You are on page 1of 8

Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cresp

Agentic meaning-making: Free will beliefs, sense-making, and


psychological distress following collective traumas
Joseph Maffly-Kipp a,1,∗, Chase Gause a,1, Jinhyung Kim b, Matthew Vess a, Joshua A. Hicks a
a
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Texas A&M University, 4235 TAMU, College Station, TX 77843, United States
b
Department of Psychology, Sogang University, Seoul, South Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Keywords: Meaning-making is an important component in the psychological recovery from collective traumas. However,
Collective trauma not enough is known about what individual differences might facilitate meaning-oriented approaches to coping
Meaning-making and recovering from traumas. We predicted that free will beliefs, which afford people the perception of volitional
Free will beliefs
autonomy over their actions, would be an important antecedent to sense-making and meaning-focused coping.
Posttraumatic stress, coping
We tested this conceptual hypothesis in the context of two distinct collective traumas. In Study 1 (n = 342), the
comprehension facet of meaning in life (which is the facet most related to sense-making) mediated the relationship
between free will beliefs and lower psychological distress related to the COVID-19 global pandemic. In Study 2
(n = 571), meaning-focused coping, but not other coping styles, mediated the relationship between free will beliefs
and psychological recovery (i.e., lower distress over time) in a longitudinal sample collected in the aftermath
of Hurricane Harvey. We interpreted our findings as robust support for our hypothesis, the strength of which
is bolstered by the distinct settings and methodologies with which the data were collected. We conclude by
discussing our results in relation to the view that free will beliefs afford a more agentic approach to meaning-
making, which ultimately facilities more effective psychological recovery from collective trauma.

Introduction might be an important antecedent in this process. Free will beliefs are
associated with greater meaning in life (Crescioni et al., 2016) and self-
Traumatic events that impact entire communities—such as natural efficacy (Charzyńska and Wysocka, 2014), which are both resilience
disasters, terrorist attacks, or pandemics—have the potential to under- factors in the context of collective trauma (Luszczynska et al., 2009;
mine mental health on a large scale. These events, known as collec- Updegraff et al., 2008). Further, there is reason to believe that people
tive traumas, may have unique psychological and existential implica- rely on global existential beliefs (e.g., belief in free will) in order to
tions due to their impact on collective identity and symbolic sources of make-meaning out of stressful life events (Park, 2010), ultimately aid-
meaning (Hirschberger, 2018). Indeed, previous research has demon- ing recovery (Park et al., 2021). Thus, we predicted that general beliefs
strated that, while finding meaning in these events bodes well for psy- in free will would facilitate a more agentic orientation towards meaning-
chological recovery, continuing to search for meaning predicts worse making, which would in turn relate to more effective psychological re-
outcomes (Updegraff et al., 2008). Following from this, it is perhaps covery from a collective trauma. The purpose of this paper is to explore
unsurprising that existential factors like meaning (Maffly-Kipp et al., this prediction in the context of two separate collective traumas: the
2021; Park et al., 2021) and authenticity (Maffly-Kipp et al., 2020) COVID-19 global pandemic and exposure to a Category 4 hurricane.
are associated with better psychological recovery. However, there is a
shortage of research examining what factors might better equip peo- Collective trauma and meaning
ple to use coping strategies related to meaning-making (e.g., positive
reframing) to recover from a collective trauma. Based on the existen- Trauma is defined as the exposure to “actual or threatened death,
tial notion that people must actualize their freedom before construct- serious injury, or sexual violence” (American Psychiatric Associa-
ing meaning (Sartre, 1946; Smith and Lapansky, 2021; Yalom, 1980; tion, 2013, p. 271), and exposure to traumatic stimuli can cause intense
see also, Ryan and Deci, 2001), we expected that a belief in free will feelings of fear and helplessness in victims, as well as various long-term


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: jmaffkipp@tamu.edu (J. Maffly-Kipp), gause@tamu.edu (C. Gause), jinhyung@sogang.ac.kr (J. Kim), vess@tamu.edu (M. Vess),
joshua.hicks@tamu.edu (J.A. Hicks).
1
These authors contributed equally to this work.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100074
Received 29 June 2022; Received in revised form 26 October 2022; Accepted 16 November 2022
2666-6227/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

