You are on page 1of 7

Law and Psychiatry

Performance Validity Testing in Neuropsychology:


Scientific Basis and Clinical Application—
MICHAEL R. GREHER, PhD, ABPP
A Brief Review THOMAS R. WODUSHEK, PhD, ABPP

Performance validity testing refers to neuro- published an article on the concept of “compensation
psychologists’ methodology for determining neurosis.”2 In 1980, Waddell et al3 described phys-
whether neuropsychological test performances ical signs to help identify nonorganic explanations
completed in the course of an evaluation are for low back pain, which have since been used to
valid (ie, the results of true neurocognitive identify symptom magnification. In 2008, Rogers4
function) or invalid (ie, overly impacted by the indicated that the concept of symptom magnification
patient’s effort/engagement in testing). This in psychology was initiated by the advent of the
determination relies upon the use of either Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory,5
standalone tests designed for this sole purpose, which included scales to detect overreporting of
or specific scores/indicators embedded within psychological symptoms compared with a normative
traditional neuropsychological measures that population. These types of scales for measuring
have demonstrated this utility. In response to a overreporting and underreporting of symptoms,
greater appreciation for the critical role that frequently referred to as symptom validity tests, are
performance validity issues play in neuro- now utilized in multiple psychological inventories
psychological testing and the need to measure and structured interviews; however, their form and
this variable to the best of our ability, the sci- function are outside the scope of this review.
entific base for performance validity testing Although assessing for overreporting of psycho-
has expanded greatly over the last 20 to logical and physical symptoms is an important facet
30 years. As such, the majority of current day of neuropsychological evaluations, an additional
neuropsychologists in the United States use a interest among current day neuropsychologists is
variety of measures for the purpose of per- the detection of feigned or inaccurately measured
formance validity testing as part of everyday cognitive deficits, such as difficulties with memory,
forensic and clinical practice and address this attention, or executive functions. Efforts to uncover
issue directly in their evaluations. The follow- invalid responding in cognitive testing also have a
ing is the first article of a 2-part series that will long history, with early efforts by André Rey6 in the
address the evolution of performance validity 1940s resulting in techniques that continue to be
testing in the field of neuropsychology, both in used to this day.
terms of the science as well as the clinical
application of this measurement technique.
The second article of this series will review TERMINOLOGY
performance validity tests in terms of methods Over time, a variety of terms have been used to
for development of these measures, and max- describe the measurement tools neuropsychologists
imizing of diagnostic accuracy. use for the purpose of discerning valid versus
(Journal of Psychiatric Practice 2017;23;134–
140) GREHER and WODUSHEK: Guest columnists, Departments
of Neurosurgery and Neurology, School of Medicine, Univer-
KEY WORDS: performance validity testing, symp- sity of Colorado, Aurora, CO
tom validity testing, effort testing, malingering, Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights
neuropsychology reserved.
Please send correspondence to: Michael R. Greher, PhD,
ABPP, Department of Neurosurgery, University of Colorado
The possibility of symptom magnification has long School of Medicine, 12631 E. 17th Ave, C307, Aurora, CO
been recognized in the practice of medicine. In 1912, 80045 (e-mail: michael.greher@ucdenver.edu).
Sir John Collie1 wrote of fraud in medicolegal set- The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
tings, and in 1961 the neurologist Henry Miller DOI: 10.1097/PRA.0000000000000218

134 March 2017 Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

TABLE 1. Most Commonly Used Performance Validity Tests (PVTs)*

Standalone PVTs Embedded PVTs

Test of Memory Malingering WAIS-III or WAIS-IV Digit Span: Reliable Digit Span
Word Memory Test California Verbal Learning Test-2: Forced choice
Medical Symptom Validity Test Motor/Sensory Indices: Finger Tapping Indices
Dot Counting Test Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Failure to Maintain Set
Rey 15-item Test WMS-III or WMS-IV: LM II Recognition score
Victoria Symptom Validity Test WMS-III or WMS-IV: VR II Recognition score

