You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Development Economics


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/devec

Violence, psychological trauma, and risk attitudes: Evidence from victims of


violence in Colombia
Andres Moya
Economics Department, Universidad de los Andes, Calle 19A #1–37E, Bogot
a, 111711, Colombia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Traumatic experiences can alter individuals' behavior and deplete their ability to make economic decisions. In this
Risk aversion article, I analyze the effects of violence on risk attitudes. For this purpose, I sampled a group of victims of violence
Violence in Colombia and collected data on the severity and temporal proximity of their exposure to violence and on their
Psychological trauma symptoms of psychological trauma. I find that more severe violence brings about higher levels of risk aversion in
Colombia the gains and ambiguity domains but that the behavioral effect is not permanent. I also find that the effect is
driven by severe anxiety disorders, which is consistent with studies in psychology. The results suggest that
violence can affect poverty dynamics through a behavioral channel. (JEL: D1; C9; O1; I1; I3).

Wars, civil conflicts, and large-scale episodes of violence can leave a Raghunathan and Pham, 1999).2 Taken together, these two strands of
legacy of poverty. Although macro-oriented studies find that war-torn literature suggest that the experience of severe and recent violence can
countries and regions are able to recover in the long term (Brakman bring about higher levels of risk aversion by inducing anxiety disorders.
et al., 2004; Davis and Weinstein, 2002; Miguel and Roland, 2011), Hence, violence can alter victims' economic decisions and contribute to
recent microlevel evidence indicates that individual victims can be thrust their vulnerability to poverty through a behavioral channel.
into poverty through a variety of mechanisms that stem from violence, To analyze the effects of violence on risk aversion, I sampled a group
including loss of income and productive assets, forced displacement, of 284 victims of violence in Colombia. As I describe in detail in section 2,
child malnutrition, and interruption of education.1 Identifying such I collected data on individual decisions made during an incentivized lab-
channels is fundamental to designing suitable policies that help victims of in-the-field experiment; the severity and temporal proximity of
violence to recover. household-level exposure to different violent events and atrocities, such
In this article, I analyze whether the experience of violence induces as assassinations, massacres, and threats; and symptoms of nine different
higher levels of risk aversion and thus whether it affects poverty dy- psychopathologies, including two anxiety disorders. Subjects in the
namics through a behavioral channel. This hypothesis is motivated by sample experienced intense violence, which brought about severe psy-
research on the psychological consequences of violence and on the effects chological consequences: 94 percent of them were either directly
of emotions on behavior, both of which I discuss in detail in the following victimized or belong to a household in which a member was victimized,
section. Studies in clinical psychology establishes that victims of violence 80 percent experienced a multiplicity of violent events, and more than 30
may suffer an array of psychological disorders, including chronic anxiety, percent were experiencing severe symptoms of anxiety and phobic anx-
depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (Kessler et al., 1995; Vinck iety at the time of the lab-in-the-field experiment.
et al., 2007; Yehuda, 2002). Importantly, psychological trauma follows a Using this detailed data, I find that violence affects risk attitudes at
dose-response relationship—that is, the experience of more severe and the intensive margin, and that such effect is driven by severe symptoms of
recent violence brings about more severe symptoms of trauma (Doctors phobic anxiety. First, more severe and recent violence, the two factors
Without Borders, 2010; Mollica et al., 1988). Moreover, laboratory that mediate psychological trauma, bring about higher levels of risk
studies in social psychology have found that anxiety and fear induce aversion over gains and ambiguity. Second, the effect is not permanent
higher levels of risk aversion, whereas anger has the opposite effect but rather fades out with time. Third, the observed behavioral effect is
(Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Lerner and Tiedens, 2006; Lerner et al., 2015; explained by the way in which more severe and recent violence brings

E-mail address: a.moya@uniandes.edu.co.


1
See Blattman and Miguel (2010) and Iban~ ez and Moya (2016) for reviews of the literature.
2
To analyze the role of emotions on behavior these studies have followed an experimental approach that relies on the administration of random psychological primes, which are meant
to induce specific emotions prior to the behavioral task.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2017.11.001
Received 10 January 2017; Received in revised form 19 October 2017; Accepted 8 November 2017
Available online 10 November 2017
0304-3878/© 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

about severe symptoms of phobic anxiety, which in turn induce higher suggested that violence toward civilians was indiscriminate (Centro
levels of risk aversion. Taken together, these results are consistent with Nacional de Memoria Hist orica [CNMH], 2013). Second, in the empirical
the hypotheses that follow the research outlined above on the dose- analysis of this paper, I follow Bellows and Miguel (2009) and demon-
response relationship between violence and psychological trauma, and strate that the variations in the severity and temporal proximity of
on the role of emotions on risk aversion. I discuss the results in detail in violence were not correlated with observable ex-ante characteristics.
Sections 3 and 4. Third, I analyze the robustness of the results on a subsample of victims
This research builds upon a growing body of literature on the who were victimized en masse in the crossfire of armed combat and for
behavioral consequences of violence, and especially upon two recent whom the severity of violence was as good as exogenous. Finally, by
articles that have analyzed the effects on risk aversion (Bauer et al., 2014; testing the psychological mechanism, I demonstrate that the results are
Bellows and Miguel, 2009; Blattman, 2009; Callen et al., 2014; Cassar not driven by unobserved heterogeneity but rather by the psychological
et al., 2013; Cassar et al., 2014; and Voors et al., 2012).3 In Burundi, consequences of violence. Taken together, these strategies allow me to
Voors et al. (2012) found that a higher share of deaths at the community overcome the methodological difficulties inherent in the study of
level over the 1993–2003 period induced less risk aversion, but that the violence and to provide a picture of the behavioral consequences of
severity of the exposure to violence at the household level did not bring violence that is consistent with research in psychology.
any significant effects. In Afghanistan, Callen et al. (2014) combined The results of this article have important implications for policy
administrative and individual-level reports on terrorist attacks over the design and for future research. First, they suggest a different channel
2002–2010 period with random psychological primes, as in the studies in through which violence can affect poverty dynamics and emphasize the
social psychology cited above. Their results indicate that the exposure to need to incorporate mental health programs into the aid packages for
violence at the district or individual level did not lead to significant ef- victims of violence, internally displaced persons (IDPs), and refugees.
fects on subjects' preference for certainty. However, when subjects had Second, they demonstrate that risk attitudes are not a stable and exoge-
been exposed to violence and were asked to recall fearful experiences, nous preference. In doing so, the results speak to the research in
their certainty premium increased. In other words, Callen et al. found behavioral economics that contests the canonical models of behavior
that the exposure to violence increased the susceptibility to being under uncertainty (Ledoux, 1998; Loewenstein, 2000; Loewenstein and
emotionally primed. Lerner, 2003). I conclude and discuss these implications in section 5.
While my results seem at odds with those of Voors et al. (2012) and
Callen et al. (2014), a closer look at the samples of each study indicates 1. Violence and psychological trauma in Colombia
that we answer closely related but different questions. For instance,
Voors et al. (2012) and Callen et al. (2014) explore the effects of violence Colombia has been ravaged by violence since the late 1940s. The
among residents of war-torn districts or communities who may or may origins of the modern conflict can be traced to La Violencia (1948–1958),
not have been victimized.4 By contrast, I analyze the effects of violence a period of civil violence triggered by political disputes and tensions
among victims who suffered a multiplicity of violent events and were between landowners and peasants. Despite the signing in 1958 of a peace
experiencing severe symptoms of psychological trauma. This distinction agreement between the two main political parties, violence persisted in
allows us to reconcile the coexistence of seemingly opposite results and to rural areas, and different guerrilla and paramilitary groups emerged soon
understand that these two bodies of work complement each other—this after. Since the mid-1980s, violence toward civilians escalated as these
is, we provide evidence of the effects of violence on risk attitudes for groups clashed throughout the country in struggles for the control of
populations that experienced a different level of violence. territories suitable for producing and trafficking illegal drugs and
I thus contribute to the literature in the following ways: First, I pro- extracting natural resources. Significant changes have occurred during
vide evidence on the behavioral responses for victims of violence that the last decade: paramilitary groups were demobilized in 2006; neo-
complements the findings of previous studies. Second, I highlight that the paramilitary factions fought to control regions previously under para-
behavioral effects of violence emerge at the intensive margin as a func- military control; and the Colombian government and the Revolutionary
tion of the severity and temporal proximity of the experience of violence. Armed Forces of Colombia (FARC) signed a peace agreement in 2016.
Finally, I provide a rigorous measurement of the psychological conse- Although the Colombian conflict has been categorized as low in-
quences of violence, which allows me to identify the channel that ex- tensity, 8.3 million civilians have been victimized since 1985, including
plains how it influences risk attitudes. To the best of my knowledge, this 7.3 million IDPs (National Victims Unit, n.d). The majority of these IDPs
is the first article that links data on risk attitudes and symptoms of psy- were forced to migrate from rural to urban areas. The number of IDPs in
chological trauma and that tests the channel through which violence Colombia is the highest in the world (United Nations High Commissioner
affects behavior. for Refugees [UNHCR], 2016) and represents 15 percent of the country's
The results outlined above portray the causal effects of violence if the population, making IDPs the most visible group of victims in Colombia.
patterns of selection governing violence did not change over time. Hence, Violence and displacement bring about disturbing consequences and
the results would be invalidated only if armed groups had originally can drive victims into poverty. Victims suffer a severe loss of physical,
targeted less risk-averse subjects but then switched their strategy and in social, and human assets, and these losses undermine their ability to
recent years targeted more risk-averse subjects. This is unlikely. None- generate income (Iban ~ ez and Moya, 2010a, 2010b). Traumatic episodes
theless, I have implemented different strategies to demonstrate that the of violence and displacement also have profound psychological conse-
patterns of violence were exogenous to ex-ante risk attitudes. First, quences, including psychopathological levels of anxiety, depression, and
during fieldwork I sampled the victims in regions that were being con- posttraumatic stress disorder (Doctors Without Borders, 2006; Londo~ no
tested over the period of analysis and where anecdotal evidence et al., 2012; Richards et al., 2011; Shultz et al., 2014). While the sus-
ceptibility to psychological trauma varies among individuals as a func-
tion of their psychological and social resources, it also follows a
3 dose–response relationship: more severe and recent episodes of violence
Related research has also analyzed the effect of genetics, financial shocks, natural
disasters, and stress on risk attitudes, impatience, and prosocial behavior (Cesarini et al., increase the likelihood and severity of symptoms of trauma (Mollica
2009; Cameron and Shah, 2015; Castillo and Carter, 2011; Cassar et al., 2011; Eckel et al., et al., 1998). For instance, the incidence of chronic anxiety in Colombia is
2009; Kandasamy et al., 2014; Malmendier and Nagel, 2011; Page et al., 2014). 2.6 times higher for victims than for bystanders (Doctors Without Bor-
4
For example, in Callen et al. (2014), 24 percent of the subjects reported that there was ders, 2010).
a successful terrorist attack in their neighborhood in the previous 5 years. Likewise, in
Voors et al. (2012), 34 percent of the subjects reported that someone in their household
The prevalence of anxiety among victims of violence in Colombia
had been victimized at some point between 1993 and 2003–at least 7 years before the suggests that violence can induce higher levels of risk aversion. This idea
field experiment. is, of course, at odds with the canonical models of behavior under