emotional disturbances (Jones and Cureton, 2014). Traumatic experi- like general beliefs in the existence of free will (see Baumeister and
ences often directly lead to post-traumatic stress disorder, and are asso- Monroe, 2014), might foster a greater tendency toward positive coping
ciated with a host of other negative physical and mental health outcomes strategies. Below we discuss how free will beliefs may be linked to less
(Dixon et al., 2005; KauerSant’Anna et al., 2007). Given its prevalence stress during collective traumas, and the role of meaning-making in this
(see Benjet et al., 2016), trauma is generally considered a serious public relationship.
health issue (Magruder et al., 2017). As such, identifying factors that
aid recovery from trauma is vitally important. Free will beliefs
A significant body of work has examined the existential implications
of trauma, due to its potential impact on basic beliefs and orientations A belief in free will involves the perspective that people have the
towards the self and world. For example, Janoff-Bulman (1985) sug- autonomous ability to choose amongst various options in life, de-
gests that the experience of trauma erodes basic meaning-related as- spite external social and environmental factors, internal personality,
sumptions, namely that the world is a good/safe place and that the self or biological factors (Kane, 2011). Those who believe in free will de-
has worth within it (see also, Tolstikova et al., 2005). This may trigger rive many intrapersonal benefits in this outlook of the world and, as
sense-making efforts and potentially a search for meaning in the event such, trust their personal capabilities. Such individuals are more grate-
itself, the success of which has important implications for psychological ful (Crescioni et al., 2016), more forward-looking (Seligman et al.,
health (Janoff-Bulman, 1992) and more global judgments of meaning 2013), are more satisfied with life, experience more positive affect
(Park, 2010). The underlying assumption of these views is that those (Li et al., 2016), and derive more meaning from their place in the world
who search but struggle to find meaning in a traumatic event are not able (Moynihan et al., 2017). Additionally, they perceive themselves as more
to resolve the dissonance between the trauma and their basic assump- efficacious (Baumeister and Brewer, 2012), autonomous (Alquist et al.,
tions/worldviews, leading to continued psychological distress. 2013), capable of growth (Feldman et al., 2016), better able to di-
Indeed, people who report searching for meaning in the wake rect their own decisions, and reflect more fondly on choices made
of a trauma report worse psychological adjustment (Davis et al., (Feldman et al., 2014). Perhaps unsurprisingly, undermining beliefs in
1998), a higher likelihood of developing depression (Coleman and free will leads to greater feelings of alienation from one’s true self (Seto
Neimeyer, 2010), and greater general distress (Silver et al., 1983). Con- and Hicks, 2016). Clearly, these beliefs are intimately connected to a
versely, finding meaning in a traumatic event requires some sort of healthy and productive orientation towards both the self and the world.
reconciliation of previous meaning-frameworks (Park, 2010), and al- How, then, might free will beliefs be specifically implicated in
lows people to achieve consonance in their worldviews and regain psy- meaning-making following trauma? Some theoretical work might pro-
chological equanimity (Smith and Lapansky, 2021). Perhaps unsurpris- vide clues. Existential and humanistic psychologists/philosophers, no-
ingly, then, finding meaning in trauma tends to be associated with better tably Sartre (1946), Frankl (1959), and May (1975), all articulated the
psychological recovery (Park and Folkman, 1997; Triplett et al., 2012; broad view that humans must recognize themselves as agents who pos-
Updegraff et al., 2008). In essence, the ability to make sense of and ac- sess the capacity for free deliberation and choice in order to actively
cept a traumatic event within one’s personal framework of meaning is create a subjective sense of meaning out of their experiences and ex-
a crucial step in the trauma recovery process. istence. Perhaps more concretely, Yalom (1980), and more recently
Recent work has suggested that meaning-related processes might Smith and Lapansky (2021), have conceptualized psychotherapy as (in
be particularly relevant in the context of collective traumas. part) an effort to help individuals actualize their free will in order to
These community-wide events are unique not only in that they make meaning out of their suffering and trauma. In other words, an
threaten/impact symbolic meaning on a larger scale, but also that peo- individual who does not believe that they have conscious control over
ple are more able to rely on community and communal resources for re- their thoughts and actions will be less optimally positioned to manipu-
covery due to the collective nature of the crisis (Luszczynska et al., 2009; late their interpretations and beliefs surrounding a traumatic event (i.e.,
Saul, 2014). Therefore, the utilization of collective perspectives like cul- meaning-making), leading to poorer adjustment. This hypothetical indi-
tural worldviews should be particularly important. Indeed, Park and col- vidual might be inclined to simply accept their default interpretation of
leagues (2021) found that coping strategies centered around meaning- that event. A person high in free will beliefs, however, should be better
making (i.e., positive reappraisal, positive refocusing, acceptance, and able to decide how to navigate the accommodation and adjustment of
perspective-taking), in addition to social support-seeking, predicted bet- meaning-structures that appears critical to the trauma recovery process
ter psychological adjustment to the COVID-19 global pandemic. This (Park and Folkman, 1997; Triplett et al., 2012; Updegraff et al., 2008).
complements previous research suggesting that the inability to make Though scant, there is some empirical work that might bolster this
sense of collective traumas is associated with increased stress (Maffly- theoretical account. For example, Bergner and Ramon (2013) found that
Kipp et al., 2021; Updegraff et al., 2008). Given the importance of mean- free will beliefs predicted greater meaning in life. Separately, coun-
ing in these contexts, identifying factors and antecedents that promote terfactual reflection (i.e., mentally redoing past events) aids meaning-
meaning-making and meaning-focused coping in the face of community- making only for people with high free will beliefs (Seto et al., 2015). This
wide traumas is important. suggests that free will beliefs are an important requisite for the types of
Basic beliefs about the world and oneself are essential for meaning- cognitive processing involved in meaning-making, which is consistent
making (Park, 2010; Schlegel et al., 2009), and thus may be important with the role of global world beliefs in Park (2010) model of meaning-
to the role of meaning in trauma recovery. Indeed, variables reflecting making following stressful events. So, there is strong theoretical, and
one’s orientation towards themself are important predictors of trauma some empirical, reason to suggest that beliefs about one’s own voli-
recovery. For example, perceived authenticity predicts greater recovery tional autonomy (i.e., free will beliefs) are an important antecedent to
from collective trauma (Maffly-Kipp et al., 2020). Further, in a review the active meaning-making that allows one to effectively recover from
and meta-analysis, Luszczynska and colleagues (2009) found that self- collective trauma. Critically, of all the coping strategies that Park and
efficacy beliefs were predictive of better health-related outcomes in the colleagues (2021) focused on as predictors of recovery from collective
context of collective traumas. Similarly, Benight and Bandura (2004) re- trauma, meaning-making is the only one that has clear and strong theo-
viewed a diverse series of studies on collective traumas and concluded retical ties to existential world beliefs like free will. An individual with
that self-efficacy beliefs aid psychological recovery in part through their low belief in free will might be more likely to accept their default inter-
ability to afford the sufferer some sense of control over the adversity. pretation of a trauma, but there is no reason to expect that they would
However, considerably less research has focused on the role of beliefs rely any less on something like social support, for example, which is
about the world (as opposed to the self) in trauma recovery. It stands to thought to be instinctual (Cacioppo and Patrick, 2008). Thus, the agency
reason that world beliefs which afford people a sense of volition/agency, afforded by free will beliefs (Baumeister and Brewer, 2012) may posi-

