You are on page 1of 11

Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

DOI 10.1007/s00213-009-1649-6

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Differential effects of escitalopram on attention:


a placebo-controlled, double-blind cross-over study
Barbara Drueke & Julia Baetz & Maren Boecker &
Olaf Moeller & Christoph Hiemke & Gerd Gründer &
Siegfried Gauggel

Received: 26 January 2009 / Accepted: 17 August 2009 / Published online: 16 September 2009
# Springer-Verlag 2009

Abstract showed significant slower reaction times in all warning


Rationale The role of serotonin (5-HT) in attention is not conditions as compared with placebo while participants
fully understood yet. receiving escitalopram as second treatment showed signif-
Objective We aimed to investigate whether attention is icant faster reaction times as compared with placebo. For
modulated after treatment with escitalopram, a selective the sub-chronic treatment, we found significant differences
serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI). between escitalopram and placebo depending on sequence
Methods We administered 10 mg of escitalopram to 20 of intake, but only for the flanker condition: participants
healthy subjects in a placebo-controlled, double-blind receiving escitalopram first had significant slower reaction
cross-over design for 1 day or to another 20 participants times in incongruent trials with escitalopram as compared
for a period of 7 days. Attention was assessed at time of with placebo while participants starting with placebo had
plasma peak escitalopram concentration using the compu- significant shorter reaction times in incongruent trials with
terised Attention Network Test (ANT), which is a combined escitalopram.
flanker and cued reaction time task. Conclusions Thus, the results showed a differential effect
Results The results showed differential effects of seroto- of escitalopram in cognition, especially in attention, and are
nergic manipulation on attention depending on sequence of discussed with regard to an interaction between serotonin
intake. For the acute treatment, we found significant and familiarity with the attention test.
differences between escitalopram and placebo for all
warning conditions dependent of sequence of intake: Keywords 5-HT . Escitalopram . Attention .
participants receiving escitalopram as first treatment Attention Network Test (ANT) . Humans

B. Drueke (*) : J. Baetz : M. Boecker : S. Gauggel


Department of Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology,
RWTH Aachen University, Introduction
Pauwelsstr. 30,
52074 Aachen, Germany Attention processes can be modulated by the action of
e-mail: bdrueke@ukaachen.de
different neurotransmitters in the brain via administration of
O. Moeller : G. Gründer specific psychotropic substances (Robbins 1997). Of special
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy interest is the neurotransmitter serotonin (5-HT) as it is
and JARA - Translational Brain Medicine, involved in different cognitive processes, e.g. learning and
RWTH Aachen University,
memory (Riedel et al. 1999). Serotonin is also a critical
Pauwelsstr. 30,
52074 Aachen, Germany neurotransmitter in several mental disorders including major
depression and schizophrenia, which seem to be associated
C. Hiemke with dysregulated 5-HT transmission and accompanied by
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy,
neurocognitive impairments. In schizophrenia for example,
University of Mainz,
Untere Zahlbacher Str. 8, serotonin receptors are increasingly recognised as major
55131 Mainz, Germany targets for cognitive enhancement (Meltzer et al. 2003).
214 Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

Indeed, second-generation antipsychotics such as clozapine and a sophisticated measure of attention (i.e. Attention
or risperidone, which are potent 5-HT2A receptor antagonists Network Test (ANT); Fan et al. 2002) which allows a
(Meltzer 1999), are able to improve vigilance. detailed measurement of attentional functions via presenta-
Various studies have demonstrated a modulating role for tion of different cue and flanker conditions. We hypoth-
serotonin in impulsivity and aggression (Hennig et al. 1997; esised that an administration of 10 mg escitalopram should
Hennig et al. 1998; Netter et al. 1999). In contrast, the role of have a selective influence on attention because 5-HT seems
serotonin in attention is not fully understood yet. Animal to play a modulating role in cognition (Robbins and
studies provide evidence for 5-HT influence on attention: the Roberts 2007). However, no clear hypothesis can be
selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonist M100907 improves specified in regard to different attention components due
attentional performance in rats performing a five-choice serial to inconsistent findings in the past. Therefore, we were not
reaction time (5-CSRT) task (Mirjana et al. 2004). In contrast, only interested in the acute but also in the effects of sub-
5-HT2A receptor agonists impair attentional functioning in rats chronic administration of escitalopram. We expected a
(Carli and Samanin 1992; Koskinen et al. 2000). In addition, larger impact on attention performance after sub-chronic
5-HT1A receptor agonists also impair rats’ accuracy in 5- treatment and therefore, we investigated the same hypothesis
CSRT (Carli and Samanin 2000), while 5-HT1A antagonists for an effect on ANT performance after 7 days of treatment.
such as WAY100635 reduce attentional deficits induced by
cortical cholinergic dysfunction (Balducci et al. 2003).
In healthy volunteers, there is evidence that 5-HT Materials and methods
stimulation has a negative influence on attention processes.
Acute and subchronic oral administration of 20 and 40 mg A total number of (n=40) healthy male subjects aged
citalopram, a selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitor, led to an between 18 and 39 years participated in this study; 20 of
impaired performance in the Mackworth Clock Test (MCT) them received an acute treatment and another 20, a sub-
which is a measure for vigilance (Riedel et al. 2005). chronic treatment. Most of them were students. The means
Similar results for the MCT were also found by Schmitt et and standard deviation for body weight (kilogram) and
al. (2002) with paroxetine, whereas the intake of 50 and height (centimetre) were 83.1±14.6 and 183.6±8.5 for the
100 mg sertraline did not show any effect on vigilance acute treatment group and 87.1±18.1 and 181.4±5.3 for the
(Riedel et al. 2005). Further evidence for impairments of sub-chronic group, respectively. All participants were drug-
attention performance by administering serotonergic drugs free and non-smokers and underwent a drug screening
comes from a study by Wingen et al. (2007). Wingen et al. before inclusion. To establish physical and mental health,
showed an impaired performance in a divided attention task participants underwent a careful screening for any hormon-
after administration of escitalopram (SSRI) alone and after al, cardiovascular or neurological diseases. This screening
combined administration of escitalopram+pindolol (5-HT1A was based on health questionnaires and interviews per-
antagonist) and escitalopram+ketanserin (5-HT2a antago- formed by a physician. Furthermore, we analysed blood
nist), respectively, as compared with placebo. In contrast to samples of each participant to control physical health. In
these findings, it is also postulated that SSRIs are able to addition, all participants underwent a semi-structured
increase cerebral arousal, for example in the critical flicker clinical interview for mental disorders. Exclusion criteria
fusion threshold (CFFT), a task used for the assessment of were a personal or family history of major mental or
alertness, vigilance, and arousal. Acute and subchronic neurological disorder in first-degree relatives, somatic
administration of 20 mg citalopram (Nathan et al. 2000) illness and regular alcohol or illicit drug abuse. No
and 60 mg fluoxetine (Rammsayer and Netter 1988), participant had to be excluded due to any kind of acute or
respectively, improved attention performance (i.e. performance chronic physical disease or mental disorder. All participants
in the CFFT). were informed by a written study description on the aim of
Taken together, results of animal and human studies the study, possible side effects, previous toxicology study
reveal inconsistent results regarding the role of the results and potential health risks. All participants gave their
serotonergic system in attention. Possible reasons for written informed consent.
inconsistent findings could be seen in the heterogeneity of Challenge tests were performed at two sessions (placebo
paradigms used to assess attention, differences in the and escitalopram) 1-week apart. Participants of the
substances used (i.e. the potency for blocking 5-HT multiple-doses group were also instructed about timing of
reuptake), duration of drug administration (single vs. intake at home and how to handle problems during time of
multiple dosing) and dosages used. Therefore, the present intake. Furthermore, they were informed about the collec-
study investigated whether 5-HT release has an influence tion of a blood sample to control for their compliance
on attention performance exerted by administration of a before starting the experiment on test day. Subjects received
highly selective 5-HT reuptake inhibitor (i.e. escitalopram) detailed written instructions of what to do and what to
Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223 215

