You are on page 1of 7

Rev. BioI. Trop.

, 49(1 ): 383-389, 200 I


www.ucr. ac.cr www.ot s.ac.cr www.ots.duke.edu

Sex ratio and breedin g of white-lipped peccaries Tayassu pecari


(Artiodactyla: Tayassuidae) in a Costa Rican rain forest

Marian a Altrichter*, Carlos Drews, Eduardo Carrillo and Joel Saenz


Progr ama Regional en Manejo de Vida Silvestre, Universidad Nacio nal, Apartado 1350-3000 He redia, Costa Rica.
Fa x: (506) 237-70 36.
* Corresponding author. m.altricher@yaho o.com . New address: School of Renew able Natural Resources, Universit y of
Ar izona. 104 Bio Sc iences Eas t. Tucson, Arizon a, AZ 8572 1.

Received 27-IX-1999. Corrected ll-Vll-2000 . Accepted 3 1-Vll -2000.

Abstract: White-lipped peccaries are non-season al breeders in South America, but little is known about their
reprodu ction in Central America. T here are few studies about the sex ratio of this species in the field . We stu­
died the reprodu ction and sex ratio of white-lipp ed peccaries dur ing 200 hours of field observation of four ra­
diom arked and two unmark ed herds, from July 1996 to April 1997, in Corcov ado Nation al Park, Costa Rica. Sex
ratio data of three add itional, radiomarked herds obser ved in 1998 were also included. We record ed number s of
mountin gs, presen ce of newborn s and numb ers of nursing interactio ns. The pecca ries showed a disti nct repro­
ductive seasonal ity, with one mating peak in February and another in Jul y. The greatest numb er of newborns
and the peak in nursing activity were observed dur ing July and August, when fruit ava ilability for the peccaries
was high. The adult sex rat io was sign ificantly female biased (1.4: I - 1.8:1), also in cont rast with So uth Am e­
rican popul ations.

Key wo rds: Corcovado, Costa Rica, mating, nursing, reproduction , sex ratio, Tayassu pecari, white-lipp ed peccary.

The white-lipped peccary Tayassu pecari There is little published information on


L. (1795) is one of the most threatened neotro­ reproduction in the white-lipp ed peccary.
pical mammal s and its status deteriorates due Anecdotal observations (Miller 1930, Leopold
to habitat loss and hunting (Emmons 1984, 1959, Sowls 1984) and resear ch in the Peru­
Oliver 1990). Littl e is known about its popul a­ vian Amazon (Gottdenker and Bodmer 1998),
tion dynamics and behavior. White-lipp ed indicate that white-lipped peccaries breed th­
peccaries range from southern Mexico to nort­ roughout the year. Gottdenker and Bodmer
hern Argentina , and prefer unaltered humid (1998) suggest that this breeding pattern may
and semideciduous fore sts (Sowl s 1997) . result from the relatively constant rainfall and
They are social animals, living in herds from the resulting year long food availability. Ho­
fifteen to several hundred individu als (Sowl s wever, in many Central American rain forests
1997). This species has been characterized as a pronounced seasonality delineated by rain­
mainly frugivorous in the Peruvian Amazon fall causes a variable fruit production during
(Kiltie 1981, Bodmer 1989) and the rain fo­ the year with periods of food scarcity (Janzen
rests of Cos ta Rica (Altrichter et al. 2000). For J983, Boinski 1986, Altrichter J (97) . There­
an overview of peccary natural history see Ma­ fore, one might expect that peccaries inhabi­
yer and Brandt (1982) and Sowls (1997). ting Central Americ an rain forests breed so as
3R4 REYISTADE BIOLOGIA TROPICAL