2
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

tion them as a unique pre-requisite for the meaning-oriented recovery Meaning in life. To evaluate meaning in life, participants completed the
strategies that aid recovery from collective trauma (Park, 2021). Multidimensional Meaning in Life Scale (Costin & Vignoles, 2020). The
scale is composed of separate subscales for comprehension, purpose,
The current research and existential mattering. Although we were primarily interested in as-
sociations between free will beliefs, comprehension, and psychological
Drawing on the above reasoning, we examined the role of free will distress, we included the purpose and mattering subscales for both ex-
beliefs in meaning-oriented coping and recovery from collective trauma. ploratory purposes, as well as to demonstrate the unique role of sense-
To do so, we tested our conceptual predictions in two datasets collected making in coping with collective trauma. Comprehension (M = 4.97,
during two separate traumatic events: The COVID-19 global pandemic, SD = 1.13, 𝛼 = 0.74) was composed of four items (e.g., “Looking at my
and Hurricane Harvey, which impacted Houston, TX, in 2017. These life as whole, things seem clear to me.”; “I can’t make sense of events
studies are thus retrospective and exploratory, which is a significant in my life.” (reversed). Purpose (M = 5.53, SD = 1.15, 𝛼 = 0.82) was
limitation, though we approached our research questions through the composed of four items (e.g., “I have a good sense of what I am trying
frame of replicating and extending the findings of Park and colleagues to accomplish in life.”; “I don’t have compelling life goals that keep me
(2021) regarding meaning-oriented coping and psychological resilience going” (reversed). Existential mattering (M = 5.16, SD = 1.35, 𝛼 = 0.79)
to the pandemic. Our central aim was to explore free will beliefs as an was composed of four items (e.g., “Even considering how big the uni-
antecedent to meaning-making and meaning-focused coping in psycho- verse is, I can see that my life matters”; “My existence is not significant
logical recovery. This project addresses the apparent gap in the literature in the grand scheme of things” (reversed). In each subscale, participants
of the role of existential worldview beliefs in trauma recovery. We based rated their agreement with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
our predictions on the rationale that free will beliefs should facilitate a to 7 (strongly agree).
more agentic sense-making in response to stressful events, which should
facilitate meaning-oriented coping and psychological recovery. In Study Acute stress symptoms. We administered the 30-item Stanford Acute
1, we used a cross-sectional dataset to test the hypothesis that free will Stress Reaction Questionnaire (Cardeña et al., 2000) to assess for acute
beliefs would predict greater comprehension (the facet of meaning most stress symptoms. This scale is intended to measure stress following a
related to sense-making), which would in turn be associated with lower traumatic event, and we modified the items to specifically refer to the
psychological distress associated with the COVID-19 pandemic. Study 2 pandemic. Participants indicated the extent to which they experienced
was longitudinal, and allowed us to more directly test the prediction that each symptom (e.g., “I had difficulty falling or staying asleep.”; “I felt
free will beliefs would predict greater meaning-focused coping, which restless.”) on a scale ranging from 1 (not experienced) to 5 (very often
would, in turn, predict greater psychological recovery from a natural dis- experienced; (M = 2.26, SD = 1.00, 𝛼 = 0.96).
aster (i.e., Hurricane Harvey). While these studies are limited by their
divergent mediating variables, they ultimately allowed us to test our Post-Traumatic Stress (PTS) symptoms. Participants also responded to
conceptual hypothesis using diverse methodology in the context of two the 17-item PTSD Checklist-Civilian Version (Weathers et al., 1993),
distinct collective traumas. which we adapted to capture how distressed participants were to events
related to the pandemic (e.g., “Having physical reactions [e.g., heart
pounding, trouble breathing, or sweating] when something reminded
Study 1 you of the pandemic.”). Participants answered on a scale ranging from
1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely), and we averaged their responses to the
Method items to form a composite PTS score (M = 1.94, SD = 0.72, 𝛼 = 0.92).

Participants Results
We recruited 401 undergraduate students through the subject pool
at Texas A&M University, all of whom were enrolled in an introductory Preliminary analyses
psychology course and received course credit for their participation. We
excluded 59 participants who indicated their data was invalid because First, we ran a correlational analysis of all variables of interest. As
they just “clicked through” the survey. Our final sample included 342 can be seen in our supplementary materials1 , all variables were signif-
participants. A majority of participants identified as female (58.5%), icantly correlated with each other in the expected directions. Specifi-
White (77.8%), and non-Hispanic (71.9%). Ages ranged from 18 to 28 cally, free will beliefs were positively associated with all meaning facets,
(M = 19.48, SD = 1.27). A sensitivity analysis in G∗ power for a multiple and all of these variables negatively predicted all stress-related out-
(4) predictor regression indicated that our sample size would allow us comes.
to reliably detect an effect size of f2 = .02 or greater with 80% power
and alpha at .05 (Faul et al., 2009). Primary analyses

Materials and procedure For our primary analyses, we conducted two separate mediational
In Study 1, participants completed an online survey. The survey was analyses to assess the role of free will beliefs and sense-making, as indi-
administered between March 25 and April 27, 2019, approximately 10– cated by life comprehension, on psychological recovery from COVID-19;
40 days since the university had shut down in-person classes due to the we ran a multiple mediation model with 10,000 bootstrapping samples
emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic. (Hayes, PROCESS, 2013). Our first analysis examined whether compre-
hension uniquely mediated the relationship between free will beliefs
Measures and acute stress due to the pandemic. As expected, comprehension was
Free will. To measure free will beliefs, we administered the Belief in shown to uniquely mediate the relationship between free will beliefs
Free Will Subscale of the Free Will and Scientific Determinism Ques- and acute stress. Having a sense of purpose was also shown to uniquely
tionnaire (FAD-Plus; Paulhus and Carey, 2011). Participants responded mediate this relationship. Existential mattering, however, did not pre-
to 7 items intended to measure general beliefs that people have agency dict differences in acute stress after controlling for comprehension and
and control over their decisions (e.g., “People have complete control purpose. Full results are shown in Fig. 1.
over the decisions they make.”). These items were responded to along a
scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree; M = 5.06, SD = 1.05, 1
All data, materials and supplementary analyses can be found at:
𝛼 = 0.85). https://osf.io/29wkq/?view_only=5b1070f475d34be2ac60b51a51876ade.