16
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

uncertainty. According to such models, decisions are driven solely by a to minimize concerns that violence was targeted.
cognitive process in which individuals address the desirability and like- Second, I used administrative data from the National Victims Unit to
lihood of uncertain outcomes based on predetermined and exogenous identify the municipalities of residence of victims who had been dis-
risk preferences (DellaVigna, 2009; Loewenstein, 2000). However, placed from the vicinity of the above corridors. Initially, I identified three
research in social psychology has demonstrated that decisions result departmental capitals and six municipal centers. Yet, due to increasing
instead from an interplay between cognitive and emotional inputs and levels of violence, I was only able to visit two of the departmental capitals
are often driven by the individual's emotional state (Ledoux, 1998; and two of the municipal centers.7
Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003). Moreover, these studies find that fear I followed a protocol that addressed some of the challenges of con-
and anxiety bring about higher levels of risk aversion, while anger has the ducting fieldwork in contexts of on-going civil conflict and with victim-
opposite effect (Lerner et al., 2015; Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Lerner and ized populations. These include interacting with populations who may
Tiedens, 2006; Raghunathan and Pham, 1999). Hence, risk aversion is want to remain anonymous or may have low levels of trust towards
not a stable preference but is rather an attitude that varies according to outsiders, ensuring the safety of both participants and enumerators, and
different emotional states. collecting sensitive information on the experiences of violence. Below I
In the laboratory, the effect of emotions on risk attitudes has been describe the process followed to contact the sample of victims, and in the
established using random primes to induce specific emotional states. This following subsection I discuss the protocol to collect sensitive informa-
is precisely the approach of Callen et al. (2014), who, in addition to tion and protect enumerators and participants.
collecting data on exposure to violence, implemented random recollec- In each location, I reached out to the local government officials,
tions of fearful experiences. In the context of victims of violence, changes ombudsmen and Catholic priest. All of them had close relations with the
in risk attitudes should be instead explained by psychological trauma, communities and the latter two were recognized and trusted by victim-
which triggers a series of processes that affect individual behavior, ized and displaced communities. These actors organized community
especially under circumstances of stress and uncertainty. These include meetings in which they explained the project's broad objectives. Then, I
physiological changes in the brain, the inhibition of emotional regula- read an informed consent script in which I asked individuals if they were
tion, and the disruption of cognitive and socio-emotional functions willing to answer questions on their experiences of violence, and high-
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Kessler et al., 1995; Mcewen lighted that their participation was voluntary and they could opt-out at
and Sapolsky, 1995; Yehuda, 2002). any time. Finally, I screened interested attendees to ensure that they had
Two hypotheses follow: First, since psychological trauma is mediated been displaced from the rural areas near the three geographical corridors
by the severity and temporal proximity of violence, risk attitudes should mentioned above, and I invited one-third of them to participate.
follow a similar dose–response relationship—that is, more severe and Subjects who declined the invitation –around 7 percent of those
recent violence should lead to higher levels of risk aversion. Second, we invited– had been victimized and displaced in the previous six months
should observe shifts towards higher levels of risk aversion among vic- and it is thus likely that they were suffering from higher levels of trauma.
tims of violence who experience severe symptoms of anxiety disorders. In such case, the patterns of self-selection would imply that I sampled the
relatively less traumatized and that the resulting bias would work against
2. Sample selection and data the hypothesis that violence induces psychological trauma and risk
aversion. Nevertheless, the rate of non-response is low considering the
Between January and July 2011, I conducted fieldwork in the de- context. Moreover, as I discuss shortly, I was able to sample victims who
partments of Bolivar, C
ordoba, and Sucre, in Colombia's Atlantic region, experienced various degrees of violence at different times, and who were
and Tolima, in the Central region, where I sampled a group of victims of experiencing high levels of psychological trauma at the time of the data
forced displacement. I focused on IDPs because their displacement was collection.8
triggered by multiple atrocities and violent events and, as discussed Following the protocol above, I sampled 284 victims of violence who
above, they constitute the most visible group of victims of violence had been displaced from the rural areas of 34 different municipalities and
in Colombia. resided in one of the four urban locations where fieldwork was con-
ducted. Importantly, the sample includes 132 individuals from nine
2.1. Sample selection different villages where the populations had been victimized and dis-
placed en masse. Eight of these villages were victimized by cross fire from
The sample was designed in two steps: First, I selected the four de- armed groups that were engaged in combat in the village boundaries. In
partments above, noting that these areas had experienced increasing such episodes of en masse victimization, the severity of the victimization
levels of violence since the early 2000s and that the patterns of violence is thus exogenous to individual and household characteristics.9 Fig. A1 in
had been driven by struggles between armed groups for the control of the Appendix shows the geographical distribution of the sample, iden-
three geographical corridors.5 Consistent with the dynamics of civilian tifying the municipalities from which IDPs were displaced and the in-
victimization in civil conflicts (Kalyvas, 2006), anecdotal evidence sug- tensity of displacement across Colombian municipalities to highlight how
gested that the patterns of violence were indiscriminate in the rural areas the regions where I conducted fieldwork had been heavily affected
near these contested corridors (Human Rights Watch, 2010; Instituto de by violence.
Estudios para el Desarrollo y la Paz [Indepaz], 2011; National Ombuds-
man's Office, 2009).6 Hence, I conducted fieldwork in these departments

7
Departmental capitals: Sincelejo and Ibague in the departments of Sucre and Tolima,
5
In the Atlantic region, several neo-paramilitary groups emerged after the paramilitary respectively. Urban centers: Tierralta and Montelíbano in the department of C ordoba.
8
peace demobilization in 2006. These groups clashed over control of the Nudo del Paramillo The low rate of non-response, and the ability to effectively interact with victims of
and Montes the María—two corridors with valuable lands and favorable conditions for the violence, is likely explained by the support of the local ombudsmen and Catholic priests,
illegal drug trade (Human Rights Watch, 2010; Indepaz, 2011). In the Central region, the by providing subjects with an opportunity to pick the survey date that worked better for
Colombian military intensified its operations against the FARC in 2004. The FARC them during a three-week window, and by conducting all fieldwork at the local church,
responded by retreating to the Ca~ non de las Hermosas—a corridor in the Central Andes that which constituted a safe and private environment.
9
facilitates the movement of troops and the trafficking of illegal drugs to the Pacific Ocean The characteristics of massive displacements are illustrated by the emblematic
(National Ombudsman's Office, 2009). massacre of Bojaya, Choc o, where guerrilla and paramilitary forces clashed in this town of
6
Former combatants acknowledge that armed groups in these regions viciously and approximately 1000 inhabitants. Guerrilla forces launched improvised mortars toward the
indiscriminately victimized civilians as a strategy of war to spread fear among the pop- paramilitaries, and one of these exploded in the local church, where over half of the
ulation and undermine their opponents' popular support. By doing this, armed groups population was taking shelter. Of this population, 119 civilians were killed, 98 more were
were able to control such strategic and contested regions (CNMH, 2013). wounded, and all survivors forcibly migrated days later (CNMH, 2013).

