You are on page 1of 19

Received: 6 September 2022 Revised: 30 August 2023 Accepted: 9 October 2023

DOI: 10.1002/cb.2274

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Female advertising models and their body mass index (BMI):


How does BMI similarity affect perceived product quality?

Erik Ernesto Vazquez 1 | Reto Felix 2 | Lorena Siliceo 3

1
Léonard De Vinci Pôle Universitaire, Research
Center, Paris La Défense, France Abstract
2
Robert C. Vackar College of Business and Recent research has pointed out the importance of appearance similarity as a special
Entrepreneurship, University of Texas Rio
Grande Valley, Edinburg, Texas, USA instance of homophily. The current paper introduces body mass index (BMI) similarity
3
Universidad Anáhuac México, Facultad de as a convenient and powerful proxy of perceived appearance homophily in the con-
Economía y Negocios, Huixquilucan, Estado de
text of female advertising models by showing process evidence and boundary condi-
México, Mexico
tions for the effect of BMI similarity on marketing-relevant downstream variables.
Correspondence
Study 1 tests how BMI similarity relates to a traditional measure of appearance
Erik Ernesto Vazquez, Léonard De Vinci Pôle
Universitaire, Research Center, Paris La homophily and perceived reliability of a female advertising model. Study 2 shows that
Défense 92916, France. BMI similarity influences perceived product quality and purchase intention. Website
Email: erik.vazquez@devinci.fr
vividness negatively moderates the relationship between BMI similarity and product
quality. Study 3 tests for alternative explanations and provides support for the medi-
ating effect of appearance homophily for the relationship between BMI similarity and
perceived product quality. The findings provide marketing managers with important
insights on how to increase their marketing effectiveness by integrating BMI similar-
ity into their marketing communications. Additionally, using BMI similarity serves as
an alternative way to promote diversity and inclusion of models with plus-size body
type often sought by societal brands.

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N which refers to a general assessment of appearance and which may


include different criteria such as facial features and properties of the
Similarity is a powerful construct for human relationships. People tend body (Bekk et al., 2017; Ladhari et al., 2020). It has been suggested
to be homophilous—that is, they usually develop stronger ties with that appearance similarity tends to be powerful in human relationships
other people who are similar to them (Borgatti & Halgin, 2011). Previ- because it allows people to infer socially relevant information about
ous research suggests that similarity between two persons increases others (Bjornsdottir et al., 2021). For example, Ladhari et al. (2020)
attraction (Rodrigues et al., 2017), communication effectiveness find that appearance homophily (defined as the extent to which a per-
(Thakor et al., 2008), perceived trust (Leonhardt et al., 2020), per- son perceives that another person looks similar to them) between a
ceived credibility (Sokolova & Kefi, 2020), and influence (Balabanis & video blogger (vlogger) and a viewer increases the vlogger's perceived
Chatzopoulou, 2019). In a consumption context, messages from con- popularity, which in turn increases purchase intention. In a similar
sumers who are perceived to be similar increase message persuasive- vein, Bekk et al. (2017) show that similarity in perceived attractiveness
ness and purchase intention (Prendergast et al., 2010). In online moderates the influence of endorser attractiveness on consumer
support groups, dissimilarity among group members impedes self- responses to advertising.
disclosure, which in turn weakens the social ties of such groups Despite the importance that appearance homophily has in a mar-
(Pechman et al., 2021; Vazquez, 2021). To summarize, the extant liter- keting context, a major drawback for providing managerially relevant
ature finds important effects of similarity in demographics, values, and opportunities for intervention is that appearance homophily implies a
beliefs for marketing-relevant downstream variables. However, recent subjective assessment of another person and hence depends on mea-
research has pointed out the importance of appearance similarity, surement scales to capture consumers' perceptions. For example,

J Consumer Behav. 2023;1–19. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cb © 2023 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 1
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

based on McCroskey et al.'s (2006) measure of homophily, Ladhari Furthermore, the present research introduces perceived website viv-
et al. (2020) use a 16-item scale to measure consumers' perceived idness as a moderator for the relationship between BMI similarity on
similarity with a vlogger in terms of attitudes, general background, perceived product quality in a retailing context for fashion apparel.
values, and appearance. Applying such scales is indeed feasible for Study 3 explores alternative explanations for our findings by testing
market research purposes, but marketers are usually not able to the mediating effects of appearance homophily, trustworthiness,
receive appearance-related homophily information in a comprehen- expertise, familiarity, and likability on the relationship between BMI
sive way from all customers as long as this information is based on similarity and perceived product quality. A supplementary study
subjective measurement scales. (reported in the Web Appendix) on group similarity provides addi-
The current research addresses this issue in a novel way by dem- tional evidence for the importance of similarity measures for per-
onstrating that body mass index (BMI) functions satisfactorily as a ceived product quality in the context of an existing fashion clothing
proxy for perceived appearance homophily in a fashion retail setting. company (boohoo.com). Finally, we provide a general discussion of
Situating our research in the context of comparably thin female adver- our findings, including implications for academia and marketing man-
tising models allows us to avoid confounds related to the body size agers, and suggest avenues for future research.
(e.g., thin, average, plus-size) of the model and to explore the phenom-
enon under a theoretical perspective that builds on the prevalence of
thin models in contemporary advertising (Cohen et al., 2017; 2 | THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK
Volonté, 2019). Hence, our work complements research about female
portrayals of diverse body sizes and the positive effects of inclusive 2.1 | BMI similarity, appearance homophily,
advertising of plus-size models (e.g., Åkestam et al., 2017; Cinelli & and perceived reliability of the advertising model
Yang, 2016; Janssen & Paas, 2014; Joo & Wu, 2021; Pounders, 2018).
Furthermore, focusing on advertising models in combination with Homophily is a specific type of social contagion, that is, people's reli-
same-gender (i.e., women) participants in the present studies ance on information from others (Bilgicer et al., 2015). Early conceptu-
averts gender confounds (compare previous studies, e.g., Felix & alizations of homophily distinguished between status homophily and
Garza, 2012; Lv et al., 2022, who use female samples in their research value homophily (Lazarsfeld & Merton, 1954). Whereas status homo-
on body appearance). phily refers to formal, informal, or ascribed status (which includes
Using BMI rather than subjective measurement scales as a proxy sociodemographic characteristics such as ethnicity, gender, and age),
for appearance homophily has several advantages. First, it is relatively value homophily alludes to similarity based on values, beliefs, and atti-
easy to obtain BMI data because they are based on consumers' height tudes (McPherson et al., 2001). More recent research suggests that
and weight—two metrics that marketers may already have in their apart from status and values, similarity of appearance is an important
customer databases. Further, advances in face recognition software factor when predicting attitudes toward influencers and salespersons
and artificial intelligence now allow for a relatively reliable calculation (Bjornsdottir et al., 2021; Ladhari et al., 2020). However, the disadvan-
of BMI based on just a facial image (Dantcheva et al., 2018; Wen & tage of appearance similarity is that it is a subjective rather than
Guo, 2013). Hence, we argue that using BMI rather than traditional objective measure of similarity, and hence it depends on psycho-
measurement scales provides marketers with important opportunities graphic measurement scales to capture how similar a customer feels
to predict managerially relevant outcome variables, such as perceived in comparison to a salesperson or brand endorser. Table 1 provides an
product quality and purchase intention. overview of selected studies on homophily and source similarity in
We also include an important boundary condition, website vivid- marketing and consumer research. As can be seen from the table, pre-
ness, for the effect of BMI on quality perception and purchase inten- vious studies on homophily relating to personality traits, values, or
tion in our research. Specifically, we observe that many marketers use appearance depend on subjective measures of homophily, except for
elements such as animations, dynamic website objects, sound, and demographic similarity based on variables such as age or gender.
action buttons to enhance the vividness of their websites and thereby Importantly, similarity in appearance is commonly measured through
capture consumers' attention. Drawing on cognitive depletion theory self-reported scales (Ladhari et al., 2020; Torres, 2007). Addressing
(e.g., Hildebrand et al., 2021; Sokolova & Krishna, 2016), we expect this issue, the current research suggests that similarity based on BMI
that the use of such elements may decrease the salience of may act as a parsimonious alternative for appearance homophily.
appearance-related information and attenuate the relationship Traditionally, marketers have postulated that extremely thin
between BMI similarity and perceived product quality. models with a very low BMI convey ideals of beauty and aesthetics
Our paper is structured as follows: We first provide the theoreti- (Venkatesh et al., 2010) and draw consumer purchases through aspira-
cal background for our studies and comment on our hypotheses. Next, tional values (Barry, 2014). The rationale for promoting an extremely
Study 1 tests our assumption that BMI similarity is positively related thin body image that is unattainable for most women is grounded in
with appearance homophily, which in turn is positively associated with the expectation that unfulfilled aspirations secure continued pur-
the perceived reliability toward a fashion model. In addition, Study chases from customers hunting their appearance-based ideals
1 excludes perceived originality as an alternative explanation for this (Borland & Akram, 2007). Thus, it is not surprising that women with
effect. Study 2 demonstrates the predicted association between BMI high BMI are typically less satisfied with their body image and desire
similarity and perceived product quality/purchase intention. to be thinner (D'Alessandro & Bill Chitty, 2011). The complexity of
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 3

TABLE 1 Selective literature overview of homophily measures in marketing and consumer research.