to have litters during periods of high food (5-15 m) during maximum possible diurnal time
abundance. by the first author and an assistant. The pecca­
Studies of collared peccary (Tayassu taja­ ries seemed habituated to our presence and allo­
cu, L. 1758) from Arizona and of Chacoan pec­ wed us to move around and within the herd wit­
caries (Catagonus wagner i, R. 1930) from Pa­ hout fleeing. Observability of the knimals did
raguay suggest that adult sex ratios are close to not vary noticeably between seasons because
even in these species (reviews in Mayer and the forest is evergreen and because herd cohe ­
Brandt 1982, Sowls 1984, 1997). An essen­ sion remains roughly constant throughout the
tially I: I fetal sex ratio in white-lipped pecca­ year (Campero 1999, Fragoso 1998). We recor­
ries of the Peruvian Amazon (Gottdenker and ded copulations, nursing interactions, perc enta­
Bodmer 1998) and an adult sex ratio of 1.8 fe­ ge of newborns and sex composition of herds .
males per male in one herd in the Brazilian Mean values are given in the text with their co­
Amazon (Fragoso 1994) is the only informa­ rresponding standard deviation.
tion available for this species. In this paper we Fruit av a ilab ili ty: The availability of
address adult sex ratio and the seasonality of fruit components of white-lipped peccary diet
reproduction in white-lipped peccaries of a in Corcovado National Park was estimated
Costa Rican rain forest. This information is concurrently (Altrichter 1997). An index was
pertinent to the design of management and used that included number of trees fruiting and
conservation strategies for this species. amounts of fruits on the floor, underneath the
trees. The index expresses a monthly mean of
fruit availability.
MATERIALS AND METHODS Mating and nursin g: We recorded ma­
ting interactions ad libitum whenever pecca­
Study area: We conducted this study in ries were seen interacting, and made concomi­
Corcovado National Park (Osa Peninsula), Costa tant counts of all visible individuals. Nursing
Rica (8°26' to 8°39' Nand 83°25' to 83°45' W). events were recorded during scan samples at
The park extension is about 46 774 ha (Garcia 10 min intervals. We recorded number of copu­
1997), most of which is "very humid tropical fo­ lations and nursing events observed daily and
rest" according to Holdridge's (1967) life zones calculated a mean hourly rate every two weeks
classification. The dry season extends from De­ and monthly. Such hourly rates allow direct
cember to April and the rainy season from May comparisons between days and other time pe­
to November. October and November are riods, given that observation time differed bet­
months of maximum precipitation (>500 mm­ ween units. The rates, however, are not abso­
/month) and February and March are the driest lute values corresponding to overall interaction
months «100 mm/month) (Vaughan 1981). frequencies in the herd , because only a propor­
Mean annual temperature is 26° C and the preci­ tion of herd animals were visible at any given
pitation ranges from 3800 mm in lowland to time. Errors resulting in omissions of copula­
6500 mm in the mountains (Harthsorn 1983). tions in our ad libitum records are probably
The majority of the park is lowland, covered syst ematic and unlikely to bias the patterns
mainly with primary forest (Naranjo 19(4). described because both observers and observa­
Observations: We report data from nine tion conditions were constant throughout the
peccary herds. We radiotracked seven pecca­ study. Since observation time differed between
ries, from four different herds and observed days, we indicate frequency of interactions per
two additional herds without radiotransmitters, hour of observation to facilitate comparisons.
from July 1996 to April 1997. Three herds we­ Sex ratio : Body size was used to assign
re radiomarked and their sex ratio recorded in individuals to the adult class. Sex determina­
1998. Observations were made at close range tion was possible because the adult males '
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF TROPICAL BIOLOGY AND CONSERVATION 385