3
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

Fig. 1. Facets of meaning in life mediating the relationship between free will beliefs and acute stress.

Fig. 2. Facets of meaning in life mediating the relationship between free will beliefs and post-traumatic stress symptoms.

Our second analysis examined whether comprehension uniquely me- many students from the university reside or have family in the areas
diated the relationship between free will beliefs and post-traumatic affected by hurricanes and floods. In 2017, Hurricane Harvey was espe-
stress symptoms due to the pandemic. Again, comprehension was shown cially catastrophic. Hurricane Harvey was a Category 4 hurricane that
to uniquely mediate the relationship between free will beliefs and stress. was one of the costliest hurricanes to make landfall in the United States
In this analysis, neither purpose nor existential mattering predicted post- (“Hurricane Harvey”, 2017). Indeed, soon after the hurricane, the uni-
traumatic stress symptoms after controlling for comprehension. Full re- versity president sent an email stating: “approximately 45 percent—or
sults are shown in Fig. 2. more than 31,000 members of [the] student body—come from one of
the counties affected by Hurricane Harvey, many students arrived for
classes with literally just the clothes on their backs after wading through
Discussion
waist-deep water in their hometowns to get here” (M. K. Young, Personal
Communication, September 8, 2017).
The findings of Study 1 provided preliminary support for the idea
We administered two surveys throughout the semester to study re-
that free will beliefs afford agentic meaning-making through increased
covery from this event. The resulting data allowed us to test whether
sense-making, which, in turn, relates less acute and post-event psycho-
free will beliefs would predict less psychological distress over time, and
logical distress from collective trauma. For both analyses, life compre-
whether meaning-focused coping (Park et al., 2010; Park et al., 2021),
hension uniquely mediated the relationship between free will beliefs and
would mediate this relationship.
distress related to the pandemic. Purpose also mediated the relation-
ship between free will beliefs and acute, but not post-traumatic stress.
Overall, this provided preliminary support for our hypothesis, but our Study 2
interpretation was limited based on the global nature of our mediating
variables. Study 2 allowed us to seek further support for our hypothesis Method
with a more situationally specific mediator.
For Study 2, we assessed free will beliefs, meaning-focused coping, Participants
and distress following a natural disaster, Hurricane Harvey (Maffly- We recruited 571 undergraduate students through the subject pool
Kipp et al., 2020; Maffly-Kipp et al., 2021). Texas A&M University in at Texas A&M University, all of whom were enrolled in an introduc-
College Station is about 90 miles from Houston, Texas, and as such, tory psychology course and received course credit for their participa-