Measurement of
homophily
(subjective vs.
Authors Journal Homophily definition Homophily domain objective) Operationalization
Balabanis and Psychology & Communication Similarity in social Subjective Four items of perceived subjective
Chatzopoulou Marketing volume is higher class, background, similarity to a blogger.
(2019) when the source and status
and the recipient
are similar.
Bekk et al. (2017) Marketing The notion that Similarity in physical Subjective Comparison of subjectively
Letters similarity leads to attractiveness measured physical
liking and attractiveness for an advertising
attraction, whereas endorser and one's own
dissimilarity leads attractiveness.
to repulsion.
Bilgicer et al. Journal of The principle that a Similarity based on Objective Clustering approach based on
(2015) Retailing contact between demographic demographic variables.
similar people variables, such as
occurs at a higher income and
rate than among education
dissimilar people.
Campbell et al. Journal of The idea that the Similarity based on Subjective Comparison of subjectively
(1988) Marketing more similar the self-esteem, values, measured self-esteem, values,
parties in a dyad and and interorganizational contact
are, the more likely interorganizational between buyers and sellers.
is a favorable contact
outcome.
De Bruyn and International Perceptual homophily Perceptual homophily Subjective Respondents' perceptions of
Lilien (2008) Journal of is defined as values, likes and dislikes and
Research in similarities experiences regarding an
Marketing between two acquaintance.
people's values,
likes, dislikes, and
experience.
Fu et al. (2019) Journal of External similarity External and internal Objective and External similarity: Respondents'
Strategic refers to similarity subjective information on name, age,
Marketing characteristics that birthday, and place of residence
can be obtained of other people in an online
without deep community. Internal similarity:
interaction. Internal Respondents' perception
similarity refers to regarding values, interests,
characteristics that attitudes, and preferences of
can only be other people in an online
obtained through community
deep interaction.
Ladhari et al. Journal of The degree to which Similarity in attitudes, Subjective Sixteen-item scale measuring
(2020) Retailing and people who background, value, perceived similarity to a vlogger
Consumer interact are similar and appearance based on attitude, background,
Services in beliefs, value, and appearance.
education, social
status and the like.
Leonhardt et al. Journal of Extent to which Similarity in attitudes Subjective Four items referring to similar
(2020) Business individuals are and behaviors attitudes and behaviors as
Research similar to others. compared to a member of a
Facebook group.
Pechman et al. Journal of The tendency of Demographic Objective Dyadic dissimilarity index based
(2021) Consumer interpersonal similarity on observed demographics
Psychology similarities to breed (gender, age, marital status,
social connection. employment status) between
two people in a support group.

(Continues)
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

TABLE 1 (Continued)

Measurement of
homophily
(subjective vs.
Authors Journal Homophily definition Homophily domain objective) Operationalization
Prendergast et al. International The degree to which Similarity in interests Subjective Four items measuring similarity of
(2010) Journal of individuals are interests between respondent
Advertising similar in terms of and an online forum.
certain shared
social
characteristics
Risselada et al. Journal of The principle that a Demographic Objective Similarity score between
(2014) Marketing contact between similarity customers based on age,
similar people gender, education level, and
occurs at a higher income for the adoption of an
rate than among innovative product.
dissimilar people.
Rodrigues et al. Computers in The degree of Attitude homophily Subjective Three items referring to similar
(2017) Human perceived (rather ideas, attitudes, and beliefs as
Behavior than actual) compared to unfamiliar people.
similarity with
another person.
Sokolova and Kefi Journal of The degree to which Attitude homophily Subjective Two items referring to similar
(2020) Retailing and people tend to thinking and values as
Consumer bond with similar compared to an influencer.
Services others.
Streukens and Psychology & Preference of people Similarity in Subjective Comparison of subjectively
Andreassen Marketing who are similar to conscientiousness, measured personality traits for
(2013) oneself, leading to extraversion, customers and frontline
attraction. agreeableness, and employees.
stability
Thakor et al. Journal of Common meanings, Age similarity as a Subjective Estimation of reference
(2008) Retailing attitudes, or beliefs proxy for consumers' age in a service
lead to increased stereotyping encounter.
and more effective
interaction.
Torres (2007) Journal of The degree to which Multi-dimensional Subjective Twenty-seven items measuring
Business pairs of individuals perceived perceived similarity to
Research are similar with homophily advertising models based on
respect to attitudes, beliefs, personality,
attributes such as and appearance.
beliefs, values,
education, and
socials status.
van Esch et al. Marketing A tendency for Perceived general Subjective Three items measuring perceived
(2018) Intelligence & consumers to similarity general similarity to a blogger.
Planning gravitate toward
and form
relationships with
those who are
similar to them in
values, lifestyle,
gender, education
and other
attributes.
Von Wangenheim European The degree to which Similarity in Subjective Two items measuring
n
and Bayo Journal of individuals are preferences and respondents' perceived
(2004) Marketing similar in terms of values. similarity in terms of
certain attributes. preferences and values to a
consumer providing word-
of-mouth communication.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 5

BMI effects on marketing-relevant downstream variables is further perceived reliability of a message source is an important component
demonstrated by findings which suggest that average advertising of trust (Soh et al., 2009), we hypothesize the following:
models, as compared to thin models, may attenuate consumers' body
anxiety, but at the same time may have negative effects on brand atti- Hypothesis 2. Perceived appearance homophily is posi-
tudes and purchase intention through a competing path that triggers tively associated with perceived reliability of the adver-
repulsion of natural models (Borau & Bonnefon, 2017). tising model.
The current research reconciles the inconclusive and in part con-
flicting results from previous studies on BMI by suggesting that BMI
difference between a customer and an endorser, rather than the abso- 2.2 | BMI similarity, perceived product quality,
lute BMI of the endorser, provides meaningful insights for marketing and purchase intention
theory and practice. For example, rather than arguing that “moder-
ately thin advertising models are optimal, most of the time” H1 in Study 1 establishes our baseline assumption of a positive rela-
(Janssen & Paas, 2014, p. 167), we posit that smaller differences tionship between BMI similarity and appearance homophily. Further-
between the customer's and the advertising model's BMI lead to more more, H2 predicts a positive indirect relationship between BMI
positive marketing outcomes, such as attitudes toward the advertising similarity and perceived reliability of the advertising model. The out-
model and purchase intention. We test this proposition in the context come variable of perceived reliability of the advertising model is rele-
of thin female advertising models, which allows us to avoid confounds vant for marketers but does not necessarily translate into higher
based on the size (e.g., thin, average, or plus-size) and gender of the performance for the brand. Building on this limitation from H2,
advertising model. Furthermore, we intentionally focus on thin female hypotheses H3 and H4 in this section posit a more immediate associa-
advertising models because even though average and plus-size models tion between BMI similarity and two performance-based marketing
have become more popular in recent years (Cinelli & Yang, 2016; metrics: perceived product quality and purchase intention.
Pounders, 2018), the female thin-ideal still prevails in contemporary We focus on perceived product quality in Studies 2 and 3 since
advertising (Cohen et al., 2017; Volonté, 2019). Hence, we hypothe- the main purpose of employing an advertising model in the context of
size and empirically test in Study 1 that BMI similarity toward thin fashion apparel is indeed the promotion of the product the company
advertising models is positively associated with a conventional mea- is trying to sell, and perceived product quality is one of the main
sure of appearance-based homophily. drivers of purchase intention (Bagozzi & Khoshnevis, 2022). For con-
sumer behavior and decision making in marketing, understanding per-
Hypothesis 1. BMI similarity is positively associated ceptions customers have regarding the quality of products is usually
with perceived appearance homophily. more relevant than (a) manufacturers' claims about product quality or
(b) objective measures of product quality based on engineering stan-
We further posit that appearance homophily relates positively to dards (Stone-Romero et al., 1997). Following Golder et al. (2012), we
how reliable the advertising model is perceived.1 Similar to the reliabil- conceptualize perceived product quality as a conversion of perceived
ity of influencers (Filieri et al., 2023) and salespersons (Homburg & attributes into an aggregated evaluation of quality. Drawing on
Stock, 2005), we argue that the perceived reliability of advertising insights from source attractiveness model (McCracken, 1989;
models is crucial for marketers because it is an important component McGuire, 1985; Montoya & Horton, 2013), we predict that a high sim-
of source credibility and trust (Soh et al., 2009). Our hypothesis draws ilarity between the BMI of the customer and the advertising model
on the source attractiveness model (McGuire, 1985), which identifies will not only affect perceived traits of the endorser, but also extend to
familiarity, likability, and similarity to the source of a message as the the products that are promoted by the advertising model. This predic-
three main predictors of message effectiveness (McCracken, 1989). tion is based on the observation that consumers take peripheral cues
This positive effect of similarity between the source and the receiver (such as the attractiveness of the source triggered by the similarity to
of a message on attraction has also been emphasized by Byrne (1971) the advertising model) as indicators for product quality (Petty &
in his similarity-attraction theory. The underlying psychological mech- Cacioppo, 1986). Similar spillover effects have been shown in the con-
anism for the similarity-attraction effect contends that individuals text of traditional homophily measures for attitudes toward the prod-
favor stimuli that reinforce the logic and consistency of their world, uct (Bekk et al., 2017; Thakor et al., 2008), as well as for purchase
and that people who are similar to us generate such a reinforcement intention (Ismagilova et al., 2020; Lou et al., 2019).
(Montoya & Horton, 2013). Hence, similarity between a source and
receiver of a message results in higher levels of attractiveness, with Hypothesis 3. BMI similarity is positively associated
positive effects on trust (Kim & Kim, 2021; Racherla et al., 2012) and with perceived product quality of the apparel promoted
a variety of other outcome variables, such as message effectiveness, by the advertising model.
credibility, and product perceptions (Ertug et al., 2022). Given that
In addition, Study 2 also tests whether the association between
1
Congruent with prior literature (Bekk et al., 2017; Bjornsdottir et al., 2021; Ladhari
BMI similarity and perceived product quality extends to purchase
et al., 2020), we conceptualize appearance homophily as one form of similarity between a
reference person and a consumer. intention for the apparel product promoted by the advertising model.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

Including purchase intention into our framework allows us to test the effect of a message because these elements distract the viewer. Thus,
association of BMI similarity with a variable that is of direct economic vividness in communication can backfire and distract from other ele-
relevance to managers. The positive relationship between perceived ments of a communication message (Guadagno et al., 2011). Hence, in
product quality and purchase intention has been demonstrated in the the context of the current research, we expect that vivid elements on
extant literature (Bagozzi & Khoshnevis, 2022; Das Guru & a website (e.g., animations and additional sounds) will distract the
Paulssen, 2020). Hence, viewer's attention and interfere with the processing of other informa-
tion, such as the comparison of the viewer's own with the advertising
Hypothesis 4. Perceived product quality of the apparel model's BMI. We posit that cognitive depletion theory (Sokolova &
promoted by the advertising model is positively associ- Krishna, 2016) can explain this effect. Specifically, cognitive depletion
ated with purchase intention. theory suggests that tasks that draw consumers' attention will require
them to assign cognitive resources to this task, which in turn
Based on the hypothesized associations between BMI similarity, decreases their propensity to assign cognitive resources to other tasks
perceived appearance homophily, and perceived product quality, we (Hildebrand et al., 2021). Consequently, when viewers' attention is
further postulate that perceived appearance homophily mediates the drawn toward vivid message elements, such as animations, sounds,
relationship between BMI similarity and perceived product quality. flashing fonts, etc., more cognitive capacity is assigned to the proces-
Specifically, we posit that BMI similarity has a positive relationship sing of the vivid message elements and less cognitive capacity is
with perceived appearance homophily (as stated in H1), and we assigned to other message elements. Hence,
expect that perceived appearance homophily is positively associated
with perceived product quality. We investigate this mediating effect Hypothesis 6. Website vividness negatively moderates
of perceived appearance homophily in Study 3, where we also test for the relationship between BMI similarity and perceived
alternative explanations. Hence, product quality.

