You are on page 1of 3

ll

Dispatches

Motor control: Neural correlates of optimal feedback


control theory
Mackenzie W. Mathis and Steffen Schneider
Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland
Correspondence: mackenzie.mathis@epfl.ch (M.W.M.), steffen.schneider@epfl.ch (S.S.)
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.087

Recent work is revealing neural correlates of a leading theory of motor control. By linking an elegant series of
behavioral experiments with neural inactivation in macaques with computational models, a new study shows
that premotor and parietal areas can be mapped onto a model for optimal feedback control.

We are constantly adapting. Whether it is goal-directed nature of the motor during motor behaviors6–9. While this is
to new shoes, or walking on icy terrain, we corrections beautifully matched many mostly limited to rodent studies, the future
have the capacity to continually update observations. The model is allowed to holds these types of experiments in
our motor actions in a goal-directed exploit redundancy to improve non-human primates. In their new work,
manner. The leading theory of how we do performance, which matches to Takei et al.1 have causally tested OFC
this — optimal feedback control (OFC) observations of motor variability, and theory in a non-human primate species
theory — has been upheld by an task-relevant motor corrections that with an elegant use of cooling probes in
impressive number of elegant behavioral occur mostly closest to the end-goal (for multiple areas of the macaque cortex1,10.
studies in humans and other animals over example, correcting hand movements In prior work, Scott and colleagues11
the last 20 years, yet the neural circuits mid-trajectory to match an average showed that many regions of cortex
that implement such control continue to trajectory template is not required; what is respond rapidly to limb perturbations.
remain elusive. Now, in a study reported required is hitting the end-goal). Lastly, in Specifically, they recorded in five cortical
in this issue of Current Biology, Takei contrast to other theories of motor regions that are involved in motor control:
et al.1 have chipped away at this control, such as active inference3 or parietal area 5 (A5), primary
long-standing question in motor control feedback error learning4, OFC theory somatosensory cortex areas 1 and 2
by providing important clues to how does not require a trajectory template and (S1, S2), M1, and dorsal premotor cortex
neural circuits may begin to be mapped explicitly considers noise and delays, (PMd)). They found that the delays of
onto theory. which are important constraints of sensory information from the periphery
The origins of optimal control date back biological systems. could change in a task-relevant manner.
nearly 300 years, but in the 1950s Shortly thereafter, Scott5 Namely, if limb-perturbations were
Wiener’s cybernetics movement brought hypothesized that, because OFC theory important for the ongoing task, the
forth the notion that intelligent behavior is was so powerful at explaining human measured response times in A5, S1
rooted in feedback control. OFC theory movement and consists of multiple were only 25 milliseconds, yet a
burst onto the motor control scene in elements, it could be highly valuable for target-selection change first causes
2002 with seminal work by Todorov and mapping the neural basis of goal- responses in PMd and M1 (with other
Jordan2. They postulated that, by directed (volitional) motor control. He regions trailing behind). This was
deploying stochastic optimal feedback laid out several crucial neural intriguing evidence that parietal areas,
control, the motor system would only observations that matched with OFC such as A5, might play a crucial role in
correct movements in task-relevant theory. For example, the observed task-relevant corretions.
dimensions. This allows variability in tuning-properties of motor cortex (M1) Building on their earlier work5,11,12,
task-irrelevant ones, and this ‘minimum neurons activity were found to change in Takei et al.1 have now causally tested the
intervention’ principle nicely explains a behaviorally dependent manner. This role of A5 and PMd during limb
many motor behaviors of humans. powerful idea has spawned years of perturbations applied during a postural
The model was a breakthrough in research into how the observed stabilization task. First, they mapped their
several respects. Firstly, this framework behaviors of the motor system — which behavioral paradigm onto the OFC theory
requires multiple parts: an internal without a doubt must include such model and performed a series of
forward model that enables the notions as internal forward models and ‘inactivation’ experiments to investigate
computation of optimal control signals goal-directed corrections — can be what model parameters have differential
(given noisy, delayed feedback), which is mapped onto neural circuits. effects on the predicted behavior. The
integrated with knowledge of the body Techniques such as optogenetics or model has several key parameters that
dynamics and the available copy of the chemogenetics allow experimentalists to they test: the ‘Kalman gain’ (K), which is
out-going motor command (the so-called design studies where one can spatially (in the term that provides an uncertainty
efference copy; Figure 1A) via a the brain) and temporally (in the measure (that modulates the learning
state-estimator. Secondly, the behavioral space) perturb neural circuits rate); the internal forward model

