You are on page 1of 27

Dispositional Fear of Missing Out Susceptibility: Development of a Trait-Scale

Dominik Neumanna, b * & Nancy Rhodesb


a
Leibniz Institut für Wissensmedien, Everyday Media Lab, Tübingen, Germany
b
Michigan State University, Department of Advertising and Public Relations, East Lansing (MI), United States

Popular culture, advertising, marketing strategies, and academia recently paid increased attention to the experience of
Fear of Missing Out (FOMO). Yet, there is only scant research on its complex nature and the personality-based
dispositional susceptibility fostering the FOMO experience. In the present investigation, the authors present a robust
Dispositional FOMO Susceptibility (DFS) scale based on a mixed-methods approach. They show that fear of exclusion
and worries about inclusion, regret anticipation, and experiential envy comprise the dispositional susceptibility for
FOMO. In the subsequent preregistered studies, the authors experimentally test the predictive validity of this DFS scale
in the context of Instagram content viewed by users. They demonstrate how high-DFS people report higher explicitly
(self-reported) and implicitly (word fragmentation task) expressed negative and lower positive emotions after viewing
social information on Instagram. Consequently, the authors provide a novel, conceptually matured, and evolved
viewpoint on why consumers experience FOMO and what underlying personality traits drive the experience. They offer
a survey instrument to help scholars conduct fine-grained, context-independent research on FOMO.

Keywords: Fear of Missing Out, Emotions, Scale development, Social media, Instagram

When becoming aware of rewarding experiences excluded in the future (Quelette, 2019). Prior
others enjoy, consumers often struggle with a research, too, has inked FOMO to other negative
nagging feeling of being less fortunate, possibly consequences. These consequences include fear of
excluded, and even envious. These processes are evaluation (Wolniewicz et al., 2018), advertising
essential to consumer behavior as prior work fatigue (Bright & Logan, 2018), and lack of healthy
identified anticipated regret as a driver of impulse sleep (Milyavskaya et al., 2018).
purchasing (Simonson, 1992), social exclusion as a When reviewing prior literature on the
driver for positive ad and brand evaluations (Wu & phenomenon, it seems like scholarly attention
Dodoo, 2020), and envy as a motivator for purchase focused predominantly on the consequences of
decisions (Salerno et al., 2019). FOMO and, only to a lesser degree, on its
These psychological processes also describe the antecedents, such as an underlying personality trait
Fear of Missing Out (FOMO), a well-known leading to higher susceptibility to experience FOMO.
phenomenon in popular culture, business strategy, This research focused on the dispositional
and academia (Neumann, 2020). Businesses design susceptibility to experiencing FOMO as a
advertising and marketing strategies that leverage multidimensional measure of personality. Thus, we
consumers’ FOMO to motivate them to purchase developed a new measurement instrument to assess
products and services. One notable recent example the dispositional FOMO susceptibility following
was the promotion of the FYRE festival, which scale development best practices (Churchill, 1979).
leveraged FOMO to sell tickets to a non-existent, This work is essential as it answers the call for a more
fraudulent music festival (Talbot, 2019). However, nuanced understanding of the underlying drivers of
scholarly research, particularly in consumer behavior FOMO, providing a conceptual and theoretical
and marketing, examining the individual dispositions foundation for future FOMO research, and identifies
that further cognitions resulting in the experience of personality-based antecedents leading to
FOMO is still in a nascent phase. Even though the experiencing FOMO. Based on this dispositional
underlying personality traits driving FOMO are not FOMO susceptibility, we tested the scale predicting
well known, seven in ten Millennials regularly affective outcomes of social media content.
experience FOMO, half of Millennials overspent in Fear of Missing Out Across Contexts
the past to keep up with their friends, and more than
40% submit to peer pressure out of fear of being Initially, Przybylski et al. (2013, p. 1841) defined

*
Correspondence at Dominik Neumann, contact@dominikneumann.com, www.dominikneumann.com
1
the state-concept of FOMO as "a pervasive (SDT) (Ryan & Deci, 2017). Individuals experience
apprehension that others might be having rewarding FOMO when they seek to satisfy threatened
experiences from which one is absent." Prior research psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and
has conceptualized FOMO as a negative affective relatedness (Przybylski et al., 2013). FOMO triggers
experience that threatens well-being and tied it to intrinsic motivation to engage in behaviors that help
social media use (e.g., Xie et al., 2018). satisfy needs, such as seeking connectedness to peers
For instance, businesses use the FOMO and friends.
experience when promoting their products. Further, FOMO is closely linked to the Need-to-
Experiencing FOMO might impact negative and Belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Exposing high-
positive affective prospects, such as anticipated FOMO individuals to social inclusion cues may lead
elation and self-enhancement (positive affective) and to better processing of social stimuli (Lai et al.,
anticipated expense regret (negative affective). These 2016). Better processing may bring advantages when
prospects, in turn, influence consumers' purchase sustaining an inclusionary status within valued social
likelihood of experiential products (Good & Hyman, groups. Therefore, the FOMO experience constitutes
2020). In addition, news outlets report on investors' an affective reaction based on a comparative process
FOMO in the financial markets (Hardin & Bischof, because observing even strangers together may
2020) and how the experience might be a driver for exacerbate FOMO-related negative emotions
financial decision-making, especially amongst non- (Reagle, 2015). These negative emotions may
professional investors. Prior research showed that motivate consumers to change their consumption and
professional (vs. non-professional) investors showed spending patterns to uphold their inclusionary status
higher levels of FOMO (Kim et al., 2020) and that with relevant peers and friends (Duclos et al., 2012).
smartphone push notifications have a motivational Additionally, an association between
effect on investment allocations for professional interdependent self-construal and FOMO suggests
investors with high investment information FOMO that high trait-FOMO individuals attribute more
(Clor-Proell et al., 2019). FOMO might also affect importance to what others think (Dogan, 2019),
career paths. FOMO motivates people to expand their possibly affecting consumer decisions (Mandel,
horizons within and between professions (Hanlon, 2003). But, in addition to how individuals construe
2016) or increases the risk for burnout and distraction themselves based on social characteristics
because of more message-checking behavior (interdependently), FOMO might also have a
(Budnick et al., 2020). personal side grounded in the self-concept
Consumers differ in how they experience FOMO (Rosenberg, 1979). Besides the feelings of missing
and how often they engage in this FOMO experience. out associated with what others enjoy, a personal
But, little research aimed to define the personality FOMO describes how feelings of missing out are
trait of FOMO and the dimensions it comprises. This related to judgments about the private self (Zhang et
lack of understanding is problematic because our al., 2020). Product and brand choices may affect self-
digital society offers individuals much social judgments (Weiss & Johar, 2016).
information, which may negatively affect their well- Hayran et al. (2020) conceptually differentiated
being (Reer et al., 2019). Earlier studies showed that FOMO from experiences of social exclusion,
FOMO might lead to more favorable product anticipated regret, and envy. However, we argue here
evaluations in online shops after exposure to these that individuals’ susceptibility to experiencing these
products on social media (Neumann et al., 2021) cognitions forms a personality trait indicative of an
because high-FOMO individuals are more motivated individual’s susceptibility to experiencing FOMO
to process available information. Consequently, more often.
conducting a fine-grained exploration of the concept First, perceived disconnectedness, social
is vital to offer fundamental insights into how exclusion, and threats to social standings might be a
differences in dispositional FOMO susceptibility part of FOMO (Zhang et al., 2020). Lai et al. (2016),
affect individual FOMO experiences. for example, reported a positive correlation between
individuals’ trait-FOMO and the processing of social
Fear of Missing Out and its Components
stimuli. They suggested that high-FOMO individuals
Initial research on FOMO proposed that the feel more excluded when observing others being
concept is embedded in Self-Determination Theory together, similar to the experiences of social pain.
2
Holte et al. (2022) demonstrated the centrality of the these scales are problematic, according to Haws et al.
need to belong and fears of exclusion in the FOMO (accepted), as frequently, researchers fail to test
experience using the cyberball paradigm. Consumers whether adapted measurement tools are still valid.
who are dispositionally concerned about their For example, Przybylski et al. (2013) initially
inclusionary status within their peer group might be developed the most widely used FOMO scale
more likely to experience FOMO regularly. independent of a context. However, Zhang et al.
Second, the FOMO experience may come with (2020) argued that this scale mainly assesses social
anticipated regret. For example, Hodkinson (2019) media-driven FOMO as reflected in some scale
found that anticipated regret as consumers’ items. Consequently, they developed a state FOMO
deliberations might be related to the negative, scale based on an individual’s self-concept. Abel et
affective FOMO experience. He suggested that regret al. (2016) developed a trait FOMO scale that
occurs at two stages of a FOMO-laden situation, pre- examines FOMO predominantly in social media,
and post-decisional. However, as a component of the based explicitly on social media anxieties and self-
personality trait associated with FOMO, anticipated esteem. Then, Clor-Proell et al. (2019) developed and
(pre-decisional) regret might be more relevant. validated a scale that described investors' FOMO.
Third, an envious social comparison might be a This scale offers limited comparability across
cognition that plays an important role when contexts other than financial news. Good (2019)
experiencing FOMO (Reagle, 2015). FOMO is not developed a situational FOMO measure to examine
only about feeling excluded but also about others experiential product consumption.
being included instead. Prior research found an In summary, although several measurement
association between envy and problematic instruments exist, especially for state FOMO, it is
smartphone use, mediated by FOMO (Wang et al., unclear whether these constructs measure the same
2019). Thus, consumers who tend to socially experience (i.e., FOMO) since the authors often tailor
compare their opportunities and lives to their peers’ them to specific contexts. Clearly, the existing
to create value for themselves within a given social FOMO scales each have an application within a
group (Festinger, 1954) may experience FOMO more particular context. But, as state measures, they fail to
often. provide a more general understanding of individual
Consequently, this current research contrasted differences engaging more or less often in the
other research in the past (Hayran et al., 2020) by cognitive processes that lead to the negative affective
assuming that social disconnectedness, anticipated FOMO experience. Therefore, we aimed to provide
regret, and experiential envy proneness are not an instrument that captures the dispositional FOMO
conceptually distinct traits of FOMO proneness. susceptibility independent of context to address this
Instead, we suggested that the susceptibility to these lack of suitable measurement instruments currently
concepts are distinct personality-based drivers of available to researchers.
FOMO. Consumers’ proneness to experience these Prior Scales Lack Conceptual Complexity
cognitions may form a personality trait that affects
how likely they are to experience FOMO across A second central argument for a new
different situations and contexts. measurement tool was the lack of conceptual
maturity of currently available survey instruments.
Why Existing Fear of Missing Out Scales are Prior research called for a conceptual evolution of the
Problematic FOMO concept to help researchers to understand
better the underlying psychological processes
Prior Scales are Context-Dependent (Tandon et al., 2021). Although it is clear from prior
People can experience FOMO in many different literature that FOMO is a complex experience, the
situations, such as social get-togethers and events or most widely used scale of FOMO has a
financial investments. Prior research developed unidimensional structure (Przybylski et al., 2013). It
several scales to assess the experience of FOMO as a fails to capture essential FOMO components, such as
state, but these scales often depend on a context social exclusion, regret, and envy. A later scale
(overview of scales in Table 1). The context measuring social media FOMO (Abel et al., 2016)
dependency and consequent ongoing adaptation of reflected a more complex trait-like FOMO. In that

3
Table 1. Overview of existing FOMO scales
Source Items
I fear others have more rewarding experiences than me.
I fear my friends have more rewarding experiences than me.
I get worried when I find out my friends are having fun without me.
I get anxious when I don’t know what my friends are up to.
Trait FOMO It is important that I understand my friends ‘‘inside jokes’’.
Przybylski et al. (2013) Sometimes, I wonder if I spend too much time keeping up with what is going on.
It bothers me when I miss an opportunity to meet up with friends.
When I have a good time, it is important for me to share the details online.
When I miss out on a planned get-together it bothers me.
When I go on vacation, I continue to keep tabs on what my friends.