*Data from a survey of 316 neuropsychologists practicing in North America published by Martin et al8 in 2015.
LM II indicates Logical Memory II Recognition of WMS-III or WMS-IV; WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd
Edition; WAIS-IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition; WMS-III = Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition; WMS-
IV = Wechsler Memory Scale, 4th Edition; VR II = Visual Reproduction II Recognition of WMS-III or WMS-IV.

invalid responding, such as “malingering tests,” scores may no longer reflect cognitive difficulty but
“effort tests,” and most recently “performance val- instead indicate issues related to effort/task
idity tests.”7 There were many reasons for these engagement. Table 1, which is based on the results
shifts in terminology but, put simply, in large part of a survey of 316 neuropsychologists practicing in
they reflect an understanding of the complexities North America, presents the 6 most commonly used
involved in determining performance validity and standalone and embedded PVTs according to this
discussing notions of intent as well as sensitivity to study. Crucially important to the validation of all of
the negative implications involved in identifying these measures is the demonstration that per-
patients as engaging in “malingering” or “poor formance on these measures is largely unaffected by
effort” in the course of their examination. As such, various neurological diseases and insults. Most
current day neuropsychologists view the term such measures have been validated in wide-ranging
“performance validity tests” or “PVTs” as providing neurological patient samples, and these inves-
an accurate description while avoiding terms that tigations have frequently focused on those with
are potentially pejorative, damaging, or presump- severe impairment to ensure that the measures and
tuous. For this reason, the authors have used this associated cut scores greatly limit the chance of
term in this series of columns. However, for the false-positive diagnoses of invalid responding.
sake of convenience and ease of communication, Although the patient samples vary depending on
occasional references are made to issues related to the test in question, examples include conditions
“poor effort” or “task engagement,” the meanings of such as moderate/severe traumatic brain injury,
which are largely synonymous and indicate invalid multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, stroke, and even early
test-taking approaches without conclusions as to dementia, among others. Validation of these tests
intent. Practically speaking, “standalone” PVTs are and their associated cut scores with frank neuro-
tests designed and administered for the sole pur- logical samples such as these provides an evidence-
pose of determining valid or invalid responding. based criterion by which to argue that scores below
They come in both paper/pencil and computerized the established cutoffs are unlikely to be the result
form and, at face value, seem to be quite similar in of neurologically based cognitive dysfunction.
most respects to other neuropsychological tests,
though they are largely insensitive to brain dys-
JUSTIFICATION FOR PVTs
function and do not provide measurements of actual
cognitive ability. Embedded indicators frequently Despite the early work of Rey,6 until recently most
represent “built-in” scores on traditional neuro- neuropsychologists largely, though not exclusively,
psychological measures (those that measure mem- conceptualized low scores on neuropsychological
ory, executive functioning, and so on) that have testing as indicative of brain dysfunction.9 However,
demonstrated a psychometric floor, below which in recent decades, the neuropsychology literature

Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2 March 2017 135

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

has shifted significantly with growing recognition of highlight the need for frequent use of PVTs. A 2002
the clearly demonstrated role that non-neurological survey demonstrated that neuropsychologists esti-
factors can play with regard to performance on mated probable malingering or symptom exagger-
neuropsychological testing. An incomplete list of ation in compensation-seeking cases to be as high as
these non-neurological factors includes severe emo- 40% for certain patient groups.15 In addition, studies
tional distress, sleep disturbance, pain, fatigue, cog- of personal injury litigants, Social Security disability
niphobia (avoidance of cognitively effortful tasks to applicants, military veterans, workers’ compensation
avoid onset of or worsening of headache pain), and patients, and even certain child populations have
medication side effects.10 Additional concerns that demonstrated increased PVT failure rates reflective
neuropsychologists need to consider in the inter- of the impact of motivation on testing. Accordingly, a
pretation of cognitive and PVT results include factors 2005 position paper by the National Academy of
such as sensory disturbances (eg, a visual field cut), Neuropsychology discussed the importance of utiliz-
limited facility with the testing language, and the ing at least 1 PVT (then described as a test of “effort”)
possibility of subclinical seizures or other significant within the context of neuropsychological exams being
mental status fluctuations during the testing process. conducted when issues of secondary gain are pres-
Put differently, neuropsychologists have become ent.16 In a short time, neuropsychologists came to
increasingly aware that, although neuropsychological recognize that, although issues of dissimulation are
testing is highly sensitive to brain dysfunction, poor of greatest concern in cases involving known issues of
test results are nonspecific and can arise for a wide secondary gain (eg, civil litigation, disability bene-
variety of reasons. We have also come to realize that fits), these same issues are also of concern in patient-
among these non-neurological causes for low neuro- focused care settings when there are no apparent
psychological test scores are issues such as malin- secondary gain issues present, but just to a some-
gering, poor effort, and insufficient task engagement, what lesser degree. A survey of American Board of
which are more common and more nuanced than Clinical Neuropsychology–certified neuropsycho-
initially considered. logists15 revealed an estimated rate of invalid testing
The widespread use of standalone PVTs in neuro- results of 8% in a nonlitigating sample; in another
psychological practice began as 2 factors about cog- survey study,17 respondents estimated this failure
nitive testing gained more attention. First, unlike rate to be around 10% for their overall practice. To
traditional medical tests such as blood labs or struc- this end, the official position of the American Acad-
tural neuroimaging, neuropsychologists recognized emy of Clinical Neuropsychology is to recommend
that obtaining valid test results is reliant upon the near universal application of PVTs in both clinical
examinee’s full engagement with testing, and this and forensic settings through utilization of multiple
necessary full level of engagement cannot always be standalone and embedded measures.18
assumed simply because a provider has encouraged
best effort or the examinee reported providing his or
INCREASED PVT USE IN CLINICAL PRACTICE
her best effort. Second, neuropsychologists learned
that clinical judgment is often insufficient to effec- Not surprisingly, the use of at least some form of
tively distinguish valid from invalid testing without PVT has climbed dramatically in forensic and clin-
these types of objective indicators.9,11 Along with the ical practice. In 2015, Sweet et al19 surveyed
growing understanding of the limitations of clinical approximately 1500 neuropsychologists across the
judgment as it relates to performance validity, new United States and found that 99% of practitioners
attempts to refine early PVTs were undertaken with who do at least some forensic evaluations support
the hope of increasing the standardization and the utility of PVTs, and 98% of providers with no
applicability of such measures.12,13 For a review of forensic practice also support use of PVTs. Indeed,
these concepts and their history, readers are referred in a 2015 article reporting the results of another
to a 2001 article by Bianchini et al.14 survey of neuropsychologists, Martin et al8 stated
Not only is it important to improve detection of that a “significant paradigm shift” had occurred in
response validity, but survey data from neuro- the neuropsychology community over the past
psychologists suggesting high base rates of insuffi- 8 years, whereby PVTs are considered mandatory or
cient effort in compensation-seeking cases further desirable irrespective of setting, as endorsed by