17
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

2.2. Data of 8.31 violent events in the full sample and 7.64 in the sample of subjects
who were victimized en masse. For the full sample, the most frequent
Data collection took place as follows: in each location enumerators events included being caught in the cross fire of armed combat (59%),
first administered a household survey at the local church during receiving threats (51%), suffering the assassination of a household
weekdays. After all surveys were completed, subjects were invited to a member (26%), suffering a violent attack (16%), and/or experiencing
weekend group activity. There, enumerators administered the victim- and surviving a massacre (10%). For the subjects who were victimized en
ization questionnaire and psychometric scale in private, and I then led masse, 87 percent of them reported that they had been victimized during
an incentivized lab-in-the-field experiment to elicit risk attitudes.10 armed combat between two rival groups, supporting the idea that the
Both stages of the data collection took place at the local church to severity of the victimization is exogenous for this group. Third, the
guarantee safe, private, and trusted environments for respondents and average time between the victimization and the field experiment was 2.4
enumerators. I describe each instrument below. For descriptive pur- years for the full sample and less than a year for the victims of mass
poses, and to contextualize the empirical strategy, I provide sample violence; the latter includes 32 subjects who were victimized 10 days
statistics for an arbitrary stratification of the sample: individuals whose before the data collection.
households experienced a number of violent events below or above the
median, who are identified as moderately or severely victimized, 2.2.2. Psychometric scale
respectively. After the victimization questionnaire, enumerators privately admin-
istered a locally adapted version of the Symptoms Checklist 90 R (SCL 90-
2.2.1. Household victimization questionnaire R), which measured symptoms of nine different psychopathologies in the
At the beginning of the group activity, enumerators privately three months prior to the field experiment.13 I chose the adapted version
administered a victimization questionnaire, which captured whether a of the SCL 90-R over other common scales of posttraumatic stress dis-
household member suffered different conflict-related violent events and order since it captures symptoms of different disorders, including anxi-
the number of times that each event had occurred in the last 10 years. The ety,14 phobic anxiety,15 and hostility.16 Therefore, it allows testing for
data from such questionnaire allows me to measure the severity of the psychological mechanism through which violence affects behav-
violence using the number of violent events suffered by the household, ior—something that has not been analyzed in the literature.
and to use the number of years since the episodes of displacement as a Fig. 1 illustrates the distributions of symptoms of different disorders
proxy for the temporal proximity of violence. While not all violent events for the full sample.17 On the one hand, the data in the figure confirm the
occurred at the same time, subjects stated that their displacement was predictions from the literature in clinical psychology and indicates that
triggered by a peak in violence.11 The years since the episodes of the psychological consequences follow a dose–response relationship: a
displacement therefore capture the temporal proximity of the moment more severe exposure to violence brings about more symptoms of anxiety
when violence was at its highest. and phobic anxiety and a higher score for the global severity index
Eliciting information on victimization experiences has special ethical (GSI).18 For instance, 40 and 34 percent of the subjects in the severely
considerations. In particular, the recollection of traumatic experiences of victimized group score above the critical threshold for phobic anxiety
violence could have induced strong emotional reactions and affected and anxiety disorders, respectively, and are therefore at risk of devel-
participants. For these reasons, at the beginning of the weekend activity I oping clinical psychopathologies. These figures are statistically different
stressed that subjects did not have to answer any specific questions or from those from the moderately victimized group (25 and 32 percent).
entire modules and that their participation was entirely voluntary. In For the massively displaced sample, the extent of trauma is much higher,
addition, prior to fieldwork, I received training in psychological possibly because of the more recent exposure to violence (see Table 1 and
containment, anticipating that some subjects could be emotionally Appendix Table A1). Taken together, average levels for the two anxiety
affected throughout the different stages of fieldwork. Finally, in consul- disorders place the sample of victims of violence in the top 90th
tation with the local ombudsmen and priests, I defined a protocol in case
any subject required special assistance. While no subjects required acti-
vating such protocol, I did have to provide emotional contention to 8 13
The SCL 90-R has reliable psychometric properties and has been widely implemented
subjects after the victimization questionnaire.12 in developing countries and in conflict scenarios (Casullo, 2004). The scale inquires about
Table 1 presents sample statistics on the severity and temporal 90 symptoms associated with psychological trauma, such as headaches, back pains, and
proximity of violence and the incidence of different violent events. uneasiness among others, rather than asking directly about trauma. Responses for each
question are scaled from 0 to 4, indicating a range of no symptoms to daily symptoms in
Overall, the data highlights two main features of this article: namely, that
the last three months. Scores on the relevant questions for each disorder are added and
the sample focuses on victims of violence and captures considerable divided by the total number of questions answered. For the global severity index, this
heterogeneity in the severity and temporal proximity of violence—the corresponds to all 90 questions. This score is converted into a T-score with mean 50 and
two factors that mediate psychological trauma. First, the data reveals that standard deviation 10 (Ti ¼ 10 þ 50  score). Cronbach alpha ¼ 0.94 indicates an
excellent internal consistency—the extent to which all items measure the same constructs.
all subjects had been displaced, 94 percent of them had suffered at least 14
Anxiety: Psychological state of distress and fear (Casullo, 2004). In moderate levels,
one violent event. Second, subjects reported being exposed to an average this is a normal reaction to stress, but it becomes a disorder when it exceeds certain levels.
Anxiety can be caused by many circumstances, including violence and poverty, among
others. Therefore, above-normal levels of anxiety cannot be exclusively associated with
exposure to violence and are also driven by the different circumstances brought about by
10
Sessions were conducted after the surveys were completed in each location to mini- violence and displacement.
15
mize sharing of information regarding the experiments and to control for self-selection Phobic anxiety: Abnormal fear and behavior of avoidance of regular objects or situ-
based on the monetary prizes. This also minimized the time I spent in each municipal- ations. This disorder is characterized by intense fear that is triggered by an unthreatening
ity after communities learned that an outside researcher was handing out monetary prizes, stimulus that brings about recollections of traumatic episodes (Casullo, 2004). Testimonies
which was useful given that security conditions were not ideal. Sessions also elicited ex- of IDPs suggest that some of these stimuli included walking into public spaces or places
pressions of hopelessness regarding expectations of socioeconomic mobility (Moya and with large crowds or being in the dark, since these experiences triggered the recollection
Carter, 2014). of episodes of victimization. Hence, phobic anxiety seems to be more directly tied to the
11
A different version of the victimization questionnaire inquired about the year in which episodes of violence than general anxiety.
16
each event had occurred. During the pilot tests, such a level of detail significantly Hostility: Thoughts, feelings, and actions characterized by the negative anger affect.
17
increased the time of response, and subjects generally stated that most of the violence Table A1 in the Appendix reports the sample means for the continuous T-scores and
occurred in the weeks before their displacement. the percentage of subjects who scored above the critical threshold for each disorder in the
12
All of them decided to continue with the rest of the data collection and participate in full sample and the sample of subjects who were victimized en masse.
18
the lab-in-the-field experiment. In the discussion section, I address whether the victimi- Kolmogorov Smirnov tests indicate that the distributions of anxiety, phobic anxiety,
zation questionnaire implicitly worked as a priming mechanism, as in the psychology and IGS are statistically different between the moderately and severely victimized groups.
literature and in Callen et al. (2014). The distribution for the hostility disorder, in turn, is not different.

18
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Table 1
Exposure to violence.

Full Sample Massively Victimized

Total Moderate Severe Total Moderate Severe

[1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6]

Severity: # of violent events 8.31 2.07 16.73 7.64 2.34 15.09


[16.120] [1.239] [22.070] [11.520] [1.138] [14.980]
Temporal proximity: years since violence 2.40 2.32 2.51 0.94 0.85 1.06
[3.138] [3.072] [3.233] [1.241] [0.652] [1.749]
Victim of (%):
Combats 0.59 0.42 0.81 0.87 0.82 0.94
[0.494] [0.495] [0.395] [0.338] [0.390] [0.231]
Threats 0.51 0.40 0.68 0.29 0.18 0.43
[0.501] [0.490] [0.470] [0.453] [0.390] [0.499]
Assassinations 0.26 0.12 0.44 0.20 0.08 0.37
[0.439] [0.330] [0.499] [0.402] [0.271] [0.487]
Attacks 0.16 0.05 0.31 0.07 0.03 0.13
[0.367] [0.217] [0.464] [0.255] [0.161] [0.339]
Massacres 0.10 0.02 0.21 0.03 0.00 0.07
[0.300] [0.135] [0.408] [0.173] [0.000] [0.264]
Ordered to migrate 0.43 0.36 0.53 0.38 0.45 0.28
[0.496] [0.483] [0.501] [0.486] [0.501] [0.452]
Other 0.27 0.12 0.46 0.15 0.07 0.27
[0.443] [0.330] [0.500] [0.360] [0.250] [0.447]

Observations 284 162 122 132 76 56

Notes: Summary statistics on household-level exposure to violence. Columns 1–3 report sample statistics for the full sample, while columns 4–6 report statistics for subjects who were
victimized en masse. For each sample, the table reports the exposure to violence according to an arbitrary stratification of the data: whether the household was exposed to a number of violent
events that were below (moderate violence) or above (severe violence) the median number of violent events. Standard deviations are reported in brackets.

Fig. 1. Psychological Trauma. Notes: Kernel density estimates for the distribution of the Global Severity Index, and the Anxiety, Phobic Anxiety, and Hostility disorders using the data from
the SCL-90-R scale. As in the previous table, sample statistics are reported according to an arbitrary stratification of the data: whether the household was exposed to a number of violent
events that were below (moderate violence) or above (severe violence) the median number of violent events. The dotted vertical line depicts the critical level above which individuals are
at risk of developing clinical cases for each disorder.

19
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Table 2 neuroticism—the predisposition to experience anxiety—explains risk


