You are on page 1of 361

Ex

am

Co

py

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Identity and Agency


in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

Dorothy Holland
William Lachicotte Jr.
Debra Skinner
Carole Cain

H A R VA R D U N I V E R S I T Y P R E S S
Cambridge, Massachusetts
London, England
1998

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Copyright 1998 by the President and Fellows of Harvard College


All rights reserved
Printed in the United States of America
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Ex

am

Co

Identity and agency in cultural worlds / Dorothy Holland . . . [et al.].


p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 0-674-81566-1 (alk. paper)
1. Culture. 2. Identity (Psychology) 3. Agent (Philosophy)
I. Holland, Dorothy C.
HM101.I28935 1998
306dc21
98-19269

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Contents

vii

Preface

III

IV

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

19

PLACING IDENTITY AND AGENCY

49

Figured Worlds

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

66

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

98

POWER AND PRIVILEGE


6

Positional Identities

125

The Sexual Auction Block

144

THE SPACE OF AUTHORING


8

Authoring Selves

169

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

192

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

214

10

MAKING WORLDS
11

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources 235

12

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

253

13

Identity in Practice

270

Ex

Co

II

ON THE SHOULDERS OF BAKHTIN AND VYGOTSKY

am

Notes

289

References

317

Credits

339

Index

341

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Preface

In Marxism and Literature Raymond Williams says:

Co

The strongest barrier to the recognition of human cultural activity is


this immediate and regular conversion of experience into nished
products. [. . . It is misleading to habitually project endings] not only
into the always moving substance of the past, but into contemporary
life, in which relationships, institutions and formations in which we
are still actively involved are converted, by this procedural mode,
into formed wholes rather than forming and formative processes. (Williams 1977: 128129; quoted in Harvey 1996: 2425; interpolation by
Harvey)

Ex

am

Williamss admonition is important for cultural studies of the person


and for our focus on identity. Identitiesif they are alive, if they are
being livedare unnished and in process. Whether they be specic to
the imagined worlds of romance or the careers of mental illness, or
generic to ethnic, gender, race, and class divisions, identities never arrive in persons or in their immediate social milieux already formed. They
do not come into being, take hold in lives, or remain vibrant without
considerable social work in and for the person. They happen in social
practice.
Cultural studies of the person, in particular, need to move more solidly to process. They must be predicated upon continuing cultural production: a development, or interlocking genesis, that is actually a codevelopment of identities, discourses, embodiments, and imagined
worlds that inform each moment of joint production and are themselves
transformed by that moment. It is this processual understanding of identity and agency that we seek to build, through concepts offered by two
scholars of society and the person, whose work began in the aftermath of
the Soviet Revolution of 1917, L. S. Vygotsky and M. M. Bakhtin.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

viii

Preface

Ex

am

Co

py

Although not, as far as we know, directly intertwined, the intellectual


lives of Bakhtin and Vygotsky were inuenced by the same currents of
thought in 1920s Russia. The two men addressed the common questions
and objectives that characterized the great ferment of social theory about
the new society and its inhabitants, the new socialist man. Each had
a thoroughly sociocentric view of human thought and feeling, and both
saw speech, language, literature, and art as the pivotal media through
which consciousness and subjectivity develop. Bakhtin relied upon some
of the same concepts as Vygotsky did, such as the important notion of
inner speech, but Bakhtins center of gravity remained in the areas of
sociolinguistics and literary criticism, while Vygotsky turned to the task
of building a sociohistorical school of psychology. Bakhtins search was
less for a social science of what we will call heuristic developmentthe
contingent formations of subjectivity over timeand more for the social
grounds of personal creativity and authority. Vygotsky and Bakhtin
together articulated a powerful version of human life as necessarily mediated: as produced by social interchange among persons whose activity,
however circumscribed by material and social circumstances, and however cast in forms of discursive and practical genres, nonetheless remakes these conditions. Theirs is a social world that is necessarily personal, not automatic; a world of agents and of hope carried forward from
the Revolution. It still has much to say to us today.
Williamss admonition against supposed endings reminds us of our
manuscript. This book has been in process for a long time, and the
intellectual projects it reects are certainly not nished. Carole Cain,
Dorothy Holland, Rene Prillaman, and Debra Skinner started talking
about the subject matter more than ten years ago. Because of work and
family obligations, Rene Prillaman had to drop out of the project, but
the book proted from her ideas. Meanwhile, William Lachicotte joined
our discussions and writing.
The plan for the book has remained fairly constant. We wanted to
develop our ideas about identity collectively through our discussions of
other scholars writings and through our own research projects, which
we undertook individually or in pairs. The result is a book that sometimes plays havoc with current notions of authorship, just as our view of
subjectivity must surely grate against common ideas of identity and personhood.
If authorship of a collective venture is hard to square with individual-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Preface

ix

Ex

am

Co

py

istic notions of cultural production, so too are our acknowledgments. A


multitude of people have contributed to our eld research, among them
the residents of the area in Nepal where Holland and Skinner lived and
worked, the members of the Alcoholics Anonymous groups attended
by Cain (especially those we call Hank, Ellen, and Andrew), the clients
and staff members of mental health institutions who gave their time and
assistance to Lachicotte, and the students of the two universities where
Holland learned about the gured world of romance. A host of readers,
listeners, and partners in discussion have also been crucial to the development of our ideas. We cannot name them all. Our solution is to thank
the following people, who have signicantly aided one or more of us:
G. B. Adhikari, Richard Ashmore, Alan Benjamin, Katherine BuntingHowarth, Roy DAndrade, Robert Daniels, Mary Des Chene, Margaret
Eisenhart, Glen Elder, Sue Estroff, Terry Evens, Judith Farquhar, Alison
Greene, Marilyn Grunkemeyer, Erin Hannan, Jean Harris, Peter Hervik,
Lee Jussim, Willett Kempton, Jean Lave, Bradley Levinson, Robert Levy,
Tanya Luhrmann, Donald Nonini, Alfred Pach III, Maheswor Pahari,
Steven Parish, Jim Peacock, John Peterson, James Poling, Laurie Price,
James Reeves, Bob S., Sapana Sharma, Claudia Strauss, Renu Lama
Thapa, Julia Thompson, Jaan Valsiner, Joanne Waghorne, and several
anonymous reviewers.
Funders for our research projects were crucial as well: the National
Science Foundation (BNS-9110010), the Fulbright Foundation, the National Institutes of Mental Health (Grant RO1 MH40314), the National
Institute of Education, the University Research Council of the University
of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and the University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill Dissertation Fellowships. Thanks also to the Nepal Ofces of
the United States Educational Foundation and Save the Children, U.S.A.
We dedicate this book to some special people whose voices we
brought with us to our task: Ashby Gaines Holland; William S. Lachicotte Sr.; Willard Skinner; Mack and Delores Cain. They are part-authors
of this story and shape it still in ways not even we can fully tell.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Ex

am

Co

py

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Ex

am

Co

py

On the Shoulders of
Bakhtin and Vygotsky

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

am

People tell others who they are, but even more important, they tell
themselves and then try to act as though they are who they say they are.
These self-understandings, especially those with strong emotional resonance for the teller, are what we refer to as identities. A man interviewed
by Lachicotte, Roger Kelly,1 diagnosed as having a mental disorder, struggled with who to be: Im only 35. I mean I wikind of wish I was 60,
and I was done with all . . . It would make things simpler, you know, Im
disabled. Ill just live out my last few years and do a little good works and
try to enjoy my day. But when youre only 35 theres pressure to get back
in there and try it again. Ive tried four or ve times. Ive been up in the
hospital ve times now. How many more times do I have to try?
Tika Damai, a girl of fteen in a community in central Nepal, when
asked by Skinner to tell about herself and her life, answered by singing a
series of folksongs, which began:

Ex

Parents have earned a little money,


But my life will be spent in cutting grass and wood only.
Daughters are married off, crossing nine great mountains,
But the sons are always kept at home.
Mother, am I not your daughter,
Am I not your daughter?
Brothers are given wealth and property, but nothing is given to
me. (Skinner, Valsiner, and Basnet 1991: 1920)

Although from widely divergent cultural worlds, Tika Damai and


Roger Kelly were engaged in similar identity-making processes. They
3
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

were producing, from the cultural resources available to them, understandings of themselves that seemed to be not only of (about) themselves, representing the dilemmas of their respective social situations,
but also for themselves. These productions gured in their communication with themselves about their past and present actions. We picture
Roger, Tika, ourselves, and other individuals and groups as always engaged in forming identities, in producing objectications of self-understandings that may guide subsequent behavior. This vision emphasizes
that identities are improvisedin the ow of activity within specic
social situationsfrom the cultural resources at hand. Thus persons
and, to a lesser extent, groups are caught in the tensions between past
histories that have settled in them and the present discourses and images
that attract them or somehow impinge upon them. In this continuous
self-fashioning, identities are hard-won standpoints that, however dependent upon social support and however vulnerable to change, make at
least a modicum of self-direction possible. They are possibilities for mediating agency.

Identity and Its Theories

Ex

am

Identity is a concept that invokes and relates theories from various


streams of psychology, social psychology, anthropology, sociology, and
now from such interdisciplinary elds as cultural studies. Our own
take on this concept is at heart an anthropological and cultural studies
adaptation of sociogenic concepts of personhood developed within
the American school of social psychology that claims G. H. Mead as its
founder.
Mead began with humans instinctual ability to coordinate their actions and traced the career of this coordination through the persons
engagement in social life. Self-consciousness and self-reection develop
in the active child as the product of a social history. The person acquires
the ability to take the standpoint of others as she learns to objectify
herself by the qualities of her performance in and commitment to various
social positions (for example, in the United States, mother, activist, black
woman, Marxist anthropologist, student, environmentalist, public servant, good person).2 Such objectications, especially those to which
one is strongly emotionally attached, become cores of ones proactive
identities.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

Ex

am

Co

py

Identity is a concept that guratively combines the intimate or personal world with the collective space of cultural forms and social relations. We are interested in identities, the imaginings of self in worlds of
action, as social products; indeed, we begin with the premise that identities are lived in and through activity and so must be conceptualized as
they develop in social practice. But we are also interested in identities as
psychohistorical formations that develop over a persons lifetime, populating intimate terrain and motivating social life. Identities are a key
means through which people care about and care for what is going on
around them. They are important bases from which people create new
activities, new worlds, and new ways of being. In some chapters we will
pay great attention to the ways in which persons are malleable, changeable, and subject to discursive powers. In others we will show more
respect for persons generativity, their capacitiesembedded always in
collective meanings and social relationsto imagine and create new
ways of being. Human agency may be frail, especially among those with
little power, but it happens daily and mundanely, and it deserves our
attention. Humans capacity for self-objecticationand, through objectication, for self-directionplays into both their domination by social
relations of power and their possibilities for (partial) liberation from
these forces.
The metatheoretical shift to practice, coupled with what might be
called the critical disruption in the social sciences, the humanities, and
some areas of psychology, has cleared a space for new understandings of
persons. Cultural studies, joined to feminist theorys engagement with
issues of identity, invite us to see persons taking form in the ow of
historically, socially, culturally, and materially shaped lives.3 At the same
time, a more sociogenic approach to personhood has developed. The
task we undertake here is inspired not only by contemporary cultural
studies but also by the intellectual tradition that includes the great Soviet
psychologist L. S. Vygotsky, his students, and other members of what has
come to be called the cultural-historical or sociohistorical school of psychology. Equally important to our tradition is another gure from the
same period and place, the critical theorist and semiotician M. M. Bakhtin. To comprehend and articulate a vision of both person and society
true to the parts played by cultural forms, the machinery of power and
social positioning, and the continual processes of identication, we join
the work of Bakhtin, Vygotsky, and their colleagues to contemporary

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

debates (a link that, especially in the case of Vygotsky, has been made
before; see, for example, Gergen 1994; van der Veer and Valsiner 1991;
Wertsch 1991).
Bakhtins work was inspired by a neo-Kantian tradition of philosophy
that emphasized aesthetics.4 But Bakhtin did not rest content with this
difcult kind of idealism. He took his work in two directions, both of
which were more concerned with specic practices and persons than
with the generic practice of abstract (indeed quasi-transcendental) subjects. First, he studied literature and verbal art, looking most closely at
the means for representing characters and (therefore) authors. Second,
he began, with V. N. Voloshinov, a study of the sociology of human
expression, which also became a social psychology. Their works of 1929,
Marxism and the Philosophy of Language and Problems of Dostoevskys
Poetics, intertwine these two lines to create an understanding of human
expression that seems more at home in the cultural studies of today than
in either the social science or the literary studies of the time.
Vygotsky came to his study of persons from interests in literature, art,
and linguistics. His 1925 dissertation, The Psychology of Art, included an
analysis of a short story, Bunins Gentle Breath. His fascination with the
way the closing image of the story transformed the whole feeling of the
piece was part of his more general engagement with the potential of
symbols to affect and reorganize experience. He carried this fascination
with symbols into his psychology and organized his later studies around
it. He construed symbols, particularly cultural symbols learned through
social interaction, as so many ways in which people free themselves from
the tyranny of environmental stimuli. But this work of freedom, for
Vygotsky, demanded an appreciation of time and timing. He took a genetic or developmental approach because, in his handling, it was
difcult, if not impossible, to understand a persons behavior without
knowing the opportunistic history of its formation.
Bakhtins concern with the social weighing of expression and the creative life of association, combined with Vygotskys emphases on historical
development and on the potentiality of symbols for (re)formation, affords a means by which cultural studies of the person may avoid a common conceptual dilemmaone that traps persons permanently, either in
cultural logics or in subject positions or in some combination of the
two. This is our objective here: to respect humans as social and cultural
creatures and therefore bounded, yet to recognize the processes whereby

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

Ex

am

Co

py

human collectives and individuals often move themselvesled by hope,


desperation, or even playfulness, but certainly by no rational planfrom
one set of socially and culturally formed subjectivities to another.
Our view of identity and human agency differs in some important
ways from two interrelated and inuential streams of work that have also
treated these issues. When anthropologists and other contributors to
cultural studies of the person write about identities they are usually
concerned with cultural identities, identities that form in relation to
major structural features of society: ethnicity, gender, race, nationality,
and sexual orientation. Our concept of identity is both broader and more
particular. We focus on the development of identities and agency specic
to practices and activities situated in historically contingent, socially
enacted, culturally constructed worlds: recognized elds or frames of
social life, such as romance, mental illness and its treatment, domestic
relations, Alcoholics Anonymous, academia, and local politics.
Identications of people within these worlds are often, if not always,
structurally marked. That is, they may be divided by gender, race, ethnicity, or any other durable structural feature of the society. We address the
structural aspects of world-specic identities in Chapter 6. There are also
culturally constructed worlds where woman or lesbian or white
woman is a salient identity in and of itself (for example, some worlds of
feminist debate) or where wife or daughter or daughter-in-law is
practically coterminous with female as a general status. Chapters 10 and
12, on Nepal, address the latter case. Still, we are always interested in the
grounding of cultural identities in the specic worlds of which they are
a part.
Cultural studies of the personincluding a signicant proportion of
the anthropological research on (cultural) identityshare an opposition
to a general Western notion of identity that takes as its prototype a
coherent, unied, and originary subject (see, for an extreme example,
Stuart Hall 1996). The projects of these newer studies are quite unlike
that of Erik Erikson (1963, 1968), for example, with its focus on the
tasks any person must successfully accomplish to resolve the psychodynamic dilemmas of maturation and thus establish a consistent identity.
Instead the newer studies rely upon feminist and later psychodynamic
approaches, both of which recognize those social forces which make
such an integrated subject an extremely unlikely occurrence.
At this point we differ. It is not that we have an inclination to the idea

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

of a unied subject; we conceive persons as composites of many, often


contradictory, self-understandings and identities, whose loci are often
not conned to the body but spread over the material and social environment, and few of which are completely durable. We also concur with
the emphasis on cultural forms and the inevitable importance of socially
distributed power. But we take a different path to theorizing identities
and processes of identication. Though we share an interest in many of
the same issues and a critical focus on the importance of cultural forms
in the production of subjectivities, we develop different aspects of the
relation between society and subjectivity. We do not attend to the psychodynamic processes that may lead to the identication of self within
the structural inscriptions of gender, race, and so forth. Rather we build
upon notions of inner activity and inner life, drawn from Bakhtin and the
sociohistorical school, that cast the development of self-understandings
(identities) on intimate terrain as an outcome of living in, through, and
around the cultural forms practiced in social life. Bakhtin and Vygotsky
give us a means of theorizing history-in-person that allows us to conceive of identities as always forming, but that differs from history-in-person conceived primarily through psychodynamics (see Chapter 2).
The case studies we employ to trace out these processes include culturally constructed worlds in the United States: the world of Alcoholics
Anonymous, the world of romance and attractiveness, and the world of
mental health care. They also cross what are usually considered more
profound cultural boundaries into the constructed worlds of domestic
and political relations in Nepal. How do these culturally constructed
worlds become matters of desire? How do peoples senses of themselves
in these worlds become engaging? How do persons become agents in
these worlds? How do these worlds become prisons? How do people
together create new cultural worlds? How do their subjectivities expand
or reform so that they are able to inhabit new worlds? These are the sorts
of questions that we try to answer through alternating expositions of
concepts and the ethnographic details of the case studies.
Our frame is what might be called, following Bakhtin, a dialogic one;
following Vygotsky, a developmental one. We aim to build upon and
move beyond two central approachesthe culturalist and the constructivistto understand peoples actions and possibilities. All the perspectives we discuss assume, at least implicitly, that behavior is mediated by
senses of self or what we call identities.5 In the older of the two ap-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

Two Perspectives on Identity

py

proaches that we work to transcend, the person is driven by an internalized cultural logic; in the more recent, by social situation. In order to
communicate what we nd important about these two approaches, we
simplify them to two ideal types.

Ex

am

Co

An incident told from Dorothy Hollands perspective can be used to


exemplify the two approaches.6 The incident happened in Naudada, a
rural hill community in central Nepal:
Debra Skinner and I were interviewing people for a small research
project. Debra had been carrying out ethnographic eldwork in the community for about a year and a half. I had recently joined her.
Naudada was occupied by people of different jat (caste/ethnic
groups). Since we wanted to interview people from a range of jat, we
had asked members of the various castes, from the highest (Bahun) to
the lowest (Damai), to come to Debras house to be interviewed.
In Naudada, persons of lower caste were usually prohibited from entering the houses of those of higher caste. Food and cooking were considered especially vulnerable to pollution, and, since the only entrance
to most houses was on the rst oor, where the hearths were, people
of lower castes kept away. Under normal village circumstances, then,
lower-caste people would have expected to be barred from Debras house.
She was accorded, in most contexts, a status analogous to that of a
higher-caste person, and, in addition, her Bahun landlord lived on the
terrace above her, practically next door. Debra, however, throughout her
residence in Naudada, had insisted that lower-caste people enter her
house to talk. People all around had come to know of her unusual
practice.
One day we were interviewing on the second-oor balcony of the
three-story house. We were in the process of interviewing a woman of
the Sunar castehistorically associated with the occupation of
goldsmithing and considered untouchable. The woman seemed uncomfortable. We guessed that her discomfort was due to the proximity of
the next person to be interviewed, a Chetri (higher-caste) woman, who
was sitting around the corner of the balcony.
About that time another woman arrived at the house to be interviewed. The new arrival, Gyanumaya, a woman in her late fties, was

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

10

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

also of the Sunar caste. Debra called down to Gyanumaya that we wanted
to talk to her on the balcony, and went down to greet her, intending to
bring her through the kitchen and up the stairs. However, as Debra was
on her way downstairs, Gyanumaya took a different route: she scaled the
outside of the house. She somehow crawled up the vertical outside wall,
made her way around the balcony to an opening in the railing, came
through the opening, and sat down. Gyanumaya knew that Debra did
not prohibit lower-caste people from entering her kitchen, but she found
a way to get to the place of our interview without going through the
house.
I later questioned Debra about her knowledge of the community. Did
she know of other people who scaled the outside of houses to get to the
second oor? Was Gyanumaya in particular known to enter houses in
this way? As far as we could discern, no community members were
accustomed to getting to the second stories of houses by climbing up the
walls. Nor did Gyanumaya have a reputation for whimsical or unusual
behavior.
How Gyanumaya herself interpreted her climb we do not know. We
did not ask her to explain her action. For me, her climb became an icon
of limitationan example to use in writing, in talks, and in teaching to
point out the limitations of culturalist approaches (see, for example,
Holland 1992b). Here the story lends itself to both a culturalist and an
opposing constructivist interpretation. Gyanumaya had undeniably
climbed above a culturalist approach, but her actions raise issues about
the constructivist one as well.
After I had said to myself, Yes, she really did climb up the house! my
next thought was about caste. I had an image of caste identication,
ingrained in the Sunar woman, propelling her away from the sin of
polluting anothers hearth, to the extreme that sent her up the wall. I
tried in vain to think of a culturally given sense of myself that could have
moved me to such a physical feat. My image, rapidly conceived in reaction to a startling event, provides a good caricature of the culturalist
approach. Culturethe cultural signicance of caste and pollution
learned from childhoodwas an essential force inside Gyanumaya, directing her behavior.
According to this rst interpretation, Gyanumayas climb lent dramatic corroboration to a culturalist theory and justied careful and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

11

Ex

am

Co

py

painstaking attention to all of the details of caste, pollution, sin, dharma,


and other related concepts. It demonstrated the potency of cultural
meaning systems. My attention, for a time, was directed toward a
deeper understanding of caste. Only later did other aspects of the situation begin to intrude on my attempts at culturalist analysis, accumulating as contradictory evidence, until the incident slowly became for me a
reminder of lacunae in the culturalist approach.7
I remembered, for instance, that Debra and I had attributed the discomfort of our other Sunar interviewee to the presence of the Chetri
woman. We had also wondered, in retrospect, if Gyanumaya had somehow been reminded of the close proximity of the Bahun landlords house.
This was another way to understand Gyanumayas action. Perhaps the
presence and proximity of these higher-caste persons evoked an implicit
discourse of caste that held sway in many contexts of life in Naudada, a
discourse about relations between members of different castes. In avoiding the kitchen Gyanumaya conformed to the positioning that the discourse of caste imposed upon her and the other Naudadan participants
in the situation. Had the social situation been differenthad Debra been
there alone, for examplethen perhaps Gyanumaya would not have
been obliged to take up her caste position; rather she would have complied with Debras wishes, entering through the kitchen and using the
stairs. But as the situation stood, Gyanumaya was liable to construal, and
subordination, by the discourses of caste that were hegemonic there and
then.8 Her behavior was not an index of the cultural impetus of a caste
identity embodied in her, but of the social signicance of a caste identity
imposed upon her. She was propelled up the house, not by cultural tenets
that long ago had formed her outlook on hearths, food, and pollution,
but by the social forces that determined whether the discourse of caste
would be salient in that particular situation.
The constructivist position emphasizes the social positioning that
goes on whenever people interact. Constructivists extend to all behavior
the point that sociolinguists and ethnographers of communication have
made about linguistic behavior. No matter what one person says to another, there is always more to the message than its semantic content.
How the message is saidthrough accent, tone, or tempo, what language or dialect it is said in, what style (formal or informal), what mode
(whether phrased as a question or command)all these index the rela-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

12

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

tionship among speaker, addressee, and audience and constitute signs of


the speakers claim to social position.
Irvine (1980) is one of many who makes this point. Her elegant example addresses the social meaning of asking a favor in Wolof. One day
early in her stay in Senegal, she asked a woman, very politely, to make
room for her on a bench. Taking pity on her ignorance of sociolinguistic
convention, a bystander pointed out that she should not put such small
requests in such polite form. In fact, the request should not even be
phrased as a question (Would you please do x?) but as an imperative
(Do x). Her request was more than a simple indication of need. It was
also an indication, inexplicit to be sure, of her status relative to that of
the other woman. And, Irvine later learned, she made three mistakes in
the space of one short utterance, twice claiming a lower status than she
merited and once a higher status. To give another example, Lutz (1987)
argues that Ifaluk attributions of emotion were not just about feelings,
but also about social position. To attribute fago (glossed in English as
compassion/love/sadness) to another or oneself was to make a claim of
high social position. Only a person of accomplishment, status, or wealth
could have cultivated the sort of sensitivities, and carried through the
kinds of action, that fago required.9
Gal (1987) and Irvine (1985) point out that studies of language use
show situation to be extremely important; people usually command a
range of alternative ways of speaking, which they vary in response to
social situations. It is not that American women, for example, use hedges
and other linguistic markers of deference automatically, as though they
were characteristically deferential. Both women and men use deferential
markers when and only when they are in situations that demand their
deference (OBarr and Atkins 1980).10
Social constructivists urge the same conclusion with respect to other
behavior. For Hindu Nepalis, approaching the hearth of someone whose
caste is higher than ones ownno matter whether it is an act signicant
in a moral worldis a sign of and a claim about social position. In
Gyanumayas case, entering through the kitchen could be construed as
claiming a higher-caste status than she was known to have. She avoided
such a claim by avoiding the kitchen. According to constructivists, Gyanumaya and others from Nepal, like everyone else, are always propelled
by their subject position within the social situation in which they nd
themselves.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

13

Cultural Logic or Subject Position?

Ex

am

Co

py

It is possible to think of linguistic behavior from a culturalist position,


explaining how one speaks (for example, saying please when asking
for favors) as reecting precepts learned in childhood about conducting
(and thus identifying) oneself as a moral person. One strives to say
whatever upholds the culturally constructed, moral world. A constructivist approach, in contrast, would emphasize that linguistic behavior
depends upon maneuverings, negotiations, impositions, and recreations
of relations of status and entitlement. Saying or not saying please
makes a social claim about ones status or relationship vis--vis ones
addressee. It is as though a speaker is always of two minds, attending to
two consuming concerns. The culturalist speaker seeks to conduct herself so as to do right by a preconstituted, culturally given, and moral
world. The constructivist speaker responds instead to the social claims
implied by the utterance, making and sustaining the claims that the
particular situation allows.
Bourdieu (1977a), for one, has shown that the two perspectives we
have considered cannot be collapsed into each other. Culturalists might
be tempted to claim that they can encompass the constructivist position.
They might say that, as a child, Gyanumaya learned a context-sensitive
cultural principle: in the presence of higher-caste persons, ideas about
the hearth and pollution are important; otherwise, they are not. But this
argument by context rule turns out to be unending, impossible. We
know from our ethnographic research in Naudada that certain caste-related restrictions are contested and often not followed in the local
schools. Does this mean we should add another aspect of context sensitivity: when in school ignore the former context-sensitive rule? We know
that Debra, who was neither within nor without the caste system, invited
people of lower caste into her kitchen. Had the lower-caste people who
came into her kitchen really learned context-sensitive rules to take care
of such an eventuality? Bourdieu explains that context-sensitive rules are
impossible because situations always arise that do not t the rules. It is
simpler to admit the possibility that different discourses can be
hegemonic in different locales, an admission that obviates the need for a
myriad of context-sensitive rules.
Once culturalists admit that people use culturally meaningful behavior to bring about contested social ends, they have stepped outside their

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

14

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

own position. Disagreements over (and even recognitions of) a persons


social position, relative status, and relationships belie the possibility that
the social uses of culture can be totally dictated by cultural logics. Instead differences are resolved by other means. The constructivists suggest that power makes a difference as to whose version will prevail.
Yet the culturalist position cannot be totally rejected, as it might be by
a radical constructivist.11 Clearly there is more to saying please in
Wolof than one more way of indicating ones social position. No doubt
for Gyanumaya there was more to her climb than simply indicating to
the Chetri woman that she knew her place. Culturally constructed moral
worlds, aside and apart from how their discourses can be used to position others, have a persuasion of their own.
In a familiar optical illusion, two faces, silhouetted in prole, look at
each other across a small space. But looking again we see the space in the
middle as not-space, and the lines of the faces disappear into the lines of
a vase. The extreme culturalist and constructivist perspectives on behavior are also a sort of interpretive illusion. In the extreme culturalist
vision, Gyanumaya, the person, is a bearer of culturehistorical
events and conditions that have been distilled through group processes
into culture and passed somehow into her mind/body. In the radical
constructivist vision, Gyanumaya is an instrument and an outcome of
the situation, a person pushed rst into one subject position (when
Debras greetings and invitations positioned her as a person whom Debra
welcomed into her house) and then into another (when the higher-caste
woman saw Gyanumaya, and, perhaps through posture or movements,
communicated to Gyanumaya that she saw her as a bearer of pollution).
Looked at from the culturalist position, behavior follows cultural principles; it falls in line with heartfelt moral precepts that transcend the actual
people with whom one interacts and the actual situations surrounding
those interactions. From the constructivist position, behavior instead is
the acting out (or refusal) of subject positions; it is pushed into line by
relations of power and inuence that obtain in the venues where, and
among the particular people with whom, one interacts.
What propelled Gyanumaya, then? Must we choose one perspective or
the other? Are we doomed to oscillate between the two visions of what
we all know to be a continuous eld? The answer, we think, is clearly
no. These perspectives can be separated, and indeed we have found it
immensely helpful to recognize dimensions missing from the culturalist

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

15

Co

py

perspective and offered by the constructivist one, and vice versa. But
there is no reason to pretend that they are so separate in action. Gyanumaya was no doubt swayed, as most of us are in any situation, by multiple and mixed thoughts and feelings. Dialogic perspectives, such as Bakhtins (1981), explicitly free us from the idea that we as a group or as
individuals can hold only one perspective at a time. Humans are both
blessed and cursed by their dialogic naturetheir tendency to encompass a number of views in virtual simultaneity and tension, regardless of
their logical compatibility. Our own perspective puts together the culturalist and the constructivist positions in a dialogic frame.

Improvisation and Heuristic Development

Ex

am

Gyanumayas climb can also introduce another parameter of our dialogic


position. It brings into focus the iconic potential of both the behavior
and the artifacts that are produced in interaction, and the roles these
symbols may play in changing or preserving identity and subjectivity.
Again, the move to such a perspective depends upon entertaining the
wisdom of both the culturalist and the constructivist.
As the vision of Gyanumaya being propelled up the house by the
cultural principles of caste identity faded, I (Holland) began to marvel at
the improvisational nature of her entry onto the balcony. Gyanumaya
was, as far as we could learn, not accustomed to making her way to the
second stories of houses by scaling exterior walls. Instead she had come
up with a spectacular improvisation in the face of a problematic situation. The house belonged, in her eyes, to an upper-caste person, and
thusat least in the usual circumstances of community lifeshe would
not be allowed to enter. Yet she needed to get to the second-oor balcony.
She devised the solution of climbing up the outside of the house.
My original training as an anthropologist did not direct me to attend
to such improvisation. Rather I was taught to attend to culture, to common conventions, to shared organizing themes. Even though ethnographic studies invariably noted variations and sometimes marked dissent, the job of the anthropologist was to nd the culture. One of my rst
experiences with eldwork took place in Trinidad, where I went in 1967
to study the religious practices referred to as Shango. At rst, as I attended Shango ceremonies around the island and talked to leaders and
participants, I was greatly impressed by the differences among the cere-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

16

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

monies and the ideas and practices of the leaders. Within weeks I was
mired in a crisis of condence. I had been taught that people followed
cultural patterns. Where were the obedient pattern-followers? I even
began to doubt that anyone ever coordinated his or her behavior with
anyone else. If the newspaper reported that two people had engaged in
the same activity, I was skeptical.
But I continued to attend Shango feasts. After several more weeks I
calmed down and concentrated on the commonalties I had started to
notice across the Shango practitioners. I noted the conventionalized emphases and understandings that threaded their way through the cacophony of differences. I was at last a successful anthropologist: I had found
the culture that was there. Other truthsa great deal of improvisation,
provoked by a great variety of social and material conditionsbecame
evident (see Holland and Crane 1987). The connections of the forms
created by the participants to postcolonial relations, to the location of the
particular groups in relation to urban centers, and to the position of
Shango in relation to the larger eld of religious activity (including the
established churches, Spiritual Baptism, and a more secretive activity,
circle-side work) are all clear in hindsight. But in my writing I paid
little attention to these features and instead distilled the culture of
Shango and detailed its effects, reshaping the values and lifestyles of
participants.
Gyanumayas climb up the house repeated, in microcosm, the same
opportunity to focus on commonalities and forget the differences. One
could ignore the improvisational character of her feat and instead wonder, as I rst did, at the power of the cultural proscriptions of caste, the
products of collective social history, to guide an individuals actions.
Constructivism takes the opposite tack. It invites notice of the improvisation as a sign of positioning by powerful discourses. Gyanumaya
then becomes an instrument and an outcome of the situation, a person
pushed rst into one subject position (as a welcome guest) and then into
another (as a bearer of pollution). In the extreme, especially in circumstances deemed postmodern, according to constructivists, people constantly face such disjoint situations. Feminist poststructuralists emphasize that women and other oppressed people are especially subject to
situations like Gyanumayas, situations replete with contradictions. Oppressed people are constantly climbing up houses.
We advocate paying more attention to the improvisation itself, to what
was produced. We try to see both culture and subject position at the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Woman Who Climbed up the House

17

Ex

am

Co

py

same time. We do not look directly at either cultural logic or subject


position as a phenomenon unto itself. Instead we consider the practical
artifacts of the momentthe verbal, gestural, and material productionsemerging from the situation, and ask how, and to what end, these
artifacts might be taken up and, in later events perhaps, become conventionalized or made into culture. In our vision, Gyanumaya and the rest of
us, individually and collectively, are not just products of our culture, not
just respondents to the situation, but also and critically appropriators of
cultural artifacts that we and others produce.
By climbing up the house Gyanumaya produced an unusual form of
behavior, one that did not, but conceivably could have, become an icon
for reection. There were other such productions as well. Debra and I,
together with the people of Naudada, co-constructed the interview situation, making Debras house a somewhat unusual social space in the
community and a notional artifact perhaps more appealing for future
appropriation than the idea of climbing up houses. The space was one
where prevalent caste restrictions were not authorized by the person
who was at least in nominal charge.12 The situation also gured in various improvisations: of material that could be incorporated into later
gossip sessions about the strange foreigners, of research materials for
later use by Debra and me, and, obviously, of an icon that could be useful
for our purposes in this chapter.
Constructivists think of improvisation as an expected outcome when
people are simultaneously engaged with or pushed by contradictory discourses. They view it as an endpoint, however, not a beginning. Others
have attempted to connect culture and subject position conceptually in
order to understand peoples behavior. Bourdieu has made one of the
most signicant efforts in this regard, and his metatheory of practice has
been extremely consequential for anthropological theories of culture.
Under the tutelage of Bourdieu and others, anthropological attention has
turned from the analysis of culture as an objectied and abstract system
to the immanent analysis of cultural forms, of their constant improvisation within ever-changing social and material conditions (see Ortner
1984 for an early review).
Gyanumayas climb up the house, no matter how striking and dramatic an improvisation, is, in the regard of Bourdieus practice theory,
characteristic of everyday behavior. Improvisations are the sort of impromptu actions that occur when our past, brought to the present as
habitus, meets with a particular combination of circumstances and condi-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

18

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

tions for which we have no set response. Such improvisations are the
openings by which change comes about from generation to generation.
They constitute the environment or landscape in which the experience
of the next generation sediments, falls out, into expectation and disposition. The improvisations of the parental generation are the beginning of
a new habitus for the next generation. Pace Bourdieu, we suggest that the
process is also condensed into the space of a lifetime. In our view, improvisations, from a cultural base and in response to the subject positions offered in situ, are, when taken up as symbol, potential beginnings
of an altered subjectivity, an altered identity.13
Such productions, we believe, are always being appropriated by people
as heuristic means to guide, authorize, legitimate, and encourage their
own and others behavior. As an often unintended but sometimes purposive consequence, there is a continual process of heuristic development: individuals and groups are always (re)forming themselves as persons and collectives through cultural materials created in the immediate
and the more distant past. In this process of heuristic development,
culture and subject position are joined in the production of cultural
resources that are then subjectively taken up.
The possibilities of heuristic development do not mean that humans
are free to develop whatever subjectivity they wish and to do whatever
strikes them at the moment. Far from it. Ones history-in-person is the
sediment from past experiences upon which one improvises, using the
cultural resources available, in response to the subject positions afforded
one in the present. The constraints are overpowering, yet not hermetically sealed. Improvisation can become the basis for a reformed subjectivity.
The ethnographic case studies we present in later chapters range
widely in their subjectsfrom persons coming to identify themselves as
non-drinking alcoholics to women workers and students living and
sometimes resisting the cultural politics of gender that position them;
from Roger Kelly, assembling identities from his psychiatric diagnoses, to
Tika Damai and other young women in Nepal imagining, through songs,
a world in which women escape patriarchal control. The conceptual
discussions interspersed among these ethnographic chapters emphasize
not only the contemporaneous processes but also the long-term, developmental character of identity formation as it moves through improvisation, conict, embodiment, and dialogue in social and historical time.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

Discourses and Practices of the Self

Ex

am

In all places across the worldin Nepal, for example, or American Samoathere are ways of talking about what we might call a persons self
(or selves). That is, there are conventional means for representing the
intrinsic capacities and processes that are presumed to shape a persons
actions. These intrinsic capacities and processes are implicated in
reexive mediation of behavior, the objectifying, monitoring, and evaluating stances that a person sometimes takes toward his or her own behavior.1 This talk of, this discourse about, what we will call selves takes
many forms. It may consist of expressions implying a subjective sense of
oneself as an actor/subject; it may be embedded in claims about others or
expressed directly in pontications about the self by specialists. Nepalis
and Samoans have ways of talking about selves, but their discourses
differ from those prevalent in the United States. They also engage in
activities thought to affect the self, practices of the self, that are not
necessarily common in the United States (see, for example, Clement
1982 on Samoan therapeutic procedures for mental disorders).
Parish (1991, 1994) has written about people living in the Kathmandu
Valley of Nepal who identify themselves as members of the Newar ethnic
group.2 At the time of his eldwork in the 1980s the Newar people he
met did not use any term that might be easily glossed in English as
self, but they talked about the heart in ways akin to our talk about the
self.3 Memories, thoughts, and feelings were stored in the heart (nuga);
willful actions came from the heart. English speakers in the United States
sometimes say things like Who knows whats in his heart? but the
19
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

20

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

Newars talked about the heart in ways that would be readily understood
in English. They spoke, for example, of someone with no ability to
commit himself or follow through on moral action as lacking heart
blood. Furthermore, the self/heart was an abode of the sacred. They
spoke of a deity residing in their hearts and dictating morally commendable actions.
Parishs account of a distinctive self-discourse is not without company.
Fifty years of research by anthropologists and other cross-cultural researchers have provided examples from all parts of the globe. It is now
clear that there exists a huge variety of discourses, practices, concepts,
means, and modalities of the self.4 The question now is not so much
whether there are differences but rather what they signify. At issue is the
role of historical, social, and cultural phenomena in constituting the self.
How, if at all, do they inform the self that is their object? Stances toward
the ontological status of these self-discourses and practices lie at the
heart of the debates in the vast literature on the self.