4
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

tion. Four hundred and thirty of these participants completed a second and Time 2 stress as one of our dependent variables in our main analyses
wave of the study. We excluded 6 participants, who had scores on the (M = 0.12, SD = 0.57).
main DVs more than 4 SD from the mean, from the analyses. Our sample
was majority female (79.3%), White (80%), and non-Hispanic (79.1%).
PTS symptoms. At both time periods, participants again responded to
Ages ranged from 18 to 40 (M = 18.76, SD = 1.35). A sensitivity analy-
the 17-item PTSD Checklist-Civilian Version (Weathers et al., 1993),
sis in G∗ power for a multiple (5) predictor regression indicated that our
adapted to capture how distressed participants were to the events re-
sample size would allow us to reliably detect an effect size of f2 = .02
lated to Hurricane Harvey. Participants answered on a scale ranging
or greater with 80% power and alpha at .05 (Faul et al., 2009).
from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely), and we summed their responses to
the items to form a composite PTS score (M = 1.34, SD = 0.29, 𝛼 = .93,
Materials and procedure
for Time 1; M = 1.17, SD = 0.30, 𝛼 = 0.90, for Time 2). We again created
Participants completed an online survey twice. The first survey was
a difference score between Time 1 and Time 2 PTS scores as one of our
one month after Hurricane Harvey (Time 1), and the second was ap-
dependent variables in our main analyses (M = 0.12, SD = 5.12).
proximately 9 weeks after the storm (Time 2). Some of our measures
were adapted to assess perceptions of the effects of Hurricane Harvey.
Results
Measures
Free will. To measure free will beliefs, we again administered the Be- Preliminary analyses
lief in Free Will Subscale of the Free Will and Scientific Determinism
Questionnaire (FAD-Plus; Paulhus and Carey, 2011) at both time points. Again, we ran a correlational analysis of all variables of interest (see
Participants responded to 7 items intended to measure general beliefs supplementary materials). Notably, and in line with predictions, free
that people have agency and control over their decisions. These items will beliefs were positively associated with meaning-focused coping, and
were responded to along a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly both of these variables were positively correlated with improvements in
agree; M = 5.35, SD = 1.01, 𝛼 = 0.86, for Time 1; M = 5.22, SD = 1.06, psychological stress.
𝛼 = 0.89, for Time 2).
Primary analyses
Coping strategies. We administered the 28 item Brief COPE (Carver,
1997) at Time 1 and Time 2 to gauge each participant’s use of 14 com-
For our primary analyses, we again conducted two separate media-
mon coping strategies (positive reframing, humor, self-distraction, ac-
tional analyses to assess the role of free will beliefs and coping styles
ceptance, etc.) that may have been used in response to the disaster.
on psychological recovery from COVID-19; we ran a multiple mediation
Participants responded how often over the past week they engaged in
model with 10,000 bootstrapping samples (Hayes, PROCESS). Our first
a variety of behaviors on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (I haven’t been
analysis examined whether meaning-focused coping at Time 1 uniquely
doing this at all) to 4 (I’ve been doing this a lot). Subscale scores were
mediated the relationship between Time 1 free will beliefs and changes
taken as the average of relevant items. Following Park et al. (2021), we
in acute stress due to the Hurricane3 . As expected, meaning-focused cop-
created a meaning-focused coping variable by averaging the positive re-
ing was shown to uniquely mediate the relationship between free will
framing and acceptance subscales (e.g., “I’ve been trying to see it in a
beliefs and acute stress. None of the other Time 1 coping mechanisms
different light, to make it seem more positive,” “I’ve been accepting the
showed the same pattern. Full results are shown in Fig. 3.
reality of the fact that it has happened,” M = 1.90, SD = 0.69), as well
Our second analysis examined whether meaning-focused coping at
as a problem-focused coping variable by averaging the planning and ac-
Time 1 uniquely mediated the relationship between Time 1 free will
tive coping subscales (e.g., “I’ve been thinking hard about what steps to
beliefs and changes in PTS symptoms. As expected, meaning-focused
take,” M = 1.50, SD = 0.64), a social support variable by averaging the
coping was shown to uniquely mediate the relationship between free
instrumental and emotional support coping variables (e.g., “I’ve been
will beliefs and post-traumatic stress. Again, none of the other coping
getting emotional support from others,” M = 1.29, SD = 0.47), and an
mechanisms showed the same pattern. Full results are shown in Fig. 4.
avoidance coping variable by averaging the denial, substance use, vent-
ing, behavioral disengagement, self-distraction and self-blame variables
(e.g., “I’ve been saying to myself "this isn’t real," M = 1.10, SD = 0.24). Discussion
These later variables were included in the analyses, again, for both ex-
ploratory purposes, as well as to examine the unique influence of free The findings for Study 2 further supported our hypothesis that free
will beliefs on psychological distress though meaning-focused coping in will beliefs lend themselves to agentic meaning-making. The longitudi-
particular. nal nature of Study 2 allowed us to more clearly see how free will beliefs
not only act as an initial buffer of distress in the face of collective trauma,
Acute stress symptoms. We again administered the 30-item Stanford but also lowered distress over time. Study 2 replicates and extends Park
Acute Stress Reaction Questionnaire (Cardeña et al., 2000) to assess for and colleagues’ (2021) findings that meaning-focused coping strategies
acute stress symptoms. We modified the items to specifically refer to confer resilience in the face of collective trauma and uniquely demon-
Hurricane Harvey. Participants indicated the extent to which they ex- strates the importance of existential worldviews in making sense of and
perienced each symptom on a scale ranging from 1 (not experienced), to coping with traumatic events.
6 (very often experienced). They completed the same measure at Time
2 (M = 1.43, SD = 0.57, 𝛼 = 0.94, for Time 1; M = 1.31, SD = 0.55,
𝛼 = 0.96, for Time 2) We computed a difference score2 between Time 1 framework is generally recommended as the most robust test of Time 1 to Time
2 change (Castro-Schilo and Grimm, 2018; Mun et al., 2009). Since the differ-
ence score approach was our original analysis plan, we present that here. How-
2
There is some dispute about the reliability of difference scores, with ever, we also present the results of residualized change analyses, which were
some researchers preferring residualized change scores (e.g., Cronbach and nonsignificant, and our SEM analyses, which were significant, in supplemental
Furby, 1970). This is a complex topic, however, multiple researchers have sug- materials.
3
gested that difference scores are indeed a preferable approach when there are We also ran these analyses switching free will beliefs and meaning-focused
likely to be intraindividual difference in changes between Time 1 and Time 2 coping in the model. These analyses were nonsignificant, which strengthens our
measurements (Castro-Schilo and Grimm, 2018; Gollwitzer et al., 2014), as is interpretation of the directionality of these relationships. These analyses can be
the case with trauma recovery. That said, SEM modeling with a latent variable found in supplemental materials.

5
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

Fig. 3. Coping strategies mediating the relationship between free will beliefs and acute stress.

Fig. 4. Coping strategies mediating the relationship between free will beliefs and post-traumatic stress symptoms.

General discussion meaning-focused coping strategies mediated the relationship between


free will beliefs and psychological recovery from the event, and were
Meaning-making, which depends upon basic schemas and beliefs re- in fact the only type of coping strategy to do so. This study confirmed
lated to the self and the world (Park, 2010), is a vital component of the suggestive evidence provided by Study 1 and robustly supported our
psychological recovery from difficult experiences like collective trau- primary hypothesis in a unique longitudinal sample.
mas (Updegraff et al., 2008). We predicted that free will beliefs would These findings affirm and extend the body of work involving the
be an important antecedent to meaning-making and meaning-focused importance of meaning-making in the context of recovery from collec-
coping in the context of trauma recovery. Our results across two stud- tive traumas. Multiple studies have demonstrated that meaning-making
ies, drawn from datasets collected in the wake of two distinct collective predicts better recovery from these events (Maffly-Kipp et al., 2021;
traumas, broadly supported this prediction. In Study 1, we explored Park, 2021; Updegraff et al., 2008), so the discovery of factors that
a cross-sectional sample collected during the height of the COVID-19 might facilitate this process, like free will beliefs, is a novel contribution.
global pandemic. Our results showed that free will beliefs uniquely pre- If worldview-dissonant traumas trigger accommodative efforts (Janoff-
dicted comprehension, the facet of meaning in life most conceptually Bulman, 1985; Schwartzberg and Janoff-Bulman, 1991), then it is per-
related to sense-making, which in turn predicted lower acute stress and haps unsurprising that pre-existing beliefs about one’s ability to freely
post-event psychological distress. In Study 2, we analyzed a longitudi- navigate themselves throughout the world would relate to this process.
nal sample collected in the aftermath of a deadly 2017 natural disas- Indeed, this is consistent with Park (2010) model of meaning-making,
ter in Houston, Texas: Hurricane Harvey. These analyses revealed that which suggests that the interplay between stressful experiences, situ-