Lottery design. attitudes over gains but not over losses. For the latter, behavior is driven
Payoffs Gains Losses Ambiguity by the individual's cognitive ability rather than by any emotional influ-
Red Blue Red Blue Red Blue ence (see for example Rustichini et al., 2011). Hence, if psychological
trauma is indeed the underlying mechanism through which violence
Choice 1 13,000 13,000 7,000 7,000 13,000 13,000
Choice 2 10,000 19,000 10,000 1,000 10,000 19,000
induces changes in individual risk attitudes, we should not observe any
Choice 3 7,000 25,000 13,000 5,000 7,000 25,000 effects over the loss domain. On the contrary, if violence and psycho-
Choice 4 4,000 31,000 16,000 11,000 4,000 31,000 logical trauma do affect loss aversion it would suggest that other mech-
Choice 5 2,000 36,000 18,000 16,000 2,000 36,000 anisms are at play or even to spurious relationships.
Choice 6 0 38,000 20,000 18,000 0 38,000
To help subjects understand the experiment, each domain had a
practice round, lotteries were each depicted as an oval divided into one
Endowment 0 20,000 0
# of balls 5 5 5 5 3: 7 3: 7 red and one blue segment to indicate two outcomes per lottery, and
payoffs were displayed with pictures of the local currency. Subjects were
Notes: All payoffs are in Colombian Pesos (COP). At the time of the experiments, the ex-
change rate was approximately COP1.70 ¼ USD1.00. In the gains and losses task, outcomes told that they would be paid according to their decision in one of three
had an equal probability of occurrence and were determined by picking a ball from a bag domains, which was chosen at random at the end of the session. Earnings
containing five red and blue balls. In the ambiguity task, outcomes were determined by were then determined based on a blind selection of either a red or blue
picking a ball from a bag that included ten balls: three red balls, three blue balls, and four ball from a bag containing 10 balls.21 Subjects were paid according to the
balls that were blindly chosen from a bag containing 50 red and 50 blue balls.
red (blue) payoff in the lottery they chose.22
Fig. 2 reports the proportion of safe choices in each domain for the
same stratification of the data as before, while Appendix Table A2 reports
percentile of the national distribution.19 On the other hand, symptoms of the raw data. On average, the data in the figure indicates that subjects
hostility, which have been found to induce lower levels of risk aversion behave in conformity with the predictions from the literature—that is,
(Lerner and Keltner, 2001), are less severe and do not seem to vary ac- they are risk averse over gains and ambiguity, and risk seeking over
cording to the severity of the victimization. losses. The patterns of behavior, in addition, are in general consistent
with those of Cardenas and Carpenter (2013). Moreover, the data sug-
2.2.3. Elicitation of risk attitudes gests that higher levels of violence are associated with even higher levels
To measure risk attitudes, I implemented an incentivized lottery of risk aversion over gains and ambiguity, but also with more
design over the gains, losses, and ambiguity domains that follows from loss-averse choices.
Cardenas and Carpenter (2013). In each domain, subjects were offered
six lotteries and had to choose one of them. One lottery offered the lowest 2.2.4. Household survey
expected payoffs and zero risk, while the expected payoffs and variance As discussed before, the household survey was administered in pri-
gradually increased for the other lotteries. The choices during the vate in the weeks prior to the weekend activity above. The household
experiment thus revealed individuals' willingness to take risks: more survey captured detailed information on the current and retrospective
risk-averse individuals should choose lotteries with lower expected socioeconomic and demographic characteristics of subjects. Retrospec-
payoffs and less risk. Table 2 describes the lotteries, endowments, and tive questions were included to identify the characteristics of the
payoffs in each domain, which ranged from USD6.50 to household before the episodes of violence and to analyze whether they
USD17.00—approximately one to two days of off-farm wages. were associated with the severity of violence.
The experiment was designed in the following way: The gains Table 3 reports sample statistics and differences between the current
domain, included payoffs with an equal probability of occurrence and and retrospective characteristics of the subjects in the full sample (col-
measures standard risk aversion. Following the standard approach in the umns 1–3) and the sample of subjects victimized en masse (columns 4–6).
literature, the loss domain in this experiment included positive and In general, there are no significant differences between the current and
negative payoffs and a $20,000 endowment. Once the endowment is previolence characteristics of subjects who were moderately and severely
accounted for, expected payoffs are the same as for gains. Yet, because victimized, other than their age. To further demonstrate that the severity
payments are framed as losses, subjects should be loss averse—that is, and temporal proximity to violence are exogenous to ex-ante observable
they should be willing to take additional risks to preserve their endow- characteristics, I follow Bellows and Miguel (2009) and regress both the
ment (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Finally, in the ambiguity domain, number of violent events to which a household was exposed and the
neither the subjects nor the experimenter (myself) knew the first-order years since the peak of violence on a set of ex-ante household charac-
probability distribution. While payoffs were the same as before, ambi- teristics. Results confirm the inference from Table 3: neither the severity
guity regarding the first-order probability distribution should lead to nor the temporal proximity of violence were driven by previolence ob-
more conservative choices (Ellsberg, 1961; Hartley and Phelps, 2012).20 servables (see Appendix Table A3).
The experiment included the three dimensions describe above for two
reasons: First, because the literature consistently has identified different 3. Violence & risk aversion
behavioral patterns in each dimension—that is, lower willingness to take
risk over gains and ambiguity, but a higher willingness over losses. In this section, I estimate the effects of violence on risk attitudes over
Hence, by including the three dimensions, I can observe whether subjects gains and ambiguity and discuss the underlying assumptions for identi-
behave in conformity with such behavioral patterns and whether they fication. Except when noted otherwise, models are estimated using or-
understood the different tasks. Second, and perhaps more important, the dered probit models that rank choices from the safest (Lottery 1) to the
inclusion of the loss dimensions allows a falsification test of the under- riskiest (Lottery 6) and do not assume a specific utility function. A
lying conceptual framework. Studies in neuro-economics find that negative coefficient indicates a positive effect on the probability of

19
Reports from the National Mental Health Survey (2015) indicate that only 5.3 percent
21
of the Colombian population has suffered from severe anxiety disorders over the course of In the gains and loss domains, the bag contained 5 blue and 5 red balls. In the am-
their lives—a much lower percentage than that of the victims of violence surveyed here. biguity domain, 10 balls were introduced into the black bag: three red and three blue balls,
20
In this experimental design, it is not possible to identify separate parameters for gains, and four balls blindly chosen from a bag containing 50 blue and 50 red balls.
22
loss, and ambiguity aversion. Instead, individual choices indicate the reduced-form will- See the appendices B & C for the graphical depictions of the lotteries in each domain
ingness to take risks under each domain. and the experimental protocol.

20
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Fig. 2. Lottery Choices. Notes: Histograms of frequency of lottery choices in each domain for the moderately and severely victimized groups, as defined in Fig. 1. Lottery 1 corresponds to
the lottery with the lowest expected value and zero variance, while Lottery 6 corresponds to the lottery with the highest expected value and variance.

choosing the safest lottery and higher levels of risk aversion. 0 t


yihd ¼ β1 Severityh þ β2 Recencyh þ β3 Severityh  Recencyh þ … Γ1 X i
0 t1
3.1. Results þ Γ2 X h þ νr þ εim
(1)
To analyze the effect of violence on risk attitudes, I estimate the index Columns 1 and 2 of Table 4 report the results of model 1 when I
model 1, where I regress yihd —the lottery choice of individual i, from exploit only the variation in the severity of violence over the gains and
household h, in the experimental domain d (gains; ambiguity)—on ambiguity domains.24 The negative and significant coefficients indicate
Severityh , the standardized number of violent events to which household that more severe violence leads to higher levels of risk aversion in both
h was exposed, Recencyh , the number of years since the episodes of domains. An increase of one standard deviation in the severity of
violence and displacement, and their interaction. The model includes a violence raises the probability of selecting the safest lottery by 5 and 7
t
matrix X i of current covariates, such as each subject's age and gender, percentage points over gains and ambiguity, respectively (see Appendix
whether she is the household head, and the hypothetical earnings in the Table A4 for the average marginal effects [AME]). These effects entail
corresponding practice round to control for their effect on risk atti- considerable 13 and 21 percent changes in the proportion of safe choices
tudes.23 The model also controls for a set of retrospective household relative to the mean.
t1
characteristics X h , including the size of the household's landholdings Columns 2 and 4 report the results of model 1 when I include the
and whether a household member participated in local organizations or temporal proximity of violence and its interaction with the severity of
worked in agriculture. These retrospective characteristics are included violence. The data highlights two important results: First, the severity of
since qualitative evidence finds that they are associated with the likeli- violence is again associated with more conservative choices in the gains
hood of being victimized, although not with the severity of the victimi- and ambiguity domains. For instance, an increase of one standard devi-
zation (CNMH, 2013). Finally, the model includes a region-specific fixed ation in the number of violent events raises the probability of selecting
effect νr and an error term εim , which is clustered at the current munic- the safest lottery by 7 and 10 percentage points over gains and ambiguity,
ipality of residence m and estimated through bootstrapping with 1000 respectively (see Appendix Table A4). These effects account to changes of
repetitions. 17 and 32 percent in the proportion of safe choices relative to the mean.

23
In general, the coefficient of the hypothetical earnings in the preceding practice round
24
is always significant, and I also obtain robust results if I instead control for the color of the For expositional purposes, all tables omit the coefficients for the covariates; full results
ball or the actual choice in the practice round. are available upon request.

21
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Table 3
Sample balance.

Full Sample Massively Victimized

Total Moderate Severe Total Moderate Severe

[1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6]

A. Current Characteristics
Age 41.46 39.96 43.46** 39.31 37.64 41.58
[13.463] [13.081] [13.753] [13.793] [12.766] [14.894]
Male (%) 0.39 0.37 0.41 0.39 0.40 0.38
[0.488] [0.484] [0.494] [0.489] [0.492] [0.489]
Hh head (%) 0.77 0.80 0.73 0.67 0.72 0.61
[0.421] [0.399] [0.446] [0.470] [0.450] [0.493]
Literate (%) 0.82 0.81 0.83 0.78 0.79 0.77
[0.387] [0.395] [0.379] [0.416] [0.410] [0.426]
Indigenous hh 0.17 0.15 0.19 0.19 0.15 0.25
[0.375] [0.362] [0.393] [0.393] [0.354] [0.437]
B. Exante (pre-violence) characteristics
Male-headed hh (%) 0.66 0.65 0.68 0.76 0.76 0.74
[0.474] [0.479] [0.469] [0.432] [0.429] [0.441]
Household size 4.89 4.98 4.78 4.96 4.88 5.07
[2.252] [2.294] [2.199] [2.113] [2.085] [2.165]
Highest level of education in hh (yrs) 8.36 8.52 8.14 7.69 7.42 8.05
[3.812] [3.689] [3.976] [3.793] [3.630] [4.006]
Hh member participated in an organization (%) 0.42 0.39 0.47 0.61 0.59 0.64
[0.495] [0.489] [0.501] [0.489] [0.495] [0.483]
Hh member participated in org. decisions (%) 0.45 0.46 0.43 0.73 0.78 0.65
[0.692] [0.708] [0.672] [0.798] [0.811] [0.783]
Hh head was off-farm laborer (%) 0.47 0.45 0.50 0.53 0.48 0.61
[0.500] [0.499] [0.502] [0.501] [0.504] [0.495]
Hh head worked in agriculture (%) 0.57 0.54 0.60 0.61 0.57 0.66
[0.496] [0.500] [0.492] [0.490] [0.499] [0.478]
Size of lands owned (ha) 8.48 6.67 10.88 5.99 7.18 4.38
[34.868] [16.005] [49.924] [13.674] [14.707] [12.075]

Observations 284 162 122 132 76 56

Notes: Sample balance using data from the household-level survey for the full sample (columns 1–3) and the sample of subjects who were victimized en masse (columns 4–6). Panel A reports
data on subjects' current characteristics while Panel B reports data on subjects' characteristics before the peak of violence and displacement. As in the previous tables, the data is stratified
according to the severity of the exposure to violence. Asterisks in columns 3 and 6 indicate the significance of the mean-difference test between the moderate and severe victimization groups.
Standard deviations are reported in brackets. *p < 0.1. **p < 0.05. ***p < 0.01.