The Universal versus the Culturally Specic Self

Ex

am

Before the 1970s, anthropological work on culture and the self, like other
cross-cultural inquiries, developed primarily through a dialogue, sometimes implicit, sometimes explicit, between universalist and culturalist
perspectives.5 The oft-remarked division of labor between anthropology
and psychology (for example, Bloch 1985, Miller 1988) cast psychology
and psychologists as the champions of universalist perspectives and anthropology as the bastion of culturalist positions, but the dialogue was
also conducted within anthropology.
The debate is very familiar and can be recounted quickly. The point of
particular importance is the status attributed to specic discourses and
practices of the self. For those who assign theoretical priority to the
natural self, culture is subordinate to universal properties of human
psychology. The human self is, rst and foremost, a complex of natural,
species-given structures and processes. The selves found in Samoa are
simply refractions of an underlying natural self, as are those in Nepal.
Pliant to scientic probing, the natural self exists beneath the sometimes
dazzling but always thin overlay of cultural expression, of the ways of
enacting and talking about the selfmuch as the species-given human
body exists beneath culturally variable kinds of clothing.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

21

Ex

am

Co

py

In this view, the natural self is akin to the heart or the liver. It is as
invariant as these organs and for the same reason: its ontology is that of a
natural phenomenon. From such a perspective, culturally distinct discourses of the self are simply more or less correct, more or less productive of scientically sound interventions, but scarcely signicant in any
other way. Even in the eld, we could often amalgamate the understandings of different groups to our own. So the Newars of Nepal talk
about a deity in their hearts. Could we not simply say that they have a
metaphorical way of talking about the superego? Misguided childrearing
practices may lead to pathologies of the natural self, just as poor eating
habits affect whether the heart becomes diseased, but the relationship is
the same no matter what the culture. Ones cultural background may
affect such things as whether ones self-esteem is high or low, yet self-esteem remains not only ascertainable but also a signicant correlate of
other aspects of the person, no matter the culture. Whether in a given
culture there exists a comparable concept, a similar discourse about
self-esteem, is irrelevant.
Countervailing culturalist positions cannot be captured by the clothing/body analogy. For them, culturally specic concepts are not coverings of the universal self. Instead they are indicators of contours of
culture that shape the self in profound ways. South Asian religious and
philosophical writings and pronouncements on the self, for example,
have been read by anthropologists such as Dumont (1980) and Marriott
(1976a, 1976b) as indicators of a culture deeply at odds with Western
culture. These anthropologists argue that South Asian cultures, especially dominant Hindu forms, grant value principally to the collective.
The culture derogates individuals to the point that, especially for Dumont, the Western concept of the individual is not very helpful in the
study of Indian societies.
Culturally variable discourses and concepts of the self are, in the culturalist view, not simply more or less accurate maps of the same territory,
but different maps that bespeak a difference in the territory. Shweder and
Bourne (1984), summarizing a large amount of cross-cultural literature,
identify two ideal types of self: a Western concept that is autonomous, acontextual, abstract, and independent; and a non-Western notion that is context-dependent, concrete, and socially dened: To the
question Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? our answer is obviously yes; we have tried to identify two major alternative

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

22

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

conceptualizations of the individual-social relationship, viz., the egocentric contractual and the sociocentric organic (193).
For many anthropologists this research implies the proposition that
culture profoundly shapes selves.6 Different cultural conceptions of the
self are treated as indicators of underlying cultural themes that affect not
only conceptions of the self but also conceptions of emotion, child development, and mental disorders, as well as individuals patterns of cognition and affect. As Miller (1988: 280) puts it, the studies imply that
cultural content must be regarded as an essential inuence on the patterning of psychological structures and processes. The differences in
collective meaning systems, in other words, are productive, and therefore indicative, of profound and thoroughgoing differences in the selves
of those who are cultured in these systems. These cultural discourses and
their relationship to the self are not like the relation of the clothes to the
body, but more like that of a bottle to the liquid it contains. Self-discourses and practices must be scrutinized, for they are clues to the contours of the bottlethe culturethat shapes the malleable self.
Few anthropologists, if any, fully embrace the extremes of either the
universalist or the culturalist position. Rather they try to reconcile the
tensions between universalism and cultural specicity. For example, Hallowell (1955a, 1955b), who pioneered anthropological theories of the
self, argued that every person anywhere had a sense of himself or herself
as a separate being and could thus become an object to himself or herself.
For Hallowell, self-awareness and self-reexivity were universal, speciesgiven characteristics.7 He pointed out other invariant aspects of selves as
well, yet argued that many seemingly natural aspects of the self are, in
fact, culturally shaped and cannot be universally interpreted.8 For instance, in a culture where demons are taken seriously, a feeling that one
is being threatened by demons is indicative of culture, not of pathology
as it probably would be in a demonless culture.
More recent approaches to the problem of articulating the universal with the cultural include the work of the anthropologist Gananath
Obeyesekere (1981), who builds upon psychodynamic theories an account of important, universal structures and processes that, at the same
time, emphasizes cultural symbols. In his view, individuals appropriate
cultural symbols to assuage and manage the psychodynamically generated dilemmas that we all face.9 The medical anthropologist Thomas
Csordas (1994) draws upon the phenomenological psychology of Mer-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

23

The Critical Disruption

Co

py

leau-Ponty to articulate a notion of the self embodied withinthat is,


universally oriented toan experiential world that is deeply and inevitably informed by its particular cultural environment. He too seeks to
reconcile the universal and culturally specic aspects of self.
In this debate, then, we see broad differences in the assessment of
culturally specic discourses and practices of the self. From the universalist position these discourses are largely irrelevant; from the culturalist they are indicative of key contours of cultures. The more recent perspectives (introduced in Chapter 1) certainly consider these discourses
relevant, but they do not take them to be indicators of an underlying,
pervasive culture. Rather they conceive them as what we will call living
artifacts or living tools. Obeyesekeres conceptualization of cultural symbolsas therapeutic media taken up by individuals faced with psychodynamic dilemmashints at this direction, but the new development moves outside the old issues.

Ex

am

Roughly two decades ago powerful cross-currents began to disrupt the


universalist-culturalist debate. Anthropology entered into a period of
critical examination of its relations with its subjects. The discipline came
under sustained criticism from within. The critics reected upon the
collaboration of anthropologists with colonial powers (Asad 1973), for
example, and upon the tendency of anthropological eldwork to focus
myopically on men and male-centered activities (Weiner 1976; Reiter
1975). Somewhat later, anthropologists began asking questions about
their representations of others. What messages were conveyed by anthropological writing practices? Clifford (1988), Fabian (1983), and others
harshly judged the device of the ethnographic present (writing in the
present tense) used to describe peoples who were actually studied during
a particular period. Such usage suspended these peoples in time, removed them from history, and treated them as though they were simply
pliable specimens of science, comparable to other cultural species.
Anthropologists, along with some psychologists, also adopted Foucaults work, especially his formulation of power/knowledge, as an impetus for critical reection. Foucault depicted social and psychological
sciences as constructing, rather than objectively studying, their subjects.
In the post-Enlightenment West, the scientic management of popula-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

24

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

tions places people in categories that determine the treatment they receive in such institutions as mental institutions, prisons, and schools.
Technical categories, such as schizophrenic or at risk children, the
argument goes, also enter into or return to everyday discourse, and so are
used in schools and other institutions. People learn to treat one another
and themselves according to these categories.
This trafc between the scientic and the institutional, between the
scientic and the popular, belies the possibility that social scientists
could be what they claim to be: non-interfering, truth-telling observers.
Instead scientists, most obviously those whose ndings enter directly
into institutional treatments, become implicated in a kind of forced reductionism. Local knowledge is disregarded and replaced by scientic
categories imposed by those with power. No matter how scrupulous the
attempts of individual researchers to be objective, social scientists, today
as in the past, are studying what their eld of study has helped to create.
In this Foucauldian vision, unreexive claims to objectivity are hollow
at best; at worst they are a self-serving means by which science rhetorically claims authority.10
This critical stance has had a number of ramications in anthropology.
For one, culture, that is, anthropological descriptions of cultures, was
subjected to scrutiny and seen to be suspiciously entangled with systems
of power/knowledge. Looking back to past collaborations with colonial
governments, for example, anthropologists analyzed how anthropological constructions of the colonized informed governmental actions. Governments efforts to subjugate their colonized populations powerfully
constrained these peoples to reconstruct themselves, either in embrace
of or in opposition to the categories used to describe them.11 The guise of
scientist has thus become suspect to the point that many anthropologists
meet claims to scientic objectivity and authority with incredulity, if not
antagonism.
Feminists too have rigorously criticized past representations of culture, claiming that anthropologists often put forward patriarchal and
thus partial accounts. Twenty-ve years of feminist scrutiny have made it
impossible to ignore the importance of gender in all societies or to avoid
the recognition that what men may take to be important may well be
experienced in quite different ways by women. Twenty years ago Geertz
(1973a) could get away with analyzing the cockght in Bali, a solely
masculine activity, without paying particular attention to the social sig-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

25

Ex

am

Co

py

nicance of its gendered quality, of who participated and who did not.
Today it is clear even to those who are not engaged by feminist theory
that women have different perspectives on the apparatuses of male privilegesuch as mens houses in Amazonian culturesthan men do; and
that their perspectives are not necessarily those which men wish them to
adopt (Gregor 1977). Clearly ones social positiondened by gender,
race, class, and any other division that is structurally signicantpotentially affects ones perspective on cultural institutions and the ardor of
ones subscription to the values and interpretations that are promoted in
rituals and other socially produced cultural forms.
These feminist critiques see many cultural discourses as impositions,
pushing women and men to behavior compatible with the structures and
institutions that favor members of one social category over another.
Feminists, then, provide yet another basis for suspecting descriptions of
seemingly holistic, coherent, integrated cultures. They ask: Whose account of the culture is being privileged? Whose view is being constructed as though it were the only one? And they answer: Not the view
of those in positions of restricted privilege. Accounts of culture that
ignore the importance of social position surreptitiously participate in the
silencing of those who lack privilege and power.
The critical disruption has further affected the writing of ethnographies across cultural boundaries. Texts, when conceived consistently
with a Foucauldian logic, invite the reader to regard anthropological
subjects from a certain perspective. Thus the writings of contemporary
ethnographers, as well as those of researchers in other elds, are now
vulnerable to criticism of their textual strategies. These strategies include more than theories about signicant structures and processes important to the question at hand; they also include the texts implications
about the position of the researcher relative to the people being studied
and relative to the reader. Ethnographic accounts are now critically read
for their portrayal of the relationship between anthropologist and subject, and many texts now include explicit sections that problematize
these relationships (for early examples see Favret-Saada 1980; Crapanzano 1980). It is now common for writers to consider critically the
perspective that they convey.
This critical disruption has a number of implications for comparison
across cultural boundaries. The very conceptions of culture have
changed drastically. Anthropology no longer endeavors to describe cul-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

26

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

tures as though they were coherent, integrated, timeless wholes. The


object of study has shifted away from Samoan culture, or American
culture, or any culture taken as a whole. Anthropology is much less
willing to treat the cultural discourses and practices of a group of people
as indicative of one underlying cultural logic or essence equally compelling to all members of the group. Instead, contest, struggle, and power
have been brought to the foreground. The objects of cultural study are
now particular, circumscribed, historically and socially situated texts
or forms and the processes through which they are negotiated, resisted, institutionalized, and internalized. As Foucault insisted, signicantly for the study of culture and self, cultural forms are presumed
to affect and shape subjectivity, and these cultural forms come in great
variety.12

The Socially Constructed Self

Ex

am

In addition to bringing about a reguring of culture as an object of study


and of the scientists position with regard to those being studied, the
critical disruption in anthropology and other elds has had a powerful
impact on dialogues about the self. Discourse or discursive theory, as
Foucauldian and related understandings are sometimes called, has provoked a new concept of the selfas socially constructed. Here we return
to the consideration of constructivism we began in Chapter 1. The discourses and categories dominant in a society, the argument goes, are
inscribed upon people, both interpersonally and institutionally, and
(as we shall see later in this chapter) within them. Selves are socially
constructed through the mediation of powerful discourses and their artifactstax forms, census categories, curriculum vitae, and the like.
To reiterate, social constructivists emphasize that our communications
with one another not only convey messages but also always make claims
about who we are relative to one another and the nature of our relationships. When we speak we afford subject positions to one another. In this
chapter we afford certain positions to anthropologists and others to psychologists, those who identify themselves as scientists, those who suspect scientic representations, those who subscribe to older anthropological views of the self, and so on. In like manner, genres such as
censuses or curriculum vitae require us to present ourselves according to
categories obligatory to the form. Resistance to such affordances is possi-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

27

Ex

am

Co

py

ble, of course. But the texts and other cultural media we create constrain,
according to social constructivists, readers to two choices: to comply
or to resist. Perhaps the discourses we use here are not hegemonic, and
perhaps we lack even momentary power to make the constraints we
place on our readers chafe; nonetheless, there exist other texts and
other discourses that cannot be similarly put aside. The subject of the
self is always open to the power of the discourses and practices that
describe it.13
The social constructivist position cannot be contained by the terms of
the prior universalist-culturalist debate. Indeed, from the standpoint of
constructivism all the phenomena addressed (and differentiated) by the
participants in that older debate might as well be collapsed into one
construct, an essential self. This essential self, a durable organization
of the mind/body, is perennially suspect as a product of essentialist
thinking, while the socially constructed self remains a paragon of antiessentialism.
The culturalist versions of essential selves have been especially subject
to criticism. Envisioned as stable and enduring characters set in place
(by the end of childhood) through rituals and other socializing practices
that distill and inculcate the core values of what were assumed to be
pervasive cultures, they are anathema to constructivism, a kind of double essentialism. Such selves persist through time, regardless of change
in social and material conditions. Socially constructed selves, in sharp
contrast, are subject to positioning by whatever powerful discourses they
happen to encounterchanging state policies that dictate new ways
of categorizing people in the census, educational diagnostics that label
some children at risk, or new forms of racist discourse taken up from
right-wing talk shows. Perhaps they are resistant to such social forces;
they nonetheless remain provisionally at their mercy. Social constructivism conceives discourses and practices to be the tools that build the self
in contexts of power, rather than as expressions of stable interpretations
of world and values that have been imparted to the person through
enculturation.
Indeed, the universalists and the culturalists (insofar as they still exist)
nd themselves unwitting allies, cast into the same essentialist camp.
The heated debates now concern the degree to which the essentialist self
is important at all. Again, when portraying this second debate, one must
not imagine two exclusive categories but rather a continuum between an

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

28

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

extreme essentialist view, which pays absolutely no attention to the socially positioning power of discourses, and an extreme constructivist
position, which has no interest in any durable aspects of self. Most if not
all actual statements lie somewhere along the continuum and seek to
reconcile the tensions between what can be depicted as incompatible
views.14 One nds social constructivists, such as Hollway (1984), who,
despite their emphasis on discursive positioning, consider Lacanian concepts useful to delineate parts of the embodied self that endure and make
a difference in capitulation or resistance to social positioningor even
Foucault (1978) himself, who nds, beyond the discourses of sexuality, a
surplus of experience in the sexual pleasures of the body (Harvey
1996).

Emerging Directions in the Study of the Self

Ex

am

The critical disruption has, to put it mildly, interfered with the kind of
questions posed by Shweder and Bourne (1984) in their summary of
cross-cultural variation in concepts of the person. The intellectual and
political climate within anthropology has decisively changed, affecting
the questions that those of us engaged in studies of self and identity are
now pursuing.15
The newer research, in combination with past work, provides a basis
for a practice theory of self, one informed and fundamentally reformed,
but not determined, by the extreme versions of the critiques mounted in
the last twenty years.16 The more recent inquiries, though not always
undertaken in the name of critical psychological anthropology, nonetheless reect dimensions of the critical disruption. We can discern at
least three interrelated components of a theoretical reguring of the
relationship between culture and self. First, culturally and socially constructed discourses and practices of the self are recognized as neither the
clothes of a universally identical self nor the (static) elements of cultural molds into which the self is cast. Rather, differentiated by relations
of power and the associated institutional infrastructure, they are conceived as living tools of the selfas artifacts or media that gure the self
constitutively, in open-ended ways. Second, and correlatively, the self is
treated as always embedded in (social) practice, and as itself a kind of
practice. Third, sites of the self, the loci of self-production or self-process, are recognized as plural.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

29

Sites of the Self

Ex

am

Co

py

The idea of plural, even competing sites of the self, is now common to a
variety of disciplines (see, for example, Smith 1988). In anthropology
the demise of the privileged concept of bounded, discrete, coherent cultures has made room for the recognition that people are exposed to
competing and differentially powerful and authoritative discourses and
practices of the self.
A series of articles in the journal Ethos in the early 1990s made this
point. This series challenges Shweder and Bournes separation of a Western egocentric or independent self from a non-Western sociocentric
or interdependent self. The articles particularly attack any monolithic,
essentialist version of the Western self. Their arguments take us
through some of the same terrain we have already explored. For instance, Ewing (1990) and Murray (1993) inveigh against the past tendency of anthropologists to write as though cultural discourses and practices of the self could be taken as indicators of core cultural themes
whose internalization yielded culturalized selves. But they resurvey
this ground with specic reference to culture and self, and they illustrate
the conceptual move to plural sites of self-fashioning. They call for the
disambiguation of the sites or sources of the self and imply that the
works drawn from these different production sites of self-discourses
and practices are not necessarily complementary.
Ewing, for example, says that the authoritative discourses on the self
found in religious and philosophical texts hardly exhaust the sites of
self-production. Popular discourses about the self are another important
locus. Anthropologists like Marriott and Dumont, Ewing argues, fail to
take account of the self as it is expressed by individuals in everyday talk
and action. Hence their account of self in South Asia, derived from
analyses of the talk and texts of specialists, is always partial and may not
coincide with the self construed by popular genres of speaking and acting. In particular, Ewing turns her criticism to the depiction of the Western self. Although Western representations of the self found in philosophical and psychological texts may indeed bespeak a person who is
autonomous, bounded, and transcontextual, peoples representations of
themselves in the stream of everyday life reveal a multitude of selves that
are neither bounded, stable, perduring, nor impermeable. Spiro (1993),
addressing a broader set of topics, makes some of the same points to

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

30

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

support his thesis that the differences between the West and the nonWest have been greatly exaggerated.
Murray (1993) speaks to a different point. His argument has to do less
with discrepancies between selves founded in authoritative discourses
and selves depicted in everyday talk than with discrepant characterizations within Western authoritative discourse on the self. Western
philosophy, Western psychology, and Western religion do not, he argues,
present a unied conception of the self. He cites David Hume as a major philosophical gure whose depictions of the self were quite at odds
with those which supposedly characterize the West. If even authoritative
sources are far from being in agreement, where then does a Western
conception of self live?
Holland and Kipnis (1994) return to the venue of popular discourse.
They describe everyday discourses on embarrassment in the United
States and analyze the cultural model of self that underlies this talk. The
self that gets embarrassed is a sociocentric or, to use Markus and
Kitayamas (1991) terms, an interdependent self. Western ethnopsychologies, Holland and Kipnis conclude, like Murrays formal psychologies and philosophies, do not uniformly posit an egocentric self. Americans engage in discourses that are not consistent on the subject of
sociocentrism/egocentrism. Some posit a sociocentric or interdependent
self; others, an egocentric or independent self.

Selves in Practice

Ex

These articles do not say that differences we think of as cultural are


insignicant. Nor do they deny the utility of the distinction between
sociocentric and egocentric. Self-discourses and practices do vary across
cultures in ways that may be, for some purposes, usefully glossed as
sociocentric and egocentric. Markus and Kitayama, for example, are convincing in their argument that accommodating ones self to others is
important in Japan, and Kondos (1990) ethnography, also set in Japan,
vividly communicates the same valuing of accommodation. In some of
our own research in American Samoa on interpretations and treatments
of mental disorders (Clement [formerly Holland] 1982) we found that
Samoan cultural models of mental problems rested upon a notion of
persons affected more by their social situations than by their enduring
intrapersonal conicts or tendencies.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

31

Ex

am

Co

py

As Miller (1988) points out in her review of the anthropological literature on concepts of self, these ndings t the generalization that some
groups emphasize more relational notions of the self while others, such
as those of the West, emphasize more individualistic ones. The caveat is
that such characterizations lend themselves to reication. We must take
care that American culture and Americans, for example, are not
construed in our specialist discourses as inherently egocentric or independent, or that Samoans culture and Samoans are not treated as though
they were, in essence, sociocentric or interdependent. Behavior is better
viewed as a sign of self in practice, not as a sign of self in essence. It is
sometimes useful to extract from the ow of events and discourses in a
particular locale important conceits or conceptsrecurring conventions
of thinking and behaving and constantly defended values. But the further stepassuming that this abstraction, this culture or the self
associated with it, has an essence, an impetus of its own that will continue through time, absent any social machinationshas been decisively
challenged (White 1993).17
Whether subject to direct criticism or not, any anthropological idea of
an unproblematic relation between culture and self would have no doubt
eroded on its own. The relationship between cultural forms and personhood is no longer taken for granted, and as a result subjectivity is both
more signicant and more interesting. Persons are now recognized to
have perspectives on their cultural worlds that are likely to differ by
gender and other markers of social position.18 They are no longer considered to be unproblematically shaped by rituals and other key events of an
enculturation that solely reects cultural ideology. If public institutions
and the rituals they stage are important to, but not determinant of,
subjectivity, then researchers need to address personhood directly. If people are not seen simply as living enactments of core cultural themes, then
anthropologists are free, indeed pushed, to ask a broader range of questions about experience and subjectivity and the role of cultural resources
in the constitution of this experience.
New ethnographers of personhood have begun to ask such questions.19 They share some implicit or explicit components of a theory of
the relationship between culture and self. All of them emphasize specic
cultural discourses and practices. All of them show interest in the plurality of sites of the self. They do not regard these discourses and practices
as indicators of essential features or themes of the cultures in which they

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

32

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

work. Rather, and these particulars are important, they treat these discourses as the media around which socially and historically positioned
persons construct their subjectivities in practice. The critical developments of the past twenty-ve years not only have revived anthropological interest in culture and self but also have fundamentally shifted anthropologists view of the relationship between the two. The new
ethnographers of personhood describe how specic, often socially powerful, cultural discourses and practices both position people and provide
them with the resources to respond to the problematic situations in
which they nd themselves.
Subjectivity, in these works, is seen to be developing at an interface,
within the interplay between the social and embodied sources of the self,
in what might be called the self-in-practice or, to use a label inspired by
Bakhtin, the authoring self (see Chapter 8). This self-in-practice occupies the interface between intimate discourses, inner speaking, and bodily practices formed in the past and the discourses and practices to which
people are exposed, willingly or not, in the present.20 It authors or orchestrates the products of these sites of self. These ethnographies sidestep a possible impasse between social constructivist and embodied
views of the self, and see instead a mediation between thema mediation produced, in part, by a developing self-in-practice.

A Next Step

Ex

This view of self and identity emerging from ethnographic studies speaks
to the critical concerns of our postmodern disciplines. Our purpose in
this book is to extend, through concepts drawn from Bakhtin and Vygotsky, the lessons learned from anthropological encounters with self and
identity to a cultural studies of the person. Our theoretical path diverges
from, though it may ultimately be compatible with, other efforts in cultural studies.
Hall (1996) provides a state-of-the-art account of efforts to overcome
a problem left by Foucaults success in killing off the Western concept of
the integral, originary subject. Foucault persuasively detailed the historical processes of subjectication by discursive practices, and the politics of exclusion which all such subjectication appears to entail (Hall
1996: 2). But he died early in the process of correcting an over-emphasis
on the determinants of the subject. Subjects, especially in his earlier
writings, were left with no surplus (Harvey 1996) by which to escape

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

33

Ex

am

Co

py

complete and utter domination and compliance. The notion that an


effective suturing of the subject to a subject-position requires, not only
that the subject is hailed, but that the subject invests in the position,
means that suturing has to be thought of as an articulation, rather than a
one-sided process, and that in turn places identication, if not identities,
rmly on the theoretical agenda (Hall 1996: 6).
To solve this problem of why and how the subject invests in discursively afforded positions, cultural studies has embraced psychoanalytic
theory. Hall situates this choice in the prehistory of present-day theorizing about articulation. He goes back to debates over Althussers essay
on ideological state apparatuses and its reliance upon a Lacanian vision of the psychodynamic constitution of the subject. For many reasons, this approach proved deeply awed. Modications and reformulations have overcome some of the central critiques and show promise, in
Halls estimation, for avoiding the older visions shortcomings: lack of a
developmental perspective (identications happened all at once in early
childhood); and insufcient attention to whether psychodynamic understandings of sexual and gender identications could be extended or otherwise prove useful in accounting for racial identication, for instance,
or national identity. He mentions the work of Butler (1993). Identication, as reworked by Butler, is a promissory note for a productive theory
of articulation, a suturing [of subject and discursive practices], an
over-determination not a subsumption. There is always too much or
too littlean over-determination or a lack, but never a proper t, a
totality (Hall 1996: 3).
Our plan is to follow a different possibility for understanding the
suturing of person to position. From a Bakhtinian-sociohistoric perspective, persons develop through and around the cultural forms by
which they are identied, and identify themselves, in the context of their
afliation or disafliation with those associated with those forms and
practices (see Chapter 8). A better metaphor for us is not suture, which
makes the person and the position seem to arrive preformed at the moment of suturing, but codevelopmentthe linked development of people, cultural forms, and social positions in particular historical worlds.21

Building a Bakhtinian-Sociohistoric View of Self


Our notions of the role of cultural forms in the heuristic development of
identity are strongly inuenced by a stream of thought that began in

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

34

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

postrevolutionary Russia and matured there in the late 1920s and 1930s.
The sociohistorical school of psychology was the product of an earlier
critical disruption. Its members took as their task the creation of a psychology founded upon Marxs historical, social, and economic precepts, a
psychology that would nonetheless give proper attention to the (always
relative) autonomy of personal, cognitive, affective, and conative phenomena. Vygotsky and his followers were not alone in their search for an
understanding of persons that took Marx into account. Among the many
philosophers, writers, artists, and human scientists who considered it,
Bakhtin and the friends who wrote with him (Voloshinov preeminently)
contributed one of the most signicant explorations of personhood
one that is socially and historically construed, yet creative. Although
they worked virtually contemporaneously, there is little evidence that the
two groups of theorists knew much of one another.22 Their similarities
are a testament to the shared problematics of the postrevolutionary intellectual milieu.
The period in Soviet history immediately following the 1917 Revolution was one of intense intellectual as well as social ferment. Utopian
conceptions of the New Men that must populate a new world arose
in many elds and ranged from the romantic and heroic to the scientistic and systematic. In the arts, cubism, futurism, even suprematism
sought to break the old forms of perception and replace them with newly
expressed and various understandings of the sensuous. Symbolists and
formalists debated the forms of literary expression proper to the new
order, weighing poetry against prose and the intuitive and spoken
against the structural and written. Philosophical schools and the sciences, both social and natural, strove to come to terms with a Marxist
tradition that had yet to solidify. And all these idioms and ideologies
were intended to inform the living world, the lives of people beyond the
rings of intellectual debate.
Marxs followers had never settled on a common understanding of
human beings apart from the necessities of political economy and revolutionary action. After a successful revolution, however, a new establishment awaited, one whose concerns no longer had to adhere to the
exigent but could (indeed must) expand to the horizons of the everyday.
Others have interpreted this move as a return to the broader, more philosophical concerns of the young Marx. But that work was little known,
and it was unnished in comparison to Kapital. An old problem re-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

35

Semiotic Mediation

Co

py

mained, which we might now call one of social psychology. What was
the proper relation of the social realm to the psyche, to the interior life of
people? If Marx had said, in the theses on Feuerbach, that the human
essence is no abstraction inherent in each single individual . . . in its
reality it is the ensemble of social relations, he had not said how this
ensemble was composed (Marx 1975). Vygotsky and his students, Bakhtin and his collaborators were among the many who attempted to meet
this problem (see Joravsky 1989; Luria 1979). In the remainder of this
chapter we will focus on the answers proposed by Vygotsky and the
sociohistorical school; the wonderful complications drawn by Bakhtin
and Voloshinov will meet fuller exposition in Chapter 8.

Ex

am

Vygotsky was primarily interested not in conceptualizing activity (or


practice, in todays terminology) but in the process of semiotic mediation, and in the development of voluntary control over behaviorthe
hallmark of higher mental functionsthrough this mediation by cultural devices. Vygotsky formulated his thoughts in opposition to the
stimulus-response psychology of his day. For him the key to human
existence was the ability of humans to escape enslavement to whatever
stimuli they happened to encounter. And the way they did this was
(broadly) linguistic, that is, through the active construction and use of
symbols.23 Just as humans can modify the environment physically
thanks to their production of and facility with tools and symbolsthey
can also modify the environments stimulus value for their own mental
states.
In devising his concept of mediating devices, Vygotsky drew from
Engelss ideas about the role of the making and use of tools in the history
of the human species. Engels had observed that tool use, over evolutionary time, changed not only human environments but human physiology
as well. Musculature, cerebral architecture, and other biobehavioral systemsEngelss example was the hand and handednesswere shaped
to the tool as effectively as the tool was shaped to human purpose.
Vygotsky drew an analogy between tools and signs, suggesting that the
use of signs altered not only the social environment but also the very
behavioral architecture of the users. Cross-cultural studies assured him
that the analogy could be fruitfully applied. The descriptions available

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

36

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

to him, drawn from the work of Levy-Bruhl, Thurnwald, and Taylor,


proved to Vygotsky (1930) that human beings frequently use culturally
constructed external objects as signals to control their own psychological processes. Vygotsky especially liked examples of mnemonics. He
cited these techniquesfrom the elaborate mnemonic objects used by
messengers in traditional cultures to the contemporary Westerners knot
in her handkerchiefto show that mediating devices signal a turning
point in human cultural history: the transition from the use of ones
memory to active control over it.
A typical mediating device is constructed by the assigning of meaning
to an object or a behavior. This symbolic object or behavior is then
placed in the environment so as to affect mental events. Vygotsky, however, saw these tools for the selfs control of cognition and affect as, above
all, social and cultural. Assigning meaning and placing in the environment are not just individual acts. Rather mediating devices are part of
collectively formed systems of meaning, products of social history. Although individuals constantly construct and reconstruct their own mediating devices, most of their constructions are not original. They have
been appropriated in the course of social interaction with others who, in
turn, had appropriated the devices from others. Even productions we
might call innovative have developed in the ow of social interaction and
depend intimately upon it for their signicance (and hence their effectiveness). Mediating devices (Bakhtin and Voloshinov will cap the argument in Chapter 8) develop within a locus of social activity, a place in the
social world, that identies and organizes them.
Vygotsky (1960) used the example of tying a knot in a handkerchief to
remind oneself of something. The knot is given meaning for the purpose
of organizing a mental event, for remembering. Yet the meaning depends
upon a collectively remembered history of use and interpretation that is
a common part of the social commentary that accompanies most interaction. Similarly, the dieter uses a mediating device when he tapes a picture
of an obese person to his refrigerator door; the purpose of the image is to
affect his intentions toward the refrigerators contents. But these intentions did not originate with the dieter, they were once social contentions
in the discourses relating objects and desires.
At rst, in the development of a mediated complex of thought and
feeling, mediating devices may be tangible, used voluntarily and consciously: a word said to oneself to encourage oneself to action, a piece of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

37

Ex

am

Co

py

music to which one listens to change ones mood, a chart that one consults to know what to do next in a work routine. Repeated experience
with the tangible device may eventually become unnecessary, and its
function may be internalized (Vygotsky 1978; Wertsch 1985b). The
sign-image, that is, its representation within the inner speaking (and
more generally, inner activity) that constitutes whatever substance there
is to self-consciousness, comes to evoke the routine originally organized
in relation to the external sign. Finally, the process of self-training may
even be forgotten, and the evocation of control may become automatic
(see Hutchins 1986). Even the sign-image escapes notice, and the behavior becomes in a translation of Vygotskys Russian, fossilized.
Although Vygotsky stressed the construction of higher psychological
functions, such as purposive remembering, the idea of mediating devices is appropriate far beyond the domain of humans control over their
memory and even their problem-solving and inferencing. Luria (cited in
Cole 1985), for example, gives a case of the mediation of will, and
Vygotsky (1984: 379) discusses the development of a logic of emotion.
Through signs and words, children learn to talk about, compare, classify,
and thus manage their own emotions. These ideas inform more recent
studies of and popular notions about artices for modulating emotion.
Arlie Hochschild (1983) describes the training of ight attendants and
the devices they were taught to use to control their anger at obnoxious
passengers. They were taught to imagine that something traumatic had
happened in the passengers life, or to remind themselves that the passenger was behaving childishly because of his fear of ying. Hochschild
shows that these devices were carefully inculcated and continually reproduced in practice, under a regimen of training that was instituted and
maintained by the corporation but supported as well by a popular tradition of such means to self-control. So successful was this training that
many ight attendants began to lose any sense of their own anger, on or
off the job.
Another example could be taken from Spradleys account of urban
nomads in Seattle. Spradley (1970) describes a part of the nomad tradition that had to do with surviving incarceration. He details the degradation and brutality that urban nomads faced at the hands of the police,
and he recounts the nomads response, a way of organizing thoughts and
feelings that they taught one another so as to do easy time. Easy time
was a way of minimizing the effect of incarceration, a sign-routine medi-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

38

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Semiotic Mediation and Agency

py

ating interpersonally the course it would take, and inner-actively the


cognitive-affective response to it. In Vygotskian theory, humans often
develop qualitatively different mental functions as a result of learning to
use collectively derived symbols in regulating their own behavior. Supported in using these tools by their fellows, Spradleys urban nomads
were able to learn to organize and, to some extent, consciously control
their thoughts and emotional reactions to jail.

am

Co

Semiotic mediation provides for the capacity that may be called symbolic
bootstrapping. One of the convincing points about this tool of agency,
this tool for gaining control over ones behavior, is its appropriate modesty. It is an indirect meansone modies ones environment with the
aim, but not the certainty, of affecting ones own behaviorand it requires a sustained effort. As Hochschild observed among the ight attendants, by relying rst upon training wheels of a symbolic sort, one can
learn to ignore aspects of situations to which one would have previously
responded.24 One can even, through similar processes, master ones history of alcohol addiction (see Chapter 4). But the process involves effort,
may not succeed, and, except for rather small-scale changes, is clearly
beyond the ability of any individual to accomplish alone. These tools of
agency are highly social in several senses: the symbols of mediation are
collectively produced, learned in practice, and remain distributed over
others for a long period of time.25

Activity and the Self in Practice

Ex

The sociohistorical schools vision has points in common with that of


Bourdieu. For instance, compare the work of Vygotskys inuential student A. N. Leontiev (1978) with Bourdieus concern with the concept of
practice. Leontieva gure often associated with the sociohistorical
school26has insisted on the importance of activity in understanding
human behavior.
According to Leontiev, accounts of behavior by anthropologists, sociologists, and psychologists tend to reduce to two terms only: person (or
group) and environment. Ordinarily in these accounts the person reacts to the conditions of the environment, which are conceived to vary

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

39

Ex

am

Co

py

freelyreminding us of the stimulus-response pairs of behaviorist psychology. Culture, for Leontiev, does not qualify as a third term because,
in its anthropological renderings, it simply becomes a reading, a conceptual guration, of the environment.27 Nor do psychodynamic processes count as a third term; they simply explain the complexities of
response formation in the person.
The third term Leontiev proposed to add indicates that people respond
to what they nd in the environment in the context of a historical,
socially and culturally constructed form of social (inter)action that he
called an activity. In the two-term accounts, persons passively encounter
things in the (culturally interpreted) environment to which they, for
psychodynamic or other reasons, are sensitive. Then they react. Activity
theory instead views people as actively engaged with the environment.
They are always in the ow of doing somethingthe something being
a historical, collectively dened, socially produced activityand it is
within this meaningful intent toward their surroundings that they respond to whatever they encounter in the environment. (Indeed, the fact
of an encounter with an environmental stimulus is itself mediated by
the activity.) Tomorrow they may be engaged in another activity in which
the same object, for example, will be used in a different way, if noticed at
all. Practice theory shares this understanding of people as actively engaged with their environments. Bourdieu (1977a) highlights the similarities between the two theories when he explicitly describes practice
as a third way to grasp social action, one that mediates between objectivism (environment) and subjectivism (person or group).28
One signicant entailment of the activity theory described by Leontiev
is the implied dialectic of person and environment within the context
of activity. The boundary between person and environment becomes less
denite, especially when viewed over time. Persons oriented toward
their surroundings by desire for food, for example, have that desire
shaped in relation to the food they encounter and become familiar with.
The desire is objectied or made more concrete through encounters with
possible objects that could satisfy it in the particular environment.
Bakhtins associate Voloshinov (1986) provides an even more important example of this process for our purposes because he addresses the
social aspects of the environment that affect the objectication of hunger.29 He made an extended argument that experiences, even of internal sensations such as hunger, are formed in and so bear the marks of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

40

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

social context (see Chapter 8). Utterances about ones hunger, even those
which never quite become public utterances, are shaped to the social
situation, and they in turn affect ones subjective sense of hunger. Hunger when one feels no right to request food from ones hosts is likely to
feel different from hunger when one is a customer in an expensive restaurant and the food has been inordinately delayed. There is, in each
instance, a microgenesis, a microdevelopment as the person goes from a
personal, subjective sense to its objectication, responding to and drawing upon past experience as well as the resources and demands of the
particular situation.30 The ultimate behaviorwhether an expression of
hunger, a solution to a problem, a climb up a house, or a work of artis
a product of the person-in-practice. This product, in turn, can become a
symbol or icon important in the mediation of future behavior. The process is one of heuristic development.