avoid in the evening, night and morning before the reduced libido. Maximal plasma concentrations have been
experiment. Instructions included for example: to go to noted approximately 3–4 h after dosing (Sogaard et al.
bed not later than midnight and to guarantee more than 6 h 2005). Escitalopram and placebo were administered as
of sleep, to avoid any medication as well as alcoholic or white tablets of identical appearance and were provided by
caffeine-containing beverages etc. On the day of the H. Lundbeck A/S (Copenhagen, Denmark).
experiment, lunch was taken between 12:00 and 12:30 p.m.
Afterwards, no food intake was allowed. Experimental procedure
The study was approved by the ethics committee of the
Medical Faculty of the RWTH Aachen University and the On the day of the tests (at 1:30 pm), participants were
National Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices and was seated in a comfortable chair in the laboratory. For
conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. participants with 7-days intake plasma-concentration of
Participants were paid and allowed to quit the study any escitalopram were analysed from a blood sample to control
time they wished. for compliance with drug administration. Blood samples
were analysed in the laboratory of the university hospital to
Design determine plasma concentrations of escitalopram. Drugs
were administered 15 min later (for all participants, 1-day
The study was performed in a double-blind cross-over intake and 7-days intake) in identical tablets to guarantee
design with two treatment levels (placebo and 10 mg blindness for subjects and experimenters. After drug
escitalopram [Cipralex®]) as a single or a repeated oral administration, subjects were asked to complete different
dose: 20 participants received a single oral dose of 10 mg personality questionnaires. At the time of expected plasma
escitalopram or placebo and another 20 participants peak (about 200 min after tablet intake) participants
received 10 mg escitalopram or placebo for 7 days. performed the Attention Network Test (Fan et al. 2002).
Participants were free to decide whether they would get
the short-term treatment (single intake) or the long-term Attention Network Test
treatment (repeated intake). The participants were allocated
to their starting substance (placebo/escitalopram) at random, The ANT is a computer-based choice reaction time (RT) task
but this was not fully counterbalanced for each length of which was designed for the assessment of reaction times
intake (1 vs. 7 days of intake). For the single intake, we estimating alerting, orienting, and executive attention which
chose a time interval of 1 week between each session, and are proposed to describe functions of three attentional
for the multiple doses, we chose a time interval of 2 weeks networks (Fan et al. 2002). Subjects were required to
as it is sufficient for complete washout since the elimination identify whether a central arrow presented on a computer
half-life of escitalopram is 27–32 h (Aronson and Delgado screen pointed left or right by pressing the equivalent button
2004). We decided to investigate sub-chronic effects after a on the keyboard always using the same hand. This target
drug intake of 7 days as we expected a steady-state arrow was flanked on either side by lines (neutral condition),
concentration of escitalopram in plasma after five elimination by two arrows in the same direction (congruent condition),
half-life times. Group size of 20 participants was based on a or in the opposite direction (incongruent condition). The
power calculation (Faul et al. 2007). stimuli (one central arrow accompanied by four flankers)
appeared either above or below a fixation point. The three
Drugs conditions are depicted in Fig. 1.
There were four warning conditions indicating the
Escitalopram is a highly selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor imminent appearance and/or location of the target: (1) no
indicated for the treatment of major depressive disorder cue (only fixation cross), (2) centre cue, (3) double cue and
(MDD), panic disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, (4) spatial cue (see Fig. 2 for details). The ANT is a
obsessive-compulsive disorder and social phobia (Burke combination of the cued reaction time (Fernandez-Duque
2002; Burke et al. 2002). Escitalopram is the therapeutically
active S-enantiomer of citalopram. Approved doses are 10–
20 mg as indicated by manufacturer Lundbeck (Copenhagen,
Denmark). Escitalopram has been shown to be an effective
and well-tolerated treatment for MDD in placebo-controlled
trials and to have a similar safety profile to citalopram
(Yevtushenko et al. 2007).
Possible side effects observed after escitalopram are
nausea, vertigo, reduced appetite, sleep disturbances, and Fig. 1 The three ANT target conditions
216 Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

