You are on page 1of 11

Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Biological Psychology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho

Prolonged NoGo P3 latency as a possible neurobehavioral correlate of


aggressive and antisocial behaviors: A Go/NoGo ERP study
Carl Delfin a, b, c, *, Märta Wallinius a, b, c, Malin Björnsdotter d, Emily Ruzich e, Peter Andiné b, f, g
a
Lund Clinical Research on Externalizing and Developmental Psychopathology, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Clinical Sciences Lund, Lund University,
Lund, Sweden
b
Centre for Ethics, Law and Mental Health, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of
Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
c
Research Department, Regional Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Växjö, Sweden
d
Department of Affective Psychiatry, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
e
MedTech West, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
f
Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
g
Department of Forensic Psychiatry, National Board of Forensic Medicine, Gothenburg, Sweden

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Aggressive and antisocial behaviors are detrimental to society and constitute major challenges in forensic mental
Aggression health settings, yet the associated neural circuitry remains poorly understood. Here, we investigated differences
Antisocial behaviors in aggressive and antisocial behaviors between healthy controls (n = 20) and violent mentally disordered of­
Event-related potentials
fenders (MDOs; n = 26), and examined associations between aggressive and antisocial behaviors, behavioral
Go/NoGo
inhibitory control, and neurophysiological activity across the whole sample (n = 46). Event-related potentials
were obtained using EEG while participants completed a Go/NoGo response inhibition task, and aggressive and
antisocial behaviors were assessed with the Life History of Aggression (LHA) instrument. Using a robust Bayesian
linear regression approach, we found that MDOs scored substantially higher than healthy controls on LHA
Aggression and Antisocial subscales. Using the whole sample and after adjusting for age, we found that scores on
the LHA Aggression and Antisocial subscales were robustly associated with longer NoGo P3 latency, and less
robustly with longer NoGo N2 latency. Post-hoc analyzes suggested that healthy controls and MDOs exhibited
similar associations. With several limitations in mind, we suggest that prolonged NoGo P3 latency, reflecting
decreased neural efficiency during the later stages of conflict monitoring or outcome evaluation, is a potential
neurobehavioral correlate of aggressive and antisocial behaviors.

1. Introduction Baker, 2013). Still, some important differences between the two forms
have been highlighted. Covertly aggressive forms of antisocial behav­
Antisocial behaviors, usually defined as actions and attitudes that iors, for instance, appear more closely associated with impaired
violate societal norms and the rights of others (Burt, 2012), exact a behavioral control, whereas overtly aggressive forms may instead pri­
tremendous toll on society, both financially (Scott, Knapp, Henderson, & marily reflect affective dysregulation (Burt & Donnellan, 2008; Hop­
Maughan, 2001) and in terms of reduced quality of life and well-being wood et al., 2009).
among those affected (Hanson, Sawyer, Begle, & Hubel, 2010; Tan & In forensic mental health settings, aggressive and antisocial behav­
Haining, 2016). Although the term ‘antisocial behavior’ is broad and iors represent both prime targets for therapeutic interventions as well as
often parsed into overtly aggressive (such as fighting, bullying, and risk factors for an unsafe treatment environment (Lane, Kjome, &
threatening) and covertly aggressive (such as lying, stealing, and Moeller, 2011; Meyer, Cummings, Proctor, & Stahl, 2016; Trestman,
vandalizing) behaviors (Burt, 2012; Tackett, Krueger, Iacono, & McGue, 2017). The importance of aggressive and antisocial behaviors in this
2005), both forms appear to stem from a common latent factor, with context is underscored by research showing that between ~ 30% and ~
joint genetic and environmental influences (Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, & 60% of forensic psychiatric inpatients commit at least one violent assault

* Correspondence to: Research Department, Regional Forensic Psychiatric Clinic, Johan Allgulins väg 1, Växjö, Sweden.
E-mail address: carl.delfin@med.lu.se (C. Delfin).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108245
Received 11 October 2021; Received in revised form 29 November 2021; Accepted 22 December 2021
Available online 24 December 2021
0301-0511/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

during their hospitalization (Broderick, Azizian, Kornbluh, & Warbur­ latency is believed to be an index of “neural efficiency”, or processing
ton, 2015; Verstegen, de Vogel, de Vries Robbé, & Helmerhorst, 2017). speed (van Dinteren, Arns, Jongsma, & Kessels, 2014). The
Recently, Klein Tuente, Bogaerts, and Veling (2021) found a staff-rated meta-analysis by Pasion et al. (2019) focused solely on amplitude, likely
prevalence as high as 86% for at least one occurrence of moderate reflecting a lack of research on N2 latency, and out of the 36 studies
aggressive behavior, with a corresponding prevalence of 65% for severe included in Pasion et al. (2018) only nine analyzed latency effects on a
aggressive behavior and 37% for physical aggression, during 30 weeks group level. Even fewer, a mere six studies, included latency in dimen­
of monitoring of male forensic psychiatric patients. While patients with sional analyses. Thus, in contrast to previous meta-analytic research
at least one incident of physical aggression had a higher prevalence of demonstrating delayed P3 latency in aggressive and antisocial in­
Cluster B personality disorders in the study by Klein Tuente et al. (2021), dividuals (Gao & Raine, 2009), the findings of Pasion et al. (2018) were
there is, unfortunately, little to no systematic evidence of effective too inconsistent to draw firm conclusions. Similarly, while some studies
treatment of aggressive and antisocial behaviors at the severe end of the have observed delayed N2 latencies in individuals with poor response
spectrum (Bateman, Gunderson, & Mulder, 2015; van den Bosch, inhibition (Falkenstein, Hoormann, & Hohnsbein, 1999) and alcohol
Rijckmans, Decoene, & Chapman, 2018). In fact, the aggressive aspects dependence (Porjesz, Begleiter, Bihari, & Kissin, 1987), others report no
of mental disorders appear notably understudied compared to other delays in N2 latency in individuals with alcohol abuse (Pandey et al.,
emotional behaviors (Flanigan & Russo, 2019). 2012) and ADHD (Woltering, Liu, Rokeach, & Tannock, 2013). The lack
In light of the apparent lack of evidence of effective treatments, of studies including latency analyses, in conjunction with the inconsis­
developing novel interventions that can prevent aggressive outbursts tent findings of available research, signals an apparent need for further
and antisocial tendencies remains a key objective in forensic mental investigation of the role of ERP latency in aggressive and antisocial
health research. For the development of novel interventions to succeed, behaviors.
some researchers argue, a better understanding of the neural circuitry To date, most ERP research has focused on the so-called P3a and P3b
associated with aggressive and antisocial behaviors is vital (Flanigan & components. The frontally distributed P3a component, also called the
Russo, 2019), and using a combination of biological and behavioral “novelty P3”, is elicited by task-irrelevant infrequent stimuli, whereas
measures may be an especially effective strategy in this regard (Nelson & the more parietally distributed P3b component is believed to reflect the
Trainor, 2007). Indeed, such a “multimethod” approach aligns well with detection of task-relevant infrequent stimuli. The frontally distributed
recent initiatives calling for research across multiple “levels of analysis”, NoGo P3 (and the preceding NoGo N2), however, is elicited by infre­
including the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Cuthbert, 2014) and the quent stimuli that requires withholding a response, such as in response
Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; Perkins et al., inhibition tasks (Polich, 2007). Although still debated, the NoGo
2020). component is generally believed to reflect processes associated with
Response inhibition — the ability to inhibit unwanted or inappro­ monitoring and outcome evaluation (Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2013;
priate actions — is a core component of executive control (Friedman & Huster et al., 2013). The NoGo component is also especially meaningful
Miyake, 2017; Miyake et al., 2000) and an integral part of both the RDoC in a forensic mental health context, since it may be used to examine the
and the HiTOP frameworks. Poor response inhibition has been robustly “frontal dysfunction hypothesis”, which suggests that aggressive and
associated with both aggressive and antisocial behaviors in the general antisocial behaviors may be associated with weakened prefrontal ac­
population (Dambacher et al., 2015; Madole, Johnson, & Carver, 2020) tivity during response inhibition (Pasion et al., 2018; Rodman et al.,
as well as among mentally disordered offenders (MDOs) (Tonnaer, Cima, 2016).
& Arntz, 2016a, 2016b). Longitudinal studies have shown that poor Taken together, reduced amplitude and prolonged latency of the
response inhibition in childhood and adolescence is predictive of a wide NoGo N2 and P3 components may constitute transdiagnostic neuro­
range of externalizing behaviors later in life (Bohlin, Eninger, Brocki, & behavioral correlates of aggressive and antisocial behaviors. So far,
Thorell, 2012; Nigg et al., 2006), and the association appears primarily however, few studies have investigated the NoGo N2/P3 components in
genetic in origin (Young et al., 2009), suggesting a shared liability. relation to aggressive and antisocial behaviors, and fewer still have
Neurophysiological activity related to response inhibition can be included latency in dimensional analyses. Here, we addressed this
investigated using event-related potentials (ERPs) acquired with elec­ knowledge gap using data from healthy controls and violent MDOs. We
troencephalography (EEG). Two ERP components in particular, the N2 first investigated differences in aggressive and antisocial behaviors be­
and the P3 — both typically elicited using variants of the Go/NoGo tween healthy controls and MDOs, and then, using the whole sample,
response inhibition task (Luijten et al., 2014) — are believed to reflect examined associations between aggressive and antisocial behaviors,
processes either directly or indirectly related to inhibition (Huster, behavioral inhibitory control, and NoGo N2 and P3 amplitude and la­
Enriquez-Geppert, Lavallee, Falkenstein, & Herrmann, 2013). Of the tency. We hypothesized that MDOs would exhibit higher levels of
two, the P3 component is probably the best known, and reduced P3 aggressive and antisocial behaviors, and that aggressive and antisocial
amplitude has long been envisioned as a neurobiological marker of behaviors would be associated with decreased behavioral inhibitory
aggressive and antisocial behaviors (e.g., Patrick et al., 2006). Still, a control, decreased NoGo N2 and NoGo P3 amplitude, and prolonged
moderating effect of N2 (but not P3) amplitude on the relationship be­ NoGo N2 and NoGo P3 latency.
tween inhibitory control and aggression has also been observed (Rawls,
Jabr, Moody, & Lamm, 2018), and that both components may be of 2. Methods
importance is further supported by two recent meta-analyses: Pasion
et al. (2019) found that severe manifestations of aggressive and anti­ 2.1. Ethics
social behaviors, including violence and antisocial personality disorder,
was associated with reduced N2 amplitude, ostensibly related to deficits The study was approved by the regional ethics review board in
in response inhibition, and Pasion, Fernandes, Pereira, and Barbosa Linköping, Sweden (2017/56-31, 2018/7-32, 2018/321-32) and was
(2018) found a clear relationship between reduced P3 amplitude and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All partici­
antisocial behaviors. pants provided voluntary, informed consent prior to participation.
Although most ERP research has focused on the amplitude of the N2
and P3, distinguishing between the amplitude and the latency, usually 2.2. Participants
defined as the time point where the amplitude peaks, of an ERP
component may be important, since the two might reflect different kinds Inpatient MDOs were recruited from an ongoing research project at a
of neural activity. Specifically, amplitude is believed to be an index of maximum security forensic psychiatric hospital in Sweden (details on
“neural power”, or the amount of allocated cognitive resources, whereas the project and the full cohort are available in Laporte, Ozolins,