I feel anxious when I do not experience events/opportunities


I believe I am falling behind compared with others when I miss events/opportunities
I feel anxious because I know something important or fun must happen when I miss events
I feel sad if I am not capable of participating in events due to constraints of other things
Self-concept
I feel regretful of missing events/opportunities
FOMO Zhang et al. (2020)
I think my social groups view me as unimportant when I miss events/opportunities
I think I do not fit in social groups when I miss events/opportunities
I think I am excluded by my social groups when I miss events/opportunities
I feel ignored/forgotten by my social groups when I miss events/opportunities

I take a positive attitude toward myself.


On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.
I feel I have a number of good qualities.
All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure.
Trait FOMO I feel I do not have much to be proud of.
Abel et al. (2016) I feel uncomfortable meeting new people.
I am troubled by shyness.
When in a group of people, I have trouble thinking of the right things to talk about.
I feel frightened.
I feel nervous.

It bothers me when I don’t hear news about my investments until the next day
I get anxious when I don’t know what the companies I’m investing in are planning.
It is important to me that I am “on top” of the latest news for the companies I’m interested in investing in.
When I go on vacation, I continue to keep tabs on my investments.
I feel anxious if I’m “unplugged” from news about my investments.
Investment FOMO
I would prefer to be interrupted by breaking news about my investments rather than to receive the
Clor-Proell et al. (2019)
information later at a more convenient time
I get worried when I am not able to check in on my portfolio.
I would get anxious if my phone battery ran out when I was expecting news about one of my stocks.
When I miss out on industry news it bothers me.
I fear being the last to know about news that is relevant to my portfolio.

I’m afraid later I will feel sorry I didn’t go with my friends.


I will worry about what I’m missing.
I will worry my friends are doing more rewarding things than me.
I will feel concerned that my friends are having more fun without me.
State FOMO
I will feel left out.
Good (2019)
I will feel sorry that I didn’t experience an event with friends.
I will feel anxious about not being with my friends.
I will feel bothered that I missed an opportunity to be with friends.
I’m afraid later I will feel sorry I didn’t go with my friends.

4
research, Abel et al. (2016) provided initial evidence findings in a between-subjects design because our
for the multidimensional nature of FOMO. However, initial evidence resulted from a small student sample
this scale focused on social anxiety, ignoring other in a within-participants survey. We further extended
aspects, such as regret. They offered self-esteem, the results by employing measures of explicit (self-
introversion, and social-media anxiety as proposed reported) and implicit (word fragmentation task)
dimensions. Zhang et al. (2020) developed a scale affect (Study 2b). Table 2 provides an overview of all
that reflected individuals' self-concepts. Whereas samples used in this study, and we provide more
most other scales focused on comparisons between information about the data cleaning procedure in
the self and others, Zhang et al. proposed an Figure 1.
additional dimension to FOMO that is personal (and
not social). They suggested that the FOMO Figure 1. Cleaning procedures
experience best represented a self-concept
maintenance construct based on individuals’
motivations but failed to account for their everyday
experiences, emotions, and cognitions.
Some of these scales provide a more nuanced view
of FOMO than the original, unidimensional scale
(Przybylski et al., 2013). However, these scales are
also too context-confined (Abel et al., 2016) or too
narrow in scope (Zhang et al., 2020), not allowing
scholars to examine what complex cognitive
dispositions lead to the FOMO experience. Further,
conceptually none of the previously developed scales
reflect personality traits that lead to experiencing
FOMO more often and account for essential
cognitions closely connected to FOMO. These scales
failed to help understand what differences in
individuals may result in variation or similarity of
FOMO experiences.

Overview of Present Research

Our goal was to develop the Dispositional FOMO


Susceptibility (DFS) scale. This multidimensional
measurement instrument reflects a complex
personality trait responsible for experiencing
negative affective FOMO more often. The DFS scale
allows researchers to study FOMO independent of
context by representing a multidimensional
personality trait. Thus, we used a mixed-methods
scale development approach (Study 1).
Additionally, we tested the scale’s predictive Ethics Approval, Openness, and Transparency
validity by examining how the DFS scale was At [omitted for blind review], the Institutional
associated with the affective nature of the FOMO Review Board approved all studies. We provide the
experience in social media (Study 2). Prior research interview guide, survey instruments, data, and scripts
suggested that FOMO leads to decreased positive for analyses on our Open Science Framework (OSF)
affect and increased negative affect (e.g., websites (see links below). We offer additional study
Milyavskaya et al., 2018). Thus, we examined how materials on these OSF websites and in the
individuals with different scores on the DFS scale are Supplemental Materials of this research article. We
affected positively or negatively based on social report all manipulations, measures, and exclusions in
media content (Study 2a). We replicated these these studies. Qualitative interview data is excluded
5
Table 2. Demographic composition of samples in the present research.
Sample A Sample B Sample C Sample D
(Dynata) (Prolific) (Students) (Prolific)
Recruited N 1,161 275 101 261
Valid N 731 248 90 244

Age Median (Range) 39 [18, 87] 30 [18, 80] 20 [18, 35] 29 [18, 50]
Gender (% female) 52.7 52.8 71.1 59.0

Race and Ethnicity(%)


White or Caucasian 75.4 54.0 66.7 61.9
Black or African American 8.6 6.0 11.1 10.2
Asian 6.0 25.4 15.6 12.3
Hispanic 6.0 9.7 3.3 9.0
Mixed 1.5 2.4 1.1 5.7
Other 1.4 1.6 2.2 0.8

Household income (%)


Less than $10,000 9.2 6.0 4.4 8.2
$10,000 to $49,999 34.3 30.6 13.3 32
$50,000 to $99,999 32.0 38.7 25.6 34.4
$100,000 to $149,999 13.1 16.9 23.3 16.8
$150,000 or more 10.5 7.3 33.3 8.2

Highest education (%)


Less than high school 1.9 0.4 - 1.6
High school graduate 23.0 15.3 - 13.5
Some college, no degree 20.9 22.2 - 30.7
Associate degree 11.4 7.7 - 9.4
Bachelor's degree 25.9 35.9 - 29.5
Master's degree 11.6 14.5 - 12.3
Professional degree 3.1 2.0 - 2.0
Doctoral degree 1.6 2.0 - 0.8

NOTE. – Sample A: Study 1; Sample B: Study 1; Sample C: Study 2a; Sample D: Study 1 and Study 2b; Valid N shows the number of participants
included in statistical analyses after excluding participants based on quality concerns (more details about cleaning procedure in Web Appendix
B); all percentages computed based on Valid N

from this shared repository to protect participants' phenomenological approach to scale development
anonymity and privacy. We (partially) preregistered was appropriate because it reflected a shared
all studies, including hypotheses, study design, meaning of FOMO grounded in individual
sample size, exclusion criteria, and planned analyses. perspectives (Arksey & Knight, 1999; DeVellis,
We computed all analyses using R (v 4.0.3). 2017). Thus, we conducted 27 semi-structured, in-
depth interviews between December 2018 and
Study 1: The Dispositional Fear of Missing Out August 2019 and compensated interviewees $10 to
Susceptibility Scale participate in a study titled "When do you feel you
miss out?". We asked participants to describe the
Item Generation FOMO experience across ten contexts (e.g., brands
This research (OSF: www.bit.ly/3JwstHC) aimed and products, financial investments, social get-
to develop an instrument measuring individuals' togethers, and career opportunities). The first author
dispositional FOMO susceptibility. We used an screened all transcribed interviews and extracted
exploratory approach to the scale development statements that mirrored the FOMO experience. We
process because the theoretical grounds of the rephrased the extracted statements to fit the trait scale
affective and cognitive FOMO experience have instructions: "Generally speaking, how often do you
evolved (Tandon et al., 2021). We developed a experience each of the following statements or
comprehensive pool of items describing the FOMO emotions." The resulting initial item pool included
experience and allowed the item generation process 235 items.
to be open to new emerging patterns. The
6
To ensure the face validity of the initial item pool, appropriate. We retained three factors after
we consulted an expert panel (Churchill, 1979; Haws consulting the parallel analysis results (extract five
et al., accepted) from academic research in social factors) and an initial EFA (items loaded on three of
psychology, communication, and media. These five factors), which also matched our prior
experts built academic careers on social norms, theorizing. A series of EFA showed evidence for
cognitive processing, attitude accessibility, and these three factors explaining 52% of the variance.
media effects and processes. We asked them to According to our preregistered analysis plan,
review the statements extracted by the first author. throughout four iterations of item-exclusion and re-
Before the review procedure, we discussed prior submitting to follow-up EFA, we excluded 29 items
conceptual definitions and research on FOMO in from further analyses because they showed cross-
face-to-face conversations. We retained those items loadings or small factor loadings (λ < .50). We
marked as relevant to FOMO by at least two experts. ensured that items were not highly correlated (-.50 <=
We copy-edited items and eliminated one item due to r <= .50) with participants’ social desirability bias
close similarity to another item. The resulting pool of (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972). Results indicated
items consisted of 79 items (reviewed item pool acceptable average correlations of r = -.26, so no
available in Supplemental Material B). items were excluded due to social desirability
(Cohen, 1988). Last, an additional 12 items were
Initial Exploratory Factor Analysis
excluded based on redundancies (e.g., "you think
Method people in an in-group are happier than you") or lack
This preregistered study (www.bit.ly/3dpAr7h) of specificity (e.g., "you want something").
aimed at reducing the initial pool of items to create As proposed a priori, the structure of the DFS
the first draft of the DFS scale (OSF: scale yielded three factors that described social
www.bit.ly/3vaxD70). We recruited participants in disconnectedness (e.g., "you worry that friends will
an online survey for exploratory factor analysis not invite you in the future"), regret anticipation
(EFA) in October 2019 from the general U.S. (e.g., "you think of all the things you wish you would
population using the Dynata sampling service have done differently"), and experiential envy (e.g.,
(Sample A, N = 731). We compensated participants "you want to have the same fun other people are
according to their panel membership and the Dynata having"). Thus, our analyses initially indicated—
compensation scheme and asked them to respond to contrasting Hayran et al. (2020)—that social
all 79 items of the reviewed item pool. However, exclusion, regret, and envy are not necessarily
after completing the data collection for Sample A, we conceptually different from FOMO. Instead,
inspected the item pool of 79 items again and proneness to these experiences may make it more
excluded five items that reflected romantic regrets likely to experience FOMO.
(e.g., "You wonder if you maybe have missed the However, 33 items still described these factors
opportunity to be with your soul mate"). We after our initial scale purifying efforts. We
submitted data for 74 items to parallel analysis (Horn, considered it problematic to retain that many items,
1965) to determine the number of factors to extract in as a large number of items within single factors might
consequent EFA using unrotated Eigenvalues and yield alpha inflation (DeVellis, 2017). Thus, we
5,000 bootstrapping intervals. After selecting the recruited another sample to reduce the number of
number of factors to extract (Ford et al., 1986; Kline, items included in the final scale.
2013), we submitted data to EFA using maximum Further Item Reduction and Scale Dimensionality
likelihood estimation and Promax rotation
Method
(Churchill, 1979). We excluded items with cross-
loadings or factor loadings < .50. This preregistered study (www.bit.ly/3dpAr7h)
aimed at reducing the number of items on the DFS
Results and Discussion
scale (OSF: www.bit.ly/38yVFkg). We recruited
Results of Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (p < .01) participants from the international opt-in
and Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin value (.99) exceeded crowdsourcing panel Prolific (Sample B, N = 248).
recommendations (.60) (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007), We compensated them with an average of £7.24/hr.
indicating that submitting data to EFA was We defined panel members as eligible when they
7
Table 3. Factor loadings for first- and second-order exploratory factor analyses.
Social Regret Experiential Fear of Threats to
disconnectedness anticipation envy exclusion inclusion
You think you will get pushed away for possibly being absent from something. .82 .51
You worry that friends will not invite you in the future. .77 .63
You think you are missing out on fun activities because you are not part of the "in-group." .70 .67
You think your friends like each other better than they like you because you are not spending time with them. .77 1.00
You think there might be negative effects on your social relationships when you are not part of a shared activity. .76 .47
You think something cool might happen in your absence, and you are afraid you won't be able to talk about it. .78 .95
You are afraid you will not be able to talk about events you have not been invited to. .82 .61
You just want to be a part of something because everyone else is a part of it. .61 .81
You think you are missing out on bonding with friends by not owning the same things they do. .74 .59
You do not want to miss out on things others are doing, even though you are not really interested in it. .62 .57

