You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1995, Vol. 80, No. 3, 371-385 0021-9010/95/S3.00

Volunteer Motivation and Attendance Decisions: Competitive Theory


Testing in Multiple Samples From a Homeless Shelter
David A. Harrison
University of Texas at Arlington

The process underlying participation in episodic volunteer work is conceptualized as a


form of attendance motivation. A decision-making theory of attendance motivation is
expanded for the voluntarism context, then competitively tested against nested theories:
the theory of planned behavior (I. Ajzen, 1991), the theory of reasoned action (M. Fish-
bein, 1980), and a benchmark theory emphasizing the subjective expected utility of an-
ticipated rewards. Tests are conducted in a field study (n = 53) predicting the motivation
and attendance of male volunteers scheduled to work roughly 1 night per month at a
homeless shelter. One panel (n = 53) and 2 cross-sectional (« = 51;« = 53) replications
of the field study are also described. Results are consistent across time and samples in
their support of the expanded theory primarily because it includes a moral obligation
component.

Roughly 50% of American adults volunteer their is little systematic organizational research on volunteer
time to nonprofit organizations, with increasing motivation. One exception might be the research of Or-
amounts of that time spent on social service activities gan and his colleagues on organizational citizenship be-
(U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1991). Volunteer workers havior (Organ, 1988). However, those studies are not
generate services worth more than $ 150 billion annu- targeted at volunteer motivation per se but on volunteer-
ally (Independent Sector, 1990). They provide an ing for additional work in a paid job as one of many
inexpensive source of labor for expanding and support- forms of extrarole behavior. Research by Eden and Kin-
ing resources in nonprofit agencies and public service nar (1991) and Leana, Ahlbrandt, and Murrell (1992)
organizations (J. M. Ferris, 1988). Indeed, the strategy deals more specifically with volunteering to take on more
most frequently cited by such organizations for coping tasks or special responsibilities in one's paid job. Al-
with governmental funding cuts and shrinking budgets though these investigations have some bearing on the
has been greater reliance on volunteers (Weisbrod, conceptual development discussed, the primary interest
1988). For many of these organizations, the work of of this article is unpaid volunteer work.
volunteers is vital to their success and survival Most studies of volunteer motivation focus on the im-
(Brudney, 1990). A primary task for nonprofit and pact of individual satisfactions and reward evaluations.
public service organizations, then, is to motivate the In a seminal study using data from matched groups of
participation of new and continuing volunteers. There- employees in eight types of organizations, Pearce
fore, it is important to identify the motivational mech- (1983a) found that job satisfaction, job praiseworthi-
anisms underlying volunteer work. ness, and the evaluation of social- and service-related re-
Despite its importance to so many organizations, there wards were higher for volunteer workers than for paid
employees in the same jobs. Intentions to quit were lower.
The value of task-related, intrinsic rewards did not differ
This work was supported in part by a National Science Foun- across the paid and volunteer employees. However,
dation Fellowship. I am grateful to Ulf Bockenholt, Fritz Dras- Schaubroeck and Ganster (1991) found that among
gow, Joe McGrath, Susan Middlestat, and Julian Rappaport for members of voluntary organizations, those with higher
suggestions on the design of this study, and to Mary McLaughlin intrinsic satisfaction were more likely to take part in a
and Laurie Liska for comments and editing of earlier versions specific volunteer activity. This relation was mediated by
of this article. I also thank the Christian Social Action Forum
for administrative support and access to the data site.
members' affective commitment to their organization,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed which, in turn, was moderated by the organization's ser-
to David A. Harrison, Department of Management, University vice versus nonservice purpose.
of Texas, UTA Box 19467, Arlington, Texas 76019-0467. Elec- Much of the research on volunteer motivation ema-
tronic mail may be sent via Bitnet to d089dhrs@utarlvm 1. nates from social work and community psychology. As
371
372 DAVID A. HARRISON

with the organizationally based studies, most of this re- volunteer motivation and to use more programmatic re-
search focuses on individual satisfactions and reward search strategies in testing those theories.
evaluations, assuming that the intangible rewards of vol- The purposes and contributions of this article fit with
unteering serve functions similar to pay and other tangi- Snyder and Omoto's (1992) suggestions. First, I describe
ble rewards in profit-making firms (e.g., Gidron, 1983; a principal form of "episodic" volunteer work. Second,
Naylor, 1985). Nevertheless, there is considerable debate I characterize the motivation to perform such episodic
about the structure and dimensionality of these motiva- volunteer work as a decision to take part in activities at
tional constructs. Some authors contend that volun- one of several rival settings. I also review and expand on
tarism flows from a single, undifferentiated helping mo- a decision-making theory of attendance motivation to be
tive (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). Others propound used as a model for episodic volunteer motivation. Third,
a two-dimensional structure: egoistic motives (defined as I present a field study at a homeless shelter in which the
satisfactions with tangible rewards) and altruistic motives expanded theory is competitively tested against others,
(defined as satisfactions with the intangible reward of including a benchmark theory containing the motiva-
feeling that one has helped someone else; Frisch & Ger- tional components emphasized by current organization
rard, 1981). A different set of researchers asserts a three- research on voluntarism: anticipated satisfactions or re-
dimensional grouping, adding social motives (defined as ward evaluations. The study involves a combination of
satisfactions with the rewards of social interaction) to the self-report measures of motivational constructs and ob-
previous two-category classification (Morrow-Howell & servational measures of volunteer participation. Fourth,
Mui, 1989). Still others argue for five- and six-dimen- the study is replicated over time in one sample and cross-
sional divisions (e.g., Clary & Snyder, 1991; Snyder & sectionally in two independent samples.
Omoto, 1991). Evidence for most of these motive struc-
tures comes from content analyses or exploratory factor Episodic Nature of Volunteer Work
analyses, with limited theory guiding either analytic
strategy. In paid or conventional work settings, employees often
perform sets of well-learned tasks during contiguous
Another research stream has compared profiles of sat-
hours, days, weeks, and so on. In contrast, potential vol-
isfactions or reward evaluations across demographic
unteers in nonprofit and service organizations are often
groups, for public service volunteers and nonvolunteers
called on to perform fairly novel tasks during one or a few
(e.g., Lafer, 1989; Latting, 1990; Pierucci & Noel, 1980;
isolated occasions (Brudney, 1990). Examples include
Tapp & Spanier, 1973). Several demographic character- raising or refurbishing homes in impoverished areas one
istics have been implicated as inputs to volunteer moti- week (or one weekend) per year, delivering Christmas
vation, including age, marital status, and gender. Older toys to hospitalized children, staffing phone lines in a sea-
adults volunteer more readily and for more altruistic rea- sonal telethon, serving Thanksgiving meals to the home-
sons than do younger adults. Men are more likely than less, retrieving litter from beaches on designated cleanup
women to volunteer in the hopes of obtaining job-related days, judging a Special Olympics track meet, and so on.
skills or developing business contacts. Single adults vol- Only 20%-25% of volunteers work on a regular weekly or
unteer more often than those who are married, perhaps biweekly basis. Even regular volunteers tend to do their
to meet potential companions (Gillespie & King, 1985). work only 1-2 days per week (Gallup Organization,
Adding to the complexity and miscellany of findings 1986; Independent Sector, 1990).
about volunteer motivation, Omoto and Snyder (1990) Most volunteer participation is therefore discrete or
did not find a systematic demographic or motivational episodic, rather than continuous or successive. It is a
pattern among volunteers doing AIDS-related work. Af- carefully considered or controlled departure from one's
ter summarizing much of the literature on voluntarism, attendance routine rather than a scripted or automatic
Omoto and Snyder advised that "organizations may do part of it (Feldman & Lynch, 1988; Poole, Gray, & Gioia,
well to consider targeting their recruitment efforts at the 1990). As volunteers themselves recount, volunteering
particular motivations of selected sets of potential volun- demands attention and deliberation (Snyder & Omoto,
teers" (p. 155) and implied that the selection of those 1992). It involves a decision-making process.
volunteers will depend on the organizational, social, and
task-related context of volunteering. In the absence of A Theory of Episodic Volunteer Motivation as an
strong theory or consistent evidence, however, organiza- Attendance Decision
tions have little guidance for choosing either the types of
individuals or the types of motivational constructs to Individuals actively decide to donate specific portions
target. Consequently, Snyder and Omoto exhort them- of their time to volunteer tasks. Just as with paid work,
selves and others to develop more integrative theories of these tasks are usually performed in particular settings
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 373