2
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Westling, Westrin, & Wallinius, 2021). Following participation in the items depicting school disciplinary problems, problems with supervisors
project, all male MDOs were approached and asked to participate in the at work, and antisocial behaviors both with and without police
current study if they had, at some point in their life, been sentenced for a involvement, with possible scores ranging from 0 to 20. The LHA may be
violent crime, on the condition that they did not have a history of brain scored using either self-reports or clinical ratings. In the current study,
damage with lasting effects and that the treating psychiatrist did not self-reports were used for healthy controls, and clinical ratings, per­
deem participation unsuitable due to current psychiatric status or safety formed by one out of three research assistants with several years of
concerns. experience in forensic psychiatry and subsequently validated by a
Out of 35 available and eligible MDOs, a total of 29 agreed to licensed psychologist, were used for MDOs.
participate, although two MDOs were omitted due to missing or In line with current recommendations (Hayes & Coutts, 2020;
incomplete self-report and/or file data, and one MDO was omitted from McNeish, 2018), the internal reliability of the included LHA subscales
further analyzes due to having only two correct NoGo trials left after was assessed separately for healthy controls and MDOs using McDo­
EEG data preprocessing. Previous work has shown that the P3, when nald’s Omega total (ωt ). Both subscales demonstrated satisfactory reli­
elicited using a Go/NoGo task, reaches adequate internal consistency ability, with ωt = 0.77 and ωt = 0.78 for LHAAGG in healthy controls and
after 8–14 trials, depending on which measure is used (Rietdijk, MDOs, respectively, and ωt = 0.68 and ωt = 0.78 for LHAANTI in healthy
Franken, & Thurik, 2014). More recently, it was demonstrated that the controls and MDOs, respectively.
P3 may reach adequate internal consistency with as few as 10 trials,
especially if averaging over multiple sensors and time points, as is the 2.3.2. Inhibitory control
case in the current study, whereas excellent internal consistency may be A Go/NoGo task based on previous work (Kiehl, Liddle, & Hopfinger,
achieved at 20 trials (Thigpen, Kappenman, & Keil, 2017). Although the 2000; Steele, Maurer, Bernat, Calhoun, & Kiehl, 2016) was implemented
participant with only two NoGo trials was omitted, it should be noted using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc.) in order to
that two MDOs had 9 correct NoGo trials each, and two healthy controls assess inhibitory control and elicit ERPs. Participants were comfortably
had 6 and 12 correct NoGo trials each. We decided to keep these par­ seated approximately 60 cm from a 22-inch widescreen LCD TFT
ticipants for subsequent analyzes in order to keep the sample size at a monitor (1680 × 1050 resolution @ 60 Hz). With both oral and
reasonable level. Remaining participants (N = 42, 91.3%) had 14 or on-screen instructions, participants were told to respond as quickly and
more correct NoGo trials available for analysis, with the majority of accurately as they could, by pressing the left mouse button, every time
participants (N = 37, 80.4%) having 20 or more, and with an average of the Go stimulus (a white “X”) appeared on-screen, but to withhold their
28 trials (SD = 10.2) for MDOs and 28.2 trials (SD = 9.9) for healthy response every time a NoGo stimulus (a white “K”) appeared. Both Go
controls. Thus, a total of 26 MDOs, aged 20–57 years (M = 37.2, SD = and NoGo stimuli were presented on-screen for 250 ms in white text
9.5), were included in the current study. In one MDO approximately one against a black background. A fixation point (+) located at the center of
third of the EEG data was lost due to technical issues. All MDOs the screen was shown between trials, and we used an inter-stimulus
participated while on their usual treatment plan, including medication, interval pseudorandomly distributed between 1000 and 3000 ms at
and received a voucher (~ $10) for use either in the hospital’s kiosk or at 500 ms increments. Participants completed 10 practice trials prior to
a local mall as reimbursement directly after participation. beginning the task, and had to answer correct on at least 50% of these
Healthy controls were recruited among hospital staff and university trials in order to proceed. The task then consisted of 326 trials, 264
students using posters, e-mail, and verbal information. We aimed to (84%) of which were Go trials, in order to establish a dominant response,
match control participants as closely as possible to MDOs with regards to and 52 (16%) of which were NoGo trials. These trials were divided into
education and age, and thus having received a degree after three or more two blocks of 162 trials each, and participants could rest for as long as
years of higher education was an exclusion criteria, in addition to having they wanted in between.
a history of brain damage with lasting effects, having a current major
mental disorder, and self-reported drug use within the last 6 months. 2.4. EEG data acquisition, preprocessing, and reliability
Although a total of 25 control group participants were recruited, five
had to be excluded after participation (but prior to data analysis) due to 2.4.1. EEG data acquisition
self-reported drug use within the last 6 months and/or having a history A high-impedance NetStation NA400 amplifier (Electrical Geodesics,
of brain damage with lasting effects, resulting in 20 healthy controls, Inc.) and a 128 Ag/AgCl electrode Hydrocel Geodesic sensor net was
aged 20–58 years (M = 33.1, SD = 11.8), included in the current study. used to record EEG signals. The EEG signals were recorded at 1000 Hz
All healthy controls received either a voucher (~ $10) for use at a local while referenced to the midline vertex electrode (Cz), and we aimed to
mall or a movie ticket as reimbursement directly after participation. keep impedances below 100 kΩ. Due to, for instance, time constraints,
With minor differences, the sample used in the current study has this was not achieved for all relevant electrodes on all participants.
been used in a previous publication, in which further details on partic­ Although lower impedances are preferable, a study conducted by re­
ipant and clinical characteristics are available (Delfin, Ruzich, Wall­ searchers from the equipment manufacturer showed that impedances
inius, Björnsdotter, & Andiné, 2020). below 200 kΩ still allow for accurate (less than 0.1% error) signal
acquisition (Ferree, Luu, Russell, & Tucker, 2001). We achieved im­
2.3. Measures pedances below 100 kΩ for 84% of healthy controls and 94% of MDOs,
and impedances below 200 kΩ for 94% of healthy controls and 96% of
2.3.1. Aggressive and antisocial behaviors MDOs, with average impedances of 51.5 kΩ (SD = 143.9) for MDOs and
The Life History of Aggression (LHA; Coccaro, Berman, & Kavoussi, 60.7 kΩ (SD = 70.6) for healthy controls (note that due to technical
1997) instrument, consisting of 11 items rated from 0 (has never errors during data collection, impedance data was missing for three
occurred) to 5 (has occurred an uncountable number of times), was used healthy controls). Furthermore, since all recordings were carried out in
to assess lifetime aggressive and antisocial behaviors. The 11 LHA items cool and dry environments, and since we used state-of-the-art filtering,
are summed in a total score and divided into three separate subscales: artifact detection, and artifact rejection methods, any remaining high
Aggression (LHAAGG), Consequences/Antisocial Behavior (LHAANTI), impedances should have negligible impact on our results (Kappenman &
and Self-directed Aggression. Here, only scores on the LHAAGG and Luck, 2010). The entire EEG acquisition procedure, including an addi­
LHAANTI subscales were used. The LHAAGG subscale consists of 5 items tional resting-state task, took approximately 45 min for each
describing physical fights, physical assaults on people, animals, or participant.
property, as well as temper tantrums and verbal aggression, with
possible scores ranging from 0 to 25. The LHAANTI subscale consists of 4