a four-factor ANOVA for repeated measures was computed


with the repeated measurement factors ‘substance’ (10 mg
escitalopram vs. placebo), ‘cue’ (no cue, doublecue,
centrecue and spatialcue) and ‘congruency’ (congruent,
incongruent and neutral trials) and an independent factor
‘sequence of intake’ which was included as participants
were assigned randomly to the starting substance (S1=first
escitalopram, then placebo vs. S2=first placebo, then
escitalopram). The factor ‘sequence’ is of special interest
as the randomization to the starting substance was not
Fig. 2 The four ANT warning conditions fully counterbalanced. Furthermore, a new study (Verheij
et al. 2009) indicates differential effects of 5-HT on
measures of cognition in rats and therefore, we were also
and Posner 1997) and the flanker task (Eriksen and interested in possible interactions of 5-HT and sequence as
Ericksen 1974). this was never done in humans before. For the second
So, the ANT provides multiple conditions for the group (sub-chronic treatment), we conducted another
measurement of attentional functions in a single task, and ANOVA with the same factors as above. For significant
it is possible to find out if reaction times are influenced by effects, post-hoc comparisons were computed at α=0.05
alerting cues, spatial cues and flankers. and corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni) by the
The ANT included a practise block with 24 trials and number of possible comparisons. Reported in the results
three assessment blocks with 96 trials each (total duration, section are only those post-hoc tests relevant for our
30 min). During the practise block, the participants received analyses, but all possible comparisons were computed
immediate feedback whether their response was right or automatically. Due to small and different sample sizes, we
wrong or too slow. Each trial lasted exactly 4,000 ms and also calculated standardised effect sizes according to Cohen
consisted of five different periods. In the first period, a (1988). As effect size estimates are slightly biassed, they
fixation cross was presented for 400–1,600 ms. An are corrected using a factor provided by Hedges and Olkin
additional cue was then shown for 100 ms in the second (1985).
period followed by the fixation cross for 400 ms in the third In addition, we analysed plasma levels of escitalopram
period. In the fourth period, the target arrow with flankers from participants of the multiple-dose group before
was shown for at most 1,700 ms. After a response, the starting the experiment on test days: participants reached
fixation cross was presented again in the last period. The a mean plasma level of 6.9±4.1 ng/ml for escitalopram and a
duration of the last period depends on the duration of the first mean plasma level for desmethylcitalopram of 5.5±2.2 ng/ml.
fixation period and the reaction time (3,500 ms—first fixation Plasma levels were determined about 24 h after the last
period—reaction time). intake. Plasma levels of two participants were below
For data analysis, only valid trials and trials with a detection limit, and these participants were excluded
reaction time between 200 and 2,000 ms were considered. from further analysis. So, data of all 20 participants were
In addition, an outlier analysis was performed. Trials with a analysed for the acute treatment with nine participants for
reaction time two standard deviations above or below the S1 and 11 participants for S2. For the sub-chronic
condition mean were not used for further analyses. In addition treatment, data of 18 subjects were included, whereas nine
to condition means, the overall mean reaction times as well as participants were analysed for S1 and another nine for S2.
the accuracy (percentage of correct responses) were recorded. All analyses were computed with Statistica 8.0.

Statistics
Results
All data were screened for deviation from normality,
outliers and homogeneity of variance. Since the study was For the acute treatment group, we found an overall RT for
conducted in a cross-over design with two different treat- correct trials of 470 ms with placebo and 496 ms with
ments (escitalopram vs. placebo) and two different admin- escitalopram for S1 which did not differ significantly from
istration durations (1-day intake vs. 7-days intake), we each other (T=2.04, p=0.07) but showing a trend for
conducted two analyses of variance (ANOVAs), one for the shorter reaction times with placebo. For S2, we found an
acute treatment group and one for the group with sub- overall reaction time of 444 ms with escitalopram and
chronic treatment. Dependent variables were the reaction 459 ms with placebo which differed significantly (T=−4.5,
times in ANT conditions. For the first group (1-day intake), p<0.01) indicating shorter reaction times with escitalo-
Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223 217

pram. For the accuracy of responding we did neither find no-cue condition (pno cue =0.13, ESno cue =−0.17; pdouble cue <
differences between escitalopram and placebo for S1 0.01, ESdouble cue =−0.22; pcentre cue <0.01, EScentre cue =
(98.2% with placebo and 98.6% with escitalopram, T= −0.37; pspatial cue =0.02, ESspatial cue =−0.21) (Table 1).
0.82, p=0.44) nor for S2 (97.2% with placebo and 98% Because training could also have an influence on attention
with escitalopram, T=1.06, p=0.31). performance, reaction times for both sequences were plotted
For the acute treatment, the ANOVA for repeated separately for substances (Fig. 3, bottom row). It can be seen
measures revealed significant main effects of ‘cue’ (F= that reaction times for all warning conditions were nearly the
64.7, p<0.01) and congruency (F=255.2, p<0.01). We did same with placebo independent of sequence of intake (see
neither find a significant main effect of ‘substance’ (F=1.2, Fig. 4, bottom row on the right side), while reaction times
p>0.05) nor of ‘sequence of intake’ (F=1.6, p>0.05). with escitalopram were differing. Calculated effect sizes for
Furthermore, we found a significant interaction between escitalopram (ESno cue =0.66, ESdouble cue =0.76, EScentre cue =
‘cue’ and ‘congruency’ (F=17.1, p<0.01) pointing out that 0.88, ESspatial cue =0.67) indicated moderate to large effects
for all cueing conditions, the presence of incongruent for differences in reaction times with escitalopram between
flankers increased RT, and however, this effect was enhanced S1 and S2 and calculated effect sizes for placebo revealed
when subjects were given alerting cues (centre or double almost no effects in the warning conditions (ESno cue =0.38,
cues) that contained no spatial information. Interestingly, we ESdouble cue =−0.14, EScentre cue =0.12, ESspatial cue =0.15)
also found a highly significant ‘substance x cue x sequence’ between S1 and S2. To investigate possible effects on
interaction (F=4.9, p<0.01) which is depicted in Fig. 3 (top training on placebo, we also calculated a multivariate
row). Post-hoc tests revealed for all cue conditions for S1 analysis of variance on placebo data with ‘sequence’ (S1
(first escitalopram, then placebo) significant differences vs. S2) as independent factor and reaction times in cue
indicating shorter reaction times with placebo (pno cue < conditions as dependent variables. Results revealed no effect
0.01, ESno cue =0.24; pdouble cue <0.01, ESdouble cue =0.51; on sequence for all cue conditions (Fno cue =0.83, pno cue >
pcentre cue <0.01, EScentre cue =0.55; pspatial cue <0.01, ESspatial 0.05, Fdouble cue =0.1, pdouble cue >0.05, Fcentre cue =0.08,
cue =0.43). Post-hoc tests for S2 (first placebo, then escita- pcentre cue >0.05, Fspatial cue =0.12, pspatial cue >0.05).
lopram) indicated shorter reaction times with escitalopram as For the sub-chronic treatment group, we found an
compared with placebo in all warning conditions except the overall reaction time for correct trials of 452 ms with