scrotum is conspicuous at close range. Since Litters conceived in February, the highest
during 1996 and 1997 we determined the sex mating peak (Fig. I) were born during months
of every adult individual for only one herd, of high fruit availability, as indicated by peaks
the sex composition of the other five herds in nursing activity in August (Fig. 2). We obser­
was estimated using two indirect approaches. ved 46 nursing events. Nursing frequency was
Firstly, sexes were recorded in groups of at greatest in the latter 15 days of August (2.22
least five adult animals . We assumed that the events/h), and no nursing was observed from Ja­
sex composition of visible groups over the nuary to April (Fig. 2, Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA,
course of several observations would be a p< 0.00 I). Some suckling youngs were as large
random samp le of herd composition. These as three quarters the size of the mother.
groups were not apart from the herd but co­
rresponded to those individuals directly visi­
ble to us. Secondly, we determined daily sex 0.8
ratios of randomly selected adults for beha­ 0.7
vioral focal-samples on days with four or mo­
'" 0.6
re focal animals (Altrichter 1997). In 1998
~
all adults from three herds were sexed repea­ o 0.5

tedly. Since herds split and merged in new ~I:: 0.4


o
combinations during 1998, sex ratios reported 'aro 0.3
here for these three herds are considered sta­ "5
tistically independent from the 1996 and
g. 02
U
0.1
1997 data.
SoN D J F M A
Months
RESULTS
Fig. 1. Copulation frequency of white-lipped peccaries on
During the 10 months of study, herds we­ 15 day intervals between July 1996 and April 1997 in Cor­
covado National Park, Costa Rica (for methods see text) .
.re observed and their behavior recorded on 71 In some months there was only one IS-day interval of ob­
days. Herd sizes varied from 21 to 70 indivi­ servations. No peccaries were observed in December.
duals. The number of animals counted during
interactions was variable (range 3 - 21), but no
obvious differences or patterns in this varia­
2.5 160
tion were found between months (Kruskal­
Wallis ANOVA, p = 0.11). During December _ Nursing 140
...
~

-e
we were unable to find any peccary herd. _ Fnti t 8vailkabilily 120.5

Copulations and nursin g frequency: We 100~


observed 33 copulations. The frequency of co­ ~
pulations per hour of observation varied signi­
SO'§
~
60 <U
ficantly between periods of 15 days during the
study (Fig. 1, Kruskal- Wallis ANOVA, p< 40""
]
0.05). It was highest during the first IS days 20

of February, with a mean of 0.72 ± 0.17 copu­


ASONDJ F MA
lations per hour. No copulations were obser­
Months
ved during September, October, November,
and April. The frequency of copulations was Fig. 2. Nurs ing frequency and fruit availability for white ­
significantly higher in the dry season (0.45/h ± lipped pecc aries on 15 day intervals between July 1996 and
April 1997 in Corcovado National Park, Costa Rica. On
0.16) than in the wet season (0.16/h ± 0.09)
some months there was only IS-day interval of observa­
(Mann-Whitney U-test, p< 0.05). . tions. No pec caries were observed in December.
386 REVISTA DE BIOLOofA TRO PICAL