6
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

ational meaning-making, and global beliefs/meaning is an interactive posure therapy (Gwozdziewycz and Mehl-Madrona, 2013) or cognitive
and ongoing process by which people create and update their world- processing therapy (see Chard et al., 2012). Future work could investi-
views. While other global beliefs are likely important in this process (see gate whether free will beliefs are predictive of the effectiveness of such
Benight and Bandura, 2004), we interpreted our results to suggest that approaches.
free will beliefs might afford people a more agentic orientation towards This work has several important limitations. First, the mediating
their own recovery and meaning-making processes. Although our find- variables differ between our two studies, limiting the extent to which
ings, as well as previous research (e.g., Baumeister and Brewer, 2012; we can interpret our findings as evidence of the same processes. Global
Seto et al., 2015), support this latter notion, future work is needed to judgments of meaning/comprehension (as were measured in Study 1)
confirm the precise mechanistic nature by which free will beliefs are are undoubtedly different from the specific utilization of meaning-
related to improved meaning-making and psychological recovery. oriented coping strategies (as were measured in Study 2). Further, de-
It is also notable that comprehension, the facet of meaning that cap- spite Study 2 being longitudinal, the relationship between free will and
tures the perceived coherence of one’s worldview (George and Park, meaning was cross-sectional in both studies, limiting the extent to which
2016), was the only consistent mediator of the pathway between free we can infer directionality from our findings. That said, Park’s (2010)
will beliefs and psychological recovery in Study 1 (though purpose meaning-making model suggests that situational sense-making directly
showed some evidence of this pattern). This is consistent with our pre- interacts with global meaning in a cyclical and recursive fashion, so
dictions, and may suggest that free will beliefs are most relevant to the there is at least theoretical reason to see valuable commonalities across
ways that one makes sense of a traumatic event, rather than ways that our two distinct mediating variables. Ultimately, both studies showed
one finds purpose or mattering in its wake. Indeed, Park (2010) model that free will beliefs were an important predictor of meaning-related
is most focused on sense-making as a mechanism for recovery, which processes, which in turn predicted lower distress/better recovery. This
is consistent with the relatively cognitive form of meaning-making es- is thus an imperfect, though still valuable, exploration of an under-
poused by existentialists (e.g., Sartre, 1946; Yalom, 1980). Previous explored and difficult to study area of research. Second, our study relied
work has also shown that comprehension is the facet of meaning most solely upon self-reported variables. Though there is often no clear way
protective against the experience of chronic pain (Boring et al., 2022), to more effectively measure abstract psychological constructs like be-
which further validates its potential importance for psychological health liefs in free will or meaning, it should not be overlooked that the ways
and managing stressful realities. Future work should investigate the role in which people answer survey items might not perfectly relate to how
of comprehension, relative to purpose and mattering, in mental health they engage with the world through a personal existential framework
and mental illness (i.e., a worldview). Third, all of our participants were undergraduates
Our work also holds important implications for the literature involv- at a single university, which of course limits our ability to generalize
ing free will beliefs. Lay beliefs about free will are thought to function as these findings to other populations. Finally, all analyses were retrospec-
a basis for the volitional control that allows people to function in society tive and exploratory. While a commitment to open science practices like
in an autonomous and adaptive way (Baumeister and Monroe, 2014). pre-registration is vital, it is also the case that collective traumas are
Of course, it makes intuitive sense that this perspective might afford unpredictable and difficult to study, therefore any opportunity to do
a more volitional approach to one’s own recovery. However, this per- so should be considered valuable. Nevertheless, our findings should be
spective also raises the possibility that the framing of our research in interpreted with these important caveats in mind.
the context of collective traumas specifically might be especially im- Collective traumas are unique events both at the broad societal level
portant. If recovery from these community-wide events indeed requires and at the individual psychological level. As such, responses to them
engagement with collective systems of meaning (Hirschberger, 2018), may carry important wisdom about the interplay between self and cul-
it stands to reason that factors facilitating collective and prosocial atti- ture. In this paper, we have provided evidence that a basic belief about
tudes might be uniquely relevant. Of course, some existing research is the nature of this interplay, belief in free will, is an important factor in
suggestive that free will beliefs can facilitate meaning-making related how people cope with and recover from these community-wide traumas.
to individual events (Seto et al., 2015), but future research could at- We have interpreted our findings to suggest that free will beliefs may af-
tempt to investigate whether our findings might differ in the context of ford people a more agentic approach to meaning-making, ultimately al-
individual traumas. Further, this research joins a small number of stud- lowing for more effective recovery from the traumatic event. In a chaotic
ies suggesting that free will beliefs have important relationships with world, such orientations might be evermore important to retain.
mental health constructs (Borunova, 2019; Li and Wong, 2020), a topic
which is understudied. In consideration of our findings, free will and Declaration of Competing Interest
agentic meaning-making might be important concepts to consider re-
garding the etiology of, and recovery from, experiences like depression, The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
anxiety, and post-traumatic stress. interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
Finally, our findings may carry important clinical implications. the work reported in this paper.
Trauma is a major public health concern (Magruder et al., 2017),
and mental health professionals devote significant efforts towards ef- Ethical Approval Statement
fectively managing and treating the psychological effects of trauma
(Perrotta, 2019). Of course, the identification of any factors that pre- This research was reviewed and approved by the ethics committee
dict trauma recovery can be useful to this aim, but these findings (Institutional Review Board) at Texas A&M University. The research
might provide some particularly actionable insights. That meaning- for Study 1 was approved on 02/20/2020, with the approval number
making might facilitate trauma therapy is not a novel concept (see IRB2020-0173, reference number 105701. The research for Study 2 was
Steger and Park, 2012), and many existing therapies may already im- approved on 09/28/2017, with the approval number IRB2017-0671,
plicitly facilitate growth in this way (e.g., insight-oriented approaches, reference number 063578.
see Hamm et al., 2021). However, the knowledge that a belief in All participants of both studies read and signed informed consent
personal volitional autonomy is important to meaning-related coping forms.
could be useful for clinicians and those developing therapeutic in-
terventions. For example, a careful examination of one’s worldview CRediT authorship contribution statement
and existential beliefs, as is suggested by existential therapies (e.g.,
Yalom, 1980), might be a critical first step before the more active ap- Joseph Maffly-Kipp: Writing – original draft, Formal analysis, Writ-
proaches that are often taken in trauma therapies, like narrative ex- ing – review & editing, Formal analysis. Chase Gause: Writing – original