3
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

2.4.2. EEG data preprocessing Group differences were modeled using a robust simple linear
All EEG data preprocessing was carried out using the MNE-Python regression approach that allowed for unequal variances between the
module, version 0.20.4 (Gramfort, 2013; Gramfort et al., 2014) groups, with LHAAGG and LHAANTI scores used as outcome variables and
running on Python version 3.8.2. The preprocessing pipeline largely a dummy coded group variable used as predictor variable. Similarly, a
adhered to recommended procedures (Jas et al., 2018) and was, with the robust multiple linear regression approach was used to model the as­
exception of the removal and interpolation of bad channels, independent sociation between aggressive and antisocial behaviors and inhibitory
component analysis (ICA) components, and artifacts following visual control and ERP measures. Specifically, LHAAGG and LHAANTI scores
inspection, fully automated. Prior to the automated procedure all EEG were entered as predictor variables, with NoGo accuracy and ERP
data was visually inspected and bad channels marked. Then, a bandpass measures entered as outcome variables. Due to its robust association
filter from 0.1 to 30 Hz was applied, bad channels interpolated, and ICA with both P3 amplitude and latency (e.g., Johnstone, Pleffer, Barry,
was used to identify and remove artifacts. To circumvent issues related Clarke, & Smith, 2005), we included age as a covariate in these models.
to ICA being sensitive to low frequency drifts, we bandpass filtered a raw Finally, a robust correlation approach was used for reliability analysis.
copy of the data from 1 to 30 Hz and used that to find ICA components. All variables were centered and scaled prior to analysis in order to
The ICA components were visually assessed, and components judged to obtain standardized estimates, robustness was achieved by using a
represent eye blinks, saccades, heartbeats, and other non-brain related Student’s T likelihood (Lange, Little, & Taylor, 1989), and all priors
signals were marked and zeroed out from the raw data. Data was then were chosen to be weakly informative, with negligible impact on ob­
epoched around a stimulus-locked window of 1000 ms (− 200 to 800 ms tained estimates, but still providing moderate regularization (Gelman,
after stimulus presentation) as well as baseline corrected (− 200 to Simpson, & Betancourt, 2017). Model sampling using Markov chain
0 ms). Finally, automated artifact rejection using Autoreject version Monte Carlo (MCMC) was carried out using 12 chains with 4000 itera­
0.2.1 (Jas, Engemann, Bekhti, Raimondo, & Gramfort, 2017), with tions each, after 1000 warm-up iterations were discarded. All models
default values for peak-to-peak thresholding, was applied in order to converged well, with Gelman-Rubin diagnostics ( R)
̂ of 1.00 (Gelman &
interpolate artefactual channels and remove contaminated epochs. The Rubin, 1992) and number of effective samples well above 10,000 at the
preprocessed data was then imported into R for further statistical tails for each parameter.
analysis. Results from Bayesian analyses are presented as median posterior
Only non-subtracted, correct NoGo trials were used for further estimates along with 90% highest density intervals (HDIs) presented
analysis (Gajewski & Falkenstein, 2013). Since research suggests that within square brackets, which may be interpreted such that it has a 90%
ERP-based measures may be more reliable when averaged across mul­ probability of containing the actual estimate. Since there is no notion of
tiple electrode sites (e.g., Ribes-Guardiola, Poy, Patrick, & Moltó, 2020; “statistical significance” in Bayesian statistics, we adopted guidelines
Baldwin, Larson, & Clayson, 2015), we based our ERP measures on a recommending that a probability of 90% or higher can be considered
frontocentral region of interest by averaging data from nine electrodes “very likely” (Mastrandrea et al., 2011). Thus, findings were considered
(E20, E12, E5, E118, E13, E6, E112, E7, E106). We defined NoGo N2 as robust if the 90% HDI did not contain zero, and thus were very likely
amplitude as the mean amplitude in a 225–325 ms window post-stimuli different from zero. A Bayesian variant of the conventional R2 (Gelman,
(NoGo N2WIN) and NoGo P3 amplitude as the mean amplitude in a Goodrich, Gabry, & Vehtari, 2019) was used to estimate the amount of
325–625 ms window post-stimuli (NoGo P3WIN). These time windows explained variance of the multiple linear regression models, and we used
are similar to windows used in previous studies on aggressive and the leave-one-out cross-validated information criterion (LOOIC; Vehtari,
antisocial behaviors (e.g., Brennan & Baskin-Sommers, 2018; Guan Gelman, & Gabry, 2017) to assess whether adding group as an interac­
et al., 2015; Verona & Bresin, 2015), and were chosen to avoid overlap tion term increased model fit in post-hoc, exploratory analyses. The
between components. Latency was defined, using a fractional area LOOIC is a Bayesian analog of the Akaike Information Criterion often
approach, as the time point before which 50% of the negative (for N2) or used in frequentist statistics as a measure of model fit, with the added
positive (for P3) area of the waveform is observed (Luck, 2014). Finally, benefit that the LOOIC does not make distributional assumptions and
moving window amplitudes (labeled NoGo N2MOV and NoGo P3MOV), includes prior information. Lower LOOIC values indicate better model
defined as the mean amplitude 50 ms before and 50 ms after the 50% fit.
fractional area latency was observed, were calculated as complementary
measures in order to account for potential latency effects. 3. Results

2.4.3. Reliability analysis MDOs scored substantially higher than healthy controls on both LHA
The reliability of Go/NoGo task performance and all ERP measures subscales, as detailed in Table 1 and Fig. 1.
was assessed using split-half robust correlations between the averages of Using the whole sample, and after controlling for age, we found that
odd and even trials (Venables et al., 2018; Ribes-Guardiola et al., 2020), NoGo P3 latency was robustly and positively associated with both
corrected using the Spearman-Brown formula (de Vet, Mokkink, Mos­ LHAAGG and LHAANTI scores. These estimates were very likely different
muller, & Terwee, 2017). The Go/NoGo paradigm exhibited high reli­ from zero, with corresponding Bayesian R2 values of 0.15 [0.02, 0.28]
ability, with ρSB = 0.79 [0.66, 0.89] for median NoGo response time and and 0.16 [0.03, 0.29], respectively, indicating moderately strong linear
ρSB = 0.90 [0.84, 0.94] for NoGo accuracy. NoGo N2 latency exhibited relationships. Back-transforming the standardized estimates to their
moderate reliability, with ρSB = 0.59 [0.36, 0.77], and all remaining original scale, each point on the LHAAGG subscale was associated with a
ERP measurements exhibited high reliability, with ρSB = 0.76 [0.60, 1.21 millisecond increase in NoGo P3 latency, and each point on the
0.88] for NoGo N2WIN, ρSB = 0.72 [0.53, 0.85] for NoGo N2MOV, ρSB LHAANTI subscale was associated with a 1.33 millisecond increase in
= 0.82 [0.70, 0.91] for NoGo P3WIN, ρSB = 0.84 [0.73, 0.92] for NoGo NoGo P3 latency. We also saw a less robust, positive association between
P3MOV, and ρSB = 0.80 [0.67, 0.90] for NoGo P3 latency. NoGo N2 latency and LHAAGG, although the 90% HDI did contain zero.
Remaining associations were small and non-robust. Full details are
2.5. Statistical analysis presented in Table 2 and visualized in Fig. 2. The ERP waveform as well
as predicted associations between LHAAGG and LHAANTI scores and
Statistical analysis was carried out using the R statistical language, NoGo P3 latency, based on the posterior predictive distribution, are
version 4.1.1 (R Core Team, 2021). Due to the relatively small sample visualized in Fig. 3.
size, we opted for a fully Bayesian approach. All statistical models were
specified using the R package brms (Bürkner, 2017), bridging R with the
Stan probabilistic programming language (Carpenter et al., 2017).

4
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Table 1
Descriptive overview and estimated group differences in aggressive and antisocial behaviors.
Full sample (n = 46) HCs (n = 20) MDOs (n = 26)

Subscale Mean ± SD Range Mean ± SD Range Mean ± SD Range Est. diff. [90% HDI]

LHA Aggression 13.02 ± 6.97 1–25 8.4 ± 5.81 1–20 16.58 ± 5.63 4–25 8.49 [5.45, 11.42]
LHA Antisocial 8.57 ± 7.13 0–20 1.95 ± 2.72 0–8 13.65 ± 4.88 3–20 12.09 [10.06, 14.14]

Note. HCs, healthy controls; MDOs, mentally disordered offenders; LHA, Life History of Aggression; SD, standard deviation; HDI, highest density interval.