The Heuristic Development of Identity and Agency

Ex

am

Vygotskys exposition of semiotic mediation as a means to agency gives


us a good vantage on the social and historical creation of identities as
means to self-activity. Heuristic development clearly directs attention
away from the extremes of cultural determination of behavior on the one
hand and situational totalitarianism on the other. Rather it directs attention to two processes: (1) the genesis of the products (improvisations)
that come from the meeting of persons, cultural resources, and situations
in practice; and (2) the appropriation of these products as heuristics for
the next moment of activity. To the extent that these productions are
used again and again, they can become tools of agency or self-control
and change.
Persons develop more or less conscious conceptions of themselves as
actors in socially and culturally constructed worlds, and these senses of
themselves, these identities, to the degree that they are conscious and
objectied, permit these persons, through the kinds of semiotic mediation described by Vygotsky, at least a modicum of agency or control over
their own behavior.31
Signicant to our concept is the situatedness of identity in collectively
formed activities. The identities that concern us are ones that trace our
participation, especially our agency, in socially produced, culturally con-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

41

Ex

am

Co

py

structed activitieswhat we call gured worldssuch as the worlds of


romance and therapy in the United States or the expected life path of
women in Nepal.
The concept of gured worlds, which we elaborate in Chapter 3,
clearly draws upon Leontievs notion of activity, and helps us make several points about the heuristic development of identities and their signicance. First, gured worlds are historical phenomena, to which we
are recruited or into which we enter, which themselves develop through
the works of their participants. Figured worlds, like activities, are not so
much things or objects to be apprehended, as processes or traditions of
apprehension which gather us up and give us form as our lives intersect
them.
Second, gured worlds, like activities, are social encounters in which
participants positions matter. They proceed and are socially instanced
and located in times and places, not in the everywhere that seems to
encompass cultural worlds as they are usually conceived. Some gured
worlds we may never enter because of our social position or rank; some
we may deny to others; some we may simply miss by contingency; some
we may learn fully.
Third, gured worlds are socially organized and reproduced; they are
like activities in the usual, institutional sense. They divide and relate
participants (almost as roles), and they depend upon the interaction and
the intersubjectivity for perpetuation. The signicance (indeed the existence) of cultural worlds in our lives does not derive from holding them
in mind as some whole image (we may or may not do this), but from
re-creating them by work with others.
Fourth, gured worlds distribute us, not only by relating actors to
landscapes of action (as personae) and spreading our senses of self across
many different elds of activity, but also by giving the landscape human
voice and tone. This last point goes beyond Leontiev, recalls Voloshinov,
and presages Bakhtin. Cultural worlds are populated by familiar social
types and even identiable persons, not simply differentiated by some
abstract division of labor. The identities we gain within gured worlds
are thus specically historical developments, grown through continued
participation in the positions dened by the social organization of those
worlds activity. They are characteristic of humans and societies.
Figured world then provides a means to conceptualize historical

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

42

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

subjectivities, consciousnesses and agency, persons (and collective


agents) forming in practice. It also provides the terms for answering a
conundrum of personal agency.
Inden (1990: 23) denes human agency as

Co

the realized capacity of people to act upon their world and not only to
know about or give personal or intersubjective signicance to it. That
capacity is the power of people to act purposively and reectively, in
more or less complex interrelationships with one another, to reiterate
and remake the world in which they live, in circumstances where they
may consider different courses of action possible and desirable, though
not necessarily from the same point of view.

The conundrum is the seeming contradiction between humans as social


producers and as social products. Inden continues: People do not act
only as agents. They also have the capacity to act as instruments of
other agents, and to be patients, to be the recipients of the acts of
others.

Case Studies

Ex

am

Along with our theoretical discussions, we include case studies of identity in lived worlds: the constitution and interpretation of personal action in historically circumscribed, though never closed, venues of social
activity. We tell of romance at two universities, of drinking and not
drinking in a chapter of Alcoholics Anonymous, and of the treatment of
mental illness, all in the United States, and of gender relations in Nepal.
Participants in all these worlds undergo processes of heuristic development which result, for some, in identities: of self as a romantic type, as a
non-drinking alcoholic, as a manic-depressive or borderline, as an unjustly treated woman.
Hannerz (1983) describes various tools of identity, notably the mirror, whose importance he noted during his research among the Fulani of
Nigeria. His conception of identity indexes a general notion of oneself.
The tools of identity that we examine are those of particular lived worlds.
In Alcoholics Anonymous, for example, participants are coached to tell
their lives according to a format, a genre of story, that has been collectively developed from the beginnings of AA in the 1930s. These emblematic narratives become a major means of the symbolic bootstrapping that

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

43

Ex

am

Co

py

creates a revised sense of self. Through their forms, participants develop


an understanding of themselves as persons who are, and always will be,
sensitive to drink. They learn to shun alcohol or else lead a life controlled by it. Chapter 4 is devoted to a close examination of this heuristic
development organized around life stories.
Other tools of identity we explore include the lexicon of types from
the world of romance (jerk, nerd, fox, loose woman, dog) by
which American women and men position one another and, in the process, themselves. A lexicon of gender types is also an important tool of
identity in Nepal (sande aimai, bull woman; aputri, barren woman;
alacchini, woman who brings bad luck; nathi, irt), especially as it is
realized through songs and the internalized interlocutors (personied
forms of inner speaking) that originate from scoldings and gossip (see
Chapter 10). Songs are, of themselves, also important tools of identity for
women in parts of Nepal. When Skinner asked young girls to tell about
themselves and their lives, they often replied with lines from folksongs.
Both girls and women sing songs to themselves, among themselves, as
one means of objectifying their senses of who they are and the kinds of
lives they lead (Skinner 1990; Holland and Skinner 1995a; see Chapters
10 and 12).
Diagnosis is an obvious tool of identity in the gured world of mental
illness. But diagnoses are not necessarily subjectied (personalized,
drawn into inner speech) by the persons who are their targets in ways
that t the descriptions authorized by the DSM-IV, the diagnostic manual
of the American Psychiatric Association. In Chapter 9 we describe the
identities that one man, Roger Kelly, formed around the diagnoses he
received in the course of his treatmentrst as a manic-depressive, then
as a borderline. With Rogers case we begin to see the play that there
is in symbolic bootstrapping, and the interplay that sometimes takes
place among different sites of identity, different venues of self-fashioning.
Roger arranges, indeed counterposes, these diagnostic tools of identity
strategically, to his advantage in the elds of activity and the relationships that compose his social world. His various identities serve as complicated mediating devices, yet, like all such works of heuristic development, they take on a life of their own, whose effects cannot be reduced to
Rogers intentions.
In Chapter 6 we return to the issue emphasized by constructivists, that
of positionality, and its relationship to gured worlds and to the narra-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

44

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

tivized identities shaped within such worlds. Positionality refers to the


fact that personal activity (the identied action of a person) always occurs from a particular place in a social eld of ordered and interrelated
points or positions of possible activity. In Harrs work (1979, 1984),
perhaps the most rigorous theoretical exposition of a constructivist (social) psychology, a person is really only this, a position that locates an
act within a universe of possible locations dened by discourse.
A person engaged in social life, a person involved in an activity or
practice, is presumed to have a perspective.32 One looks at the world
from the angle of what one is trying to do.33 Postmodernists and feminists also attend to perspective, especially to the perspectives that come
from being treated according to broad social divisions such as gender,
race, class, ethnicity, and sexual orientation. Persons look at the world
from the positions into which they are persistently cast.
We are especially interested in the ways in which peoples perspectives
develop over time. We explore, particularly in Chapters 6 and 7, the
degree to which, and the conditions under which, experiences of being
positioned (which often occur out of awareness) develop through symbolic bootstrapping into identities that afford a modicum of control over
ones own behavior. In the gured world of Alcoholics Anonymous, the
newcomer is discursively positioned as an alcoholic, an identity that
most newcomers are reluctant to accept. And not all newcomers end up
developing a sense of themselves as an alcoholic. How do we understand
the various courses people take from the social work of positioning to
the cultural means of its guration? How do people come to terms with
their lot in order to do something?
Another type of placement is at least as important as narrativized or
discursive placement: positioning by access to space, to associates, to
activities, and to genres. Women of Naudada, Nepal, were largely barred
from certain governmental and ritual activities. Lower-caste people were
usually not allowed to enter the kitchens of higher-caste people. We
discuss these forms of social positioning by inclusion and exclusion in
Chapter 6. Perspectives are tied to a sense of entitlement or disentitlement to the particular spaces, relationships, activities, and forms of expression that together make up the indices of identity. In Chapter 7 we
sketch the heuristic development of womens positionality in the world
of romance, where attractiveness mediates ones access to partners of
higher or lower status as well as ones entitlement to good treatment from

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

45

Ex

am

Co

py

men. The positional aspects of perspective are often less symbolized, less
developed as explicit aspects of ones identity than narrativized aspects.
Thus, behavior that proclaims these socially dened entitlements may be
less susceptible to voluntary control.
Still, position is not fate. Cultural resources, including the activities
and landscapesthe gured worldsthat give meaning to peoples interaction, change historically in ways that are marked by the political
struggles and social valuation of their users. The particular inspiration
that we draw from the sociohistorical school is its attention to continuing
adjustment, reorganization, and movement. This continual development
contrasts with Bourdieus conception: for Bourdieu, the improvisations
that are characteristic of all social behavior make a difference to the
habitus of the next generation. That is, the forms of novel activity created
by a senior generation provide the experiential context in which their
children develop the habitus of the group. Such innovation has little
impact on the established habitus of the elders. For proponents of the
sociohistorical school, in contrast, improvisations make a difference for
the next moment of production.34
It is not impossible for people to gure and remake the conditions of
their lives. Bakhtin shows that, from the very fact that cultural resources
are indelibly marked by social position, people can reassert a point of
control through the rearrangement of cultural forms as evocations of
position (see Chapter 8). The equation of the means of expression and
social forcethe notion of voiceworks both ways. It positions persons
as it provides them with the tools to re-create their positions. The elds
of cultural production that circumscribe perspectives become, in Bakhtins handling, spaces of authoring.
Groups of women in Naudada joined together at the yearly festival of
Tij and produced songs that described and analyzed the sad and unfair
position of women in Nepali society (see Chapter 12). The women narrativized and symbolized their social positions in the family and in society,
and through the activity and around its productions they were developing both critical understandings of their position and new identities
related to their Tij groups. Small groups like these develop heuristically
as well.
In fact, we believe identity formation must be understood as the heuristic codevelopment of cultural media and forms of identity. In Chapter
11 we discuss the ways in which the development of courtly love in the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

46

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

eleventh and twelfth centuries, its activities (the courts of love), and its
songs envisioned not only new forms of association among men and
women but also new gures of possible selves-in-practice. The opening
of an originally specialized, nearly ritual, performative world into the
everydaya long transformationis part of what has made possible the
American world of romance that we studied in the 1970s and 1980s. We
also discuss Andersons (1983) imagined communities, the grounds of
modern nationalism, in that chapter.
Heuristic development results in persons whose history-in-person is
likely to have an agenda and momentum of its own. Persons do bring a
history to the presentan important aspect of which is usually an untidy compilation of perspectives, some developed into symbolized identities.35 Even in the face of powerful situational determinants, such as
inducements to drink among members of Alcoholics Anonymous, these
identities, especially when supported by others of like perspective, afford
some self-control and agency. Nonetheless persons remain susceptible to
the situational determinants of their reproduction, including the subject
positions foisted upon them. The process is a composite one of slow,
sometimes erratic, but continuous change. History-in-person collides
with combinations of circumstances that are by degree precedented and
unprecedented. The behaviors, the products of the moment, then become available as mediators to change oneself and others, and perhaps
even the gured worlds in which one acts. Culture and subject positions
are important components of the working of identity, but cultural production and heuristic development are the keys to its analysis.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

II

Ex

am

Co

py

Placing Identity and Agency

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

Figured Worlds

Ex

am

John Caughey (1984), among others, has remarked upon humans penchant to enter into imaginary worldsvia dreams, daydreams, or spectatorship (lms, sports events, reading). He sees the signicance of this as
twofold. By modeling possibilities, imaginary worlds can inspire new
actions; or, paradoxically, their alternative pleasures can encourage escape and a withdrawal from action. Benedict Anderson (1983) draws out
yet another signicance of human fantasy by understanding nationalism
as participation in an imagined community. Many of the activities that
engage human energy and interest have an imaginative component.
Figured worlds rest upon peoples abilities to form and be formed in
collectively realized as if realms. What if gender relations were dened
so that women had to worry about whether they were attractive? What
if, as in the Trobriand Islands described by Weiner (1976), bundles of
banana leaves were so important that older women spent much time and
energy assembling them?1 What if there were a world called academia,
where books were so signicant that people would sit for hours on end,
away from friends and family, writing them? People have the propensity
to be drawn to, recruited for, and formed in these worlds, and to become
active in and passionate about them. Peoples identities and agency are
formed dialectically and dialogically in these as if worlds.
Toward the end of his life, Vygotsky (1978) wrote a paper in which he
expanded his notion of childrens potential for development. In that
paper we see Vygotskys usual fascination with the ability of humans to
manipulate their worlds and themselves by means of symbols. In it he
analyzed young childrens play and their later abilities with games. Early
49
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

50

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

in life children begin a type of play in which the everyday meaning of


objects is suspended and new meaning is assigned. Behind the couch
becomes the bad guys hideout, under the table becomes the jail. Or
perhaps a certain set of everyday meanings is retained and highlighted,
and other features drop away; Vygotsky describes two sisters playing at
being sisters.
In either type of play, meanings are manipulated to point to other
(absent or distilled) social settings, and ones motivations and feelings
are geared to participation in that scene. Children use a piece of candy to
represent, say, a jewel that robbers have stolen, and treat it as a jewel.
They resist the temptation to eat it. Children may also ignore pain and
fatigue for the purpose of continuing play. When they run a race, the goal
of winning the race, or at least reaching the nish line, overcomes any
desire to stop from fatigue. Sometimes children do stop before the race is
over, or eat the candy, or tire of playing sister. But remarkably often they
do not. They learn to detach themselves from their reactions to their
immediate surroundings, to enter a play worlda conceptual world that
differs from the everydayand to react to the imagined objects and
events of that world.
Vygotsky gave central place to collectively developed signs and symbols as the media by which childrens mental and emotional faculties
were culturally formed.2 He paid special attention to the role of tangible
objects, made collectively into artifacts by the attribution of meaning, as
tools that people use to affect their own and others thinking, feeling, and
behavior. Through habitual use these cultural tools become resources
available for personal use, mnemonics of the activities they facilitate, and
nally constitutive of thought, emotion, and behavior. Describing how
children develop the ability to enter into an imagined world, Vygotsky
speaks of a pivot, a mediating or symbolic device that the child uses
not just to organize a particular response but to pivot or shift into the
frame of a different world. Toys, even sticks assigned the status of horse,
can be the pivots. The tangible symbol may eventually be discarded and
the child may be able to enter the play world without physical props.
Games with more explicit rules and less concrete fantasy become more
prominent as children grow older. Still the children must shift themselves to a conceptual world beyond their immediate surroundings in
order to become actors who submit to the games premises and treat its
events as real. A childs desires become related to a ctitious I, to her

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

51

Ex

am

Co

py

role in the game and its rules (Vygotsky 1978: 100). It is this competence that makes possible culturally constituted or gured worlds and,
consequently, the range of human institutions. Lee (1985) points out the
denite link between play worlds and institutional life. Fantasy and
game play serve as precursors to participation in an institutional life,
where individuals are treated as scholars, bosses, or at-risk children and
events such as the granting of tenure, a corporate raid, and the self-esteem of at-risk children are taken in all seriousness. But to see imagination extended so is simply to recognize that it pervades cultural life.
Under the rubric of culturally gured worlds or gured worlds we
include all those cultural realms peopled by characters from collective
imaginings: academia, the factory, crime, romance, environmental activism, games of Dungeons and Dragons, the mens house among the Mehinaku of Brazil (Gregor 1977). These are worlds made up of Geertzs
(1973b) webs of meaning. Figured worlds take shape within and grant
shape to the coproduction of activities, discourses, performances, and
artifacts. A gured world is peopled by the gures, characters, and types
who carry out its tasks and who also have styles of interacting within,
distinguishable perspectives on, and orientations toward it.
Consider the cultural world of Alcoholics Anonymous (explored in
Chapter 4). In AA meetings participants tell stories about their lives
before they joined the organization. They collect tokens for the periods
of time they have remained sober. They come to name themselves, and
often to see themselves, as alcoholics and not just drinkers. All these
elements of AA are meaningful in, relevant to, and valued (or not) in
relation to a frame of meaning, a virtual world, a world that has been
gured.
When Cain did her research the tokens used to mark length of sobriety were the same plastic chips sold for playing poker. In the world of AA
these chips were not won by holding a straight ush. Rather, they were
meaningfully revalued to a world where the stake, the thing at wager,
was staying sober; the chip became an emblem of a different achievement, another kind of success. On the stores shelf a poker chip is worth
little, but within the world of AA the signicance of a chip, color-coded
for length of time without a drink, is great. The difference between
picking up and not picking up a chip is, for some, the difference between
a good and a bad life.
Likewise, the stories that AA participants learn to tell, about their

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

52

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

former lives and their current temptations, are revalued because they
signify experience and place in a world that differs from that of the
non-alcoholic. These life stories, too, take on the extended meanings
characteristic of play and are thus re-formed within the larger frame of
reference, the gured world of AA. They become the cultural resource
that mediates members identities as non-drinking alcoholics.
These socially generated, culturally gured worlds, many linguists
believe, are necessary for understanding the meaning of words. When
talking and acting, people assume that their words and behavior will be
interpreted according to a context of meaningas indexing or pointing
to a culturally gured world. Violations of this assumption cause confusion and prevarication. Fillmore (1975, 1982), a key gure in the development of frame semantics, asked his readers to consider the word
bachelor. Why is it confusing to ask if the Pope is a bachelor? After all,
a bachelor is an unmarried man and the Pope is an unmarried man. Yet
there is something peculiar about referring to the Pope as a bachelor. The
problem is that bachelor frames . . . a gured world in which prototypical events unfold: men marry at a certain age; marriages last for life; and
in such a world, a bachelor is a man who stays unmarried beyond the
usual age, thereby becoming eminently marriageable (Quinn and Holland 1987: 23). The Pope, under a vow of celibacy, is not a relevant
character in the cultural world of marriageof marriageable men, and of
not-yet-married bachelors. Asking if the Pope is a bachelor is akin to
trying to redeem ones poker chips for money at an AA meeting. The
questioner and the would-be redeemer have both mistaken the relevant
gured world.
By gured world, then, we mean a socially and culturally constructed realm of interpretation in which particular characters and actors
are recognized, signicance is assigned to certain acts, and particular
outcomes are valued over others. Each is a simplied world populated by
a set of agents (in the world of romance: attractive women, boyfriends,
lovers, ancs) who engage in a limited range of meaningful acts or
changes of state (irting with, falling in love with, dumping, having sex
with) as moved by a specic set of forces (attractiveness, love, lust).3
These collective as-if worlds are sociohistoric, contrived interpretations or imaginations that mediate behavior and so, from the perspective
of heuristic development, inform participants outlooks. The ability to
sense (see, hear, touch, taste, feel) the gured world becomes embodied

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

53

Cultural Means

Co

py

over time, through continual participation. One can, in the current state
of technology, put on a bulky headset with connections to computers,
television cameras, and data gloves, and enter into a virtual reality. A
gured world, too, is played out; a frame becomes a worlda space and
time established imaginativelythat one can come to sense after a process of experiencing, acting by virtue of its rules (see Shotter 1982). No
technology, no headset is necessary. Players become ever more familiar
with the happenings of a gured worldthe stories told in AA, for
exampleand learn to author their own and make them available to
other participants. By means of such appropriation, objectication, and
communication, the world itself is also reproduced, forming and reforming in the practices of its participants.

Ex

am

Figured worlds could also be called gurative, narrativized, or dramatized worlds. Figurative, according to Websters Third International Dictionary, means transferred in sense from literal or plain to abstract or
hypothetical; representing or represented by a gure. The production
and reproduction of gured worlds involves both abstraction of signicant regularities from everyday life into expectations about how particular types of events unfold and interpretation of the everyday according to these distillations of past experiences. A gured world is formed
and re-formed in relation to the everyday activities and events that ordain happenings within it. It is certainly not divorced from these happenings, but neither is it identical to the particulars of any one event. It is an
abstraction, an extraction carried out under guidance.4
Narrativized and dramatized convey the idea that many of the
elements of a world relate to one another in the form of a story or drama,
a standard plot against which narratives of unusual events are told.5
For example, Holland and Eisenhart (1990) and Holland and Skinner
(1987) found a taken-for-granted sequence of events in the world of
romance on two university campuses; we present it in Chapter 5.
Skinners studies in Naudada, a mixed-caste, predominantly Hindu
community in central Nepal, provide another example of a gured world
organized around a sort of narrative. For Naudadas women the gured
world of domestic relations was dominated by a narrativized account of
the life path of a good woman. This life path was set forth in the Hindu

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

54

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

moral and religious texts that were read in Naudada and explicated by
priests, and so had more of a prescriptive avor than the taken-forgranted sequence of romantic relations. Along this life path (Skinner
1990: 7377):

Co

Girls are good, hard-working, and obedient daughters.


Eventually they marry, leaving their natal homes (maita) for the
homes of their husbands (ghar).
At their ghar, good daughters-in-law are obedient, respectful, and diligent in their household and agricultural duties, laboring from
dawn to dark for their in-laws.
As wives, women devote themselves to their husbands, seeing to
their needs and obeying their demands.
A good woman bears sons to carry on the patriline.
As she gives birth to and raises sons, she attains more status in the
household.
After the marriage of her own sons, she directs the activities of the
daughters-in-law.
A good woman dies before her husband does.

Ex

am

Again the storyline was not prescriptive but was signicant as a backdrop for interpretation. The meaning of characters, acts, and events in
everyday life was gured against this storyline. Radi (widow), for example, was used as a general term of insult for women and girls of any
age, even very young girls. Its connotations were denitely and potently
negative because of its relation to the life path of a good woman: a widow
was a woman who had decisively departed from the course of the moral
narrative by outliving her husband.
The conceptual importance of gured worlds has been emphasized in
anthropology for some time. Hallowell (1955a) argued that individuals
live in worlds that are culturally dened and understand themselves in
relation to these worlds. In a classic article, The Self and Its Behavioral
Environment, he notes that, for those whose cultural world gives them
credence, ghosts and other nonmaterial beings have a psychological reality that denotes nothing about a persons sanity or insanityas it might
in a culture where such beings are considered gures of fantasy, imagination, and superstition. Shweder (1991) devotes much space to intentional worlds, a concept reminiscent of Hallowells behavioral environ-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

55

Beyond Imagination

Co

py

ment. Quinn and Holland and other authors in their edited volume
(Holland and Quinn 1987) similarly write about simplied worlds,
taken-for-granted worlds that are culturally modeled.
Cultural schemas or cultural models are stereotypical distillates, generalizations from past experience that people make. They are akin to
what Crapanzano (1990), speaking about the processes that maintain
the self in continual change, calls arrestsrepresentations of self at a
particular time that people try to reassert, even under new conditions.6
According to the cognitive architecture currently theorized for them,
cultural models (conceptualizations of gured worlds) certainly may and
are likely to change (see DAndrade 1995; Quinn and Strauss 1994), but
here we emphasize gured worlds that have some durability; those that
are, for various reasons, reproduced socially. In such cases these culturalist perspectives offer an important grasp on lived worlds.

Ex

am

Despite the importance of these as if meanings, these gurings,


through which lived worlds are interpreted and taken to heart, we cannot stop with culturalist perspectives. We must also appreciate what
social constructivism and practice theories tell us about the imagined
worlds in which we conduct our lives. Figured worlds happen, as social
process and in historical time. For instance, Favret-Saada (1980), in a
vivid practice-theoretical account of witchcraft in the Bocage of France
(ca. 1970), describes the social construction, through discourse, of certain people in the region as actors in a gured world populated by
witches, unwitchers, and the bewitched. She shows how she herself
inadvertently entered the practice of witchcraft by the simple act of
questioning. The kinds of questions she was asking were, it turned out,
meaningful as actsand markers of identityin the gured world of
witchcraft. Only gradually did she realizea realization of great moment
to her ethnographythat she had become (that is, become construed as)
an actor in that world.7
Favret-Saada makes crucial points about living the gured world of
witchcraft. Acts in the world of witchcraft (or any gured world) happen
in time. Caught in the moment, far from a timeless omniscience, the
participants in witchcrafts world experience degrees of uncertainty. They

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

56

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

worry: Will I die from the witchcraft I suspect has been worked upon
me? Do these people suspect that I am a witch? Am I suffering misfortune after misfortune because someone wishes me ill?
Another part of this uncertainty is the very multiplicity and partiality
of the gured worlds that hedge the interpretation of action. It is often
unclear which (or how many) gurations are instanced by interaction.
Was that persons intent gaze a sign of witchcraft directed at me, a mark
of interest in the color of my dress, or simply an accidental glance,
looking through me to the thought beyond? Was she (at this time, in
this place) acting as a witch, an admirer or critic, or an uninterested
bystander?
Favret-Saadas work dramatically exemplies the ways gured worlds
are encountered in day-to-day social activity. Her work, with its constructivist dimension, directs attention to the living of gured worlds
through practices and activities. Holland and Eisenhart (1990), in their
ethnographic study at American universities, found that women spent a
great deal of time, alone and together, in activities devoted to improving
their bodies and appearance. Motivations for these beautication practices were expressed via the discourse of romance. Attractiveness to men
was an important quality in the students world of romance. A womans
prestige derived largely from the treatment she received from men, and
that treatment was formative of and responsive to her attractiveness.
Attractiveness was, in Bourdieus terms, a symbolic capital of the eld.
The endless energy and hours spent on beautication made sense in such
a world.8
Activities as a meaningful context of action have been a traditional
theoretical concern of the sociohistorical school for some sixty or seventy years. Leontiev and many others have conceived peoples actions
and their individual development within the larger frame of a historically
assembled, socially and culturally constructed activity. These activities
(work, play, teaching are often the matters of interest in this literature)9
establish particular sets of roles, actors, institutions, settings, durations,
and organizational requirements (see Wertsch 1981; Engestrm 1990,
1992).
Drawing a link between identities and activities similar to the one we
propose, Lave and Wenger (1991) begin to explore the consequences of
self-development in and through activities. Lave and Wenger are concerned with situated learning in communities of practicethat is,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

57

Ex

am

Co

py

with how newcomers are inducted into socially enduring and complex
activities. They conceive learning as simultaneously a form of association (situation), an act of recruitment, and a (re)constitution of the basis
for communities. They follow the learning of neophytes brought into
communities of practitioners who may tailor in Liberia, deliver babies
in the Yucatn, or navigate large vessels in the U.S. Navy. Communities
of practice recast the practices and productions of working groups as
power/knowledge (to use Foucaults term), that is, as knowledge which
cannot be divorced from position, and as position married to knowledge,
within social groups. Identities become important outcomes of participation in communities of practice in ways analogous to our notion that
identities are formed in the process of participating in activities organized by gured worlds.
Goodenough (1994) suggests that we should theorize culture in relation to activities. Peoples activitieseven within the small, isolated
communities, crisscrossed by multi-stranded social relations, so familiar
to ethnographersdemand and are organized around different sets of
situated understandings and expectations. There is, Goodenough concludes, no uniform, consistent, or coherent set of meaningsno culturethat applies equally in every activity. Goodenoughs notion of
activities is reminiscent of the concept dened by the sociohistoric
school, and it has inspired intriguing work on the development of consciousness (see Amit-Talai 1995).10 But the sociohistorical schools concept of activity remains more useful to us. Because of its roots in Marxian
analyses of capitalism and other historically specic modes of production, activity theory pays more attention to the articulation of activities
within larger systems of power and privilege.11 This embedding of activities is central to an understanding of gured worlds as well: as we shall
see in Chapter 7, the world of romance and attractiveness plays a prominent role in the production and reproduction of gender privilege in the
United States.

Power and Privilege


The relationships of the practices and activities that instance gured
worlds with larger, institutionalized structures of power confer a depth
to gured landscapes that extends beyond the immediate order of interaction. Imagined acts, courses and places of action, actors, and even the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

58

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

whole of a gured world take on an element of rank and status according


to this relational hierarchy. None of the concepts we have so far used to
elucidate social construction in gured worlds accounts well for this
aspect of hierarchy and status. Bourdieus concept of eld helps to bring
out this additional aspect of gured worlds.
Like habitus, eld is too potent and pervasive a theme of Bourdieus
thought to be precisely delimited. Bourdieu uses the world of authors
and writings as an example:

am

Co

What do I mean by eld? As I use the term, a eld is a separate social


universe having its own laws of functioning independent of those of
politics and the economy. The existence of the writer, as fact and as
value, is inseparable from the existence of the literary eld as an
autonomous universe endowed with specic principles of evaluation
of practices and works. To understand Flaubert or Baudelaire, or any
writer, major or minor, is rst of all to understand what the status of
writer consists of at the moment considered; that is, more precisely, the
social conditions of the possibility of this social function, of this social
personage. In fact, the invention of the writer, in the modern sense of
the term, is inseparable from the invention of a particular social game,
which I term the literary eld and which is constituted as it establishes
its autonomy, that is to say, its specic laws of functioning, within the
eld of power. (1993: 162163)

Ex

Fields are not absolutely autonomous, for they subsist in what


Bourdieu calls the eld of power, which is itself an aspect of class relations. They are instead relatively autonomous, for the relationship any
eld has to other elds or to the eld of power is refracted by the mode
of cultural production specic to the eld. In this sense the eld is a
game like the games Vygotsky described. Bourdieu further intended the
concept to be a kind of translation of structure that would not stand
apart from persons. A eld is structure-in-practice, and as such is a
world of relationships, of social positions dened only against one another (Bourdieu 1985a). It is also a peopled world; its positions, which
are producers as well as products, are also social personages. Field thus
closely parallels our notion of gured world and elucidates our later
emphasis on positionality (Chapter 6).12
Bourdieu elaborated the concept from his sociological studies of
French arts and letters and the French academic scene as it was in the
1960s. The concept of eld directed attention to the aspects of the collec-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

59

Ex

am

Co

py

tively dened activities that make up the arts and the academy that relate
to what Weber (1978) would have called status.13 Had Bourdieu mediated his understanding through gured world instead of eld, he
would have told us more about the discourses of academia and the
cultural constructions that constituted the familiar aspects of academic
life: the taken-for-granted generic gures (professors, graduate students,
undergraduates, provosts, secretaries) and their generic actsboth such
formal tasks as giving tutorials, administering tests, ring, hiring, and
granting degrees, and the less formal stories of tenure granted, tenure
denied, and teaching responsibilities juggled against writing and scholarly researchas situated in a particular institution.14 He would have
more closely detailed the terms of academic discoursesuch as quality, originality, and brillianceas ways in which academics come to
evaluate their efforts, understand themselves, and interpret the positions
they hold in the academy.
In Homo Academicus Bourdieu directed his eld analysis less to these
day-to-day aspects of cultural guring than to social relations among the
more powerful and inuential players. He paid attention to the positions
of academic personages and institutions in the French educational hierarchy, the markers of position, the symbolic capital accruing to the scholars and institutions, and the cultural productions created by them.15
We learn much from Homo Academicus about the interrelations of scholars and institutions in France according to their relative prestige and
inuence; we learn much less about the day-to-day content of activity
and the ways positions of prestige play out locallyfor the vast number
of academics. Hence we miss, for French academics, the focus that
Favret-Saada provides for French witchcraft: the everyday construction
of actors.16
Although Bourdieus interests in Homo Academicus were directed toward the prominent men and women of the system, a eld analysis is
relevant to any gured world.17 Indeed, as we have said, lived worlds
are organized around positions of status and inuence (in the case of
American college students, attractiveness and prestige; in the case of the
Naudadans of Nepal, caste and wealth as well as gender) and the cultural
narratives that posit particular sorts of characters and their dealings with
one another (the loving boyfriend and the irt; the attentive therapist
and the recalcitrant borderline; the Naudadan husband who threatens
his wife by telling her he will bring another woman into the house).

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

60

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

We agree with Bourdieus vision: the as-if character of possibility that


marks elds (and gured worlds) is not an indifferent, mental abstraction, an imaginary in its usual sense, but a social reality that lives
within dispositions mediated by relations of power. This point is most
clearly made in Bourdieus analyses of silencing or self-censorship, a
theme later amplied by Skinner and Holland in their exploration of
womens experience in Nepal. Indeed, positions that have become dispositions, of privilege and prestige are especially important to our case
analyses of the worlds of romance in the United States and gender relations in Nepal.18 Identities form in these gured worlds through the
day-to-day activities undertaken in their name. Neophytes are recruited
into and gain perspective on such practices and come to identify themselves as actors of more or less inuence, more or less privilege, and
more or less power in these worlds.
As we situate it among the related concepts of elds, practices, activities, and communities of practice, the place of gured worlds takes a
clearer shape. It is a landscape of objectied (materially and perceptibly expressed) meanings, joint activities, and structures of privilege and
inuenceall partly contingent upon and partly independent of other
gured worlds, the interconnections among gured worlds, and larger
societal and trans-societal forces. Figured worlds in their conceptual
dimensions supply the contexts of meaning for actions, cultural productions, performances, disputes, for the understandings that people come
to make of themselves, and for the capabilities that people develop to
direct their own behavior in these worlds.19 Materially, gured worlds are
manifest in peoples activities and practices; the idioms of the world
realize selves and others in the familiar narratives and everyday performances that constantiate relative positions of inuence and prestige. Figured worlds provide the contexts of meaning and action in which social
positions and social relationships are named and conducted. They also
provide the loci in which people fashion senses of selfthat is, develop
identities.