136 March 2017 Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

99.7% of survey respondents. More specifically, described and interpreted as “malingering tests,”
Sweet et al19 found that among providers who do the benefit of additional research and theoretical
forensic evaluations, approximately 90% use some discussion about this subject has yielded an
combination of standalone and embedded PVTs in increasingly conservative and refined approach to
forensic evaluations and approximately 80% do so interpreting performance validity failures. One
in clinical evaluations; among providers with no example of this refinement is reflected in the 2012
forensic practice whatsoever, approximately 70% Slick and Sherman update21 of the original 1999
use some combination of PVTs with their patients. Slick, Sherman, and Iverson criteria22 for diagnos-
An important consideration for the combination ing malingered neurocognitive dysfunction. The
of PVTs that a clinician may choose to administer is update responded to a number of critiques of the
understanding the sensitivity and specificity or original criteria that related to both newly gained
range of applicability of various indicators. methodological improvements in PVT assessment
Although the psychometric quality of embedded as well as expanded conceptual and diagnostic
indicators is usually fairly good, they are often less notions of malingering.23–25 In clinical practice, the
sensitive than standalone measures and they are frequency of diagnosed definite malingered neuro-
frequently validated using a more restricted pop- cognitive dysfunction is typically limited to a
ulation compared with standalone PVTs. Accord- minority of individuals who are being seen in for-
ingly, the value of failed embedded PVTs is quite ensic settings, with an even lower number in gen-
clear when appropriately applied but tends to carry eral clinical settings. Data from a survey of mem-
less weight than standalone tests with regard to a bers of the National Academy of Neuropsychology
possible diagnosis of performance invalidity. A found estimated rates of probable and definite
related point is that increasing the numbers of malingering at 3% and 1% of cases, respectively,
PVTs in a given battery improves sensitivity to task with only a minority of respondents (29%) indicating
invalidity and the presence of multiple PVT fail- a willingness to make this diagnosis.17 This limited
ures, either standalone or embedded, has been willingness in part reflects that one of the greatest
shown to provide improved detection of significant challenges to the detection of malingering lies in our
performance validity issues.20 That said, increasing ability to establish the intent of the examinee/
the number of PVTs in a given battery also has the patient to feign symptoms for secondary gain, as
potential to increase the probability of tipping the required by the criteria, and indeed some have
scales toward a false-positive diagnosis of failed argued that determining intent may not be scien-
performance validity testing, if not appropriately tifically plausible.26,27 A conservative approach is all
considered. As will be discussed in greater depth in the more important given the potentially negative
the second installment of this series, ongoing neu- impact of a false-positive malingering diagnosis.
ropsychological research is now providing clinicians As discussed previously, there has also been
with a better understanding of appropriate deci- increased understanding that factors besides con-
sion-making criteria when utilizing multiple PVTs scious intent can negatively impact scores on PVTs,
simultaneously. particularly for examinees in clinical care settings.
Psychological factors such as somatoform disorders/
conversion disorders are certainly a consideration,
INTERPRETATION OF PVT FAILURES
if present, as evidenced by a well-established asso-
A substantial and impressive body of evidence on ciation between psychogenic nonepileptic seizures
neuropsychology PVTs obtained over the last and failed PVTs.28 Other psychological conditions
30 years clearly demonstrates that failed PVTs such as depression or posttraumatic stress disorder
overwhelmingly occur more often in cases where are typically not a sufficient explanation for PVT
there is incentive/secondary gain present (eg, failure, with issues related to performance validity
compensation/disability seeking) than in those or effort being far more likely to account for low
cases in which secondary gain is not present. That PVT scores in these conditions, with the possible
said, an evolving understanding of the complexities exception of extreme cases such as those involving
involved in the clinical interpretation of failed PVTs vegetative depression.29 Short of psychopathology,
has also developed. Whereas PVTs were once widely it has been argued that a patient’s interest in

Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2 March 2017 137

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

undergoing the evaluation and his or her beliefs Nonetheless, the possibility of poor effort in this
about the fairness of the evaluation and resultant patient population should not be overlooked either.
investment in the testing process sometimes As demonstrated by Chafetz et al,33 the vast majority
account for performance validity issues.30 However, of patients with low IQs who were motivated to per-
we would note that such explanations for PVT form well on cognitive testing passed typical PVTs,
failure are likely the exception rather than the rule. whereas patients with similar low IQs who were
It is important to note that the well-informed neu- potentially motivated to appear impaired demon-
ropsychologist has a wealth of PVT research data as strated a much greater failure rate.
well as case-specific clinical data to rely upon when Even with the benefit of consistent PVT failures,
attempting to make judgments regarding these providers should be cautious given the limitations
more challenging cases and will of course also need mentioned above when considering use of the term
to consider the possibility of mixed presentations in “malingering” in a given individual, except poten-
which both pathology and invalid responding exist tially on the rare occasion when the evidence for
simultaneously. As demonstrated by the previously this is nearly irrefutable. Boone34 suggested criteria
noted position statements and survey data, the for malingering that include an established history
voluminous PVT research of the last 30 years has of consciously false reporting by the patient/exam-
provided neuropsychologists with the confidence inee, clear indication of secondary gain, and scores
that these measures are highly valuable tools that on PVTs well below established cutoffs or even more
have greatly increased the value of neuro- significantly, below chance levels of performance
psychological assessment. Continuing research (which many infer demonstrates the intent of the
efforts should push toward ever improving classi- patient/examinee to purposefully pick the incorrect
fication rates and greater guidance regarding more answer). Sharland and Gfeller17 found that, in lieu
challenging mixed or ambiguous presentations. of diagnosing malingering, 85% of neuro-
The high frequency use of PVTs across forensic psychologists they surveyed described failed PVT
and patient-focused care settings is not to suggest results as representing performances that are
that all clinical patients must undergo performance inconsistent with the injury sustained, and 59% to
validity testing irrespective of their clinical pre- 66% of respondents stated that these cognitive
sentation, or that PVT failures should apply equally performances are invalid and that no conclusion
across patient groups. Patients potentially suffering about true cognitive function can be drawn. How-
severe cognitive deficits present a particular chal- ever, 81% of respondents also indicated a willing-
lenge for empirical floor PVTs. For example, those ness to state that test results suggest exaggeration,
with genuine severe memory impairments are at and 23% endorsed use of language to suggest that
increased risk for false-positive findings on certain test results indicate malingering. A more recent
PVTs that involve a mild degree of memory survey suggested a shift in this practice in recent
demand. Fortunately, additional PVT detection years, with 82% to 91% of respondents endorsing
strategies such as performance curve discrepancies interpretation of failed PVTs as indicative of test
can often be used to distinguish these instances of results being inconsistent with the severity of
genuine impairment from invalid responding, injury, that no firm conclusions can be drawn, or
reducing the potential for false-positive errors.31 that test data are invalid.8 Only 65% of respondents
Like those with dementia, individuals with partic- endorsed using language suggesting that test
ularly low IQs have also been found to be at risk for a results indicate exaggeration, and only 11% of this
false-positive diagnosis of dissimulation on PVTs, same sample endorsed use of language suggesting
and clinicians are encouraged to consider this in their that test results indicate malingering. These find-
cut-off selection and interpretation of PVT scores.32 ings confirmed the authors’ sense of a trend toward
In some such instances, “failed” PVTs may represent utilizing PVT results in most instances to help
a direct function of the individual’s cognitive weak- establish the validity (or lack thereof) of the cogni-
nesses. As such, the clinical data acquired in the tive test data which make up the bulk of the
course of the evaluation (eg, on attention and execu- examination process. Irrespective of the explan-
tive and memory functions) may still be reasonably ation provided for failed PVTs, it should be under-
accurate reflections of those cognitive domains. stood that once a performance validity issue is