Fig. 1. Boxplots with individual data points showing group differences in (A) aggressive and (B) antisocial behaviors between healthy controls (n = 20) and mentally
disordered offenders (n = 26). LHA, Life History of Aggression; HCs, healthy controls; MDOs, mentally disordered offenders.

interaction term, respectively, and 135.14 (SE = 9.45) and 131.75


Table 2
(SE = 9.05) for LHAANTI with and without the interaction term,
Standardized estimates (β) from robust Bayesian regression models, using the
respectively. Thus, our findings suggest that both healthy controls and
whole sample (n = 46) and adjusted for age.
MDOs exhibit similar patterns of association between NoGo P3 latency
LHA Aggression LHA Antisocial and aggressive and antisocial behaviors.
β [90% HDI] β [90% HDI]

NoGo accuracy 0.01 [− 0.25, 0.27] 0.1 [− 0.16, 0.36] 4. Discussion


NoGo N2 latency 0.06 [− 0.19, 0.31] 0.19 [− 0.06, 0.45]
NoGo N2WIN 0.06 [− 0.17, 0.3] 0.07 [− 0.17, 0.31]
NoGo N2MOV 0.07 [− 0.17, 0.32] 0.09 [− 0.16, 0.34] The current study examined differences in aggressive and antisocial
NoGo P3 latency 0.28 [0.03, 0.52] 0.31 [0.07, 0.55] behaviors between healthy controls and MDOs, and then, using the
NoGo P3WIN -0.03 [− 0.21, 0.15] -0.05 [− 0.23, 0.12] whole sample and controlling for the effect of age, investigated whether
NoGo P3MOV 0.07 [− 0.19, 0.32] -0.08 [− 0.34, 0.17]
aggressive and antisocial behaviors were associated with behavioral
Note. LHA, Life History of Aggression; HDI, highest density interval. Estimates inhibitory control and associated neurophysiological activity. We found
where the 90% HDI does not contain zero are shown in bold. that MDOs, in line with our hypothesis, scored substantially higher than
healthy controls on both the LHAAGG and the LHAANTI subscales. Also in
3.1. Post-hoc examination of group interaction line with our hypothesis, we saw a robust, positive association between
LHAAGG and LHAANTI scores and NoGo P3 latency, and a positive yet
Following the above findings, and with the robust group difference in smaller and less robust association with NoGo N2 latency. Post-hoc
LHA scores in mind, we also wanted to explore whether there was a analyses revealed no effect of group, indicating that healthy controls
group effect on the association between NoGo P3 latency and aggressive and MDOs exhibit similar associations. Against our hypotheses, how­
and antisocial behaviors (i.e., whether healthy controls and MDOs ever, we saw no robust associations between LHAAGG or LHAANTI scores
demonstrated similar or different patterns of association). We re-ran the and NoGo N2/P3 amplitude or performance on the Go/NoGo task.
relevant regression models, but this time included a Group × LHAAGG/
LHAANTI interaction term. Adding the interaction did not result in
4.1. Group differences in aggressive and antisocial behaviors
improved model fit for either model, with LOOIC values of 136.54
(SE = 8.99) and 132.28 (SE = 8.32) for LHAAGG with and without the
That MDOs would score substantially higher than healthy controls on

Fig. 2. Standardized estimates (β) showing the


association between behavioral inhibitory con­
trol, event-related potentials, and (A) aggres­
sive and (B) antisocial behaviors in a combined
sample (n = 46) of healthy controls (n = 20)
and mentally disordered offenders (n = 26). All
estimates are adjusted for age. Dots represent
posterior medians and lines represent 90%
highest density intervals. Estimates where the
90% highest density interval does not contain
zero are highlighted in green. LHA, Life History
of Aggression.

5
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Fig. 3. (A) The ERP waveform, with the NoGo N2 and P3 components marked by arrows. (B–C) The predicted association between NoGo P3 latency and (B)
aggressive and (C) antisocial behaviors, with age held constant at 35 years (the sample mean), in a combined sample (n = 46) of healthy controls (n = 20) and
mentally disordered offenders (n = 26). Scattered dots show the actual data whereas the dark green middle line shows the predicted regression line and the lighter
shaded green region shows the 90% highest density interval. LHA, Life History of Aggression; HC, healthy controls; MDO, mentally disordered offenders.

both the LHAAGG and LHAANTI subscales was expected, and falls in line NoGo N2 latency. Mindful of the scarcity of research on P3 latency
with previous research on Swedish psychiatric patients (including overall and NoGo P3 latency in particular, this finding is difficult to
MDOs) demonstrating that scores on both these subscales exhibit robust, interpret considering that we, contrary to our expectations, saw no as­
medium to large correlations (ρs between ~ 0.30 and ~ 0.70) with sociations with NoGo N2 or P3 amplitude. Since each point on the
several of the disorders and behaviors often observed among MDOs, LHAANTI scale, for instance, predicted an increase in NoGo P3 latency of
including childhood-onset problems such as attention deficits, hyper­ 1.33 ms, and since MDOs scored an estimated 12.09 points higher on the
activity, and conduct problems, personality traits such as novelty LHAANTI scale than healthy controls, the predicted difference in NoGo P3
seeking, and externalizing behaviors such as alcohol and drug abuse latency between the two groups was 16.08 ms. The prediction is well in
(Hofvander et al., 2011). Notably, however, while MDOs scored line with the estimated 17.41 ms difference in NoGo P3 latency between
approximately twice as high on the LHAAGG subscale as healthy controls MDOs and healthy controls we have reported in previous work based on,
in the current study, their score on the LHAANTI scale was almost five with minor differences, the same sample (Delfin et al., 2020). In addi­
times higher than that of healthy controls. The LHAANTI scale taps be­ tion, in our previous work, we found that NoGo P3 latency was associ­
haviors that, to a certain extent, are more closely related to covertly ated with higher levels of self-reported disinhibition, whereas NoGo P3
aggressive or rule-breaking aspects of antisociality, such as problems at amplitude was not. The results of the current study, focused specifically
school and work and encounters with law enforcement. While the small on aggressive and antisocial behavior, thus seem to mirror our previous
sample size prohibits firm conclusions to be drawn from this, the results findings in this regard.
do seem logical given the criminological background of Swedish, The lack of a clear association between aggressive and antisocial
forensic psychiatric patients (Laporte et al., 2021; Swedish National behaviors and NoGo N2 and particularly NoGo P3 amplitude, as well as
Forensic Psychiatric Register, 2020). with behavioral inhibitory control, in the current study was unexpected
Since the more covertly aggressive, rule-breaking form of anti­ in light of the rather robust association between aggressive and antiso­
sociality seems primarily associated with impaired behavioral control cial behavior and reduced P3 (including NoGo P3) amplitude reported
and personality traits such as low constraint (Burt, 2012), a potential by Pasion et al. (2018). Differences in sample size and participant
clinical implication, following the high LHAANTI scores among MDOs in characteristics, such as the combined sample of healthy controls and
the current study, is that therapeutic interventions might benefit from MDOs, is a possible explanation. For instance, the pharmacological
increased focus on improving self-control. Indeed, such improvements treatment of MDOs in the current study often included multiple sub­
seem especially important when bearing in mind recent research stances, primarily antipsychotics (for details, see Delfin et al., 2020).
observing reductions in self-control among prisoners after just three Since NoGo P3 amplitude is modulated by dopamine D2 receptors, a
months of imprisonment, possibly linked to the impoverished environ­ primary target of antipsychotics, this could have affected our results
ment (i.e., a sedentary lifestyle, social isolation, lack of cognitive chal­ (Beste, Stock, Epplen, & Arning, 2016; Beste, Willemssen, Saft, & Fal­
lenges) that imprisonment often entails (Meijers, Harte, Meynen, kenstein, 2010).
Cuijpers, & Scherder, 2018). Although, as far as we can tell, relatively Another possible source of discrepancy is our choice of task. We used
few studies have been carried out, there is some evidence suggesting that a “cool” Go/NoGo task with neutral stimuli instead of a “hot” version,
cognitive and self-control training might be effective in reducing overt with emotionally salient stimuli (e.g., Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). Casey
aggression (Denson, Capper, Oaten, Friese, & Schofield, 2011; Papalia, et al. (2011) used both a “cool” and a “hot” version of the Go/NoGo to
Spivak, Daffern, & Ogloff, 2019; Wilkowski, Crowe, & Ferguson, 2015) differentiate between adult individuals that had completed a delay of
and other externalizing behaviors, such as problematic alcohol con­ gratification task in childhood. Measures from the “cool” version could
sumption (Walters, 2000). Still, the exact mechanisms underlying not reliably differentiate between low- and high-delay individuals,
improved self-control remain poorly understood (Friese, Frankenbach, whereas measures from the “hot” version could, suggesting that the
Job, & Loschelder, 2017), and further research aimed specifically at emotional valence of stimuli may be important. Recently, Sun et al.
MDOs is warranted. (2020) used an emotional Go/NoGo task and observed lower NoGo P3
amplitude in participants with high compared to low levels of trait
aggression, and Madole et al. (2020) used an emotional Go/NoGo task
4.2. The association between aggressive and antisocial behaviors, and found that behavioral inhibitory control was directly related to
inhibitory control, and ERP measures higher levels of self-reported aggression. Neither of these two studies
included a neutral comparison task, however, so the effect attributable
Using the whole sample, and after controlling for age, we found that specifically to the use of a “hot” task is difficult to evaluate. There is also
both aggressive and antisocial behaviors were robustly associated with evidence suggesting that “cool” and “hot”, or neutral and emotional,
increased NoGo P3 latency, and to a certain extent also with increased