You think you missed your "one opportunity." .65


You think about potential failure in what you are doing. .74
You think of all the things you wish you would have done differently. .74
You are worrying about "what could have been." .74
You experience uncertainty. .76

You wish you could have been with your friends when they were having fun. .50
You want to have the same fun other people are having. .76
You think other people are doing really cool things. .66
You want to have access to opportunities others are having. .57

Proportion Variance explained (%) .30 .15 .10 .25 .29


Cumulative Variance explained (%) .30 .45 .55 .25 .54
Cronbach’s alpha .94 .86 .80 .90 .88
Average variance extracted .59 .55 .50 .64 .61

NOTE. – Values based on data from Sample A

8
reported their country of residence to be the U.S., samples A, B, and D (more details on Sample D, see
were fluent in English and had an approval rating of below) to confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) to
prior studies of > 95% on Prolific. We asked examine the hierarchical four-factor solution.
participants to respond to the 33-item DFS scale in an Method
online study. We submitted data for the DFS scale to
EFA. To develop a parsimonious scale, we decided a We used common goodness-of-fit indices
priori to exclude items when dropping them led to an (RMSEA < 0.08, SRMR < 0.08, TLI > 0.95, CFI >
increase or no change in Cronbach's alpha of the 0.95, χ2/df-ratio < 3) to assess the final DFS scale and
respective component. To reduce the number of factor structure (DeVellis, 2017; Homburg et al.,
items in the two largest factors, we subjected the 2015; Kline, 2013). We determined the model fit of
items of the respective large factor to an additional the hierarchical four-factor solution by comparing
EFA (DeVellis, 2017). We examined reliabilities the final model to a baseline model, a unidimensional
using Cronbach’s alpha > .70, composite reliabilities model, and a three-factor model. The latter model
> .70, and average variance extracted (AVE) > .50 combined the fear of social exclusion and worries
within Sample A and B. about inclusion as a single social disconnectedness
factor.
Results and Discussion
Results and Discussion
Again, the resulting factor structure in this study
was consistent with our a priori explication of the Based on all three samples, the CFA provided
dispositional FOMO susceptibility dimensions and evidence for the hierarchical four-factor model
our earlier findings in Sample A: (1) social specification, which showed excellent model fit
disconnectedness, (2) regret anticipation, and (3) based on χ2/df, CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR (Table
experiential envy. Only six items did not load on any 4). The hierarchical four-factor solution performed
of these factors, and we excluded them. Two factors consistently better than other model specifications
remained large: social disconnectedness with 14 across samples. These results show that the
items and regret anticipation with nine items. We hierarchical structure best described the data in three
examined a potential hierarchical structure by independent samples. We consider this strong
submitting these factors to additional EFAs. Within evidence for the hierarchical factor structure of the
the second-order factor of social disconnectedness, DFS scale.
we found two first-order factors: (1) fear of exclusion The Nomological Net
(e.g., "you think you will get pushed away for
possibly being absent from something") and (2) We examined the DFS scale within its
worries about inclusion (e.g., "you are afraid you will nomological net. As the DFS scale represents a trait
not be able to talk about events you have not been assessment of individuals’ dispositional FOMO, we
invited to"). Five items described these two first- expected that the DFS scale positively correlated
order factors. We reduced regret anticipation to a with other FOMO measures developed by Przybylski
single first-order factor comprised of five items. et al. (2013) and Abel et al. (2016). Given the
Table 3 displays factor loadings of first- and second- negative affective nature of the FOMO experience,
order factors. Thus, a partially nested hierarchical we expected that individuals high in measures of
four-factor solution with 19 items reflected the final negative trait affect would also score higher on the
DFS scale. DFS scale (Milyavskaya et al., 2018). Because the
DFS scale represented experiential envy and
Structural Validity anticipated regret as sub-dimensions, we expected
This partially preregistered study positive correlations with social comparison
(www.bit.ly/3dpAr7h) provided evidence for the processes and individuals' tendencies to appraise
DFS scale's structural validity (OSF: counterfactuals, which are related constructs
www.bit.ly/3Kz7V2x). We cross-validated our (Neumann, 2020). To reflect on prior theorizing, we
findings with an independent sample among the expected our scale would positively associate with
general population (Brakus et al., 2009) and followed interdependent self-construal, as shown by Dogan
prior research to assess our scale’s goodness of fit (2019), and negatively with need satisfaction (e.g.,
(Homburg et al., 2015). We submitted data from relatedness), as shown by Przybylski et al. (2013).
9
Table 4. Assessing model fit.
χ2 Goodness-of-fit
Δχ2 p-value
Value df p χ2/df-ratio RMSEA SRMR TLI CFI
Sample A (N = 731)
Hierarchical model 345.9 148 < .001 2.34 .04 .03 .98 .98
Null model 9406.6 171 < .001 55.01 .27 .50 9,060.7 < .001
Unidimensional 1244.2 152 < .001 8.19 .10 .06 .87 .88 898.3 < .001
Three factors 433.5 149 < .001 2.91 .05 .03 .97 .97 87.7 < .001

Sample B (N = 248)
Hierarchical model 301.5 148 < .001 2.04 .07 .05 .94 .94
Null model 2924.5 171 < .001 17.10 .26 .43 2,622.9 < .001
Unidimensional 781.3 152 < .001 5.14 .13 .10 .74 .77 479.8 < .001
Three factors 341.9 149 < .001 2.30 .07 .06 .92 .93 40.4 < .001

Sample D (N = 244)
Hierarchical model 332.1 148 < .001 2.24 .07 .06 .93 .94
Null model 3189.2 171 < .001 18.65 .27 .47 2,857.1 < .001
Unidimensional 755.2 152 < .001 4.97 .13 .08 .78 .80 423.1 < .001
Three factors 405.7 149 < .001 2.72 .08 .06 .90 .92 73.6 < .001