Artifacts and Materiality


Figured worlds rely upon artifacts. The sociohistorical school has long
emphasized the importance of artifacts as mediators in human action, as
psychological tools (Kozulin 1986). We followed this lead in Chapters

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

61

am

Co

py

1 and 2. Now we are in a better position to explain the signicance of


this point. Figured worlds are evinced in practice through the artifacts
employed by people in their performances. Such artifacts are pivotal in
the sense Vygotsky attributed to them in play. Artifacts open up gured
worlds. They are the means by which gured worlds are evoked, collectively developed, individually learned, and made socially and personally
powerful.
Cole (1996) provides a useful discussion of artifacts. In his conceptualization, cultural artifactswhether the poker chips and life stories of
Alcoholics Anonymous, the sexy clothes and gender-marked stereotypes of the world of romance, or patients charts and medications in the
world of mental health carehave developmental histories by virtue
of the activities of which they were previously a part and which they
mediate in the present. They have been manufactured or produced and
continue to be used as part of, and in relation to, intentional human
actions. Hence artifacts assume both an obvious and necessary material
aspect and an ideal or conceptual aspect, an intentionality, whose substance is embedded in the gured world of their use. By the same token
they are both instrument and collective remembrance.
Holland and Cole (1995: 482) suggest that we consider a hammer as a
tool/artifact/mediator:

Ex

The generic function of hammers is to connect two or more objects by


driving a nail (or its equivalent) through them. Although hammers can
be used for a (restricted variety) of other tasks (those where hitting
something is functional, as in war; as a door jamb or a paperweight),
their shapes are predominantly shaped by their nail driving function. In
Gibsons (1979) terms, hammers afford hitting nails.
In this respect, every hammer can be seen as an encapsulated theory
of the task and simultaneously a theory of the person who fullls the
task. The head of the hammer is typically made of a hard material so
that it does not shatter when used. The handle is shaped to t the
human hand. Such tailoring can be quite specic, but it can never be
totally general. A tack hammer has not only a small head, but a small
handle that affords light tapping. A railway hammer (a sledge hammer)
has not only a large and heavy head, but a handle of such a shape and
such a length to accommodate a person of sufcient strength and falling within a height range holding it with two hands and swinging it
after raising it over the head with legs spread apart.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

62

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

Note that mediation through artifacts applies equally to objects and to


people. Consider poker chips as they are ordinarily used in Alcoholics
Anonymous. Consider, too, the pronoun most husbands used to address
their wives in Naudada: ta (you), the least respectful form employed also
for children, animals, and other inferiors. Poker chips and pronouns,
through their collective use in common practices, have come to embody
for, and so impose on, people in AA and in Naudada a conception of the
tasks to which they are put, and a conception of the person(s) who will
use them and be the object(s) of them.20
This vantage on cultural artifacts has also been shaped by the critical
disruption and inspired by Foucault. Discourse (or discursive) theory
emphasizes many of the aspects of cultural resources that we discuss
here, especially their existence as public forms and social tools. Discourses and their categories are like the artifacts that Cole describes.
They originate outside their performers and are imposed upon people,
through recurrent institutional treatments and within interaction, to the
point that they become self-administered. Categories carry an association to those who use them and are subject to theman association with
poweras artifacts do an association with tasks and those who perform
them. In fact, for a Vygotskian like Cole, categories of expression and
more tangible artifacts like hammers are the same.21 They afford ways
of doing not only things but also people.
Social constructivists or discourse theorists point to important aspects
of gured worlds and the cultural artifacts that are associated with them.
Although they have usually considered only privileged or hegemonic
discourses, their points apply equally to all cultural artifactsfrom hammers, poker chips, clothing, and psychiatric diagnoses to advertisements and lms. All these have a history of development in relation to
particular tasks, undertaken by people in particular environments.
Just as lms afford particular gazes on their subject matter (see
Traube 1992), poker chips, in their association with the gured world of
Alcoholics Anonymous, afford certain ways of thinking and feeling about
sobriety and about the people who employ them to mediate their relation
to alcohol and drinking. Of course, some AA members-in-the-making
do at times let go of the meaning of the chip, tire of being sober, or
wander away to other pursuits. The point is how often they do not. They
learn to detach themselves from their reactions to earlier understandings
of poker chips, to enter the world of AAa conceptual world that differs

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

63

am

Co

py

from what they knew beforeand so learn to react to objects, to events,


and to themselves as gured in that world. Just as a child enters into a
game.
Cultural artifacts gain a kind of force by connection to their social and
cultural contexts, to their gured worlds. These artifacts and peoples
relationship to them are decidedly unlike the artifacts and relations in
the scenarios of problem-solving, or the conduct of other goal-directed
tasks, imagined by those who subscribe to the dominant perspectives in
psychology. They are very unlikely to be the product of a lone individuals conscious contemplation, of the solution to a problem rationally
dened.22 Rather, as Davies and Harr (1990: 48) put it, actions become
determinant for participants as acts that position them in relation to
the local expressive order which they presume is in use and towards
which they orient themselves.23 In our terms, the actions, the deployments of artifacts such as pronouns and chips, evoke the worlds to
which they were relevant, and position individuals with respect to those
worlds. It is their pivotal role, as Vygotsky called ittheir capacity to
shift the perceptual, cognitive, affective, and practical frame of activity
that makes cultural artifacts so signicant in human life.
To attend to the materiality of cultural artifacts is also to recognize the
force of their use in practicespractices responsive to changing historical circumstances (see Holland and Cole 1995). The conceptual and
material aspects of gured worlds, and of the artifacts through which
they are evinced, are constantly changing through the improvisations of
actors. This context of ux is the ground for identity development. It sets
the conditions for what we called in Chapter 2 the authoring selfwhat
in Chapter 8 we will expand to call the space of authoring.

Ex

The Paradoxical Possibility of Self-Control


The authoring self becomes a possibility because of another aspect of
cultural artifacts. Cultural artifacts were central to Vygotskys conceptualization not only because they could be the media of consciousness, of
higher mental functions, but because, as such, they could serve as
tools of liberation from control by environmental stimuli. As we use
artifacts to affect others, we become, at some point in our growing up,
aware of and capable of using artifacts to affect ourselves. We achieve
self-control, albeit of a very limited sort, by the mediation of our

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

64

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

thoughts and feelings through artifacts. We learn how to control ourselves from the outside, so to speak (Vygotsky 1978); we learn how to
position ourselves for ourselves.
Social constructivists consider discourses principally as the means by
which power/knowledge (or some equivalent) inscribes bodies and
(thus) creates persons. Vygotsky conceived cultural artifacts as learned
within social interaction; they serve rst as interpsychic tools and only
afterwardwhen one takes them as meaningful for oneselfdo they
function on intimate terrain. Although Vygotsky and the other members
of the sociohistorical school sought to build a psychology in accordance
with Marxist thought, they emphasized a side of Marxs work that is
often forgotten these days: the possibilities for becoming, and the sense of
freedom so pervasive in the period immediately after the Revolution.
They paid most attention to humans potential to expand, rather than
limit, their abilities and horizons. Foucault (among others) would later
elaborate the controlling aspects of human artifacts, the inscriptions of
discourses and the impositions of discipline. The sociohistorical school,
initially at least, took the path of optimism, and explored the potential
for the liberation and expansion of human capacities that artifactual
mediation afforded. Its members mapped the routes of escape from the
tyranny of environmental stimuli (including, presumably, the stimuli of
the social environment).
Here we have a paradox. As Shepel (1995: 428) puts it: The accumulation and mastery of a cultural tool kit and its use in overcoming the
dependency on a particular culture is one of the basic contradictions of
human development. How does liberation from the particular determinationsthe entrapmentsof our cultural worlds come about through
the tools shaped in those worlds for their perpetuation? In our case
studies we follow this paradox and play out this contradiction. By creating and embracing an identity in the world of Alcoholics Anonymous,
the former drinker can achieve a modicum of control over his relation
to alcohol, but in so doing he learns a kind of discipline, a way of looking
and behaving that often condemns what his friends, and often his other
selves, would have him do. Through participation in the womens
groups that create songs envisioning a new world of gender relations in
Nepal, individual women nd a place to stand, a perspective on themselves and their lives not otherwise offered in the texts and rituals of
their daily lives. They achieve their standpoint, however, only by submit-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Figured Worlds

65

Ex

am

Co

py

ting themselves to another set of cultural forms that have their own
peculiar limitations and constraints (see Chapters 10 and 12).
The next two chapters concern the lived worlds of romance and Alcoholics Anonymous, and how people take form, building understandings
of themselves as agents, patients, and instruments24 in these gured
worlds. These understandingsthese identitiesare unstable, especially as people are rst inducted to a gured world, and they continue to
undergo heuristic development in concert with peoples acclimation to
new spheres of activity. They remain multiple, as peoples trajectories
through gured worlds neither take one path nor remain in the ambit of
one cultural space, one gured world. Nonetheless, identities constitute
an enduring and signicant aspect of history-in-person, history that is
brought to current situations. They are a pivotal element of the perspective that persons bring to the construal of new activities and even new
gured worlds.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

Personal Stories in
Alcoholics Anonymous

Ex

am

Consider a group of men and women who are becoming alcoholics, not
by drinking, but rather by learning not to drink. These individuals have
decided to stop drinking because they have come to understand that
alcohol is controlling their lives. The change they undergo is much
more than a change in behavior. It is a transformation of their identities,
from drinking non-alcoholics to non-drinking alcoholics, and it affects
how they view and act in the world. It requires not only a particular
understanding of the world but a new understanding of their selves and
their lives and a reinterpretation of their own pasts. They enter, or rather
are recruited to, a new gured world, a new frame of understanding. One
device in particular that helps one identify oneself in this world is the
telling of a particular sort of personal story.
The identity and interpretation at which these men and women arrive
are ones dened by the self-help group Alcoholics Anonymous.1 In this
sense AA has created a cultural world, albeit a limited one, which is its
institutional reason for being. AA has constructed a particular interpretation of what it means to be an alcoholic, what typical alcoholics are like,
and what kinds of incidents mark a typical alcoholics life. This cultural
knowledge about alcoholism and the alcoholic shared by members of AA
differs both from the cultural knowledge of alcoholism shared by those
outside of AA and from the self-understanding of most potential members before they enter AA. The self-understandings of the individuals
who join AA must come to reect and incorporate the knowledge organized within AAs gured world; cultural knowledge must become selfknowledge. For some, though not all, self-understanding is transformed.
66
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

67

Ex

am

Co

py

One symbolic device that helps mediate this transformation is the personal stories told by members of Alcoholics Anonymous.
In the culture of middle-class America drinking is ambivalently
viewed. In many contexts it is a marker of adulthood and a frequent part
of social interaction. But drinking too much or too often, or behaving
inappropriately while drinking, may place one in the stigmatized status
of problem drinker or alcoholic. No distinct line runs between social or normal drinker and problem drinker or alcoholic, and
different sectors of U.S. society do not agree on what these terms mean.
In the past alcoholism was widely interpreted as a defect of character
or moral weakness. To some extent this view persists (Robins 1980), but
since the early 1940s an interpretation of alcoholism as a disease has
largely replaced it. This reects the general Western trend toward medicalizing deviance (Conrad and Schneider 1980). Alternative interpretations are possible. Like the mentally retarded people Edgerton (1967)
describes, whose disorder is largely an inadequacy in social conduct, the
alcoholic fails to meet social obligations. Alcoholics are labeled alcoholic
by others when their behavior becomes problematic by normal standards, not on the basis of the volume of alcohol drunk or the frequency
of drunkenness. Alcoholism describes a tendency toward inappropriate, out-of-control behavior.
Nor is the deviance simply a quality of the acts or behaviors so labeled.
It is a consequence of the breaking of rules and sanctions created by a
group that is in a position of power, and that nds the behavior unsettling. Deviance is a matter of politics (Schur 1980). As MacAndrew and
Edgerton (1969) argue, drunken comportment is learned, and varies
cross-culturally. Americans drunkenness is a learned behavior and has
its appropriate and inappropriate forms. Alcoholics are a labeled group of
people who behave inappropriately after drinking alcohol.
There is disagreement on who should be categorized as a problem
drinker or alcoholic. A successful writer who nds she writes better with
a few drinks, a college student who only gets drunk on the weekends, a
businessman who drinks to loosen up in social situationsthese people
may not consider themselves alcoholics, but those around them may
consider them so. Certainly there is a signicant difference between
members of Alcoholics Anonymous and the urban nomads described by
Spradley in You Owe Yourself a Drunk (1970). Yet the people in both
groups are labeled alcoholics.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

68

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

Once the drinker is labeled as an alcoholic he may or may not accept


this label himself, and it may or may not become an identity. Even should
he accept this interpretation of his drinking behavior, so long as the
interpretation remains unassimilated to a gured world such as that
created by AA, he need not see it as an aspect of himself that carries over
into other areas of his life. He may even learn about AA without that
knowledge reorganizing or transforming his own understanding of himself.2 By identity we mean the way a person understands and views
himself, and is often viewed by others, at least in certain situationsa
perception of self that can be fairly constantly achieved (see Schwartz
and Merten 1968). These forms of self-understanding are always construed relative to a gured world of social life. An alcoholic, a father,
and a judge are all particular answers to the question Who am I?
(Kuhn and McPartland 1954), where the implicit condition is relative
to such and such a social world.
These identities affect how a person acts in the world. If a drinker does
accept the label of alcoholic as applying to himself, and accepts and acts
on the identity of an alcoholic, this transformation of identity, from a
drinking non-alcoholic (normal drinker) to an alcoholic, requires a
radical reinterpretation of who he is, of self. It requires a new way of
guring the activity of drinking, those who drink, and the place of drinking in a broader social and personal context. This process of reinterpretation of self, the formation of a new identity, is a major component of
Alcoholics Anonymous.

AA and the Disease of Alcoholism

Ex

Members of Alcoholics Anonymous have found a particular denition of


a problem and a particular way to deal with it. AA constitutes a gured
world, a meaning system within a meaning system. It draws upon specic sets of interpretations and assumptions which circulate alongside
many others in the United States. AA is the best-known program for
attempting to deal with alcoholism and is widely held to be the most
successful, although there is no agreed-upon way to measure this claim.
Many of its principles, and often the program itself, are used in treatment
centers for alcohol and drug addiction.
From its beginning AA used an illness model to describe alcoholism.
Peterson (1988) traces this terminology to AAs roots in the Oxford
Group Movement of the 1930s. This religious movement in the United

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

69

Ex

am

Co

py

States and England drew an analogy between sin and illness in a program
of personal evangelism. Early AA members, including the founders, participated in Oxford Group meetings and were inuenced by the oral
tradition and terminology and the methods of personal contact. The
original medical model of alcoholism used by AA was borrowed from the
medical model of sin popular in the Oxford Groups. The original methods section of Alcoholics Anonymous (published in 1939) uses the words
illness, malady, and habit to describe alcoholism; the word disease appears only once, to describe alcoholism as a spiritual disease
(Peterson 1988: 5). Later, as the medicalized view of alcoholism as a
disease became popular, it spread back to AA from the medical profession (John Peterson, personal communication).
AA members are told that alcoholism is a progressive and incurable
disease that, if unchecked, is fatal. The drinking alcoholic is powerless
over alcohol, and out of control. Once one becomes an alcoholic, ones
whole life is centered around alcohol, and once the drinking alcoholic
takes one drink, she cannot resist the next, or the next . . . The disease
affects all areas of ones life, and evidence for alcoholism cannot be found
in any one symptom but rather in the life as a whole. The only way to
arrest the disease is to stop drinking completely, and since the alcoholic
is powerless to resist alcohol, the only way to do this is to turn ones life
and will over to a Higher Power. The individuals Higher Power may be
God, the AA group, or any other conception of something larger than
self. The surrender must be complete, and can only be accomplished
when the drinker is ready to stop drinking, when she admits not only
that she may have a problem with drinking but that she is powerless
over alcohol and that her life has become unmanageable (AA 1976).
For this reason the drinker must hit bottom before she can stop; it is
not enough that others want her to stop. The bottom is not directly
determined by how much one drinks, but by what alcohol has done in
ones life:
Whether or not you are an alcoholic is not determined by where you
drink, when you started drinking, how long youve been drinking, with
whom you drink, what, or even how much. The true test is in the
answer to this question: What has alcohol done to you? If it has affected
your relationships with your family, friends, former or present employers; if it has inuenced the way you schedule your days; if it has affected
your health; if it determines or affects your nondrinking moods or your

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

70

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds


state of mind; if you are in any way preoccupied with alcoholthen the
likelihood is that you have a problem. (AA 1979c: 3)

Ex

am

Co

py

Hitting bottom is a point of crisis in the drinkers life. It is a point at


which life apparently cannot go on as it has. And if the drinker can
understand drinking as the source of the problem, then drinking is the
behavior that must change. One way AA members deal with their problem is to stop drinking. But becoming sober in AA involves much more
than not drinking. Since drinking affected all areas of ones life, sobriety
must also affect all areas of life. Sobriety, when talked about by AA
members, means a new way of life, with spiritual aspects of surrender to
God or a Higher Power, humility, trust, honesty, and making amends for
wrongs committed in the past or present. The self-understanding, the
identity of a sober AA alcoholic ideally includes all of these as well as a
particular drinking history. There are therefore two aspects important to
membership in AA: qualication as an alcoholic, based on ones past, and
continued non-drinking behavior, or effort at not drinking, which is a
negation of the behavior that rst qualied one for membership.3
Since alcoholism is an incurable disease, once one is an alcoholic one
remains an alcoholic for the rest of ones life. The AA member comes to
see not only his drinking as alcoholic but his self as an alcoholic. The
disease is a part of ones self. Alcoholism is not something one has; rather
an alcoholic is what one is. This is a situation in which many of ones
basic assumptions must be changed or reorganized, in which one must
learn a new understanding of ones problem, ones self, and what the
world is like. Bateson (1972) addresses this transformation that members of AA must undergo as a change in epistemology. We address it as a
change in identity, that is, of self-understanding within a new guration
of the world.
As a cultural system, and one that no one is born into, this entire
gured world of AA is new to neophytes. The propositions and interpretations of events and experiences, the appropriate behaviors and values
of an AA alcoholic, and the appropriate placement of the alcoholic identity in the hierarchy of identities one holds must be learned. In short, an
AA identity must be constructed and its moral and aesthetic distinctions
made into personal knowledge. This cultural (and literal) information is
communicated through the AA literature, and through talk in AA meetings and one-to-one interactions. One important vehicle for this communication is the personal story.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

71

Ex

am

Co

py

In personal stories, oldtimers in AA tell their own life stories or part of


themtheir drinking historiesand how they came to be involved in
AA. These stories contribute to cultural production and reproduction in
three ways. First, they objectify many cultural elementsthe beliefs,
propositions, and interpretationsthat new or potential AA members
learn through listening. Second, by virtue of their narrative forms, the
stories realize a model of what alcoholism is and what it means to be an
alcoholic. This model, in the absence of a well-dened, well-elaborated
model shared by the wider culture, provides a basis for potential members to identify themselves as alcoholics, through comparing their lives
to those in the stories. The storyline also provides a basis for labeling
others, as others compare the lives of suspected alcoholics to the AA
lives. Third, the AA story is a tool of subjectivity, a mediating device for
self-understanding. As the AA member learns the AA story model, by
listening to and telling stories, and comes to place the events and experiences of his own life into the model, he learns to understand his own life
as an AA life, and himself as an AA alcoholic. The personal story is a
cultural vehicle for identity formation.
In AA, personal stories are told for the explicit, stated purpose of
providing an understanding of alcoholism, so that other drinkers may
nd so much of themselves in the lives of professed alcoholics that they
cannot help asking whether they, too, are alcoholics. Since the denition
of an alcoholic is not agreed upon in the wider society, arriving at this
interpretation of events is a process negotiated between the drinker and
those around her. AA stories provide a set of criteria by which the alcoholic can be identied. Like Ecuadorian illness stories or the stories told
by the Ifaluk in recounting troublesome family situations (Price 1987;
Lutz 1987), they provide a means of socially negotiating the interpretations of events. For potential members, who may have been labeled by
others as alcoholic, listening to AA stories provides an alternative understanding for reinterpreting their own behaviors that more closely
matches the interpretations others have made of them.
Simply learning the propositions about alcoholism and its nature is
not enough. The drinker must apply them to his own life, and this
application must be demonstrated. Scott (1969) notes that, for workers
in blindness agencies, one key indicator of the success of rehabilitation is
that the client comes to understand himself and his problem from the
workers perspective. Similarly, in AA, success or recovery requires coming to perceive oneself and ones problems from an AA perspective. AA

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

72

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

members must learn to experience their problems as drinking problems,


and themselves as alcoholics. This is why, as members often say, AA is
for those who want it, not for those who need it. AA members must
agree to become tellers, as well as listeners, of AA stories.
Telling AA stories is a way of demonstrating that one has mastered, or
at least has begun to gain facility with, the appropriate understandings.
Telling an appropriate story is thus a means of gaining some validation
from listeners for ones AA identity. But telling is an active process. It is a
process of construction. Using the AA model and applying it to her own
life, the drinker comes to understand better how and why she is an
alcoholic. She locates herself within the gured world of AA. The stories
are used in what is simultaneously a social and a cognitive and affective, personal process. In the process of identity formation, the AA member undergoes a kind of reorientation in her self-understandings, a detachment from identities subsisting in other gured worlds, followed
by the reconstitutiona process not only of learning but also of valuation, indeed elevationof an identity predicated within AAs world. She
accomplishes this transition primarily by reinterpreting her life as an
AA story.

am

The Process of Identity Formation

Ex

Arnold van Gennep, in Rites of Passage (1960), described the patterns of


movement from one position within society to another. The ceremonies
that take place at these points in an individuals life ritually express the
transition from one status to another. The status movements in van
Genneps scheme are spatial and structural changes. Rites of separation,
transition, and incorporation move the individual from one condition to
another and cushion the disturbance in the equilibrium of both the
individual and the group. Van Genneps concern, reecting the interests
of the time in which he wrote, was with the impact of these rites on the
social grouphow the group marks the movement of individuals between subgroups. He did not ask how the rituals transform the individuals image of himself, his identity.
Schwartz and Merten (1968), in contrast, address the question of how
an initiation ritual transforms the initiates self-image and social identity.
They develop a scheme for identity transformation in which the initiate
undergoes what they term identity diffusion and identity reconstitu-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

73

Ex

am

Co

py

tion. In their example of a high school sorority, identity diffusion takes


place as hazing disrupts the initiates perception of herself, raising doubts
about the superior ability she had assumed made her sorority material.
This disruption opens her to the possibility of a radical reinterpretation
of self. During a rite called mock the initiate is forced to take on
exaggerated attributes of the morally devalued groups against which
socies dene themselves: hoods and others. Highly stylized and
stigmatized negative features of dress, hairstyle, and behavior associated
with the hood and other identities are attached temporarily to the
potential socies, expressing what these girls fear are latent possibilities
in their own character.
Reconstitution takes place as the negative is exorcized and the initiate
is allowed to return to her normal state. This use of expressive, enacted
symbols is more than a representation to the community of a transition.
When the initiate attaches to all the negative possibilities she has feared
in herself, and realizes that this attachment is a transient state, the resumption of normal dress is accompanied by a transformation in her
image of herself: now she is forever cleansed of any possible latent moral
impurities, and her identity as a socie is publicly conrmed. She is
ready for induction (see Chapter 11 for more on this process of loosening
oneself, or being loosened, from a gured world).
Schwartz and Mertens scheme for identity (trans)formation applies to
cases in which the new self-understanding can develop only at the expense of an existing one. This is true for people who come into Alcoholics Anonymous: the old conception of self in relation to drinking is
weakened through a process of discrediting not only the old identity as
such but also the gured world that gives it meaning. We conceive this
step as devaluation, rather than diffusion, as Schwartz and Merten call
it. In the formation of a new identity an individual comes, with the
social encouragement and insistence of others, to interpret the world
in new ways, and to position herself and emotionally invest herself in
that world. Individuals do this through participating in group activities,
learning to produce and enact cultural forms particular to that world,
and taking up these forms as devices for mediating their own conception
of self and world.
For AA, the process of identity devaluation must begin before one
enters the program. For a drinker to have reason to come to AA, she, or
those around her, must realize that she is not what she thought she was,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

74

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

a normal drinker. A normal drinker has no reason to go to AA. But if


she has begun to suspect that she may have a drinking problem, or if
those around her have begun to point out that drinking may be the
source of problems, or even identify her as an alcoholic, then some
disruption in the previous perception of herself as normal drinker has
occurred. It may be that she does not yet understand or desire the identity of an alcoholic, but the disruption opens the possibility of this transformation in self-image.
Once the drinker makes contact with AA and begins to participate in
meetings, she must come to understand drinking and alcoholism as AA
understands them, and must come to see herself as a potential AA member. This involves making an appropriate connection between alcohol
and the problems it has led to. She must replace her previous image of an
alcoholic as a drunk or Skid Row bum with the AA denition of an ill
person, learning to see the identity of an AA (non-drinking) alcoholic as
desirable, and learning to see herself as like or potentially like those who
hold this identity.
Making the appropriate connection between alcohol and the problems
one is having requires seeing the problems as negative effects of drinking. This often leads the drinker to rethink who she is or why she drinks.
Most of the personal stories told in AA include a segment in which the
drinker justies her drinking when people point out its negative effects.
She blames her drinking on her problems, for example, rather than seeing her problems as the effects of drinking. This is one example of how
the connection may be interpreted in ways inappropriate to AA. Other
examples are given in published AA stories. Jim saw himself as a person
for whom trouble was a natural part of his personality, and saw drinking
as one aspect of that. When he joined AA he had to learn to understand
himself as an alcoholic for whom trouble was an aspect of his drinking
(AA 1979c). Similarly, a woman who understood drinking as one more
symbol of [her] neurosis had to come to understand her neurosis as one
more symptom of her alcoholism (AA 1968a).
Learning the appropriate storyline of an alcoholic is also represented
in the AA stories. At rst a potential member may reject the suggestion
that he may be alcoholic, and that AA may be relevant to him. Typically,
his conception of an alcoholic is someone who lives on the bowery and
panhandles for a drink, and he believes this person arrived there because
of a moral weakness or lack of willpower. He must learn that alcoholism

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

75

Ex

am

Co

py

is a disease that can happen to anyone (AA 1967, 1968b), and he must
sensitize himself to the appropriate evidence for it. He must also see the
identity of AA alcoholic as a desirable one. As expressed in AA literature
and AA talk, the program cannot work unless a person wants to stop
drinking, and this change usually occurs when a person hits bottom.
Finally, the drinker must see the identity of AA alcoholic as one that is
possible for him to attain. Cain heard several members say that even after
they admitted that they had drinking problems and that AA was relevant
to them they still could not identify with AA oldtimers: They had their
lives so together, and I was so fucked up.
Personalization of the identity takes place as the initiate begins to
identify with AA members. Through comparing his life to theirs, he sees
that other members are like him and he is like them. As he becomes
involved in AA, he learns to introduce himself as an alcoholic every time
he speaks. The behaviors that mark progress in the AA program become
subjective mediating devices; they become means by which the new
member directs and evaluates his own behavior. At meetings he picks up
colored chips, amid applause, to mark the amount of time he has been
without a drink. If he has a slip and takes a drink, he must start over
with a white chip, or clean chip. He works his way through the Twelve
Steps that guide, and mark ones progress in, sober living. The identity of
alcoholic gains salience for him and takes a more central place in his
conception of himself.
He becomes emotionally attached to the identity of AA alcoholic as
he begins to care how others in the group perceive him. He comes to
understand himself as a non-drinking alcoholic, and to reinterpret his
past life as evidence. As he becomes an oldtimer in AA, he tells his
personal story to newcomers to help them identify themselves as alcoholics. As Schwartz and Merten argue that identity reconstitution in
sorority initiation takes place through the attaching and exorcizing of
negative identities, we argue that identity reconstitution in AA takes
place through reinterpretation of self and of ones life, and that the major
vehicle for this reinterpretation is the AA personal story.
As mentioned earlier, there are two important dimensions to the identity of AA alcoholic. The rst distinction AA makes is between alcoholic
and non-alcoholic, where alcoholic refers to a state that, once attained, is
not reversible. The second is between drinking and non-drinking, and
refers to a potentially controllable activity. Normal drinkers, in the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

76

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

A Learned Form

Co

py

gured world of AA, can control their drinking, while alcoholic drinkers
cannot; in order to stop drinking, AA members must turn their will and
lives over to a Higher Power. Together these two dimensions divide
people into four types, one of which is the AA alcoholic identity. For
continuing membership in AA, one must qualify as an alcoholic, based
on ones past, and make a continued effort at not drinking. The normative AA identity requires a behaviornot drinkingthat is a negation of
the behavior that originally qualied one for membership. One of the
functions of the AA personal story is to establish both aspects of membership in an individual. This parallels Schwartz and Merten in that the
AA story serves to attach what has now become understood as negative
behaviorattributes of the active alcoholicto the member, but demonstrates that these are in the past. In their place it attaches the attributes of
the non-drinking alcoholic, as demonstrated in behaviors appropriate to
AA and involvement in AA activities. Not the least of these activities is
the telling of the personal story.

Ex

am

AA dedicates a signicant amount of meeting time and publishing space


to the telling of personal stories. In these stories AA members relate their
own drinking histories, how they came to understand that they are alcoholics, how they got into AA, and what their life has been like since they
joined. From the founding of AA, the telling of personal stories has
been both the major vehicle for carrying the message to alcoholics who
still suffer (the Twelfth Step), and an important tool for maintaining
sobriety.
Ever since Bill W. overcame the compulsion to drink by telling his
own story to Dr. Bob in May 1935, these cofounders and those who have
followed them in the program have claimed that telling their own stories
to other alcoholics, and thus helping other alcoholics to achieve sobriety, is an important part of maintaining their own sobriety (AA 1976;
Thomsen 1975). As one member told Cain, We keep what we have by
giving it away.
As a tool for carrying the message, the personal story helps alcoholics
identify themselves as such. In a pamphlet directed at young people,
Carmen says: I heard a woman begin her story with her teenage drinking. It sounded familiar. Perhaps I might become an alcoholic, I thought.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

77

Ex

am

Co

py

Perhaps I might already be one (AA 1979c: 20). The role of the personal
story in helping others to identify is important because of the AA test for
alcoholism. Evidence is found not in how much you drink, or how often,
but in what alcohol has done to you (AA 1979b). The evidence for
alcoholism must be found in ones entire life, and the personal story
provides a prototypic alcoholic life for comparison with the potential
members own. Hearing AA stories, the drinking alcoholic thinks, Weve
done the same things. This person is like me, and so, Maybe I am an
alcoholic.
This aspect of personal stories accounts for their widespread telling
in AA. Alcoholics Anonymous (1976), the Big Book, rst published
in 1939, includes twenty-nine personal stories of some of AAs earliest
members, including Bill W. and Dr. Bob. These stories are prefaced by the
hope that many alcoholic men and women, desperately in need, will see
these pages, and we believe that it is only by fully disclosing ourselves
and our problems that they will be persuaded to say, Yes I am one of
them too; I must have this thing (29). This early use of personal stories
has been continued in much of the AA literature published since. Pamphlets written for specic audiences (young people, women, prison inmates, the elderly) devote considerable space to personal stories. Biographies of the cofounders and comic-book style publications are other
forms the AA story takes.
AA members tell personal stories formally in speakers meetings. In
this type of meeting, speakers are prearranged, and, other than a short
opening and closing, the meeting consists entirely of these tellings.
These meetings are the most likely meetings for newcomers to attend,
and often form the outsiders impression of what an AA meeting is like.
Less formally, members tell shortened versions of their stories at discussion meetings. Sometimes, for example, when a newcomer is present,
each oldtimer in the group will tell, in short form, how he or she got
into AA.
Another important context for telling personal stories is Twelfth Step
calls. When AA members talk to outsiders who may be alcoholics in a
one-to-one interaction, they are following the last of the Twelve Steps,
carrying the message to alcoholics who still suffer. When called, AA
members will meet with nonmembers who are having problems related
to drinking. Ideally, at these individual meetings, the member tells his
story, describes the AA program, tries to help the drinker see herself as an

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

78

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

alcoholic if she is ready. The Big Book chapter entitled Working


with Others provides instructions for telling the personal story in this
context:

am

Co

py

See your man alone, if possible. At rst engage in general conversation.


After a while, turn the talk to some phase of drinking. Tell him enough
about your drinking habits, symptoms, and experiences to encourage
him to speak of himself. If he wishes to talk, let him do so. You will thus
get a better idea of how you ought to proceed. If he is not communicative, give him a sketch of your drinking career up to the time you quit.
But say nothing, for the moment, of how that was accomplished . . .
When he sees you know all about the drinking game, commence to
describe yourself as an alcoholic. Tell him how bafed you were, how
you nally learned that you were sick. Give him an account of the
struggles you made to stop. Show him the mental twist which leads to
the rst drink of a spree . . .
If he is an alcoholic, he will understand you at once. He will match
your mental inconsistencies with some of his own. If you are satised
that he is a real alcoholic, begin to dwell on the hopeless feature of the
malady . . . And be careful not to brand him an alcoholic. Let him draw
his own conclusion . . .
Continue to speak of alcoholism as a sickness, a fatal malady. Talk
about the conditions of body and mind which accompany it. Keep his
attention focused mainly on your personal experience. Explain that
many are doomed who never realize their predicament . . . You will
soon have your friend admitting that he has many, if not all, of the traits
of the alcoholic . . . Even if your protg may not have entirely admitted
his condition, he has become very curious to know how you got well
. . . Tell him exactly what happened to you. (1976: 9193)

Ex

These instructions give an indication of the structure of the AA personal story. There is a general structure, in the form of a scenario in
which the decision to join AA is made. This general structure is what we
will call the AA story. Apparently the story was consciously developed
from the beginning as a vehicle for presenting an experience or person
with whom the prospect could identify. Interviews published in 1980
show that Dr. Bob and early group members experimented with various
explanations of alcoholismincluding an allergy explanation and an
analogy to diabetesand methods of attracting prospects to their program. Members of the emerging group worked together to perfect their

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

79

Ex

am

Co

py

techniques: We talked about how to get more members and how to


handle them. Then we talked about the mistakes we had made in telling
our stories. We didnt hesitate to criticize each other. We suggested certain words to leave out and certain words to add in order to make a more
effective talk. It made a bunch of amateur psychologists and after-dinner
speakers out of us (AA 1980: 114). Since the time being described is
before the writing of the Big Book, it seems safe to conclude that the
framework for the AA story and the AA word choice were worked out in
practical presentations and critiques by emerging groups long before the
Big Book, with its personal stories, was published (John Peterson,
personal communication).
The AA story usually begins with the teller categorizing his early
drinking either as normal social drinking or as alcoholic from the start.
He then describes how the drinking progressed, leading to negative effects such as loss of jobs, domestic problems, arrests, car accidents, or
hospitalizations. Still, the drinking continues, even gets worse. The
drinker justies it by blaming it and its negative effects on circumstances
or other people, or by explaining what drinking does for him. Perhaps
drinking provides escape from pressures, or perhaps it makes him feel
condent in situations in which he is usually insecure. People begin to
point out that he may have a drinking problem, and perhaps they suggest
that he try AA. He rejects the suggestion, though he may go to an AA
meeting or talk to some AA members. He believes alcoholics are hopeless drunken bums, and since he is not that, he thinks AA has nothing to
offer him.
His drinking continues, and with it the negative effects. His struggles
to control it fail. Finally he hits bottom, recognizes that he is powerless
over alcohol, and gives AA an honest try. He nds out that members are
really just like him. His denition of an alcoholic changes from the
drunken bum to the everyday person aficted with a disease. The story
ends with a description of what AA has done for him and what his life is
like now. A typical example from the pamphlet Young People and AA
illustrates the structure. This is Als personal story:
My drinking resulted in trouble from the very rst. I was a sophomore
in high school when I went to my rst prom. We brought the girls
home early and then went to one of the guys homes. His parents
happened to be away that weekend. We all did some very heavy drink-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds


ing, nally winding up by taking it straight from the bottle. That night,
at the age of 14, I experienced my rst blackout.
The next seven years, my drinking and trouble got progressively
worse. All through high school, I drank whenever the opportunity
presented itself. I managed to secure phony proof of age when I was 15,
so I could get served in bars. At 16, I bought my rst car and began to
mix drinking and driving, with the usual results.
I gained admission to a very good college. I knew why I was going to
collegeto have a good time and get a degree. If I got an education in
the process, then that would be a fringe benet. I got into the best
fraternity on campus. Most of the brothers were athletes; the remainder,
party boys. Since I had little athletic promise, I joined the ranks of the
party boys.
Success in school was measured by the number of parties I went to,
the dates I had, and the times I got drunk. I never did any more work
than was necessary to stay in school. Anything more than just passing
was wasted effort and could have been better spent on having a good
time. The blackouts were becoming more frequent. I never paid any
attention to them except to note that they were probably a sign that I
had enjoyed myself the night before.
At this point the proctor called me to his ofce. A friend and I had
gone to a secretarys apartment after the bar closed, and had not been
treated with the proper respect. Just to show her, we walked off with
half her belongings. She reported us to the authorities, and we received
a warning. This was in my freshman year.
My sophomore year, I brought a car to school. I could now have
more dates, go away on weekends to other schools. In the spring, the
president of the fraternity warned me that the brothers thought it was
a good idea for me to cut down, as I was giving the fraternity a bad
name on campus. It was none of his business, I told him; they were
jealous because I was having all the fun, while they had to work to
stay in school. Shortly thereafter, I was called to the proctors ofce
again.
My junior year was by far the worst. I started out by going back to
school a week early, and for a week I never drew a sober breath. The
progression had really set in. After school started, most days I made no
attempt to attend classes.
In December, I was again called to the proctors ofce, and sent to the
mental-health clinic to see a psychiatrist and take some tests. The doctor told me that I would have to leave school to do something about my
drinking problem. I was shocked. What drinking problem? I said I

Ex

am

Co

py

80

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

Ex

am

Co

py

would stop drinking if they allowed me to stay, but he tried to impress


upon me that I had lost control.
My bubble had burst. All of a sudden, the party had come to an
abrupt halt. I left school that afternoon.
The day after Christmas, I was admitted to a psychiatric clinic in
Manhattan. My condition could best be described as confused, about
what had happened and what was going to happen. When someone
tried to talk to me, my only response was to cry. As time went on, I was
able to talk to the doctor quite freely about my drinking. The time came
when I was nally able to admit that I might be an alcoholic.
After six months, I was discharged from the clinic. My father had
gone to his rst AA meeting years ago, in 1959, and my mother was a
member of Al-Anon (for relatives and friends of alcoholics). I had
attended many meetings in the past with my parents. Yet I made no
attempt to contact AA when I left the hospital. I stayed sober for two
months and then picked up the rst drink, looking for the good time
I was missing.
I drank for two months, and things got progressively worse. The day
nally came when I was convinced alcohol had the best of me, and I
needed help. I went to my rst meeting that night, seeking an answer to
my problem. That was over two years ago. I have not had a drink since,
one day at a time. The understanding that people in AA showed was the
rst thing that impressed me. They were not shocked by my drinking
history. They just nodded; they knew what I was talking about.
Two things that I caught on to right from the start were: constant
attendance at meetings, and sticking with the winners. I went to meetings every night, and tried to attend midnight meetings as often as
possible. After two months, I asked a man to be my sponsor. He proved
to be the biggest help, providing the answers and encouragement necessary for me to make this program.
Being young bothered me at rst. But the men who came in when
they were old, and stayed with the program, gave me the incentive to
do the same. I thought a man of 60 or more faced the same problem as
I, only at the other end of the spectrum.
AA has given me my life and my sanity, two things I hold dear today.
It has been a slow process of building a new life, one I never dreamed
existed for me. I am the product of many peoples devotion of time and
effort, and I always welcome the chance to pass on what I have to
someone else.
I am now back at the university, and will probably be on the deans
list this term. My concept of what a good time is has changed consid-

81

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

82

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

erably. There is a balance in my life today between studies, AA, and


doing other things I enjoy. All this is mine by staying away from one
drink, one day at a time. I probably have one drunk left in me, but Im
not so sure I have another recovery left in me. (1979c: 1013)

Ex

am

Co

The AA story provides a general framework, of which the individual


personal stories are specic examples. It is a learned structure, developed
over time, and of course there are variations. One reason for variation is
that any one personal story is created from the interaction of the learned
AA story form, the actual events of an individuals life, and individual or
group agendas. Many of the personal stories published in pamphlets
include paragraphs or sections addressing such issues as being a teenage alcoholic or a female alcoholic. The point these stories make is that,
while a person may be different from others in AA, the differences
are unimportant in light of the similarities: all are alcoholics. The Big
Book personal stories almost all address the issue of religious or spiritual beliefs.
There is also variation over time and situation. The personal stories in
the Big Book are the earliest published AA stories. The Big Book
stories tend to be longer than those in other literature, and they tend to
give more autobiographical information. Some of the stories in the Big
Book also seem more literary in style than those in other publications.
Perhaps the shorter and more prototypic form of the published AA story
has evolved since the publication of this book in 1939.
Stories also vary by whether they are told in speakers meetings or in
discussion meetings. In speakers meetings a single person may talk for
forty-ve minutes to an hour. In discussion meetings, in contrast, nearly
everyone takes part, and if personal stories are told they are abbreviated.
Sometimes the teller concentrates on a single aspect of the story, such as
what hitting bottom was like for him, or how he rst heard about AA, or
how he reacted to his rst meeting. If a fuller story is told, recurring
events, such as repeated negative effects of drinking, are condensed into
generalized statements or illustrative episodes.
Other variation stems from differences in individual lives, and from
differences in the extent to which individuals learn the model AA story
and use it to organize self-understanding. In spite of all the possible
variations, the personal stories told in AA are remarkably similar. Such
similarity comes about through participation in AA, where members are

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

83

Learning to Tell an AA Story

py

supported in re-forming their life histories as AA stories. AA members


come to t the events and experiences of their own lives into the AA
story structure and so to identify themselves in the gured world of AA.