Second, the coefficient on the interaction term indicates that the attitudes. The results would therefore be biased if the patterns of selec-
behavioral effect vanishes over time and does so more quickly for those tion into violence were driven by individual or household characteristics
who were severely victimized. The AME suggest that the effect of that were correlated with ex-ante risk attitudes. However, the analysis of
violence on risk attitudes over gains and ambiguity vanish after Table 3 and Appendix Table A3, discussed in the previous section, sug-
approximately 4 and 6 years, respectively. Since there are relatively few gest that the patterns of violence were not based on observables.
subjects in the sample who had been victimized for over 5 years, these Of course, this does not rule out selection based on unobservables.
two figures are imprecise and should not be taken at face value. Never- Yet, in contested territories such as those where I conducted fieldwork,
theless, the key point is that they indicate that the effect of violence on armed groups lack the information needed to target specific individuals.
risk attitudes is not permanent. For this reason, they turn to indiscriminate violence against civilians as a
In fact, note that after accounting for the temporal proximity of strategy to obtain territorial supremacy (CNMH, 2013).26 The severity of
violence, the AME for the severity of violence increases considerably. violence would thus be driven by unobserved selection if civilians made
Hence, the results of columns 1 and 3, which did not consider the het- themselves more conspicuous through certain behaviors, such as making
erogeneity in the temporal proximity of violence, correspond to average their preferences public, supporting a rival group, or gathering in public
effects of the severity of violence for subjects who were victimized at spaces. In the context of war, such actions would characterize subjects
different times. Once the temporal proximity of violence is incorporated with low levels of fear and risk aversion. The resulting bias would work
into the analysis, results indicate that the effect of violence looms larger against the hypothesis that violence induces higher levels of
but also that it is temporal.25 risk aversion.
More importantly, the analysis of columns 2 and 4 relies on the
exogeneity of the severity and temporal proximity of violence and
3.2. Identification assumptions therefore has weaker identification conditions. In particular, the results
would only be invalid if the patterns of selection changed over time. This
The validity of the results of columns 1 and 3 hinges on the would occur if less risk-averse subjects had been victimized with a higher
assumption that the severity of violence was exogenous to ex-ante risk severity earlier in time, while the more risk averse had been victimized
with a higher severity in recent years. This is unlikely.
25
Nevertheless, I analyze the robustness of the results on the sample of
These results are also robust under different specifications of model 1: first, if I
measure the severity of violence using a victimization index, which was constructed
through principal component analysis that weighs each event differently (Table A5 in the
26
Appendix); and second, if I drop subjects above the 95th, 90th, 85th, or 80th percentiles in According to reports from the Ombudsman's Office and different nongovernmental
the distribution of violence to rule out the possibility that the results are driven by outliers organizations, this is a good depiction of conflict dynamics in the regions where data was
or by subjects who overstated their true experiences of violence. Results are available collected (National Ombudsman's Office, 2009; Human Rights Watch, 2010; Indepaz,
upon request. 2011).

22
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

subjects who were victimized en masse. These subjects were victimized Table 4
with their entire villages in the crossfire of combat between illegal armed Violence & risk aversion.

groups, meaning that the severity of their exposure to violence is as good Ordered Probit Estimates Gains Ambiguity
as random. Results from Appendix Table A6 indicate that despite the [1] [2] [3] [4]
smaller sample, the severity of violence again brings about higher levels
# of violent events 0.14*** 0.19*** 0.24*** 0.32***
of risk aversion. For instance, an increase of one standard deviation in the (standardized) [0.022] [0.050] [0.052] [0.059]
number of violent events brings about 6 and 19 percentage point changes Years since violence 0.00 0.04
in the likelihood of selecting the safest lotteries in the risk and ambiguity [0.02] [0.027]
domains, respectively (see Appendix Table A7 for the AME). These effects # of violent events * years 0.04* 0.05*
since violence [0.02] [0.027]
account for significant and sizeable increases of 16 and 59 percent in the
proportion of more risk-averse choices in these domains. Note that the
Sample Full Full Full Full
effect of violence on risk attitudes in this case is higher than for the full Hh controls Yes Yes Yes Yes
sample possibly because of the more recent experience of violence of Regional Fixed Effect Yes Yes Yes Yes
subjects victimized en masse.27 Observations 282 282 282 282
The results from this section contribute to the literature by illustrating Notes: Estimates from the ordered probit model 1, which ranks lottery choices from the
how the direct experience of violence influences victims' risk attitudes safest gamble (Lottery 1) to the riskiest (Lottery 6). A positive (negative) coefficient in-
and by revealing a considerable heterogeneity in their behavioral re- dicates that the variable of interest has a negative (positive) effect on the probability of
choosing the safest lottery. Columns 1 and 2 focus on the decisions during the gains task,
sponses. First, risk attitudes shift as a function of the severity of the ep-
while Columns 3 and 4 focus on the decisions during the ambiguity task. The table reports
isodes of direct victimization. Second, the behavioral effect is not the estimated coefficients for the severity of violence, measured by the standardized
permanent and instead it gradually vanishes with time. Third, the effects number of all violent events to which a household was exposed, the temporal proximity of
are stronger under the ambiguity domain, when the first order proba- violence, measured by the years since the peak of violence and displacement, and their
interaction. As described in equation (1), each model includes a set of current and ex-ante
bility distribution of payoffs was unknown.
covariates and a regional fixed effect. Current covariates include the subject's age and
gender, whether he or she is the household head, whether the household experienced an
4. Unpacking the mechanism: psychological trauma and risk economic shock or the death of a household member for reasons not related to violence,
aversion and the hypothetical earnings in the previous practice round. Ex-ante (pre-violence)
covariates include the household size, size of land holdings, participation of a household
member in local organizations, and participation of the household in agricultural work.
In this section, I analyze the psychological channel through which Estimated coefficients of the covariates and fixed effect are not reported for expositional
violence affects risk attitudes. For this purpose, I first establish the rela- purposes but are available upon request. Average marginal effects of the severity of
tion between violence and symptoms of different psychopathologies, and violence on the probability of selecting each lottery are reported in Appendix Table A4.
then I test whether anxiety disorders explain the shift in risk attitudes. Standard errors are reported in brackets, and were clustered at the level of the reception site
(n ¼ 4) through a bootstrapping procedure with 1000 replications. *p < 0.10. **p < 0.05.
Finally, I explore two falsification tests of the underlying psychologi-
***p < 0.01.
cal framework.

these two anxiety disorders and are approximately three times higher
4.1. Violence and psychological trauma than the effects for the hostility disorder. Moreover, the coefficients for
the temporal proximity of violence and its interaction with the severity of
To understand how psychological trauma responds to the severity and violence, indicate that the symptoms of both anxiety disorders become
temporal proximity of violence, I estimate model 2 in which I regress the less severe with time. In the case of phobic anxiety, symptoms fade out
T-score for the symptoms of disorder j on the severity and recency of over time independently of the severity of the victimization.28
violence and on their interaction. I focus on symptoms of anxiety, phobic
anxiety, and hostility, given their effects on risk attitudes in the lab. As
before, the model includes a regional fixed effect, and the error term is 4.2. Psychological trauma & risk aversion
clustered at the municipality level and estimated through a bootstrapping
procedure. In addition, I control for the gender of the respondent to ac- Having established the effects of violence on different psychopa-
count for the well-known differences in the susceptibility to psycholog- thologies, I now address whether psychological trauma is the mechanism
ical trauma among men and women. through which violence influences risk attitudes. For this purpose, I first
estimate model 3, in which I regress the lottery choice of individual i,
Tjih ¼ β1 Severityh þ β2 Recencyh þ β3 Severityh  Recencyh þ … β4 Male from household h, in the experimental domain d (yihd ) on PhobicAnxietyi
and Hostilityi , two indicator variables denoting whether subject i scored
þ νr þ εim
above the critical threshold. I focus on phobic anxiety since it is more
(2) closely related to the episodes of victimization than the general anxiety
Table 5 reports the results of model 2 for the full sample and confirms disorder (see footnotes 14 and 15). Likewise, I focus on the hostility dis-
the predictions of the literature in psychology—namely, that more severe order since the laboratory studies cited above find that it brings about
and recent episodes of violence produce more severe symptoms of psy- lower levels of risk aversion, although few subjects score above the
chological trauma, especially for the two anxiety disorders. For instance, corresponding threshold. Again, I include a set of individual household
a one-standard-deviation increase in the number of violent events suf- characteristics (current and ex-ante) and a regional fixed effect, and I
fered by the household increases the T-scores for the anxiety and phobic estimate the error term, clustered at the municipality level, through
anxiety disorders by 1.4 and 1.5 points, respectively. These effects ac- bootstrapping with 1000 repetitions.
count for 0.14 and 0.15 standard deviation increases in the symptoms of 0 t 0 t1
yihmd ¼ β1 PhobicAnxietyi þ β2 Hostilityi þ Γ1 X i þ Γ2 X h þ νr þ εim (3)

27
Panel A of Table 6 reports the results of model 3 above. Overall, the
For this subsample, I fail to reject the null hypothesis when I include the temporal
proximity of violence and its interaction with the severity of violence, although the sign of
results in the table indicate that higher symptoms of phobic anxiety bring
the coefficient still points towards an increase in risk aversion due to violence (see
Table A7). This is likely explained by the limited variation in timing of violence for this
28
subsample. Recall that most subjects in this subsample migrated within a year of the data For the sample of subjects who were victimized en masse, the effect of the severity of
collection (see Table 1), and that they were victimized at the same time as their neighbors, violence on psychological trauma is qualitatively robust but no longer statistically sig-
meaning that the timing of violence only varies across 9 villages. nificant (Appendix Table A8).