Hypothesis 5. Perceived appearance homophily posi-


tively mediates the relationship between BMI similarity 3 | ST UDY 1
and perceived product quality.
Study 1 explores the effects of BMI similarity on consumers' percep-
tions of appearance homophily and their subsequent perceptions of reli-
2.3 | Website vividness ability toward a human advertising model (H1 and H2) in the context of
an online apparel retail store. To address a potential alternative explana-
Vividness has been defined as the “ability of a technology to produce tion for our hypothesized effect of BMI similarity, we include con-
a sensorially rich mediated environment” (Steuer, 1992, p. 80). The sumers' perceptions of originality as a second mediator in our model.
effect of a vivid (as opposed to a less vivid) communication message
on downstream variables such as persuasion and message liking typi-
cally depends on whether the vivid message is more (vs. less) resource 3.1 | Method
demanding than the less vivid message (Keller & Block, 1997;
Vazquez, 2020). For example, Coyle and Thorson (2001) manipulate 3.1.1 | Participants and procedure
vividness of a commercial website through the addition of audio
effects and animation, and they report higher levels of telepresence Two-hundred and three women (Mage = 38.6, SDage = 12.8) were
and more positive and enduring attitudes for consumers in the high recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) to take part in an
vividness condition. In a similar vein, Fortin and Dholakia (2005) show online study in exchange for a small cash incentive following recent
that for a web-based advertisement, higher levels of vividness related studies (e.g., Vazquez, 2021). The majority of the participants
increase social presence and involvement, which in turn positively (59.4%) had an annual household income of $50,000 or above. Further,
influence attitudes toward the ad, attitudes toward the brand, and 66.7% of the sample had an associate degree or higher level of educa-
purchase intention. Yousaf et al. (2020) report that greater message tion. Participants first saw an image of a human advertising model
vividness (manipulated through interactive/audio-visual content) on (21 years of age, height 50 600 , 110.2 lbs, BMI of 17.8) (Appendix A,
social media platforms leads to higher customer engagement. In sum- Figure A1). Hence, our advertising model had a substantially lower BMI
mary, these studies find that vividness facilitates consumer informa- than the average US female population which is around 28.42 (Centers
tion processing. However, if a vivid communication message requires for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). Our use of a comparably
additional cognitive resources and attention, the effect of the vivid skinny advertising model is consistent with the predominant tendency
elements in the message will most likely be distracting and have a in the industry to use skinny advertising models since they are fre-
negative effect on marketing-relevant downstream variables. For quently deemed to be culturally more attractive (Lin et al., 2018;
example, Frey and Eagly (1993) find that vivid elements, such as color- Semaan et al., 2018). We acknowledge that using only one (comparably
ful language or picturesque examples, can undermine the persuasive skinny) advertising model does not allow us to claim that the effects of
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 7

BMI similarity on downstream variables will also hold with a regular extant literature. For example, Li and Peng (2021) report a positive
or overweight advertising model. However, showing the same ad- relationship of r = .37 (p < .001) between homophily (i.e., the degree
vertising model to all participants allows us to exclude confounds of similarity between a consumer and a social media influencer) and
(e.g., differences in perceived attractiveness) that would occur when perceived originality of the influencer. Furthermore, prior research
using different advertising models. Next, participants were asked to suggests that perceived originality conveys an aspirational desire in
respond to items relating to their perceptions of the advertising model. apparel markets (Fratto et al., 2006; Zebal & Jackson, 2019) that may
These items measured the latent constructs of interest which were ran- translate into positive attitudes toward the advertising model. We
domly shown to participants to avoid priming effects. measured perceived originality with a five-item semantic differential
originally developed by O'Quin and Besemer (1989). This scale mea-
sures the degree to which an advertisement, sales presentation, sales
3.1.2 | Measures promotion, or event marketing communication is viewed as being
original (Heiser et al., 2008; White & Smith, 2001). Since the human
The dependent variable—perceived reliability of the advertising model— model used in our study also portrayed a specific age and ethnicity
was measured with an adapted version of the advertisement trust- (21 years, white), we included the similarity between participants' eth-
reliability scale from Soh et al. (2009). This nine-item Likert scale mea- nicity (Torres, 2007) and age (Dwyer et al., 1998) as control variables.
sures the degree to which a person believes that an advertisement is Specifically, we calculated the following two similarity indexes. For
truthful and accurate. The mediator variable—perceived appearance age, we subtracted the absolute age difference between the human
homophily—was measured with the scale originally developed by model and each participant's age from 100. Hence, smaller age differ-
McCroskey et al. (1975). This four-item bipolar scale measures the ences result in a higher number on the index, which represents higher
degree to which a person perceives appearance homophily and interper- levels of age similarity. For ethnicity, we generated a dummy variable
sonal similarity in human communication. It has been widely used in pre- that was coded 1 when the human model and participant had the
vious research in marketing and apparel contexts (Hudders & De same ethnicity or 0 otherwise. Appendix A, Table A1, shows the mea-
Jans, 2022; Ladhari et al., 2020) as well as health and human communica- surement items and reliability assessment for Study 1. The psycho-
tion research (Navarro et al., 2022; van der Land et al., 2015). The inde- metric properties of our measurement scales were satisfying.
pendent variable—BMI similarity—was calculated by comparing the BMI Specifically, the factor loadings exceeded the recommended threshold
of the advertising model with the BMI of each participant. Specifically, of 0.6 for each item and the composite reliability scores for each
participants indicated their height and weight, which in turn allowed us latent construct were beyond the recommended minimum value of
to determine each participants' BMI (Appendix A, Figure A2 shows a dis- 0.7, confirming the reliability of the measures (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988).
tribution of BMI of each participant). To ensure that higher values on our Each latent construct had an AVE above the minimum recommended
BMI-based similarity measure indicated higher (as opposed to smaller) value of 0.5 (MacKenzie et al., 2011) Furthermore, all AVE values
levels of similarity, we constructed an index of BMI similarity by subtract- were larger than the highest squared correlation between different
ing the absolute difference between the BMI of the advertising model latent constructs, thus confirming the discriminant validity of the mea-
and the BMI of the participant from 100. For example, for a participant sures (Fornell & Larcker, 1981; Voorhees et al., 2016). The descriptive
with a BMI of 24.5, the absolute BMI difference would be statistics and correlation matrix for Study 1 are shown in Table 2.
24.5  17.8 = 6.7, and the resulting BMI similarity index would be
100  6.7 = 93.3. This procedure resulted in a maximum value of
100 for the BMI similarity index when participants' BMI was identical to 3.2 | Results
the BMI of 17.8 for the advertising model appearing in the online store.
To test H1 and H2, we used a parallel mediation model which evalu-
ates the relationship between BMI similarity and perceived reliability
3.1.3 | Controlling for alternative explanations of the advertising model mediated by (1) perceived appearance
homophily and (2) perceived originality. Tests for significance and the
To address a potential alternative explanation for the relationship estimation of confidence intervals at 95% followed the 10,000 bias-
between BMI similarity and perceived reliability of the advertising corrected bootstrap samples procedure as recommended by Hayes
model, we included perceived originality of the advertising model as a (2022). All analyses were conducted using the processR package of
second mediator into our model. Originality has been defined as the the statistical software R (Moon & Hong, 2021). Figure 1 shows the
degree of newness, innovation, sophistication, and differentiation of a standardized path coefficients for this model.
 et al., 2020). Because a source that
source, action, or product (Casalo Most importantly, BMI similarity was significantly and positively
is closer to oneself should be perceived as more elaborated, sophisti- related with perceived appearance homophily (0.373, p < .001). Further,
cated, and unique, we posit that consumers will feel that a source or perceived appearance homophily had a positive association with per-
stimulus is more original if the source is more similar to how they are ceived reliability of the advertising model (0.250, p < .001). These find-
or perceive themselves. Preliminary evidence for a positive relation- ings support H1 and H2. On the other hand, the relationship between
ship between similarity and perceived originality can be found in the BMI similarity and perceived originality was not statistically significant
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

TABLE 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations for Study 1.

Variable M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
1. Perceived reliability 4.31 1.24 -
2. Perceived appearance homophily 3.25 1.64 0.30*** -
3. BMI similarity 91.82 5.34 0.17* 0.36*** -
4. Perceived originality 3.56 1.44 0.38*** 0.14* 0.12 -
5. Age similarity 82.33 12.50 0.07 0.13 0.09 0.06 -

Note: Ethnicity: White = 140, Asian = 21, Afro-American = 14, Other = 27, Native = 1.
***p < .001; *p < .05.

F I G U R E 1 Path coefficients for Study


1—BMI similarity, perceived appearance
homophily, originality, and perceived
reliability of advertising model.
Standardized coefficients. Significance
level: ***p < .001.