138 March 2017 Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

identified, clinical test scores (eg, those on tests of play in the course of neuropsychological evaluation.
attention and executive and memory functions) We have come to understand our inability to accu-
which fall in the “impaired,” “deficit,” or “deficient” rately detect issues of reduced effort/task engage-
range can no longer be considered interpretable ment without appropriate measures, and the
because it remains unknown whether those scores frequency of this issue in both forensic and clinical
are a reflection of actual cognitive difficulty, insuf- contexts. Accordingly, performance validity testing
ficient effort/task engagement, or some combination has been a significant area of growth and develop-
thereof. In contrast, scores that fall in the normal ment from both a research and clinical perspective
ranges of performance are typically regarded as and is now strongly recommended by leading
reasonably interpretable as a basal level of cogni- national associations in the field. The authors’ own
tive ability, though even these “normal” scores may experience is that physicians are increasingly
be an underestimate of actual cognitive functions. aware of this trend, as reflected by referral ques-
Ultimately, the outcome in such instances is to tions to neuropsychologists in which concerns about
severely limit the neuropsychologist’s ability to performance validity are being considered. Many in
make conclusions about the examinee’s cognitive neuropsychology accept this as part of the neuro-
status. In clinical cases, this in turn limits the psychologists’ clinical role in light of the significant
provider’s ability to offer recommendations to the advances made in neuropsychology PVT science
patient or the referring physician with regard to over the last few decades. Indeed, this focus in
cognitive issues (eg, cognitive rehabilitation). neuropsychology over recent years has created a
substantial evidence base for making these deter-
minations, distinguishing the field in the world of
PVT DISCLOSURE
health care. Nonetheless, neuropsychologists will
Finally, on the topic of whether to introduce continue to push for even finer refinement in
patients to the notion that PVTs will be adminis- research and clinical practice moving forward.
tered, Sharland and Gfeller17 found that only 22%
of respondents do so. More recent data from Martin
et al8 indicated that 38% of respondents provide a
warning about PVTs. Although this represents an REFERENCES
increase, this approach is still only practiced by a 1. Obituary of Sir John Collie, C.M.G., M.D. Br Med J.
minority of providers and probably with good rea- 1935;1:807–808.
2. Levy A. Compensation neurosis rides again. Brain Inj.
son. A review by Youngjohn et al35 suggested that 1992;6:401–410.
doing so does not actually improve effort but simply 3. Waddell G, McCulloch JA, Kummel E, et al. Nonorganic
makes those providing insufficient effort more dif- physical signs in low-back pain. Spine. 1980;5:117–125.
4. Rogers R. Clinical Assessment of Malingering and
ficult to identify. Instead, the authors recommend Deception. New York: Guilford Press; 2008.
asking examinees if there is anything impeding 5. Hathaway SR, McKinley JC. A multiphasic personality
their ability to answer questions accurately to the schedule (Minnesota): I. Construction of the schedule.
J Psychol. 1940;10:249–254.
best of their ability besides their cognitive issues, 6. Frederick RI. A review of Rey’s strategies for detecting
and encouraging them to give their best effort. malingered neuropsychological impairment. J Forens
Although there is certainly room for discussion on Neuropsychol. 2003;2:1–25.
7. Larrabee GJ. Assessment of malingering. In: Larrabee
the merits of introducing PVTs at the outset of GJ, editor. Forensic Neuropsychology: A Scientific
testing to examinees, as a general rule the authors Approach, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press;
tend to err on the side of caution in this regard and 2012:pp. 116–159.
8. Martin PK, Schroeder RW, Odland AP. Neuropsycholo-
heed the warnings of Youngjohn et al,35 in an effort gists’ validity testing beliefs and practices: a survey of
to maximize the probability of valid test results. North American professionals. Clin Neuropsychol.
2015;29:741–776.
9. Faust D, Hart KJ, Guilmette TJ, et al. Neuropsycholo-
CONCLUSIONS gists’ capacity to detect adolescent malingerers. Prof
Psychol Res Pr. 1988;19:508–515.
Over the last few decades, the field of neuro- 10. Suhr J, Spickard B. Pain-related fear is associated with
cognitive task avoidance: exploration of the cogniphobia
psychology has gained a far greater appreciation for construct in a recurrent headache sample. Clin Neuro-
the critical role that performance validity issues psychol. 2012;26:1128–1141.

Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2 March 2017 139

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


LAW AND PSYCHIATRY

11. Heaton RK, Smith HH, Lehman RA, et al. Prospects for implications for forensic practice and research. Psychol
faking believable deficits on neuropsychological testing. Inj Law. 2011;4:147–156.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1978;46:892–900. 24. Slick DJ, Tan JE, Sherman EMS, et al. Malingering and
12. Binder LM, Willis SC. Assesment of motivation after related conditions in pediatric populations. In: Davis AS,
financially compensable minor head trauma. Psychol editor. Handbook of Pediatric Neuropsychology. New
Assess J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991;3:175–181. York: Springer; 2010:pp. 457–470.
13. Hiscock M, Hiscock CK. Refining the forced-choice 25. Larrabee GJ, Greiffenstein MF, Greve KW, et al. Refin-
method for the detection of malingering. J Clin Exp ing diagnostic criteria for malingering. In: Larrabee GJ,
Neuropsychol. 1989;11:967–974. editor. Assessment of Malingered Neuropsychological
14. Bianchini KJ, Mathias CW, Greve KW. Symptom validity Deficits. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007:
testing: a critical review. Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;15:19–45. pp. 334–371.
15. Mittenberg W, Patton C, Canyock EM, et al. Base rates of 26. Drob SL, Meehan KB, Waxman SE. Clinical and
malingering and symptom exeggeration. J Clin Exp conceptual problems in the attribution of malingering in
Neuropsychol. 2002;24:1094–1102. forensic evaluations. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law. 2009;
16. Bush SS, Ruff RM, Tröster AI, et al. Symptom validity 37:98–106.
assessment: practice issues and medical necessity: NAN 27. American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and
Policy & Planning Committee. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition
2005;20:419–426. (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: APA; 2013.
17. Sharland MJ, Gfeller JD. A survey of neuropsychologists’ 28. Drane DL, Williamson DJ, Stroup ES, et al. Cognitive
beliefs and practices with respect to the assessment impairment is not equal in patients with epileptic and
of effort. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;22:213–223. psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia. 2006;47:
18. Heilbronner RL, Sweet JJ, Morgan JE, et al. American 1879–1886.
Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology Consensus Confer- 29. Green P, Merten T. Noncredible explanations of non-
ence Statement on the neuropsychological assessment of credible performance on symptom validity tests. In:
effort, response bias, and malingering. Clin Neuropsy- Carone DA, Bush SS, editors. Mild Traumatic Brain
chol. 2009;23:1093–1129. Injury: Symptom Validity Assessment and Malingering.
19. Sweet JJ, Benson LM, Nelson NW, et al. The American New York: Springer Publishing; 2013:pp. 73–99.
Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology, National Academy 30. Silver JM. Invalid symptom reporting and performance:
of Neuropsychology, and Society for Clinical Neuro- what are we missing? NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;36:
psychology (APA Division 40) 2015 TCN Professional 463–469.
Practice and ‘Salary Survey’: professional practices, 31. Green P, Montijo J, Brockhaus R. High specificity of the
beliefs, and incomes of US neuropsychologists. Clin Word Memory Test and Medical Symptom Validity Test
Neuropsychol. 2015;29:1069–1162. in groups with severe verbal memory impairment. Appl
20. Larrabee GJ. Aggregation across multiple indicators Neuropsychol. 2011;18:86–94.
improves the detection of malingering: relationship to 32. Dean AC, Victor TL, Boone KB, et al. The relationship of
likelihood ratios. Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;22:666–679. IQ to effort test performance. Clin Neuropsychol.
21. Slick DJ, Sherman EMS. Differential diagnosis of 2008;22:705–722.
malingering and related clinical presentations. In: Sher- 33. Chafetz MD, Prentkowski E, Rao A. To work or not to
man EMS, Brooks BL, editors. Pediatric Forensic Neuro- work: motivation (not low IQ) determines symptom
psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012: validity test findings. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2011;26:
pp. 113–135. 306–313.
22. Slick DJ, Sherman EM, Iverson GL. Diagnostic criteria 34. Boone KB. Assessment of Feigned Cognitive Impairment:
for malingered neurocognitive dysfunction: proposed A Neuropsychological Perspective. New York: Guilford
standards for clinical practice and research. Clin Neuro- Press; 2007.
psychol. 1999;13:545–561. 35. Youngjohn JR, Lees-Haley PR, Binder LM. Comment:
23. Rogers R, Bender SD, Johnson SF. A critical analysis of warning malingerers produces more sophisticated malin-
the MND criteria for feigned cognitive impairment: gering. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14:511–515.

140 March 2017 Journal of Psychiatric Practice Vol. 23, No. 2

Copyright © 2017 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like