where services can be provided directly to clients unteer attendance is what Ajzen and Madden (1986) re-
(Brudney, 1990). Thus, the basic question of episodic fer to as actual control: the level of resources, opportuni-
volunteer motivation is this: What influences an individ- ties, and abilities one possesses for attendance volunteer
ual to participate in activities at a volunteer setting—to work. Indeed, the influence of controls and constraints on
attend the volunteer setting—rather than attend settings attendance motivation are now being recognized (Johns,
that compete for the same portion of time? 1991; Peters, O'Connor, & Eulberg, 1985). Unfortu-
A parallel version of this question has been proposed nately, the environmental factors that inhibit actual
as fundamental to the choice between missing or at- control are often unanticipated, and it is currently not
tending scheduled work, a choice brought into active feasible to validly measure it without equating it with per-
processing by some triggering event (Martocchio & ceived control, a distinct, though related construct in the
Harrison, 1993; Rosse & Miller, 1984). Drawing from theory.
literature in social and behavioral decision making, es- Choicefrom competing intentions. Choice is a central
pecially the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein & Aj- element of any decision-making theory. Both the theory
zen, 1975) and the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, of reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior,
1985, 1987, 1991), Martocchio and Harrison focus on however, were developed primarily to predict a single be-
intentions as the motivational conduit through which havior. They lack an explicit framework for dealing with
other variables converge to affect eventual attendance choices of behavior from a set of alternatives. Because the
choices. They also outline some of the critical compo- motivation to perform episodic volunteer work has been
nents of attendance intentions. This theoretical ap- cast as one that involves attendance alternatives, a choice
proach is reviewed below (in Determinants of Volun- mechanism is proposed for the theory of episodic volun-
teer Intentions) and expanded to include constructs teer motivation that expands the earlier theories.
and relations that might be important in the context In the expanded theory, whether or not one takes part
of voluntarism: specifically, a choice from competing in volunteer work is the manifest consequence of choos-
intentions mechanism, a moral obligation to volunteer, ing between intentions or motivational forces to attend
and a feedback loop linking continued participation in competing settings. The attendance-choice function re-
volunteer work to changes in the pattern of motivational in- flects how much stronger one intention is than the others.
puts. The expanded theory is hereafter referred to as a theory In this article for example, decisions about volunteer par-
of episodic volunteer motivation. All of its elements and re- ticipation often involve a choice between attending work
lations are presented in Figure 1. at an overnight homeless shelter and staying home. The
probability that a volunteer would work at the shelter
Determinants of Volunteer Attendance (i.e., attend volunteer work) rather than stay at home re-
Intention. A central proposition of the theory of epi- flects the difference:
sodic volunteer motivation is that taking part in volun- Prob(Attendsheiter) = g+ {u, (Mv) - M2(Mstay home)}. (1)
teer work at a specified time and place (hereafter, attend-
ing volunteer work) is a direct, positive function of the In this equation, g+ is a positive nonlinear function
intention to do so. This proposition is part of several mo- (because probabilities are bounded by 0 and 1), and u:
tivation theories (Ajzen, 1991; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; and u-i are weights. If there are two or more alternatives
Triandis, 1977; Tubbs & Ekeberg, 1991) and has consid- that compete with attendance at the volunteer setting
erable empirical support (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Shep- (i.e., there are three or more settings to choose from),
pard, Hartwick, & Warshaw, 1988; Steele & Ovalle, Int terms for the other alternatives will also have negative
1984). An intention is a cognition, defined here as the weights.
strength of one's conscious plans to take part in sched- This hypothesis can be stated verbally as
uled activities at a volunteer setting (Intv). It is the im-
mediate motivational antecedent of attending volunteer Hypothesis Ib: Volunteer attendance is a function of com-
peting intentions, rather than simply the intention to vol-
work if one considers motivation to be an internally reg- unteer. It is determined by a positive function of intention
ulated, dynamic process (Bandura, 1991). That is, in the to attend the volunteer setting and simultaneous negative
present theory, the intention to attend a volunteer setting functions of intentions to attend competing settings.
is equated with the motivational force to do episodic vol-
unteer work. Therefore, the first hypothesis is Other variables. Consistent with its predecessors, the
theory of episodic volunteer motivation postulates that
Hypothesis la: Intention to take part in volunteer work intentions are the most proximal motivational determi-
(attend a volunteer setting) is the immediate positive de-
terminant of actual volunteer attendance. nants of attending volunteer work; they mediate the
effects of all other internal variables. Other motivational
Another variable that might independently affect vol- variables, such as past participation or anticipated re-
374 DAVID A. HARRISON

Figure 1. Constructs and relations in nested decision-making theories of volunteer motivation. Subjective
expected utility-based components are presented in the black box, the theory of reasoned action is presented
in boxes with dark shading, the theory of planned behavior is presented in boxes with light shading, and the
theory of episodic volunteer motivation is presented in the white boxes, ab; = beliefs about attendance; 6;
= evaluations about consequences; nbj = normative belief; mcj = motivation to comply; subj. = subjective;
oblig. = obligation; K. = number of settings considered.

wards, should affect actual volunteer attendance only the original theory of reasoned action, attitude and social
through their impact on intentions: norm have been studied extensively (see Sheppard et al,
Hypothesis Ic: No other motivational variables affect vol-
1988, for a recent review). Perceived behavioral control
unteer participation except through their effects on inten- has received less research attention in the more recent
tions to attend volunteer work or on intentions to attend theory of planned behavior, but if it is equated with task-
competing settings. specific self-efficacy, it has a broad empirical foundation
(Ajzen, 1991). Moral obligation has been added for its
relevance and potential importance in the voluntarism
Determinants of Volunteer Intentions context.
As shown in Figure 1, the intention to take part in epi- A ttitude or subjective expected utility. The typical ap-
sodic volunteer work is proposed to be determined by proach to volunteer motivation has been implicitly based
four factors: an individual's attitude toward attending a on expectancy value or subjective expected utility theo-
volunteer setting (^4 V ), subjective norm about attending ries (SEU). These SEU-based approaches assume that
volunteer work (SNV), perceived behavioral control over an individual's attraction or motivational force to take
attending volunteer work (PBCV), and moral obligation part in episodic volunteer work depends on a sum of per-
to attend volunteer work (MOV). As key components of ceived contingencies between volunteer attendance and
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 375