TABLE 1 ted individuals from six herds was 1.4: I, co­


rresponding to 59% ± 3.7 females (n = 34 days
Sex ratio ofadult white-lipped peccar ies in free-living
herds is f emale biased
with four or more focal animals). The units
contributing to the means of the latter two in­
direct methods are not statistically indepen­
Herd Method I Method 2 Method 3
dent, because the same herds and, on some oc­
casions probably the same individuals, were
I 1.8 (3) 2.3 (5) 2 (21) sampled for sex ratio data repeatedly.
2 1.5 (9) 2.0 (3)
3 1.6 (9) 1.6 (9)
4 1.4 (6) 1.6 (13)
5 0.8 (2) 1.2 (I)
DISC USSION
6 1.2 (8)
7 1.4 (44) M ating seasonality : In contrast to the
8 2.0 (42) non-seasonal patterns of breeding observed for
9 1.7 (35)
white-lipped and collared peccaries in the Pe­
10 1.8 (25)
ruvian Amazon (Gottdenker and Bodmer
Numbers indicate the proport ion of female s to males . Met­ 1998), T. pecari in Corcovado National Park is
hod I = Mean sex ratio of observations of n groups in the a seasonal breeder with tw o peaks of mating
herd. Method 2 = Mean daily sex ratio of randomly chosen events. Mating interactions occurred mainly in
individuals for focal samples of behavi or on n days. Met­
February and Ma rch and to a lesser extent in
hod 3 = All n adults of the herd were sexed by direct obser­
vation. The number of individual observed, n is shown in
July and August. Indeed, the greatest abun­
brackets. Herd 10: Frago so (1994), others: this study. dance of newborns observed from June to Au­
gust results from the February and March ma­
tings, in accordance to a gestation of about fi­
Presence of newborns: The highest num­ ve months (Roots 1966, Sowls 1997). This
bers of newborns (one month old or less) were pattern was also confirmed by the nursing acti­
recorded in July and August (range 15.2 % ­ vity, which peaked in late August. In addition,
31.6 % of the herd) and the lowest in February. during the year previous to this study (April
In February, we observed only one newborn 1995 to March 1996) in one herd we observed
among all herds under observation. This new­ a few newborns each month, with peaks during
born disappeared within three weeks of birth. the months previously mentioned (unpublished
Actually the lowest numbers of newborns we­ data). This indicates that fertile matings may
re recorded in January, March and Apr il, with occur throughout the year. Apparently, howe­
zero observations of newborns. ver, not many pregnancies result from the ma­
Sex r atio: We rejected the null hypothesis ting events of July and August in the lowland
that in herds of white-lipped peccaries the pro­ rainforest of southern Costa Rica.
bability of observing a male or a female biased In other latitudes where there are diffe­
sex ratio among adults is equal. In eight out of rences in resource availability between dry
nine herds observed in this study the sex ratio and wet season (Texas: Bissonette 1982, Ari­
of adults was consistently female biase d (Bi­ zona: Day 1985, French Guyana: Henry
nomial test, n = 9 herds, p< 0.05, Table I). The 1994), collared peccaries give birth during the
mean adult sex ratio of four herds where all rainy season whe n food av ailability is highest.
adults were sexed by direct observation was The same pattern has been observed in the
1.8: 1. The mean adult sex ratio obtained from Chaco region for chacoan peccary (Sowls
pooled observations of peccary groups from 1997). In Corcovado National Par k, the rela­
six herds was 1.7:1, corresponding to 63% ± tionship between availabi lity of fruits consu­
2.4 females (n = 31 group observations). The med by pec caries and rainfall patterns is not
mean daily adult sex ratio of randomly selec­ simple. Fruit avai lability for peccaries peaks
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF TROPICAL BIOLOGY AND CONSERVATION 387