23
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Table 5
Violence and psychological trauma.

Anxiety Phobic Anxiety Hostility

[1] [2] [3] [4] [5] [6]

# of violent events (standardized) 1.26** 1.36*** 1.38y 1.48* 0.25 0.54**


[0.544] [0.300] [0.883] [0.853] [0.396] [0.212]
Years since violence 0.52 0.58* 0.08
[0.384] [0.338] [0.128]
# of violent events * years since violence 0.19** 0.20 0.30
[0.077] [0.239] [0.296]

Sample Full Full Full Full Full Full


Regional Fixed Effects Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
R2 0.04 0.09 0.06 0.10 0.04 0.05
Observations 282 282 282 282 282 282

Notes: Each column reports the estimated coefficients of an ordinary least squares (OLS) regression of the T-score of different dimensions of psychological trauma (indicated at the top of the
column) on the severity and temporal proximity of violence and its interaction. Standard errors, which are reported in brackets, were clustered at the municipality of reception (n ¼ 4) and
estimated through bootstrapping using 1000 replications. yp < 0.12. *p < 0.10. **p < 0.05. ***p < 0.01.

Table 6 Table 6 also highlights that subjects who score above the critical
Psychological trauma and risk aversion. threshold for the hostility disorder are less risk averse. This effect is also
Ordered Probit Estimates Gains Ambiguity consistent with the findings of the previously cited studies in social
[1] [2] [3] [4] psychology, which find that emotions of anger lead to lower levels of risk
aversion. However, to the extent that violence has moderate effects on
A. Reduced Form: T-scores above the critical threshold
Phobic Anxiety 0.23** 0.33** 0.20** 0.34**
the incidence of hostility disorders and that a few subjects score above
[0.110] [0.153] [0.101] [0.169] the critical threshold for this psychopathology (see Fig. 1 and Table 5),
Hostility 0.38* 0.53* this result does not contradict the results from Table 4, which indicate
[0.219] [0.277] that by and large violence lead to higher levels of risk aversion.

Sample Full Full Full Full


4.3. Average controlled direct effect of violence
Hh controls Yes Yes Yes Yes
Regional fixed effect Yes Yes Yes Yes
Observations 282 282 282 282 The results from Tables 5 and 6 far indicate that the experience of
B. Mediation Analysis severe and recent violence induces higher levels of phobic anxiety dis-
Direct Controlled Effect of 0.164 0.174 0.183 0.164 orders, which bring about higher levels of risk aversion. While this
Violence [0.115] [0.117] [0.117] [0.115] analysis is a first step in the analysis of the underlying mechanism, it does
not demonstrate the mediating role of psychological trauma. For this
Sample Full Full Full Full
reason, I follow Acharya et al. (2016) who provide a method to estimate
Hh controls Yes Yes Yes Yes
Regional fixed effect Yes Yes Yes Yes
the unbiased average controlled direct effect (ACDE) of a treatment; this
Observations 282 282 282 282 is, the causal effect of a treatment (violence) when the effect of the
mediator (psychological trauma) is already taken into account. The
Notes: Panel A estimates model (3) and explores the effect of scoring above the critical
threshold for each disorder (Ti > 63) on the gains (Columns 1 and 2) and ambiguity do- approach of Acharya et al. (2016) is useful in this context for two reasons:
mains (Columns 3 and 4). Panel B reports the estimated coefficient of the ACDE of violence First, because it allows me to analyze whether anxiety disorders are one
on risk aversion after controlling for the extent of severe phobic anxiety disorders (Columns mechanism through which violence influences risk attitudes, and
1 and 3) or the extent of severe phobic anxiety and hostility disorders (Columns 2 and 4) in
whether there are other mechanisms that contribute to explaining this
the first stage. Covariates are the same as in Table 4. The estimated coefficients of the
covariates and fixed effect are not reported but are available upon request. Standard errors
relationship. Second, because it provides an alternative to the standard
are reported in brackets. For Panel A, they were estimated through a bootstrapping pro- method of testing for mechanisms, where one simultaneously controls for
cedure using 1000 replications and clustering at the level of the reception site (n ¼ 4). For the treatment and mediating variables, but that often leads to biased and
Panel B, errors were estimated following Acharya et al. (2016). *p < 0.10. **p < 0.05. inconsistent estimates.30
***p < 0.01.
The ACDE is estimated through the following two-stage regression: In
the first stage, one transforms the dependent variable by removing the
about higher levels of risk aversion over the gains and ambiguity do- effect of the mediator variable. In the second stage, one estimates the
mains. When I do not control for the extent of hostility disorders (Col- ACDE by regressing the demediated dependent variable on the treatment.
umns 1 and 3), the AME indicate that the probability of selecting the If the null hypothesis is rejected, one can conclude that the treatment
safest lotteries in the gains and ambiguity domains is 8 and 7 percentage influences the variable of interest through other pathways in addition to
points higher, respectively, for subjects experiencing severe symptoms of the proposed mechanism. By contrast, if one cannot reject the null hy-
phobic anxiety (see Appendix Table A9). When I control for both disor- pothesis, this means that treatment has no effect on the variable of in-
ders (Columns 2 and 4), I find a positive effect of 11 percentage points on terest, once the mechanism is accounted for. In other words, that the
the probability of selecting the safest lottery in each domain. These re- effect is exclusively driven by the proposed mechanism.
sults correspond to significant changes of 30 and 35 percent in the pro- I run the following two-stage model to estimate the ACDE of the
portion of safe choices relative to the mean.29 severity of violence and test whether its influence on risk attitudes runs
exclusively through psychological trauma:

29
These results are robust to different specifications, including when I control for the
symptoms of general anxiety, when control for the symptoms of the three psychopathol-
ogies at the same time, or when I focus on the continuous T-scores for both disorders (see
30
Table A11). In addition, results are qualitatively robust when I focus on the subjects who See Acharya et al. (2016) for a detailed discussion on the way in which simultaneously
were victimized en masse. Although the point estimates are not significant for this smaller controlling for the treatment and the proposed mechanism leads to either M-bias or
subsample, the signs if the coefficients are still consistent with the results above. posttreatment bias.

24
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

0 t for the proposed psychological channel. First, I test whether the GSI has
yihmd ¼ β1 Severityh þ β2 PhobicAnxietyi þ β3 Hostilityi þ … Γ1 X i
0 t1 the same effect as phobic anxiety disorders. Following the research in
þ Γ2 X h þ νr þ εim (4) social psychology, we should not observe any effects since the GSI pro-
vides an aggregate measure of psychological trauma and combines
t1 different emotional responses, including anxiety and hostility, which
~yihmd ¼ γ 1 Severityh þ Γ2 'X h þ νr þ μim
have distinct effects on risk aversion. Hence, if the GSI is correlated with
where y~ihmd ¼ yihmd  b β 2 PhobicAnxietyi þ b
β 3 Hostilityi is the demediated risk attitudes this would suggest that violence is a proxy for some of the
measure of risk aversion, and μim; is the consistent error term estimated additional consequences brought about the displacement and victimi-
zation. Nevertheless, I find that neither of these measures have a statis-
through bootstrapping.31 As in the previous models PhobicAnxietyi and
tically significant effect on risk attitudes (Appendix Table A13).
Hostilityi are indicator variables denoting whether subject i scored above
t1 t Second, I address whether the severity and temporal proximity of
the critical threshold for each disorder, X h and X i are the matrices of ex- violence and the incidence of severe anxiety disorders also influence risk
ante and current (posttreatment) covariates, and νr is a regional attitudes over the loss domain. This serves as a falsification test since the
fixed effect.32 studies in neuroeconomics cited before indicate that loss aversion is not
Panel B of Table 6 reports the estimated ACDE of the severity of influenced by emotions. Therefore, if psychological trauma is indeed the
violence on risk attitudes over the gains and ambiguity domains. Col- channel through which violence influences risk attitudes, we should not
umns 1 and 3 report the results when I only consider the effect of the observe any effects on the loss domain. Appendix Table A14 reports the
severe symptoms of phobic anxiety in the first stage of model 4, while results when I replicate the analysis of Tables 4 and 6 above for the loss
Columns 2 and 4 report the results when I also control for the severe domain. Results indicate that neither the severity of violence nor the
symptoms of hostility. The results in the table indicate that the co- experience of severe phobic anxiety or hostility bring about significant
efficients for the ACDE of violence are much smaller than those presented changes in the decisions over the loss domain (Column 1 and 3). How-
in Table 4 and none of them is statistically significant. This means that ever, subjects who had been victimized earlier become more loss averse,
once we account for the effect of the symptoms of phobic anxiety, the especially if they were more severely victimized (Column 2). Taken
severity of violence does not have a significant effect on risk attitudes. In together, the results above suggest that while differences in the temporal
other words, that the effect of violence on risk aversion is in fact proximity to violence may influence loss aversion, they do independently
explained by the severity of anxiety disorders, as it was hypothesized at of the psychological consequences of violence.
the beginning of this article. To the extent that anxiety disorders follow a
dose-response relationship, this result is also consistent with the results
in Table 4.33 4.5. Priming effects & external validity
The analysis carried out in this section contributes to the literature in
different ways. First, it provides first-time evidence on the behavioral An alternative explanation to the results is that they result from a
consequences of violence for victims and highlights considerable het- priming effect induced by the victimization questionnaire. This would
erogeneity based on the severity of symptoms of psychological trauma. occur if subjects who were experiencing higher symptoms of psycho-
Second, it demonstrates the psychological mechanism that explains logical trauma were more affected when revisiting these experiences of
violence influences risk aversion. This is something that had not been violence.35 Nevertheless, if this were the case, the priming effect would
analyzed yet by previous studies in the literature on the behavioral precisely reflect the psychological consequences of violence and their
consequences of violence.34 influence on individual behavior. Moreover, the research on the effects of
psychological trauma on behavior and emotional regulation suggests a
direct pathway between trauma and risk attitudes that is independent of
4.4. Falsification tests
any priming mechanism (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Kess-
ler et al., 1995; Mcewen and Sapolsky, 1995; Yehuda, 2002).
Finally, I estimate two separate models that serve as falsification tests
This discussion also speaks to the external validity of the results.
Unfortunately, I did not collect data on real-life economic decisions and I
31
As Acharya et al. (2016) discuss, the standard errors are biased in the second stage cannot test whether my results illustrate how victims behave outside of
estimation since they do not consider the first-stage estimation of b β 2 . Unbiased and the laboratory. However, previous research has demonstrated that lab-
consistent standard errors can be obtained deriving a consistent estimator for the variance oratory experiments have the potential to inform about real economic
of c
γ 1 for linear models or through bootstrapping. Here, I employ the latter method. decisions, especially if they identify the principles governing behavior
32
This method provides an unbiased estimate of the ADCE under two conditions:
(Camerer, 2015; Kessler and Vesterlund, 2015). The results on the
Sequential Unconfoundedness and No Intermediate Interactions. The first condition en-
tails two related assumptions: First, that there is no omitted variable bias for the effect of mediating role of psychological trauma, which are consistent with those
the treatment (violence) on the outcome (risk attitudes), conditional on pretreatment of studies in psychology on the physiological and emotional effects of
confounders. Second, that there is no omitted variable bias for the effect of the mediator trauma, are a first step in this direction. Yet, more work is needed linking
(trauma) on the outcome, conditional on the treatment, and pretreatment and interme-
data on psychological trauma, risk attitudes, and decisions outside of a
diate confounders. The first assumption was discussed thoroughly in section 3.2. The
second assumption would not hold if the susceptibility to experience psychological
lab-in-the-field experiment.
trauma, conditional on a given level of violence, is related to the individual's underlying
risk attitudes or preferences in the absence of violence. The condition of No Intermediate 5. Discussion
Interactions requires that the effect of the mediator on the outcomes is not a function of
intermediate confounders. When this assumption fails to hold, the estimated effect will be
a weighted average of the ACDE for different levels of the intermediate confounders. This In this article, I analyze whether the experience of severe and recent
implies that the estimated effect would not be biased but could mask key heterogeneity. violence brings about changes in risk attitudes, and I also explore the
33
Results are robust if I control for the temporal proximity of violence and its interaction underlying psychological channel. For this purpose, I collected rich data
with the severity of violence in the first stage of model 4. from victims of violence in Colombia, who have experienced different
34
I also explore whether the results above are driven by more standard mechanisms; in
particular, by the economic consequences of violence and displacement. Under the stan-
levels of violence at different times and suffered above-normal levels of
dard assumption of decreasing absolute risk aversion, victims who become impoverished
should become more risk averse independently of their exposure to violence. For this
35
reason, I regress the choices during the experiment on the levels of per-capita consump- More recent traumatic events may be recalled more vividly and prime subjects at a
tion, per-capita value of purchased goods, whether the household was a recipient of greater extent. However, the null effect of the temporal proximity of violence on risk at-
governmental aid, or the size of the lands that was lost during the displacement. None of titudes suggests that the effect on risk attitudes is explained by a priming effect among
these variables is significantly associated with risk attitudes (see Appendix Table A12). those victimized more recently.