Based on prior research on the emotional H2. The DFS scale will be negatively correlated
character of FOMO, we expected that people who with satisfaction with the needs for (a) autonomy,
score highly on the DFS scale should also score (b) competence, (c) relatedness, and (d) risk
highly on trait anxiety (Hayran et al., 2020). High aversion.
trait-FOMO individuals were more neurotic and
anxious than low trait-FOMO individuals H3. The DFS scale will be uncorrelated with (a)
(Rozgonjuk et al., 2021). Based on findings by behavioral activation drive, (b) behavioral
Riordan et al. (2019) and Popovac and Hadlington activation reward responsiveness, and (c)
(2020), we expected high DFS participants also to be independent self-construal.
less risk-averse. Additionally, we expected a
complex relationship between the DFS scale and the RQ1. How are the individual components of the
behavioral inhibition (BIS), activation (BAS), and DFS scale correlated with trait anxiety,
fight-flight-freeze (FFFS) system sensitivities based interdependent and independent self-construal,
on the revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory behavioral activation, behavioral inhibition, basic
(rRST) (Gray & McNaughton, 2000). Because high- need satisfaction, and risk aversion?
FOMO individuals have a higher drive for more
rewarding social experiences (Riordan et al., 2019), Method
those people may also score high on BAS (fun- We preregistered (http://www.bit.ly/3dpAr7h) the
seeking). But, as they also tend to be more neurotic research design and hypotheses for the variables in
and anxious, they may further score highly on BIS. Sample B (OSF: https://bit.ly/37BzSIi). We
We expected no correlational relationships between submitted data (Sample A and B) for the DFS scale,
BAS (drive), BAS (reward responsiveness), and its sub-scales, and all variables of interest to (partial)
independent self-construal. Finally, we explored the Pearson correlations, using pairwise-complete
correlations of each DFS sub-dimension with the observations and Holm's adjustment for p-values.
measures above to investigate differences in the Deviating from our preregistration, we computed
dimensions of the DFS scale. partial correlations when looking at the DFS sub-
scales, controlling for the other sub-scales. We
H1. The DFS scale will be positively correlated considered this more appropriate to capture the
with but discriminate from (a) prior FOMO unique variance explained by each scale. We looked
scales, (b) trait-anxiety, (c) interdependent self- at the square root of average variance extracted
construal, (d) behavioral activation fun-seeking, (√AVE) of the DFS scale and compared it to the
and (e) behavioral inhibition. construct's correlations with each other. We
established discriminant validity when a factor's
10
Table 5. (Partial) Pearson correlations between the DFS scale and related variables.
DFS Fear of Exclusion Threat to Inclusion Regret Anticipation Experiential Envy
M SD α r P adj. rpartial P adj. rpartial P adj. rpartial P adj. rpartial P adj.
SAMPLE A (N = 731) α = .95, √AVE = .72 α = .91, √AVE = .81 α = .90, √AVE = .81 α = .87, √AVE = .76 α = .83, √AVE = .74
FOMO (Przybyslski et at.) 3.07 1.48 .93 .77 <.001 .21 <.001 .34 <.001 .10 .030 .05 .366
FOMO (Abel et al.) 3.24 1.29 .89 .64 <.001 .22 <.001 .01 .711 .49 <.001 –.19 <.001
Self-esteem 3.06 1.31 .81 .50 <.001 .15 <.0001 .00 .950 .44 <.001 –.20 <.001
Introversion 3.58 1.73 .85 .57 <.001 .20 <.001 –.01 .852 .33 <.001 –.07 .127
Social anxiety 3.15 1.70 .81 .60 <.001 .17 <.001 .06 .167 .40 <.001 –.15 <.001
Positive trait-affect 3.23 0.83 .90 –.04 .251 –.02 .516 .06 .209 –.28 <.001 .19 <.001
Negative trait-affect 2.16 0.95 .94 .67 <.001 .11 .010 .11 .010 .45 <.001 –.07 .087
Social comparison orientation 3.07 0.77 .85 .62 <.001 .09 .049 .11 .011 .05 .149 .27 <.001
Opinion-focused 3.33 0.86 .77 .44 <.001 .04 .907 .03 .907 –.01 .907 .29 <.001
Ability-focused 2.89 0.86 .80 .63 <.001 .10 .015 .15 <.001 .09 .040 .19 <.001
Counterfactual thinking 2.82 0.80 .92 .74 <.001 .01 1.000 .16 <.001 .40 <.001 .23 .000
None-referent down 3.05 0.91 .82 .43 <.001 –.02 .546 .09 .049 .07 .102 .21 <.001
Other-referent down 2.70 1.01 .87 .62 <.001 .01 .733 .17 <.001 .23 <.001 .14 .001
Self-referent up 2.74 0.94 .82 .69 <.001 .03 .779 .12 .005 .40 <.001 .15 <.001
None-referent up 2.80 0.97 .85 .70 <.001 .03 .510 .07 .235 .49 <.001 .16 <.001

SAMPLE B (N = 248) α = .93, √AVE = .66 α = .90, √AVE = .80 α = .88, √AVE = .78 α = .86, √AVE = .74 α = .80, √AVE = .71
Risk aversion 4.67 0.97 .81 .31 <.001 .10 .324 –.04 .577 .36 <.001 –.11 .324
Trait-anxiety 2.33 0.72 .96 .60 <.001 .07 .557 .00 .972 .73 <.001 –.09 .557
FFFS 8.84 1.83 .65 .37 <.001 .01 1.000 –.04 1.000 .41 <.001 .05 1.000
BIS 12.35 2.29 .77 .52 <.001 .18 .020 –.14 .103 .35 <.001 .19 .013
BAS 36.96 4.79 .79 .09 .477 –.13 .183 .07 .299 –.10 .275 .27 .000
Fun-seeking 10.62 2.09 .67 .12 .207 –.11 .171 .09 .171 –.15 .114 .28 <.001
Drive 10.19 2.17 .73 .00 .980 –.03 .839 .08 .711 –.12 .262 .05 .839
Reward responsiveness 16.14 2.13 .73 .08 .477 –.15 .086 –.02 .796 .06 .708 .27 <.001
Independent construal 4.53 1.09 .73 –.38 <.001 –.16 .081 –.03 1.000 –.11 .352 .00 1.000
Interdependent construal 3.87 0.95 .62 .37 <.001 .09 .481 .14 .152 .02 1.000 .01 1.000
Need satisfaction 4.91 0.95 .89 –.53 <.001 –.17 .044 –.01 .932 –.57 <.001 .20 .010
Autonomy 5.03 1.27 .76 –.52 <.001 –.08 .624 –.08 .624 –.44 <.001 .06 .624
Competence 4.55 1.16 .78 –.50 <.001 –.18 .027 .10 .226 –.61 <.001 .15 .094
Relatedness 5.16 0.98 .81 –.37 <.001 –.12 .140 –.05 .393 –.38 <.001 .22 .003
NOTE. – ‘FFFS’ = fight-flight-freeze system, ‘BIS’ = behavioral inhibition system, ‘BAS’ = behavioral activation system; a total correlation computed using Pearson correlations for pairwise complete observations
(Holm’s adjustment); b partial correlations for each factor calculated by controlling for the respective other two factors (Holm’s adjustment)

11
√AVE exceeded its correlations with the other participants' need satisfaction. In contrast to our prior
variables (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). We deviated review, we found only a weak, non-significant
from our preregistration by computing partial correlation of the DFS scale with fun-seeking instead
correlations of each FOMO measure (DFS and of a positive correlation and a negative instead of
FOMO by Przybylski et al. (2013) and Abel et al. non-significant correlation with independent self-
(2015)) with relevant correlates (i.e., positive and construal. Thus, FOMO seems more dependent on
negative trait-affect, social comparison orientation, others and less appetitive than initially anticipated.
and counterfactual thinking), controlling for the We also found a positive instead of a negative
respective other two FOMO measures. Thus, we correlation between the DFS scale and risk aversion.
exemplified how the DFS scale is superior to Although surprising at first, this finding aligns with
previously proposed FOMO measures by better the DFS scale’s high correlation with behavioral
predicting relevant correlations. inhibition, reflecting a form of situational anxiety
when anticipating punishment or no rewards (Carver
Measures
& White, 1994). Thus, FOMO might be a
In addition to the DFS scale, in Sample A, we behaviorally inhibiting but cognitively engaging
included other FOMO measures (Abel et al., 2016; experience.
Przybylski et al., 2013), positive and negative trait-
affect (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988), social Table 6. Partial Pearson correlations between
comparison orientation (Gibbons & Buunk, 1999), FOMO scales and related variables.
and counterfactual thinking for negative events (Rye
DFS FOMOa FOMO b
et al., 2008). In Sample B, we included trait-anxiety Positive trait-affect .09* .21*** –.49***
(Spielberger, 1983), risk aversion (Mandrik & Bao, Negative trait-affect .30*** .03 .48***
2004), interdependent and independent self-construal
SCO .40*** .14*** –.13***
(D’Amico & Scrima, 2016), FFFS, BAS, and BIS Opinion-focused .33*** .09* –.23***
(Carver & White, 1994), and basic need satisfaction Ability-focused .37*** .14*** –.02
(Johnston & Finney, 2010).
Counterfactual thinking .52*** .02 .04
Results and Discussion None-referent down .32*** .03 –.15***
Other-referent down .38*** .07 .03
Table 5 summarizes the results of (partial) Self-referent up .47*** –.01 .09*
correlations. We found a high correlation between None-referent up .47*** –.04 .20***
the DFS scale and all three Abel et al. (2016) scale NOTE. – a Przybylski et al. (2013), b Abel, Buff, and Burr (2015).
components. We found a strong correlation between SCO = social comparison orientation. Partial correlations of each
the DFS scale and Przybylski et al. (2013) scale, but FOMO measure controlling for the other two FOMO measures,
predicting personality traits related to FOMO (Holm’s adjusted).
the √AVE of the DFS scale was lower than its
correlation. We submitted data (Sample A) for both
Table 6 summarizes our results concerning the
scales to CFA, similar to approaches in prior research
partial correlations with the DFS and FOMO scales
(Homburg et al., 2015). We specified a
developed in the past (Abel et al., 2016; Przybylski
unidimensional model, with all items for both scales
et al., 2013) with relevant correlates of FOMO. Our
loading on a single factor, and a two-dimensional
results provide evidence for the predictive validity of
model with items of each FOMO measure loading on
our scale above and beyond other FOMO measures.
a different factor. Results indicated that the two-
The scale developed by Abel et al. (2016) represented
factor solution provides a better model fit (AIC Δ = -
increased negative and decreased positive affect, was
61). These results provided evidence for the
unrelated to counterfactual thinking, and negatively
convergent and discriminant validity (Haws et al.,
related to social comparison orientation. Thus,
accepted) of the DFS scale with the FOMO scale
although prior research developed this scale initially
developed by Przybylski et al. (2013).
to capture FOMO, this measure reflected only the
Aligning with our expectations, we found strong
affective aspect of FOMO. The scale developed by
positive correlations of the DFS scale with negative
Przybylski et al. (2013) focused on the social
trait affect, social comparison orientation,
comparison aspect of FOMO while unable to capture
counterfactual thinking, trait anxiety, and BIS. In
additional variance in counterfactual thinking.
addition, we found strong negative correlations with
12
Surprisingly, this scale also was positively related prior research showed, these two higher-order factors
to positive trait-affect and did not account for any to social disconnectedness may affect consumer
additional variance of negative trait-affect. The DFS behavior by inducing feelings of social exclusion,
scale captured correlational variance above and possibly leading to brand and product switching
beyond the other concepts in negative trait-affect, behavior (Su et al., 2016).
social comparison, and counterfactual thinking. We define regret anticipation as susceptibility to
Thus, the DFS scale reflected all aspects of rumination, counterfactual thinking, and worrying. A
participants’ FOMO even when controlling for other large body of past consumer behavior research has
FOMO measures. Therefore, these other FOMO investigated the effects of anticipated regret on
measures only captured some aspects of FOMO purchase decisions (e.g., Tsiros & Mittal, 2000).
without providing a complete picture. Some work showed that these reflections on future
We explored the partial correlations of each factor regret are potent drivers of spontaneous purchases
of the DFS scale with the personality variables of (Walchli & Landman, 2003). Our DFS scale accounts
interest to elucidate potential differences between the for consumers’ susceptibility to these feelings before,
distinct factors. Experiential envy represented a during, and after purchase decisions. Experiential
conceptually different aspect of what led to FOMO. envy is the proneness to an upward comparative
Specifically, we found a positive correlation between process between one’s experiences and the
experiential envy and positive trait-affect, BAS fun- experiences of others. Envy is an inherently social
seeking, BAS reward responsiveness, and comparison process, which prior research identified
satisfaction with relatedness when controlling for the as a motivational driver for purchase decisions
other factors. These correlations stand diametrically (Salerno et al., 2019). The comparative nature of
opposed to the correlations of the other factors. They FOMO is a known feature of the experience
indicate two general motivational drives comprising (Milyavskaya et al., 2018), and early consumer
the antecedents of FOMO: approach (experiential research has identified social comparison processes
envy) and avoidance (fear of exclusion, worries as essential for consumer behavior (Moschis, 1976).
about inclusion, and regret anticipation). Developing the DFS scale was to explicate a
Individuals who score high on the DFS scale may theoretically robust and evolved conceptual
want to live eventful lives, similar to their peers understanding of character traits that lead to the
(experiential envy). Still, the fear of social complex FOMO phenomenon. By showing that our
consequences for missing out on these experiences four-dimensional hierarchical DFS scale correlated
(fear of exclusion and worries about inclusion) may highly with other state and trait FOMO measures but
plague them. They mentally predict possible adverse predicted relevant correlates above and beyond these
outcomes when deciding about these exciting measures, we offer a research tool that helps to
experiences (regret anticipation). Our scale captures conduct detailed process-oriented investigations of
people’s dispositions to the conflicting cognitions consumers’ FOMO susceptibility. Though the DFS
that may lead to these motivational tensions while scale highly correlated with existing measures of
other measurement instruments do not. FOMO, here we explicated a granular personality
measure of individuals' likelihood to experience
Summary of the Scale Development Process
FOMO. We showed that the DFS scale and its sub-
Using a mixed-methods approach, we developed dimensions were associated with relevant correlates
a 19-item trait scale for individuals’ dispositional of FOMO above and beyond other scales, aiding in
FOMO susceptibility (more information about the its conceptual development yet assessing the same
final scale, such as means and standard deviations, in general phenomenon (Tandon et al., 2021). Thus, in
Supplemental Material C). In the context of the DFS the following, we used the newly developed DFS
scale, we defined the fear of exclusion as individuals’ scale to measure consumers’ sensitivity to
dispositional fear of being actively excluded by their experiencing FOMO to investigate the role of social
peers because of missing out on shared experiences. media content on subjective consumer well-being
We defined worries about inclusion as individuals’ (affective outcomes).
continued concerns about maintaining their current
inclusionary status within a peer group because
missing out on events leads to no future invites. As
13
Study 2a: FOMO, Social Media, and Affect event, they may anticipate regret missing a good time
(Hayran et al., 2020). When they miss purchasing an
We examined the effects of DFS on the emotional in-style branded product, they anticipate regret of
outcomes of FOMO-relevant social media content. missed reputational gain.
Mainly, we focused on Instagram as the platform has
recently grown in popularity. The platform also H5. Higher scores for anticipated regret will be
became a research focus due to its effects on purchase associated with more negative and less positive
behavior (Djafarova & Bowes, 2021) and the affect for social (vs. control) and branded
detrimental effects on mental health due to social products (vs. control) social media stimuli. There
comparison materials (Faelens et al., 2021). We will be no difference in negative and positive
deemed this important as affect serves as an affect for social stimuli compared to branded
information system when consumers make purchase products stimuli.
decisions (Chang & Tuan Pham, 2013).
A large body of research shows that FOMO and Last, we expected that individuals prone to
social media use negatively affect consumers experiential envy might experience more negative
(Milyavskaya et al., 2018). Thus, to test the and more positive affect when exposed to social
predictive validity of the DFS scale, we contrasted content on social media as compared to control
social media contents that were either explicitly (e.g., content. This expectation was guided by our previous
social get-togethers) or implicitly (e.g., brand findings when examining the DFS scale’s
products) social with control content (e.g., unbranded nomological net. As envy can be benign and
kitchen utensils). We aimed to show that the malicious (Lange & Crusius, 2014; Salerno et al.,
underlying personality traits of dispositional FOMO 2019), we expected both mechanisms to be active in
would predict higher negative and lesser positive FOMO proneness, leading to positive and negative
affect. We expected that the DFS sub-scales would affect.
differentially predict emotional outcomes relevant to
FOMO. The feeling of belonging and the stress H6. Higher scores for experiential envy will be
induced by a lack of that feeling have an inherently associated with more negative and positive affect
social nature (Baumeister et al., 2007; Baumeister & for social (vs. control) and branded products (vs.
Leary, 1995). We predicted that for high (vs. low) control) social media stimuli. There will be no
DFS individuals, exposure to social Instagram difference in negative and positive affect due to
content would lead to more negative and less positive social stimuli compared to branded products
affect than control content. We expected this effect to stimuli.
be most potent in explicit expressions of sociality and
significant in implicit expressions than in control Method
content. Procedure

