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1979). Characterological versus behavioral Margalit, M. (1995a). Social skills learning for students with learn-
self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. Journal of Per- ing disabilities and students with behaviour disorders. Educa-
sonality and Social Psychology, 37, 1798-1809. tional Psychology, 15, 445-456.
Jensen, E., Dehlin, O., Hagberg, B., Samuelsson, G., Svensson, T., Margalit, M. (1995b). Effects of social skills training for students
& Hultberg, B. (1994). Medical, psychological, and sociologi- with an intellectual disability. International Journal of Disabili-
cal aspects of drug treatments in 80-year-olds. Zeitschrift fur ty, Development and Education, 42, 75-85.
Gerontologie, 27, 140-144. Margalit, M., & Ben-Dov, I. (1995). Learning disabilities and so-
Jones, W. H., & Kugler, K. (1993). Interpersonal correlates of the cial environments: Kibbutz versus city comparisons of loneli-
Guilt Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 246- ness and social competence. International Journal of Behav-
258. ioral Development, 18, 519-536.
Ji3resborg, K. G., & Srrbom, D. (1981). LISREL-V, analysis of Margalit, M., & Efrati, M. (1996). Loneliness, coherence and com-
linear structural relationships by maximum likelihood and least panionship among children with learning disorders. Education-
squares methods'. Uppsala, Sweden: University of Uppsala, al Psychology, 16, 69-79.
Department of Statistics. Margalit, M., & Ronen, T. (1993). Loneliness and social compe-
Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. (1980). Convoys over the life-course: tence among preadolescents and adolescents with mild mental
Attachment, roles and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. Brim retardation. Mental Handicap Research, 6, 97-111.
(Eds.) Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 252- Marshall, W. L., Bryce, P , Hudson, S. M., Ward, T., & Moth, B.
286). Boston: Lexington Press. (1996). The enhancement of intimacy and the reduction of
Kamath, M., & Kanekar, S. (1993). Loneliness, shyness, self- loneliness among child molesters. Journal of Family Violence,
esteem, and extraversion. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 11, 219-235.
855-857. Martin, J. I. (1995). Intimacy, loneliness, and openness to feelings
Kent, S., & Yellowlees, P. (1995). The relationship between social in adult children of alcoholics. Health and Social Work, 20, 52-
factors and frequent use of psychiatric services. Australian and 59.
New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 29, 403-408. McIntosh, J. L. & Kelly, L. D. (1992). Survivors' reactions: Sui-
King, K., & Stephens, D. (1992). Auditory and psychological fac- cide vs. other causes. Crisis, 13, 82-93.
tors in "auditory disability with normal hearing." Scandinavian McKibben, A., Proulx, J., & Lusignan, R. (1994). Relationships
Audiology, 21, 109-114. between conflict, affect and deviant sexual behaviors in rapists
Kirkpatrick-Smith, J., Rich, A. R., & Bonner, J. F. (1991-92). and pedophiles. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 571-
Psychological vulnerability and substance abuse as predictors 575.
of suicide ideation among adolescents. Omega--Journal of McWhirter, B. T. (1990). Loneliness: A review of the current
Death and Dying, 24, 21-33. literature, with implications for counseling and research. Jour-
Klingman, A., & Hochdorf, Z. (1993). Coping with distress and nal of Counseling and Development, 68, 417-422.
self harm: The impact of a primary prevention program among McWhirter, B. T., & Horan, J. J. (1996). Construct validity of
adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 121-140. cognitive-behavioral treatments for intimate and social loneli-
Koenig, L. J., Isaacs, A. M., & Schwartz, J. A. J. (1994). Sex ness. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Person-
differences in adolescent depression and loneliness: Why are ality, Social, 15, 42-52.
boys lonelier if girls are more depressed? Journal of Research Mehrabian, A., & Stefl, C. A. (1995). Basic temperament compo-
in Personality, 28, 27-43. nents of loneliness, shyness, and conformity. Social Behavior
Kostyk, D., Lindblom, L., Fuchs, D., Tabisz, E., & Jacyk, W. R. and Personality, 23, 253-264.
(1994). Chemical dependency in the elderly: Treatment phase. Mitchell-Flynn, C., & Hutchinson, R. L. (1993). A longitudinal
Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 22, 175-191. study of the problems and concerns of urban divorced men.