Fig. 3 Mean ANT reaction times of cue conditions (no, double, centre squares) and placebo (white circles) on the right side. On the bottom
and spatial cue) and their standard deviations (in millisecond) for the row, the same reaction times are plotted separated for substances: on
acute treatment group. The top row shows reaction times for sequence the left side reaction times in cue conditions with escitalopram for S1
1 (S1 first escitalopram, then placebo) with escitalopram (black (black squares) and S2 (white squares) are shown and on the right
squares) and placebo (black stars) on the left side and for sequence side, reaction times with placebo for S1 (black stars) and S2 (white
2 (S2 first placebo, then escitalopram) with escitalopram (white circles)
218 Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

Table 1 Means (M) and stan-


dard deviations (s.d.) of median Sequence ANT conditions Acute treatment Repeated treatment
reaction times (in millisecond)
of different ANT conditions Escitalopram Placebo Escitalopram Placebo
(cue: no, double, centre and
spatial; flanker: congruent, Cue Congruency M s.d. M s.d. M s.d. M s.d.
incongruent and neutral) with
escitalopram and placebo for S1 No Congruent 506 83 490 51 466 53 470 37
n=20 participants of the acute Incongruent 551 79 532 43 526 64 511 50
treatment group (with S1=
Neutral 500 74 487 44 467 40 467 34
sequence 1: first escitalopram,
then placebo of n=11; S2= S2 No Congruent 459 76 466 61 456 25 474 31
sequence 2: first placebo, then Incongruent 489 66 507 61 497 26 523 25
escitalopram of n=9) and for Neutral 460 62 470 60 460 27 474 27
n=18 participants of the repeat-
S1 Double Congruent 469 74 442 49 431 50 418 34
ed treatment group (with S1=
sequence1: first escitalopram, Incongruent 548 73 505 46 517 59 496 43
then placebo of n=9; S2= Neutral 471 70 444 49 431 43 426 45
sequence 2: first placebo, then S2 Double congruent 419 67 434 67 414 27 426 45
escitalopram of n=9)
incongruent 488 71 500 63 480 33 505 35
Neutral 415 67 434 61 416 32 428 39
S1 Centre Congruent 472 71 450 46 435 47 429 31
Incongruent 552 69 516 36 530 58 498 45
Neutral 476 60 444 39 439 57 427 34
S2 Centre Congruent 417 61 443 59 409 33 418 31
Incongruent 487 66 508 58 489 30 511 26
Neutral 419 65 440 53 411 25 428 35
S1 Spatial Congruent 452 69 424 38 420 46 410 37
Incongruent 503 81 484 48 487 68 467 54
Neutral 454 65 427 40 418 47 413 24
S2 Spatial Congruent 405 66 418 62 404 23 410 27
Incongruent 457 70 469 75 454 22 477 31
Neutral 404 63 419 59 402 26 411 35

placebo and 464 ms with escitalopram for S1 which did ing interaction was found for ‘substance x sequence x
not differ significantly from each other (T=−1.0, p=0.35). congruency’ (F=11.8, p<0.01) which is shown in Fig. 4.
For S2, we found an overall reaction time of 441 ms with Post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between
escitalopram and 457 ms with placebo which differed escitalopram and placebo for incongruent trials for S1
significantly (T=−2.7, p<0.05) indicating shorter reaction indicating shorter reaction times with placebo (pcongruent >
times with escitalopram. For the accuracy of responding, 0.05, EScongruent =0.14; pincongruent <0.01, ESincongruent =0.37;
we did neither find differences between escitalopram and pneutral >0.05, ESneutral =0.15). For S2, we found significant
placebo for S1 (98.2% with placebo and 98.6% with differences indicating shorter reaction times with escitalo-
escitalopram, T=0.82, p=0.44) nor for S2 (97.2% with pram for incongruent and neutral trials (pcongruent >0.05,
placebo and 98% with escitalopram). We did not find EScongruent =−0.38; pincongruent <0.01, ESincongruent =−0.91;
significant correlations between plasma escitalopram and pneutral >0.05, ESneutral =−0.44). So, for the sub-chronic
any behavioural measure. treatment we found moderate to high effects for incongruent
The ANOVA for repeated measures for the sub-chronic trials indicating an influence of escitalopram on flanker task
group revealed neither a significant main effect for that depends on sequence of intake (S1 vs. S2).
‘substance’ (F=0.2, p=0.6) nor for ‘sequence of intake’ Additionally, to rule out possible training effects, we
(F=0.3, p=0.5). A significant main effect for ‘cue’ (F= calculated a multivariate analysis of variance on placebo
88.1, p<0.01) and ‘congruency’ (F=255.2, p<0.01) was data with ‘sequence’ as independent factor and reaction
found. Furthermore, we found a significant interaction times in congruency conditions as dependent variables and
between ‘cue’ and ‘congruency’ (F=15.6, p<0.01) as for we found no significant effects of sequence on congruent
the acute treatment group and between ‘substance’ and (F=0.001, p>0.05), incongruent (F=0.41, p>0.05) or
‘sequence of intake’ (F=7.7, p=0.014). The most interest- neutral trials (F=0.02, p>0.05) indicating that sequence
Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223 219