H4. Higher scores for social disconnectedness are We recruited participants (Sample C, N = 90) from
associated with more negative and less positive a public university student pool in March 2021 for
affect for social (vs. branded products and vs. this preregistered study (www.bit.ly/3sJku1g; OSF:
control) and branded products (vs. control) social www.bit.ly/3v9Fz8t). We screened participants for
media stimuli. having an active Instagram account and compensated
them with course credit. They answered the DFS
Like the effects of dispositional feelings of scale and completed the Positive and Negative Affect
disconnectedness, we expected that individuals Scale (PANAS, Watson et al., 1988). We asked them
susceptible to anticipating regret would experience to imagine a short scenario in which they evaluated
more negative and less positive affect when exposed twelve mock-up Instagram posts in block-
to social content (Roese & Summerville, 2005). randomized order. We determined a priori that 100
However, we expected explicit and implicit participants should suffice based on the within-
expressions of sociality would not differ because participants design.
both represent decisions that could have lasting
effects. For example, when individuals miss a social
14
Research Design and Dependent Variables sub-scales and the post category. Additional to our
preregistered analyses, we grand-mean-centered all
We employed a three (post category: social vs.
continuous independent variables.
branded products vs. control) by four (examples
Our findings (Table 7) partially aligned with our
within a category) within-participants design. We
expectations and with results from Study 1. Partially
asked participants to imagine seeing the twelve
aligning with H4, disponsitional social
following posts on Instagram and instructed them to
disconnectedness was associated with greater
imagine that the person posting the images was one
negative affect (social vs. control: b = 0.23, 95% CI
of their friends. Four individual mock-up posts
[0.12; 0.34], p < .001; branded vs. control: b = 0.10,
formed one of three stimulus blocks, respectively: (1)
95% CI [-0.00; 0.21], p = .056) and not related to
social situations, (2) branded products, and (3)
positive affect. The standardized effect of social
kitchen utensils (control). Within each block, the
media content on negative affect was larger for social
information provided in the posts was the same.
(vs. control) than for branded products (vs. control)
Across all blocks, the number of likes, profile
posts. In our prior study, proneness for regret
pictures, and the person's name posted on the image
anticipation correlated positively with negative trait-
was constant. We used single-item scales (1 = “not at
affect and negatively with positive trait-affect (Study
all” to 10 = “very”) for positive and negative affect
1). Similarly, and in line with H5, here we found
as dependent variables and the likelihood to
greater negative affect (b = 0.11, 95% CI [-0.00;
experience FOMO as a manipulation-check. Single-
0.23], p = .060) and less positive affect (b = -0.14,
item measures were appropriate as they respect
95% CI [-0.25; -0.03], p = .014) as a result of social
participants’ time in a repeated measures design
(vs. control) content for increasing reported levels of
(Allen et al., 2022). Prior research identified them as
regret anticipation. Thus, individuals high in
not inferior to multi-item scales (Bergkvist &
dispositions for regret anticipation are generally
Rossiter, 2007).
feeling less positive and more negative (results from
Results and Discussion Study 1) and are emotionally affected more severely
Manipulation-Check by situational stimuli (results from this study)
However, the findings of this current study also
Participants were significantly more likely to differed from the findings of our earlier research. In
experience FOMO when seeing social posts vs. Study 1, susceptibility to experiential envy was
branded products posts (Msocial = 5.37, Mbranded = associated with greater positive trait affect and
2.02, t(359) = 22.3, p < .001, d = 1.18) and vs. control marginally lower negative trait affect. In contrast to
posts (Mcontrol = 1.61, t(359) = 23.3, p < .001, d = these findings and H6, here, experiential envy was
1.23). Participants were slightly more likely to associated with lower positive affect (b = -0.19, 95%
experience FOMO when seeing branded products vs. CI [-0.31; -0.06], p = .005) for social (vs. control)
control posts (t(359) = 4.8, p < .001, d = 0.25). content. Individuals high in trait experiential envy
Multilevel Regression Analyses generally report more subjective well-being (or
positive trait-affect, according to Study 1). But, there
We submitted the data to a series of eight are adverse situational effects when observing social
multilevel regression analyses with positive and media posts that picture other people having
negative affect as dependent variables. We included enjoyable experiences. Thus, individuals who score
the general DFS scale (α = .95), and its sub-scales high in experiential envy seem to feel more positive
social disconnectedness (α = .94), regret anticipation (Study 1) but are severely affected by being absent
(α = .85), and experiential envy (α = .79) as the from rewarding experiences (results from this study).
independent variables. Deviating from our Overall, we found that social content induced
preregistration, we added positive (α = .91) and marginally lower positive affect (b = -0.10, 95% CI
negative (α = .90) pre-experimental mood as control [-0.23; 0.02], p = .099) and significantly higher
variables; results did not differ when adding this negative affect (b = 0.22, 95% CI [0.10; 0.35], p <
control variable. Observations were nested within .001) with higher levels of DFS as compared to
individuals, and we standardized all continuous control content. Branded products had no effect on
dependent variables before submitting them to positive (b = -0.02, n.s.), and only marginal effects
analyses. We added interactions between the DFS on negative affect (b = 0.11, 95% CI [-0.02; 0.24],
15
Table 7. Effects on positive and negative affect by users' DFS score.
DFS – Social DFS – Regret DFS – Experiential
DFS – overall a
disconnectedness a anticipation a envy a
Positive Negative Positive Negative Positive Negative Positive Negative
affect b affect b affect b affect b affect b affect b affect b affect b
(Intercept) –0.29** –0.26** –0.29** –0.26** –0.29** –0.26** –0.29** –0.26**
DFS c –0.17 0.09 –0.17+ 0.09 –0.12 0.07 –0.01 0
Branded products c 0.22** 0.17** 0.22** 0.17** 0.22** 0.17** 0.22** 0.17**
Social posts d 0.63** 0.61** 0.63** 0.61** 0.63** 0.61** 0.63** 0.61**
Positive mood e 0.06 –0.07 0.06 –0.07 0.06 –0.07 0.07 –0.07
Negative mood e 0.20+ 0.09 0.20+ 0.09 0.22+ 0.11 0.13 0.17
DFS x branded products –0.02 0.11+ 0.00 0.10+ –0.04 0.07 –0.06 0.05
DFS x social posts –0.10+ 0.22** –0.03 0.23** –0.14* 0.11+ –0.19** 0.09

Random Effects
Within-subjects variance .45 .50 .45 .49 .45 .50 .45 .50
Between-subjects variance .47 .41 .46 .40 .47 .43 .49 .44
Intraclass correlation coef. .51 .45 .51 .45 .51 .46 .52 .46
R2Marginal .11 .11 .11 .12 .10 .09 .09 .08
R2Conditional .56 .51 .56 .52 .56 .51 .56 .51

NOTE. – Number of participants = 90, number of evaluation objects = 12, total N = 1,080. Significance levels: ‘+’ < .10, ‘*’ < .05, ‘**’ < .01. DFS scale and its respective factors are independent variables,
including their interaction with the condition. Positive and negative mood (control) were assessed using PANAS asking participants how they feel right now before evaluating stimuli. a Top-row in the table
indicates the respective DFS sub-scale as the independent variable in the models. b Positive and negative state-affect were standardized & centered before analysis. c 0 = Control, 1 = Branded products. d 0 =
Control, 1 = Social posts. e Independent continuous variables in the models were grand-mean-centered before analysis.