6
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

versions of the Go/NoGo actually capture the same basic neuropsy­ single laboratory task does not fully capture the variance associated with
chological constructs (Littman & Takács, 2017; Schulz et al., 2007), and aggressive and antisocial behaviors (Young et al., 2009). Future studies
it thus remains unclear whether our choice of a neutral Go/NoGo task should further explore whether myelination, NoGo P3/N2 latency, and
could explain the lack of association between aggressive and antisocial aggressive and antisocial behaviors are indeed related, perhaps by
behaviors and NoGo N2/P3 amplitude. combining suitable techniques for probing the brain’s white-matter
In addition to the difference between “cool” and “hot” tasks, the structure with ERP measures.
meta-analysis by Pasion et al. (2019) suggests that reduced NoGo N2
amplitudes are consistently elicited when using the Stop Signal Task 4.2.2. Gray-matter regions involved in generating NoGo ERPs
(SST), but not when using the Go/NoGo. Although both tasks are Besides the putative role of white-matter alterations such as differ­
believed to tap inhibitory control, the SST reflects the ability to inhibit ences in myelination, several gray-matter regions have been implicated
an already ongoing response, while the Go/NoGo instead reflects the in the generation of the NoGo N2/P3 components. Although likely the
ability to withhold a response altogether (Littman & Takács, 2017). sum of multiple neural networks acting in concert (Huster, West­
Importantly, while they are often used seemingly interchangeably to erhausen, Pantev, & Konrad, 2010), the midcingulate cortex (MCC) has
capture individual differences in behavioral inhibitory control, they emerged as a particularly important region in the generation of the
nonetheless appear to operate through different neural mechanisms NoGo N2/P3 (Baumeister et al., 2014; Beste, Saft, Andrich, Gold, &
(Raud, Westerhausen, Dooley, & Huster, 2020), and it is possible, Falkenstein, 2008; Hong, Wang, Sun, Li, & Tong, 2017; Huster et al.,
therefore, that aggressive and antisocial behaviors may be more closely 2010). Research into its functions is still ongoing, but the MCC, covering
related to the stopping aspects of inhibitory control, rather than to what is sometimes also labeled the dorsal part of the anterior cingulate
withholding. Whether this also applies to the P3 component remains cortex (ACC), appears to be a key region involved in the monitoring of
unclear, however. conflicts and outcomes of actions (Botvinick, 2007; Jahn, Nee, Alex­
ander, & Brown, 2014), which corresponds to the processes believed to
4.2.1. A possible relationship between NoGo P3 latency and myelination be reflected in the NoGo N2/P3 components (Gajewski & Falkenstein,
Finally, one interpretation is that our findings instead highlight the 2013). It seems possible, therefore, that the positive association between
importance of distinguishing between amplitude and latency. The NoGo aggressive and antisocial behaviors and NoGo P3 latency (and, to some
P3 component is believed to reflect processes related to the monitoring degree, NoGo N2 latency) observed in the current study reflects
evaluation of conflict and the outcome of actions (Gajewski & Falken­ abnormal functioning of the MCC/ACC, which in turn would offer at
stein, 2013; Huster et al., 2013). If NoGo P3 amplitude reflects “neural least partial support for the “frontal dysfunction hypothesis” (Rodman
power”, or the amount of allocated cognitive resources, and if NoGo P3 et al., 2016). The lack of neuroimaging data prohibits us from drawing
latency reflects “neural efficiency” (van Dinteren et al., 2014), then firm conclusions about particular brain regions, however, and further
perhaps it is not the magnitude of allocated resources but rather the speed studies are required in order to elucidate the complex relationship be­
with which such allocation occurs that is relevant in aggressive and tween aggressive and antisocial behaviors, NoGo N2/P3 latency, and
antisocial behaviors. This speed of allocation has been linked to the brain structure and function.
so-called “myelin hypothesis”, which suggests that increased thickness
of the brain’s myelin sheath leads to increased nerve conduction ve­ 4.3. Relevance in clinical settings
locities, in turn leading to reduced response times on inhibition tasks
and a general improvement of cognitive and behavioral inhibitory From a clinical point of view, and if NoGo latency is indeed related to
control (Bartzokis, 2005; Tuch et al., 2005). the degree of myelination, then longitudinally investigating whether
There is a close relationship between the degree of myelination and early interventions targeting substance abuse have an effect on white-
P3 latency across the lifespan, indicating that the two are indeed closely matter structure and, subsequently, NoGo latency would shed further
intertwined (van Dinteren et al., 2014), further corroborated by research light on some of the questions arising from our findings. Another avenue
showing that individuals with multiple sclerosis, a disease characterized worth exploring more in-depth is whether it is possible to improve (i.e.,
by the destruction of the myelin sheath, exhibit delayed P3 latencies for shorten) NoGo latency using neuromodulation techniques. Some
certain types of stimuli (Ivica, Titlic, & Pavelin, 2013). Decreased rates promising results were recently presented by Sergiou et al. (2021), who
of myelination during adolescence have also been associated with an used high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) to
increased expression of psychopathology later in life (Vanes et al., modulate activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and found that
2020). Substance use and impulse control problems, both closely related tDCS could reduce aggression and alter electrophysiological responses in
to aggressive and antisocial behaviors (Alcorn et al., 2013; Krueger, a sample of male forensic patients. Studies targeting the MCC/ACC using
Markon, Patrick, Benning, & Kramer, 2007), may also play a role here, tDCS have observed improvements in cognitive function (Khan, Wang,
since the use of alcohol (Bjork, Grant, & Hommer, 2003; McQueeny Ti, Tse, & Tong, 2020), but to the best of our knowledge, no study has
et al., 2009), opioids (Velasco, Mohamed, & Sato-Bigbee, 2021), employed these techniques to target the ACC/MCC in a forensic psy­
cannabis (Bava et al., 2009), and cocaine (Bartzokis et al., 2002) is toxic chiatric sample.
to the vulnerable myelination process, and may in turn exacerbate
already existing impulse control problems and contribute to poor out­ 4.4. Strengths and limitations
comes in individuals prone to substance abuse (Bartzokis, 2005). While
speculative, it is possible that aggressive and antisocial individuals This study has some methodological strengths that should be noted.
exhibit aberrant myelination, which in turn could manifest as decreased The combination of dimensional assessments of aggressive and antiso­
NoGo P3 latency. Speaking against this interpretation is the fact that we cial behaviors, a behavioral response inhibition task, and neurophysio­
saw no association between aggressive and antisocial behaviors and logical measures is in line with research along multiple “levels of
performance on the response inhibition task. If the positive association analysis” encouraged by emerging frameworks such as the RDoC
between aggressive and antisocial behaviors and NoGo P3 latency (Cuthbert, 2014) and HiTOP (Perkins et al., 2020). The Bayesian sta­
observed in the current study is in fact due to individual differences in tistical approach has several advantages, such as allowing genuine
myelination, then a negative association between aggressive and anti­ probabilistic statements that remain valid regardless of sample size
social behaviors and performance on the Go/NoGo task would be ex­ (Wagenmakers et al., 2018) and representing a move away from strict
pected. That we did not observe such an association could be due to the reliance on p-values and associated interpretations of (statistical) sig­
task itself being too easy to detect meaningful variance in inhibitory nificance (Wasserstein, Schirm, & Lazar, 2019). Still, in order to facili­
control (see discussion in Delfin et al., 2020), or that performance on a tate interpretation for readers unaccustomed to the Bayesian approach,