its expected outcomes (costs and rewards), weighted by volunteer work if he or she attempts to do so. Conceptu-
the evaluation or personal satisfaction anticipated from ally, perceived behavioral control is quite similar to the
those outcomes (Schoemaker, 1982). In general, persons notion of task-specific self-efficacy (Bandura, 1991) and
having stronger perceived contingencies for, greater satis- is nearly identical to Vroom's (1964) expectancy link be-
factions with, or more positive evaluations of the rewards tween effort and performance (if performance is merely
that might come from volunteer work—in sum, a higher successful attendance at the volunteer setting). The
SEU—are presumed to be more likely to take part in it unique contribution of perceived behavioral control to
(e.g., Horton-Smith, 1981). intentions has empirical support in a variety of social
The logic underlying the SEU approach is virtually the psychological studies (Ajzen, 1991). And, once again, ev-
same as that underlying current notions of attitude idence of the motivational influence of constructs similar
(Ajzen, 1988; Jaccard, 1981). Attitude toward attending to PBCV exists in both the organizational and public pol-
a volunteer setting is denned as someone's personal eval- icy literatures (Adsett, 1976; Frayne & Latham, 1987;
uation of performing that behavior or as its location on Gabard, 1990; Johns, 1991; Locke & Latham, 1990; Pe-
a bipolar affective dimension. In the theory of reasoned ters et al., 1985), including at least one study of volunteer
action or the theory of planned behavior, components of behavior (D. Smith, 1966).
attitude (see Components of Attitude and Subjective This body of evidence suggests that a potential
Norm section) are also virtually identical to the compo- volunteer, even though he or she recognizes some per-
nents of SEU. Just as with SEU, individuals who have sonal utility and feels some social expectation to take
more positive attitudes toward attending episodic volun- part in volunteer work, may not intend to do so be-
teer work should be more likely to actually take part in cause of the beliefs "my schedule won't allow it," or
it. However, in research on volunteers, there is seldom "my health and age can't take it," and so on (Unger,
consideration of attitudes or SEU toward competing al- 1987). The theory of reasoned action includes attitu-
ternatives (Jaccard, 1981). dinal and subjective norm influences, but it does not
Subjective norm. One element missing from a com- recognize the effects of perceived behavioral control on
pletely SEU-based or attitudinal theory of volunteer mo- intention. The theory of planned behavior was devel-
tivation is the social context of volunteering. Evidence oped to extend the applicability of the theory of rea-
in the organizational literature (G. R. Ferris & Mitchell, soned action, and it includes the motivational impact
1987; Miller & Crush, 1988; Prestholdt, Lane, & Ma- of such control-related perceptions. According to the
thews, 1987) suggests that social influence has important theory of planned behavior and the expanded version
effects on many behavioral decisions. Studies in public of it developed in this article for the context of episodic
policy have also shown that social factors influence volunteering, individuals should form intentions to at-
choices regarding voluntarism (Amenta, 1984). tend volunteer settings under the unique influences of
Both the theory of reasoned action and the theory of attitudinal, social, and perceived control factors.
planned behavior augment the attitudinal or SEU com- Moral obligation. A fourth determinant of episodic
ponent of most decision theories with a social compo- volunteer motivation, not included in previous theories
nent. This subjective norm reflects the strength of per- but part of the present theory, is moral obligation: the
ceived expectations to participate in volunteer work, perception that participating in volunteer work is correct
linked to important others in one's social environment. or incorrect, good or bad, in an ethical or moral (rather
In other words, episodic volunteer motivation may be a than instrumental) sense. It does not reflect perceived ex-
function of the desire to conform to the perceived wishes pectations from specific or salient social referents (as
of friends or family in addition to gaining some personal does subjective norm). Rather, it reflects an internalized
utility. Subjective norm is conceptually distinct from at- pressure to be consistent with one's value system.
titude in the same way that normative and informational The moral obligation component should be especially
influence have been distinguished in social psychology relevant to the motivation underlying taking part in vol-
(Miller & Crush, 1986, p. 200). Subjective norms are unteer work because volunteering satisfies many of the
perceptions of "what important others think I should criteria for a salient moral issue or moral situation, espe-
do"; possible social dimensions of attitude involve "how cially that the morally correct behavior is clearly defined
others will reward my behavior." The unique contribu- and that it will have meaningful consequences for others
tions of attitude and subjective norm to intention are (Gorsuch&Ortberg, 1983; Jones, 1991). Many theorists
well-documented (Ajzen, 1991;Sheppardetal., 1988). have argued for the inclusion of a moral-ethical compo-
Perceived behavioral control. Another important ele- nent in models of behavioral decisions (Etzioni, 1988;
ment of episodic volunteer motivation might be per- Pomozal & Jaccard, 1976; Schwartz & Tessler, 1972; Tri-
ceived behavioral control: an individual's subjective andis, 1977), and empirical support exists in a number
probability that he or she can successfully take part in of relevant studies (Ilgen & Hollenback, 1977; Prestholdt
376 DAVID A. HARRISON

et al., 1987; D. Smith, 1966; Trevino & Youngblood, though the authors of the theory of reasoned action and
1990). the theory of planned behavior briefly discuss such a pos-
Synthesis. Although attitude, subjective norm, per- sibility, they do not propose any specific hypotheses
ceived behavioral control, and moral obligation can be about it. In a further extension of those theories, the fol-
correlated (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Ajzen & Madden, lowing hypothesis is proposed for the theory of episodic
1986), each construct is proposed to make some unique volunteer motivation:
contribution to the formation of intention. This leads to
the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 2f: Volunteer participation has a feedback
effect (feedback loop) on the relative weights of the four
Hypotheses 2a-2d: Attitude (2a), subjective norm (2b), motivational components. That is, as volunteering behav-
perceived behavioral control (2c), and moral obligation ior continues overtime (i.e., persists), the pattern of moti-
(2d) toward attending volunteer work all have positive and vational weights for attitude, subjective norm, perceived
unique effects on the intention to attend volunteer work. control, or moral obligation will change.

Because the contribution of each component can vary


across volunteering situations, Hypotheses 2a-2d can be Components of Attitude and Subjective Norm
written mathematically to include weights: Just as intention to attend volunteer work can be bro-
ken down into its various determinants, those determi-
Intv = \v3PBCv + v/4MOv. (2)
nants can be explained in terms of other components (see
Sufficiency. In the theory of episodic volunteer moti- Figure 1). These components form the explanatory
vation, as well as the theory of reasoned action and the rather than the predictive nucleus of the present theory,
theory of planned behavior nested within it, global atti- and they underlie much of its practical utility. In the pres-
tudes (such as overall satisfaction with volunteering), sit- ent study, the focus will be on attitudinal and subjective
uational variables, personality traits, or demographic norm components, as the theoretical components of per-
characteristics should influence volunteer motivation ceived behavioral control are evolving (Ajzen, 1985,
only through their influence on the weights of the four 1987, 1991; Ajzen & Madden, 1986).
components reviewed above, or on the components Attitudinal components. An individual's attitude
themselves. For example, older people volunteer more of- toward taking part in volunteer work can be thought of
ten than younger people (Gillespie & King, 1985). Ac- as being determined by his or her specific beliefs about
cording to the sufficiency hypothesis, the effect of age on t h e z = l , . . . 7 salient personal consequences of atten-
intention should be mediated or moderated by a subset dance ( a b j ) and his or her general evaluations (et) of
of the components of intention. This hypothesis is clear those consequences. Attendance beliefs are an individ-
from Figure 1 and is stated formally below: ual's perceived connection between attending the vol-
unteer setting and receiving consequence i. Evalu-
Hypothesis 2e: Variables external to the theory, including ations of consequence / are analogous to anticipated
general attitudes, demographic variables, and personality satisfactions or valences (Vroom, 1964). For example,
constructs should not affect volunteer intention except someone might have a strong belief that attending vol-
through their effects on attitude, subjective norm, per-
ceived behavioral control, and moral obligation. unteer work will lead to "meeting new and interesting
people." If that consequence is positively evaluated, the
Feedback loop. Many authors have proposed that the belief and its corresponding evaluation will make
motivation to start volunteer work is different from the someone's attitude more positive. Together, the ab/ and
motivation to persist in it (Gidron, 1984; Hodgkinson & ei are predicted to compose the SEU-type structure of
Weitzman, 1988; Pearce, 1983b; D. Smith, Reddy, & Bald- attitude (Ajzen, 1991; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) in the
win, 1972;Snyder&Omoto, 1992), which has direct im- following way:
plications for recruiting versus retaining volunteers. The
motivational inputs to take part in volunteer work may Av oc 2 abi X et. (3)
change over a volunteer's accumulating experience. For The hypothesis can be expressed verbally as:
example, the initial decision to do volunteer work may
be motivated by a desire to get positive outcomes from Hypothesis 3a: Attitudes toward attending volunteer work
participation, such as meeting potential business con- are a positive function of the beliefs that taking part in vol-
unteer work will lead to each of a set of salient personal
tacts (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). That motiva- outcomes, multiplied by how positive or negative those out-
tional emphasis may shift over time to an increased con- comes are expected to be.
cern with social expectation, "the other volunteers are
counting on me," or perhaps to moral obligation, "I have Subjective norm components. Subjective norm is de-
a responsibility to help others" (see Pearce, 1983b). Al- termined by an individual's specific perceptions about
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 377