16
p = .091) with increasing scores on the DFS scale as dependent variables were single-item positive and
compared to control content. These findings show negative affect scales, and we added the likelihood of
that individuals high in dispositional FOMO experiencing FOMO as a manipulation check. After
susceptibility are more likely to be negatively evaluating all four mock-up posts, we added a word
affected by FOMO-inducing content on Instagram. fragmentation task as another dependent variable
However, as we conducted this initial study using a (Rusting & Larsen, 1998).
within-participants design, we could not rule out a Word Fragmentation Task Cleaning Procedure
possible carry-over effect, even though we used a
block-wise randomization design. Further, our In this word fragmentation task, participants
affective results were based on self-reported single- viewed 32 ambiguous word fragments (e.g., “CAL_”
item scales. We wanted to replicate these findings or “DIS_US_”) in random order. Participants could
using an implicit measure of affect in the following complete these word fragments either as neutral (e.g.,
study to provide additional evidence. “CALL” or “DISCUSS”), positive or negative (e.g.,
“CALM” or “DISGUST”). The measure was not part
Study 2b: Replicating Affective Outcomes of our preregistration because we had no prior
experience using it.
Due to our results and theorizing in Study 2a, we We coded answers with no values (including
aimed to replicate our prior results. Therefore, we participants answering "?") as missing. We re-coded
conducted an additional experimental online survey answers for which participants only typed the
study to examine the effect of dispositional FOMO missing characters and not the entire word to the
susceptibility on affect based on Instagram content in word (e.g., word fragment "CAL_," participant
a between-participants design. This replication was answer "M," re-coded value "CALM"). In this step,
essential to provide further evidence for the relevance we changed 344 values. We removed all spaces and
of the DFS scale and the need to examine FOMO and counted the characters for each reply. We coded
its personality-based antecedents more granularly. answers as missing when they were either too short
Power Analysis (e.g., word fragment: "G _ _ D," participant answer
"GOD") or too long (e.g., word fragment "THRI_ _,"
We planned to recruit 250 participants from the participant answer: "THIRSTY") to fit the word
Prolific opt-in platform. Based on an a priori power fragment. We excluded n = 285 answers. Then we
analysis with p < .05, within-subjects variance = 1, created a list with unique answers (n = 280). These
between-subjects variance = 0.60, and 2,000 unique answers also included misspellings (e.g.,
simulated repetitions of the multilevel regression word fragment: "F.R. _ T," participant answer:
analysis, we expected to detect an interaction effect "FART") and words that were not part of the English
size of b = 0.40 with a power of .801. dictionary (e.g., word fragment: "G_I_TY,"
Method participant answer: "GIGITY"). We checked each
unique answer for correctness and coded wrong
Procedure answers with a missing value (n = 303). Altogether,
To replicate Study 2a, in April 2021, we recruited participants provided 110 unique words across the
an additional Prolific sample (Sample D, N = 244, word fragmentation task. There were also 634
OSF: www.bit.ly/37hDC1E) using the same missing values in this task.
screening criteria as for Sample B and an average We included these words in an online survey to
compensation of £5.53/hr. Further, we screened for score participants' implicit affect, and 20 independent
active Instagram use and 18-50 years old participants participants on Prolific rated them as positive,
(Study 2b). In this preregistered study negative, or neutral (Fleiss' Kappa = .442). A word
(www.bit.ly/3xfRWjx), we focused on the effects of was assigned the value positive/negative/neutral
the social content condition compared to control when more participants evaluated it as
stimuli. We used the same stimuli as in Study 2a but positive/negative/neutral than the other two
omitted the branded products condition. The post categories. To compute the implicit affectivity for
category was a between-participants factor, and each participant, we created the sums of positive (M
examples within the category were a within- = 12.08, SD = 2.23) and negative words (M = 10.82,
participants factor. As in Study 2a, our self-reported
17
Table 8. Effects on explicit positive and negative affect by users' DFS scores.
DFS – Social DFS – Regret DFS – Experiential
DFS – overall a
disconnectedness a anticipation a envy a
Positive Negative affect Positive Negative affect Positive Negative affect
Mixed-effects model (explicit affect) Positive affect b Negative affect b
affect b b affect b b affect b b

(Intercept) –0.20** –0.42** –0.19** –0.42** –0.19** –0.42** –0.19** –0.42**


FOMO a 0.02 0.06 0.08 0.08 –0.03 –0.03 –0.11 0.02
Condition c 0.39** 0.89** 0.39** 0.87** 0.37** 0.91** 0.39** 0.88**
Positive mood d 0.30** –0.05 0.32** –0.07 0.27** –0.06 0.30** –0.08
Negative mood d –0.04 0.18* –0.07 0.18** –0.02 0.24** –0.05 0.25**
FOMO x Condition –0.26* 0.48** –0.24* 0.39** –0.22* 0.38** –0.08 0.43**
Random Effects
Within-subjects variance .31 .22 .31 .22 .31 .22 .31 .22
Between-subjects variance .54 .48 .55 .48 .53 .51 .54 .50
Intraclass correlation coef. (ICC) .63 .68 .64 .68 .63 .70 .63 .69
R2Marginal .16 .31 .15 .30 .16 .27 .16 .28
R2Conditional .69 .78 .69 .78 .69 .78 .69 .78

Positive Negative Positive Negative Positive Negative


Regression model (implicit affect) Positive words Negative words
words words words words words words
(Intercept) 0.37 0.61 0.36 0.55 0.35 0.51 0.41 0.65
FOMO a –0.03 –0.07 –0.02 –0.06 –0.03 –0.05 –0.04 –0.08
Condition b –0.67 –0.86+ –0.54 –0.50 –0.36 –0.63 –0.80 –1.40*
Positive mood c 0.01 –0.16* 0.00 –0.16* –0.00 –0.15* 0.00 –0.15*
Negative mood c –0.18+ 0.00 –0.18+ 0.01 –0.15 0.01 –0.16+ 0.00
FOMO x Condition 0.22 0.32* 0.20 0.22 0.11 0.23+ 0.23 0.43*
N 244 244 244 244 244 244 244 244
R2 .02 .05 .02 .04 .02 .04 .02 .06

NOTE. – Mixed-effects model: Number of participants = 244, number of evaluation object per participant = 4, total N = 976. Significance levels: ‘+’ < .10, ‘*’ < .05, ‘**’ < .01. Positive and negative mood
(control) were assessed using PANAS asking participants how they feel right now before evaluating stimuli. Regression model: We measured dependent variables as the standardized sum of positive/negative
words for each participant across 32 word fragments. Responses coded 'positive,’ 'neutral,' or 'negative' words as evaluated by an independent sample (N = 20) of raters (Fleiss' Kappa = .442). a Top-row in the
table indicates the respective DFS (sub-) scales as the independent variable in the models. b Positive and negative moods were standardized & centered before analysis. c 0 = Control, 1 = Social posts. d
Independent continuous variables in the models were grand-mean-centered before analysis.