(.128, ns). Hence, our findings indicate that perceived originality should appearance homophily in the context of thin advertising models. This
be excluded as an alternative explanation for the relationship between finding is important for marketing managers because recent advance-
BMI similarity and perceived reliability of the model. In addition, we ments in face recognition software facilitate a relatively accurate calcu-
were able to exclude similarity based on ethnicity or age as confounding lation of customers' BMI based on facial characteristics (Dantcheva
variables in our model because we included these variables as controls et al., 2018; Wen & Guo, 2013). In addition, Study 1 shows that BMI
in our model (ethnicity similarity: 0.021, ns; age similarity: 0.102, ns). similarity relates to how consumers rate the perceived reliability of the
Table 3 shows the regression coefficients and variance explained for advertising model. Thus, Study 1 provides initial evidence that BMI sim-
the different paths and stages of the model in more detail. ilarity is associated with managerially relevant outcome variables.
In addition, formal mediation tests (Table 4) based on boot-
strapping (Hayes, 2022) revealed that the mediating path from BMI
similarity to perceived reliability via perceived appearance homophily 4 | ST UDY 2
was statistically significant (.093, 95% CI 0.032 to 0.154). Furthermore,
the alternative mediation path through perceived originality was not Study 2 extends the initial findings from Study 1 in several important
statistically significant (.043, 95% CI 0.006 to 0.092). As shown in ways. First, it explores the relationship between BMI similarity for a thin,
Figure 1, the direct effect of BMI similarity on perceived reliability was female advertising model and women's perceived quality and purchase
not significant (.051, ns), which indicates full (rather than partial) media- intention for the apparel (a dress) endorsed by the advertising model (H3
tion (Pieters, 2017). Overall, the empirical results support our prediction and H4). To increase the external validity of our study, we emulated the
that perceived appearance homophily serves as a mediator between real conditions in which this phenomenon occurs by situating our study
similarity based on BMI and perceived reliability toward a human adver- in an online retail store and within a social media context. Furthermore,
tising model. This evidence suggests that in the context of thin adver- we included perceived vividness of the website as a moderator for the
tising models, women with BMIs similar to those of the advertising relationship between BMI similarity and perceived product quality (H6).
model perceive the advertising model to be more reliable.

4.1 | Method
3.3 | Discussion
4.1.1 | Participants and procedure
Study 1 demonstrates a positive relationship between BMI similarity
and appearance homophily for a human advertising model. Hence, the Three-hundred and thirty-one women (Mage = 38.8, SDage = 12.2)
results from Study 1 suggest that BMI similarity is a good proxy for were recruited through Amazon's MTurk to take part in an online
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 9

TABLE 3 Regression coefficients for Study 1.

Consequent

PAH (M1) PO (M2) PR (Y)

Antecedent Coef. SE z p Coef. SE z p Coef. SE z p


0
BMI (X) a1 0.373 0.058 6.415 <.001 a2 0.128 0.069 1.850 .064 c 0.051 0.069 0.743 .457
ES (C1) f11 0.101 0.046 2.202 .028 f11 0.113 0.052 2.203 .028 f11 0.021 0.069 0.301 .763
AS (C2) f21 0.038 0.05 0.754 .451 f21 0.043 0.056 0.761 .447 f21 0.102 0.058 1.755 .079
PAH (M1) b1 0.250 0.076 3.270 .001
PO (M2) b2 0.338 0.068 4.984 <.001
Constant iY 0 0.064 0 1 iY 0 0.069 0 1 iY 0 0.062 0 1
F(3,199) = 13.032, p < .001 F(3,199) = 1.488, p = .219 F(5.197) = 10.883, p < .001
R2 = .164 R2 = .022 R2 = .216

Note: Coefficients are standardized. Controls: ES = ethnic similarity, AS = age similarity.


Abbreviations: BMI, similarity based on body mass index; C1, first control; C2, second control; M1, first mediator; M2, second mediator; PAH, perceived
appearance homophily; PO, perceived originality; PR, perceived reliability; X, independent variable.

T A B L E 4 Mediation tests based on


Paths Coefficient Boot. SE Confidence intervala Equations
bootstrapping (Study 1).
BMI ! PAH ! PR 0.093 0.031 (0.032; 0.154) (a1b1)
BMI ! PO ! PR 0.043 0.025 (0.006; 0.092) (a2b2)
Direct effect BMI ! PR 0.051 0.069 (0.084, 0.187) (c0 )
Total effects 0.188 0.068 (0.054; 0.321) (c0 + a1b1 + a2b2)

Note: All coefficients are standardized.


Abbreviations: BMI, similarity based on body mass index; PAH, perceived appearance homophily;
PO, perceived originality; PR, perceived reliability.
a
10,000 bootstrap samples for bias-corrected confidence intervals at 95%.

study in exchange for a small cash incentive. The majority of the par- 4.1.3 | Moderating variable
ticipants had an annual household income of more than $50,000
(59.6% of the sample) and a minimum of an associate degree (67.8% Our conceptualization of website vividness follows a more compre-
of the sample). Participants entered a faux online store (see hensive approach in which a communication message includes both
Appendix B, Figures B1 and B2) where they were exposed to the central and peripheral elements. For example, Fortin and Dhola-
human advertising model (21 years of age, height 50 200 , 129.2 pounds, kia (2005, p. 395) indicate that they provided “enhanced vividness of
BMI of 20.5). Next, participants responded to questions regarding per- the message by means of colors, graphics, and animation” in their
ceived product quality and purchase intention for the dress endorsed research on social presence and involvement for web-based adver-
by the advertising model. tisements. Congruent with this conceptualization, we manipulated
website vividness in Study 2 by including animation of the top banner,
an animated cartoon image with sound of high-heeled steps, a
4.1.2 | Measures dynamic calendar, and an action button for the high vividness condi-
tion (compare Appendix B, Figures B1 and B2). The high and low viv-
The dependent variable—purchase intention for the dress endorsed idness groups resulted equivalent in terms of age, education level,
by the advertising model—was measured with a three-item, seven- income level, and time to complete the survey. As a manipulation
point Likert scale based on Wang et al. (2011). Perceived product check, participants rated perceived vividness on a four-item Likert
quality was measured with five items adapted from Jo's (2007) scale adapted from Coyle and Thorson (2001).
Absolute Quality of the Product scale. This scale measures con- In addition, we included the same control variables (age and eth-
sumers' evaluations of product quality without making comparisons nic similarity) as in Study 1. Appendix B, Table B1, shows the mea-
to any other product. The independent variable—BMI similarity— surement items and reliability assessment for Study 2. Following the
was calculated following the same procedure as in Study same criteria as in Study 1, validity and reliability of our measurement
1 (Appendix B, Figure B3 shows a distribution of BMI of each scales were satisfactory. The descriptive statistics and correlation
participant). matrix for Study 2 are shown in Table 5.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

T A B L E 5 Descriptive statistics and


Variable M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.
correlations for Study 2.
1. Purchase intention 3.33 1.71 -
2. Perceived product quality 4.26 1.17 0.73*** -
3. BMI similarity 94.16 4.87 0.24*** 0.17** -
4. Perceived vividness 3.93 1.67 0.34*** 0.28*** 0.24*** -
5. Age similarity 82.18 12.11 0.09 0.02 0.20*** 0.17** -

Note: Ethnicity: White = 254, Asian = 30, Afro-American = 19, Other = 23, Native = 5.
***p < .001; **p < .01.

4.2 | Results 4.3 | Discussion

4.2.1 | Manipulation check Study 2 provides evidence for our expectation that BMI similarity
is associated with perceived quality and purchase intention for
An ANOVA on the manipulation check question revealed that partici- fashion apparel. Hence, Study 2 shows that associations of BMI
pants in the high vividness condition rated perceived vividness of the similarity are not limited to perceptions of the advertising model.
website higher (M = 4.76, SD = 1.33) than those in the low vividness Rather, our findings suggest that this relationship relates to
condition (M = 2.93, SD = 1.48; F(1,329) = 139.8, p < .001). Hence, marketing-relevant outcome variables that ultimately relate to firm
our manipulation of website vividness was successful. performance and success. Congruent with prior research on vivid-
To test H3, H4, and H6, we used a moderated mediation model ness in communication messages (Frey & Eagly, 1993; Guadagno
which evaluates the effect of BMI similarity on purchase intention for et al., 2011), the positive association between BMI similarity and
the endorsed dress through perceived product quality. Perceived viv- perceived product quality diminishes when the retailer's website
idness moderates the relationship between BMI similarity and per- makes use of distracting elements and features aimed at increasing
ceived product quality. Hence, this model is equivalent to model 8 in the website's vividness. However, one can indeed also argue that
Hayes (2022). As in Study 1, all analyses were conducted using the this attenuating effect of vividness is a positive marketing outcome
processR package of the statistical software R. Figure 2 shows for those consumers whose BMI similarity with the advertising
the standardized path coefficients for this model. model is low. That is, when vividness is high, it does matter less
As predicted by H3, BMI similarity was positively related to per- whether BMI similarity is high or low for perceived product quality
ceived product quality (0.158, p < .01), which in turn had a positive of apparel promoted by the advertising model. In summary, the
association with purchase intention for the dress endorsed by the results from Study 2 provide process evidence and boundary con-
advertising model (0.706, p < .001) (H4). Furthermore, vividness (high ditions regarding how BMI similarity relates with purchase inten-
vs. low) had a negative moderating effect on the relationship between tion for fashion products in a context where consumers are
BMI similarity and purchase intention (0.080, p < .05). Hence, in exposed to advertising fashion models.
support of H6, perceived vividness acted as a suppressor for the rela-
tionship between BMI similarity and perceived product quality. Below
Table 6 presents the regression coefficients and Table 7 shows the 5 | ST UDY 3
mediation tests.
In Study 1, we showed the association between BMI similarity and
perceived appearance homophily, and in Study 2, we demonstrated
4.2.2 | Robustness check for BMI similarity that BMI similarity is positively related to perceived product quality
and purchase intention. However, the mechanism through which BMI
As a robustness check, we tested H3, H4, and H6 using perceived similarity affects perceived product quality may be driven also by
appearance homophily rather than BMI similarity as our independent other variables, apart from appearance homophily. For example, the
variable. The results of this model were consistent with the findings source credibility model (Ohanian, 1990; Wallace et al., 2021) sug-
presented here (see Web Appendix A, Figure I, Tables I and II). gests that trustworthiness and expertise influence attitudes toward
the source, whereas the source attractiveness model (McGuire, 1985;
Nguyen et al., 2013) suggests that familiarity with and likeability of
4.2.3 | Supplementary study on group similarity the source are related to attitudes. To test for these alternative expla-
nations, we ran five simple mediation models (Hayes PROCESS model
A supplementary study using a measure of group similarity (see Web 4) with BMI similarity as the IV and perceived product quality as the
Appendix B) provides additional evidence for the importance of simi- DV, and in each of the models we tested one of the five mediators:
larity measures for perceived product quality in the context of an appearance homophily, trustworthiness, expertise, familiarity, or
existing fashion clothing company (boohoo.com). likability.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 11

F I G U R E 2 Path coefficients for Study


3—Mediating effect of perceived product
quality and moderating effect of website
vividness. Standardized coefficients.
Significance level: ***p < .001;
**p < .01; *p < .05.