how strongly important others (salient social referents) fast, signed in clients at the door and apprised them of the rules,
think he or she should attend the volunteer setting in washed dishes and clothes, cleaned soiled bedding, gave infor-
question and how much he or she wants to conform to mal counseling, provided or acquired care for sick or injured
clients, monitored client activities and enforced rules about cli-
those social expectations in general. For example, a po-
ent behavior throughout the night until the next morning (from
tential volunteer might believe that his or her family does
9:30 p.m. to 8:00 a.m.).
not want him or her to take part in volunteer work. The A persistent problem for the shelter's administration was mo-
potential volunteer might be strongly motivated to com- tivating potential volunteers to attend work at the shelter at least
ply with his or her family, which would have a negative two nights per season. Absenteeism disrupted the performance
impact on subjective norm and eventually intention (if of even essential tasks such as laundering, counseling, and mon-
subjective norms have a nonzero weight in intention itoring. Absenteeism rates ranged from 15-35% per month. By
formation). This structure of subjective norm regarding the same token, however, the shelter setting offered unique op-
volunteer attendance can be written in the following way: portunities for investigating episodic volunteer motivation and
attendance decisions. For example, there were strong disutilities
SNV oc S (4) for attending work. The shelter was poorly heated and venti-
lated, had a foul smell, and was clearly a dangerous place to
where nbj are normative beliefs: perceptions of the likeli- work. A few volunteers were injured or threatened by abusive
hood that each of / = 1,. . . /normative referents would clients who were drunk or high, and who sometimes carried
approve or disapprove of the focal person attending the hidden weapons. Seldom were clients openly appreciative of the
volunteer setting; and mc-t are motivations to comply: the services they received and they frequently complained about
degree to which the focal person wants to do what refer- meals and laundry. The ratio of clients to volunteers was typi-
ent 2 approves of, no matter what the situation. Some ev- cally ^ 10:1, which produced a heavy task load. In addition,
idence for such a structure exists across situations volunteers often did not have time to sleep during the night,
(Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). Verbally, this hy- yet had to attend their normal (paid) jobs the following day.
Conversely, clear normative expectations for working at the
pothesis can be stated as:
shelter existed because volunteers who did not attend their
Hypothesis 3b: A person's subjective norm about volun- scheduled work created obvious hardships for those who did.
teer attendance is positively and interactively determined Participants. Because of well-grounded fears of sexual vio-
by normative beliefs that salient social referents think he lence, only male volunteers could work at the shelter. Many of
or she should take part in volunteer work and by motiva- the volunteers were community residents with families and pro-
tions to comply with those referents. fessional jobs. Some veteran volunteers had worked at the shel-
ter for several years. Beginning volunteers were often college
Method students or new members of the churches that helped to fund
Overview and operate the shelter. Fifty-three volunteers participated in
Sample Iw-ls, another 51 volunteers took part in Sample 2w,
The hypotheses composing the theory of episodic volunteer and 53 additional volunteers took part in Sample 3s (response
motivation were tested in parallel field samples, each involving rates were 83%, 91%, and 91% of the entire set of volunteers,
a set of adult men who were deciding about attending overnight respectively). The percentages of married volunteers were 72,
volunteer work at a winter shelter for homeless men. The first 70, and 47, respectively, in the three samples. Average ages were
sample (1 w) examined the fit of the hypotheses during the win- 39.3, 39.5, and 35.2 years, respectively; mean numbers of chil-
ter, in a continuing group of volunteers. A panel replication ex- dren were 1.3, 1.1, and 1.0, respectively; average frequencies of
amined the stability of the Sample 1 w results over time, testing past participation at the shelter were 12.9, 5.9, and 3.2 times,
the hypotheses with the same group of volunteers in the spring respectively. Both the percentage of married volunteers and the
(replication Is). Sample 2w was a cross-sectional replication, frequency of past participation were significantly different
involving an independent second sample of volunteers in the across the three samples (p < .05).
winter. Sample 3s involved another independent group of vol-
unteers in the spring. Grounding ofTEVM Concepts in the
Setting. The shelter, located in a medium-size city in the
Midwest, provided food and night sleeping quarters for home-
Volunteer Population
less men during the winter (November through January) and In October, 42 (24%) of 173 volunteers who had previously
early spring (February through April). An administrative com- agreed to spend at least two nights working at the shelter either
mittee elected from a group of area churches was responsible returned a questionnaire or responded to an interview designed
for the shelter's operation. The shelter received public aid and to elicit salient alternatives, consequences, and referents regard-
grant money to provide for most of the shelter's physical needs ing attendance at their scheduled volunteer work. Specifically,
such as food, bedding, and heating oil. A paid supervisor volunteers were asked to list alternatives ("other activities I
opened the shelter each evening and remained for 1-2 hr. The could do instead"), consequences ("personal advantages and
remaining staff members were three volunteers who changed disadvantages"), and referents ("people who might approve or
from night to night. Volunteers rarely worked in the same crews. disapprove") that came to mind when they thought about
Each night, they cooked and served a late meal and early break- "working at the shelter the next time they were scheduled to do
378 DAVID A. HARRISON

so." According to suggested criteria (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980), working at the shelter the next time I'm scheduled," ranged
10 salient consequences were retained for use in the main study. from strongly disapprove ( — 3 ) to strongly approve ( + 3 ) . One of
These consequences included the following: "gives me self- the perceived control items was "Getting to work at the shelter
satisfaction for doing a community service," "lets me interact the next time I'm scheduled is __ ," with a response scale
with other volunteers," "puts me in a dangerous situation," anchored by completely out of my control (—3) to completely
"makes me weary/unproductive the next day," "forces me to under my control ( +3 ) . Moral obligation was measured with a
deal with belligerent men," "puts me in a place that smells bad," similar, direct approach. For example, one of the two items
"makes me feel nervous and uncomfortable," "keeps me from i = • 93 ) was "I feel a_
_ moral obligation to work at the
getting much sleep," "lets me meet interesting people," and shelter the next time I'm scheduled." It had a 7-point, semantic
"helps me appreciate the things that I have." Five social refer- differential response scale anchored by extremely weak (—3)
ents, including "people from my church," "my wife/ and extremely strong ( + 3 ) .
girlfriend," "other family members," "people at my job," and In addition, a consistent set of external variables were mea-
"other volunteers" were also retained. In addition, two types of sured in each sample. They included satisfaction with working
salient alternatives to attending scheduled volunteer work were at the shelter (one semantic differential item and one "faces"
retained: staying home (e.g., "sleep at home" or "work at scale, amed = .75), an indicator of whether or not something
home") and socializing or recreating ("go out to a movie" or stressful occurred the current night they were working at the
"visit friends"). shelter, frequency of past volunteer work at the shelter, and sev-
Procedure. With the approval of the shelter's administra- eral demographic variables—age, marital status, and number
tion, I worked as a volunteer at the shelter each night. After the of dependent children. As was mentioned above, I measured
late meal was served and dishes were cleaned, I explained the volunteer attendance through behavioral observations while I
general purposes of the study to the other volunteers worked at the shelter each night over the course of the
(investigating the volunteer motivation), handed out question- investigation.
naires, and assured volunteers of confidentiality. In addition, I
told each volunteer that no one would see the answers on their
questionnaire until the ensuing April, when the shelter would Results
be closed and they would no longer be asked to work there (until
the following winter). I asked volunteers to complete the ques- Determinants of Volunteer Attendance
tionnaire late that night or early the next morning, when their
Correlations between intention and attendance ranged
duty was primarily to do laundry and monitor the shelter clients
as they slept. They were to drop the questionnaire through a from .57 in Sample 2w to .73 in Sample ls(allps< .01).
slot into a locked wooden box located in the shelter's kitchen. Observed attendance ( 1 ) or absence (0) from scheduled
Volunteers completed the questionnaire during their first night volunteer work was converted to a logit variable and re-
of scheduled work at the shelter during a season (winter or gressed hierarchically on intention to work at the shelter
spring). A unique code number was assigned to each survey so (Hypothesis la), as well as intention to stay home and
that the self-report data could be matched with a direct obser- intention to socialize or recreate (Hypothesis Ib). It was
vation of attendance (1 = attended, 0 = absent) the next time also regressed on attitude, subjective norm, perceived
the volunteer was scheduled, which was usually 4 to 8 weeks control, moral obligation, and amount of past participa-
later. All names and special codes were later destroyed or erased tion (Hypothesis Ic). Analyses were performed sepa-
to assure anonymity and confidentiality. rately on data collected from each sample. Base rates for
Measures. The questionnaire measured the proposed theo-
volunteer absences were 19%, 11%, 23%, and 30% in
retical components of a volunteer's motivation (intention) to-
Samples Iw, Is, 2w, and 3s, respectively. Results (see Ta-
ward attending work on the second night he was scheduled for
later in that season. The questionnaire contained a comprehen- ble 1 ) are quite consistent across panel and cross-sec-
sive set of items developed from the elicitation results. Intention tional replications.
to attend work at the shelter (two items, median a from the four As Table 1 shows, Hypothesis la was clearly supported.
samples= .91), intention to stay home (two items, amed = .86), However, Hypothesis Ib received only mixed support. In
intention to socialize or recreate (two items, amed = .73), atti- all four samples, intention to attend volunteer work at the
tude (five items, amcd = .81), subjective norm (two items, amcd shelter significantly predicted actual volunteer attendance.
= .80), perceived behavioral control (two items, own = .77), With the predicted negative coefficient, intention to stay
and attendance beliefs, evaluations, normative beliefs, and mo- home also improved the prediction of volunteer attendance
tivations to comply (one item each) were measured with proce- in Sample 3s. Similarly, intention to socialize or recreate
dures and formats detailed by Ajzen and Fishbein (1980), and improved prediction in Sample 2w. Intentions to attend
Ajzen and Madden (1986). these competing alternatives did not improve the prediction
As specified by those authors, response scales were usually 7-
of volunteer attendance in either of the Sample Iw or Is
point, semantic differentials. For example, the response scale
for an attitude item, "For me, working at the shelter the next analyses.
time I'm scheduled is ," ranged from extremely negative Recall that Sample Is-lw involved the same set of ex-
(-3) to extremely positive (+3). The response scale for a sub- perienced volunteers. To explore the idea that volunteer
jective norm item, "The other volunteers would of me experience moderated the impact of competing inten-
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 379