25
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

psychological trauma. This allows me to exploit the variations in the trauma can hinder the effectiveness of other, more traditional types of
severity and recency of violence and in the symptoms of psychological interventions.
trauma. Consistent with the findings of clinical and laboratory studies in Finally, as the refugee crisis looms throughout the world, we need a
psychology, I find that the exposure to more severe violence induces better understanding of the socioeconomic and psychological conse-
higher levels of risk aversion, that this effect is explained by the psy- quences of violence. Previous studies on the behavioral consequences of
chological consequences of violence, and that it is not permanent and violence have in general focused on residents of conflict-torn regions,
instead fades out with time. but not necessarily on the victims of violence, displaced persons, or
One of the limitations of the analysis is that the sampling strategy refugees. Yet, it is also important that we understand the socioeco-
focused exclusively on victims of violence and displacement, and does nomic, behavioral, and psychological consequences of violence for these
not allow me to analyze the effects of violence at the extensive margin. subsets of the population who were more severely affected by violence,
However, using a parallel sample of 318 subjects who reside in the same and who are more vulnerable to psychological trauma and poverty. Of
conflict regions but who had not been victimized, I do not find that course, this raises many challenges, such as conducting fieldwork in
victims are more risk averse than non-victims (see Table A15).36 While conflict regions, working with vulnerable populations, and asking sen-
this result may seem surprising, it is consistent with the conceptual sitive questions. Nonetheless, the findings and policy recommendations
framework laid out since the introduction, which suggests that it is the that follow from our research should motivate us to embrace such
experience of severe and recent violence that induces higher levels of risk challenges.
aversion by triggering severe symptoms of psychological trauma.
The results raise three important implications. First, they speak to the Acknowledgements
literature in behavioral economics that explores the influence of
noncognitive factors on behavior. Although canonical models of behavior I would like to thank Michael R. Carter for his advice and support
under uncertainty assume that risk preferences are a stable trait and that throughout this project and Diego Amador, Marc Bellemare, Steve
decisions under risk and uncertainty are driven by cognitive assessments Boucher, Juan Camilo Cardenas, Alessandra Cassar, Hillary Hoynes, Ana
(DellaVigna, 2009; Loewenstein, 2000), previous research demonstrates ~ ez, Rachid Laajaj, Travis Lybbert, Daniel Reese, Marc Rock-
María Iban
that risk attitudes and decisions are often influenced by the individual's more, Laura Schechter, and two anonymous referees for useful com-
emotional state (Ledoux, 1998; Loewenstein and Lerner, 2003). The ments. I would also like to thank participants at seminars at the
standard approach to studying the role of emotions on behavior has been University of San Francisco; University of California, Davis; University of
to administer random psychological primes that induce emotions in the Wisconsin, Madison; Universidad de los Andes; the Midwest Interna-
lab (Lerner and Keltner, 2001; Lerner and Tiedens, 2006; Lerner et al., tional Economic Development Conference; the Northeast Universities
2015; Raghunathan and Pham, 1999) and in the field (Callen et al., Development Consortium Conference; the Pacific Conference for Devel-
2014). To the best of my knowledge, this is the first study to test the opment Economics; and the Symposium for Economics Experiments in
influence of psychological trauma on risk attitudes. Developing Countries for their comments. I am grateful to the public
Second, the results indicate that the psychological and behavioral officials and community leaders who assisted me during fieldwork and to
consequences of violence can work as a different channel through which the victims who agreed to share their experiences of violence. The Pacific
violence thrusts victims into poverty. In a world characterized by Rim Research Dissertation Grant, Centro Latinoamericano para el
incomplete access to credit and insurance, higher levels of risk aversion Desarrollo Rural (RIMISP), and the Henry A. Jastro Award provided
induce an inefficient resource allocation and hinder the investments that generous funding to collect the data, while the Harry Frank Guggenheim
are necessary to move out of poverty. Foundation provided a dissertation fellowship. IRB Approval for the
Although these implications are intuitive from the perspective of collection of human subjects data was awarded by the University of
development economics, the actual effects on poverty dynamics rest on California, Davis. The usual disclaimers apply.
the magnitude and persistence of the shifts in behavior. In fact, one could
argue that the behavioral consequences of violence are not the primary Appendix A. Supplementary data
concern when designing policies aimed towards victims of violence. After
all, they also experience a massive loss of physical, human, and social Supplementary data related to this article can be found at https://doi.
assets, which hinders their ability to recover and move out of poverty org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2017.11.001.
(Iban~ ez and Moya, 2010a, 2010b). Nonetheless, victims of violence are
exposed to considerable sources of risk, and they lack access to formal References
credit and insurance markets. Therefore, a shock that induces higher
levels of risk aversion, even if it is temporary, will discourage them from Acharya, A., Blackwell, M., Sen, M., 2016. Explaining causal findings without bias:
detecting and assessing direct effects. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 110 (3), 512–529. https://
making productive investments and induce inefficient decisions. These doi.org/10.1017/S0003055416000216.
range from distress asset sales and an unproductive use of transfers to American Psychiatric Association, 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
inadequate levels of investment in education, health, and income- Disorders. American Psychiatric Association, Arlington, VA.
Bauer, M., Cassar, A., Chytilova, J., Henrich, J., 2014. War's enduring effects on the
generating activities. Such decisions are costly in the short term and development of egalitarian motivations and in-group biases. Psychol. Sci. 25 (1),
can have irreversible consequences over time and across generations. 47–57. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493444.
This article therefore provides additional justification for imple- Bellows, J., Miguel, E., 2009. War and local collective action in Sierra Leone. J. Public
Econ. 93 (11–12), 1144–1157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2009.07.012.
menting mental health programs to assist victims of violence. This does
Blattman, C., 2009. From violence to voting: war and political participation in Uganda.
not mean that such programs are necessary only because psychological Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 103 (2), 231–247. https://doi.org/10.1017/
trauma can have negative economic consequences. The psychological S0003055409090212.
Blattman, C., Miguel, E., 2010. Civil war. J. Econ. Lit. 48 (1), 3–57. https://doi.org/
consequences of violence are, of course, important by themselves and
10.1257/jel.48.1.3.
merit specific interventions and policies. Unfortunately, in countries torn Brakman, S., Garretsen, H., Schramm, M., 2004. The strategic bombing of German cities
by violence, there is often a lack of awareness of the implications of during World War II and its impact on city growth. J. Econ. Geogr. 4 (2), 201–218.
psychological trauma, and mental health programs are scarce and poorly https://doi.org/10.1093/jeg/4.2.201.
Callen, M., Isaqzadeh, M., Long, J.D., Sprenger, C., 2014. Violence and risk preference:
funded. This article makes a case for the importance of psychological experimental evidence from Afghanistan. Am. Econ. Rev. 104 (1), 123–148. https://
interventions for victims of violence by highlighting how psychological doi.org/10.1257/aer.104.1.123.
Camerer, C., 2015. The promise and success of lab–field generalizability in experimental
economics: a critical reply to levitt and list. In: Frechette, G.R., Schotter, A. (Eds.),
Handbook of Experimental Economic Methodology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
36
This sample and result is described in more detail in Moya (2013). https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195328325.003.0020.