TABLE 6 Regression coefficients for Study 2.

Consequent

PQ (M) PI (Y)

Antecedent Coef. SE z p Coef. SE z p


BMI (X) a1 0.158 0.050 3.145 .002 c10 0.093 0.040 2.347 .019
VIV (W) a2 0.018 0.054 0.340 .734 c20 0.020 0.037 0.540 .589
BMI:IN (X:W) a3 0.059 0.051 1.171 .243 c30 0.072 0.038 1.923 .055
ES (C1) f1 0.105 0.061 1.721 .089 g1 0.122 0.035 3.465 .001
AS (C2) f2 0.063 0.118 0.531 .595 g2 0.060 0.040 1.509 .131
PQ (M) b 0.710 0.025 28.846 <.001
F(5,325) = 3.080, p = .010 F(6,324) = 73.666, p < .001
R2 = .045 R2 = .577

Note: Coefficients are standardized. Controls: ES = ethnic similarity, AS = age similarity.


Abbreviations: BMI, similarity based on body mass index; C1, first control; C2, second control; M, mediator; PI, purchase intention; PQ, perceived product
quality; VIV, vividness; W, moderator; X, independent variable.

5.1 | Method similarity—was calculated following the same procedure as in Study


1 and Study 2 (Appendix C, Figure C2 shows a distribution of BMI of
5.1.1 | Participants and procedure each participant). Perceived appearance homophily was measured as
in Study 1 with an adapted scale from McCroskey et al. (1975), and
Two-hundred and eight women (Mage = 31.5, SDage = 8.9) were perceived quality was measured as in Study 2 with an adapted scale
recruited through Amazon's MTurk to take part in an online study in from Jo (2007). In Study 3, we used Ohanian's (1990) scale to measure
exchange for a small cash incentive. Participants entered a faux online trustworthiness of the source with five items and expertise of the
store (see Appendix B, Figure C1) where they were exposed to the source with four items. Familiarity with the source was measured with
human advertising model (21 years of age, height 50 700 , 161 pounds, three items from Zhou et al. (2010). Finally, likeability of the source
BMI of 25.2). All participants were exposed to the same advertising was measured with two items from Reinhard and Messner (2009). We
model. again included the same control variables (age and ethnic similarity) as
in Study 1 and Study 2. Appendix C, Table C1, shows the measure-
ment items and reliability assessment for Study 3. Following the same
5.1.2 | Measures criteria as in Study 1 and Study 2, validity and reliability of our mea-
surement scales were satisfactory. In addition, Study 3 was consistent
The dependent variable—perceived product quality—was measured with Study 1 and Study 2, as all analyses were conducted using the
with the same five items adapted from Jo's (2007) “Absolute Quality processR package of the statistical software R. The descriptive statis-
of the Product” scale as in Study 2. The independent variable—BMI tics and correlation matrix for Study 3 are shown in Table 8.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

TABLE 7 Moderated mediation tests based on bootstrapping (Study 2) to explore conditional effects.

Mediation path: BMI ! PQ ! PI conditioned by VIV Coefficient Boot. SE Confidence intervala Equations
Indirect effect  VIV = low 0.074 0.050 (0.025; 0.172) (a1 + a3WLow)b
Indirect effect  VIV = high 0.158 0.053 (0.055; 0.261) (a1 + a3WHigh)b
Total indirect effect 0.116 0.037 (0.043; 0.188) (a1 + a3W)b
Direct effect 0.086 0.040 (0.009; 0.164) c1 0 + c3 0 W
Total effects  VIV = low 0.233 0.074 (0.087; 0.378) c10 + c30 WLow + (a1 + a3WLow)b
Total effects  VIV = high 0.172 0.069 (0.036; 0.308) c10 + c30 WHigh + (a1 + a3WHigh)b

Note: All coefficients are standardized.


Abbreviations: BMI, similarity based on body mass index; PI, purchase intention; PQ, perceived product quality; VIV, vividness.
a
10,000 bootstrap samples for bias-corrected confidence intervals at 95%.

TABLE 8 Descriptive statistics and correlations for Study 3.

Variable M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
1. Perceived product quality 5.80 0.69 -
2. Source trustworthiness 5.92 0.73 0.63*** -
3. Source expertise 5.95 0.72 0.58*** 0.83*** -
4. Source likeability 6.02 0.79 0.62*** 0.76*** 0.75*** -
5. Source familiarity 5.72 0.96 0.39*** 0.56*** 0.62*** 0.47*** -
6. Perceived appearance homophily 5.79 0.85 0.36*** 0.57*** 0.52*** 0.51*** 0.39*** -
7. BMI similarity 96.19 2.52 0.04 0.14** 0.08 0.14 0.08 0.19** -

8. Age similarity 92.24 8.61 0.02 0.13 0.18** 0.12 0.24*** 0.17* .10 -

Note: Ethnicity: White = 195, Asian = 1, Other = 6, Native = 3.


***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

5.2 | Results by the fact that the mediators in Study 3 are naturally correlated
among each other (see also the correlation matrix for Study 3 in
As predicted, the indirect effect of perceived appearance homophily Table 8). Hence, our interpretation focuses on the results from the
was positive and statistically significant (.069, 95% CI 0.016 to 0.123) simple mediation models.
in the simple mediation model. Furthermore, the indirect effects of
trustworthiness (0.091, 95% CI 0.007 to 0.176) and likeability (0.089,
95% CI 0.004 to 0.174) were positive and statistically significant. 5.3 | Discussion
However, the indirect effects of expertise (0.048, 95% CI 0.032 to
0.128) and familiarity (0.030, 95% CI 0.028 to 0.087) were not sta- The results from Study 3 suggest that BMI similarity is positively
tistically significant since the confidence interval crosses zero. related to perceived product quality, and that this relationship is medi-
Figure 3 shows the simple mediation models for the three significant ated by perceived appearance homophily, thereby supporting our
mediation paths through appearance homophily, trustworthiness, and main expectations regarding the role of BMI similarity and appearance
likeability. homophily. It is important to note that BMI similarity is an objective
Following Stoner et al. (2023), we also ran a parallel mediation measure based on consumers' weight and height, and the causality of
model with all mediators included in the same model (Figure 4). For the relationships in Study 3 (as well as in Study 1 and 2) must there-
this parallel mediation model with five mediators, none of the indirect fore flow from BMI similarity to any of the latent measures in our
effects were significant (appearance homophily: 0.003, 95% CI models. In other words, it is not conceivable that consumers' weight
0.031 to 0.025; trustworthiness: 0.052, 95% CI 0.018 to 0.123; and height could change based on perceptions of, for example,
likeability: 0.052, 95% CI 0.009 to 0.113; expertise: 0.008, 95% CI appearance homophily, perceived product quality, or purchase inten-
0.018 to 0.034; familiarity: 0.004, 95% CI 0.010 to 0.018). A highly tion for the apparel endorsed by the advertising model. We believe
plausible explanation for this finding is that the total effect in this par- that this characteristic of BMI similarity as a directly observable mea-
allel mediation model is decomposed into a direct effect and five indi- sure (similar to other directly observable measures such as age, gen-
rect effects with equal signs, with the result that each indirect effect der, or income) makes it a powerful variable for marketing theory and
becomes relatively small (compare, e.g., Pieters, 2017, for the decom- practice. At the same time, Study 3 reveals (not surprisingly) that
position of total effects in mediation models). This issue is aggravated other variables than appearance homophily can explain the effect of
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 13

F I G U R E 3 Path coefficients for Study


3—Simple mediation models for
(a) appearance homophily,
(b) trustworthiness, and (c) likeability.
Standardized coefficients. Significance
level: ***p < .001; *p < .05.

BMI similarity on perceived product quality. Specifically, Study 3 finds insights on how BMI similarity relates to perceived appearance homo-
that perceived trustworthiness and likeability are also influenced by phily and marketing-relevant outcome variables for apparel retail prod-
BMI similarity. Hence, even though Study 3 finds support for the role ucts. Specifically, the results demonstrate that both perceived
of appearance homophily as a mechanism explaining the relationship appearance homophily and BMI similarity can be associated with the
between BMI similarity and perceived product quality, alternative perceived reliability of advertising models (Study 1) and perceived prod-
explanations exist in terms of trustworthiness and likeability. uct quality perceptions (Study 2). Furthermore, a series of mediation
models shows that in addition to perceived appearance homophily,
trustworthiness and likeability are identified as plausible alternative
6 | G E N E R A L D I S C U S SI O N explanations for the relationship between BMI similarity and perceived
product quality (Study 3). Vividness of the retailer's website is identified
Drawing on the results from three empirical studies and a supplemen- as an important boundary condition for these effects which attenuates
tary study2 on group similarity reported in the Web Appendix, the cur- the relationship between BMI similarity and perceived product quality.
rent research contributes to marketing theory and practice by providing

2
6.1 | Theoretical implications
All studies used samples from Amazon's MTurk, which have been shown to be of better
quality than those from professional panels (Kees et al., 2017). Additionally, we followed
steps recommended by Aguinis et al. (2021) in combination with time controls and click The results of the current research yield important contributions to
counts from Qualtrics (e.g., Vazquez et al., 2023) to increase the quality of the samples. Some
of these steps included removing responses with no input activity or IP address, as these
psychology and marketing theory by providing an alternative theoreti-
were suspected web-robots; as well as removing responses of inattentive participants who cal view from that of previous research about female portrayals
did not select their gender as female when the questionnaire instructions invited only female
involving the body size (e.g., Åkestam et al., 2017; Janssen &
participants; or the lack of attention to correctly filling in the random unique number
generated in each questionnaire to validate it. Paas, 2014). First, our findings contribute to the extant literature on
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
14 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