Table 1
Tests of Hypotheses la-lc: Logistic Regression of Volunteer Attendance on Target
Intention, Competing Intentions, and External Variables
X2 increment in prediction* Standardized coefficient estimate11

Sample Sample

Variable Iw Is 2w 3s Iw Is 2w 3s

Intention.tKnd work at si,eiKr 7.17** 8.30** 6.95** 10.05** 2.53** 2.02* 1.49 1.99*
IntentionMyhomc 2.01 0.55 2.11 4.69* -1.36 -1.00 -2.33* -1.32
IntentionjociaJjze/recreate 0.50 2.25 4.23* 0.64 -0.70 -1.47 -2.03* -0.76
Attitude, subjective norm, perceived
behavioral control, moral
obligation' 2.21 1.76 4.20 2.73
Past participation (experience) 0.60 0.04 0.24 0.49
4.83* 2.38**
•"Experience X IntentionstayhonK
•"Experience X Intentionsocjaiise/TOrate 4.80* 2.37**

Note. Iw = first group of volunteers for the winter; ls = same group of volunteers for the spring; 2w = independent second group of volunteers for
the winter; 3s = independent group of volunteers for the spring.
* Increments in prediction estimated at successive stages in the modeling process. Degrees of freedom for the chi-square equal the number of new
variables entered at each stage.
b
In the large-sample theory on which logistic regression is based, standardized coefficients are z values. In the small samples used here, the more
conservative t distribution is used to evaluate the significance of the coefficients. Coefficients are estimated for the model that contains theoretical
parameters.
c
These components of intention were entered as part of a block of external variables in the logistic regression equation.
d
Interactive effects of experience and competing intentions were estimated in an aggregate data set from all independent samples (i.e., except Is; N
= 157). Positive coefficients indicate that as a volunteer gained experience working in the shelter, the negative impact of competing intentions
decreased (moved closer to becoming a null effect).
*/7<.05. **p<.01.

tions, data were combined from Iw, 2w, and 3s into one tendance. Therefore, consistent with the arguments laid
sample (n = 157). A moderated logistic regression anal- out by James and Brett (1984), intentions fully mediated
ysis was performed by entering terms containing interac- the effects of attitude, subjective norm, perceived behav-
tions between past participation (experience) and each ioral control, and moral obligation on volunteer atten-
of the competing intentions. Table 1 shows that adding dance. Adding the main effect of past frequency of par-
the interaction terms produced a significantly better fit of ticipation also had no impact on the fit of the logistic
the logistic model. This suggests that, as volunteers regression.
gained experience at the shelter, consideration of alterna-
tive settings had less of an impact on eventual volunteer Determinants of Volunteer Intention
attendance.
(Episodic Motivation)
An alternative interpretation might be that the
insignificant effect of competing alternatives was an ar- With respect to the determinants of intentions to at-
tifact of the lower base rate of absenteeism in Samples tend volunteer work, several theories are nested within
Iw and Is. To examine this explanation, interaction the expanded theory developed in this article. The SEU-
terms between sample membership and each of the based approach is nested within the theory of reasoned
competing intentions were entered into the logistic re- action, which is nested within the theory of planned be-
gression equation (without simultaneously including havior, which in turn, is nested within the theory of epi-
experience interactions). None of these terms, how- sodic volunteer motivation. Therefore, competitive tests
ever, significantly improved the fit of the model (p > of their prediction of attendance intentions can be ac-
.05 for all chi-squares in the aggregate data). complished via hierarchical linear regression (see
Hypothesis Ic was uniformly supported. Attitude, sub- Table 2).
jective norm, perceived control, and moral obligation, In the first regression step, intention to attend volun-
added individually or as a block of variables, did not im- teer work at the shelter was regressed on the SEU-based
prove prediction of attendance above the contribution of component: attitude. In partial support of Hypothesis 2a,
intention in any sample. These variables did have sig- attitude predicted intention to attend volunteer work in
nificant zero-order correlations with intentions and at- Samples 2w and 3s but not in the continuing Sample 1 w-
380 DAVID A. HARRISON

Table 2
Tests of Hypotheses 2a-2e: Hierarchical Regression of Volunteer Attendance Intention on Attitude, Subjective Norm, Perceived
Behavioral Control, Moral Obligation, and External Variables
Change in R^ Standardized coefficient estimate1"
Sample Sample
Variable lw Is 2w 3s lw Is 2w 3s

Attitude .06 .04 .10* .16** .17 .08 .11 .25*


Subjective norm .09** .32** .20** .17** .23 .16 .03 .22*
Perceived behavioral control .10** .10** .06* .12** .26* .24* .17 .31**
Moral obligation .06* .13** .26** .09** .27** .48** .67** .31**
lab, X e, .01 .01 .01 .00
2nbj X mq .00 .00 .00 .01
General satisfaction .02 .02 .00 .00
Stressful incidents .00 .00 .00 .00
Past participation (experience) .00 .02 .00 .00
Age .02 .01 .00 .00
(Not) married .02 .00 .03* .03*
No. of children at home .00 .02 .01 .00

Note, lw = first group of volunteers for the winter; ls = same group of volunteers for the spring; 2w = independent second group of volunteers for
the winter; 3s = independent group of volunteers for the spring.
a
Increments in predictiveness were estimated at successive stages in the modeling process. b Coefficients were estimated for parameters in theoret-
ical model. Because of small samples and high multicollinearity, /3s are less stable than the hierarchical regression results.
*p<.05. **p<.01.