in July - September and January - March (AI­ Arizona and Texas (Sowls 1966, 1984, Low
trichter 1997). Therefore, litters conceived in 1970) females accounted for 53%-66% of the
the highest peak of mating (February) are born animals. Only two studies report 47% and
during months of high fruit availability. This ti­ 42.1% females among fetuses (Hellgren et al.
me of peak nursing activity is highly deman­ 1995, Gottdenker and Bodmer 1998, respecti­
ding of nutrients. If females would get preg­ vely). Sowls (1984, p. 82) suggests that sex ra­
nant from July and August matings, litters tio is biased in favor of females at birth and
would be born during months of low availabi­ shifts gradually toward males in older age co­
lity (November and December). As we were horts of hunted animals .
unable to find any herds in December, we do The sex ratio of adult white-lipped pecca­
not know whether births took place during that ries in our study was significantly female-bia­
month. No young were observed in January, sed. It ranged between 1.4:I and 1.8:I, depen­
however. Absence of young in January could ding on the methodology used. The mean sex
also result from high juvenile mortality due to ratio of four herds in which all adults were se­
food scarcity at the end of the year (Altrichter xed through direct observation was 1.8:1,
1997), or deaths in late November and Decem­ equal to the sex ratio of 25 adults of one herd
ber when peccaries travel outside the Corcova­ of white-lipped peccaries in the Amazon basin
do national park boundaries and are more expo­ reported by Fragoso (1994). Sex and age com­
sed to hunting (Altrichter 2000). position of individual collared peccary herds
Sex ratio: Published data thus far sugges­ reflect the population demography as a whole
ted that sex ratios at birth and during adulthood and do not result from behavioral subdivisions,
in peccaries are close to 1:1 (review in Sowls such as bachelor herd or harems in other ungu­
1997). In chacoan peccaries the adult sex ratio lates (Sow Is 1984). We assume that this infe­
of collected specimens and of 12 adults from fi­ rence applies to white-lipped peccaries as well,
ve herds was essentially 1:I (Mayer and Brandt for we have not commonly observed single
1982). Pooled data from five herds of collared males, any bachelor herds or obvious harems
peccaries in Arizona yielded a sex ratio of 1.0 in our study population. The factor determ i­
female to 0.65 males for all age classes combi­ ning this female-bias is unknown. Differential
ned (Mayer and Brandt 1982, calculated from exposure to hunting could account at least
data in Neal 1959 and Schweinsburg 1971).The partly for a higher mortality in male T. pecari.
sex ratios approach I:1 among older age co­ Hunting of peccaries persists in the periphery
horts, however (Sowls 1974). In the Arizona and within the national park boundary (AI­
population of collared peccaries the sex ratio of trichter 2000). We have observed that adult
large samples of collected specimens was close males tend to stay behind the herd, facing a th­
to 1:1 (Knipe 1957, Sowls 1966, Mayer and reatening situation, while females and young
Brandt 1982). In another study, average sex ra­ escape.
tio of II herds of collared peccaries was not dif­ Differential male mortality, however,
ferent from 1:1 (Bissonette 1982). One herd of seems common in mammals, including humans
seven collared peccaries from the Amazon basin (review in Trivers 1985, p.301-314), a trend
was composed by four adult females and three which results in female biased adult sex ratios.
adult males (Fragoso 1994). Strong differential male mortality is expected in
Nonetheless, reports of fetuses and new­ species where variation in male reproductive
born collared peccaries suggest a slight female success is large (e.g. Trivers 1985). We would
biased sex ratio during early development, alt­ thus predict that male-male competition over fe­
hough data in each study were not significantly males is strong in white-lipped peccaries and
different from 1:I (Sowls 1966, 1984, Low that a polygynous mating system prevails.
1970, Hellgren et al. 1995, Gottdenker and In conclusion, comparative studies of the
Bodmer 1998). In eight separate samples from socioecology of peccaries are necessary to
388 REVISTA DE BIOLOGfA TROPICAL

understand the differences in this group of y el pico de actividad de amam ant am iento fueron en j ulio
neotropical ungulates. Breeding seasonality y agosto, cuan do la dispo nibilidad de alimento era alta.
Entre adult os predominaron numeri carnente las hem bras
of white-lipped peccaries in a Central Ameri­ (1.4:1 - 1.8: I), 10 cual difiere de 10 conoci do sob re las po­
can lowland rainforest was found to be asso­ blaciones surame ricanas.
ciated with changes in fruit availability. The
association between high availability of food
and birth peaks is common to various popula­ REFERENCES
tions of peccaries in seasonal environments.
Peccaries from the Peruvian Amazon, howe­ Altr ichter, M . 1997. Estrategia de alimentaci6n y com por­
ver, are not seasonal breeders. Similarly, the tamiento del chanch o car iblanco Tayassu p ecari en
un bosque humed o trop ica l de Costa Rica. M. Sc.
female biased adult sex ratio of white-lipped
thesis, National University of Costa Rica, Heredia,
peccaries in the Costa Rican rainforest con­ Costa Rica.
trasts with the even sex ratios reported for co­
llared and Chacoan peccaries. Understanding Altricher, M . 2000. Importancia de los mamiferos silves­
the habitat differences within the distribution tres en la dieta de pobladores de la peninsula de Osa,
Costa Rica. Revi sta Mexican a de Mastozoologla, In
range of peccaries is likely to shed light on the
press.
differences in their reproductive patterns.
Altricher, M., J. Saenz, E. Carr illo & T. Fuller. 2000 . Die­
ta estaciona1 del Tayassu pecari (Art iodacthy la: Ta­
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS tassuid ae) en e1 Parqu e Nacional Corcovado, Costa
Rica . Rev. BioI. Trop. 48 : 689-702.