F I G U R E 4 Path coefficients for Study


3—Parallel mediation model for the
association between BMI similarity and
perceived product quality. Standardized
coefficients. Significance level: **p < .01;
*p < .05;  p < .10.

homophily (Bilgicer et al., 2015; McPherson et al., 2001) and similarity draws cognitive resources, information processing will be hindered.
(Rodrigues et al., 2017; Thakor et al., 2008) by showing the link Even though previous research predominantly reports situations in
between traditional appearance homophily (Ladhari et al., 2020; which vivid messages have positive outcomes on attitudes toward the
McCroskey et al., 2006) and our refined construct of BMI similarity. message and message engagement (Coyle & Thorson, 2001; Fortin &
Specifically, the results from Study 1 demonstrate that BMI similarity Dholakia, 2005; Yousaf et al., 2020), our research shows that vivid-
is a parsimonious measure that captures major aspects of appearance ness can attenuate the processing of key information (such as differ-
homophily without the need to gather additional customer informa- ences in BMI between the advertising model and the customer).
tion, such as body shape, skin color, or facial features. Furthermore, Hence, in line with Frey and Eagly (1993), we find that website vivid-
our research demonstrates that consumers' BMI similarity to a human ness can distract the viewer and undermine information processing.
advertising model is positively associated with marketing-relevant This implies that vividness in communication messages can act as a
downstream variables, such as the perceived reliability of the advertis- double-edged sword: For consumers with high BMI similarity to the
ing model, perceived product quality, and purchase intention. advertising model, website vividness decreases the relationship
Second, our research reconciles conflicting streams of literature on between BMI similarity and perceived quality of the apparel promoted
the BMI of advertising models which either predict higher advertising by the advertising model, which is indeed an undesired outcome for
effectiveness for relatively thin (Borland & Akram, 2007; Janssen & marketers. On the other hand, website vividness has the effect that
Paas, 2014) or average-size models (Lou et al., 2019). Specifically, our BMI similarity matters less for perceived product quality, which is a
research finds that rather than an ideal BMI value fitting all consumers positive marketing outcome for those consumers who score very low
simultaneously, it is the match between the consumer's BMI with the on BMI similarity. Finally, the present work contributes to theory of
BMI of the advertising model that covaries with marketing-relevant social comparison processes (e.g., Festinger, 1954; Mussweiler, 2003)
outcome variables in the context of a thin, female advertising model. by showing that perceptions of BMI depend on the BMI of the
Furthermore, our research extends those streams of BMI literature that observer through an implicit comparison of an advertising model's
focus predominantly on obesity and health-related public policy impli- BMI with one's own BMI.
cations (Cornil et al., 2022; Zlatevska et al., 2018).
Third, the current research adds to the extant literature on vivid-
ness in communication by demonstrating that vivid elements 6.2 | Managerial implications
(e.g., peripheral animations on a website) can inhibit the processing of
relevant information. Keller and Block (1997) suggested that when a The results from our research also have important managerial implica-
vivid communication message is less resource demanding, the vivid- tions. First, building on our finding that BMI similarity (rather than an
ness of the message is going to facilitate the processing of the core absolute BMI value) positively relates to perceived product quality
content of the message. However, when the vividness of the message and purchase intention for female consumers, dynamic displays of
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 15

advertising models that adjust to the consumer's own BMI can pro- holds in the context of plus-size advertising models, given that some
vide a competitive advantage as compared to traditional marketing marketers have begun to employ plus-size advertising models in their
strategies relying on static reproductions of apparel models. Apparel marketing campaigns (Cinelli & Yang, 2016; Joo & Wu, 2021;
retailers aiming at enhancing their marketing effectiveness could Pounders, 2018). For example, future research on BMI similarity might
implement BMI recognition technology (Wen & Guo, 2013) in their build on the research design from McFerran et al. (2010a, 2010b) who
online and physical stores, which would allow them to dynamically tai- manipulated body type by exposing participants to either thin or
lor displays of apparel models according to their customers' BMI. Such heavy people around them. Such an extension of our research would
an approach would also help marketers with an organic inclusion of be timely and relevant since scholars have raised concerns about how
plus-size models in their marketing campaigns (Joo & Wu, 2021). This to mitigate the negative emotional effects of promoting thinness and
is consistent with the benefits from message personalization, which idealized images of “perfect” thin models to young women (Holland &
has proved to be effective in digital environments to target diverse Tiggemann, 2016; Kim & Chock, 2015; Perloff, 2014).
retail consumers as in the case of social commerce (e.g., Vazquez Third, BMI similarity is a relatively stable individual difference
et al., 2023). Beyond the online context, marketers could employ characteristic, similar to, for example, age or marital status. It can
smart shelves with dynamic LED screens displaying marketing mes- indeed change over time, but it is not influenced by situational factors.
sages based on the proximity and BMI of shoppers (Grewal Hence, we argue that BMI similarity can influence consumer judg-
et al., 2021). Because personalizing advertising based on BMI similar- ments and perceptions, such as perceived homophily, liking of the
ity promotes body diversity and inclusivity among apparel models and advertising model, or perceived product quality, but none of these
consumers, the use of BMI similarity could be crucial for brands that consumer perceptions can situationally change BMI similarity, in the
appeal for societal trends such as diversity and inclusion of varied same vein that situationally triggered consumer perceptions cannot
body types or body builds (e.g., heavy body type or overweight); change the age or marital status of a respondent. Nevertheless, we
which is in line with social movements promoting body positivity, fat acknowledge that our work builds on associations rather than causal
acceptance, plus-size, health at every size, anti-diet, and body neutral- relationships triggered by the manipulation of variables. Hence, even
ity. Furthermore, our research suggests that marketing managers aim- though BMI cannot be directly manipulated since it is based on the
ing at making use of the effects of BMI similarity should be careful height and weight of respondents, future research could manipulate
with deploying overly vivid websites in their customer communication perceived appearance homophily to show causality on outcome vari-
to avoid distracting consumers from the core message. ables such as perceived reliability of the advertising model, perceived
product quality, and purchase intention. Furthermore, future research
could follow very recent recommendations for more stringent tests of
7 | L I M I T A TI O NS A ND F U T U R E R E S E A R CH causality in models that measure both mediators and outcome vari-
ables (Laghaie & Otter, 2023).
Even though BMI has been widely accepted among health experts, it is Finally, the current research investigates the relationship between
an imperfect measure of body appearance. For example, BMI typically BMI similarity and website vividness in the context of online fashion
fails to detect the substitution of lean with fat body tissue that accom- apparel. We argue that this specific context is meaningful because
panies normal aging (Prentice & Jebb, 2001). Furthermore, an individual fashion apparel is closely related to the body and may in fact be seen
engaging in exercising or body building may have the same weight and as an extension of the self (Ahuvia, 2005). However, bodily appear-
height as an overweight person, but body appearance in such a case ance of advertising models may also be relevant for product catego-
may be quite dissimilar. Hence, BMI as a measure of appearance simi- ries that are not directly related to the body. Thus, future research
larity toward the advertising model may be biased, which in turn would may extend our work to product categories such as food, consumer
inflate or deflate relationships with downstream marketing variables electronics, automobiles, or services.
such as product perceptions and purchase intention for fashion apparel.
Future research may try to collect additional data on respondents' body ACKNOWLEDG MENTS
fat to determine potential bias in the BMI similarity measure. The authors are grateful for the helpful comments and guidance by
Second, since the female thin-ideal still prevails in mainstream two anonymous reviewers and the editors during the review process.
advertising (Cohen et al., 2017; Volonté, 2019), our research inten- The first author would also like to thank the National System of
tionally focuses on advertising models with a comparably low BMI Researchers (SNI) of CONAHCYT Mexico for providing incentives to
(Study 1: 17.8; Study 2: 20.5) but also extends these findings to a conduct this academic research.
slightly overweight advertising model (Study 3: 25.2). Even though
these BMI values are not necessarily exceptional and fall in the range CONFLIC T OF INTER E ST STATEMENT
of healthy weight (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2021), The authors state there are no conflicts of interest.
they are substantially below the average US female population of
about 28.42 (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). DATA AVAILABILITY STAT EMEN T
Thus, there is an opportunity for future research to investigate The data that support the findings of this study are available from the
whether the effect of BMI similarity shown in the current research corresponding author upon reasonable request.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
16 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