Fig. 4 Mean reaction times of flanker conditions (congruent, incongruent and neutral) with escitalopram (black) and placebo (striped) separated
for sequence 1 (S1 first escitalopram, then placebo) and sequence 2 (S2 first placebo, then escitalopram) for the sub chronic treatment group

has no influence on reaction times with placebo. Here as treatment groups, one group with a single dose of 10 mg
well, we calculated effect sizes for the comparison of S1 escitalopram and another group with multiple doses of
and S2 with escitalopram. Calculated effect sizes for 10 mg escitalopram.
escitalopram (EScongruent =0.46, ESincongruent =0.72, ESneutral For the acute treatment, we found a significant main
=0.43) indicated moderate to large effects for differences in effect of ‘cue’, indicating shortest reaction times for trials
reaction times with escitalopram between S1 and S2 and with spatial cues followed by reaction times in the double-
calculated effect sizes for placebo revealed almost no effects and centre-cue condition and the slowest reaction times
in the flanker conditions (EScongruent =0.00, ESincongruent = were found in the no-cue condition. Furthermore, we found
0.27, ESneutral =0.07). So here again, for all congruency a significant main effect for congruency (flanker task),
conditions, we found differences between escitalopram and indicating slower reaction times for incongruent trials than
placebo that depend on sequence of intake or the starting for neutral or congruent trials. We found no significant
substance, respectively. main effect for ‘substance’ or ‘sequence of intake’.
In summary, reaction times with escitalopram differed Additionally, we found a significant interaction between
significantly from reaction times with placebo but depended ‘cue x congruency’ indicating increased reaction times for
on sequence of intake and treatment duration: for the incongruent trials in the presence of alerting cues (double
participants who took a single dose of 10 mg escitalopram/ cue and centre cue). So far, these results are in line with the
placebo reaction times in cue conditions with escitalopram results from the original investigation of Fan et al. (2002).
were significantly slower when escitalopram was taken first But, the most interesting result for the acute treatment
and significantly faster, when placebo was taken first. And group was the significant interaction between ‘substance x
for the sub-chronic treatment, we found significant slower cue x sequence of intake’. In the present study, participants
reaction times in incongruent trials with escitalopram when receiving an acute dose of 10 mg escitalopram and starting
taken escitalopram as first treatment and significant faster their intake with escitalopram (S1) showed significantly
reaction times in incongruent and neutral trials when slower reaction times with escitalopram compared with
placebo was taken first. placebo in all cue conditions with no cue, double cue,
centre cue and spatial cue. This supports the results of
studies indicating a negative influence of 5-HT on attention
Discussion (Riedel et al. 2005). However, participants of the acute
treatment group starting with placebo (S2) had a significant
The present study was conducted to determine the influence benefit in all cue conditions with escitalopram which is
of serotonin on attention. We analysed two different consistent with the results from Nathan et al. (2000) who
220 Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

found an improvement of attention after escitalopram in the (for a detailed review, see Ogren et al. 2008). Clarke et al.
CFFT which is a measure for alertness, vigilance and (2005, 2007) even found differential effects of 5-HT
arousal. This benefit may be explained with a general depletion in reversal learning but not in developing or
increase of central arousal as we also found an improve- shifting an attentional set in several monkey studies. They
ment in overall reaction time. Arousal has been associated suggest a differential sensitivity of prefrontal regions (e.g.
with the 5-HT-system for a long time: Gray and Smith ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) to 5-HT modulation, and
(1969) and Gray (1982) postulated 5-HT having an indirect that the distribution of 5-HT and its receptors may exhibit
influence on signal-to-noise ratio from gyrus dentatus to regional and laminar selectivity. Robbins (2007) and
subiculum which is an important structure for the encoding Robbins and Roberts (2007) suggested the differential
of specific environmental information. These findings are effects of 5-HT on prefrontal functioning may reflect a
also supported by results of newer studies, as for example differential sensitivity of specific control processes to 5-HT
from Abrams et al. (2005) who found a strong relationship modulation. All in all, it seems quite likely that 5-HT is a
between 5-HT and arousal. Our results even indicate that 5- modulating neurotransmitter in adaptation processes and
HT not only plays an important role in the general concept that possible interactions have to be an important issue in
of arousal and tonic alertness but—as overall reaction times future research.
may be understood as a measure of tonic alertness—in For the sub-chronic treatment group, we also found
attention, too. Furthermore, our results not only indicate the significant differences depending on sequence of intake not
role of 5-HT in attention, but this effect seems to be for warning conditions but for the flanker conditions. We
moderated by sequence of the experimental condition. All found significant slower reaction times in incongruent trials
of our results concerning an acute treatment with escitalo- with escitalopram as compared with placebo for partic-
pram were dependent on the starting substance—interest- ipants starting with escitalopram as first treatment (S1),
ingly, reaction times with placebo were nearly independent while participants who had escitalopram as second treat-
of sequence of intake (see Fig. 3). One possible explanation ment (S2) showed significant shorter reaction times in
for these findings may be due to the fact that 5-HT may incongruent and neutral trials. However, for the multiple-
interact with the level of familiarity or novelty of the doses group, we were not able to find differences in cue
applied test: acute stimulation of 5-HT may interfere with conditions but in the flanker condition. As previously
novel tasks while it benefits attentional processing with mentioned, there may be an interaction with the familiarity
familiar tasks. Ballaz et al. (2007) found a mediation of of the task and memory processes as we found a worsening
attentional and memory processes in rats through 5-HT7 in reaction times with escitalopram for participants starting
receptors and novelty-seeking behaviour which is thought with escitalopram and a benefit in reaction times with
to model some aspects of sensation seeking in humans. escitalopram for participants starting with placebo.
Even though escitalopram does not selectively propagate In both cases (1 and 7 days intake), there might be an
serotonergic neurotransmission exclusively via the 5-HT7 effect of training, but this counter-argument is not
receptor, this receptor subtype might play a critical role for convincing as we calculated two multivariate analyses
the interpretation of our results. This explanation is also in of variance for the comparison of placebo data for the
line with the results of Verheij et al. (2009) who also found acute and the repeated intake group and we did not find
hints for a possible interaction of 5-HT and novelty in rats any differences between S1 and S2. Additionally, we
and supposed other 5-HT receptors (5-HT1A, 5-HT2 and 5- calculated effect sizes for the comparisons between S1
HT3) to play an important role in this interaction. and S2 separated for escitalopram and placebo. If there is
The discussion on familiarity or novelty of tasks, a pure effect of training, we should have found at least
respectively, is a phenomenon which has been discussed moderate effect sizes for placebo, but we found small or
in the literature on memory performance. It seems probable almost no differences in reaction times with placebo for
that 5-HT has a larger effect on learning than on attention. S1 and S2.
This may be the case either via direct influence of 5-HT on All in all, as we found interactions of substance with
cortical or subcortical structures or via postsynaptic hetero- sequence of intake in different conditions (cue vs. flanker)
ceptors that may regulate the function of several other it is possible that there may be an influence of serotonin on
neurotransmitter systems (e.g. GABA; Stahl 2005; Mongeau attention which interacts with learning. Furthermore, it may
et al. 1997). Based on studies in rodents, the stimulation of also be possible that the effect of serotonin on attention
5-HT1A receptors generally produces learning impairments manifests in different processing stages and depends on
by interfering with memory-encoding mechanisms (Yasuno treatment duration. So, it seems necessary to have a look at
et al. 2004). In contrast, antagonists of 5-HT1A receptors what happens during cue and flanker processing. Posner
facilitate certain types of memory by enhancing hippocampal/ hypothesised the existence of three distinct networks
cortical cholinergic and/or glutamatergic neurotransmission (alerting, orienting and conflict system) which are supposed
Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223 221