7
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

we opted to use the 90% HDI as a heuristic for determining the observed a robust, positive association between NoGo P3 latency (and to
robustness of our findings; some readers may prefer different heuristics, a lesser extent NoGo N2 latency) and aggressive and antisocial behav­
and our results should be interpreted in light of this. We also used iors. We propose that this finding reflects decreased neural efficiency
non-Gaussian likelihoods, which are robust to outliers (Lange et al., primarily during the later stages of conflict monitoring and outcome
1989), and weakly informative priors centered around zero, which evaluation, and further speculate that this in turn could be related to
ameliorates concerns of multiple comparisons. individual differences in myelination. These findings add to a scarce
Despite these strengths, the small sample size remains a concern. literature on the role of the NoGo ERP overall and NoGo P3 latency in
From a statistical point of view, it makes it difficult to include additional particular, and may be used to guide further research aimed at early
covariates, such as data on pharmacological treatment, in our models. interventions and the evaluation of novel treatment approaches.
Thus, we opted to just include age, given its strong association with N2/
P3 amplitude and latency, but are cognizant of the fact that the inclusion Funding
of additional covariates could have affected our results. The small
sample size also leads to relatively wide HDIs, limiting the precision of This study was supported by grants from Region Kronoberg, Södra
our findings. Sjukvårdsregionen, the Swedish state under the agreement between the
Another limitation is that aggression was only measured using a Swedish Government and the county councils, the ALF-agreement
single instrument; using a different, or a combination of several in­ (432551 and 819681), and from the Swedish Research Council for
struments, may also have rendered different results. Furthermore, Health, Working Life and Welfare (2018-01409). The grant providers
although it would be preferable to use the same method of obtaining had no role in study design, recruitment and data collection, analysis, or
LHA scores for both MDOs and healthy controls, clinical ratings were in the interpretation of findings.
unfortunately not available for healthy controls. Previous research has
voiced some concern about socially desirable responding when obtain­ Software availability
ing self-reported LHA scores in a psychiatric population (Dellazizzo et al.
2017). In particular, a lack of self-awareness may cause participants to All code used is publicly available at the Open Science Framework.
deny the extent of their own aggressiveness (Ramírez & Andreu, 2006). All code relevant to the Go/NoGo task and for EEG data preprocessing is
Since very few – approximately 13% – of inpatient MDOs in Sweden are available at https://osf.io/yscdh/, and the code used for all statistical
assessed to have full insight into their own mental illness and the effect it analyses and the creation of figures and tables in the current study is
has on their behavior and experiences, with 52% being assessed to only available at https://osf.io/3wzrx/.
have partial insight (Swedish National Forensic Psychiatric Register,
2020), we chose to use clinically rated LHA scores for MDOs.1 Declaration of Competing Interest
One limitation is that the experimental paradigm used in the current
study did not counterbalance target (“K”) and non-target (“X”) letters None to disclose.
between participants, and we therefore cannot rule out that we did not
observe any letter-specific activity. Furthermore, behavioral inhibitory
Acknowledgments
control was also measured using a single task, which may not fully
capture the variance associated with aggressive and antisocial behav­
The authors are grateful to Natalie Laporte, Johan Berlin and Christel
iors. Therefore, the use of multiple tasks is recommended (Young et al.,
Karlsson for their work with data collection and administration, and to
2009), and a structural equation modeling approach using a combina­
the Regional Forensic Psychiatric Clinic in Växjö, Sweden, the Rågården
tion of multiple tasks and instruments, along the lines of Venables et al.
Forensic Psychiatric Hospital in Gothenburg, Sweden, and Marie Tor­
(2018), is likely a more optimal approach. It should be noted, however,
stensson Levander and her colleagues at the Department of Criminology,
that such an approach requires larger data set and more time spent on
Malmö University, Sweden, for help and support during data collection.
participation, which may not be feasible in forensic mental health set­
tings given the many difficulties encountered when recruiting MDOs (e.
Appendix A. Supporting information
g., Pedersen, Bergman, Berlin, & Hartvigsson, 2021; Bergman et al.,
2020). To get around this limitation, we encourage multisite collabo­
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the
rations. Finally, despite these limitations, it should be acknowledged
online version at doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108245.
that small sample studies employing a “bivariate mapping” approach (i.
e., investigating the association between just two measures), as in the
References
current study, remain important both for continued validation of
emerging research frameworks and for discovering potential neuro­ Alcorn, J. L., Gowin, J. L., Green, C. E., Swann, A. C., Moeller, F. G., & Lane, S. D. (2013).
behavioral correlates that may be further investigated (e.g., Perkins, Aggression, impulsivity, and psychopathic traits in combined antisocial personality
Latzman, & Patrick, 2020). disorder and substance use disorder. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
Neurosciences, 25(3), 229–232. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.12030060
Baldwin, S. A., Larson, M. J., & Clayson, P. E. (2015). The dependability of
5. Conclusions electrophysiological measurements of performance monitoring in a clinical sample:
A generalizability and decision analysis of the ERN and Pe. Psychophysiology, 52(6),
790–800. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12401
We found that MDOs presented with substantially higher levels of
Bartzokis, G. (2005). Brain myelination in prevalent neuropsychiatric developmental
aggressive and antisocial behaviors than healthy controls. In line with disorders: Primary and comorbid addiction. Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 55–96.
prior work, this finding further highlights a need for effective treatment Bartzokis, G., Beckson, M., Lu, P. H., Edwards, N., Bridge, P., & Mintz, J. (2002). Brain
maturation may be arrested in chronic cocaine addicts. Biological Psychiatry, 51(8),
of aggressive and antisocial behaviors in forensic mental health settings.
605–611. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01315-x
Using the entire sample, and controlling for the effect of age, we also Bateman, A. W., Gunderson, J., & Mulder, R. (2015). Treatment of personality disorder.
The Lancet, 385(9969), 735–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61394-5
Baumeister, S., Hohmann, S., Wolf, I., Plichta, M. M., Rechtsteiner, S., Zangl, M., …
Brandeis, D. (2014). Sequential inhibitory control processes assessed through
1
Following review, we re-analyzed group differences and associations using simultaneous EEG-fMRI. NeuroImage, 94, 349–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
self-reported LHA scores for MDOs. The LHA scores were lower, but overall, neuroimage.2014.01.023
Bava, S., Frank, L. R., McQueeny, T., Schweinsburg, B. C., Schweinsburg, A. D., &
results were very similar, and the association between LHAAGG and LHAANTI Tapert, S. F. (2009). Altered white matter microstructure in adolescent substance
scores and NoGo P3 latency remained virtually unchanged. Full details are users. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173(3), 228–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/
available in the Supplementary material. j.pscychresns.2009.04.005