Is. One possible reason for the lack of effect in Sample ple of volunteers, supporting Hypothesis 2b. This also
1 w-1 s might have been the significant difference in expe- confirms that the TRA approach to volunteer motiva-
rience between that sample and the others. A moderating tion, which accounts for the perceived social context of
effect of past participation on the attitude-intention rela- decisions, fits better than a simple SEU-based approach
tion would be consistent with Hypothesis 2f regarding a (when using either a direct measure of attitude and a re-
feedback loop that connects volunteer experience with scaled 2abi X e\ that corresponds more closely to SEU-
changes in the pattern of motivational inputs (see also type theories).
Ajzen, 1988, for a review of studies showing an effect of In the third regression step, perceived behavioral con-
direct experience on the prediction of behavior by atti- trol was added to the intention model. The results in Ta-
tudes; note, however, that in this case the effect is in the ble 2 show that it significantly improved the prediction of
opposite direction: Attitude-intention relations are intention when subjective norm and attitude were pres-
stronger for those with less direct experience). ent in the equation. This occurred in each sample, de-
To test this possibility, a hierarchical moderated regres- spite the small sample sizes and significant amount of
sion analysis was performed in which intention was the variance already being explained by those other compo-
dependent variable. Data from all independent samples nents. This finding supports Hypothesis 2c and demon-
were aggregated into a single sample in the same way as strates that the theory of planned behavior is a better-fit-
described above (in the Determinants of Volunteer Atten- ting theory of volunteers' attendance intentions than
dance section). After including both attitude and experi- either the theory of reasoned action or SEU-based
ence as main effects in the regression equation, an A* X approaches.
Experience interaction term was added. This term sig- In the fourth regression step, moral obligation was
nificantly improved the prediction of intention, A/?2 = added to the regression equation. As Hypothesis 2d pro-
.03, F( 1, 153) = 4.54, p < .05, supporting Hypothesis 2f poses, moral obligation explained additional variance in
and explaining the mixed support for Hypothesis 2a. In intentions. The results in Table 2 reveal that this im-
other words, there is evidence from the these samples provement in prediction was consistent across panel and
that, as volunteers gained experience, the amount of sat- cross-sectional replications. Thus, the theory of episodic
isfaction they anticipated from doing volunteer work had volunteer motivation (to which moral obligation is
less of an impact on their motivation to take part in it. unique) is a better description of the components of vol-
In the second regression step, subjective norm was unteer intentions than the theory of planned behavior,
added to the regression model after attitude. Table 2 the theory of reasoned action, or an SEU-based
shows that there was a significant improvement in the approach.
prediction of intention by subjective norm for each sam- Finally, to test Hypothesis le (that attitude, subjective
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 381

norm, perceived behavioral control and moral obligation A/?2 = .03, F(\, 136) = 7.63, p < 01. This regression
are the only direct determinants of intention), several equation produced an "optimally rescaled" composite
other variables were added to the regression equations. (Ajzen, 1991, p. 193) that had a multiple correlation of
Satisfaction with working at the shelter, an indicator of .59 (p < .01) with attitude toward attending volunteer
whether something stressful occurred at the shelter, and work. Similarly, the multiplicative term ZH&J X me, ex-
the demographic characteristics: age, marital status (1 = plained unique variance in subjective norm beyond the
married, 0 = not married), and number of children liv- additive contributions of normative beliefs and motiva-
ing at home were added to the regression models in each tion to comply terms, Afl2 = .05, F(l, 146)= 12.73, p<
sample. One of these variables, marital status, improved 01. The resulting optimally rescaled composite also had
the prediction of intention in Samples 2w and 3s. The a strong multiple correlation with subjective norm about
sign of the coefficient for the marital status term was pos- attending volunteer work (R - .70, p < .01). In sum-
itive, indicating that, in those samples, some extra level of mary, Hypotheses 3a and 3b were supported.
motivation or commitment to perform one's scheduled
volunteer work was associated with being married. This Discussion
motivational increment was not captured by the four Intentions and Volunteer Attendance
components of volunteer motivation in the theory of ep-
isodic volunteer motivation and therefore weakens the As the theory of episodic volunteer motivation
support for Hypothesis le found in the continuing Sam- proposes, evidence from the four samples in this
ple Iw-ls. In the terms of James and Brett (1984), atti- investigation indicates that overt choices to take part
tude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, and or not take part in episodic volunteer work can be
moral obligation are only partial mediators of the effect traced to covert cognitions: the relative strengths of in-
of marital status on intentions in that sample. tentions. Despite the dichotomous nature and limited
variance in volunteer attendance, correlations between
it and attendance intentions measured 4 to 8 weeks ear-
Determinants of Attitude and Subjective Norm lier were all high and positive (rs from .57 to .73, all
Relations of attitude and subjective norm to the sum ps < .01); logistic regression analyses also showed that
of cross-products of their proposed antecedents were sta- these intentions had a strong, positive relationship with
tistically significant and in the proposed direction in all volunteer attendance. The predicted effects of compet-
four samples. Correlations between attitude and 2aZ?j X ing intentions (motivations to take part in settings that
e\ were .60, .31, .59, and .49 for Samples Iw, Is, 2w, and conflicted with volunteer attendance) were important
3s, respectively (p < .01 for each, except p < .05 for r = and negative for relatively new volunteers, but they
.31). Correlations between subjective norm and 2n6j X were less so for more experienced volunteers, who also
mcj were .65, .58, .74, and .66 for the same samples, re- had slightly higher attendance rates.
spectively (all ps < .01). These correlations are similar in This latter finding suggests some interesting questions
magnitude to previous studies of the theory of reasoned for future research. The theory presented in this article
action and the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991; presupposes deliberate decision processes. The present
Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Ajzen& Madden, 1986). methodology focuses attention on specific (categories of)
Zero-order correlations, however, do not provide un- cognitions, which has been criticized as a means for par-
equivocal support that attitude or subjective norm stem ticipants to construct cognitions at the point of measure-
from multiplicative composites. The relations of such ment rather than retrieve existing ones from memory and
composites to other variables are dependent on the scale create spurious support or self-generated validity for a
intervals and origin of the variables in the composite. theory (Feldman & Lynch, 1988). Perhaps the theory is
Both Evans (1991) and Ajzen (1991) outline procedures less applicable to veteran volunteers, who may be more
for testing the predictive contribution of the sum of routinized or script based in their approach to volunteer
multiplicative terms to a dependent variable, unique work and therefore less conscious of alternative activities.
from the additive contributions of their constituent In an interesting extension of the present theory to atten-
terms. These procedures require large sample sizes to at- dance or absence from work, perhaps the opposite is true
tain even moderate statistical power (McClelland & for those who miss work: They may be more likely to be
Judd, 1993). Therefore, data from the three samples in aware of and actively process the factors associated with
the present investigation were combined (in the same attending alternative (to work) settings.
way as described above).
In the aggregate sample, the multiplicative term "Zabi Components of Intention
X e\ explained unique variance in attitude beyond the ad- Competitive tests of the predictions of the SEU-based
ditive effects of attendance beliefs and evaluations terms, approach, the theory of reasoned action, the theory of
382 DAVID A. HARRISON