R356 Current Biology 31, R330–R358, April 12, 2021 ª 2021 Elsevier Inc.
ll
Dispatches
^ and the ‘feedback’ (L)
observation (H); A Motor command Noise
from the ‘state estimator’ (the estimated PMd
u
Control policy
state of the body) to the controller (the A5
region that sends out motor commands). (L) Internal forward
Formally, Takei et al. test variations of all Feedback Predicted model
gain Estimated state ^ Actual
three components — the internal forward (H)
state X* X state
model, state estimator and feedback ^ Noise
X Sensory
State estimator
control policy — of an OFC model. The feedback
B
true system state x is transformed into Kalman gain (K) Observation matrix (H)
measurements (observations) y by means Optimal
feedback L normal Reduce L slightly Reduce L Reduce H
of an observation matrix H and passed to control
the state estimator. Based on the K normal Endpoint error (0) Endpoint error (+) Endpoint error (+)
measurement noise, the state estimator Response speed (-) Response speed (-) Response speed (-)

trades off these measurements with a


current state estimate, controlled by the Reduce K Endpoint error (+) Endpoint error (++) Endpoint error (++++)
Response speed (0) Response speed (-) Response speed (--)
Kalman gain K (Figure 1A). The state
PMd
estimator computes the difference of the A5

actual vs. expected sensory feedback by PMd normal PMd slightly cooled PMd fully cooled

transforming the output of the internal


Endpoint error (0) Endpoint error (+)
forward model by an observation matrix A5 normal Response speed (-) Response speed (-)
^ The state estimation result is finally fed
H.
back into the control policy, where it is A5 fully Endpoint error (+) Endpoint error (++)
scaled with the feedback gain L cooled Response speed (0) Response speed (-)
(Figure 1A). Modifying the aforementioned
parameters can non-trivially affect the Current Biology

system behavior. The results of this


parameter testing are summarized in Figure 1. OFC theory and the brain.
(A) Highlights optimal feedback control theory, the brain regions perturbed, and the colors highlight the
Figure 1B; namely, if they inactivate ‘L’ matching model-to-brain predictions. ‘L’ is the feedback gain, ‘K’ is the Kalman gain in the state
they predict a change in response speed, estimator, ‘H’ is the observation matrix modeling the measurement of the actual state x, while H ^
and if a strong reduction in L, or in H, there transforms the corresponding estimated states x* the state estimator receives from the forward model.
Other terms are defined on the figure, but not directly tested in the author’s report. (B) The main results
is also an endpoint error. In contrast, if L is
from both the modelling work and the experimental testing are summarized. Note, the paired colors
normal, but K is modified, they predict an highlight where the model matched. The (0) means no change predicted/observed; (+) means
endpoint error, but no response-speed increased, (–) means decreased behavioral changes. During combinations of cooling experiments,
changes. these changes can add up (thereby increasing the number of + or – displayed in the table). (Cartoon
images are by Smith Breault, Macauley (CC 4.0) from scidraw.io.)
Next, in an elegant series of
experiments in which cooling probes
were used to inactivate spatial regions of simultaneous attenuation of the Kalman parameters, respectively. However, the
the macaque brain10,13, Takei et al.1 found gain and the feedback control gains in results of the partial cooling experiment
differential effects by cooling A5 and OFC theory, but not attenuation of could be solely explained by downscaling
PMd, suggesting they may play different Kalman gain and sensory input, could the feedback gain.
roles that can be mapped to OFC theory. reproduce these observations in the Overall, Takei et al.1 build on previous
Cooling only A5 increased endpoint error, model (Figure 1B, grey box prediction). findings regarding the role of A5 and PMd
while inhibition of PMd both increased Adding to this evidence of a link in motor control by mapping their role to
endpoint error and reduced the response between PMd cooling and reduction of specific parameters in optimal feedback
accuracy, among other metrics. While the the feedback control gain, Takei et al.1 control theory. Specifically, the model
former finding directly links to the effect of consider partial cooling of PMd as a third predictions matched the experimental
downscaling the Kalman gain in an OFC paradigm and model this as a smaller predictions in several key domains. As we
theory model — highlighting that A5 reduction in the feedback gain. The model summarize in Figure 1B, parameters
cooling impairs state estimation — both yields a characteristic response in this involved in both control policy (L), and the
reduction of sensory input and the setting: in contrast to the full reduction state estimator (K, H), could be mapped to
feedback control gain in OFC theory experiment, partial impairment no longer PMd and A5, respectively.
qualitatively predict the effect of cooling impacts the endpoint error (previously, an In the future, it will be important to
PMd (Figure 1). In a series of additional increase was observed in all of the A5, address some limitations of this work, and
experiments combining cooling of both PMd and A5 plus PMd cooling build on it in new ways. While Takei et al.1
PMd and A5, the authors went on to show experiments). Alternatively to reducing demonstrate links between OFC theory
that impairments in endpoint errors and parameters in the OFC theory model, predictions, A5, PMd and behavior, it is
response speed add up linearly and scale single impairments of PMd or A5 can also likely that the model can be enriched by
sublinear for the maximally observed be modeled by increasing the noise levels taking into account the circuits
deviation. They found that only on the feedback or Kalman gain themselves. Fundamentally, OFC theory