8
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Bergman, H., Nilsson, T., Andiné, P., Degl’Innocenti, A., Thomeé, R., & Gutke, A. (2020). Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions:
Physical performance and physical activity of patients under compulsory forensic Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186–204.
psychiatric inpatient care. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice, 36(4), 507–515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
https://doi.org/10.1080/09593985.2018.1488320 Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., & Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training
Beste, C., Saft, C., Andrich, J., Gold, R., & Falkenstein, M. (2008). Response inhibition in improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6),
Huntington’s disease – A study using ERPs and sLORETA. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1077–1099. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617697076
1290–1297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.008 Gajewski, P. D., & Falkenstein, M. (2013). Effects of task complexity on ERP components
Beste, C., Stock, A.-K., Epplen, J. T., & Arning, L. (2016). Dissociable electrophysiological in Go/NoGo tasks. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(3), 273–278. https://
subprocesses during response inhibition are differentially modulated by dopamine doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.007
D1 and D2 receptors. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 26(6), 1029–1036. Gao, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). P3 event-related potential impairments in antisocial and
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.03.002 psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 82(3), 199–210.
Beste, C., Willemssen, R., Saft, C., & Falkenstein, M. (2010). Response inhibition https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.06.006
subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects. Gelman, A., Goodrich, B., Gabry, J., & Vehtari, A. (2019). R-squared for Bayesian
Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 366–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. regression models. The American Statistician, 73(3), 307–309. https://doi.org/
neuropsychologia.2009.09.023 10.1080/00031305.2018.1549100
Bjork, J. M., Grant, S. J., & Hommer, D. W. (2003). Cross-sectional volumetric analysis of Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple
brain atrophy in alcohol dependence: Effects of drinking history and comorbid sequences. Statistical Science, 7(4), 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1214/ss/
substance use disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 2038–2045. 1177011136
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2038 Gelman, A., Simpson, D., & Betancourt, M. (2017). The prior can often only be
Bohlin, G., Eninger, L., Brocki, K. C., & Thorell, L. B. (2012). Disorganized attachment understood in the context of the likelihood. Entropy, 19(10), 555. https://doi.org/
and inhibitory capacity: Predicting externalizing problem behaviors. Journal of 10.3390/e19100555
Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(3), 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011- Gramfort, A. (2013). MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Frontiers in
9574-7 Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00267
Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two Gramfort, A., Luessi, M., Larson, E., Engemann, D. A., Strohmeier, D., Brodbeck, C., …
perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Hämäläinen, M. S. (2014). MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data.
Neuroscience, 7(4), 356–366. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.4.356 NeuroImage, 86, 446–460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.027
Brennan, G. M., & Baskin-Sommers, A. R. (2018). Brain-behavior relationships in Guan, M., Liao, Y., Ren, H., Wang, X., Yang, Q., Liu, X., & Wang, W. (2015). Impaired
externalizing: P3 amplitude reduction reflects deficient inhibitory control. response inhibition in juvenile delinquents with antisocial personality
Behavioural Brain Research, 337, 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.045 characteristics: A preliminary ERP study in a Go/Nogo task. Neuroscience Letters, 603,
Broderick, C., Azizian, A., Kornbluh, R., & Warburton, K. (2015). Prevalence of physical 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2015.06.062
violence in a forensic psychiatric hospital system during 2011–2013: Patient Hanson, R. F., Sawyer, G. K., Begle, A. M., & Hubel, G. S. (2010). The impact of crime
assaults, staff assaults, and repeatedly violent patients. CNS Spectrums, 20(3), victimization on quality of life. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23(2), 189–197. https://
319–330. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852915000188 doi.org/10.1002/jts.20508
Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). Brms: An R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. Hayes, A. F., & Coutts, J. J. (2020). Use Omega rather than Cronbach’s Alpha for
Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01 estimating reliability. But…. Communication Methods and Measures, 14(1), 1–24.
Burt, S. A. (2012). How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629
antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral Hofvander, B., Ståhlberg, O., Nydén, A., Wentz, E., degl’Innocenti, A., Billstedt, E., …
dimensions. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(4), 263–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Anckarsäter, H. (2011). Life history of aggression scores are predicted by childhood
cpr.2012.02.006 hyperactivity, conduct disorder, adult substance abuse, and low cooperativeness in
Burt, S. A., & Donnellan, M. B. (2008). Personality correlates of aggressive and non- adult psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Research, 185(1–2), 280–285. https://doi.org/
aggressive antisocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(1), 53–63. 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.05.008
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.07.022 Hong, X., Wang, Y., Sun, J., Li, C., & Tong, S. (2017). Segregating top-down selective
Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M. D., Lee, D., Goodrich, B., Betancourt, M., … attention from response inhibition in a spatial cueing Go/NoGo task: An ERP and
Riddell, A. (2017). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of Statistical source localization study. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/
Software, 76(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v076.i01 s41598-017-08807-z
Casey, B. J., Somerville, L. H., Gotlib, I. H., Ayduk, O., Franklin, N. T., Askren, M. K., … Hopwood, C. J., Burt, S. A., Markowitz, J. C., Yen, S., Shea, M. T., Sanislow, C. A., …
Shoda, Y. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years Morey, L. C. (2009). The construct validity of rule-breaking and aggression in an
later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(36), 14998–15003. adult clinical sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(8), 803–808. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1108561108 org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.07.008
Coccaro, E. F., Berman, M. E., & Kavoussi, R. J. (1997). Assessment of life-history of Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., & Herrmann, C. S.
aggression: Development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Research, 73 (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: Functional networks
(3), 147–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(97)00119-4 and cognitive contributions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87(3),
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to 217–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001
dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Huster, R. J., Westerhausen, R., Pantev, C., & Konrad, C. (2010). The role of the cingulate
Psychiatry, 13(1), 28–35. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20087 cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task.
Dambacher, F., Schuhmann, T., Lobbestael, J., Arntz, A., Brugman, S., & Sack, A. T. Human Brain Mapping, 31(8), 1260–1271. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20933
(2015). No effects of bilateral tDCS over inferior frontal gyrus on response inhibition Ivica, N., Titlic, M., & Pavelin, S. (2013). P300 wave changes in patients with multiple
and aggression. PLoS One, 10(7), Article e0132170. https://doi.org/10.1371/ sclerosis. Acta Informatica Medica, 21(3), 205–207. https://doi.org/10.5455/
journal.pone.0132170 aim.2013.21.205-207
de Vet, H. C. W., Mokkink, L. B., Mosmuller, D. G., & Terwee, C. B. (2017). Spearman- Jahn, A., Nee, D. E., Alexander, W. H., & Brown, J. W. (2014). Distinct regions of anterior
Brown prophecy formula and Cronbach’s alpha: Different faces of reliability and cingulate cortex signal prediction and outcome evaluation. NeuroImage, 95, 80–89.
opportunities for new applications. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 85, 45–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.050
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2017.01.013 Jas, M., Engemann, D. A., Bekhti, Y., Raimondo, F., & Gramfort, A. (2017). Autoreject:
Dellazizzo, L., Potvin, S., Giguère, C.-É., Berwald, M., Dugré, J. R., Dumais, A., & Automated artifact rejection for MEG and EEG data. NeuroImage, 159, 417–429.
Signature Consortium. (2017). The psychometric properties of the Life History of https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.030
Aggression evaluated in patients from a psychiatric emergency setting. Psychiatry Jas, M., Larson, E., Engemann, D. A., Leppäkangas, J., Taulu, S., Hämäläinen, M., &
Research, 257, 485–489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.031 Gramfort, A. (2018). A reproducible MEG/EEG group study with the MNE doftware:
Delfin, C., Ruzich, E., Wallinius, M., Björnsdotter, M., & Andiné, P. (2020). Trait Recommendations, quality assessments, and good practices. Frontiers in Neuroscience,
disinhibition and NoGo event-related potentials in violent mentally disordered 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00530
offenders and healthy controls. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/ Johnstone, S. J., Pleffer, C. B., Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., & Smith, J. L. (2005).
fpsyt.2020.577491 Development of inhibitory processing during the Go/NoGo task. Journal of
Denson, T. F., Capper, M. M., Oaten, M., Friese, M., & Schofield, T. P. (2011). Self-control Psychophysiology, 19(1), 11–23. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.1.11
training decreases aggression in response to provocation in aggressive individuals. Kappenman, E. S., & Luck, S. J. (2010). The effects of electrode impedance on data
Journal of Research in Personality, 45(2), 252–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. quality and statistical significance in ERP recordings. Psychophysiology, 47(5),
jrp.2011.02.001 888–904. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01009.x
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (1999). ERP components in Go/Nogo Khan, A., Wang, X., Ti, C. H. E., Tse, C.-Y., & Tong, K.-Y. (2020). Anodal transcranial
tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychologica, 101(2–3), 267–291. https:// direct current stimulation of anterior cingulate cortex modulates subcortical brain
doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6 regions resulting in cognitive enhancement. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14,
Ferree, T. C., Luu, P., Russell, G. S., & Tucker, D. M. (2001). Scalp electrode impedance, 563. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.584136
infection risk, and EEG data quality. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(3), 536–544. Kiehl, K. A., Liddle, P. F., & Hopfinger, J. B. (2000). Error processing and the rostral
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00533-2 anterior cingulate: An event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 216–223.
Flanigan, M. E., & Russo, S. J. (2019). Recent advances in the study of aggression. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720216
Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Klein Tuente, S., Bogaerts, S., & Veling, W. (2021). Mapping aggressive behavior of
Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(2), 241–244. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018- forensic psychiatric inpatients with self-report and structured staff-monitoring.
0226-2 Psychiatry Research, 301, Article 113983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
psychres.2021.113983