Ex

am

Co

How to tell an AA story is not something one learns through explicit


teaching. Newcomers are not told how to tell their stories, yet most
people who remain in AA learn to do this. There are several ways in
which an AA member is helped to tell an appropriate story. First, he is
exposed to AA lives. The newcomer hears and reads personal stories
from the time of early contact with the programthrough meetings,
literature, and talk with oldtimers. In addition to the structure of the AA
story, the newcomer is also learning the gured world of alcoholism
encoded in them, including AA propositions: alcoholism is a progressive disease; the alcoholic is powerless over alcohol; the alcoholic
drinker is out of control; AA is for those who want it, not for those who
need it; AA is a program for living, not just for not drinking. These
points enter, often verbatim, into stories as guidelines for describing the
progression of drinking, the desire and inability to stop, the necessity of
hitting bottom before the program can work, and the changes that take
place in ones life after one joins AA.
Members also begin to view episodes of their lives as behavioral evidence of alcoholic drinking, and to supply the interpretations of these
episodes sanctioned by AA. Thus blackouts, car accidents, loss of job,
divorce, loss of friends, depression, involvement in illegal activities, hospitalization for mental illness or for detoxication enter into their stories
as evidence. Drinking patterns taken to be alcoholic, such as morning
drinking, bingeing, gulping drinks quickly, or hiding the amount drunk,
are also taken as evidence.
In addition to learning from the stories and statements of others, new
members begin to craft their own stories in social interaction. All members are encouraged to speak at discussion meetings and to maintain
friendships with other AA members. In this social interaction the new
member is called on to talk about her own life. As she does so a process
of scaffolding occurs (Wood, Bruner, and Ross 1976). In scaffolding,
individual skills originate in cooperative activity. In informal learning
activities the teacher builds a scaffold around the limited abilities of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

84

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

the learner, as they cooperatively perform the task. The teacher begins
with the skills the learner possesses, and builds on those, while taking
responsibility for the aspects of the task the learner has not yet mastered.
Over time the learner is given, and takes, responsibility for more of the
task. Scaffolding is derived from the developmental psychology of Vygotsky, most particularly from his notion of the zone of proximal development (see Chapter 8). The scaffold that the teacher has built diminishes, and the skills that had been inter-individual in the cooperative task
become intra-individual, personally produced and relied upon by the
learner (Greeneld 1984).
Scaffolding of AA members who are beginning to tell their stories
indicates that the AA identity is not natural or simple to develop. People
do not describe themselves as alcoholics and their lives as alcoholic
lives because this is natural and obvious; rather they learn to tell about
themselves and their lives in this way, and the process of learning can
take much effort and cooperation between the neophyte and other AA
members.
Scaffolding occurs as the newcomer begins to talk about his life. For
example, in discussion meetings the topic may be admitting you are
powerless, making amends, how to avoid the rst drink, or shared
experiences in dealing with common problems. Each person present
usually relates some incident or insight. One speaker follows another by
picking out certain pieces of what has previously been said, telling why it
was relevant to him, and elaborating on it with some episode of his own.
Sometimes a newcomer will say something that seems to parallel the
episodes or comments of others, but will give an interpretation of it that
is inappropriate according to AA beliefs. Sometimes the episode itself
will seem to have little to do with those others are telling. Usually, unless
the interpretation really runs counter to AA beliefs, the speaker is not
corrected. Rather, other speakers take the appropriate parts of the newcomers comments and build on them, giving parallel accounts with
different interpretations, or expanding on parts of their own stories that
are similar to parts of the newcomers story while ignoring the inappropriate parts of his story.
At one discussion meeting members traded stories of how they had
come to AA. A college athlete, a newcomer who had not yet quit drinking or using drugs, gave instrumental reasons for needing to quithe
had to undergo urine tests and would lose his athletic scholarship if
drugs were detected. This was an inappropriate reason, since AA stresses

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

85

Ex

am

Co

py

that you must see yourself as an alcoholic, and must want to quit for
yourself rather than for someone else. Most of the oldtimers who spoke
after the newcomer focused on how they had realized they had a problem
they couldnt control. Several of the stories had the themes of needing to
see yourself as like other members of AA and needing to see getting sober
as something desirable.
In another meeting Brad, who had just entered AA and had had his last
drink three days before, said that he would still do pot except that he
always drank when he did pot: Pot is not a problem; I can give it up
anytime. Steve, an oldtimer, said he had done a lot of different drugs
that he could give up anytime . . . as long as he replaced them with a new
drug. Mark said, I experimented with pot . . . for twenty years. The
implication of these comments was clearly that Brad was only kidding
himself. In this way, members point out appropriate interpretations as
they make jokes about their own past wrong interpretations. One night
Gary told this tale: One morning I woke up after a night of drinking,
and I thought Id had this bad dream about running into the side of a
bridge at 55 miles an hour. Then I went outside. Three inches off the side
of my car were gone. And I thought, Man, Ive got to stop driving.
Members try to reinforce the appropriate parts of a story rather than
point out the inappropriate. When called for, however, correction does
occur. If someone says something that directly contradicts the basic AA
propositions and interpretations, she will be called on it. Clair, who had
been in the program for some time, said, I thought I wasnt powerless
over anything. William interrupted her: You werent powerless over
anything? She answered, I thought I wasnt powerless over anything.
On another night Eugene, who was fairly new to the program, was
worried that he was about to lose his job because he believed his boss
had found out that he was an alcoholic living in a halfway house. Its not
going to do me any good to be sober and not have a job, Eugene said. I
dont want to be sober with ragged underwear. An oldtimer answered,
If youre drinking, your underwear will get pretty ragged anyway.
At times, correct and incorrect interpretations are contrasted explicitly, as in these comments during a discussion on honesty:
At least when I was drunk, and lying on the oor, if someone came by
and asked me, Hey, whats wrong with you? at least I could say, Im
drunk! I might say, Its that stinkin rotten gin that did it to me, but I
would say, Im drunk. Ive seen a lot of people who would be lying on

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

86

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

the oorand there arent that many reasons you would be thereand
if you asked them whats wrong, theyd tell you, Oh, Im just tired, just
resting. Theyd tell themselves that. They just cant be honest. You
dont see that kind of person get sober very often. At least if they can
say, Im drunk, they might be able to get sober.

Ex

am

Co

Sometimes members interpret newcomers lives, more or less explicitly, according to AA principles. Patricia, a nonmember, spoke to Hank
on the AA hotline and asked how she could know if she was an alcoholic. Hank brought her to a discussion meeting and suggested the topic
How we got into AA. Everyone gave an abbreviated personal story,
focusing on the things that had led them to see themselves as alcoholics
and to come to AA for help. After listening to these stories, Patricia
spoke. She said she didnt think she was an alcoholic, but she did have
a drinking problem. She often drank all day on weekends, and often
didnt remember those days. She said she drank a lot, sometimes lied
about the amount, and hid bottles. Sometimes she had blackouts. Her
boyfriend was concerned about how much she drank, and he had talked
her into contacting AA. Then she compared herself with her uncle, who
was an alcoholic, and with the other members of the discussion group
whose stories she had heard, and declared that she was not that bad:
she had never wrecked a car or had DUIs; she had never been hospitalized, or had deep depressions, so she didnt think she was an alcoholic.
While she was speaking no one interrupted, but after she had nished
several people spoke. Without actually telling her she was an alcoholic,
they pointed out that the stories they told were their own, not hers, and
the events they told about were not preconditions but symptoms. Gary,
an oldtimer, said that just because he had never been strung out in a
hospital like Elizabeth, he could not use that to say he was not an
alcoholic any more than Elizabeth could say she was not one because she
had never had DUIs as he had. You dont have to get all the way to the
bottom before you quit, Elizabeth told Patricia. The only requirement
for membership is a desire to stop drinking. At the end of the meeting
Patricia picked up her white chip, indicating her intention to stop drinking and start the AA program. As another characteristic response to this
form of denial, when newcomers state that they havent done something,
others will reply yet.
Personal stories, as AA recognizes, are important in helping some

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

87

Ex

am

Co

py

drinkers identify themselves as alcoholics. Hearing or reading personal


stories, potential members compare their lives to the alcoholic life, readjust their denition of alcoholic, and say, these people are like me.
Telling an AA personal story also helps members construct the identity of
AA alcoholic by signaling real membership and through transforming
the members self-understanding.
Telling AA personal stories also helps members identify with one another in ways that are harder to document. As a public event, one that is
not only observable but material and co-participatory, the telling encompasses body practices, including vocalization, that realize structures of
affect and disposition. Not only social theorists, from Durkheim and
Mauss to Bourdieu, but any participant in such performances would tell
you that the fellow-feeling born in these ceremonies is a powerful means
of identication.
Telling a personal story, especially at a speakers meeting or on a
Twelfth Step call, signals membership because this is the time that they
[members] feel that they belong enough to carry the message (Robinson 1979: 54). Twelfth Stepping and speaking at speakers meetings are
often represented in the AA literature as the nal evidence that the
members life has turned around from the drinking days to a life in which
he helps others. The personal stories themselves are valued as the vehicles by which the member transforms his past experience into something
useful.
Telling a personal story is also important as a process of self-understanding. The example of Patricia illustrates that, despite the hopes of
AA members, drinking alcoholics do not always recognize themselves in
the stories told at meetings and realize that they too must be alcoholics.
The AA story structure has a stage in which the drinker rejects the
suggestion that he might be an alcoholic, despite the negative effects of
his drinking, because he does not match his own denition of alcoholic. The drinker must learn an appropriate denition, and must learn
the events and episodes that are evidence of alcoholism. As he learns the
AA story structure, he must learn to see the events and experiences of his
own life as evidence for alcoholism. He learns to put his own events and
experiences into an AA story, and thus learns to tell, and to understand,
his own life as an AA story. He reinterprets his own past, from the
understanding he once had of himself as a normal drinker to the understanding he now has of himself as an alcoholic.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

88

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Reinterpreting the Past

Ex

am

Co

py

The AA personal story is a cultural form that the teller learns to produce,
and the process transforms her self-understanding from that of a drinker
to that of an AA recovering alcoholic. The stories encode the guring
of alcoholism held by AA, and this guring organizes knowledge and
guides inferences. As we argue in Chapter 5, identication with the
world gured by a cultural model develops together with becoming an
actor in that world.4 As the newcomer learns to produce an AA story and
identies herself as an AA alcoholic, she comes to use the cultural model
to direct her present actions and self-understanding and to reinterpret
her past. The stories are tools for reinterpreting the past and putting the
self into the gured world of AA. That is, the stories are cultural mediating devices, similar to the Nepalese rituals and songs that mediate the
formation of gender identities (see Chapters 10 and 12).
In the course of my eldwork with AA, I (Carole Cain) heard stories
told in three settings: discussion meetings, speakers meetings, and interviews. In some cases I heard the same persons tell their personal stories,
or parts of them, in more than one settingin an interview and a discussion meeting, for example, or in a discussion meeting and a speakers
meeting. It became clear that there were regularities in the stories each
person told, even as they were adapted to the different settings. There
also seemed to be a relationship between the way the stories were told
and the extent to which the teller had made the AA identity and the AA
model of the alcoholic life his own. The longer a person had been in AA,
the closer his story was to the AA story structure. There were also markers incorporated into the stories that indicated a process of reinterpretation, continuing or complete.
Much of this section is based on stories collected during three interviews, although I occasionally refer to stories told in other settings. One
advantage of the interviews is that they were tape-recorded and I have
been able to work from transcriptions. Also, in the interviews the speakers clearly consented to my use of the information. Although AA members knew that I was attending meetings as part of my research, what was
said at those meetings was intended for other members, not for the
anthropologist, and much was sensitive in nature and private. I have
edited out some material that I felt would be inappropriate to include.5
Interviews were done with people I met through AA meetings, and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

89

Ex

am

Co

py

consisted of three parts (see Cain 1991). In the part presented here I
asked for the individuals accounts of their drinking experiences.
Before analyzing the interviews I collected personal stories published
in AA literature: 29 from the Big Book (AA 1976) and 17 from two
pamphlets (AA 1968a, 1979c). For these 46 accounts I did a careful
analysis, for each paragraph noting main points, episodes and events
included, and propositions made about alcohol, self, and AA. I sketched
the overall structure of each story. I then did the same type of analysis
(by frame) for two ctional stories presented in comic-book style (AA
1967, 1968b). Taking these two very similar stories, which I concluded
were typical according to AA beliefs, as prototypes, and referring back
to the 46 published personal accounts, I abstracted a general AA story
structure. It included categories such as rst drink, negative effects
of drinking, progression of drinking, suggestion (by others) that
drinking may be a problem, denial, attempts to control drinking,
entering AA, giving AA an honest try, and becoming sober. After I
modied the structure to account for differences (for example, some
sequences may be repeated; some categories are optional), the variation
that remained could be accounted for as attempts to address specic
issues, the agendas of particular publications. All the personal stories in
the AA publication Young People and AA (1979) address the issue of
being a young alcoholic; all the stories in AA for the Woman (1968a)
address the issue of being a female alcoholic. The Big Book stories were
all longer than those in the pamphlets and generally addressed the issue
of religious or spiritual beliefs and AA (for more details about my analysis see Cain 1991).
I compared the stories I heard told in AA discussion and speakers
meetings with those in the literature, and found that they t the structure quite well, with two qualications. First, some of the stories in the
discussion meetings were short versionseither telescoped accounts of
a drinking history or pieces of a drinking history used to make a particular point. Second, at the four speakers meetings I attended, three of the
speakers told stories that closely followed the structure. The fourth story
had events, episodes, and interpretations clearly matching AA beliefs,
but the speaker did not present them in chronological order; rather he
told episodes illustrating various points about his drinking and life. His
interpretations and choice of events matched the AA model in all ways
except the order in which they were told.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

90

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

I applied the same analysis to the narratives recorded in interviews and


compared these narratives to the stories told in meetings and published
in the AA literature. The ndings suggest that, over time, the individual
develops his drinking history according to the AA structure, and as the
AA identity is formed, the life story narrated comes to resemble the
prototypical AA story more and more closely.
In the interviews I asked each person to tell me about her or his
drinking experiences. Beyond this I tried not to direct the narrative until
it reached what seemed to be a natural ending. Of the three people
interviewed, two seemed to have a clear concept of a set of episodes that
constituted a response to my request. The narrative told by Hank, who
had been sober in AA for fourteen years at the time of the interview, most
closely approximated the AA story structure. Ellen, who had been sober
for two years, also told a narrative that closely resembled the AA story,
but some of her interpretations differed from those heard most often in
AA. While Hank seemed to have completed the process of placing his life
into the AA story form, Ellen was well along in the process but had not
yet completed it. The third informant, Andrew, did not have such a
narrative. Andrew had been in and out of AA for over twenty years but
had never stayed in the program for more than a few months at a time.
That the stories of Ellen and Hank were lengthy narratives requiring
no prompting from me beyond the initial request, and that they paralleled the AA story in structure, indicates that Ellen and Hank are learning, or have learned, to tell their lives as AA personal stories. They
proceeded from early drinking, through its progression and negative
effects, to hitting bottom and getting into AA. Each of these narratives,
excluding follow-up questions and an additional lengthy discussion of
Hanks life now and his role in AA, lasted about forty-ve minutes
(which is also the approximate length of personal stories told in speakers
meetings). Each seemed to form a unit that was a natural unit of discourse for the narrators. Both Hank and Ellen seemed to have a clear
concept of a set of material that constituted such a story, and of how it
should unfold.
The narrative of drinking experience as a bounded unit is named. AA
calls it a personal story. Hank referred to his own narrative as a drinking
story or a drinking history, and indicated that there were some criteria
for such a history: Im doing this more or less, Im tryin to keep this
down to drinkin and nothin else. But you know, there was a life . . . And

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

91

Ex

am

Co

py

I will say that, you know, all this time there is another life going on. And
this, I think, is important . . . A lot of time drinkin stories look like
theres nothin in life but drinkin, dont you see.
Neither Hanks nor Ellens narrative matched the AA story structure
perfectly, but both were very close. Both included a description of early
drinking (or, for Ellen, drug use), the progression, the negative effects,
others reactions, hitting bottom, getting into AA, and life since starting
AA. Interestingly, they did not say much about their rst contact with
AA, their impression of it, or their ability to identify with other members,
a theme prominent in many of the published personal stories. This may
be a result of differing agendas: the published stories have the purpose of
trying to convince the reader to identify with the teller; in the interview
situation this was not the purpose of the telling. But it may be that Hank
and Ellen have not incorporated this part of the AA story structure into
their own personal stories.
Andrew, the third AA member interviewed, seemed to have no such
drinking experience story. My questions prompted only short answers
about specic events rather than the progression of drinking that makes
up the AA story. He spoke of himself as an alcoholic, but did not seem to
understand this in AA terms. Alcoholism was not something he thought
of as affecting all areas of his life. Similarly, he did not consider AA
something he would have to remain in all the time, or for the rest of his
life, in order to stay sober. Rather, he viewed coming to AA meetings, or
into the program, as a measure to take when things got bad. AA was
where he went when he started to have DTs (delirium tremens).
Andrew seemed to understand the events of his life not the way other
AA members would, as a progression of drinking and negative effects
leading to a bottom, but rather as a series of unrelated points where
drinking was enough of a problem, based on its physical effects (DTs),
that he had to do something about it. After a thirty-minute interview, the
drinking history I had recorded resembled the sort of medical history
that a doctor might collect, rather than an AA story. Andrew had accepted the label of alcoholic as applying to himself, but he had not
accepted the identity of an AA alcoholic. He did not understand himself
this way, and he did not present his life as an AA life.
In addition to evidence that AA members learn to tell their lives in the
structure of the AA story, there are other indicators that they are reinterpreting their lives as they tell their stories. One of these is the incorpora-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

92

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

tion of AA propositions into the stories. Propositions such as alcoholism is a progressive disease, the alcoholic drinker is out of control,
and to recover, the alcoholic drinker must admit that he is powerless
over alcohol appear in the narratives of Hank and Ellen.

Co

Hank: After my last drink, and coming back in, I realized that if I was
gonna live, and live comfortable, and live happily, that I was gonna have
to take the AA program, its directions, and live it . . . And it was rough
at rst . . . and so . . . I hadnt lived that way. The other two times that I
was in, you know, it was something like, you know, making my own
decisions and writing my own program. But this time, I decided that,
you know, I was gonna do it . . . that way. And, and part of it is realizing
that, you know, from the very beginning, that Im powerless over it, that
theres nothing I can do about my drinking . . .

Ellen: So, everyone says, you know, its a progressive disease. And I can see
the progression, how I went from bad to worse, whenever I try to relate my
story . . . I swear, from that day on, for the next two, almost three years,
I was high every day. And after about a month I realized that this is
something that I couldnt stop. Its getting out of hand, because I would
wake up in the morning . . . Id go to bed at night and Id make sure I
had the dope in the morning to get up with.

Ex

am

Ellens narrative also includes sections interpreted in ways other than


those common in AA, sometimes in opposition to certain AA propositions. For example, she makes a distinction between different types of
addiction, a distinction AA does not make: And I know, I realize that
marihuana is not physically addicting, but for me it was very psychologically addicting. And she tells about a slip but minimizes its importance, in opposition to the AA belief that a single drink (or joint) can
lead to the previous level of drinking or using: I told God and myself I
would, if I ever get that baby back, I would never do anything like that
. . . again. And so, I havent. I, I smoked a joint at [a psychiatric hospital],
but I havent gotten back into that lifestyle that, that I was at that point.
And I dont want to. When I smoked that joint, I think I did it mostly to
see if I still got high, what it was like, and anything like that. It really
didnt do much for me.
At some points Ellen seems to be working out her story, or at least the
story she tells, as she tells it: I was looking for a job. Thats what I was
doing. I was living with her and looking for a job. And . . . Except that Id

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

93

am

Co

py

never look for a job. Id just go out and get stoned. This quotation
reects a process that is recognized as necessary for progress in AA: that
of getting honest, or admitting that you have not been honest with
yourself or others previously. Well, they accused me of stealing money
from the cash register at that job I had. And I said, No I didnt. And they
kept, and then, and then it would start. The lady, my boss lady said, well
. . . you know, I just was real paranoid. I thought she thought I was
stealing money from her, or something. So I quit. I got . . . I told my
boyfriend that I couldnt take her telling me that I stole from her, cause I
didnt steal anything from her.
These two quotations show that Ellen has not yet arrived at a nished
version of her story. The second also reects another important aspect of
Ellens life. Two years before this interview she was diagnosed as paranoid schizophrenicthe result, her doctors said, of her heavy drug use.
In this passage she interprets her actions as the result of her paranoid
thought system, although she seems unsure of this interpretation. The
quotation illustrates a process she is still going through of choosing
between two versions of events, posited within two different, if often
related, gured worlds.
Ellens and Hanks narratives also include verbal markers indicating
that reinterpretation of the past has occurred. Hanks is the most fully
reinterpreted life of the three interview narratives. He compares the way
he now understands his past behavior with the way he understood it at
the time.
I know now, what I have learned later, that within two years of having my
rst drink, that I had trouble with alcohol. I look back. I did not drink
like my peers. It affected me differently than others.

Ex

Oh, I was a very, very sick man, I know now, before anyone ever recommended treatment.

This was the rst time I went to having serious problems with it. Two or
three things . . . There was the separation, and then the other things.
You know, these things, Ive learned later, were perfectly normal. Divorce,
separate and divorce. And the other things were perfectly normal.
Well, like a lot of the other recovering alcoholics, and like some Ive heard
about, I wound up by my wife and myself getting back together. Oh,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

94

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds


outside of being something typical that a drunk would do, without any
more thought than that . . . we did it.

py

Ellen has not reinterpreted all episodes she includes in her narrative as
fully as Hank has reinterpreted his. Her markers of reinterpretation take
the form of comparison of events in her life with events she has heard
other AA members tell.

Co

And it seems that to justify smoking dope and drinking, I would always
say something about my parents. Id say, like, My parents dont love
me, or They dont understand me, or something, So Im gonna go
get high. And that would get them. The other night I guess you heard
Mark . . . talking about, you know, he was bored and didnt have anything to do. I think I used anger a lot instead.

And then starts a big period in my life where I was just depressed. I
just was so depressed. And part of it, I was so depressed because Id
have periods where Id be totally . . . And this can happen with drinking, too. Ive heard a lot of people say, you know, like, they, the blackouts,
the insanity, that comes from a lot of drinking. About seeing hallucinations, and all that kind of thing. So its not just the drugs that you can do
that with.

am

Other examples of marked reinterpretation of past events come from


the personal stories told at speakers meetings:
I used to say, even after I got into AA, that I had never lost a job because
of my drinking. And its true that I was never red with drinking given
as a reason. But I was red because I had not nished my thesis, and I
had never nished my thesis because I was drinking. Put two and two
together . . .

Ex

I used to say that prostitution was one thing I had never done to get
money for drugs. But I realize now that I was prostituting myself to my
husband. He would call [after she had left him] and say he had cocaine.
Id be there.

The stories change over time as the understanding of oneself and ones
life changes. There seems to be a fairly strong relationship among the
extent to which the AA identity has been formed, the extent to which the
narrative of drinking experience resembles the AA story structure, and
the salience of the AA identity to the individual. Hank had been in AA by

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

95

Ex

am

Co

py

far the longest time, and the identity of AA alcoholic was very prominent
in his self-understanding: of 13 responses he gave to the question Who
am I? 6 (46 percent), including the rst 3, referred to his identity as
an alcoholic or his participation in AA:6 (1) I am a recovering alcoholic; (2) 14 years sober; (3) drinking historytwenty years or better; (10) spend quite a bit of time in AA service work; (12) love to
talk to school and university classes [about alcoholism and AA]; and
(13) man on AA hotline in my home (eight years). His other 7 responses referred to family and career.
Ellens responses to the question Who am I? indicated that she was
not as far along as Hank in the process of incorporating the AA identity
as a prominent self-understanding. Of 11 responses, 2 (18 percent) related directly to this identity, and these were within the rst four
responses: (3) exdrug addict; and (4) sober. Her other responses referred to social categories and personality traits, including (1) woman,
(2) mother, (9) bride, (5) social, (7) outgoing, and (8) people
pleaser.
In contrast, only one (5 percent) of Andrews 20 responses to the
question Who am I? related to the AA identity: (13) non-drinker.
The response preceding this was non-smoker. His rst response was
his full name. The remainder dealt mainly with work and avocations
(2) dishwasher, (3) composer, (4) performer, (5) cook, (18)
writerand with his current living situation and his dissatisfaction
with it: (7) product of divorce, (8) single, (9) under-employed,
(10) unhappy with living arrangement, (11) overweight, (14) disappointed with locale, (15) no children/wife, (20) alone. These
results support the conclusion that there is a relationship between the
prominence of the AA alcoholic identity and the extent to which the
narrative of drinking experience resembles the AA story structure.
Comparison of the stories told in various settings with one another
and with the published AA stories indicates that the form of ones personal story changes over time as one develops and becomes emotionally
attached to the AA identity. Ellens and Hanks interviews suggest that
these stories actually mediate self-understanding, by helping the tellers
learn to interpret their lives in the same way other alcoholics interpret
theirs. Episodes from ones life are grouped into a drinking experience
story, or drinking history, that resembles the AA story in structure. Ones
understanding of the self and the past changes as AA propositions and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

96

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

interpretations are learned. As the AA member learns to place the events


of his life into the structure of the AA story, he learns to tell his life as an
AA story, and reinterprets his past in this way. He learns to understand
himself as an AA person. By this same process, from the standpoint of the
group, the neophyte is fully recruited, partaking both of AAs activities
and of the imagined life, the gured world, that sustains AAs separate
and recognizable sphere.

Ex

am

Co

Members of Alcoholics Anonymous have found a particular denition of


alcoholism and a particular way to deal with problem drinking. Their
program for recovery requires accepting the idea of alcoholism as an
incurable disease that affects all areas of ones life, and accepting the idea
of oneself as an alcoholic, one who is powerless over alcohol. The
change that the men and women of AA undergo is more than one of
behaviorfrom drinking to not drinking. It is a transformation of the
meaning and the referability of behaviorthat is, of identity, of how one
understands oneselffrom a drinking non-alcoholic to a non-drinking
alcoholic.
Newcomers to AA must give up an old identity, that of normal drinker,
and develop a new one. They go through a process of identity devaluation followed by a process of identity formation. This transformation
takes place through reinterpretation, as members come to understand
that their pasts have been a progression of alcoholic drinking and alcoholic behavior. They learn to cast their (drinking) lives in AAs distinctive
way of guring such a life. One way this reinterpretation takes place is
through the AA personal story. The AA story is a genre, a cultural device,
that newcomers can learn to produce and use to mediate self-understanding. As the newcomer learns the structure of the AA story, and
learns the model of alcoholism encoded in the story, he begins to place
the events and experiences of his own life into this form. He learns to tell
his own life as an AA personal story, and through this, to understand his
life as an AA life and himself as an AA person. He comes to understand
why, and how, he is an alcoholic.
What happens when an AA member fails to compose this new understanding of himself and of his life? The example of Andrew suggests that
such people may be those for whom AA does not work. Andrew has
accepted the label of alcoholic, and the idea of alcoholism as a disease,
but has not accepted the AA identity, with its interpretation of what this

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

97

Ex

am

Co

py

means. He does not gure his life in AAs terms. He views AA as a


measure to take when things get really bad. He does not share the set of
values and distinctions that unites other AA members. The identity of
alcoholic does not affect his actions, or his perceptions of self, beyond
his drinking behavior. Andrew has never stayed in AA for more than one
year, although he has been in treatment for alcoholism four times and in
and out of AA at least three times.
A follow-up with Hank and Andrew further supported the importance
of acquiring the AA identity. Two years after the original interviews,
Hank remained sober and active in AA. Although a stroke had made him
decrease his participation in activities, AA held a central and important
place in his life. He talked at length about the role of speaking in AA and
what speaking in meetings does for members.
Andrew had left the AA program within a month of the rst interview.
Two years later I spoke with him again at a community soup kitchen
where he had come for lunch. He told me he had been hitting it [the
bottle] pretty hard and was in bad shape. He said he had used the death
of his mother as an excuse to go on a drinking binge, though, if its a
disease, I guess you dont need an excuse. He had lost his job as a cab
driver, partly because one night, while a customer went into a liquor
store, he also went in and made a purchase. Now unemployed, he was
about to be evicted from his apartment because he could not pay the
rent. I asked him what plans he had. He said he would take a job as a
dishwasher, but he thought he needed some time off to get straightened
out before he took another job. He told me he planned either to check
himself into a detoxication and treatment center or to get back into AA:
Anyway, Im gonna go today and tomorrow . . . Im gonna drink today
and tomorrow. Sort of a binge, though I dont know if youd call it a
binge. Then after that, Saturday Im gonna go back to the program, start
meetings. Perhaps this time, this Saturday, Andrew would begin a journey toward what Hank had long ago reached, an enduring identity as an
AA alcoholic.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

How Figured Worlds of


Romance Become Desire

Ex

am

She took him to the prom . . . he was about 26 . . . he was with a whole
lot of girls, it wasnt like he was just talking to her . . . she really didnt
know what she was getting into. Americans often speak of romance as
though it were a natural and intrinsically motivating activity that most
people, by the time they reach a certain age, engage in at a reasonable
level of competence. In the quotation a young woman named Cylene
alludes to a much less common conception: that savoir faire in romantic
pursuits is learned and that the motivating force of romance may not
come about automatically, but may rather be formed in social life.
The larger issue here concerns how gured worlds come to engage
people, to shape and be shaped by their actions. Not only the skills and
competencies involved in cultural performances but also the meanings
and salience of such activities and the desire to participate in them vary
from person to person. As we saw in Chapter 4 with regard to Alcoholics
Anonymous, participants develop an identication with a gured world
in different degrees of engagement, and these levels of identity are
products of peoples social histories, lived as history-in-person. These
processes of identication are evident in the specialized world of AA and
also in the more widespread activities of romance and attraction.
In a study of college student life (see Holland and Eisenhart 1990) we
followed Cylene and twenty-two other women for a year and a half,
through three semesters. The study was designed to investigate how
womens peer groups affected their choice of career. As it turned out,
much of the womens time and energy, much of what they said in their
interviews, and much of what we observed in their peer activities had
98
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

99

Co

py

to do with romantic relationships. Because we interviewed the women


again and again over the three semesters, we were able to follow their
developing skills and ideas about the conduct of romantic affairs. Because we participated in some of their peer activities, we also were able to
describe the social-interactional context in which these skills and ideas
developed. The gured world of romance acquired motivating force as
the women developed mastery of it, and their mastery, in turn, depended
upon their development of a concept of themselves as actors in the world
of romance. Although American culture tells us that the urge to romance
is natural, our ndings suggest that the desire for romance is formed
over time and in social lifein a process of recruitment and identication to action in a gured world.

The Directive Force of Figured Worlds

Ex

am

How do meaning systems become desire? In other words, how does a


culturally constructed world encourage people to action? This question
lies at the interface between the collective and the person and is implicated in any theory that gives weight to cultural logics. Parsons alluded
to the importance of the issue; in his opinion the question of the relations of social structure and personality had been solved by a convergence between Freuds account of the ego, superego, and id and Durkheims, Cooleys, and Meads accounts of society. His excitement was
evident: This convergence is one of the few truly momentous developments of modern social science (1961: 166).
Parsonss enthusiasm notwithstanding, there was and still is serious
contention within the social sciences, and within anthropology in particular, over the proper conceptualization of the relationships among
society, behavior, and the presocial nature of humans. In anthropological accounts the questions have often been phrased in terms of culture.
To what extent does culture determine behavior? Is culturedened as
collective interpretations of social and material experiencemerely an
after-the-fact labeling of deep-seated human needs and interests stemming, say, from psychodynamic forces or from in-built materialist orientations? Or are cultural meaning systems inuential to the point that
they profoundly shape and dene human needs? As Spiro (1982) points
out, ones assumptions about this issue determine ones interpretation of
the problem of the directive force of cultural systems.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

100

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

If culture is assumed to dene and determine individual human needs,


then the challenge is to explicate the form and nature of a particular
gured world and so its power to dictate action. The directive force is
supplied by the gured world itself. Many anthropologists take culture to
be a profoundly important determinant of human motivation and thus
adopt this rst approach.
If, in contrast, culture is conceived as a supercial labeling of deepseated human needs, then questions about the directive force of cultural
systems become questions about how a cultural systemthe surface
formis harnessed to and so derives its power from underlying (psychodynamic) forces. This second approach can be found in the literature
of anthropology as well. An anthropological position need not exclude
the possibility that cross-culturally variable meaning systems are but gilt
upon a more fundamentalpsychodynamic, materialist, or even structuralistsubstrate (see Quinn 1992; Hutchins 1987).
A third approach, one that emphasizes the formation of motivation
during development, is less familiar in the anthropological literature.
This third position is the one we take here.
In our neo-Vygotskian developmental approach, thoughts and feelings, will and motivation are formed as the individual develops. The
individual comes, in the recurrent contexts of social interaction, to personalize cultural resources, such as gured worlds, languages, and symbols, as means to organize and modify thoughts and emotions. These
personalized cultural devices enable and become part of the persons
higher mental functions, to use Vygotskys terms.
Beckers (1963) analysis of the use of marijuana exemplies the kind
of study and conclusions implied by Vygotskian theory. Although Becker
does not refer to Vygotskys concepts, his study of how people become
inveterate pot smokers clearly shows that motivation is developed within
social process. Beckers ndings challenged the then-prevalent idea that
personality traits or the physiological effects of the drug motivated
use. He found that the full measure of the motivation to smoke was
neither brought by the individual to the activity nor compelled by the
nature of the drug. Rather the compelling nature of the activity developed and was maintained in the context of interaction with others. The
neophyte learned from others not only how to use the drug but also how
to attend to and value the experience. Similarly, although we do not

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

101

py

wish to draw parallels between using drugs and being involved in romance, our research suggests that the compelling nature of romantic
pursuits comes about, or is constructed, in the process of learning the
gured world.