We thank Omar Laquis for his invaluable Bissonette, J. A. 1982. Ecology and social behavior of the co­
help in the field. Ricardo Ferrari and two llared peccary in Big Bend National Park, Texas. Nat.
anonymous referees reviewed and conside­ Park ServoSci. Monogr, 16, Washington, D.C. 85 p.
rably improved earlier version s of this manus­
cript. This research was supported by Wildlife Bodmer, R. E. 1989. Frugivory in amazon ungul ates. Ph.
D. thesis. University of Michigan, Michigan.
Conservation Society, World Wide Fund
(WWF) and the Regional Wildlife Manage­ Boinski, S. H. 1986. The eco logy of squ irrel monkeys in
ment Program of the National University of Costa Rica . Ph. D. thesis, University of Texas, Aus­
Costa Rica. We are grateful to the Ministry of tin, Texas.
the Environment and Energy of Costa Rica,
Campe ro, H. 1999. Variaci6n y estructura genetica dentro
which authorized our research in Corcovado
y entre gru pos de chanchos de monte Tayassu pe cari
National Park. en el Parque Nacion a1 Corcovado, Costa Rica. M.
Sc. thesis, National University of Costa Rica, Here­
dia, Cos ta Rica.
RESUMEN
Day, G. I. 1985. Javelina, research and management in Ari­
Los "chanchos carib Iancos" Tayassu pecari no so n zona. Ar izona Game and Fish Department. Phoenix,
reproductores estacionales en Sur Ame rica. En Centro Arizona. 122 p.
America se conoce muy poco sobre la reproducci6n de es­
ta especie en la naturaleza. Estudiamos reproducci 6n y la Emmons, L. H. 1984. Geographic variation in densities
proporci6n de sexos durante 200 horns de observaci6n de and diversities of non-fl ying mamm als in Amazon ia.
cuatro manad as marcadas con radioco llares y de dos ma­ Biotrop ica 16: 2 10-222.
nadas sin marcar, desde j ulio de 1996 hasta abril de 1997
en el Parque Nacional Corcovado, Costa Rica, agregando Fragoso, J. M. 1994. Large mammals and the community
datos de prop orci6n sexual de tres man adas observad as e n of an Am azonian rain forest. Ph. D. thesis , Un iver­
1998. Registram os el numero de c6pul as y de amamant a­ sity of Florid a, Flor ida.
mientos, asi como la presencia de recien nacidos en las
manadas . O bservamos una marcada estacionalidad en fa Fragoso, J. M. 1998. Home range and movement patterns
reproduccion, con un pico en el numer o de c6pulas en fe­ of whit e-lippe d peccary (Tayas su pecari) herds in the
brero y otro en j ulio. La mayor cantid ad de recien nacidos Northern Brazilian Am azon. Biotropi ca 30: 458-46 9.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF TROPICAL BIOLOGY AND CONSERVATION 389