9
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. D. (2007). Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical
Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative Neurophysiology, 118(10), 2128–2148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal clinph.2007.04.019
Psychology, 116(4), 645–666. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645 Porjesz, B., Begleiter, H., Bihari, B., & Kissin, B. (1987). The N2 component of the event-
Lane, S. D., Kjome, K. L., & Moeller, F. G. (2011). Neuropsychiatry of aggression. related brain potential in abstinent alcoholics. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurologic Clinics, 29(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ncl.2010.10.006 (49–vii). Neurophysiology, 66(2), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(87)90181-7
Lange, K. L., Little, R. J. A., & Taylor, J. M. G. (1989). Robust statistical modeling using Ramírez, J. M., & Andreu, J. M. (2006). Aggression, and some related psychological
the t distribution. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 84(408), 881–896. constructs (anger, hostility, and impulsivity); Some comments from a research
Laporte, N., Ozolins, A., Westling, S., Westrin, Å., & Wallinius, M. (2021). Clinical project. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 276–291. https://doi.org/
characteristics and self-harm in forensic psychiatric patients. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.015
12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.698372 Raud, L., Westerhausen, R., Dooley, N., & Huster, R. J. (2020). Differences in unity: The
Littman, R., & Takács, Á. (2017). Do all inhibitions act alike? A study of Go/No-Go and go/no-go and stop signal tasks rely on different mechanisms. NeuroImage, 210,
Stop-Signal paradigms. PLoS One, 12(10), Article e0186774. https://doi.org/ Article 116582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116582
10.1371/journal.pone.0186774 Rawls, E., Jabr, M. M., Moody, S. N., & Lamm, C. (2018). Neural mechanisms underlying
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. MIT press. the link between effortful control and aggression: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia,
Luijten, M., Machielsen, M., Veltman, D., Hester, R., de Haan, L., & Franken, I. (2014). 117, 302–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.015
Systematic review of ERP and fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control and error Core Team, R. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna,
processing in people. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 39(3), 149–169. https:// Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
doi.org/10.1503/jpn.130052 Ribes-Guardiola, P., Poy, R., Patrick, C. J., & Moltó, J. (2020). Electrocortical measures of
Madole, J. W., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (2020). A model of aggressive behavior: performance monitoring from Go/No-Go and flanker tasks: Differential relations
Early adversity, impulsivity, and response inhibition. Journal of Aggression, with trait dimensions of the triarchic model of psychopathy. Psychophysiology, 57(6).
Maltreatment & Trauma, 29(5), 594–610. https://doi.org/10.1080/ https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13573
10926771.2019.1591561 Rietdijk, W. J. R., Franken, I. H. A., & Thurik, A. R. (2014). Internal consistency of event-
Mastrandrea, M. D., Mach, K. J., Plattner, G.-K., Edenhofer, O., Stocker, T. F., Field, C. B., related potentials associated with cognitive control: N2/P3 and ERN/Pe. PLoS One, 9
… Matschoss, P. R. (2011). The IPCC AR5 guidance note on consistent treatment of (7), Article e102672. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102672
uncertainties: A common approach across the working groups. Climatic Change, 108 Rodman, A. M., Kastman, E. K., Dorfman, H. M., Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Kiehl, K. A.,
(4), 675–691. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-011-0178-6 Newman, J. P., & Buckholtz, J. W. (2016). Selective mapping of psychopathy and
McNeish, D. (2018). Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychological externalizing to dissociable circuits for inhibitory self-control. Clinical Psychological
Methods, 23(3), 412–433. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000144 Science, 4(3), 559–571. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702616631495
McQueeny, T., Schweinsburg, B. C., Schweinsburg, A. D., Jacobus, J., Bava, S., Schulz, K. P., Fan, J., Magidina, O., Marks, D. J., Hahn, B., & Halperin, J. M. (2007). Does
Frank, L. R., & Tapert, S. F. (2009). Altered white matter integrity in adolescent the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence
binge drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(7), 1278–1285. with measures on a non-emotional analog. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00953.x (2), 151–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001
Meijers, J., Harte, J. M., Meynen, G., Cuijpers, P., & Scherder, E. J. A. (2018). Reduced Scott, S., Knapp, M., Henderson, J., & Maughan, B. (2001). Financial cost of social
self-control after 3 months of imprisonment; A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, exclusion: Follow up study of antisocial children into adulthood. BMJ, 323(7306),
69. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00069 191. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.323.7306.191
Meyer, J. M., Cummings, M. A., Proctor, G., & Stahl, S. M. (2016). Psychopharmacology Sergiou, C. S., Santarnecchi, E., Romanella, S. M., Wieser, M. J., Franken, I. H. A.,
of persistent violence and aggression. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 39(4), Rassin, E. G. C., & van Dongen, J. D. M. (2021). Transcranial direct current
541–556. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2016.07.012 stimulation targeting the ventromedial prefrontal cortex reduces reactive aggression
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. and modulates electrophysiological responses in a forensic population. Biological
(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), bpsc.2021.05.007
49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 Steele, V. R., Maurer, J. M., Bernat, E. M., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2016). Error-
Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews related processing in adult males with elevated psychopathic traits. Personality
Neuroscience, 8(7), 536–546. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2174 Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7(1), 80–90. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Martel, M. M., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., Glass, J. M., … per0000143
Zucker, R. A. (2006). Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and Sun, L., Niu, G., Li, J., Du, H., Hu, X., Yang, S., & Luo, Y. (2020). Trait aggression affects
illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use the response inhibition to angry expressions: An event-related brain potential study.
disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(4), Personality and Individual Differences, 152, Article 109553. https://doi.org/10.1016/
468–475. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9 j.paid.2019.109553
Niv, S., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2013). Aggression and rule-breaking: Swedish National Forensic Psychiatric Register, RättspsyK (2020). Annual report 2020.
Heritability and stability of antisocial behavior problems in childhood and Gothenburg: Swedish National Forensic Psychiatric Register.
adolescence. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(5). https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2005). Symptom-based
jcrimjus.2013.06.014 subfactors of DSM-defined conduct disorder: Evidence for etiologic distinctions.
Pandey, A. K., Kamarajan, C., Tang, Y., Chorlian, D. B., Roopesh, B. N., Manz, N., … Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(3), 483–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-
Porjesz, B. (2012). Neurocognitive deficits in male alcoholics: An ERP/sLORETA 843X.114.3.483
analysis of the N2 component in an equal probability Go/NoGo task. Biological Tan, S.-Y., & Haining, R. (2016). Crime victimization and the implications for individual
Psychology, 89(1), 170–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.009 health and wellbeing: A Sheffield case study. Social Science & Medicine, 167,
Papalia, N., Spivak, B., Daffern, M., & Ogloff, J. R. (2019). A meta-analytic review of the 128–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.018
efficacy of psychological treatments for violent offenders in correctional and forensic Thigpen, N. N., Kappenman, E. S., & Keil, A. (2017). Assessing the internal consistency of
mental health settings. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 26(2), Article the event-related potential: An example analysis. Psychophysiology, 54, 123–138.
e12282. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12282 https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12629
Pasion, R., Fernandes, C., Pereira, M. R., & Barbosa, F. (2018). Antisocial behaviour and Tonnaer, F., Cima, M., & Arntz, A. (2016a). Executive (dys)Functioning and impulsivity
psychopathy: Uncovering the externalizing link in the P3 modulation. Neuroscience & as possible vulnerability factors for aggression in forensic patients. The Journal of
Biobehavioral Reviews, 91, 170–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Nervous and Mental Disease, 204(4), 280–286. https://doi.org/10.1097/
neubiorev.2017.03.012 NMD.0000000000000485
Pasion, R., Prata, C., Fernandes, M., Almeida, R., Garcez, H., Araújo, C., & Barbosa, F. Tonnaer, F., Cima, M., & Arntz, A. (2016b). Modeling impulsivity in forensic patients: A
(2019). N2 amplitude modulation across the antisocial spectrum: A meta-analysis. three-dimensional model of impulsivity. The American Journal of Psychology, 129(4),
Reviews in the Neurosciences, 30(7), 781–794. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro- 429–441. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.129.4.0429
2018-0116 Trestman, R. L. (2017). Treating aggression in forensic psychiatric settings. The Journal of
Patrick, C. J., Bernat, E. M., Malone, S. M., Iacono, W. G., Krueger, R. F., & McGue, M. the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 45(1), 40–43.
(2006). P300 amplitude as an indicator of externalizing in adolescent males. Tuch, D. S., Salat, D. H., Wisco, J. J., Zaleta, A. K., Hevelone, N. D., & Rosas, H. D. (2005).
Psychophysiology, 43(1), 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00376.x Choice reaction time performance correlates with diffusion anisotropy in white
Pedersen, S. H., Bergman, H., Berlin, J., & Hartvigsson, T. (2021). Perspectives on matter pathways supporting visuospatial attention. Proceedings of the National
recruitment and representativeness in forensic psychiatric research. Frontiers in Academy of Sciences, 102(34), 12212–12217. https://doi.org/10.1073/
Psychiatry, 12, 937. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.647450 pnas.0407259102
Perkins, E. R., Joyner, K. J., Patrick, C. J., Bartholow, B. D., Latzman, R. D., van den Bosch, L. M. C., Rijckmans, M. J. N., Decoene, S., & Chapman, A. L. (2018).
DeYoung, C. G., … Zald, D. H. (2020). Neurobiology and the hierarchical taxonomy Treatment of antisocial personality disorder: Development of a practice focused
of psychopathology: Progress toward ontogenetically informed and clinically useful framework. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 58, 72–78. https://doi.org/
nosology. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 22(1), 51–63. https://doi.org/10.31887/ 10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.03.002
DCNS.2020.22.1/eperkins van Dinteren, R., Arns, M., Jongsma, M. L. A., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2014). P300
Perkins, E. R., Latzman, R. D., & Patrick, C. J. (2020). Interfacing neural constructs with development across the lifespan: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One, 9
the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology: “Why” and “how”. Personality and (2), Article e87347. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087347
Mental Health, 14(1), 106–122. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1460 Vanes, L. D., Moutoussis, M., Ziegler, G., Goodyer, I. M., Fonagy, P., Jones, P. B., …
Dolan, R. J. (2020). White matter tract myelin maturation and its association with

10
C. Delfin et al. Biological Psychology 168 (2022) 108245

general psychopathology in adolescence and early adulthood. Human Brain Mapping, Walters, G. D. (2000). Behavioral self-control training for problem drinkers: A meta-
41(3), 827–839. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24842 analysis of randomized control studies. Behavior Therapy, 31(1), 135–149. https://
Vehtari, A., Gelman, A., & Gabry, J. (2017). Practical Bayesian model evaluation using doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(00)80008-8
leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC. Statistics and Computing, 27(5), Wasserstein, R. L., Schirm, A. L., & Lazar, N. A. (2019). Moving to a world beyond
1413–1432. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11222-016-9696-4 “p < 0.05”. The American Statistician, 73(sup1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Velasco, B., Mohamed, E., & Sato-Bigbee, C. (2021). Endogenous and exogenous opioid 00031305.2019.1583913
effects on oligodendrocyte biology and developmental brain myelination. Wilkowski, B. M., Crowe, S. E., & Ferguson, E. L. (2015). Learning to keep your cool:
Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 86, Article 107002. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Reducing aggression through the experimental modification of cognitive control.
ntt.2021.107002 Cognition and Emotion, 29(2), 251–265. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Venables, N. C., Foell, J., Yancey, J. R., Kane, M. J., Engle, R. W., & Patrick, C. J. (2018). 02699931.2014.911146
Quantifying inhibitory control as externalizing proneness: A cross-domain model. Woltering, S., Liu, Z., Rokeach, A., & Tannock, R. (2013). Neurophysiological differences
Clinical Psychological Science, 6(4), 561–580. https://doi.org/10.1177/ in inhibitory control between adults with ADHD and their peers. Neuropsychologia,
2167702618757690 51(10), 1888–1895. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.023
Verona, E., & Bresin, K. (2015). Aggression proneness: Transdiagnostic processes Young, S. E., Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Willcutt, E. G., Corley, R. P., Haberstick, B. C.,
involving negative valence and cognitive systems. International Journal of & Hewitt, J. K. (2009). Behavioral disinhibition: Liability for externalizing spectrum
Psychophysiology, 98(2), 321–329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.03.008 disorders and its genetic and environmental relation to response inhibition across
Verstegen, N., de Vogel, V., de Vries Robbé, M., & Helmerhorst, M. (2017). Inpatient adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 117–130. https://doi.org/
violence in a Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Forensic Practice, 19(2), 10.1037/a0014657
102–114. https://doi.org/10.1108/JFP-04-2016-0020 Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J., … adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4),
Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical 354–360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x
advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 35–57.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3

11

You might also like