The Study

am

Co

Our study of college women covered a three-semester period in 1979


1981, from near the beginning of the informants freshman year to the
middle of their sophomore year. We interviewed the women monthly
and joined in some of their campus activities. The talking diary interviews were designed to encourage the women to discuss their experiences and concerns in their own terms. A second type of interview, the
life history interview, was conducted near the end of the study.
The participants were drawn from two universities in the southern
United Statestwelve from Bradford University, a historically black
school, and eleven from Southern University (SU), a historically white
school. Bradford draws students who are predominantly African-American and lower middle class; SU draws predominantly white, middle- and
upper-middle-class students. Both are state universities. At both the ratio
of women to men is about 60:40. (For details see Holland and Eisenhart
1981, 1990.)1

Romance and Attractiveness at the Two Schools

Ex

At both Bradford and SU, social activities were usually related to the
conduct of romantic encounters and relationships. Going out with, or
seeing, friends of the opposite sex ranked high on the list of valued
activities, and places where one might meet new men and womenparties, mixers, or the local barswere favored locations. The women and
their friends spent a lot of time talking about potential romantic partners and their own and others cross-gender relationships. Attractivenessas validated by attention from a man or mencontributed in a
major way to a womans status in the peer system (Holland and Eisenhart
1990). The romantic sphere of life was especially important to the
women of SU. Those who were less successful at coursework than they
had expected began to emphasize romantic relationships even more than

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

102

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

they once had. Relationships with women were subordinated to relationships with men; romantic relationships often interfered with relationships among women (Holland and Eisenhart 1990).
The themes of male/female relationships also dominated a vocabulary
that the students used to talk about one another. Women knew hundreds of words for types of men, such as jerks, jocks, cowboys,
frattybaggers, brains, pricks, and men knew hundreds for types of
women (Holland and Skinner 1987). And romance gured profoundly in
the semiotics of clothes and personal adornment.
An earlier study of the meaning of gender-marked terms and of descriptions of male/female encounters and relationships, also conducted
at SU, had suggested that student discourse about romantic and close
cross-gender friendships presupposed a cultural model or cultural narrative of how such relationships develop.2 To understand the students
talk one had to know what they took for granted about these relationships (Holland and Skinner 1987).
As with other gured worlds, the cultural models of romance posited a
simplied world populated by a set of agents (for example, attractive
women, boyfriends, lovers, ancs) who engage in a limited range of
important acts or state changes (irting with, falling in love with, dumping, having sex with) as moved by a specic set of forces (attractiveness,
love). When the young adults talked about their acquaintances, friends,
and (potential) romantic partners in terms of gender-marked types such
as hunks, jerks, Susie sororities, or dumb broads, they were
assuming that the talk would be understood in relation to a simplied
world of cross-gender relationships. The typical progress of events posited in this gured world follows this course:

Ex

An attractive man (guy) and an attractive woman (girl) are


drawn to each other.
The man learns and appreciates the womans qualities and uniqueness as a person.
Sensitive to her desires, he shows his affection by treating her well:
he buys things for her, takes her places she likes, and shows that
he appreciates her and appreciates her uniqueness as a person.
She in turn shows her affection and interest and allows the relationship to become more intimate.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

103

This standard scenario also presupposes the motives or purposes of such


relationships:

py

The relationship provides intimacy for both the man and the woman.
The relationship validates the attractiveness of both the man and the
woman.
And it allows and accounts for some exceptions:

Co

If the mans attractiveness or prestige is less than the womans, he


compensates by treating her especially well.
If the womans attractiveness is the lower of the two, she compensates by being satised with less good treatment from the man.

Ex

am

Most of the hundreds of gender-marked terms the students used designate problematic types of men and womenproblematic in relation to
the taken-for-granted progress of male/female relations posited by the
cultural model. They are gures who cause such relations to go awry.
Jerk names a typical problematic man. Our informants described jerks
as insensitive and stupid. But they did not fully spell out why a jerk
was considered such a negative type or why women found jerks disgusting and irritating. The reason became clear when we considered the jerk
as a relevant character in the gured world. A jerk is a type of man who
is neither attractive nor sensitive to women. He cannot compensate for
his low prestige by treating a woman especially well. He is too out of it
to discern her special qualities and anticipate her desires. He cannot
gure out the things he could do to make her feel well treated. He may be
so insensitive that he cannot even tell she dislikes him. Because he cannot take a hint, he will not leave her alone, and thus he becomes more
and more irritating.3
Not only were the women expected to know these cultural assumptions about the usual romantic relationship and the many types who
populated the world of romance, they also were constantly exposed to
interpretations of their own behavior based upon this standard romantic
scenario. In short, life among women students at both universities was
dominated in large partboth socially and culturallyby romance. Social activities and relationships, including relationships between women,
often revolved around romantic encounters or around talk about romantic relationships. Status was accorded, especially for women, in relation

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

104

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

to attractiveness. The students shared ways of communicating about and


conventions for interpreting romantic relationships. And ones goals, intentions, and qualities were likely to be interpreted according to these
ideas about romance.

Differences in Romantic Expertise and Involvement

Ex

am

Co

In the peer situation, with its constant enactments of and talk about
romantic relationships, individual differences in expertise and involvement were obscured. Knowledgeability about the types of men and the
ways romantic relationships worked was more or less assumed. Furthermore, as romance was assumed to be a natural activity, a basic level of
competence in the conduct of relationships was presumed.
American culture does not generally treat romantic relationships as an
area of expertise. The women did not usually talk about romance and
romantic relationships as something at which one is good or bad, expert
or inexpert. Love and romance were often talked about fatalistically, as
something that happened to one, not something that one affected. Unsuccessful relationships were more often attributed to character aws,
luck, or mismatches of interests than to a lack of skill or savoir faire.
Nonetheless, some talk did turn upon questions of expertise and competence. The women sometimes saw themselves or others as having
made mistakes. They had a sense of romantic situations that they were
too inexperienced or too young to handle. They sometimes assessed
themselves and others as more or less procient at romantic activities.
Judging from the women in the study, this less often articulated view
was the more accurate one. The more common notion that everyone has
similar levels of competence in and involvement with romance did not
hold up. Some women were quite ambivalent about romance (southern
style) and had less-than-compelling images of themselves in romantic
relationships. And the women differed in their facility with romantic
situations. That is, they had been recruited to the gured world of romance, and they had rehearsed in the repertory of activities, the scripts,
of that world, but not all of them had rehearsed to the same level. They
differed both in their degree of involvement with the world of romance
and in their levels of expertise in conceiving and responding to romantic
situationsthat is, their skills as romantic actors and their feeling or
sense (Bourdieus sens pratique) for the game of romantic relations.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

105

Involvement in the World of Romance

Salience of the World of Romance

py

Romance was much more salient or present in the thoughts of some


women. Apart from salience, but related to it, was another variable aspect of involvement: identication of self in the world of romance.

Ex

am

Co

Spiro (1982) has described ve levels of cognitive salience of cultural


systems. At the highest levels, a cultural system rst guides and then
instigates action. Some of the women in the study had apparently
reached that degree of salience. They saw everyday life through the lens
of the gured world of romance. Romantic involvementsactual or
hoped foroccupied much of their time and thoughts. They spent far
more of their time either with boyfriends, talking about boyfriends, looking for boyfriends, or adjusting their plans and activities to boyfriends
than did others. Romance was less salient for others; they spent time
instead on classes, clubs or committees, friends or family relationships.
For several women the salience of romance changed over the course of
the study. For some, romantic activities became more important; for
others, less so. These cases helped us tease outas we shall show later
not only the concomitants of increasing and decreasing salience but also
the factors that affected its rise and fall.
Experiences in nonromantic pursuits sometimes occasioned changes
in a womans emphasis on romance. Several of the women came to college proud of having done well in high school and then got upset when
their university grades were not as good. They gradually became less
involved with their schoolwork and switched their attention and time
to romantic pursuits. Romance became an even more important world
for them.
Friends were another impetus to romance. Peers generally encouraged
the pursuit of romance. Sometimes, however, they stimulated opposition
to this cultural system, and they may have gured in one womans eventual loss of interest. For this woman, Sandy, the decrease in salience was
dramatic.
At the beginning of the study Sandy seemed as interested as her
dormmates in nding a male romantic partner (You can have the
blondes, I get the dark-haired . . . cute ones). But as time went by she

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

106

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

had trouble establishing the kind of relationships she wanted with men
at college. She also learned that a potential boyfriend from back home
did not feel equally attracted to her and was in fact involved with someone else. All her friends seemed to be having similarly unhappy romantic
experiences. Sandy also began to feel that she did not t in with the other
students at SU: In my hometown, I was pretty much respected in the
community and accepted for what I am, or was, in that community. [I
was] basically your nonconformist, and I dressed to suit me. But when I
came down here I . . . got the impression that here I was a sloppy little
girl and I didnt have any class or I didnt have any style.
She came to stress friendships more than she had before: Friendships
were . . . and probably still are one of the most important things to me.
They were important to me not so very much socially. They were important to me as a person . . . I pride myself in my friendships.
Sandy formed a special friendship with one woman, Leslie. Despite the
jealousy of other friends and the admonitions of her parents, who felt she
was not availing herself of all the options at college, Sandy pursued her
friendship with Leslie: Our friendship is terric . . . I just would like to
spend more time . . . its all been crammed into one semester . . . theres
probably not gonna be another time in my life when I can just sit down
and just be friends.
Sandy came to devote all her free time to Leslie. She had begun her
freshman year with a professed interest in romantic relationships with
men, but had dropped that interest altogether. Such romantic attachments became markedly less salient for her, and her identity as an attractive woman in the world of romance, as culturally construed, became
unimportant.

Ex

Identication with the World of Romance


Not only the importance the women attributed to romance but also the
nature of their stances toward it varied. Some seemed to have an idea
of themselves as participants in the world of male/female relationships
and to accept these romantic selves as real parts of themselves. Others
either had an unclear image or sensed a discrepancy between themselves
and the romantic role(s) they were acting out. At least one woman,
Susan, explicitly contested the way romance was conceived and enacted

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

107

py

in the student culture. Another, Natalie, devalued the importance of


some taken-for-granted ideas of romance and marriage.
The womens process of identication with the romantic world took
place in the context of constant social comment and action. Their peers,
sometimes even their professors, cast them as romantic actors. Dellas
account provides a graphic example of such typecasting as a romantic/sexual character. One day she went to class dressed in a skirt and
blouse:

Co

And I sits in front . . . of the class, and my teacher says, Whats this?
Whats this? Della, wheres she at? And I just sat there looking at him
cause he looked dumb looking around me . . . and he said, Oh, theres
Della. He looked my legs up and down, up and down, and the whole
class [was] looking at him. He said, Oh, oh, I see. I see, Della, I see.
Oh, oh. . . . I was so embarrassed; the whole class was looking at me.

Ex

am

Another woman, Karla, told about a seduction attempt by a former


teacher. Her story consisted of the following episodes. She ran into a
teacher from high school, one she had liked and respected and whose
opinion she still valued highly. He gave her a book to reada book by
Freudand they made plans to discuss it. Karla read the book and went
to meet her teacher ready for an intellectual discussion. Instead she
found out that he wanted to be her lover. Because he had a family, she
had not thought of a romantic relationship with him and she disapproved of his invitation. She deected his advances, but he persisted.
When she nally made her reluctance clear, he became angry and told
her that intellectuals were not concerned about whether their lovers
were married or not.4
These two attempts to interpret or identify or position the women
in certain sorts of romantic or sexual relationships were fairly explicit
and direct, as were many such cases. We also observed an abundance of
cases where the attributions were less direct. Even seemingly descriptive comments, such as Oh, youre wearing your add-a-beads, could
be abbreviated references to a romantic type. At the time of the study,
add-a-beads were popular among sorority women. Such a comment on
clothing, or even a pointed look, was sufcient to communicate an interpretationin the add-a-bead case, that one was acting out a certain
femininity and style of conducting romantic relationships.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

108

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

The women sometimes accepted these interpretations of themselves as


romantic types, sometimes explicitly rejected them, and sometimes ignored them. Della rejected her professors casting of her in a sexual role.
Karla ignored her former teachers claim that intellectuals had different
moral codes for extramarital affairs and that, if she wanted to prove
herself an intellectual, she had to change her ideas about romantic relationships. Cylene, however, seemed to entertain the possibility that a
characterization of her as a romantic type was true. Her father had told
her that she wanted so many thingsa house, a car, a boatthat she was
going to kill her man. The gist of his comment was that she expected a
boyfriend to treat her too wellto provide her with too many material
possessions. She did not dispute his characterization and, in fact, considered it as a possibly valid interpretation.
The women seemed to winnow the possible interpretations, to accept
some parts and reject others. Natalie talked explicitly about a vision of
herself that she had formed as a young child and had since given up, at
least in part: Marrying rich is just a term for marrying the whole ideal
guy . . . thats just a thing that most everybody thinks about . . . a goodlooking rich man who loves me very much and wont let me cook . . . and
lives in a big house. By the time of the study, Natalie had discarded a
piece of this vision of herself as a married woman. For various reasons,
particularly her sisters experience with a husband who could not hold a
job, Natalie had decided to prepare herself to have a career, or at least a
job that would free her from dependence upon a future husbands economic support.
Even though the women were bombarded with images of themselves
in the world of romance, and even though many of their peers seemed to
have sorted through these various interpretations and arrived at fairly
stable views of themselves as actors in the romantic world, some of the
women did not identify themselves as such. They had no clear idea of a
romantic selfeither a self to avoid or a self to realize.5
Susan, for example, had an extremely unsettled and ambivalent identication with romance. At one point she spoke of her trip out west,
saying what a beautiful place it was for a romance. But she had not gotten
involved with anyone because, she said, it was too much trouble. Her
boyfriend seemed more of an accommodation to peer demands than a
reality. And she was openly critical of one of the motives for romantic

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

109

Ex

am

Co

py

relationships: the idea that boyfriends should be a source of prestige. She


took the researcher to her dorm to show her how the girls brought their
boyfriends to the lounge to show them off. In Susans opinion, boyfriends were not for showing off.
Susan spent much time thinking about the sort of life she wanted to
have. Her dilemma seemed to be a choice between becoming a socialite
and becoming a hippie. She was inclined to reject the upwardly mobile, upper-middle-class lifestyle that she felt pressuredperhaps by her
familyto embrace. This struggle was related to her ambivalence about
her studies and about romantic relationships. The reliance upon boyfriends as a source of prestige enacted by women in the student culture
seemed to remind her of the socialite women in her hometown. These
women were typied, in Susans mind, by frequent attendance at the
country club, where they talked on and on about their rich husbands.
Her feelings about socialitesand her distaste when she thought about
herself as onemade identication with the world of romance at SU
difcult for her.
There also were cases in which womenincluding Susanparticipated in relationships without their hearts being totally in them, so to
speak. These women told us of a distance between their true feelings
and the ways they were acting. Cylene talked about having had a steady
boyfriend in high school, but said that, looking back, she probably had
had him as a steady not because she was particularly attached to him
and did not want to date other men but because she was worried about
people talking.6
Susan most straightforwardly expressed the felt distance between her
actions and her true feelings. Although some of the other women also
had absentee boyfriends, who seemed primarily to be useful excuses for
not going out with other men, Susans way of talking about Howard, her
boyfriend, clearly revealed her limited involvement in the relationship.
She talked about their awkwardness when they did get together. She also
talked about Howard in a sarcastic tone of voice that indicated she was
not serious. Describing her feelings about his decision to attend a university halfway across the country, she said, I dont know exactly when hes
going but, um, Im sure Ill see him sometime. So . . . heartbreak, sob and
everything like that! In another instance, when an older man from a
nearby city tried to persuade her to go out with him, she told the inter-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

110

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

viewer: But 30 years old, thats old. I mean Im 18 years old. I dont want
to go out with someone whos 30. Its not that bad butshucks, I dont
want to go out with anybody but Howard. Hes worth the wait.7
Although she was less ambivalent than Susan, Natalie also seemed to
participate only half-heartedly in activities where one might meet potential romantic partners. Natalie did attend many parties and mixers but
seemingly only for appearances sake. As she presented herself in the
interviews, Natalie was much less involved in romantic endeavors than
most of the other women, and much more invested in her schoolwork.
She consistently talked not about boyfriends but about her schoolwork
and her family. She appeared to be little identied with the world of
romance. Her childhood vision of herself as a romantic partner (described earlier) had largely disintegrated as she watched her sisters marital troubles. She spoke positively of becoming, at some future time,
romantically involved with a man, but her vision of this future relationship was vague and without detail.
As these cases suggest, although others continually cast the women as
romantic actors, their involvement with the world of romance varied.
Some resisted being identied as romantic participants; some opposed
parts of the views of romance they associated with peers; some devised
strategies for avoiding or circumscribing their romantic experiences; and
many indicated that their feelings were at odds with the way they were
acting out a relationship.
To reiterate, the women we studied were not equally involved with the
world of romance, and the components of their involvementsalience
and identicationproved to be interrelated. Talk about men, focus on
men, and orientation toward romantic relationships correlated positively
with how much the women talked about and treated themselves as actors in the world of romance. Cylene, Natalie, and Susan, in their different ways, were less involved in their romantic activities than many of the
other women. Although they continued to participate, their comments
conveyed a sense of distance. Correspondingly, they expressed less concern about romance in the interviews than women like Karla, who did
identify with the world of romance and for whom romantic relationships and activities remained salient. The womens individual social situations and histories inuenced their varying degrees of recruitment to
this gured world.
The world of romance is a powerful element of the cultural life of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

111

Romantic Savoir Faire

Co

py

college students, but, like any other frame of activity, it subsists in relation to other gured worlds. All such worlds are subject to social identications, that is, associated with the people who conventionally participate in them, and these people in turn are subject to the social differentia
of rank and prestigeclass, gender, race, and any number of more local
divisions of communitiesthat affect the evaluation, and thus the persuasiveness and authority, of the spheres they populate. This complex of
social and cultural inuence is realized personally, as in Susans case,
where a predisposition against socialites colored her relations to classmates she identied with the romantic world, and hence her vision of
herself in that world. Only by moving between the institutional and the
intimate, between history in its usual sense and history-in-person, can
we do justice to social life. It is this same analytic movement that captures psychic life, because the two aspects are, according to Vygotsky and
Bakhtin, transmutable and indissolubly related.

Ex

am

In concert with their varying degrees of involvement with the world of


romance, the women also seemed more or less expert at, more or less
agentic in, romantic relationships. They differed both in how much they
relied upon the directions and motivations of others and in how they
formulated and responded to problematic situations. Those who appeared to be less knowledgeable or less expert closely copied and took
direction from others, attended to relatively circumscribed aspects of
relationships, and had difculty generating possible responses to romantic situations.
The womens ways of talking about men and relationships with men
conveyed different degrees of facility with the cultural discourse. One
woman had such prociency with campus types that she was able to
extract the essences of the types and put them together in a skit. She
and some others were facile in talking about their own strategies for
handling relationships. Karen talked about keeping the upper hand in
a relationship, and Rosalind about putting one man up front. Though
Karen and Rosalind may not have invented these strategies, they seemed
fully familiar with their use, reecting upon and enacting them with
ease. Susan, in contrast, seemed limited to repeating and attempting to
carry out the directives of others.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

112

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

To illustrate getting the upper hand, Karen, a student at SU, described her behavior toward Hal, a man she had just met. She was trying
to convince him that she was attractivepossibly more attractive than
he was. A woman who is less attractive than a man she goes out with,
according to the conventions of the gured world, compensates by settling for less attentive and sensitive treatment and by allowing the relationship to become physically intimate faster than she might wish. If she
is more attractive than the man, he compensates by being especially
sensitive and attentive to her. Karen tried to get the upper hand by
convincing Hal that other men were interested in her. She tried to give
him the impression that she was dating other men, that she had a boyfriend back home, and that she had a full social calendar.
For Rosalind, a student at Bradford, putting one man up front meant
playing up her involvement with only one man. Romantic relationships
at Bradford were complicated by the need to maintain integrity and
self-control. One lost integrity and control if information about ones
emotional investments became known to those who might use it to
manipulate or interfere with ones relationships (Holland and Eisenhart
1990). Rosalinds idea was to mask the full range of her involvements by
emphasizing one man as her primary romantic interest, thereby deecting attention from all others.
Both getting the upper hand and keeping one man up front were
strategies for achieving the valued outcomes of romantic involvements
while avoiding the bad. These strategies were ones the women themselves had improvised or decided to use and could describe without
much trouble.
Susan gave a quite different impression. During the rst year and a half
of her college career, she talked about gender relations in a variety of
ways, most of which could be traced to her friends. Susan, like many new
students, was seeking out a new group of friends. She struck up friendships with women who pursued a variety of lifestyles and orientations
and had quite different ways of talking about men and gender relations.
In the interviews she sometimes talked about her friends relationships in
terms that we knew were the same terms her friends used. Lee, a friend
of Susans, was having to fend off sexual advances from the owners of the
restaurant where she worked. Susan said that a guy at the caf had
pinched Lee on the buns and told her to loosen up. In another description of the same incident, Susan said that a Greek was messing with her

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

113

Ex

am

Co

py

[Lee]. Buns and messing with her were not the kind of words that
Susan used in other interviews to describe similar situations.
For instance, Susan described her friend Patricias relationships as being open or not open: It seems really good, the relationship they
have, because they talk a whole lot, and its a lot more open [than the]
relationship that she had with George, as far as saying what she wants
and feels. The openness of Lees relationships was not something
Susan talked about, presumably because Lee did not talk about them in
those terms.
Susan used euphemisms to talk about sex, a topic she seemed not to
discuss with her friends. For example, she said she had learned that a
friend (and potential boyfriend) was homosexual and that she was
glad not to have to worry about that with him anymore. Knowing
Patricia and some of Susans other friends, we doubted that any of them
would have referred to sex as that.
Susan also seemed willing to be swayed by other peoples arguments
about her. She described a situation in which an older man was trying
to persuade her to go out with him. The man had called Susans dorm in
search of a woman who no longer lived there. Susan answered the phone.
They began to talk and he asked her out. She put him off rst by saying
that she had homework and then by telling him she had a boyfriend
back homeHoward. The man was not so easily discouraged. As they
continued to talk, Susan admitted to going out with someone other than
Howard. The man retorted that if she went out with other people then
she could go out with him. He almost had her persuaded. In her next
interview Susan debated going out with him, although she eventually
returned to her boyfriend-back-home position. Still she said, But I
guess I should meet him, you know, he seems like a pretty interesting
person to meet.
By the end of the study Susan had settled upon one group as her
primary set of friends, and her talk about men and gender relations had
begun to conform to the way this group talked about such relationships.
Susans motivation to participate in romance, as well as her words,
seemed to be initially imparted by her friends. She did not nd the
culturally dened motives for romantic relationshipsprestige and intimacysufciently compelling to overcome her discomfort with romantic encounters. And, as mentioned earlier, she disputed whether one of
the motivesprestigewas a proper motive for relationships.8

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

114

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

Nonetheless, in her sophomore year, Susan began to look for a boyfriend in earnest. Her efforts were spurred by the desire to participate
in the same activities and talk that her friends did. She identied with
them and their romantic practices. The following passage indicates the
difculty she had in her search and the support supplied by her friends.
She had just said she wanted to nd a boyfriend, and the interviewer had
asked her how one goes about it: You just scope out the crowd rst . . .
see, I found this guy that Im interested in . . . But I never see this guy so
that makes it difcult . . . I get all nervous and paranoid so I cant ever
talk to him. Its pretty funny, all my friends are like: Go talk to the guy,
Susan. Lets go talk to him. I just cant.
Follow-up interviews, especially one conducted in 1987, showed that
Susan had gained condence in the world of romance and spoke more
positively of her romantic experiences. But in 1980 her engagement with
the gured world seemed to be borrowed from her friends.
The women varied not only in their facility with ways of talkingand
presumably thinkingabout romance but also in the breadth of their
overviews of romantic relationships. Susan answered the interviewers
question about nding a boyfriend by outlining small-scale, step-by-step
procedures that one could follow at a mixer or a party. In contrast,
Karens and Rosalinds strategiesgetting the upper hand and keeping
one man up frontpaid attention to larger issues.
Paula, a woman at SU, became annoyed at starting relationships with
men she met at parties only to nd that they were also going out with her
friends. After several such experiences, she decided that meeting men at
parties did not work. In contrast, Aleisha, a woman at Bradford, who was
having similar trouble with the men she was meeting, decided that the
men behaved as they did because the women outnumbered the men at
Bradford. Because they were in demand, she thought, the men became
arrogant and expected more from the women than they were entitled to:
At this school, its about six girls to one guy . . . so the ugly [guys] . . .
think they look like heaven and will try to [talk to you] all the time. Its
really sick . . . Some of these guys have the cutest girlfriends, and I dont
know how they got them . . . He must have the money. Thats the reason
why an ugly guy could get a fairly decent looking girl. He has one of
two things: a car or hes got money . . . And most of the guys here that
look good, theyre real dumb and . . . as far as holding a conversation,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

115

just forget it; Id rather talk to a wall . . . But with so many girls to one
guy, he gonna get somebody regardless of how he act.

Ex

am

Co

py

This woman seemed to have developed a fairly broad overview of the


relationship between men and women, while Paula seemed only to have
learned by trial and error that relationships begun at parties did not work
out. If she had formulated a more general assessment, a theory about the
failure of these relationships, she never articulated it to us.
Besides these differences in the scope of their analysis of romantic
situations, the women also varied in how rapidly and easily they devised
responses in the world of romance. Della, in her encounter with the
teacher who embarrassed her in class, was stymied and could not think
of a response. The only solution she could reach to stop him from making similar remarks in the future was to avoid him.
Susans manner of responding to romantic situations differed from
Karlas. Recall Susans thinking about how to respond to the older man
who asked her out. Her representation showed no overview of the situation, no hypothesis about his actions. She thought about the mans
arguments and her various reactions, such as her feeling that he was
old, without piecing these incidents together into any kind of coherent
account through which she could plan a response. She fell back upon
the absentee boyfriend, Howard, and it was not clear how she might
answer the man should he call again, since he already had countered her
Howard excuse.
Karlas story about her former teachers attempt to seduce her revealed
a different sort of process. Karla seemed to form a hypothesis about the
situation, on the basis of which she then devised a response. She had to
change her hypothesis several times when events did not go as expected.
Each time, however, she seemed to reformulate a new hypothesis and
plan of response, without having to work through the situation piecemeal as Susan did. Her nal response was to have a frienda very large
football playercall the teacher and warn him to leave her alone, or
else. After that the teacher no longer bothered her.

Agency in the World of Romance


The women we studied varied in their identications with the culturally
gured world of romance. Not all found it compelling or salient. They

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

116

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

also varied in their apparent levels of expertise in romantic relationships.


The less expert women tended to repeat the words and follow the directions of others. They had less of an overview of romantic relationships;
they had to work harder to come up with responses to romantic situations. Not surprisingly, those who were less identied with the world of
romance, and for whom it was less salient, also were less expert in
managing relationships.
This suggests that involvementthe salience of and identication
with the cultural system of romancecodeveloped with expertise. If a
woman did not develop a clear identication of herself in the world of
romance, then romance was not likely to be very salient for her and she
was not likely to be much of an agent in conducting romantic relationships. Similarly, if she had not developed expertise, then she was unlikely
to have formed much of a romantic identication. Salience, identication, and savoir faire appeared to develop together in an interrelated
processa process that was continually supported and shaped in the
context of social interaction.
These connections among expertise, identication, and salience are
foreshadowed and informed by the theoretical work of Spiro and Dreyfus. Though each of these authors explicitly focuses on only one dimensionSpiro with cognitive salience, Dreyfus with expert knowledge
both refer to the other dimensions, including identication.
Spiro (1982) is concerned above all with the directive force of cultural symbols and seeks to explain why symbols become more or less
motivating for different individuals. He depicts ve levels of the cognitive salience of cultural systems that form a kind of developmental
progression. We will not recount the details here, but will limit ourselves
to two remarks. First, Spiro points out that cultural systems may or may
not reach a very high level of cognitive salience and so may or may not
have anything to do with instigating or even guiding an individuals
behavior. His postulation of variable cognitive salience certainly applies
to our ndings. Second, Spiro nds it necessary to refer to knowledgeability in order to discuss salience. His rst two stages are devoted to
the individuals developing knowledge of the system. Individuals, his
scheme implies, must develop at least a certain expertise with the cultural system before they will nd it motivating. In our study salience
appeared to codevelop with expertise (Spiros knowledge).
Dreyfus (1984) is interested in the development of expert knowledge.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

117

Ex

am

Co

py

He too proposes ve stages: (1) novice, (2) advanced beginner, (3) competency, (4) prociency, and (5) expertise. He denies the usual idea that
expertise develops through the formulation of more and more sophisticated rules or propositions from which inferences are more and more
rapidly drawn. Instead he argues that it is primarily novices and advanced beginnersnot expertswho rely upon rules. Those who advance further form a more comprehensive, three-dimensional understanding of the cultural system, be it a game such as chess, a
sensorimotor skill such as driving, or a conventionalized, culturally interpreted system of relationships such as romance.
In the novice and advanced beginner stages, the individuals knowledge is mediated by (organized around) rules and maxims. These rules,
which the learner has probably heard from others, address elements or
aspects of the situation. They are like the steps in step-by-step recipes for
winning the game, driving the car, or nding a boyfriend. Susans rules
about scoping out the crowd come to mind.
With competency, the various elements of the situation become organized into a gestalt. The individual learns to think in terms of broader
components of the overall situation. Karens concept of getting the upper hand in her relationship with Hal is an example of such a component.
Dreyfuss description recalls Vygotskys understandings of learning. In
the early stages of learning, cultural artifacts, whether objects, words, or
gures, serve as obvious mediators of peoples activity. We count our
ngers, glance at diagrams, and recite rules to ourselves. After practice
(and for the young, a degree of maturation), however, these means of
mediation are taken into inner activity; words become inner speaking,
and gures become forms of the imagination. We no longer need the
obvious, external form, relying instead on inner means that we can reproduce at will. As our experience with the activity grows ever greater, as
we grow in expertise, another change occurs. This Vygotsky called fossilization. The various parts or steps that we once learned as recognizable elements, and the cultural forms that mediated their learning even
within inner speaking and imagination, lose their separability. The process, that is, the production of activity, falls from awareness, and all that is
left is its product. We simply do something, no longer thinking how
we do it.9
Dreyfuss explanation helps us graduate this process of fossilization

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

118

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

and relate it to other features of Vygotskys work. Following Dreyfus, we


can conceive fossilization as a process of compression in which ever
larger segments of activity become sequentially the elements of attention. This notion is directly analogous, and perhaps homologous, to
Vygotskys (and Bakhtins) characterization of the developing syntax of
inner speaking as a process of compression, where more and more grammatical elements are elided. It is this compression of inner speaking, of
the imagination, that produces the markedly unconscious aspect of expert knowledge that Dreyfus emphasizes. At the competency stage one
must still work consciously, that is, within inner speaking, to arrive at
possible responses and assessments of those responses. At the more advanced stages of prociency and expertise the experience of arriving at a
response changes. The generation and assessment of responses becomes
less conscious. Susans process of thinking about her response to the
older man who asked her out exemplies a less procient stage than that
represented in Karlas account of her response to her former teachers
attempted seduction.
Just as Spiro cannot restrict his levels of salience to salience alone,
Dreyfus does not conne his scheme to expertise alone. In describing the
changes from advanced beginner and competency to more advanced
stages, Dreyfus nds it necessary to refer to emotional involvement and a
sense of responsibility in the system.
The shift between the advanced beginner stage and the later stages of
competency, prociency, and expertise is marked, says Dreyfus, by a
qualitative change in the relationship between the individual and the
system. The individual comes to experience herself not as following
rules or maxims taught by others but as devising her own moves. Dreyfus describes this change as obtaining a sense of responsibility in the
system. Perhaps a better phrasing would be that the individual gains a
sense of being in the systemunderstanding herself in terms of the
activity. She identies herself in the cultural world:
The novice and the advanced beginner applying rules and maxims feel
little or no responsibility for the outcome of their acts. If they have
made no mistakes, an unfortunate outcome is viewed as the result of
inadequately specied elements or rules. The competent performer, on
the other hand, after wrestling with the question of a choice of perspec-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

119

tive or goal, feels responsible for, and thus emotionally involved in, the
result of his choice. (1984: 30)

Co

py

In play, as discussed by Vygotsky (see Chapter 4), the child suspends


other possible interpretations of things in the environment and becomes
caught up in a pretend world. Her desires become related to a ctitious
I, to her role in the game and its rules (1978: 100). Her motives become dened by the motives of the game. Here, according to Dreyfus, the
individual gets caught up in a particular game or gured world and sees
herself as an agent in it. As with play, the overall activity is emotionally
engaging. Dreyfus argues that this sort of participation is necessary for
greater mastery: An outcome that is clearly successful is deeply satisfying and leaves a vivid memory of the situation encountered as seen from
the goal or perspective nally chosen. Disasters, likewise, are not easily
forgotten.
With competency the situation becomes three-dimensional and the
individual is gripped by it:

am

The competent performer, gripped by the situation that his decision has
produced, experiences and therefore remembers the situation not only
as foreground and background elements but also as senses of opportunity, risk, expectation, threat, etc. These gripping holistic memories
cannot guide the competent performer since he fails to include them
when he reects on problematic situations as a detached observer . . .
however, these memories become the basis of the competent performers next advance in skill. (1984: 30)

Ex

Notice that the emotional involvement or identication comes only


after a certain degree of competence is reached and that this degree of
emotional involvement is necessary for further mastery. Dreyfus made
this same point in a talk about the acquisition of expert knowledge in the
game of chess: It seems you cant acquire these skills [of having a complex view of the situation] unless youre taking the game very seriously;
it wont help to just be reading book games. Theres the story that Bobby
Fisher, whenever he plays even a book game, says: Pow, got him, killed
him that time! even though theres nobody [no live opponent] there.
If Dreyfuss notions of the development of savoir faire are accurate,
then identifying oneself as an agent in the systeman actor in the world
as dened by the gameis a necessary precursor to mastering the system

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

120

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

beyond a certain level.10 One has to develop a concept of oneself in the


activity and want either to realize that self or avoid it.
Spiros and Dreyfuss schemes are interrelated. Dreyfus is discussing
expert knowledge, but he nds emotional involvement necessary for
the achievement of the stages of prociency and expertise. Emotional
involvement also gures in Spiros account of the motivating force or
cognitive salience of a cultural system. And Spiros scheme implicates
knowledgeability. His rst two levels refer to the individuals knowledge
of the system. Put together, the two schemes begin to describe an integrated process of the development of expertise and cognitive salience.
Furthermore, both schemes predicate what we have interpreted as identication of self within the cultural world.
Dreyfus, for instance, vaguely notes a sense of responsibility as necessary for the development of expertise: he argues that one cannot go to the
more advanced stages of prociency and expertise without conceiving of
oneself as devising ones own moves rather than relying upon rules and
maxims learned from others. Spiro (1982: 48) calls the corresponding
stage the stage of believingwhen one comes to think of the system as
being true, correct, or right. He is attempting to explain why a supernatural worldpatently impossible to a Western scientic mindcould
seem true.
We believe this key transition that occurs in both schemes is best
recast as identicationthe formation of a concept of self as an actor in
the culturally devised system. Spiro suggests that belief is crucial for
advancement to higher levels of salience. Acceptance of the reality of the
gured world probably is relevant, but it may not be sufcient. An assessment that a culturally interpreted world lacks validity, truth, correctness, or rightness may indeed affect whether an individual can conceptualize the system as relevant to herself. In our study, for example, Susan
had trouble placing herself in the world of romance as interpreted by her
peers. She disagreed with, and indeed was repelled by, the association
between boyfriends and prestige. The world did not seem right to her.
Sandy, in contrast, continued to believe that the world of romantic
male/female relationships existed. She did not come to doubt the world;
rather her concept of herself in such a relationship was uncompelling
and became even less so in comparison with her valued conceptions of
herself in other elds of relationship. Identication, as the more inclusive process, seems to capture better than Spiros or Dreyfuss schemes

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

121

How Figured Worlds Take Hold

py

the point where the gured world in which one has been acting according to the directions of others becomes a world that one uses to understand and organize aspects of ones self and at least some of ones own
feelings and thoughts (see Quinn and Holland 1987).