planned behavior, and an expanded theory of episodic ies is that few voluntarism situations are thought to have
volunteer motivation indicate that the latter theory fits enough moral intensity for decision makers to recognize
better as a predictor of volunteers' intentions to attend that they are making a moral decision (Jones, 1991).
their scheduled work. If one equates the strength of inten- Some of the proposed antecedents of the moral intensity
tion with the strength of motivation, then the expanded of an issue (e.g., social consensus, probability and con-
theory seems to provide a more thorough explanation of centration of the effect, magnitude of consequences to
what guides, energizes, and sustains the tendency to take others) could be used in initial attempts to formulate and
part in volunteer work. test what generates a feeling of moral obligation. It might
With an interesting exception, the proposed theoretical also be useful to move beyond voluntarism in examining
determinants explained unique variance in intentions in the impact of moral obligation, by likening it to a per-
each sample. Just as with the impact of competing al- ceived cultural or informal norm, distinct from social ex-
ternatives on eventual attendance, attitudes were not im- pectations from identifiable persons or groups (P. B.
portant determinants of intention for the experienced Smith & Peterson, 1988). In this way, a generalized ver-
volunteers in Sample 1 w-1 s. In fact, subsequent analyses sion of the present theory could be applied to a variety
showed that experience moderated the impact of attitude of behavioral decisions, much as the theory of reasoned
toward volunteering on volunteer intentions. Detailed action and theory of planned behavior already are.
analyses of the beliefs of these volunteers indicated that
volunteers did not intend to work at the shelter to get a Components of Attitude and Subjective Norm
boost of affect, or because they anticipated some satisfac-
tion from doing that work. They were more experienced Attitudes and subjective norms were highly and consis-
and perhaps were more aware of (or more jaded about) tently correlated with their proposed components, and
what little gratification they got from it. Instead, social they form the practical hub of the present theory. From
pressure, perceived control, and moral obligation seemed a manager's viewpoint, the beliefs that differentiate the
to drive their intentions and eventual attendance. possible volunteers who do and do not attend volunteer
Data from these samples also provide consistent evi- work is informative and potentially valuable. These be-
dence that individuals consider perceived social expecta- liefs are strong candidates for interventions (Ajzen &
tions when they form voluntarism intentions. Social fac- Fishbein, 1980). For example, in the present study, anal-
tors undoubtedly deserve more research attention, as yses of the attendance beliefs yielded a three-dimensional
studies might profitably move beyond considerations of structure. However, only a factor composed of percep-
anticipated satisfactions or evaluations of the possible re- tions of the positive outcomes of volunteer attendance
wards of voluntarism. Future investigations of volunteer ("gives me a feeling of self-satisfaction," "lets me interact
motivation might benefit not only from incorporating with other volunteers," "lets me meet interesting people,"
subjective norms into a set of predictors but also from and "helps me appreciate the things I have") significantly
measuring possible sources of these perceived expecta- differentiated volunteers who did (m = 16.31) from
tions, such as the frequency of volunteer work done by those who did not (m = 12.64) attend their scheduled
family, friends, and coworkers, which is more congruent work, and only in Samples 2w and 3s of relatively new
with the definition of a social norm as a pattern of group volunteers, / ( i 0 2) = 1-97, p = .05. Interventions aimed at
behavior. strengthening these beliefs might improve initial atten-
Perceived behavioral control was also a significant dance rates in those samples.
component of volunteer intentions in all of the samples. Factor analyses of the normative beliefs revealed a uni-
Ajzen (1991) has likened perceived behavioral control to tary dimension. Strengths of perceived expectations from
task-specific self-efficacy. Empirical research on the over- all five social referents ("spouse or girlfriend," "rest of
lap between the two, both in and out of volunteer family," "people at my job," "church members," or
contexts would be useful. If these two constructs are "other shelter volunteers") were significantly higher
nearly identical, then a wealth of data from the goal-set- among those who did show up to do volunteer work than
ting literature suggests that self-efficacy is part of any among those who did not. All of these differences in nor-
goal-directed motivation (Locke & Latham, 1990). mative beliefs across attendees and absentees were at least
Techniques for strengthening efficacy-related cognitions 0.5 standard deviations. Consequently, in the year follow-
regarding job attendance (Frayne & Latham, 1987) ing the study, one of the programs adopted by the shelter
might then be effective for helping managers in social ser- administration emphasized social pressure and social
vice organizations improve volunteer attendance. support for volunteering.
Moral obligation consistently received the highest re- Because perceived social expectations are also at the
gression weight in determining volunteer motivation. heart of role theory (Katz & Kahn, 1978), normative be-
One reason that it might have been ignored in past stud- liefs could also be used in future research to describe
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 383

both role conflict and role ambiguity regarding volunteer scarce temporal resources. These deliberations are often
participation. For instance, if there is considerable varia- difficult and complex; they involve perceptions of con-
tion in perceived expectations across the salient people or flicting demands and motivations to take part in activities
groups in an individual's social environment, then role at competing settings—toward attending those settings.
conflict exists. Role ambiguity might be represented by According to the proposed theory of episodic volunteer
uniformly low or moderate normative beliefs. motivation and its converging empirical support across
time and independent samples, the specific motivation to
Limitations and Additional Research Directions take part in a volunteer setting flows from answers to four
intrapersonal questions. The importance of the answers
This research was conducted to understand more to each of these questions for one's volunteer motivation
about individuals' motivation to participate in volunteer depends on one's past experience in volunteering. These
work. Although these findings support the efficacy of the four questions are as follows: (a) How much would I like
theory of episodic volunteer motivation as a framework or get out of attending volunteer work? (b) How strongly
for understanding, they cannot yet be generalized beyond do I think that important other people expect me to at-
the sample from which the volunteers were drawn. The tend volunteer work? (c) How likely is it that I can attend
sample comprised men who already volunteered at least volunteer work? and (d) How clear is it that attending
once to work in a homeless shelter in a city in the Mid- volunteer work is the right thing for me to do?
west. The results of this study may not generalize to other
people and settings. References
The additions and expansions contained in the theory
of episodic volunteer motivation, although supported by Adsett, J. L. (1976). Locus of control, self concept and attitudes
correlational evidence in this study, have not been rigor- of Parents Anonymous volunteers. Unpublished doctoral dis-
ously tested as causal or even stable components and in- sertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
fluences. Experimental tests, especially with manipula- Ajzen,-I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of
tions of the new constructs and tests of the new relations planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-
in the theory (e.g., impact of moral obligation) are control: From cognition to behavior. Heidelberg, Germany:
Springer-Verlag.
needed to support the proposed hypotheses. Fortunately,
Ajzen, I. (1987). Attitudes, traits, and actions: Dispositional
because of the simultaneous low cost and high need for prediction of behavior in personality and social psychology.
volunteers in many organizations, there may be many op- In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy-
portunities for researchers to conduct such experiments chology (Vol. 20, pp. 1-63). New York: Academic Press.
in the field—perhaps via manipulations of the appropri- Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago:
ate theoretical attributes of "sales pitches" regarding vol- Dorsey Press.
unteer activities. Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organiza-
A focus on the belief components of the new theory is tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.
also important in subsequent studies and applications. It Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
may be possible for researchers to identify a common or predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
core set of volunteer attendance beliefs and normative Hall.
Ajzen, L, & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed
beliefs, from organizations in different regions, with
behavior from attitudinal and normative variables. Journal
different social service missions, and with samples con- of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453-474.
taining different demographic and personality profiles. Amenta, M. M. (1984). Death anxiety, purpose in life and du-
Because beliefs govern both attitudes and subjective ration of service in hospice volunteers. Psychological Re-
norms about participating in voluntary activities, such a ports, 54, 979-984.
belief set would provide volunteer managers with a pow- Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation.
erful, easily operationalized group of concepts to help Special issue: Theories of cognitive self-regulation. Organi-
generate high levels of volunteer participation through zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248-
recruitment, selection, and training strategies. 287.
Brudney, J. L. (1990). Fostering volunteer programs in the pub-
lic sector: Planning, initiating, and managing voluntary activ-
Conclusion ities. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1991). A functional analysis of al-
In this article, I have argued that much volunteer work truism and prosocial behavior: The case of volunteerism. Re-
is discrete or episodic in nature, rather than regular or view of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 119-148.
routine. When asked to do it or when made aware of it, Cnaan, R. A., & Goldberg-Glen, R. S. (1991). Measuring mo-
individuals actively deliberate and decide about taking tivation to volunteer in human services. Journal of Applied
part in it, considering how best to allocate their own Behavioral Science, 27, 269-284.
384 DAVID A. HARRISON