7
J. Maffly-Kipp, C. Gause, J. Kim et al. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3 (2022) 100074

draft, Writing – review & editing, Formal analysis. Jinhyung Kim: Data Hirschberger, G., 2018. Collective trauma and the social construction of meaning. Front.
curation, Formal analysis, Visualization. Matthew Vess: Writing – re- Psychol. 9, 1441.
Hurricane Harvey. (2017, September 24). In Wikipedia. https://en.wikipedia.org/
view & editing. Joshua A. Hicks: Writing – original draft, Writing – wiki/Hurricane_Harvey.
review & editing, Data curation, Formal analysis. Janoff-Bulman, R., 1985. The aftermath of victimization: rebuilding shattered assump-
tions. Trauma its Wake 1, 15–35.
Data availability Janoff-Bulman, R., 1992. Toward a new psychology of trauma. : Free Press, NY.
Jones, L.K., Cureton, J.L., 2014. Trauma redefined in the DSM-5: rationale and implica-
tions for counseling practice. Prof. Counselor 4, 257–271.
Our data is shared through OSF: https://osf.io/29wkq/?view_only= Kane, R., 2011. Rethinking free will: A new perspective on an ancient problem. In: Kane, R.
5b1070f475d34be2ac60b51a51876ade. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of free will. Oxford University Press, New York, NY,
pp. 381–404.
Kauer-Sant’Anna, M., Tramontina, J., Andreazza, A.C., Cereser, K., Costa, S.D., Santin, A.,
Kapczinski, F., 2007. Traumatic life events in bipolar disorder: impact on BDNF levels
and psychopathology. Bipolar Disord. 9, 128–135.
Supplementary materials Li, C., Wang, S., Zhao, Y., Kong, F., Li, J., 2016. The freedom to pursue happiness: belief
in free will predicts life satisfaction and positive affect among Chinese adolescents.
Front. Psychol. 7, 1–8.
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in
Li, P.J., Wong, Y.J., 2020. Beliefs in free will versus determinism: search for meaning
the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100074. mediates the life scheme–depressive symptom link. J. Humanist. Psychol. Adv. Online
Publ..
Luszczynska, A., Benight, C.C., Cieslak, R., 2009. Self-efficacy and health-related
References outcomes of collective trauma: a systematic review. Eur. Psychol. 14 (1),
51–62.
Alquist, J.L., Ainsworth, S.E., Baumeister, R.F., 2013. Determined to conform: disbelief in Maffly-Kipp, J., Flanagan, P., Kim, J., Schlegel, R.J., Vess, M., Hicks, J.A., 2020. The role
free will increases conformity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 80–86. of perceived authenticity in psychological recovery from collective trauma. J. Soc.
American Psychiatric Association, 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis- Clin. Psychol. 39 (5), 419–448.
orders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub. Maffly-Kipp, J., Flanagan, P., Kim, J., Rivera, G., Friedman, M.D., Vess, M., Hicks, J.A.,
Baumeister, R.F., Brewer, L.E., 2012. Believing versus disbelieving in free will: correlates 2021. Meaning-making, psychological distress, and the experience of meaning in life
and consequences. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 6, 736–745. following a natural disaster. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 12 (5), 812–820.
Baumeister, R.F., Monroe, A.E., 2014. Recent research on free will: conceptualizations, May, R., 1975. The courage to create. Norton.
beliefs, and processes. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 50, 1–52. Magruder, K.M., McLaughlin, K.A., Elmore Borbon, D.L., 2017. Trauma is a public health
Benight, C.C., Bandura, A., 2004. Social cognitive theory of posttraumatic recovery: the issue. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 8 (1), 1375338.
role of perceived self-efficacy. Behav. Res. Ther. 42 (10), 1129–1148. Moynihan, A.B., Igou, E.R., van Tilburg, W.A., 2017. Free, connected, and meaningful:
Benjet, C., Bromet, E., Karam, E.G., Kessler, R.C., McLaughlin, K.A., Ruscio, A.M., Koe- free will beliefs promote meaningfulness through belongingness. Pers. Individ Dif.
nen, K.C., 2016. The epidemiology of traumatic event exposure worldwide: results 107, 54–65.
from the world mental health survey consortium. Psychol. Med. 46 (2), 327–343. Mun, E.Y., von Eye, A., White, H.R., 2009. An SEM approach for the evaluation of inter-
Bergner, R.M., Ramon, A., 2013. Some implications of beliefs in altruism, free will, and vention effects using pre-post-post designs. Struct. Eq. Model. 16 (2), 315–337.
nonreductionism. J. Soc. Psychol. 153 (5), 598–618. Ryan, R.M., Deci, E.L., 2001. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on
Borunova, I., 2019. Blessed or condemned to be free: exploring the correlations between hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 141.
free will beliefs, anxiety and well-being [Doctoral dissertation, Empire State College]. Park, C.L., 2010. Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning
University of New, York, Prague. making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol. Bull. 136 (2),
Boring, B. L., Maffly-Kipp, J., Mathur, V. A., Hicks, J. A., 2022. Meaning in Life and Pain: 257–301.
The Differential Effects of Coherence, Purpose, and Mattering on Pain Severity, Fre- Park, C.L., Finkelstein-Fox, L., Russell, B.S., Fendrich, M., Hutchison, M., Becker, J.,
quency, and the Development of Chronic Pain. Journal of Pain Research 15, 299–314. 2021. Psychological resilience early in the COVID-19 pandemic: stressors, resources,
Cacioppo, J.T., Patrick, W., 2008. Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Con- and coping strategies in a national sample of Americans. Am. Psychol. 76 (2), 715–
nection. WW Norton & Company. 728.
Carver, C. S., 1997. You want to measure coping but your protocol’too long: Consider the Park, C.L., Folkman, S., 1997. Meaning in the context of stress and coping. Rev. Gen.