26
A. Moya Journal of Development Economics 131 (2018) 15–27

Cameron, L., Shah, M., 2015. Risk-taking behavior in the wake of natural disasters. Handbook of Experimental Economic Methodology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
J. Hum. Resour. 50 (2), 484–515. https://doi.org/10.3368/jhr.50.2.484. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195328325.003.0020.
Cardenas, J.C., Carpenter, J., 2013. Risk attitudes and economic well-being in Latin Ledoux, J., 1998. The Emotional Brain: the Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life.
America. J. Dev. Econ. 103, 52–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2013.01.008. Simon and Schuster, New York.
Cassar, A., Healy, A., Von Kessler, C., 2011. Trust, Risk, and Time Preferences after a Lerner, J.S., Keltner, D., 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81 (1),
Natural Disaster: Experimental Evidence from Thailand. Unpublished Manuscript. 146–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146.
Cassar, A., Grosjean, P., Whitt, S., 2014. Social preferences of ex-combatants: survey and Lerner, J.S., Tiedens, L.Z., 2006. Portrait of the angry decision maker: how appraisal
experimental evidence from postwar Tajikistan. In: W€arneryd, K. (Ed.), The tendencies shape angers influence on cognition. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 19 (2),
Economics of Conflict: Theory and Empirical Evidence. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, 115–137. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.515.
pp. 231–262. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262026895.001.0001. Lerner, J.S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., Kassam, K.S., 2015. Emotion and decision making. Annu.
Cassar, A., Grosjean, P., Whitt, S., 2013. Legacies of violence: trust and market Rev. Psychol. 66, 799–823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-
development. J. Econ. Growth 18 (3), 285–318. 115043.
Castillo, M., Carter, M.R., 2011. Behavioral Responses to Natural Disasters (Working Loewenstein, G., 2000. Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. Am. Econ.
Paper 1026). Arlington VA: Interdisciplinary Center for Economic Science, George Rev. 90 (2), 426–432. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.90.2.426.
Mason University. Loewenstein, G., Lerner, J.S., 2003. The role of affect in decision making. In:
Casullo, M.M., 2004. El inventario de síntomas SCL-90-R de L. Derogatis. Adaptaci on Davidson, R.J., Scherer, K.R., Goldsmith, H.H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective
UBA. Sciences. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp. 619–642.
Centro Nacional de Memoria Hist orica (CNMH), 2013. ¡Basta ya!: Colombia: Memorias de Londo~ no, A., Romero, P., Casas, G., 2012. The association between armed conflict,
guerra y dignidad. Retrieved from. http://www.centrodememoriahistorica.gov.co/ violence and mental health: a cross sectional study comparing two populations in
micrositios/informeGeneral/descargas.html. Colombia. Confl. Health 6 (1), 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1752-1505-6-12.
Cesarini, D., Dawes, C.T., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., Wallace, B., 2009. Genetic Malmendier, U., Nagel, S., 2011. Depression babies: do macroeconomic experiences affect
variation in preferences for giving and risk taking. Q. J. Econ. 124 (2), 809–842. risk-taking? Q. J. Econ. 126 (1), 373–416. https://doi.org/10.3386/w14813.
https://doi.org/10.1162/qjec.2009.124.2.809. Mcewen, B., Sapolsky, R., 1995. Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5
Davis, D.R., Weinstein, D.E., 2002. Bones, bombs, and break points: the geography of (2), 205–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X.
economic activity. Am. Econ. Rev. 92 (5), 1269–1289. https://doi.org/10.1257/ Miguel, E., Roland, G., 2011. The long-run impact of bombing Vietnam. J. Dev. Econ. 96
000282802762024502. (1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2010.07.004.
DellaVigna, S., 2009. Psychology and economics: evidence from the field. J. Econ. Lit. 47 Mollica, R.F., McInnes, K., Poole, C., Tor, S., 1998. Dose-effect relationships of trauma to
(2), 315–372. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.315. symptoms of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder among Cambodian
Doctors Without Borders, 2006. Living with Fear: the Cycle of Violence in Colombia. survivors of mass violence. Br. J. Psychiatry 173 (6), 482–488. https://doi.org/
Retrieved from. https://issuu.com/msf_australia/docs/colombia_living_in_fear_0406. 10.1192/bjp.173.6.482.
Doctors Without Borders, 2010. Three-time Victims: Victims of Violence, Silence and Moya, A., 2013. The Behavioral Consequences of Violence in Colombia: Mental Trauma,
Neglect. Retrieved from. http://www.doctorswithoutborders.org/sites/usa/files/ Economic Behavior, and the Creation of Chronic Poverty in Colombia. Doctoral
MSF-Colombia-Three-Time-Victims.pdf. Dissertation. Agricultural and Resource Economics, University of California, Davis,
Eckel, C.C., El-Gamal, M.A., Wilson, R.K., 2009. Risk loving after the storm: a Bayesian- pp. 1–182.
network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 69 (2), Moya, A., Carter, M., 2014. Violence and the Formation of Hopelessness and Pessimistic
110–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2007.08.012. Prospects of Upward Mobility in Colombia (Working Paper 20463). National Bureau
Ellsberg, Daniel, 1961. Risk, ambiguity, and the savage axioms. Q. J. Econ. 75 (4), of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA. https://doi.org/10.3386/w20463.
643–669. https://doi.org/10.2307/1884324. National Ombudsman’s Office, 2009. Evaluaci on de riesgos de la poblacion civil como
Hartley, C.A., Phelps, E.A., 2012. Anxiety and decision making. Biol. Psychiatry 72, consecuencia del conflicto armado en el sistema de alertas tempranas 2009.
113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027. Defensoría del Pueblo, Colombia).
Human Rights Watch, 2010. World Report 2010: Events of 2009. Retrieved from. http:// National Victims Unit. (n.d.). National victims' unit official registry. Retrieved from
www.hrw.org/sites/default/files/reports/wr2010.pdf. http://rni.unidadvictimas.gov.co.
Ib
an~ez, A.M., Moya, A., 2010a. Vulnerability of victims of civil conflicts: empirical Page, L., Savage, D., Torgler, B., 2014. Variation in risk seeking behavior following large
evidence for the displaced population in Colombia. World Dev. 38 (4), 647–663. losses: a natural experiment. Eur. Econ. Rev. 71, 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2009.11.015. j.euroecorev.2014.04.009.
Ib
an~ez, A.M., Moya, A., 2010b. Do conflicts create poverty traps? Asset losses and Raghunathan, R., Pham, M.T., 1999. All negative moods are not equal: motivational
recovery for displaced households in Colombia. In: Di Tella, R., Edwards, S., influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis.
Schargrodsky, E. (Eds.), The Economics of Crime: Lessons for and from Latin America. Process. 79 (1), 56–77. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1999.2838.
University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 137–172. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof: Richards, A., Ospina-Duque, J., Barrera-Valencia, M., Escobar-Rinc on, J., Ardila-
oso/9780199378296.001.0001. Gutierrez, M., Metzler, T., Marmar, C., 2011. Posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety
Ib
an~ez, A.M., Moya, A., 2016. Who stays and who leaves in genocides and mass killings? and depression symptoms, and psychosocial treatment needs in Colombians
In: Anderton, C.H., Brauer, J. (Eds.), Economics Aspects of Genocide, Mass Atrocities, internally displaced by armed conflict: a mixed-method evaluation. Psychol. Trauma
and Their Prevention. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 137–162. Theory, Res. Pract. Policy 3 (4), 384–393. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022257.
Instituto de Estudios para el Desarrollo y la Paz (Indepaz), 2011. Seventh Report of Rustichini, A., Deyoung, C.G., Anderson, J., Burks, S., 2011. Toward the Integration of
Presence of Narcoparamilitary Groups in 2011. Retrieved from. www.indepaz.org. Personality Theory and Decision Theory. University of Minnesota, Mimeo.
co/wp-content/uploads/2012/02/S%C3%A9ptimo-informe-versi%C3%B3n-ingl% Shultz, J.M., Garfin, D.R., Espinel, Z., Araya, R., Oquendo, M.A., Wainberg, M.L.,
C3%A9s-of-Narcoparamilitary-Groups-in-2011.pdf. Neria, Y., 2014. Internally displaced “victims of armed conflict” in Colombia: the
Kahneman, D., Tversky, A., 1979. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. trajectory and trauma signature of forced migration. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 16 (10),
Econometrica 47 (2), 263–291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185. 475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0475-7.
Kalyvas, S., 2006. The Logic of Violence in Civil War. Cambridge Studies in Comparative United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees (UNHCR), 2016. Population statistics.
Politics. Cambridge University Press, New York. https://doi.org/10.1086/522394. Retrieved from. http://popstats.unhcr.org/en/overview.
Kandasamy, N., Hardy, B., Page, L., Schaffner, M., Graggaber, J., Powlson, A., Coates, J., Vinck, P., Pham, P.N., Stover, E., Weinstein, H.M., 2007. Exposure to war crimes and
2014. Cortisol shifts financial risk preferences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111 (9), implications for peace building in northern Uganda. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 298 (5),
3608–3613. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317908111. 543–554. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.298.5.543.
Kessler, R.C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., Nelson, C.B., 1995. Posttraumatic Voors, M.J., Nillesen, E.E., Verwimp, P., Bulte, E.H., Lensink, R., Van Soest, D.P., 2012.
stress disorder in the national comorbidity survey. Archiv. General Psychiatry 52 Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. Am. Econ. Rev. 102 (2),
(12), 1048–1060. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. 941–964. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.102.2.941.
Kessler, J.B., Vesterlund, L., 2015. The external validity of laboratory experiments: the Yehuda, R., 2002. Post-traumatic stress disorder. N. Engl. J. Med. 346, 108–114. https://
misleading emphasis on quantitative effects. In: Frechette, G.R., Schotter, A. (Eds.), doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra012941.

27

You might also like