Ex

am

Co

Our study of young womens differential recruitment to and personalization of the gured world of romance revealed processes through which
romanceas culturally construed in the southeastern United States
became (or did not become) compelling. The larger issues concern the
directive force of cultural worlds and what the process of developing a
self in a cultural world reveals about the motivating force of such gured
worlds. The women in the study belied the commonsense notions that
romance is naturally compelling, naturally salient, and an area in which
competence is readily attained when one reaches a certain age. The commonsense notions imply a homogeneity of interest and competence that
we did not nd. The women varied in their expertise with romantic
relationships and their interest in romantic activities. Furthermore, the
commonsense notions underplay the pivotal signicance of social intercourse in the formation of romantic interests and skills.
A close look at the womens courses in the world of romance suggests
that savoir faire, salience, and identication with romance codevelop in a
process that is integrally connected to social context. In accord with
DAndrades (1992) point that cultural models (gured worlds) generate
goals rather than provide a means to satisfy preexisting ones, we found
that beginners may not know the assumed motives for romantic activityprestige and intimacyor may not nd them especially enticing.
Their knowledge of the conduct of romantic relationships may be rather
piecemeal, their overviews of romantic situations rather vague, their
responses to romantic situations rather labored, and they may not have
developed any engaging visions of themselves as participants in the
gured world described by the cultural system.
For the women we studied, the cultural interpretation of romance
became salient and compelling as their expertise with romantic relationships increased and as they came to form an engaging interpretation of
themselves in the world of romance. Women who were vague and unclear, or resistant to envisioning themselves in the world of romance,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

122

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

were the ones who remained less expert and who found romance relatively unimportant in their lives. Expert women were those who clearly
identied with the world and for whom romance was highly salient.
Ethnographic case studies such as this one and Beckers (1963) make
it clear that the social-interactional context of learning must be considered in any account of the formation of personal engagements. In our
research the ubiquitous presence of peersas participants in romantic
relationships and talk about them, as coaches and motivators, and as
targets of oppositionwas marked. If the women were not all expert
enough or engaged enough in the system of romance to nd it compelling in and of itself, they were all propelled into the activities by the
urgings of others. Further, those who resisted, rebelled, and did not form
identications with the romantic world did so, in part, in reaction to
their peers.
If we are right that expertise, salience, and identication codevelop in
an interrelated process, then descriptions of cultural worlds must become even more complicated than they are now. The manner of presenting the content of a cultural worldas if fully grasped by an expertimplies a level of savoir faire, a level of salience, and a level of identication
that may apply only to a small subset of the people who are presumably
participants in that world. The description may falsely imply a homogeneity of expertise, salience, and identication, as well as a homogeneity
of content.
Unexamined assumptions of homogeneity are a problem not so much
because they may be unjustied according to scientic canons as because
they permit inattention to the social distribution of cultural knowledge
and its role in power relations. Assumptions of homogeneity deect
attention from the social conict, the social symbolism of knowledge,
and individual appropriation and individual resistance, which are important even in an everyday activity like romance. Expertise with romance,
the salience of romantic activities, and the formation of a view of oneself
in the world of romance codevelop in a process that occurs within and is
sensitive to the social-interactional context. Thus the directive force of
romanceits compelling natureintegrally depends upon the realized
world of social position, of hierarchy and power, as well as upon cultural forces.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

III

Ex

am

Co

py

Power and Privilege

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

Positional Identities

Ex

am

So far we have emphasized lived worlds as culturally constituted of


conventional events, improvised but recognizable acts, and talked-about
characters. Another facet of lived worlds, that of power, status, relative
privilege, and their negotiation, and another facet of lived identities, that
of ones self as entitled or as disqualied and inappropriate, must also
receive theoretical attention. In order to highlight these facets, we make
an analytic distinction between aspects of identities that have to do with
gured worldsstorylines, narrativity, generic characters, and desire
and aspects that have to do with ones position relative to socially identied others, ones sense of social place, and entitlement. These gurative
and positional aspects of identity interrelate in myriad ways. Sometimes
they are completely coincident; sometimes one dominates over the other.
Two incidents from our eldwork in Nepal give a sense of the positional
aspect of identity formation.
One day when Skinner was recording songs from three of the girls of
Naudada, Tila Kumari, who owned the house Debra was renting, came
storming onto Debras porch and began to yell at the girls. She accused
them of eating fruit and cutting fodder from her property:
You stupid girls . . . Why do you cut grass from our elds? Are you not
ashamed? . . . Everyone has seen the guavas here, but only you have
eyes for them . . . Go to your husbands or fathers eld to eat. You have
no fear to cut the grass in anothers eld. After telling your parents, I
will weed your hair . . . I will complain like the daughter of Hari [a local
adolescent notorious for her bad temper].1
125
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

126

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

All three girls were upset about this scolding. Muna, one of the three,
said: We hadnt done anything. I cant gure out why they made such a
scandal out of it . . . I felt like crying. I couldnt retaliate. I do not do such
things. I thought, who was she to tell someone like my mother such
things? We did not get angry because we had not done anything wrong.
Thats why I felt bad and wanted to cry.
On another day, Shanta, Tila Kumaris six-year-old daughter, was berated because she ventured into an activity to which she had no right.
Shanta was in the eld where her older brother was plowing. As he
steered the oxen past her, she reached out playfully and touched the
plow. Immediately her brother began to hit and rebuke her. Her father
and other brothers ran from the house to join in the scolding and beating, and her father told her it was a sin for a girl to touch a plow and
threatened to beat her severely if she ever touched it again. Shanta cried
and trembled for hours. When asked about the incident several weeks
later, she remembered it well, repeating her fathers words and promising
never again to touch the plow. Although Shanta did not understand the
wider moral universe in which her act was dened as a sin, she did learn
in a dramatic way that, because she was a girl, she was restricted from
acts that her brothers could freely perform.

Positional Identities in Figured Worlds

Ex

Shanta and the three girls had become embroiled in incidents in which
their activity was constrained; they had encountered rebuffs to their
freedom of movement and freedom of action, limitations of what we will
call their social position. Others treated their actions as claims to social
positions, positions that, on account of gender in Shantas case and perhaps of relative caste position in the girls case, they were denied. In Tila
Kumaris eyes Muna and the other girls were appropriate targets for
demeaning. By the acts she suspected they had committedentering her
yard and taking her fruitthey had claimed positions to which they
were not entitled. The same fault could be found in Shantas innocent
intrusion into the male domain of the plow.
Sociolinguistics has long made much of the socially constitutive import of language choice (see Gumperz and Hymes 1972). Ones choices
of dialect, register, pronouns, and genre are not socially neutral. Such
decisions partake of powerful systems that index claims to the social

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

127

Ex

am

Co

py

relationships between speaker and hearer and to the speakers general


social position. Linguistic choice is but one of these indexical systems.
There are other systems of artifacts (returning to the language of Chapter 3) that carry with them a theory of the person who uses them.
There could well be elds of sociosartorial studies, sociocognitive studies, even socioemotive studies, since styles of dress (Turner 1969), displays of knowledge (Gearing et al. 1979; McDermott 1974; Lutz 1995),
and expressions of emotion (Lutz 1988, 1990) all index social categories
of persons. Spaces, too, imbue and are imbued by the kinds of persons
who frequent them; conventional forms of activity likewise become impersonated. The dialect we speak, the degree of formality we adopt in our
speech, the deeds we do, the places we go, the emotions we express, and
the clothes we wear are treated as indicators of claims to and identication with social categories and positions of privilege relative to those
with whom we are interacting.
This is the stuff of positional or relational identities, and much research in anthropology and other elds shows that this aspect of identities is important. Relational identities have to do with behavior as indexical of claims to social relationships with others. They have to do with
how one identies ones position relative to others, mediated through the
ways one feels comfortable or constrained, for example, to speak to
another, to command another, to enter into the space of another, to
touch the possessions of another, to dress for another, or, recalling Gyanumaya (Chapter 1), to enter the kitchen of another.
Hindu religious tenets posit a world of the sacred. In that extensively
elaborated world, different castes are more or less intrinsically pure, and
ones caste affects how polluting one can be to others. Perhaps Tila Kumaris reactions to the girls in her yard had to do with concerns about
pollution. But her treatment of Muna and the other girls was not cast in
these religious terms but rather in terms of forbidden territory and forbidden goods. It was more about what we will call positional identity
than about what we have called gurative identity. Positional identities
have to do with the day-to-day and on-the-ground relations of power,
deference and entitlement, social afliation and distancewith the social-interactional, social-relational structures of the lived world. Narrativized or gurative identities, in contrast, have to do with the stories,
acts, and characters that make the world a cultural world. Positional
identity, as we use the term, is a persons apprehension of her social

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

128

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

position in a lived world: that is, depending on the others present, of her
greater or lesser access to spaces, activities, genres, and, through those
genres, authoritative voices, or any voice at all.
Bourdieu (1977b) makes some useful observations about the way the
phenomenon that we are labeling positional identity manifests in different social situations. Here he extends his notion of habitus to speaking,
to encompass ones sense of the value that is likely to be attributed to
what one has to say in a particular situation. Bourdieu acknowledges, as
did Bakhtin (1986), speakers awareness of the differential social valuing
of languages, genres, and styles of speaking, and he emphasizes the
habitual, out-of-awareness assessments one makes before and during
conversation: judgments of the linguistic forms that are likely to be
valued, of ones command over those linguistic resources, and of the
social privilege (or lack thereof) that a person of ones relative position
has to employ such resources. The assessment reveals itself in the way
speech is marked, leading the speaker to strained, self-conscious, correct speech or to effortless, unselfconscious speech; to comfort or to
discomfort; to voice or to silence.2
We have already drawn an imperfect but helpful analogy between the
two aspects of identity we are trying to distinguish and two aspects of
linguistic utterances. Figurative identities are likened to propositionality,
to the referential, semantic facet of speech; relational identities are likened to indexicality, to the pragmatic facet (see Irvine 1989). The import
of a particular linguistic element, determined by its relationships with a
system of other linguistic elements, is different from the import of the
element dened in relation to its social situation of use. Understanding
the difference between the meanings of mother and father is not the
same as understanding the difference between the social relations indexed by calling the same person Pop rather than Daddy or Father.
Likewise, understanding and identifying with the narrativized positions that allude to ones caste, as portrayed in some Hindu religious
texts, is quite different from understanding and identifying with the
indices of privilege, power, and entitlement that mark the relation of a
Bahun to a Damai in NaudadaTila Kumari to Munaor the position of
Shanta, a girl, vis--vis her brothers. Put perhaps too simply, gurative
identities are about signs that evoke storylines or plots among generic
characters; positional identities are about acts that constitute relations of
hierarchy, distance, or perhaps afliation.
Localized gured worlds have their own valued qualities, their own

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

129

Ex

am

Co

py

means of assessing social worth, their own symbolic capital, to use


Bourdieus term. In the world of community relations gured among the
Kabyle peasants (Bourdieu 1977a), men and families were ranked by
honor. Men of high honor had more symbolic capital; what they did and
said as men of honor was likely to be accorded more credence and more
authority than the actions of men of low honor. Their word was more
likely to be honored by others; a sort of social credit was more likely to
be extended to them than to men of low honor. Likewise, relations
among Kabyle men were generally gured in the idiom of honor. Gifts
were a central cultural artifact, for example, that positioned the giver
relative to the recipient in a script of reciprocity; if the recipient could
not reciprocate he lost honor.
In the world of male-male relations in the Nicaraguan village studied
by Lancaster (1992), machismo named an important calculus of social
worth. Social position and relations among men and between men and
women were often gured in this calculus. A mans relational position
was played out in terms of the value accorded the active, penetrating role
in intimate sexual activity, for example. A man who took the active role
in sexual relationswhether with a man or womanafrmed his masculinity.
In the world of romance we studied (see Chapters 5 and 7), attractiveness was an important form of symbolic capital. Relations between
men and women were often cast into a calculus of relative attractiveness.
Hence a mans treatment of a woman was taken as an index of his assessment of her attractiveness relative to his own, and vice versa. Relational/positional identities in the world of romance have to do with the
assessment of ones attractiveness. In the taken-for-granted, unfolding
script of developing intimacy and romantic love, a man and a woman are
attracted to each other. He treats her well, and she permits the relationship to become more intimate. Actions index the development (or failure
to develop) of an intimate relationship, but they also index the relative
attractiveness of the parties to the romance and thus the entitlement of
each party to the others attention and support.

Positions and Markers That Cut across Figured Worlds


Relational identities and the cultural artifacts through which they are
claimed may be specic to a gured world. They may have to do with
ones honor in the Algerian peasant village Bourdieu tells us about, or

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

130

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

ones attractiveness in the sphere of gender relations on college campuses


in the United States, or ones machismo in the Nicaraguan village. Other
positional identities and markers may, however, be less specic and cut
across such worlds.
In the testimony of witnesses in court cases, OBarr and Atkins (1980)
identied several patterns of speechsuch as the use of hedges, locutions like there may be or perhapsthat experimental studies later
showed to undermine the credibility of the testimony of any witness who
exhibited them. There was a correlation between the use of these nonassertive patterns of speech and being female and/or of low social power.
OBarr and Atkins see these speech behaviors as responses Americans
use in formal situations that include people who are more powerful than
the speaker. These speech behaviors are not peculiar to the gured world
of law courts; instead, hedges, intensiers, and hesitation forms,
among others, communicate relative position in other gured worlds as
well.3 They are indices of relational identities that are not particular to
any gured world.
In Naudada, too, there were cultural artifacts or resources for positioning self and other that crossed a number of gured worlds, those of
domestic/household relations, school, and parma (reciprocal work
groups). One important set concerned food. Taking certain kinds of food
directly handled by another allows a kind of intimate contact that can be
considered polluting if the handler is of lower status. To refuse food
handled by another, then, is to claim greater moral worth and a higher
social position. Conversely, accepting food indicates either ones afliation and equality of status with its handler or ones deference to a highercaste donor.4
Besides artifacts, social categories also can have meaning across
many gured worlds. These categories are by and large associated with
the major social divisionsgender, class, race, ethnicitythat separate
those who are routinely privileged from those who are not. Cross-cutting
markers tend to become stereotypically associated with these social categories, if not actually demanded of their members in practice. Studies in
the United States have found that women use hedges, tag questions,
and other deferential forms more often than men (Lakoff 1975; OBarr
and Atkins 1980; Cameron et al. 1988). In Naudada, Bahuns were popularly stereotyped as greedy and maliciously clever, always looking to take
advantage of others. These stereotypic associations between marker and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

131

Ex

am

Co

py

category are the (practical) part and (cultural) parcel of the reproduction
of social division, and of the categories that are its elements.
A controversy has raged in anthropological linguistics over womens
styles of speaking in the United States and whether they should be
viewed in the enduring, almost essentialist terms of subculture or in
the relational terms of situated practice. Tannen (1990) and others take
the former position, which we have called culturalist. Women grow up
learning a set of values and ways of being in the world, which they
translate into ways of speaking. McConnell-Ginet (1988, 1989; Eckert
and McConnell-Ginet 1992), among others, takes a more constructivist
position, arguing that women (and men) grow up learning how positions
of relative power are communicated. Since women are often forced into
positions of lesser power, they end up using deferential speech strategies
more often than men.
Our approach does not automatically give us the answer to the controversy, but it does suggest that the situation is too complex to be settled by
adopting either the culturalist or the constructivist position. It seems to
us a double mistake to think of these cross-cutting relational identities as
somehow cleanly separable from gured worlds. First, it is erroneous to
believe that gured worlds all partake of these differentia in like manner.
Rather, conventions of privileged access associated with gender, caste, or
some other major social division may or may not have been taken up,
elaborated, and made hegemonic in a particular gured world or eld of
power. In the schools of Naudada, the language of hierarchy communicated through the rules for exchanging food and water was being used to
subvert the system of caste privilege; in the tea stalls, the same language
was being used to maintain it.
A second mistake, the reverse of the rst, is to think that styles of
indicating relative social position can be cleanly separated from a gured
world of moral meaningsthat they are arbitrary indicators which
evoke no gured world of their own.5 In the world of academia, being
verbally aggressive may be a sign of high status and position; but for a
person who has formed an identity in a gured world where maintaining
egalitarian relations is important, verbal aggressiveness has connotations
of a moral failing.
Signithia Fordham (1993), drawing from a long-term study of Capital
High School in Washington, D.C., provides an example of both of these
points. She reveals the nuanced ways in which major social divisions

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

132

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

may be elaborated and made crucial in a localized gured world. She


describes some of the excruciating consequences of participating in social formations where the styles of one social category are taken as indices of quality and achievement for all people. She writes especially about
African-American women and their disposition to silence themselves in
the academic activities of the high school. Although Capital Highs student body had, at the time of her study, an African-American majority,
the space of its activities ideologically privileged styles that were white
and male.6 The school was oriented toward a particular style and way of
being a student. Its model of the good student was one who behaved as
did white men. The African-American women students, as a result, were
in a place where the styles meaningful at home were considered signs of
being a poor student.
That these styles of speaking and acting could not simply be put on or
taken off at will is evident from Fordhams analysis as well. There were
especially bleak results for the African-American women who tried to
suppress their own styles and move toward the valued styleswho tried
to pass. By passing, Fordham means a more subtle and gurative kind
of performance than the literal passing for white: Passing implies impersonation, acting as if one is someone or something one is not. Hence,
gender passing [for African-American women] suggests masquerading
or presenting a persona or some personae that contradict the literal
image of the marginalized or doubly refracted Other (1993: 3). Too
much passing, she argues, drawing upon Said, can result in a potentially
subversive self where the person can no longer speak or even think in his
or her native voice. Such persons lose their creativity: because they
are compelled to assume the identity of the Otherin exchange for
academic successthey cannot represent themselves; they are forced to
masquerade as the authentic, idealized Other (26).

Taking Up Social Positions


What Fordham shows us in vivid detail are the results of a long process.
Many of the women she studied had developed what we call a positional identity, a sense of their relative social position, which, among
other things, led them to silence themselves within the gured world of
school. Fordham also describes other young women who had developed
an oppositional stance, rejecting the strategy of effacing themselves in

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

133

Ex

am

Co

py

order to conform to the model of a good student. How had the young
women at Capital High arrived at these positional identities? What interests us is the processes of development over the long term. The long
term, however, happens through day-to-day encounters and is built,
again and again, by means of artifacts, or indices of positioning, that
newcomers gradually learn to identify and then possibly to identify
themselves witheither positively or negatively, through either acceptance or rejection.
In the groups that we studied, as in any group engaged in jointly
creating and participating in a gured world, day-to-day practices always
positioned the participants situationally, relative to one another. That is,
participants in collaborative activitiesbe they staff members producing
a treatment plan in a mental health clinic, old hands welcoming a newcomer to an AA meeting, or a romantically involved couple going out to
see a lmengaged in conversation and interaction that invariably constructed their own social position and their social relations with one
another.
As social constructivists emphasize, these discourses and the other
forms of cultural artifacts used in everyday practices construct subjects
and subject positions. These positions are, at least provisionally, imposed
upon parties to the practice. The sociologist R. W. Connell provides an
example of this idea when he sums up the argument that gender is more
appropriately treated as a process or a practice than as a static attribute or
an enduring characteristic. He suggests that gender would be more
transparent if it were a verb rather than a noun: that its signicant
qualities would be clearer if we said, I gender you, you gender me, she
genders him; or, better yet, I try to gender you, you try to gender
me, and so on (Connell 1987: 140).
Entitled people speak, stand, dress, emote, hold the oorthey carry
out privileged activitiesin ways appropriate to both the situation of the
activity and their position within it. Those who speak, stand, dress, hold
the oor, emote, and carry out activities in these proper ways are seen
to be making claims to being entitled. Speaking certain dialects, giving
particular opinions, and holding the oor are indices of claims to privilege.
Examples of positional markers, and of the social work that goes on to
maintain their value, abound in ethnographies. In the gured world of
gender relations in a Mehinaku village in Brazil, described by Gregor

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

134

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

(1977), the mens house was an important part of the apparatus of male
privilege. Men, especially when gathered together, protected the inviolability of that space. Women were threatened with gang rape should
they enter the house and see the sacred utes housed there. Men also
engaged in taunting women as a group during certain festivals.7 Heln
Page (1994) details the ways in which public issues in the United States
are dened in the public sphere from a white perspective, and how this
guarded space preserves the system of race privilege. Hill (1995) shows
how the speaking of junk Spanish by nonSpanish speakers in the
United States works to debase the speaking of Spanish, keeping it an
index of lower social position.
In Naudada, husbands usually addressed their wives with the less
respectful pronominal form used to address children and animals. The
respect and deference owed to superiors were communicated through
other pronouns. Thus each time he used this form of address a husband
afforded his wife a position inferior to his own. In reply, the usual,
respectful forms of address that wives employed for husbands would
accord him the greater deference. Their social relationshiphis superordination, her subordinationwas constantly reconstituted by the very
media of this discursive practice.
Another example of the affording of a position comes from a practice
in Naudada that depends upon the food etiquette of castes. The tea stall
owners of Naudada served members of the Damai and other untouchable castes, but required them to wash their own cups and glasses.
Because people of higher castes avoided food or utensils that had been
touched by people of castes considered to be so impure as to pollute, tea
stall owners, by enforcing this rule about washing, implicitly claimed to
be, or positioned themselves as, pure, superior, and keepers of purity for
others. They claimed the qualities of the higher castes, elaborated in
relation to the gured world of caste relations, at the same time inviting
customers of lower castes to enact a subject position of inferiority. For
similar reasons, and with similar implications for the parties relative
positions, husbands refused to eat leftover food from their wives plates,
but it was not expected that wives would refuse their husbands leftover food.
Viewed over the long term, these day-to-day practices are social work,
acts of inclusion/exclusion, of allowing/compelling only certain people
to evince the sign, that maintains positions and the value of artifacts as

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

135

Ex

am

Co

py

indices of position. Bourdieu (1977a) shows that maintaining ones relative position in the Kabyles gured world of honor demanded such
social work on a day-to-day basis. The value of relational indices and the
relative right to use them are maintained through social work of the kind
performed by Tila Kumari and Shantas brothers. These valuations and
the restrictions that enforce them have to be imposed, and the outcome
of their imposition is uncertain. There is no guarantee that those upon
whom they are imposed will not try to refuse the implicit positioning, as
did Muna. There is no guarantee, in fact, that everyone will have been
brought sufciently into the language of the relational markers to
understand the would-be impositions and take them seriously.
Much of the inclusionary/exclusionary work of sign restriction is done
simply by the inclusion or exclusion of certain types of people from
sites where the signing knowledge is interacted as a matter of course
(see Rogoff 1990). Trosset (1986) describes the difculties of a foreigner
learning to speak Welsh in Wales. For various reasons, Welsh speakers
invariably switch to English in the presence of a nonWelsh speaker,
thus effectively limiting the signal means to their identity as Welsh, the
ability to speak the everyday tongue.
Gearing and colleagues (1979) describe how processes of inclusion/exclusion work in the absence of clear controlthat is, even when
dialects can be overheard, or more privileged ways of acting can be
directly observed and therefore imitated. They analyze the means by
which children in school are kept from learning skills that are taken as
indices of social position. They emphasize that knowledge (reading,
knowledge of chemistry, skills in repairing machinery) is proprietary;
that it is generally associated with, or belongs to, a recognized category
of people; and that, by virtue of this relation, the use of knowledge signals identity.8 Hence, even in situations where all students are admitted
to the arena of learning, learning is likely to become unevenly distributed in its specics. Teachers will take some students groping claims to
knowledge seriously on the basis of certain signs of identity. These students they will encourage and give informative feedback. Others, whom
they regard as unlikely or even improper students of a particular subject
(girls and shop skills, working-class students and philosophical essays),
are less likely to receive their serious response.9
Bourdieu (1984, 1977a) addresses the issue of timing: the point in
their development when children are given the opportunity to learn

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

136

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

behavioral markers of privilege. He argues that many styles of acting and


matters of taste that serve as indices of high social position are more
easily learned in childhood. Those who learn activities in childhood
perform them in a more natural, less self-conscious style, itself prestigious. Those who learn later usually retain an awkwardness, a more
mechanical than organic sense of the activity. It is a complicated
point for Bourdieu, one aspect of which concerns the proprioceptive
acuity of childhood learning.
More to our purpose, late learners are impeded not only by their sense
of inferior propriety or right to act, which their late access signals, but
also by the withholding of aid and the acts of disapproval (such as
sarcasm, practical jokes, and straight misinformation) through which
knowledgeable associates enforce their superior claim. Many never overcome that awkwardness, which inevitably affects the acceptance of their
claims and reproduces the situation of inferiority. They are always lesser
than. And they may always feel self-conscious, linking durably in their
persons the structures of privilege to the dispositions of silencing and
acceptance that such structures demand. These are the dimensions of
difference to which Lykes (1985) alludes when she argues that individuals have different senses of self because, senses of self being grounded in
experiences of power, individuals have differential access to the positions
of power that afford the experience. People develop different relational
identities in different gured worlds because they are afforded different
positions in those worlds.10
Bourdieu is careful to point out that the Kabyle mans protection of his
status in honor, and of the indicators of honor itself, goes on in a largely
automatic way; he does not mean to suggest, although he is often misinterpreted in this regard (Bourdieu 1990a), that consciously strategic maneuvers are the rule. Rather, just as Fordham focuses on the dispositions
to silence and effacement that some of the women students at Capital
High had developed in the school context, Bourdieu focuses on the
embodied dispositions developed among Kabyle men to protect their
honor. He details how adult men, in ways beyond their awareness, maintained a constant vigilance for slights to their honor. They noticed and
were ever likely to react to social claims made by others of greater honor,
and they worked indefatigably to refuse the positions of inferiority afforded them by those claims. Social positions, in other words, become
dispositions through participation in, identication with, and development of expertise within the gured world.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

137

How Social Position Becomes Disposition

Ex

am

Co

py

Within the constructivist emphasis on the importance of discursive positioningpositioning by the cultural artifacts of discoursethere is also
a counterpart open to those who, afforded positions, do not always take
them up. Della and Karla, two students discussed in Chapter 5, provide
contrasting examples of this kind of refusal.
Della rejected her professors attempt to recast her as other than a
student, his guring of her as a type from the world of romance and
attractiveness. But here we want to go beyond these ideas of positioning
and resistance. Della was inexperienced with this sort of incident. Although she did not accept the position afforded her, her rejection was
rather feeble. She showed her embarrassment and tried to forestall future
incidents simply by eluding the teachers notice.
Karla took stronger measures. For instance, during a period when she
was ill, she felt neglected by her boyfriend and thought he was making
excuses not to see her because he was afraid her illness was contagious.
Generally, she felt he had begun to take her for granted. Finally she
challenged him, claiming that he was treating her as though she were a
germ (Holland and Skinner 1987).11 In the incident described in Chapter 5, Karla was pursued by her former teacher, who saw her as a potential lover. Although he used various meansincluding trying to position
her (against her own attraction to the intellectual life) as ignorant of the
attitude of intellectuals toward adulterynone succeeded. Karla intensied her means of refusal with each new attempt, until she nally succeeded in warning off the teacher. Karla was one of the women in our
study who were more determined, more active in the world of romance,
more of an agent. She was adept at refusing positions that were afforded
her and instead creating positions she liked.12
It is important, in understanding positioning, to pay attention to the
fact that positional identities develop heuristically over time. The Vygotskian emphasis on semiotic mediation is helpful for understanding the
process by which children, or neophytes to gured worlds of any age,
develop the dispositions of relational identities. Semiotic mediation is
also a means by which these dispositions can be countered and sometimes overcome.13 People may develop a sense (in Bourdieus terms) of
their worlds, an expertise in the use of cultural artifacts, that may come
to re-mediate their positions in them.
The development of social position into a positional identityinto

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

138

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

dispositions to voice opinions or to silence oneself, to enter into activities or to refrain and self-censor, depending on the social situation
comes over the long term, in the course of social interaction. Relational
identities are publicly performed through perceptible signs. People tell
each other who they claim to be in society in myriad ways. In Naudada,
Skinner found, children learned early that a womans marital status was
expressed by diverse signs: by the tilhari (a type of necklace) that married women wear, for example, or the vermilion powder worn in the
part of their hair. They learned that tea stall owners would not wash the
glasses of a lower-caste person, nor would high-caste persons allow
lower-caste persons to enter their homes. But much of this learning may
happen in a nonreective way.

Embodiment versus Mediated Positional Identities

Ex

am

In the ordinary developmental sequence proposed by the sociohistorical


approach, the child rst interacts the sign in concert with others. The
interaction of the sign, for the child, is part of a behavioral sequence that
may have no meaning in and of itself. It is likely that relational identities
are borne in a similar way, in what phenomenologists used to call the
natural attitude, the uninterrupted ow of everyday life, Bergsons dure.
The meaning of actions remains transparent or taken for granted in the
natural attitude, and response follows as a matter of course. The formation of identity in this posture is a byproduct of doing, of imitation and
correction, and is profoundly embodied. Positional identities inhabit the
landscape of Bourdieus habitus.
Adults too can be drawn into a gured world and develop a relational
identity without much reection upon the social claims carried in the
forms of their action. Kondo (1990) provides a telling illustration of the
effects of such embodied processes. Kondo, a Japanese American who
grew up in the United States, relates something that happened to her
after a period of eldwork in Japan, a time in which she had immersed
herself in the daily lives of people in her neighborhood. She went shopping one day:
Promptly at four p.m., the hour when most Japanese housewives do
their shopping for the evening meal, I lifted the baby into her stroller
and pushed her along ahead of me as I inspected the sh, selected the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

139

py

freshest looking vegetables, and mentally planned the meal for the
evening. As I glanced into the shiny metal surface of the butchers
display case, I noticed someone who looked terribly familiar: a typical
young housewife, clad in slip-on sandals and the loose, cotton shift
called home wear, a woman walking with a characteristically Japanese bend to the knees and a sliding of the feet. Suddenly, I clutched the
handle of the stroller to steady myself as a wave of dizziness washed
over me, for I realized I had caught a glimpse of nothing less than my
own reection. (1617)

Ex

am

Co

Kondo had acquired the dress, posture, and habits of a young Japanese
housewife (or so she identied them) simply by immersing herself in
social activity from the position that her gender and her associates assigned her. Her acquisition of the dispositions that marked a particular,
gendered identity had occurred without her awareness, and the moment
of recognition was disorienting. The image of herself in the butchers
display case and the image of herself in her minds eye did not match, and
that disparity led Kondo to distance herself from her eldwork. Immersion gave way to a reticence in which she gauged her activity according
to the recalled standards of her self-image.
No doubt many aspects of positional identities develop out of ones
awareness, as Kondos had before she caught a glimpse of herself. No
doubt many behaviors develop out of awareness, unmediated by ones
reection upon them as claims to social position. One simply participates in the typical arrangements of people in houses, say, where one is
more likely to nd, in the United States, women in the kitchen and
men in the workshop or yard; or in Naudada, women at the hearth, men
at the tea shop. Or one simply takes part in everyday activities and nds
young Japanese(-American) women walking about with sliding steps
and slightly bent knees, pushing babies in strollers as they shop in the
afternoon.
Occasionally a child may be admonished in a way that includes mention of social position. A mother may tell her daughter that girls dont
do x. Shanta was certainly warned away from the plow in a dramatic
and upsetting way, but perhaps the usual path to relational identity is
through simple associations that pass unnoticed in any conscious way.14
It may not be until later that the associations between social positions
and entitlements become a matter of reection. The child at rst, and
perhaps for a very long time, may not associate the access that she gains

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

140

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

or does not gain to spaces and activities with the language of social
claims. She may not consciously realize that her developing dispositions
are caste-specic. Especially at rst, she undoubtedly does not say to
herself, Tila Kumari is a Bahun. To go into her yard is to claim to be of
higher caste than I am. If I go there, she is likely to mistreat me.
Other indices of positional identities, however, become conscious and
available as tools that can be used to affect self and other. Muna interpreted Tila Kumari as accusing her and her friends of being morally bad.
She defended herself, to herself and to Debra, as not deserving such an
evaluation. In the lived world of Naudadan domestic and community
relations, a womans social worth was gured according to the life path of
the good woman (described in Chapter 3). Girls of Munas age had long
been used to hearing and using terms such as radi (widow) to insult
others. These were derogatory terms for women who had strayed from
the life path of the good Hindu woman. They had even come to understand why radi was a negative terma widow was a woman who had
fallen from the life path by outliving her husband. Further, the girls
cared about their own moral standing in the community and often constructed for Skinner representations of themselves as good daughters and
sisters (see Chapter 10). Munas emotional response to Tila Kumaris
scoldings indicated her attachment to the understanding of herself as a
good person.
In other words, some positional identities and their associated markers are clearly gured. In Naudada, good works and good behavior, when
contrasted to the misbehavior that constitutes pap (sin), are noted and
related to the script of the life path of a good person. In the world of
romance of college students in the American South, attractiveness as a
form of symbolic capital, a measure of ones social worth, was gured
extensively, not only as a key element in the development of romantic
relationships, but also as a motive for numerous projects, practices, and
artifacts of beautication. Perhaps these gured aspects of relational
identities become relatively conscious for anyone successfully recruited
into the gured world where such qualities are deemed important. The
everyday aspects of lived identities, in contrast, may be relatively unremarked, ungured, out of awareness, and so unavailable as a tool for
affecting ones own behavior.
We do not mean to imply that identities subsist in two exclusive
forms, one type a pure product of imagination and the other of un-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

141

Ex

am

Co

py

reected action. Rather, consider two orientations or attitudes to the


production of social activity. Figured identities arise and are reproduced
in the special attitude of play or, more precisely, imaginative framing. As
we noted in Chapter 3, Vygotsky describes play as a special social form of
activity, one in which the conditions and modes of action are not those of
the actual, that is, everyday setting, but of an imagined template cast
over the everyday. He uses the simple example of a childs playing at
riding a horse, when what the child rides is merely a broom. What
counts in play, and what counts in the identities of gured worlds, is the
cultural relations, the rules, that govern the movements of a game. The
meanings of play, like the meanings of langue, are only attached or assigned to the material tokens that instantiate them. Their source lies in
the imaginative system.
Mundane activities, in contrast, have become a matter of habit. By
differentiating between mundane activities and the more dramatic, remarked activities singled out in gured worlds, we create not a dichotomy but a continuum. As Bakhtin maintained (Voloshinov 1986), even
everyday behavior has an ideology, a cultural world. Hence the relation
of play or life in a marked frame of imagination to everyday activity is
more properly one of specicity (one that is marked) to generality (one
that is unmarked). The two situations are convertible. Specic gured
worldssuch as that of romantic love, which began as play in the courts
of eleventh-century France (see Chapter 11)can be made public over
time to become more mundane genres of the everyday, and the generic
gures of social life can become specialized, survivals in Edward Tylors
sense, and turned into the stuff of folklore and exotic ethnography.
Vygotsky called the process in which the historical sources and the
distinctiveness of behavior are erased by its automation fossilization.
In a sense some imaginative frames become fossilized in mundane
daily life. But fossilization is not irreversible. Ruptures of the taken-forgranted can remove these aspects of positional identities from automatic
performance and recognition to commentary and re-cognition. (Kondos
experience is an excellent example.) This hermeneutic moment leads
persons to specify the gured world that pregures everyday activity.
This disruption happens on the collective level as well. Some signs of
relational identity become objectied, and thus available to reection
and comment, in relation to the social category. Alternative gurings
may be available for interpreting the everyday, and alternative ways of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

142

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

guring systems of privilege may be developed in contestations over


social arrangements. We heard in Naudada gurings of caste that differed from those given in Brahmanical texts. Schoolboys told Skinner
that the blood of the lower castes was red, like that of the higher castes
members of different castes were at root the same, and not naturally
different.
At some point the lower-caste child may begin to grasp consciously
the emotional, evaluative nature of the signs that are used to position
him as inferior, as a source of potential pollution for all those whose caste
is higher than his own. By drinking from the glass at a tea stall, the
Damai, the Sarki, the Kami pollutes it. At some point the child may begin
to use the sign in relation to social category and in turn as a symbolization of how others experience him. Typically a child will reproduce
the comments of adults in his own egocentric speech, that is, in the
speech stream that frames, comments upon, and comes to manage action. Through continually objectifying or symbolizing himself enacting
the sign, he becomes able to experience a version of himself as an object
in a social world given meaning by these signs. Eventually this means
of objectication, which is a form of self-management, becomes what
Vygotsky and Bakhtin called inner speech (see Chapter 2), and what
we consider an element of the imagination. One particular lower-caste
boy so resented the position assigned him because of his caste that he
dreamed of returning to Naudada as a doctor, a gure of such importance
that higher-caste people would have to accept food and water that he had
touched (Skinner and Holland 1996).
The women in Hollands study of romance often talked about such
objectications of themselves and ruminated upon them. Often these
representations took the form of voices, of Bakhtins (1981) vocal images, while others were visual images. Similar processes of objectication
were evident for the children that Skinner followed in Nepal. These
visions often seemed to motivate (plans for) action, sometimes even
life-changing action.15 Kondo suggests that for her, too, the image
reected in the display case became a mediating device; it allowed her to
think about what she had become, and to attempt to change it.
Children in their development and neophytes entering into a gured
world, then, acquire positional dispositions and identities. At some level
of apprehension, they come to know these signs as claims to categorical
and relational positions, to status. More important, they learn a feel for

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Positional Identities

143

Ex

am

Co

py

the game, as Bourdieu calls it, for how such claims on their part will be
received.16 They come to have relational identities in their most rudimentary form: a set of dispositions toward themselves in relation to
where they can enter, what they can say, what emotions they can have,
and what they can do in a given situation.
Explorations of the processes of these formations, followed in more
depth in the next chapters, suggest that the associations children and
neophytes form with different spaces and activities in their environment
are, in fact, social positions that have become dispositions. Gendered
dispositions to participate, or not, in given activities, develop in places
where gender participation in activities is treated as a claim of gender
specicity. Dispositions of lower-caste Naudadan individuals and groups
to speak, or not to speak, with an authoritative voice in public gatherings
where members of higher castes are present develop in places where
speaking is treated as a claim to caste position.
Yet positional identities are not without their disruptions. The same
semiotic mediators, adopted by people to guide their behavior, that may
serve to reproduce structures of privilege and the identities, dominant
and subordinate, dened within them, may also work as a potential for
liberation from the social environment. We will see in the next chapters
how people sometimes x upon objectications of themselves that they
nd unacceptable. These objectications become the organizing basis of
resentment and often of more active resistance. When individuals learn
about gured worlds and come, in some sense, to identify themselves in
those worlds, their participation may include reactions to the treatment
they have received as occupants of the positions gured by the worlds.17
Yet the metapragmatic capability to gure social practicethrough
narrative, drawing, singing, and other means of articulationis at the
same time a capability to gure it otherwise than it is. Its rehearsal is
the opportunity to re-create dispositions to activities-in-mind that give
shape to this dissatisfaction. The interchange or convertibility of the two
contexts of identity provides opportunities to reform either by recourse
to the other. In other words, symbols also enable the objectication of
the treatment that women or lower-caste people receive, so that narrativized worlds are created around stories of these treatments and visions
of better treatment.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

The Sexual Auction Block

am

In our study of college women in the southern United States (see Chapter 5), the womens identication within the world of romance entailed
that they form a sense of the social value of both self and others in
that world.1 Identication with that world meant taking, to varying degrees, what Bourdieu (1977a, 1977b; Bourdieu and Passeron, 1977)
called symbolic violence into ones self and perpetrating (and perpetuating) it against others. Settling social positionthe subtext of interaction and the stuff of positional identitiesis a matter of struggle, often
muted or even unrecognized, whose effects live on in personal and social
history.
To illustrate the point, let us begin with Karens description of things
that had happened since her last interview:

Ex

A friend of mine [Annette] invited us . . . to a party in the dorm. And


she told us that thered be . . . a couple of people there that she really
liked a lot: guys, that is . . . and . . . when we got there . . . the main one
she wanted to see . . . didnt even hardly acknowledge her presence. He
practically didnt even speak to her . . . And it just sort of messed up the
whole partymainly for her, and because of that, it messed it up for all
of us. I expected . . . some real nice guys. I thought theyd be really glad
to see her. But . . . the one she wanted to see acted real stuck-up, as if
she wasnt even there. . . . He ignored her . . . Shed be standing practically beside him, and he wouldnt say anything.