ORCID Coyle, J. R., & Thorson, E. (2001). The effects of progressive levels of inter-
Erik Ernesto Vazquez https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7592-1571 activity and vividness in web marketing sites. Journal of Advertising,
30(3), 65–77.
Reto Felix https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3864-2397
D'Alessandro, S., & Bill Chitty, B. (2011). Real or relevant beauty? Body
shape and endorser effects on brand attitude and body image. Psychol-
RE FE R ENC E S ogy & Marketing, 28(8), 843–878.
Aguinis, H., Villamor, I., & Ramani, R. S. (2021). MTurk research: Review Dantcheva, A., Bremond, F., & Bilinski, P. (2018). Show me your face and I
and recommendations. Journal of Management, 47(4), 823–837. will tell you your height, weight and body mass index. Proceedings
Ahuvia, A. C. (2005). Beyond the extended self: Loved objects and con- of the International Conference on Pattern Recognition, 3555–3560.
sumers' identity narratives. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), Beijing, China.
171–184. Das Guru, R. R., & Paulssen, M. (2020). Customers' experienced product
Åkestam, N., Rosengren, S., & Dahlen, M. (2017). Advertising “like a girl”: quality: Scale development and validation. European Journal of Market-
Toward a better understanding of “femvertising” and its effects. Psy- ing, 54(4), 645–670.
chology and Marketing, 34(8), 795–806. De Bruyn, A., & Lilien, G. L. (2008). A multi-stage model of word-of-mouth
Bagozzi, R. P., & Khoshnevis, M. (2022). How and when brand coolness influence through viral marketing. International Journal of Research in
transforms product quality judgments into positive word of mouth and Marketing, 25(3), 151–163.
intentions to buy/use. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 31, Dwyer, S., Richard, O., & Shepherd, C. D. (1998). An exploratory study of
383–402. https://doi.org/10.1080/10696679.2022.2081925 gender and age matching in the salesperson-prospective customer
Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation dyad: Testing similarity-performance predictions. Journal of Personal
models. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16, 74–94. Selling and Sales Management, 18(4), 55–69.
Balabanis, G., & Chatzopoulou, E. (2019). Under the influence of a blogger: Ertug, G., Brennecke, J., Kovács, B., & Zou, T. (2022). What does homo-
The role of information-seeking goals and issue involvement. Psychol- phily do? A review of the consequences of homophily. Academy of
ogy & Marketing, 36(4), 342–353. Management Annals, 16(1), 38–69.
Barry, B. (2014). Selling whose dream? A taxonomy of aspiration in fashion Felix, R., & Garza, M. R. (2012). Rethinking worldly possessions: The rela-
imagery. Fashion, Style & Popular Culture, 1(2), 175–192. tionship between materialism and body appearance for female con-
Bekk, M., Spörrle, M., Völckner, F., Spieß, E., & Woschée, R. (2017). What sumers in an emerging economy. Psychology & Marketing, 29(12),
is not beautiful should match: How attractiveness similarity affects 980–994.
consumer responses to advertising. Marketing Letters, 28, 509–522. Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Rela-
Bilgicer, T., Jedidi, K., Lehmann, D. R., & Neslin, S. A. (2015). Social conta- tions, 7(2), 117–140.
gion and customer adoption of new sales channels. Journal of Retailing, Filieri, R., Acikgoz, F., Li, C., & Alguezaui, S. (2023). Influencers' “organic”
91(2), 254–271. persuasion through electronic word of mouth: A case of sincerity over
Bjornsdottir, R. T., Hehman, E., Agboh, D., & Rule, N. O. (2021). Parsing brains and beauty. Psychology & Marketing, 40(2), 347–364.
the mechanisms underlying ingroup facial resemblance. Personality and Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with unob-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(5), 782–792. servable variables and measurement error: Algebra and statistics. Jour-
Borau, S., & Bonnefon, J.-F. (2017). The advertising performance of nal of Marketing Research, 18(3), 382–388.
non-ideal female models as a function of viewers' body mass index: A Fortin, D. R., & Dholakia, R. R. (2005). Interactivity and vividness effects
moderated mediation analysis of two competing affective pathways. on social presence and involvement with a web-based advertisement.
International Journal of Advertising, 36(3), 457–476. Journal of Business Research, 58(3), 387–396.
Borgatti, S. P., & Halgin, D. S. (2011). On network theory. Organization Sci- Fratto, G. M., Jones, M. R., & Cassill, N. L. (2006). An investigation of com-
ence, 22(5), 1168–1181. petitive pricing among apparel retailers and brands. Journal of Fashion
Borland, H., & Akram, S. (2007). Age is no barrier to wanting to look good: Marketing and Management: An International Journal, 10(4), 387–404.
Women on body image, age and advertising. Qualitative Market Frey, K. P., & Eagly, A. H. (1993). Vividness can undermine the persuasive-
Research: An International Journal, 10(3), 310–333. ness of messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1),
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. Academic Press. 32–44.
Campbell, N. C. G., Graham, J. L., Jolibert, A., & Gunther, H. (1988). Mar- Fu, S., Xu, Y., & Yan, Q. (2019). Enhancing the parasocial interaction rela-
keting negotiations in France, Germany, the United Kingdom, and the tionship between consumers through similarity effects in the context
United States. Journal of Marketing, 52(2), 49–62. of social commerce. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 27(2), 100–118.
Casalo , L. V., Flavián, C., & Ibáñez-Sánchez, S. (2020). Influencers on Insta- Golder, P. N., Mitra, D., & Moorman, C. (2012). What is quality? An integra-
gram: Antecedents and consequences of opinion leadership. Journal of tive framework of processes and states. Journal of Marketing, 76(4), 1–23.
Business Research, 117, 510–519. Grewal, D., Gauri, D. K., Roggeveen, A. L., & Sethuraman, R. (2021). Strate-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). National health and gizing retailing in the new technology area. Journal of Retailing, 97(1),
nutrition examination survey, 2017–2018. https://wwwn.cdc.gov/ 6–12.
Nchs/Nhanes/2017-2018/BMX_J.htm Guadagno, R. E., Rhoads, K. V. L., & Sagarin, B. J. (2011). Figural vividness
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2021). Defining adult over- and persuasion: Capturing the “elusive” vividness effect. Personality
weight & obesity. https://www.cdc.gov/obesity/adult/defining.html and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(5), 626–638.
Cinelli, M. D., & Yang, L. (2016). The role of implicit theories in evaluations Hayes, A. F. (2022). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
of “plus-size” advertising. Journal of Advertising, 45(4), 472–481. process analysis, second edition: A regression-based approach (3rd ed.).
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2017). The relationship between The Guilford Press.
Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activities and body image Heiser, R. S., Sierra, J. J., & Torres, I. M. (2008). Creativity via cartoon
concerns in young women. Body Image, 23, 183–187. spokespeople in print ads capitalizing on the distinctiveness effect.
Cornil, Y., Plassmann, H., Aron-Wisnewsky, J., Poitou-Bernert, C., Journal of Advertising, 37(4), 75–84.
Clément, K., Chabert, M., & Chandon, P. (2022). Obesity and respon- Hildebrand, D., Rubin, D., Hadi, R., & Kramer, T. (2021). Flavor fatigue:
siveness to food marketing before and after bariatric surgery. Journal Cognitive depletion influences consumer enjoyment of complex fla-
of Consumer Psychology, 32(1), 57–68. vors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 31(1), 103–111.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 17

Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of McCroskey, J. C., Richmond, V. P., & Daly, J. A. (1975). The development
the use of social networking sites on body image and disordered eat- of a measure of perceived homophily. Human Communication Research,
ing outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100–110. 1(4), 323–332.
Homburg, C., & Stock, R. M. (2005). Exploring the conditions under which McCroskey, L. L., McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (2006). Analysis and
salesperson work satisfaction can lead to customer satisfaction. Psy- improvement of the measurement of interpersonal attraction and
chology & Marketing, 22(5), 393–420. homophily. Communication Quarterly, 54(1), 1–31.
Hudders, L., & De Jans, S. (2022). Gender effects in influencer marketing: McFerran, B., Dahl, D. W., Fitzsimons, G. J., & Morales, A. C. (2010a). I'll
An experimental study on the efficacy of endorsements by same- have what she's having: Effects of social influence and body type on
vs. other-gender social media influencers on Instagram. International the food choices of others. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(6),
Journal of Advertising, 41(1), 128–149. 915–929.
Ismagilova, E., Slade, E., Ranac, N. P., & Dwivedi, Y. K. (2020). The effect of McFerran, B., Dahl, D. W., Fitzsimons, G. J., & Morales, A. C. (2010b).
characteristics of source credibility on consumer behaviour: A meta- Might an overweight waitress make you eat more? How the body type
analysis. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 53, 101736. of others is sufficient to alter our food consumption. Journal of Con-
Janssen, D. M., & Paas, L. J. (2014). Moderately thin advertising models sumer Psychology, 20(2), 146–151.
are optimal, most of the time: Moderating the quadratic effect of McGuire, W. J. (1985). Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E.
model body size on ad attitude by fashion leadership. Marketing Let- Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 233–346).
ters, 25, 167–177. Random House.
Jo, M. (2007). Should a quality sub-brand be located before or after the McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Beards of a feather:
parent brand? An application of composite concept theory. Journal of Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27,
the Academy of Marketing Science, 35(2), 184–196. 415–444.
Joo, B. R., & Wu, J. (2021). The impact of inclusive fashion advertising with Montoya, R. M., & Horton, R. S. (2013). A meta-analytic investigation of
plus-size models on female consumers: The mediating role of brand the processes underlying the similarity-attraction effect. Journal
warmth. Journal of Global Fashion Marketing, 12(3), 260–273. of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(1), 64–94.
Kees, J., Berry, C., Burton, S., & Sheehan, K. (2017). An analysis of data Moon, K. W., & Hong, S. (2021). Package ‘processR’. R Package version
quality: Professional panels, student subject pools, and Amazon's 0.2.6. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/processR/processR.pdf
Mechanical Turk. Journal of Advertising, 46(1), 141–155. Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mecha-
Keller, P. A., & Block, L. G. (1997). Vividness effects: A resource-matching nisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472–489.
perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(3), 295–304. Navarro, J., Peña, J., Cebolla, A., & Baños, R. (2022). Can avatar appearance
Kim, D. Y., & Kim, H.-Y. (2021). Trust me, trust me not: A nuanced view of influence physical activity? User-avatar similarity and proteus effects
influencer marketing on social media. Journal of Business Research, on cardiac frequency and step counts. Health Communication, 37(2),
134, 223–232. 222–229.
Kim, J. W., & Chock, T. M. (2015). Body image 2.0: Associations between Nguyen, B., Melewar, T. C., & Chen, J. (2013). A framework of brand like-
social grooming on Facebook and body image concerns. Computers in ability: An exploratory study of likeability in firm-level brands. Journal
Human Behavior, 48, 331–339. of Strategic Marketing, 21(4), 368–390.
Ladhari, R., Massa, E., & Skandrani, H. (2020). YouTube vloggers' popular- Ohanian, R. (1990). Construction and validation of a scale to measure
ity and influence: The roles of homophily, emotional attachment, and celebrity endorsers' perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and attrac-
expertise. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 54, 102027. tiveness. Journal of Advertising, 19(3), 39–52.
Laghaie, A., & Otter, T. (2023). Measuring evidence for mediation in the O'Quin, K., & Besemer, S. P. (1989). The development, reliability, and valid-
presence of measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 60(5), ity of the revised creative product semantic scale. Creativity Research
847–869. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222437231151873 Journal, 2(4), 267–278.
Lazarsfeld, P. F., & Merton, R. K. (1954). Friendship as a social process: A sub- Pechman, C., Yoon, K. E., Trapido, D., & Prochaska, J. J. (2021). Perceived
stantive and methodological analysis. In M. Berger, T. Abel, & C. H. Page costs vs. actual benefits of demographic self-disclosure in online sup-
(Eds.), Freedom and control in modern society (pp. 18–66). Van Nostrand. port groups. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 31(3), 450–477.
Leonhardt, J. M., Pezzuti, T., & Namkoong, J.-E. (2020). We're not so dif- Perloff, R. M. (2014). Social media effects on young women's body image
ferent: Collectivism increases perceived homophily, trust, and seeking concerns: Theoretical perspectives and an agenda for research. Sex
user-generated product information. Journal of Business Research, 112, Roles, 71(11), 363–377.
160–169. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Cen-
Li, Y., & Peng, Y. (2021). Influencer marketing: Purchase intention and its tral and peripheral routes to attitude change. Springer.
antecedents. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 39(7), 960–978. Pieters, R. (2017). Meaningful mediation analysis: Plausible causal infer-
Lin, L., Hoegg, J., & Aquino, K. (2018). When beauty backfires: The effects ence and informative communication. Journal of Consumer Research,
of server attractiveness on consumer taste perceptions. Journal of 44(3), 692–716.
Retailing, 94(3), 296–311. Pounders, K. (2018). Are portrayals of female beauty in advertising finally
Lou, C., Tse, C. H., & Lwin, M. O. (2019). “Average-sized” models do sell, changing. Journal of Advertising Research, 58(2), 133–137.
but what about east asia? A cross-cultural investigation of U.S. and Prendergast, G., Ko, D., & Yuen, S. Y. V. (2010). Online word of mouth and
Singaporean women. Journal of Advertising, 48(5), 512–531. consumer purchase intentions. International Journal of Advertising,
Lv, X., Liang, Y., Luo, J., & Liu, Y. (2022). Icing on the cake or gilding the 29(5), 687–708.
lily? The impact of high-modified model images on purchase intention. Prentice, A. M., & Jebb, S. A. (2001). Beyond body mass index. Obesity
Journal of Retailing and Consumer Studies, 68, 103078. Reviews, 2(3), 141–147.
MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2011). Construct Racherla, P., Mandviwalla, M., & Connolly, D. J. (2012). Factors affecting
measurement and validation procedures in MIS and behavioral consumers' trust in online product reviews. Journal of Consumer Behav-
research: Integrating new and existing techniques. MIS Quarterly, iour, 11(2), 94–104.
35(2), 293–334. Reinhard, M. A., & Messner, M. (2009). The effects of source likeability
McCracken, G. (1989). Who is the celebrity endorser? Cultural foundations of and need for cognition on advertising effectiveness under explicit per-
the endorsement process. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(3), 310–321. suasion. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 8(4), 179–191.
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
18 VAZQUEZ ET AL.