Kraus, L. A., Davis, M. H., Bazzini, D. G., Church, M., & Kirch- Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 19, 161-182.
man, C. M. (1993). Personal and social influences on loneli- Mullins, L. C., Cook, C., Mushel, M., Machin, G. & Georgas, J.
ness: The mediating effect of social provisions. Social Psychol- (1993). A comparative examination of the characteristics of
ogy Quarterly, 56, 37-53. participants of a senior citizens nutrition and activities program.
Lau, S., & Gruen, G. E. (1992). The social stigma of loneliness: Activities, Adaptation, and Aging, 17, 15-37.
Effect of target person's and perceiver's sex. Personality and Nah, K. (1993). Perceived problems and service delivery for Ko-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 182-189. rean immigrants. Social Work, 38, 289-296.
Loring, M. T., Smith, R. W., & Thomas, K. (1994). Utilization of a Nash, P. (1992). New horizons for widowed persons: A caring
time-limited holiday hotline by older adults. Gerontologist, 34, program. Psychology--A Quarterly Journal of Human Behav-
557-560. ior, 29, 44-47.
Lovell, M. L., & Richey, C. A. (1995). The effectiveness of social- Neeleman, J., & Power, M. J. (1994). Social support and depres-
support skill training with multiproblem families at risk for sion in three groups of psychiatric patients and a group of
child maltreatment. Canadian Journal of Community Mental medical controls. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiol-
Health, 14, 29-48. ogy, 29, 46-51.
Lonely Hearts 21

Nurmi, J. E., Toivonen, S., Salmela, K., & Eronen, S. (1996). Schachter, E. P., & Zlotogorski, Z. (1995). Self-critical and depen-
Optimistic, approach-oriented, and avoidance strategies in so- dent aspects of loneliness. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and
cial situations: Three studies on loneliness and peer relation- Related Sciences, 32, 205-211.
ships. European Journal of Personality, 10, 201-209. Schmidt, L. A., & Fox, N. A. (1995). Individual differences in young
Overholser, J. C. (1992). Sense of humor when coping with life adults' shyness and sociability: Personality and health correlates.
stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 700-804. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 455-462.
Pang, K. Y. (1995). A cross-cultural understanding of depression Schumaker, J. F., Shea, J. D., Monfries, M. M., Groth-Marnat, G.
among elderly Korean immigrants: Prevalence, symptoms and (1993). Loneliness and life satisfaction in Japan and Australia.
diagnosis. Clinical Gerontologist, 15, 3-20. Journal of Psychology, 127, 65-71.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later per- Segrin, C. (1993a). Social skills deficits and psychosocial prob-
sonal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psycho- lems: Antecedent, concomitant, or consequent? Journal of So-
logical Bulletin, 102, 357-389. cial and Clinical Psychology, 12, 336-353.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship Segrin, C. (1993b). Effects of dysphoria and loneliness on social
quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance perceptual skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77, 1315-1329.
and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Develop- Segrin, C., & Kinney, T. (1995). Social skills deficits among the
mental Psychology, 29, 611-621. socially anxious: Rejection from others and loneliness. Motiva-
Parkhurst, J. T., & Asher, S. R. (1992). Peer rejection in middle tion and Emotion, 19, 1-24.
school: Subgroup differences in behavior, loneliness, and inter- Seidman, B. T., Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. J.
personal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 28, 231-241. (1994). An examination of intimacy and loneliness in sex of-
Richman, N. E., & Sokolove, R. L. (1992). The experience of fenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 518-534.
aloneness, object representation, and evocative memory in bor- Shaver, P. R. & Brennan, K. A. (1991). Measures of depression
derline and neurotic patients. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 9, and loneliness. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S.