to be independent from each other (Fernandez-Duque and that the study participants had not taken their medication.
Posner 2001). Maybe one can speculate that escitalopram The serum concentrations measured in our sample were
as acute treatment acts on attention on early stages of similar to those observed by Klein et al. (2006). These
processing (cue processing) while as sub-chronic treatment, authors determined a serotonin reuptake transporter (SERT)
it acts on later-processing stages (flanker). Cue processing occupancy above 60% at serum concentrations of approxi-
is hypothesised to work on brain structures like thalamus mately 20 nmol/L after single escitalopram doses. Further-
and fronto-parietal areas, while flanker processing is related more, Klein at al. (2006) in another publication demonstrated
to brain areas as anterior cingulum and fusiform gyrus (Fan that 6 h after the last of multiple 10 mg escitalopram doses,
et al. 2005). So, it may be possible that a single oral dose of the SERT occupancy was 82%, while it dropped to 63% 54 h
10 mg escitalopram acts on more basal brain areas while a after the last dose (Klein et al. 2007). The serum concen-
multiple dose acts more on frontal areas. trations of 21 nmol/L determined in our study in conjunction
One critical point to discuss is the difference in results with the SERT occupancy values reported by Klein et al.
between single and repeated oral doses, even if results turn (2006, 2007) for escitalopram doses of 10 mg indicate that
to the same direction (interaction of substance x sequence). the SERT was occupied at least to a modest extent in most of
But they also support the results of Robbins (2007) and our subjects. Reis et al. (2007) reported highly variable
Robbins and Roberts (2007), suggesting a differential serum concentrations of 11 (min) to 111 (max) nmol/L for
sensitivity of control processes to 5-HT. First of all, one escitalopram and 11 to 73 nmo/L for desmethylescitalopram
can speculate that our results were due to a positive transfer for a dose of 10 mg/day in 42 patients after 14 days of drug
(training effect). Such a practise effect seems very unlikely intake. However, it cannot be ruled out that an intake of
as we found significant interaction effects ‘substance x 7 days is too short for causing adaptive changes in synaptic
sequence’ for the acute and the sub-chronic treatment. And function and attain steady state. According to the aim of drug
looking at the reaction times over all trials with placebo, compliance, our data seem to be quite reliable.
almost identical results are obtained for S1 and S2. In summary, we found differential effects of escitalo-
Finally, some limitations should be discussed. Method- pram and its ability to increase 5-HT levels in different
ological concerns come from the oral dosage of 10 mg and aspects of attention (cue and flanker processing) depending
7×10 mg/day, respectively. This dosage was tolerated well on treatment duration and sequence of intake. A single oral
by all of our participants while a higher dosage of 20 mg/ dose of 10 mg escitalopram increased reaction times in cue
day escitalopram has been reported to be associated with conditions for participants starting with escitalopram as first
higher rates of adverse effects (Aronson and Delgado treatment, while for participants who got escitalopram as
2004). As we found only slight differences, one could second treatment we found a benefit in reaction times of
argue that the applied dosage was too low to influence cue conditions as compared with placebo. For the sub-
attention performance. We cannot rule out such a possibil- chronic treatment group, we found similar results for
ity, but wanted to reduce the risk of drop outs due to incongruent trials also depending on sequence of intake:
adverse side effects. Wingen et al. (2007) used oral doses of participants starting their intake with escitalopram had
20 mg of escitalopram to enhance central 5-HT, but significant slower reaction times in incongruent trials with
nevertheless did not find any effect on vigilance. escitalopram as compared with placebo and significant
Another methodological consideration concerns the shorter reaction times when receiving escitalopram as
timing of testing that was conducted after a 200-min period second treatment. Furthermore, we hypothesised these
and was based on the pharmacokinetic profile of escitalo- differential effects of escitalopram on attention probably
pram (to achieve coincidence of testing with approximated to depend on the familiarity of testing materials: on the one
time of plasma peak concentrations). According to Aronson hand, there seems to be an inhibitory effect of increased
and Delgado (2004), the mean peak level following 20 mg synaptic 5-HT concentrations for a new task (S1), while on
escitalopram occurs at 3± 1.5 h, while Burke (2002) the other hand, an accelerating effect on performance could
described maximal plasma concentrations of escitalopram be found for a familiar task (S2). But this phenomenon of
approximately 4 h after administration and Waugh and Goa familiarity was found for different attention functions (cues
(2003) even stated maximal peak concentrations 4–5 h after vs. flanker) and may interact with treatment duration (acute
administration. Furthermore, citalopram plasma levels may vs. sub-chronic). The aspect of familiarity is especially
not necessarily correlate with the behavioural effects (Lader interesting because reaction times under placebo did not
et al. 1986). differ from each other independent from sequence of intake.
Assuming results of the repeated dose group are an Our results demonstrate a highly specific and dissociable
artefact and based on the fact that subject did not take their contribution of 5-HT in the modulation of attention processing.
medication, we would have expected the results to be the The role of 5-HT in attention still remains not clear and further
same as in the single-intake group. But it seemed unlikely investigations should seek to specify that role.
222 Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223