Eden, D., & Kinnar, J. (1991). Modeling Galatea: Boosting titudes toward behavioral alternatives. Journal oj'Experimen-
self-efficacy to increase volunteering. Journal of Applied Psy- tal Social Psychology, 17, 286-307.
chology, 76, 770-780. James, L. R., & Brett, J. M. (1984). Mediators, moderators,
Etzioni, A. (1988). The moral dimension: Toward a new eco- and tests for mediation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69,
nomics. New York: Free Press. 307-321.
Evans, M. G. (1991). The problem of analyzing multiplicative Johns, G. (1991). Substantive and methodological constraints
composites: Interactions revisited. American Psychologist, on behavior and attitudes in organizational research. Organi-
46,6-15. zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 49, 80-
Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G., Jr. (1988). Self-generated va- 102.
lidity and other effects of measurement on belief, attitude, Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in
intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Man-
421-435. agement Review, 16, 366-395.
Ferris, G. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1987). The components of Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of orga-
social influence and their importance for human resources nizations. New York: Wiley.
research. In K. R. Rowland & G. R. Ferris (Eds.), Research Lafer, B. (1989). Predicting performance and persistence in
in personnel and human resource management (Vol. 5, pp. hospice volunteers. Psychological Reports, 65, 467-472.
103-128). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Latting, J. K. (1990). Motivational differences between Black
Ferris, J. M. (1988). The use of volunteers in public service and White volunteers. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quar-
production: Some demand and supply considerations. Social terly, 19, 121-135.
Science Quarterly, 69, 3-23. Leana, C. R., Ahlbrandt, R. S., & Murrell, A. J. (1992). The
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and effects of employee involvement programs on unionized
behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, workers' attitudes, perceptions, and preferences in decision
MA: Addison-Wesley. making. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 861-873.
Frayne, C. A., & Latham, G. P. (1987). Application of social Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting
learning theory to employee self-management of attendance. and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 387-392. Martocchio, J. J., & Harrison, D. A. (1993). To be there or not
Frisch, M. B., & Gerrard, M. (1981). Natural helping systems: to be there? Questions, theories and methods in absenteeism
Red Cross volunteers. American Journal of Community Psy- research. In G. R. Ferris (Eds.), Research in personnel and
chology, 9, 567-579. human resource management (Vol. 11, pp. 259-329). Green-
Gabard, D. L. (1990). Retention of volunteers: Factors affecting wich, CT: JAI Press.
retention in volunteer associations whose primary mission is McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties
to deliver care to adults with AIDS or ARC. Unpublished doc- of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological
toral dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Bulletin, 114, 376-390.
Angeles. Miller, L. E., & Crush, J. E. (1986). Individual differences in
Gallup Organization. (1986). Americans volunteer, 1985. attitudinal versus normative determination of behavior.
Princeton, NJ: Gallup. Journal of 'Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 190-202.
Miller, L. E., & Crush, J. E. (1988). Improving predictions in
Gidron, B. (1983). Sources of job satisfaction among service
expectancy theory research: Effects of personality, expectan-
volunteers. Journal of Voluntary Action Research, 12, 20-35.
cies, and norms. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 107-
Gidron, B. (1984). Predictors of retention and turnover among 122.
service volunteer workers. Journal of Social Service Re- Morrow-Howell, N., & Mui, A. (1989). Elderly volunteers:
search, 8, 1-16. Reasons for initiating and terminating service. Journal of
Gillespie, D. F, & King, A. E. (1985). Demographic under- Gerontological Social Work, 13, 21-34.
standing of volunteerism. Journal of Sociology and Social Naylor, H. H. (1985). Beyond managing volunteers. Journal of
Welfare, 72,798-816. Voluntary Action Research, 14, 25-30.
Gorsuch, R. L., & Ortberg, J. (1983). Moral obligation and Omoto, A. M., & Snyder, M. (1990). Basic research in action:
attitudes: Their relation to behavioral intentions. Journal of Volunteerism and society's response to AIDS. Personality
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1025-1028. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 152-165.
Hodgkinson, V. A., & Weitzman, M. S. (1988). Giving and vol- Organ, D. W. (1988). A restatement of the satisfaction-perfor-
unteering in the United States: Findings from a national sur- mance hypothesis. Journal of Management, 14, 547-557.
vey. Washington, DC: Independent Sector. Pearce, J. (1983a). Job attitude and motivation differences be-
Horton-Smith, D. (1981). Altruism, volunteers, and volun- tween volunteers and employees from comparable organiza-
teerism. Journal of Voluntary Action Research, 10, 21-36. tions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 646-652.
Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenback, J. H. (1977). The role of job satis- Pearce, J. (1983b). Participation in voluntary associations:
faction in absence behavior. Organizational Behavior and How membership in a formal organization changes the re-
Human Performance, 19, 148-161. wards of participation. In D. H. Smith & J. Van Til (Eds.),
Independent Sector. (1990). Giving and caring in the United International perspectives on voluntary action research (pp.
States. Princeton, NJ: Gallup. 3-91). Washington, DC: University Press of America.
Jaccard, J. (1981). Attitudes and behavior: Implications of at- Peters, L. H., O'Connor, E. J., & Eulberg, J. R. (1985). Situa-
VOLUNTEER ATTENDANCE 385

tional constraints: Sources, consequences, and future consid- Baldwin (Eds.), Voluntary action research (pp. 4-21). Lex-
erations. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Man- ington, MA: Heath.
agement, 3, 79-113. Smith, P. B., & Peterson, M. F. (1988). Leadership, organiza-
Pierucci, J., &Noel, R. C. (1980). Duration of participation of tions, and culture: An event management model. London:
correctional volunteers as a function of personal and situa- Sage.
tional variables. Journal of Community Psychology, 8, 245- Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (1991). Who helps and why?: The
250. psychology of AIDS volunteerism. In S. Spacapan & S.
Pomozal, R., & Jaccard, J. (1976). An informational approach Oskamp (Eds.), Helping and being helped: Naturalistic stud-
to altruistic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- ies (pp. 152-165). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
chology, 33,317-326. Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (1992). Volunteerism and soci-
Poole, P. P., Gray, B., & Gioia, D. A. (1990). Organizational ety's response to the HIV epidemic. Current Directions in
script development through interactive accommodation. Psychological Science, 1, 113-116.
Group and Organization Studies, 15, 212-232. Steel, R. P., & Ovalle, N. K. (1984). A review and meta-analysis
Prestholdt, P. H., Lane, I. M., & Mathews, R. C. (1987). Nurse of research on the relationship between behavioral intentions
turnover as reasoned action: Development of a process and employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69,
model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 221-227. 673-686.
Rosse, J. G., & Miller, H. E. (1984). Relationship of absentee- Tapp, J. T., & Spanier, D. (1973). Personal characteristics of
ism and other employee behaviors. In P. S. Goodman & R. S. volunteer phone counselors. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
Atkin (Eds.), Absenteeism: New approaches to understand- ical Psychology, 41, 245-250.
ing, measuring, and managing employee absence (pp. 194- Trevino, L. K., & Youngblood, S. A. (1990). Bad apples in bad
228). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. barrels: A causal analysis of ethical decision-making behav-
Schaubroeck, J., & Ganster, D. C. (1991). Beyond the call of ior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 378-385.
duty: A field study of extra-role behavior in voluntary orga- Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, CA:
nizations. Human Relations, 44, 569-582. Brooks/Cole.
Schoemaker, P. J. (1982). The expected utility model: Its vari- Tubbs, M. E., & Ekeberg, S. E. (1991). The role of intentions
ants, purposes, evidence, and limitations. Journal of Eco- in work motivation: Implications for goal-setting theory and
nomic Literature, 20, 529-563. research. Academy of Management Review, 16, 180-199.
Schwartz, S., & Tessler, R. (1972). A test of a model for reduc- Unger, L. S. (1987). Effect of actual and perceived availability
ing measured behavior-attitude discrepancies. Journal of Per- of time on voluntarism. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 65,
sonality and Social Psychology, 24, 225-236. 524-526.
Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1991). Statistical abstract of the
theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research United States, Vol. 111. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
with recommendations for modifications and future re- Printing Office.
search. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325-343. Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Smith, D. (1966). A psychological model of individual partici- Weisbrod, B. A. (1988). The nonprofit economy. Cambridge,
pation in formal voluntary organizations: Application to MA: Harvard University Press.
some Chilean data. American Journal of Sociology, 72, 249-
266. Received October 18, 1993
Smith, D., Reddy, R., & Baldwin, B. (1972). Types of volun- Revision received July 25, 1994
tary action: A definitional essay. In D. Smith, R. Reddy, & B. Accepted July 29, 1994 •

You might also like