Current Biology 31, R330–R358, April 12, 2021 R357


ll
Dispatches

is a phenomenological model, and how REFERENCES 9. Mathis, M.W., Mathis, A., and Uchida, N.
(2017). Somatosensory cortex plays an
neurons implement its components such essential role in forelimb motor adaptation in
as the Kalman gain is unresolved. 1. Takei, T., Lomber, S.G., Cook, D.J., and Scott,
mice. Neuron 93, 1493–1503.
S.H. (2021). Transient deactivation of dorsal
Moreover, there are numerous potential premotor cortex or parietal area 5 impairs
implementations at the circuit level, such feedback control of the limb in macaques. 10. Meyer-Lohmann, J., Conrad, B., Matsunami,
Curr. Biol. 31, 1476–1487. K., and Brooks, V.B. (1975). Effects of dentate
that new models that provide concrete cooling on precentral unit activity following
experimental hypotheses will be 2. Todorov, E., and Jordan, M.I. (2002). Optimal torque pulse injections into elbow movements.
Brain Res. 94, 237–251.
important. feedback control as a theory of motor
coordination. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1226–1235.
To work in this direction, more 11. Omrani, M., Murnaghan, C.D., Pruszynski,
fine-grained cooling experiments might 3. Friston, K., Mattout, J., and Kilner, J. (2011). J.A., and Scott, S.H. (2016). Distributed
yield richer input to the model (and, of Action understanding and active inference. task-specific processing of somatosensory
Biol. Cybernet. 104, 137–160. feedback for voluntary motor control. eLife 5,
course, more temporally and cell-type- e13141.
specific approaches such as 4. Kawato, M., and Gomi, H. (1992). A
optogenetics will become important). The computational model of four regions of the 12. Pruszynski, J.A., Kurtzer, I., Nashed, J.Y.,
cerebellum based on feedback-error learning. Omrani, M., Brouwer, B., and Scott, S.H.
same applies for deactivating additional
Biol. Cybernet. 68, 95–103. (2011). Primary motor cortex underlies multi-
individual brain regions. These additional joint integration for fast feedback control.
choices for influencing the biological brain 5. Scott, S.H. (2004). Optimal feedback control Nature 478, 387–390.
will apply to identify interesting working and the neural basis of volitional motor control.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 532–545. 13. Lomber, S.G., Payne, B.R., and Horel, J.A.
points of the OFC theory model with (1999). The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible
unique response patterns. Reproducing 6. Boyden, E.S., Zhang, F., Bamberg, E., Nagel, cooling deactivation method for behavioral
G., and Deisseroth, K. (2005). Millisecond- or electrophysiological assessment of
such patterns in the biological system and neural function. J. Neurosci. Methods 86,
timescale, genetically targeted optical control
adding them in the author’s combinatorial of neural activity. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1263–1268. 179–194.
experimental design (Figure 1B) will shed
even more light on links between 7. Sternson, S.M., and Roth, B.L. (2014). 14. Shadmehr, R., and Krakauer, J. (2008).
Chemogenetic tools to interrogate brain A computational neuroanatomy for
biological brains and OFC theory model functions. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 387–407. motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185,
components, and indicate where more 359–381.
complex models are needed for an 8. Azim, E., Jiang, J., Alstermark, B., and Jessell,
T.M. (2014). Skilled reaching relies on a V2a 15. Merel, J., Botvinick, M., and Wayne, G. (2019).
accurate account of the neural propriospinal internal copy circuit. Nature 508, Hierarchical motor control in mammals and
implementation14,15. 357–363. machines. Nat. Commun. 10, 5489.

R358 Current Biology 31, R330–R358, April 12, 2021

You might also like