18
SD = 2.54) across all word fragments. In the content might threaten users’ subjective well-being
following analysis, we did not consider neutral words depending on their dispositional FOMO
(M = 6.82 SD = 2.73). susceptibility (Barry & Wong, 2020). We consider
our predictive validity testing for the DFS scale
Results
successful.
Manipulation-Check
General Discussion
Participants were significantly more likely to
experience FOMO when seeing social posts vs. The findings from explicit (self-reported) and
control posts (Mcontrol = 1.92, Msocial = 5.44, p < .001, implicit (word fragmentation task) affect assessment
d = 1.44). align with each other and partially replicated Study
Multilevel Regression Analysis 2a. The relationship between individuals' tendency to
experience FOMO and FOMO-related affect is based
We submitted standardized dependent and grand-
on complex cognitive experiences. In general, our
mean-centered independent variables to the same
findings speak to the negative affective nature of the
series of multilevel regression models as in Study 2a.
FOMO experience in the context of social media. We
Our findings (upper section of Table 8) confirmed the
show that social media content may threaten users’
findings in Study 2a. Social content induced lower
subjective well-being depending on their cognitive
positive (b = -0.26, 95% CI [-0.50; -0.02], p = .037)
disposition for FOMO, which has been suggested
and higher negative affect (b = 0.48, 95% CI [0.26;
previously (Barry & Wong, 2020). We consider these
0.71], p < .001) with increasing scores on the DFS
negative emotions not necessarily adverse effects, as
scale as compared to control content. These findings
some research found that consumers emotionally
were the same across all individual FOMO
bond with brands they encounter under fear (Dunn &
components except for experiential envy. Though
Hoegg, 2014). However, since fear appeals may
there was a negative association between positive
interfere with message processing (Rhodes, 2017),
affect and experiential envy for social content
marketers must be careful when using FOMO
compared to control content, this effect was not
appeals.
statistically significant.
Our findings have two important implications.
Word Fragmentation Task Results First, they speak to the predictive validity of our
Supplemental to our preregistered analyses, we developed measurement instrument because
conducted eight linear regression analyses. Findings increases in DFS scores were associated with
for implicit affectivity mirrored results from explicit negative affect when social information was received
affectivity analyses. Our results (lower section of via social media. Second, the main driver for the
Table 8) indicated that participants who scored (implicit) affective outcomes was experiential envy
higher on the DFS scale identified more negative and the other cognitive components to a lesser
words in the social condition than in the control degree. We show that a detailed and fine-grained
condition (b = 0.33, 95% CI [0.02; 0.63], p = .037). view of FOMO as a multidimensional concept is
Though this effect was similar across all DFS sub- essential.
scales the main driver of negative affect was
General Discussion
experiential envy (b = 0.43, 95% CI [0.13; 0.72], p =
.005). When comparing effect sizes for the Our goal was to develop a measurement
interaction term across multilevel models, instrument that captures dispositional FOMO
experiential envy (b = 0.43, p = .005) showed a more susceptibility. Based on multiple qualitative and
potent standardized effect than social quantitative samples, we found that dispositional
disconnectedness (b = 0.22, p = .109) or regret FOMO is a combination of individuals’ (1) chronic
anticipation (b = 0.23, p = .081). feelings of social disconnectedness as a function of
These findings aligned and partially replicated their fear of being excluded and perceived threats to
Study 2a. The relationship between individuals' DFS being included, (2) their disposition to anticipate
and FOMO-related affect is based on people’s regret and overthink their decisions, and (3) their
susceptibility to the three FOMO components. As susceptibility to feelings of envy towards experiences
suggested previously, we showed that Instagram
19
others are having and from which they are absent. emotions in consumer behavior as they motivate
The developed DFS scale showed reliable and process-focused (benign envy) or outcome-focused
internally valid properties and was a (malicious envy) goal pursuit and, consequently,
methodologically and theoretically robust measure. consumer self-improvement (Salerno et al., 2019).
Thus, this work answered a call by Tandon et al. Additionally, high-DFS individuals may question
(2021), who proposed the need for a more complex the decisions they make. As a result, they may
FOMO measure to aid the phenomenon’s conceptual overthink, ruminate, and constantly fear having made
evolution. Although prior work has suggested that the wrong choice. The regret anticipation sub-scale
FOMO is strongly related to but conceptually captures the concept of overthinking. This concept
different from feelings of social exclusion, was strongly tied to risk aversion, dissatisfaction with
anticipated regret, and envy (Hayran et al., 2020), we psychological needs, and anxiety. Previous studies
show that people who are more susceptible to these have linked FOMO to anticipated regrets (e.g.,
cognitions are more likely to experience FOMO. Hodkinson, 2019). When consumers expect regret in
We captured an interpersonal component of purchase decisions, they are more likely to engage in
wanting to belong. We identified the sub-scale of sales events and prefer well-known brands
social disconnectedness—the fear of social exclusion (Simonson, 1992). Thus, since we show that high-
and threats to social inclusion comprised this factor. DFS consumers are more likely to engage in these
Based on our correlational findings, the fear of ways of thinking, FOMO might be an essential driver
exclusion sub-scale mainly represented low self- for brand choice and susceptibility for sales events.
esteem, introversion, social anxiety, and the Consequently, here we offer researchers in the
importance of social groups. Threats to inclusion future a measurement tool to investigate and explore
mirrored social comparison processes. Our results are the personality traits furthering the experience of
consistent with other work in this area (e.g., Lai et al., FOMO in more detail. We found that the DFS sub-
2016). We propose that individuals who score high in scales were highly intercorrelated and often worked
the DFS are more susceptible to thoughts of being in concert. However, we provide initial exploratory
excluded from their peer groups. These ostracism and evidence that our sub-scales differentially affected
social exclusion feelings may affect consumer post-decision outcomes in some contexts. We invite
behavior, such as brand choice (Su et al., 2016). In future research to use our scale to detangle the
other words, high-DFS consumers might switch complexity of the FOMO experience.
brands towards popular brands to uphold their Testing the predictive validity of our scale, we
inclusionary status with friends and peers who additionally showed that participants’ DFS scores
endorse them. On the other side, the feeling of social anticipate adverse affective outcomes in the context
exclusion also leads to financial risk-taking (Duclos of social media. Therefore, this current work's
et al., 2012). Therefore, FOMO might nudge additional vital contribution concerns the
consumers toward brands even though these relationship between dispositional FOMO and
purchases pose a substantial financial risk, evidenced affectivity. For instance, we robustly show that
by the overprized and predatory FYRE Festival Instagram content leads to more negative and less
promotions (Poulsen, 2019). positive affect after viewing social content than non-
Furthermore, high-DFS individuals are more social content for high-DFS individuals vs. low-DFS
likely to perceive their peer group to have superior individuals. We found these effects on affectivity for
experiences, better lives, and more opportunities. explicitly expressed affect (self-report) and implicitly
This aspect of the cognitive disposition for FOMO is expressed (word fragmentation task) positive and
represented by the experiential envy sub-scale, which negative affect.
in our work was positively related to, for example, Limitations and Future Research
social comparison orientation and fun-seeking. Prior
research provided similar findings (e.g., Rifkin et al., This present research does come with several
2015) showing the impact of becoming aware of limitations. First, concerning the scale development
another attractive event while engaging socially: The process, we were limited to items based on
attractiveness of the missed event appreciates, and qualitative research, including interviewees from
the attractiveness of the current event deteriorates. [omitted for blind review). Future work should
Both benign and malicious envy are relevant conduct cross-cultural research to validate the DFS
20
scale within different cultural environments. Because Data Availability and Deposition
these constructs should reflect relatively stable
individual differences, we invite future research to Quantitative data, analyses, and materials:
establish the temporal validity of the developed scale
(DeVellis, 2017). 1. www.bit.ly/3JwstHC (Study 1)
Additionally, although we offer a more 2. www.bit.ly/3vaxD70 (Study 1)
sophisticated explication of the FOMO concept as a 3. www.bit.ly/38yVFkg (Study 1)
trait than other research, we only provide weak 4. www.bit.ly/3Kz7V2x (Study 1)
evidence for the DFS sub-scales potentially differing 5. www.bit.ly/37BzSIi (Study 1)
roles. Therefore, future research should design 6. www.bit.ly/3v9Fz8t (Study 2a)
studies specifically to explore the various 7. www.bit.ly/37hDC1E (Study 2b)
predispositions comprising FOMO as antecedents for
when one is to experience FOMO. Though we We partially preregistered this research:
provide the much-needed measurement instrument
that allows for a context-independent and 1. www.bit.ly/3dpAr7h (Study 1)
multifaceted exploration of FOMO in the future, it 2. www.bit.ly/3sJku1g (Study 2a)
was out of the scope of this current research to 3. www.bit.ly/3xfRWjx (Study 2b)
robustly test these mechanisms in detail. Research
Disclosure Statement
should design studies to establish boundary
conditions. The authors have no financial or non-financial
conflict to disclose.
Conclusion
Acknowledgment
In summary, we developed a scale representing
individuals’ personality-based cognitive We thank Drs. Esther Thorson, David Ewoldsen,
predisposition for FOMO independent of the context Patricia Huddleston, and Ashley Sanders-Jackson for
of this research. We examined the impact of their valuable contributions and mentorship in this
Instagram content on emotional outcomes dependent research project. We acknowledge the Advertising
on individuals’ dispositional FOMO susceptibility and Public Relations Department at Michigan State
scores. Thus, we offer an evolved explication of the University for providing a collegial work
FOMO phenomenon. Comprised of social environment. We are grateful for the vital work of the
disconnectedness, regret anticipation, and dean of the Advertising and Public Relations
experiential envy, the dispositional FOMO Department at Michigan State University, Dr. Teresa
susceptibility as a personality trait might be an Mastin. She works tirelessly to create a safe and
essential driver of affectivity. By identifying the inclusive academic environment that promotes open
dispositions leading to negative-affective FOMO dialogues and judgment-free exchanges.
experiences, we offer a novel perspective on FOMO
that may help investigate protective behaviors for
people who suffer from FOMO in the future.
Offering a multidimensional, context-independent
personality trait measure allows future research to
detangle the different drivers of FOMO in a diverse
set of contexts. Thus, we invite scholars to conduct
fine-grained explorations of the phenomenon to aid
its conceptual evolution.

21
Reference

Abel, J. P., Buff, C. L., & Burr, S. A. (2016). Social Media and the Fear of Missing Out: Scale Development
and Assessment. Journal of Business & Economics Research, 14(1), 33.
https://doi.org/10.19030/jber.v14i1.9554

Allen, M. S., Iliescu, D., & Greiff, S. (2022). Single Item Measures in Psychological Science. European
Journal of Psychological Assessment, 38(1), 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000699

Arksey, H., & Knight, P. (1999). Interviewing for social scientists. SAGE Publications.
Barry, C. T., & Wong, M. Y. (2020). Fear of missing out (FoMO): A generational phenomenon or an
individual difference? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(12), 2952-2966.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407520945394

Baumeister, R. F., Brewer, L. E., Tice, D. M., & Twenge, J. M. (2007). Thwarting the Need to Belong:
Understanding the Interpersonal and Inner Effects of Social Exclusion. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 1(1), 506-520. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00020.x

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a
fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497-529.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

Bergkvist, L., & Rossiter, J. R. (2007). The Predictive Validity of Multiple-Item versus Single-Item
Measures of the Same Constructs. Journal of Marketing Research, 44(2), 175-184.
https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.44.2.175

Brakus, J. J. k., Schmitt, B. H., & Zarantonello, L. (2009). Brand Experience: What Is It? How Is It
Measured? Does It Affect Loyalty? Journal of Marketing, 73(3), 52-68.
www.jstor.org/stable/20619022

Bright, L. F., & Logan, K. (2018). Is my fear of missing out (FOMO) causing fatigue? Advertising, social
media fatigue, and the implications for consumers and brands. Internet Research, 28(5), 1213-1227.
https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-03-2017-0112

Budnick, C. J., Rogers, A. P., & Barber, L. K. (2020). The fear of missing out at work: Examining costs and
benefits to employee health and motivation. Computers in Human Behavior, 104, 106161.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106161

Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to
impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 67(2), 319-333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

Chang, H. H., & Tuan Pham, M. (2013). Affect as a Decision-Making System of the Present. Journal of
Consumer Research, 40(1), 42-63. https://doi.org/10.1086/668644

Churchill, G. A., Jr. (1979). A paradigm for developing better measures of marketing constructs. JMR,
Journal of Marketing Research (pre-1986), 16(000001), 64.

22
Clor-Proell, S., Guggenmos, R. D., & Rennekamp, K. M. (2019). Mobile Devices and Investment News
Apps: The Effects of Information Release, Push Notification, and the Fear of Missing Out. The
Accounting Review, 0000-0000. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2991262

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Second ed.). Erlbaum.

D’Amico, A., & Scrima, F. (2016). The Italian Validation of Singelis’s Self-Construal Scale (SCS): a Short
10-Item Version Shows Improved Psychometric Properties. Current Psychology, 35(1), 159-168.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9378-y

DeVellis, R. F. (2017). Scale Development - Theory and Application. SAGE Publications Inc.