Risselada, H., Verhoef, P. C., & Bijmolt, T. H. (2014). Dynamic effects of proposed remedies. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 44,
social influence and direct marketing on the adoption of high-technol- 119–134.
ogy products. Journal of Marketing, 78(2), 52–68. Wallace, L. E., Simon, K. A., & Wegener, D. T. (2021). Lay concepts of
Rodrigues, D., Lopes, D., Alexopoulos, T., & Goldenberg, L. (2017). A source likeability, trustworthiness, expertise, and power: A prototype
new look at online attraction: Unilateral initial attraction and the analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 53, 1188–1201.
pivotal role of perceived similarity. Computers in Human Behavior, Wang, Y. J., Minor, M. S., & Wei, J. (2011). Aesthetics and the online shop-
74, 16–25. ping environment: Understanding consumer responses. Journal of
Semaan, R. W., Kocher, B., & Gould, S. (2018). How well will this brand Retailing, 87(1), 46–58.
work? The ironic impact of advertising disclosure of body-image Wen, L., & Guo, G. (2013). A computational approach to body mass index
retouching on brand attitudes. Psychology & Marketing, 35(10), prediction from face images. Image and Vision Computing, 31(5),
766–777. 392–400.
Soh, H., Reid, L. N., & King, K. W. (2009). Measuring trust in advertising: White, A., & Smith, B. L. (2001). Assessing advertising creativity using the
Development and validation of the ADTRUST scale. Journal of Adver- creative product semantic scale. Journal of Advertising Research, 41(6),
tising, 38(2), 83–103. 27–34.
Sokolova, K., & Kefi, H. (2020). Instagram and YouTube bloggers promote Yousaf, A., Amin, I., Jaziri, D., & Mishra, A. (2020). Effect of message
it, why should I buy? How credibility and parasocial interaction influ- orientation/vividness on consumer engagement for travel brands on
ence purchase intentions. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, social networking sites. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 30(1),
53, 101742. 44–57.
Sokolova, T., & Krishna, A. (2016). Take it or leave it: How choosing versus Zebal, M. A., & Jackson, F. H. (2019). Cues for shaping purchase of local
rejecting alternatives affects information processing. Journal of Con- retail apparel clothing brands in an emerging economy. International
sumer Research, 43(4), 614–635. Journal of Retail and Distribution Management, 47(10), 1013–1028.
Steuer, J. (1992). Defining virtual reality: Dimensions defining telepre- Zhou, L., Zhiyong, Y., & Hui, M. K. (2010). Non-local or local brands? A
sence. Journal of Communication, 42(4), 73–93. multi-level investigation into confidence in brand origin identification
Stoner, J. L., Felix, R., & Stadler Blank, A. (2023). Best practices for imple- and its strategic implications. Journal of Academy of Marketing Science,
menting experimental research methods. International Journal of Con- 38(2), 202–218.
sumer Studies, 47(4), 1579–1595. Zlatevska, N., Neumann, N., & Dubelaar, C. (2018). Mandatory calorie dis-
Stone-Romero, E. F., Stone, D. L., & Grewal, D. (1997). Development of a closure: A comprehensive analysis of its effect on consumers and
multidimensional measure of perceived product quality. Journal of retailers. Journal of Retailing, 94(1), 89–101.
Quality Management, 2(1), 87–111.
Streukens, S., & Andreassen, T. W. (2013). Customer preferences for front-
line employee traits: Homophily and heterophily effects. Psychology &
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Marketing, 30(12), 1043–1052.
Thakor, M. V., Suri, R., & Saleh, K. (2008). Effects of service setting and
other consumers' age on the service perception of young consumers. Erik Ernesto Vazquez is an Associate Professor of Digital Business
Journal of Retailing, 84(2), 137–149.
and Data Analytics and the Director of the Graduate Studies in
Torres, I. M. (2007). A tale of two theories: Sympathy or competition?
Journal of Business Research, 60(3), 197–205. Digital Business Analytics at EMLV Business School in Paris. He
Van der Land, S. F., Schouten, A. P., Feldberg, F., Huysman, M., & van den holds a Doctoral degree from the University of Newcastle upon
Hooff, B. (2015). Does avatar appearance matter? How team visual Tyne, UK and an MBA from the University of North Carolina,
similarity and member-avatar similarity influence virtual team perfor-
USA. Prof. Vazquez has led research projects that have been pub-
mance. Human Communication Research, 41(1), 128–153.
lished in journals such as Journal of Retailing and Consumer Ser-
Van Esch, P., Arli, D., Castner, J., Talukdar, N., & Northey, G. (2018). Con-
sumer attitudes towards bloggers and paid blog advertisements: vices, Electronic Markets, and Journal of Research in Interactive
What's new? Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 36(7), 778–793. Marketing. He is also a national research member Level I of the
Vazquez, E. E. (2020). Effects of enduring involvement and perceived con- CONAHCYT (also known as CONACYT), a member of the edito-
tent vividness on digital engagement. Journal of Research in Interactive
rial board of the Anahuac Journal, and an executive advisor to
Marketing, 14(1), 1–16.
Vazquez, E. E. (2021). Effect of an e-retailer's product category and social international companies, scaleups, and startups.
media platform selection on perceived quality of e-retail products.
Reto Felix is an Associate Professor of Marketing at the Univer-
Electronic Markets, 31, 139–157.
Vazquez, E. E., Patel, C., Alvidrez, S., & Siliceo, L. (2023). Images, reviews, sity of Texas Rio Grande Valley, USA and holds a PhD from the
and purchase intention on social commerce: The role of mental imag- University of St. Gallen, Switzerland. His research focuses on
ery vividness, cognitive and affective social presence. Journal of Retail- green consumption, consumer well-being, and new technologies.
ing and Consumer Services, 74, 103415.
He has been a visiting scholar at the Marketing Group, Haas
Venkatesh, A., Joy, A., Sherry, J. F., & Deschenes, J. (2010). The aesthetics
of luxury fashion, body and identify information. Journal of Consumer School of Business, University of California, Berkeley, and has
Psychology, 20(3), 459–470. published in journals such as Journal of International Marketing,
Volonté, P. (2019). The thin ideal and the practice of fashion. Journal of Journal of Business Research, International Marketing Review,
Consumer Culture, 19(2), 252–270.
European Journal of Marketing, Journal of Consumer Marketing,
Von Wangenheim, F., & Bayo  n, T. (2004). The effect of word of mouth on
services switching. European Journal of Marketing, 38(9/10), 1173– Journal of Consumer Behaviour, International Journal of Con-
1185. sumer Studies, Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, Com-
Voorhees, C. M., Brady, M. K., Calantone, R., & Ramirez, E. (2016). Discrim- puters in Human Behavior, and Psychology & Marketing.
inant validity testing in marketing: An analysis, causes for concern, and
14791838, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cb.2274 by The University Of Texas Rio Grande Vallley, Wiley Online Library on [31/10/2023]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
VAZQUEZ ET AL. 19

SUPPORTING INF ORMATION


Lorena Siliceo is an HR executive at SumUp, France and an affili-
Additional supporting information can be found online in the Support-
ated lecturer of management at the Anahuac University, Mexico.
ing Information section at the end of this article.
She holds a Master of Science in International Business from the
University of Edinburgh, UK and has conducted research related
to addressing inequality and diversity as consultant to The City of How to cite this article: Vazquez, E. E., Felix, R., & Siliceo, L.
Edinburgh Council, UK. Mrs. Siliceo has extensive experience in (2023). Female advertising models and their body mass index
the fashion, retailing, and fintech industries serving in executive (BMI): How does BMI similarity affect perceived product
positions for startups and scaleups. Mrs. Siliceo also served at quality? Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 1–19. https://doi.org/
Regnum Christi where she managed multiple private schools in 10.1002/cb.2274
Latin America. Her research interests revolve around people ana-
lytics, entrepreneurial finance, as well as diversity and inclusion.

You might also like