77-91. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psycho-
Riggio, R. E., Warring, K. P., & Throckmorton, B. (1993). Social logical attitudes. Measures of social psychological attitudes
skills, social support, and psychosocial adjustment. Personality (Vol. 1 pp. 195-289) San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
and Individual Differences, 15, 275-208. Shaver, P., Furhman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Transition to
Rodway, M. R. (1992). Self-examination of loneliness: A group college: Network changes, social skills, and loneliness. In S.
approach. Social Work with Groups, 15, 69-80. Duck & D. Perlman (Eds.), Understanding personal relation-
Rook, K. S. (1984). Promoting social bonding: Strategies for help- ships: An interdisciplinary approach. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
ing the lonely and socially isolated. American Psychologist, 39, Stacy, A. W., Newcomb, M. D., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Expect-
1389-1407. ancy in mediational models of cocaine use. Personality and
Rook, K. S. (1987). Social support versus companionship: Effects Individual Differences, 19, 655-667.
on life stress, loneliness, and evaluations by others. Journal of Strack, S., & Coyne, J. C. (1983). Social confirmation of dyspho-
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1132-1147. ria: Shared and private reactions to depression. Journal of Per-
Rotenberg, K. J. (1994). Loneliness and interpersonal trust. Jour- sonality and Social Psychology, 44, 798-806,
nal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 152-173. Stroebe, W., Stroebe, M., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996).
Rotenberg, K. J., & Kmill, J. (1992). Perception of lonely and The role of loneliness and social support in adjustment to loss:
nonlonely persons as a function of individual differences in A test of attachment versus stress theory. Journal of Personality
loneliness. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, and Social Psychology, 70, 1241-1249.
325-330. Stumfol, B. (1992). SOS telephone as an anticrisis service in war
Rowe, D. C., & Plomin, R. (1977), Temperment in early child- conditions in Osiijek. Psychologische Beitrage, 34, 366-372.
hood. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 150-156. Tornstam, L. (1992). Loneliness in marriage. Journal of Social and
Rubenstein, C., & Shaver, P. (1982a). In search of intimacy. New Personal Relationships, 9, 197-217.
York: Delacorte Press. Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The
Rubenstein, C., & Shaver, P. (1982b). The experience of loneli- relationship between social support and physiological proceses:
ness. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and impli-
sourcebook of current theory, research, and therapy (pp. 206- cations for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488-531.
223). New York: Wiley (Interscience). Upmanyu, V. V., Sehgal, R., & Upmanyu, S. (1994). Factor an-
Russell, D. (l 982). The measurement of loneliness. In L. A. Peplau alystic study of personality correlates of loneliness among Indi-
& D. Perlman (ed.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theo- an university students. Psychological Studies, 39, 28-33.
ry, research and therapy. New York: Wiley. Vallenga, B. A., & Christenson, J. (1995). Healing the wounds of
Russell, D., Cutrona, C. E., Rose, J., & Yurko, K. (1984). Social war: The Vietnam veteran after Operation Desert Storm. Per-
and emotional loneliness: An examination of Weiss's typology spectives in Psychiatric Care, 31, 5-8.
of loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vitkus, J., & Horowitz, L. M. (1987). Poor social performance of
46, 1313-1321. lonely people: Lacking skill or adopting a role? Journal of
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1266-1273.
UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant evidence. Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480. social isolation. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Sadava, S. W., & Pak, A. W. (1994). Problem drinking and close Weiss, R. S, (1974). The provisions of social relationships. In
relationships during the third decade of life. Psychology of Z. Rubin (Ed.), Doing unto others. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Addictive Behaviors, 8, 251-258. Prentice-Hall.
22 Ernst and Cacioppo

Westen, D., Moses, M. J., Silk, K. R., Lohr, N. E., Cohen, R., & Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Kessler, R. C. (1993). The mean-
Segal, H. (1992). Quality of depressive experience in bor- ing of loss and adjustment to bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, W.
derline personality disorder and major depression: When de- Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement:
pression is not just depression. Journal of Personality Disor- Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 349-366). Cambridge:
ders, 6, 382-393. Cambridge University Press.
Wheeler, L., Reis,H., & Nezlek, J. (1983). Loneliness, social inter- Yang, B., & Clum, G. A. (1994). Life stress, social support, and
action, and sex roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- problem-solving skills predictive of depressive symptoms,
chology, 45, 943-953. hopelessness, and suicide ideation in an Asian student popula-
Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, H. T. (1986). Loneliness, social skills, tion: A test of a model. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior,
and social perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- 24, 127-139.
letin, 12, 121-130.

You might also like