Djafarova, E., & Bowes, T. (2021). ‘Instagram made Me buy it’: Generation Z impulse purchases in fashion
industry. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 59, 102345.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102345

Dogan, V. (2019). Why Do People Experience the Fear of Missing Out (FoMO)? Exposing the Link
Between the Self and the FoMO Through Self-Construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
50(4), 524-538. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119839145

Duclos, R., Wan, E. W., & Jiang, Y. (2012). Show Me the Honey! Effects of Social Exclusion on Financial
Risk-Taking. Journal of Consumer Research, 40(1), 122-135. https://doi.org/10.1086/668900
Dunn, L., & Hoegg, J. (2014). The Impact of Fear on Emotional Brand Attachment. Journal of Consumer
Research, 41(1), 152-168. https://doi.org/10.1086/675377

Faelens, L., Hoorelbeke, K., Cambier, R., van Put, J., Van de Putte, E., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W.
(2021). The relationship between Instagram use and indicators of mental health: A systematic
review. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 4, 100121.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100121

Festinger, L. (1954). A Theory of Social Comparison Processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117-140.
https://doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202

Ford, J. K., MacCallum, R. C., & Tait, M. (1986). The application of exploratory factor analysis in applied
psycholoy: A critical review and analysis. Personnel Psychology, 39(2), 291-314.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1986.tb00583.x

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables
and Measurement Error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), 39-50.
https://doi.org/10.2307/3151312

Gibbons, F. X., & Buunk, B. P. (1999). Individual differences in social comparison: Development of a scale
of social comparison orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(1), 129-142.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.129

Good, M. C. (2019). Fear of missing out appeals: You can’t always get what you want [Unpublished
doctoral dissertation]. New Mexico State University.

Good, M. C., & Hyman, M. R. (2020). ‘Fear of missing out’: antecedents and influence on purchase
likelihood. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 28(3), 330-341.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10696679.2020.1766359
23
Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of
the Septo-Hippocampal System (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.

Hanlon, S. M. (2016). Managing my fear of missing out. Science, 353(6306), 1458.


https://doi.org/10.1126/science.353.6306.1458

Hardin, T., & Bischof, B. (2020). The Fear of Missing Out NASDAQ. Retrieved February, 4 from
https://www.nasdaq.com/articles/the-fear-of-missing-out-2020-06-10

Haws, K. L., Sample, K. L., & Hulland, J. (accepted). Scale use and abuse: Towards best practices in the
deployment of scales. Journal of Consumer Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1320

Hayran, C., Anik, L., & Gürhan-Canli, Z. (2020). A threat to loyalty: Fear of missing out (FOMO) leads to
reluctance to repeat current experiences. PLOS ONE, 15(4), e0232318.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0232318

Hodkinson, C. (2019). ‘Fear of Missing Out’ (FOMO) marketing appeals: A conceptual model. Journal of
Marketing Communications, 25(1), 65-88. https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2016.1234504

Holte, A. J., Fisher, W. N., & Ferraro, F. R. (2022). Afraid of Social Exclusion: Fear of Missing Out
Predicts Cyberball-Induced Ostracism. Journal of Technology in Behavioral Science.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41347-022-00251-9

Homburg, C., Schwemmle, M., & Kuehnl, C. (2015). New Product Design: Concept, Measurement, and
Consequences. Journal of Marketing, 79(3), 41-56. www.jstor.org/stable/43784405

Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2),
179-185. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447

Johnston, M. M., & Finney, S. J. (2010). Measuring basic needs satisfaction: Evaluating previous research
and conducting new psychometric evaluations of the Basic Needs Satisfaction in General Scale.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(4), 280-296.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.04.003

Kim, H. J., Hong, J. S., Hwang, H. C., Kim, S. M., & Han, D. H. (2020). Comparison of Psychological
Status and Investment Style Between Bitcoin Investors and Share Investors. Frontiers in Psychology,
11(3230). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.502295

Kline, R. B. (2013). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. In Y. Petscher, C. Schatschneider, & D.
L. Compton (Eds.), Applied quantitative analysis education and the social sciences (pp. 171–207).
Routledge.

Lai, C., Altavilla, D., Ronconi, A., & Aceto, P. (2016). Fear of missing out (FOMO) is associated with
activation of the right middle temporal gyrus during inclusion social cue. Computers in Human
Behavior, 61, 516-521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072

Lange, J., & Crusius, J. (2014). Dispositional Envy Revisited: Unraveling the Motivational Dynamics of
Benign and Malicious Envy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(2), 284-294.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214564959

24
Mandel, N. (2003). Shifting Selves and Decision Making: The Effects of Self-Construal Priming on
Consumer Risk-Taking. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(1), 30-40.
https://doi.org/10.1086/374700

Mandrik, C. A., & Bao, Y. (2004, October 7-10, 2004). Exploring the Concept and Measurement of General
Risk Aversion. Advances in Consumer Research, Portland, Oregon.

Milyavskaya, M., Saffran, M., Hope, N., & Koestner, R. (2018). Fear of missing out: prevalence, dynamics,
and consequences of experiencing FOMO. Motivation and Emotion, 42(5), 725-737.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9683-5

Moschis, G. P. (1976). Social Comparison and Informal Group Influence. Journal of Marketing Research,
13(3), 237-244. https://doi.org/10.2307/3150733

Neumann, D. (2020). Fear of Missing Out. In J. van den Bulck (Ed.), The International Encyclopedia of
Media Psychology. Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119011071.iemp0185

Neumann, D., Huddleston, P. T., & Behe, B. K. (2021). Fear of missing out as motivation to process
information: How differences in Instagram use affect attitude formation online. . New Media &
Society. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448211011834

Popovac, M., & Hadlington, L. (2020). Exploring the role of egocentrism and fear of missing out on online
risk behaviours among adolescents in South Africa. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth,
25(1), 276-291. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2019.1617171

Poulsen, V. (2019). Pics or it didn’t happen: FOMO culture and Fyre Festival. Retrieved March 7 from
https://www.adolescent.net/a/pics-or-it-didnt-happen-fomo-culture-and-fyre-festival

Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, emotional, and
behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1841-1848.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014

Quelette, C. (2019). FOMO Statistics You Need to Grow Your Business. TrustPulse. Retrieved October 20
from https://trustpulse.com/fomo-statistics/

Reagle, J. (2015). Following the Joneses: FOMO and conspicuous sociality. First Monday, 20(10).
https://doi.org/10.5210/fm.v20i10.6064

Reer, F., Tang, W. Y., & Quandt, T. (2019). Psychosocial well-being and social media engagement: The
mediating roles of social comparison orientation and fear of missing out. New Media & Society,
21(7), 1486-1505. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444818823719

Rhodes, N. (2017). Fear-Appeal Messages: Message Processing and Affective Attitudes. Communication
Research, 44(7), 952-975. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650214565916

Rifkin, J. R., Chan, C., & Kahn, B. (2015, October 1-4, 2015). Fomo: How the Fear of Missing Out Leads to
Missing Out. Advances in Consumer Research, New Orleans, Louisiana.

Riordan, B. C., Flett, J. A. M., Cody, L. M., Conner, T. S., & Scarf, D. (2019). The Fear of Missing Out
(FoMO) and event-specific drinking: The relationship between FoMO and alcohol use, harm, and

25
breath alcohol concentration during orientation week. Current Psychology.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00318-6

Roese, N. J., & Summerville, A. (2005). What We Regret Most... and Why. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 31(9), 1273-1285. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205274693

Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. Basic.

Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2021). Individual differences in Fear of Missing
Out (FoMO): Age, gender, and the Big Five personality trait domains, facets, and items. Personality
and Individual Differences, 171, 110546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110546

Rusting, C. L., & Larsen, R. J. (1998). Personality and Cognitive Processing of Affective Information.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(2), 200-213.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298242008

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation,
development, and wellness. Guilford Press.

Rye, M. S., Cahoon, M. B., Ali, R. S., & Daftary, T. (2008). Development and Validation of the
Counterfactual Thinking for Negative Events Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90(3), 261-
269. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890701884996

Salerno, A., Laran, J., & Janiszewski, C. (2019). The Bad Can Be Good: When Benign and Malicious Envy
Motivate Goal Pursuit. Journal of Consumer Research, 46(2), 388-405.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucy077

Simonson, I. (1992). The Influence of Anticipating Regret and Responsibility on Purchase Decisions.
Journal of Consumer Research, 19(1), 105-118. https://doi.org/10.1086/209290
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the Slate-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). ConsuIting Psychologists
Press.

Strahan, R., & Gerbasi, K. C. (1972). Short, homogeneous versions of the Marlow-Crowne Social
Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(2), 191-193. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-
4679(197204)28:2<191::AID-JCLP2270280220>3.0.CO;2-G

Su, L., Jiang, Y., Chen, Z., & DeWall, C. N. (2016). Social Exclusion and Consumer Switching Behavior: A
Control Restoration Mechanism. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(1), 99-117.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw075

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5 ed.). Allyn and Bacon.

Talbot, K. (2019). What the Fyre Festival Documentaries Reveal About Millennials. Retrieved March 7
from https://www.forbes.com/sites/katetalbot/2019/01/21/what-the-fyre-festival-documentaries-
revealed-about-millennials/

Tandon, A., Dhir, A., Almugren, I., AlNemer Ghada, N., & Mäntymäki, M. (2021). Fear of missing out
(FoMO) among social media users: a systematic literature review, synthesis and framework for
future research. Internet Research, online. https://doi.org/10.1108/INTR-11-2019-0455

26
Tsiros, M., & Mittal, V. (2000). Regret: A Model of Its Antecedents and Consequences in Consumer
Decision Making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(4), 401-417. https://doi.org/10.1086/209571

Walchli, S. B., & Landman, J. (2003). Effects of counterfactual thought on postpurchase consumer affect.
Psychology & Marketing, 20(1), 23-46. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.10057

Wang, P., Wang, X., Nie, J., Zeng, P., Liu, K., Wang, J., Guo, J., & Lei, L. (2019). Envy and problematic
smartphone use: The mediating role of FOMO and the moderating role of student-student
relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 136-142.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.013

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive
and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063-
1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

Weiss, L., & Johar, G. V. (2016). Products as Self-Evaluation Standards: When Owned and Unowned
Products Have Opposite Effects on Self-Judgment. Journal of Consumer Research, 42(6), 915-930.
https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucv097

Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., & Elhai, J. D. (2018). Problematic smartphone use and
relations with negative affect, fear of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation.
Psychiatry Research, 262, 618-623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058

Wu, L., & Dodoo, N. A. (2020). Being Accepted or Ostracized: How Social Experience Influences
Consumer Responses to Advertisements with Different Regulatory Focus. Journal of Advertising,
49(3), 234-249. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2020.1743217

Xie, X., Wang, Y., Wang, P., Zhao, F., & Lei, L. (2018). Basic psychological needs satisfaction and fear of
missing out: Friend support moderated the mediating effect of individual relative deprivation.
Psychiatry Research, 268, 223-228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.07.025

Zhang, Z., Jiménez, F. R., & Cicala, J. E. (2020). Fear Of Missing Out Scale: A self-concept perspective.
Psychology & Marketing. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21406

27

You might also like