Acknowledgements This study was a research grant supported in Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI (2002) Testing
part by H. Lundbeck A/S (Copenhagen, Denmark). Lundbeck was not the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn
responsible for the creation of the study, the choice of investigators, Neurosci 14(3):340–347
the control of allocation schedule, the conduct of the trial, the Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI (2005)
collection and monitoring of data, the analysis and interpretation and The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 26(2):471–
the writing of the manuscript. B. Drueke, J. Baetz, M. Boecker and 479
Prof. Dr. S. Gauggel report no other conflicts of interest. Dr. O. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A (2007) G*Power3: a
Moeller received travel support from Pfizer and Servier. Prof. Dr. C. flexible statistical power analysis program for the social,
Hiemke has served as a consultant for Servier (Paris, France) and behavioural, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Meth 39:175–
Pfizer (New York, NY). He has served on the speakers’ bureau of 191
Bristol-Myers Squibb, Janssen Cilag, Otsuka, Pfizer, Astra Zeneca, Fernandez-Duque D, Posner MI (1997) Relating the mechanisms of
Sanofi-Aventis (Berlin, Germany), Lundbeck (Copenhagen, orienting and alerting. Neuropsychologia 35(4):477–486
Denmark), Servier and Eli Lilly (Indianapolis, IN). He has received Fernandez-Duque D, Posner MI (2001) Brain imaging of attentional
grant supports from Sanofi-Aventis and Pfizer. Prof. Dr. G. Gründer networks in normal and pathological states. J Clin Exp Neuro-
has served as a consultant for Astra Zeneca (London, UK), Bristol- psychol 23:74–93
Myers Squibb (New York, NY), Johnson & Johnson (Beerse, Gray JA (1982) The neuropsychology of anxiety: an inquiry into the
Belgium), Otsuka (Rockville, MD) and Pfizer (New York, NY). He functions of the septo-hippcampal system. Clarendon Press,
has served on the speakers’ bureau of Astra Zeneca, Bristol-Myers Oxford
Squibb, Eli Lilly (Indianapolis, IN), Janssen Cilag, Otsuka, Pfizer, Gray JA, Smith T (1969) An arousal-decision model for partial
Servier (Paris, France) and Wyeth (Madison, NJ). He has received reinforcement and discrimination learning. In: Gilbert R, Suther-
grant support from Alkermes (Cambridge, Ma), Bristol-Myers Squibb, land N (eds) Animal Discrimination Learning. Academic Press,
Eli Lilly, Johnson & Johnson, and Pfizer. New York, pp 243–272
Hedges LV, Olkin I (1985) Statistical methods for meta-analysis.
Academic Press, Orlando
Hennig J, Huwe S, Netter P (1997) The antagonism of ipsapirone
induced biobehavioral responses by ±pindolol in high and low
References
impulsives. J Neural Transm 104:1027–1035
Hennig J, Kroeger A, Meyer B, Prochaska H, Krien P, Huwe S, Netter
Abrams JK, Johnson PL, Hay-Schmidt A, Mikkelsen JD, Shekhar A, P (1998) Personality correlates of ±pindolol induced decreases in
Lowry CA (2005) Serotonergic systems associated with arousal prolactin. Pharmacopsychiatry 31:19–24
and vigilance behaviors following administration of anxiogenic Klein N, Sacher J, Geiss-Granadia T, Attarbaschi T, Mossaheb N,
drugs. Neuroscience 133(4):983–97 Lanzenberger R, Pötzi C, Holik A, Spindelegger C, Asenbaum S,
Aronson S, Delgado P (2004) Escitalopram. Drugs Today 40(2):121– Dudczak R, Tauscher J, Kasper S (2006) In vivo imaging of
131 serotonin transporter occupancy by means of SPECT and [123I]
Balducci C, Nurra M, Pietropoli A, Samanin R, Carli M (2003) ADAM in healthy subjects administered different doses of
Revearsal of visual attention dysfunction after AMPA lesions of escitalopram or Citalopram. Psychpharmacology 188:263–272
the nucleus basalis magnocellularis (NBM) by cholinesterase Klein N, Sacher J, Geiss-Granadia T, Mossaheb N, Attarbaschi T,
inhibitor donezepil and by a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY Lanzenberger R, Spindelegger C, Holik a, Asenbaum A,
100635. Psychopharmacology 167:28–36 Dudczak R, Tauscher J, Kasper S (2007) Higher serotonin
Ballaz SJ, Akil H, Watson SJ (2007) The 5-HT7 receptor: role in transporter occupancy after multiple dose administration of
novel object discrimination and relation to novelty-seeking escitalopram compared to citalopram: an [123I]ADAM SPECT
behaviour. Neuroscience 149(1):192–202 study. Psychopharmacology 191:333–339
Burke WJ (2002) Escitalopram. Expert Opin Investig Drugs 11 Koskinen T, Ruotsalainen S, Puumala T, Lappalainen R, Koivisto E,
(10):1477–1486 Mannisto PT, Sirvio J (2000) Activation of 5-HT2A receptors
Burke WJ, Gergel I, Bose A (2002) Fixed-dose trial of the single impairs response control in rats in a five-choice serial reaction
isomer SSRI escitalopram in depressed patients. J Clin Psychiatry time task. Neuropharmacology 39:471–481
63:331–336 Lader M, Melhuish A, Fricka G, Fredericson Overo K, Christensen V
Carli M, Samanin R (1992) Serotonin2 receptor agonists and serotonergic (1986) The effects of citalopram in single and repeated doses and
anorectic drugs affect rats’ performance differently in a five choice with alcohol on physiological and psychological measures in
serial reaction time task. Psychopharmacology 106:228–234 healthy subjects. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 31:183–190
Carli M, Samanin R (2000) The 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-PHAT Meltzer HY (1999) The role serotonin in antipsychotic drug action.
reduces rats’ accuracy of attentional performance and enhances Neuropsychopharmacology 21:106S–115S
impulsive responding in a five-choice serial reaction time task: role Meltzer HY, LI Z, Kaneda Y, Ichikawa J (2003) Serotonin receptors:
of presynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. Psychopharmacology 149:259– their key role in drugs to treat schizophrenia. Prog Neuro-
268 psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 27(7):1159–72
Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts Mirjana C, Baviera M, Invernizzi RW, Balducci C (2004) The
AC (2005) Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects revearsal serotonin 5-HT2A receptors antagonist M100907 prevents
learning but not attentional set shifting. J Neurosci 25:532–538 impairment in attentional performance by NMDA receptor
Clarke HF, Walker SC, Dallex JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2007) blockade in the rat prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology
Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behav- 29:1637–1647
iourally and neurochemically specific. Cereb Cortex 17(1):18–27 Mongeau R, Blier P, de Montigy C (1997) The serotonergic and
Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences, noradrenergic systems of the hippocampus: their interactions and
2nd edn. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale the effects of antidepressant treatments. Brains Res Rev 23
Eriksen BA, Ericksen CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the (3):145–195
identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Nathan PJ, Sitaram G, Stough C, Silberstein RB, Sali A (2000)
Psychophys 16:143–149 Serotonin, noradrenaline and cognitive function: a preliminary
Psychopharmacology (2009) 207:213–223 223