Garcia, R. V. 1997. Biologia de la conservaci6n y areas sil­ blication Series Pymatuning Laboratory of Ecology.
vestres protegidas: situaci6n actual y perspectivas en University of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania.
Costa Rica. Institute NacionaJ de Biodiversidad. He­
redia, Costa Rica. 64 p Miller, F. N. 1930. Notes on some mammals of southern
Matte Grosso, Brazil. J. Mamm. II :18.
Gottdenker, N. & R. E. Bodmer. 1998. Reproduction and
productivity of white-lipped and collared peccaries in Naranjo, E. P. J 994. Abundancia, uso de habitat y habitos
the Peruvian Amazon. J. Zool. Lond. 245: 423-430. de alimentaci6n del tapit Tapirus bairdii En un bos­
que tropical humedo en Costa Rica. M. Sc. thesis, Na­
Hartshorn, G. S. 1983. Plants. p. 11 8-157. In D. H. Janzen tional University of Costa Rica, Heredia, Costa Rica.
(ed.). Costa Rican natural history. The University of
Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, Neal, B. 1. 1959. A contribution on the life history of the co­
llared peccary in Arizona. Am. MidI. Nat. 6 1: 177-190.
Hellgren, E. c., D. R. Synatzke, P. W. Oldenberg & F. S.
Guthery. 1995. Demography of a collared peccary po­ Roots, C. G. 1966. Notes on the breeding of the white-lipped
pulation in South Texas. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 59: 153-163. peccaries at Dudley Zoo. Int. Zoo Ybk. 6: 198-625.

Henry, O. 1994. Saisons de reproduction ehez trois Ron­ Oliver, W. L. R. 1990. Pigs and peccaries group report.
geurs et un Artiodactyle en Guyane Francaise, en Species Newsletter of IUCN/SSC No. 13-14: 80-82.
fonction des facteurs du milieu et de I'alimentation.
Mammalia 58: 183-200. Schweinsburg, R. E. 1971. The home range, movement
and herd integrity of the co llared peccary in Southern
Holdridge, L. 1967. Life Zone Ecology. Tropical Science Arizona. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 35: 433-460.
Center, San Jose, Costa Rica. 206 p.
SowIs, L. K. 1966. Reproduction in the collared peccary
Janzen, D. H. 1983. Costa Rican natural history. The Uni­ (Tayassu tajacui . p 155-172 In I. W. Rowlands (cd.).
versity of Chicago, Chicago, !llinois. 816 p. Comparative biology of reproduction in mammals.
Zoo!' Soc. London.
Kiltie, R. A. j 981. Stomach contents of rain forest peccaries
tTayassu tajacu and T. pecari) . Biotropica 13: 235-236. Sowls, L. K. 1974. Social behavior of the collared peccary,
DicotyIes taj acu L. p. 144-165. In V. Geist & F. Walt­
Knipe, T. 1957. Thejavelina in Arizona. Wildl. Bull. No 2. her (eds.). The behaviour of ungulates and its relation
Ariz. Game and Fish Department. 96 p. to management. IUCN. Morges, Switzerland.

Leopold, A. S. 1959. Wildlife of Mexico: the game birds Sowls, L. K. 1984. The Peccaries. The University of Ari­
and mammals. University of California Press, Los zona, Tucson, Arizona. 251 p.
Angeles, California. 568 p.
Sowls.L. K. 1997. Javelinas and other peccaries: their bio­
Low, W. A. 1970. The influence of aridity on reproduction logy, management and usc. Texas A&M University,
in the collared peccary Dicotyles taj acu (Linn.) in College Station, Texas. 324 p
Texas. Ph. D. thesis, University of British Columb ia,
Vancouver. Trivers, R. 1985. Social Evolution. Benj amin Cummings,
Menlo Park, California. 462 p.
Mayer, J. J.& P. N. Brandt. 1982. Identity, distribution and
natural history of the peccaries, Tayassuidae, p. 433­ Vaughan, C. 1981. Parque Naciona l Corcovado. Plan de
455 . In Mares, M. A.& H. H. Genoways (eds.). manejo y desarrollo. Universidad Nacional. San Jo­
Mammalian biology in South America. Special Pu­ se, Costa Rica. 364 p.

You might also like