brief cope. International journal of behavioral medicine 4 (1), 92–100. Psychol. 1 (2), 115–144.
Castro-Schilo, L., Grimm, K.J., 2018. Using residualized change versus difference scores Paulhus, D. L., Carey, J. M., 2011. The FAD–Plus: Measuring lay beliefs regarding free will
for longitudinal research. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 35 (1), 32–58. and related constructs. Journal of personality assessment 93 (1), 96–104.
Chard, K.M., Schuster, J.L., Resick, P.A., 2012. In: Empirically Supported Psychological Perrotta, G., 2019. Psychological trauma: definition, clinical contexts, neural correlations
Treatments: Cognitive Processing, 1. The Oxford Handbook of Traumatic Stress Dis- and therapeutic approaches. Curr. Res. Psychiatry Brain Disord., 100006 CRPBD-.
orders, pp. 439–448. Sartre, J., 1946. Existentialism is a Humanism. Yale University Press.
Charzyńska, E., Wysocka, E., 2014. The role of spirituality and belief in free will in the Saul, J., 2014. Collective Trauma, Collective Healing. Routledge, New York.
perception of self-efficacy among young adults. New Educ. Rev. 36, 194–205. Schlegel, R. J., Hicks, J. A., Arndt, J., King, L. A., 2009. Thine own self: True self-concept
Coleman, R.A., Neimeyer, R.A., 2010. Measuring meaning: searching for and making sense accessibility and meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 (2),
of spousal loss in late-life. Death Stud. 34 (9), 804–834. 473–490.
Crescioni, A.W., Baumeister, R.F., Ainsworth, S.E., Ent, M., Lambert, N.M., 2016. Subjec- Schwartzberg, S.S., Janoff-Bulman, R., 1991. Grief and the search for meaning: exploring
tive correlates and consequences of belief in free will. Philos. Psychol. 29, 41–63. the assumptive worlds of bereaved college students. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 10 (3),
Cronbach, L.J., Furby, L., 1970. How we should measure "change": or should we? Psychol. 270–288.
Bull. 74 (1), 68–80. Seligman, M.E., Railton, P., Baumeister, R.F., Sripada, C., 2013. Navigating into the future
Davis, C.G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., 1998. Making sense of loss and benefiting or driven by the past. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 119–141.
from the experience: two construals of meaning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75 (2), 561–574. Seto, E., Hicks, J.A., 2016. Disassociating the agent from the self: undermining belief in
Dixon, A., Howie, P., Starling, J., 2005. Trauma exposure, posttraumatic stress, and psy- free will diminishes true self-knowledge. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 7 (7), 726–734.
chiatric comorbidity in female juvenile offenders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolescent Psy- Seto, E., Hicks, J.A., Davis, W.E., Smallman, R., 2015. Free will, counterfactual reflection,
chiatry 44, 798–806. and the meaningfulness of life events. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 6 (3), 243–250.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., Lang, A. G., 2009. Statistical power analyses using G∗ Silver, R.L., Boon, C., Stones, M.H., 1983. Searching for meaning in misfortune: making
Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods sense of incest. J. Soc. Issues 39 (2), 81–101.
41 (4), 1149–1160. Smith, A.J., Lapsansky, E.R., 2021. Trauma and the question of free will: an existentialist’s
Feldman, G., Baumeister, R.F., Wong, K.F.E., 2014. Free will is about choosing: the link guide to trauma-informed care. J. Hum. Couns. 60 (1), 37–57.
between choice and the belief in free will. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 239–245. Steger, M.F., Park, C.L., 2012. The creation of meaning following trauma: meaning
Feldman, G., Chandrashekar, S.P., Wong, K.F.E., 2016. The freedom to excel: belief in free making and trajectories of distress and recovery. In: McMackin, R.A., Newman, E.,
will predicts better academic performance. Pers. Individ Dif. 90, 377–383. Fogler, J.M., Keane, T.M. (Eds.), Trauma Therapy in context: The Science and Craft
Frankl, V., 1959. Man’s search for meaning. Beacon Press. of Evidence-based Practice. American Psychological Association, pp. 171–191.
George, L. S., Park, C. L., 2016. Meaning in life as comprehension, purpose, and mattering: Tolstikova, K., Fleming, S., Chartier, B., 2005. Grief, complicated grief, and trauma: the
Toward integration and new research questions. Review of General Psychology 20 (3), role of the search for meaning, impaired self-reference, and death anxiety. Illness,
205–220. Crisis & Loss 13 (4), 293–313.
Gollwitzer, M., Christ, O., Lemmer, G., 2014. Individual differences make a difference: on Triplett, K.N., Tedeschi, R.G., Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., Reeve, C.L., 2012. Posttraumatic
the use and the psychometric properties of difference scores in social psychology. Eur. growth, meaning in life, and life satisfaction in response to trauma. Psychol. Trauma
J. Soc. Psychol. 44 (7), 673–682. Theory, Res. Pract. Policy 4 (4), 400–410.
Gwozdziewycz, N., Mehl-Madrona, L., 2013. Meta-analysis of the use of narrative exposure Updegraff, J.A., Silver, R.C., Holman, E.A., 2008. Searching for and finding meaning in
therapy for the effects of trauma among refugee populations. Perm J. 17 (1), 70–76. collective trauma: results from a national longitudinal study of the 9/11 terrorist at-
Hamm, J.A., Beasley, R.E., Mazor, Y., 2021. Trauma and meaning-making in the recovery tacks. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95 (3), 709–722.
of the self: implications for metacognitive reflection and insight therapy (MERIT). In: Yalom, I., 1980. Existential psychotherapy. BasicBooks.
The Recovery of the Self in Psychosis. Routledge, pp. 164–181.

You might also like