investigation of the acute pharmacodynamic effects of a serotonin Riedel SJAJ, WJ VEFPM, Kruizinga M, Ramaekers JG (2002)
versus a serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor. Behav Modulation of the critical flicker fusion effects of serotonin
Pharmacol 11:639–642 reuptake inhibitors by concomitant papillary changes. Psycho-
Netter P, Hennig J, Rohrmann S (1999) Psychobiological differences pharmacology 160:381–386
between aggression and psychoticism. Pharmacopsychiatry 32:5–12 Sogaard B, Mengel H, Rao N, Larsen F (2005) The pharmacokinetics
Ogren SO, Eriksson TM, Elvander-Tottie E, D’Addario C, Ekstroem of escitalopram after oral and intravenous administration of
JC, Svenningsson P, Meister B, Kehr J, Stiedl O (2008) The role single and multiple doses to healthy subjects. J Clin Pharmacol
of 5-HT(1A) receptors in learning and memory. Behav Brain Res. 45(12):1400–1406
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.023 Stahl SM (2005) Essential psychopharmacology. The prescriber’s
Rammsayer T, Netter P (1988) Effects of changes in brain 5-HT guide. University Press, Cambrigde
activity on indicators for cortical arousal. Int Clin Psychophar- Verheij MMM, VeenflietJV KTG, Steenhof M, Cools AR (2009)
macol 3:231–237 Individual differences in the sensitivity to serotonergic drugs: a
Riedel WJ, Klaassen T, Deutz NEP, van Someren A, van Praag HM pharmacobehavioural approach using rats selected on the basis of
(1999) Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces their response to novelty. Psychopharmacology (Berl) .
selective impairment in memory consolidation. Psychopharma- doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1552-1
cology 141:362–369 Waugh J, Goa KL (2003) Escitalopram: a review of its use in the
Riedel WJ, Eikmans K, Heldens A, Schmitt JAJ (2005) Specific management of major depressive and anxiety disorders. CNS
serotonergic reuptake inhibition impairs vigilance performance Drugs 17(5):343–362
acutely and after subchronic treatment. J Psychopharmacol 19 Wingen M, Kuypers KP, Ramaekers JG (2007) The role of 5-HT1A
(1):12–20 and 5-HT2A receptors in attention and motor control: a
Reis M, Chermá CB, Bengtsson F (2007) Therapeutic drug monitor- mechanistic study in healthy volunteers. Psychpharmacology
ing of escitalopram in an outpatient setting. Ther Drug Monit 29 190(3):391–400
(6):758–766 Yasuno F, Suhara T, Nakayama T, Ichimiya T, Okubo Y, Takano A,
Robbins TW (1997) Arousal systems and attentional processes. Biol Anso T, Inoue M, Maeda J, Suzuki K (2004) Inhibitory effect of
Psychol 45:57–71 hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors on human explicit memory. Am
Robbins TW (2007) Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, J Psychiatry 161(8):1505–1506
neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Phil Trans R Yevtushenko VY, Belous AI, Yevtushenko YG, Gsinin SE, Buzik OJ,
Soc B 362:917–932 Agibalova TV (2007) Efficacy and tolerability of escitalopram
Robbins TW, Roberts AC (2007) Differential regulation of fronto- versus citalopram in major depressive disorder: a 6-week,
executive function by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Cereb multicenter, prospective, randomized, double-blind, active-
Cortex 17:i151–i160 controlled study in adult outpatients. Clin Ther 29(11):2319–2332

You might also like