The interviewer asked why the man, Sam, had acted the way he did. I
dont know . . . maybe she just had it in her head that he liked her . . . and
144
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

145

py

he was just trying to talk to this other girl or something. But he did act,
he acted sort of too good for her, you know?
Annette was upset. Karen explained that Annette was trying to reason
out Sams actions:

Co

Interviewer: What were some of the ways she reasoned it out?


Karen: Um, well, she thought at rst maybe because he was with that
girl, he didnt want to talk to anybody else. And, but then, he was
talking to other girls that were walking by, and um, then she was
thinking, maybe he was mad at her, but she didnt know why, you
know, she was just thinking of different stuff like that.
Interviewer: Did you think of any things like that too?
Karen: Uh, not really, I, I, its gonna sound terrible. I thought, well he
just didnt want to, didnt want to see her at all, cause he just
didnt, I dont know, what I thought was that, he was like I said
before, he was some big jock on campus, you know, and he just
wanted the real, just certain girls around him, you know.
Interviewer: What . . . kinds?
Karen: Really pretty.

Ex

am

Karens embarrassment in telling this story was palpable. Her discomfort draws our attention to the importance of attractiveness. Karen was
clearly embarrassed to say explicitly that her friend was not pretty
enough for Sams tastes. It was as though Annette had something to be
ashamed of, and Karens speech marked that shame. And, even though
Annette was obviously thinking over Sams behavior, Karen did not try to
save her time and worry by explaining. This incident underscored, for
us, the pivotal aspect of attractiveness: Why should not being really
pretty be something to be embarrassed about?
No doubt there was another source of Karens embarrassment. Annette
had publicly and boldly proclaimed her attraction to Sam, and, by approaching him, had in effect laid claim to a comparable attractiveness of
her own, only to have it demolished by his lack of interest. In the student
culture attractiveness was very important. It was at the heart of the
gender status quo. Annettes claim to social worth among her peers had
been proven false (for further analysis see Holland and Skinner 1987;
Holland and Eisenhart 1990).
In Chapter 5 we noted that some women were more adept than others
at claimingand refusingroles in romantic activities and that, as a

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

146

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

matter of that same skill, some were more adept at claiming a high
level of attractiveness. We want to pursue that clue and direct our attention to how these relational aspects of identity in the world of romance
are negotiated actively within social interaction. Only by following this
course can we understand Annettes vulnerability to the kind of insult
she suffered and, the broader point, womens vulnerability to the symbolic violence that marks the ways romance is lived.
How were women developing their sense of place as gendered actors
in the world of romance, and as more attractive or less attractive individuals? An important means is participation in the discursive practices
described in the chapters on gured worlds and positionality. We noted
in Chapter 6 the idea that gender is more appropriately treated as a
process or a practice than as a static attribute or an enduring characteristic; that, in Connells words, gender would be more transparent if
it were a verb rather than a noun (1987: 84). We would argue analogously about the more specic, gendered quality of attractiveness. It
would be more accurate to use a verb that meant to make attractive: I
make you attractive, you make me attractive, she makes him attractive, and so on. This phrasing de-essentializes the quality by revealing its social-relational dynamicsthe continually negotiated constructions and reconstructions that create real attractiveness and attractive
people.
Because of its attention to everyday encounters and to the maneuvers
and dilemmas of participants actively engaged in those encounters, the
focus on practice, and on the constitution of images and statuses through
interaction, is reminiscent of symbolic interactionism. Unlike most traditional forms of symbolic interactionism, however, practice theories go
well beyond the encounters at hand. Bourdieu (1977a, 1990b), addressing practice in general, and Connell (1987), addressing practice in relation to gender, both build on symbolic interactionism, adding a critical
step. They theorize about how systems of power and privilege render the
participants of encounters more or less equal, more or less like agents,
and more or less interpersonally powerful. Here too we must situate
these interactions in a larger system, by recalling the cultural sources
that give these discourses and practices their potency. One important
source has already been introduced: the gured world of romance. Along
with it, other gured and positional aspects of identities in the United
States and the Southrace, class, sexual orientationevoke and give

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

147

Ex

am

Co

py

shape to what might be called the playing eld of day-to-day romantic


encounters.
When we submit identity to the rigors of practice, to this complicated
eld of play, an interesting thing happens. The gured world of romance
predicates certain genres of gender relations and the acts and characters or positions that are meaningful in them. In encounters these generic worlds may be evoked through gestures, words, phrases, looks, and
movements, and the relative positions of the parties to the encounter are
constructed and counter-constructed.2 Yet, in practice, these worlds are
not everywhere the same and their instantiations are not automatic.
Rather, the evocation is potentiated and differentiated by, rst, the positional markers that constrain peoples tness for certain claims and
rights of use, and, second, by evocations of other gured worlds, other
ways to dene the situation. The playing eld is the space made of these
possibilities. Southern discourses of romance, it turned out in practice,
were double-edged. They provided for the pleasurable construction of
desire, to be sure, but they also constructed a world in which the parties
to desire could gain or lose attractiveness and prestige. These gendering
discourses and practices were powerful vehicles for the playing out of
romantic selves; they were also potential means of degradation.
In open-ended interviews, the women told us about many incidents
involving actual or potential romantic/sexual partners, especially the upsetting ones, like the story of Annette, in which they or a friend had been
mistreated and insulted by men. They also talked about incidents in
which they or a friend had been, by their own assessment, inappropriately treated as a romantic/sexual partner by a professor or boss. Unlike
Annette, who willingly sought Sam as a romantic partner, these women
neither sought out nor desired romantic involvement with their bosses
and teachers. Nor did the women in similar stories Holland has since
been told, stories evoked by her lecturing on this material.3 Whether the
women invited the mens assessment or not, these instances involved
insults, insults both to their attractiveness and (consequently) to their
prestige.

Incidents of Symbolic Violence


The double-edged quality of romantic discourse can readily be discerned
from such cases. It can also be discerned from a videotaped simulation of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

148

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

such an incident. Analysis of the videotapes transcript allows us to explicate aspects of the positionality of romantic identities in general and
to explore what happens when these identities are evoked in the workplace. The video captures visually the importance of gestures and spatial
positioning. It is useful in particular for understanding how the gendered identities of the world of romance are evoked clandestinely in the
workplacehow the playing eld becomes complicated by two gured
worlds. In much of the incident, what is spoken stays in the realm of
work. The world of sex and romance and the positioning of the participants in that world are evoked largely through nonverbal gestures and
physical contacts.
The video clip we quote was part of a televised program on sexual
harassment in the workplace. The simulated incident was arranged by a
social psychologist, John Pryor, to illustrate his research on the conditions under which men are more likely to sexually harass women at work
(Pryor et al. 1993). Our focus is not on the behavior of the men in the
incident but rather on what this behavior can tell us about the readings
of like incidents by the participants in our studies.

Ex

am

Narrator: During a psychology class, Dr. Pryor re-created one of his


more revealing studies.
Two men, John (the experimenter), and Mr. Williams (the subject), walk
up to a desk on which is a computer; they shake hands.
John: What were going to do here, Im trying to evaluate a program
Ive developed for the Department of Ofce Management. Okay?
Mr. Williams sits down at the desk and John sits on the edge of the desk.
Narrator: In this scenario, a graduate student who is actually
participating in the experiment leads the research subject to
believe that he will be training undergraduate women to use a
computer.
John: What youre going to do is instruct the girl thats going to be
coming in here on how to correct these mistakes. Okay? Now I
dont know if you noticed the girl out in the hall, but the girls that
are being sent down here by the Department of Ofce
Management are real foxy.
Narrator: In reality the real purpose of the experiment was to see if
the research subject would harass given the opportunity.
John goes out and calls Cindy. A woman comes in. Mr. Williams stands
up.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

149

Ex

am

Co

py

John: Cindy! It is Cindy, isnt it? Im John. How are you?


They shake hands.
Cindy: Im ne, thank you.
John: Dont I know you from somewhere? You look like an old
girlfriend of mine. (He still has her hand. Cindy laughs.) I want
you to come on over here. Cindy, this is Mr. Williams.
Cindy and Mr. Williams shake hands.
Mr. Williams: Hi, Cindy.
John: Mr. Williams will basically be your boss here for the next fteen
minutes. So alls you have to do is listen to what Mr. Williams has
to say and follow his instructions. Okay? Okay?
Cindy sits down at the desk.
Narrator: By the design, the graduate student, who is really part of
the research design, purposively harasses the womansetting an
example for the computer trainer to do the same.
John is standing behind Cindy. He puts his hands on her bare
shouldersshe has on a boat-top sweaterand leaves them there
as he speaks.
John: Make yourself comfortable, then just follow Mr. Williamss
instructions and youll be doing okay. Okay?
Cindy nods her head.
John (to Mr. Williams): And you remember that youre going to ll
out an evaluation sheet?
Mr. Williams: Yeah. Okay.
John: Okay.
Mr. Williams: Okay, thanks a lot.
John leaves the room.
Narrator: Left alone, and left to believe that sexual harassment is
permitted and even condoned, the computer trainer takes full
advantage of the situation.
Mr. Williams: You want me to give you the sentence now?
He is standing behind the chair. He looks at his paper, then puts his right
hand on the chair behind Cindys right shoulder.
Cindy: Okay . . . Whatd I . . .
Mr. Williams: Its number three. Just hit number three up here.
He reaches around her with his left hand and hits the key. When
he removes his hand from the keyboard, he grasps her left
shoulder.
Cindy: Do I have to hit return?

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

150

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

Narrator: Although this is a re-creation, similar behavior was


observed during 90 percent of the experiments.
Mr. Williams: Thats good.
Cindy: Okay. And?
Mr. Williams: Just use those. Those are ne.
Narrator: Shannon Hoffman, who participated in the real studies and
has just played Cindy in the simulation, remembers feeling
vulnerable because of the permissive situation created by the men
in charge.
Mr. Williams kneels by Cindys right side and leaves his left arm around
her. A few seconds later he pats/rubs her back.
Mr. Williams: And thats all you really need to do. Just hit the letter
and youll be ne.
The scene changes to Shannon Hoffman being interviewed on camera.
Shannon Hoffman: It was very uncomfortable for me. I realized that, if
it had been out of the experimental setting, as a woman I would
have been very uncomfortable with someone that close to me and
reaching around me like that. So, it really made me feel a little bit
powerless because there was nothing much I could do. But I also
realized that perhaps in a business setting, if this person really was
my boss, then it wouldve been harder for me to send off the
negative signals, or whatever, to try and fend off that kind of thing.

Ex

The simulation provides a vivid case of gendering, and a plausible


example of the type of incidents described to us by our studys participants. We now want to read this incident according to the gured world
of romance that we inferred from our studies. Through such interpretations, women and men live gender ideologies in day-to-day encounters.
In order to avoid awkward, conditional phrasings, we will treat the participants of the simulation as though they were drawing upon the background knowledge we found common in our studies.

The Experimenter Genders Himself and the Other Man


The initial conversation between the two men set the stage for the incident. It drew upon the discourse of romance to construct the woman as
an object of desire. Of equal importance, the experimenter, in the process
of guring the woman, gendered both himself and the other man. The

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

151

Ex

am

Co

py

experimenter, John, said to the subject, Mr. Williams, Now I dont know
if you noticed the girl out in the hall, but the girls that are being sent
down here by the Department of Ofce Management are real foxy.
Here John drew upon a lexicon, comprising hundreds of gendermarked terms, that men in our studies had for talking about women as
gures of romance.4 This male lexicon provided many terms that ascribed value to different types of women and described the ways they
were likely to behave in romantic relationships. The term foxy positioned the womanrelative to other culturally typed women, such as
broads, babes, or cowsas one who was sexy, a companion with
whom a man would want to be seen, and likable in that she was not
likely to be overly demanding (Holland and Skinner 1987).
In our studies of the lexicons that men used to refer to women and
women to men, it became abundantly clear that these vocabularies were
not just tools of representation. The words were used to do social work
as well. What stood out for many of the men and women we studied
was less the semantics of these gender-marked lexicons than the ways
in which the terms could be used. Their rst remarks were often about
the potential use of the words as insults. For example, some respondents grouped terms such as bitch, shrew, dog, dumb blonde,
and airhead together because they could all be used to insult a woman.
(Other terms, such as sweetheart, were said to be endearments.)
Against the conceptual horizon provided by the gured world of romance, terms such as dog, bitch, and the milder womens libber
could be used to position, negatively, one woman relative to others.
Bitches were women who in the mans view overvalued their own
attractiveness and so demanded too much. A spurned lover would demean the woman who rejected him by telling friends that she had turned
out to be a bitch. Likewise, women had an arsenal of termssuch as
prick, nerd, and assholethat they used to claim that particular
men were of little value.
The social work of the terms was even more complex. Ones partners,
or those to whom one was attracted, reected back upon ones own
attractiveness. In the taken-for-granted scenario, those who were equally
attractive were attracted to one another.5 Hunks were not attracted to
nags or dogs but to foxy girls or dolls, and likewise foxy girls
were not attracted to jerks, nerds, wimps, or turkeys. Still, the
attributionexactly who was a hunk and who was a creepvaried

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

152

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

by participant and was not carved in stone. There were no absolute


measures of attractiveness; there was no authority that could denitively
assess who was and who was not attractive. Instead, attractiveness was a
symbolic capital attested to, aggrandized, or devalued by the attractiveness of those one approached and the treatment one received from them.
Both women and men read these words as making claims about
their own attractiveness as well as that of others. The naming of others in
the world of romance has a characteristic described by Boltanski and
Thevenot (1983): social classication systems tend to classify the classier as well as the classied. A woman who referred to a man as an
asshole was implicitly claiming that he held no attraction for her at all,
and thus that she was more attractive than he. By classifying a man as a
playboy, a woman admitted both his attractiveness and the possibility
that he would try to treat her as though she were more attracted to him
than he to her. In other words, she admitted the possibility that he was
more attractive than she.
On their part, men used words such as bitch or knockout to make
claims about particular women and, reexively in the process, about
themselves and the listener as well. Against the conceptual horizon provided by the world of romance, a man who referred to a woman as a
dog, a bitch, or a womens libber was positioning not only the
woman but also himself. He was claiming that the woman, as far as he
was concerned, was unattractive, and therefore (the claim reecting back
upon him) that he was more attractive than the woman.6
In the case of attractiveness, the classiers not only classied their
targets and themselves, they also classied those who were intimately
related to the classied. People of similar attractiveness became intimate
in the taken-for-granted situation of the gured world. Thus a womans
boyfriends attractiveness was a sign of her own attractiveness. If a
womans boyfriend was called a nerd, her attractiveness also was impugned. If a man called another mans girlfriend a dog, he also implied
that her suitors were unattractive. The reexivity of these categories
enabled an individual to propose a hierarchical ranking of a number of
people, including himself or herself, simply by labeling one person.
By labeling the women being sent down by the Department of Ofce
Management as foxy, the experimenter, John, evoked the world of
romance and a comparison of the women according to their attractiveness. He construed Cindy, the woman who was there for computer train-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

153

A Gendering Insult

Co

py

ing, as a prestigious object of desire, and thus when he later approached


her in an intimate way he cast himself as an attractive, desirable man.
Here was a foxy woman with whom he could be familiar. With his
comment about foxiness, John also drew the subject, Mr. Williams, into
the gured world of romance and sexuality. He gendered Mr. Williams by
implying that Mr. Williams would participatewith John and from the
same desirable perspectivein the world of male/female relations. He
implied not only that he and Mr. Williams were heterosexual but also
that they shared standards of taste in women and were roughly equal in
attractiveness.

Ex

am

In the two mens interactions with the woman, they used gestures that
invoked the world of romance and sex. John said to Cindy, Dont I know
you from somewhere? You look like an old girlfriend of mine. He put
his hands on her in an intimate manner, an intimacy that was unexplained by any prior history. His gestures and phrases, and the later
physical intimacy of Mr. Williams, all invoked a clandestine discourse, a
discourse that opened a space within the workplace that was ambiguously, if at all, governed by the work at hand, or the gured relations of
occupational specialization and merit. The space was instead infused
with gender relations, interpreted according to the model of romance,
and peopled by types who were relevant to that world.
Yet the work went on, the computer trainer continued the training.
The sexual world was always close to the surface, threatening to overwhelm the ostensible activity, but the discourse remained submerged,
clandestine. Hence, here and in the incidents described to us, it was
difcult for the women to refuse such a gendering. Especially in cases
where gestures and innuendo were the language of choice, the evocation
of the world of sex and romance could be counter-charged to the woman.
Should she too blatantly or explicitly avoid the unwanted intimacies and
communicate her refusal too forcefully, she could be accused of reading
meaning into gestures inappropriately, of seeing something that no one
else sees, or of fantasizing that she is attractive enough to be the object of
a mans desire. She could be accused of purposefully evoking the world of
romance herself. Her clothes, her makeup, her posture, and her movements could be scrutinized as cues, and put forward as evidence of her

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

154

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

desire. Why was Cindy wearing a sweater that left the tops of her shoulders bare?7
When constantly invoked, despite a womans wishes, the discourse of
romance constituted a refusal to treat her as worker or student, on a par
with other workers or students. It was a refusal to accord the woman any
value other than that of her attractiveness. This is a general strategy of
male resistance to the incorporation of women into traditionally male
worksites (Weston 1990). The situation simulated in the video clip is
more complex. The work went on; the computer trainer continued the
training; but he did so in such a way that the world of romance and
sexuality inltrated the activity at hand, giving all acts a double signicance. The incidents women described to us were similar. Sometimes
they were even more blatant, and sometimes more ambiguous. We did
not hear of situations in which male teachers or employers absolutely
refused to treat a woman as a student or employee. Instead we heard of
situations in which women were suddenly, without warning, removed
from the context of work, treated as objects of desire, and positioned in
the world of romance.
The women in our studies, most of whom were between eighteen and
twenty-two, were shocked when such incidents happened to them or
their friends. This was especially true of women who had been accustomed to deciding which men were and which were not in the group of
potential romantic/sexual interests. Some of these bosses or teachers
were considered too old to be appropriate partners, and their romantic
approach was doubly troubling to the women. Especially distressing
were those incidents that were observed by fellow students or employees. The women invariably described themselves as intensely embarrassed. They felt belittled and unable to counter-attack. (In none of the
cases did the teller report that the audience came to her defense.)
Some of the women were learning through experience that their vulnerability to what we call the sexual auction block extended into the
workplace and the classroom. They could be reconstituted, without their
invitation or permission and before their fellows, from student or worker
to object of desire and token of attractiveness. Some of the women experienced these incidents as revelations. One incident sufced to make
them realize that the clandestine discourse of romance and sexuality lay
just under the surface in every classroom and workplace: that they might
at any time be treated as a romantic or sexual object regardless of their

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

155

py

absorption in the task at hand, regardless of their desire not to be so


treated, and regardless of how inappropriate an arbiter of their attractiveness the man in question seemed to them to be. This subtext, once
revealed, led to a more complex apprehension of men in positions of
authority.
This shift in perspective was affectingly articulated by the young
woman we call Karla. After the incident described in Chapter 5, in which
one of her former teachers had approached her as a potential lover, she
concluded somewhat sadly:

Co

But, you know, this has caused me to do a lot of thinking about the
nature of the human male. Here is a man you would never suspect . . .
upstanding member of the community, seemingly happily married.
What more could he want? And trying to seduce an eighteen-year-old
girl . . . and an intellectual seduction, too. Its made me a little leery of
older male teachers who act incredibly friendly, because there have
been a lot of them, that I always looked upon as just really nice people
who were interested in their students. But now this whole other dimension has been added of, Hey, wait a minute; maybe hes not interested
in my intellectual development.

am

An Insulting Gendering

Ex

The woman in the video clip was exposed to a gendering insult: without any invitation on her part and in a work situation where it was
supposedly irrelevant, she was evaluated and treated according to her
attractiveness. But there was more to the incident: not only was the
gendering itself an insult; its substance or content was insulting as well.
The discourse of romance evoked in the incident was double-edged. It
constructed desire, but it also positioned the parties to the romantic
encounter, ranking them according to their relative amounts of the symbolic capital, attractiveness.
Similarly, on the two college campuses we studied, the gured world
of romance provided a means for creating gendered persons of more or
less value, possessing more or less symbolic capital. Students used the
lexicon of gender-marked terms to position themselves and others. Partners treatment of each other was also read as claims about their relative
amounts of symbolic capital.
Romantic and sexual activity were understood by the students to be

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

156

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

about attraction. They were organized around the taken-for-granted unfolding of a romantic/sexual relationship that we presented in Chapter 5:
Two equally attractive people were drawn to each other. The man treated
the woman well: he appreciated her; he recognized her special qualities;
he did nice things for her; and he was sensitive to her desires. The
woman, in turn, showed her affection for him and allowed the relationship to become more intimate. Both were rewarded by the intimacy of
the relationship and by the validation of their attractiveness.
In this gured world, attraction was the force that produced intimate
cross-gender relationships, and the ability to attract was a crucial quality
of those who would participate in the world of romance. Attractiveness
determined how a woman would be treated by men. Attractive women
received attention, gifts, and intimacy and gained access to whatever
social and material amenities the men wished to share. Unattractive
women either received ill treatment from attractive men or settled for
relations with unattractive, less prestigious men. And it worked similarly
in reverse: attractive men were admired and chosen as intimate partners
by attractive women; unattractive men had to settle for less attractive
women.
At rst attractiveness seemed to be construed as an intrinsic characteristic of a person, a set of qualities that some had naturally, others
cultivated, and many lacked. Closer examination, however, showed attractiveness to be less an enduring or essential quality than a product of
ones relations and activities. It was drawn from the attractiveness of
those who were attracted to one and how one fared in romantic/sexual
encounters.8 A mans attractiveness was afrmed, increased, or diminished by each of his encounters with women, according to the outcome.
The same was true for a woman. Attractiveness was a form of symbolic
capital: a putatively unchanging attribute of the person that actually
uctuated according to the treatment he or she received from others.9
There were ways to make up for mismatches in attractiveness. A relatively unattractive man could succeed with a relatively attractive woman
if he treated her especially well. He could be exceptionally sensitive to
her, especially creative and clever in pleasing her. In this way, expectations of good treatment became a language of claims to relative attractiveness. A woman who seemed to expect special treatment, with little
demonstration of affection on her part, was interpreted as claiming to be
more attractive than her partner. If a man felt a woman was demanding

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

157

Ex

am

Co

py

too much, overrating her own attractiveness relative to his, he could


retaliate by calling her a bitch. By the same token, if a man expected
intimacy and affection from a woman and yet treated her badly, he was
interpreted as claiming that she was less attractive than he. The woman
too had the verbal means to counter this assessment, and many reasons
to use them. For, in this only partly verbal language of claim and counterclaim, acquiescence demonstrated agreement. If a woman stayed with
a man who treated her as less attractive than he, she enacted her lesser
attractiveness.
In this guring of sexual relations, rape constituted an ultimate form
of ill-treatment. It was an acting out of the mans sense that the womans
prestige was negligible. And, as others have argued (Sanders 1980), because attractiveness was attested to by the treatment women received
from men, the very act of rape created the victims low prestige. Furthermore, in the logic of this system, women who accepted ill-treatment
deserved that ill-treatment and relations with unattractive men. As one
of the women in the study said, The hall slut deserves the dullest guy
around.
In the incident depicted in the video clip, both men continued to treat
the woman in intimate ways, though she had given no indication that
she desired intimacy with them. Except for Johns eeting acknowledgment that Cindy looked like the type of woman he was willing to
have as girlfriendan accolade that is more or less valuable depending
on ones assessment of his attractivenessneither man did anything for
her that counted as good treatment in the world of romance. They gave
no indication of sensitivity to her wishes. They gave no sign that they
saw her as a special person. Nor did they do anything for her in the way
of special favors. By the standards of the gured world of romance, they
were claiming to be prestigious, desirable men whose attractiveness was
so much greater than the womans that she could expect no special
treatment from them. They acted as though she would surely welcome,
or at least allow, intimacy regardless of their relative disregard for her.
In short, they played out the entitlements of men possessing large
amounts of symbolic capital, men of great attractiveness and high prestige. And in the process they devalued the womans symbolic resources;
they implied that her capital was less than theirs. The woman not only
had to contend with a gendering insult but with an insulting gendering
as well. She faced both the double-talk of romantic discourse, masked

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

158

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

within the workplace, and its double-edged nature. The discourse can
construct desire and pleasure, but it can also construct degradation.
Women in our study described many incidents in which they felt
demeaned by mens treatment. The incidents had the themes just elucidated: their (potential) boyfriends and lovers enacted positions of greater
entitlement and thus cast the women as relatively unattractive. The
women complained of being treated as though they were only one of a
crowd of women, many of whom would do just as well. They also objected to mens expectations that they would rush into intimate relations
with little prelude. They disliked being made to feel as though they were
simply props that men used to demonstrate their status to their buddies.
The incidents in the classroom or the workplace were similarly insulting and even harder to manage. At parties and social events, in dating
relationships, a woman had ways to counter a mans view of his superior attractiveness. She could oppose him by trying to insult his attractiveness. She could publicly label him a prick or a jerk or a nerd.
Or she could simply leave, without much fear of damaging retaliation on
his part.
Return for a moment to Karens account of Sam, the big man on
campus, and his treatment of Annette, the ordinary girl. Karen described
her own reaction to Sam: I didnt think he was attractive anymore. Her
inclination was to demean him, to label him an unprestigious man, an
unattractive type: I wanted to tell him . . . he was acting like an ass.
The women had more trouble responding to such insults from bosses
or instructors; they were not so ready to insult the symbolic capital of
men in positions of authority. Potential retaliation from a boss or an
instructor could be much more damaging. Compared with men of their
peer groups, bosses and instructors had more power to impose their
vision of the divisions of the social world and their position within it
(Bourdieu 1985b: 732).10 It was difcult, indeed dangerous, to resist
because the boss or employer had an independent source of attractiveness/prestige as a result of his position. It was also difcult because of the
double-talk nature of the clandestine discourse. Shannon Hoffman, who
participated in Pryors experiments, commented in the video that, even
though the situation was articial and sure to end quickly, she experienced a sense of powerlessness. She was being treated as a low-status
object, a person of few entitlements, and placed in a position where she
could be ridiculed if she protested.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

159

The Buxom Woman in the Bikini

py

An incident from a paper written by one of Hollands students, a


woman we will call Eva Haley, shows some of the same elements of the
case portrayed in the video. The paper concerned the history of pinup
calendars, and it included a brief tale of Haleys experience with such a
calendar:

Co

In 1985, I was [the general] manager at a new car automobile dealership . . . One day I entered the parts department for a consultation with
the [parts] manager. As the door closed behind me I turned and without warning came face to face with a very large pinup calendar sponsored by a tool company and displaying a bikini-clad, buxom woman
whose exaggerated body positioning and seductive gaze infused the
image with clear, culturally coded sexuality.

Ex

am

In this brief description this incident seems quite different from the
one simulated in the video. The parts manager did not put his hands on
Eva. He said nothing about his old girlfriends. He said nothing about
bikinis, nor did he call attention to the bikini-clad woman on his door.
There was no indication that he had put up the calendar in anticipation
that Eva would see it. But Haleys reaction suggests a similarity: I felt my
face ush with anger and shame as the ground holding me on an equal
level with my fellow manager shifted beneath my feet.
The same popular ideology of romance, the perceptible evocation of a
gured world, made Eva Haleys situation as potent and insulting as the
situation faced by the woman in the video. The pinup on the calendar
recalled the world of romance and attractiveness. The space that was
ostensibly a workplace was infused with gender relations. In that space,
Haleys superior position as a manager of the car dealership, relative to
her colleague, the parts manager, was rendered less potent. Instead, their
positions in the world of romance and sexuality, that is, of a woman in
relation to a man, were brought to the fore. To the extent that she had to
occupy that position, the basis for her claim to superior status as a
worker was vitiated. She experienced a gendering insult.
Furthermore, just as in the video, the gendering was an insulting one.
The means of insult were different; the outcomes were similar. In the
video simulation Cindy suffered an insulting gendering when the men
treated her as being less attractive than they were. In order to understand

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

160

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

how Eva Haley underwent an insulting gendering, recall that gender


typing affects all those involved in the world of romance and attractiveness. Gender labeling redounds upon both the classier and the person
listening to the classication. In Haleys words, the image dened a
standard of beauty and a role by which to judge women, leaving me . . .
physically substandard. The calendars picture of the woman in the
bikini evoked a ranking of women according to attractivenessa ranking in which Eva felt devalued. Like gender typing by wordsthe use of
foxy in the videoso too the image on the calendar reected upon the
person observing it.
We would extend Haleys analysis by adding that the parts manager
was making a claim about his own tastes and attractiveness. He implied
that he was attracted by handsome women, and so that he was a handsome man. The calendar evoked a three-way ranking of Eva, the woman
in the bikini, and the mana ranking in which the man was elevated as
Eva was put down. The man had created a gendering space that offered
him a claim to the symbolic capital of attractiveness.11 Women entering
the space were invited to feel disempowered.
Haley wrote that she was angry and ashamed. Yet, so far as we know,
she did not try to retaliate against the man. Why might she have refrained? After all, she was the general manager and he was her subordinate. Again, the answer lies in the double-talk that conceals the discourseor, in this case, the imageryof romance and sexuality in the
workplace. Had Eva confronted the man, she could have been accused of
raising the specter of romance and sexuality herself and of being the sole
author of any invidious comparisons between her and the bikini-clad
woman. Again, note the double-edged nature of the talk. A pinup calendar can perhaps provide a shape for desire, but, in this case, it gured an
act of degradation.

Privileging Spaces

Evas response to the pinup calendar shows that her position as a woman
in the gured world of romance has become disposition. She was very
much affected by threats to her social worth, to her symbolic capital of
attractiveness. Her dispositionto see herself through the gured world
and to recognize her vulnerability to insults to her attractiveness, even in
a situation that was supposedly divorced from romancewas also the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

161

Ex

am

Co

py

disposition whose development the women in our study described. Recall such situations as the one where Dellas looks were singled out for
sexual innuendo by her professor (Chapter 5) and the encounter between Sam and Annette that Karen recounted.
Men, too, were vulnerable to having their attractiveness insulted by
women, to having their social worth belittled, to bearing the brunt of
symbolic violence. Women could accept their gifts, their attention, and
their good treatment and still not reciprocate with any show of affection. They could refuse to allow the relationship to become more emotionally or physically intimate. Women could insult them verbally as
well, referring to them as pricks, nerds, creeps, and other unappealing names. Nonetheless, our studies showed that womens treatment
by men had a greater impact upon evaluations of their social worth than
the relationship reversed. That is, the gender ideology discussed here
had more importance for women than for men.
On the face of it, the romantic/sexual relationship that constitutes the
standard of meaning in the modelthe pairing of attractive people who
give and get equally, and the expected outcome of intimacy and prestige
for bothseems to favor neither the woman nor the man. But, in the
student world of the late 1970s and early 1980s, equality was not the
rule. Womens attractiveness and prestige were far more dependent upon
their relationships with men than vice versa. Men had diverse sources of
symbolic capital. Men on the basketball team and men of (potential)
wealth, for example, had sources of attractiveness and prestige that were
largely independent of how women treated them.
There was no such alternative source for women. Participation on
womens sports teamsno matter how many championships the teams
wonadded little to the athletes attractiveness to male students; the
same was true for womens participation in student government or university performance groups. The playing eld for romantic encounters
was not equal. When Sam snubbed her, Annette had little else to fall back
on for validation of her social worth. Had she similarly snubbed him,
Sam, a well-known athlete, would not have been as bereft.
It is clearly fairly easy to create spaces, gured by the world of romance, in which men are especially privileged. This easy access to privilege is a consequence of mens more numerous sources of symbolic capital. Yet other social and material contexts may affect the ways popular
ideologies of romance are played out in particular sites. One woman in

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

162

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

our study pointed out that women outnumbered men at her university.
Consequently, in her view, all the men enjoyed an inated sense of their
attractiveness and prestige. Men who, in a larger market, would have
been considered undesirable were treated as though they were attractive.
Hence, for that time and on that campus at least, they were attractive.
Pryors ndings, reported in the video, could be interpreted along the
same lines. The experimental conditions set up a situation in which
simply being male gave all the men who participated symbolic capital
and its attendant entitlements relative to the women. In the words of the
videos narration, the experimental conditions established a permissive
situation. The men felt free to be intimate with the woman.
We would augment this analysis by drawing upon the ideology of
romance and attractiveness. The experimental conditions established a
privileging space with two characteristics. First, male/female relations
were interpreted according to the gured world of romance and attractiveness. Through his comments and actions the person in authority, the
experimenter, evoked this world. Second, the conditions created a space
that automatically afforded the men greater symbolic capital than the
women. The comments and actions of the experimenter, from his position of authority, not only evoked the world of romance but also positioned him and his male confederate as attractive/prestigious relative to
the woman. That authoritative position gave the men reason to expect
that the woman would treat them as attractive.
The most unsettling aspect of Pryors ndings, especially to those of us
who are women living in this society, is how little effort it took to create
a privileging space in which a high percentage of the men involved felt
entitled to treat the woman as though her social worth were considerably
less than theirs.12 It sadly corroborates other evidence, such as the all
too frequent occasions when male athletes, misusing the attractiveness
and prestige attributed to them, act out their entitlement by mistreating
women. The fraternity rape described by Sanday (1990) is an even more
extreme case. Pryors ndings are also particularly distressing because
the situation was putatively a workplace, where attractiveness should be
irrelevant. His data conrm that many women in the United States are
widely subject to evaluation, and potential degradation, in terms of the
popular ideology of romance and attractiveness, no matter the context of
activity.
That a high percentage of the men in the experiment engage in harass-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

163

am

Co

py

ment does not lead us to think of the men, or men in general, as intrinsically or essentially sexual harassers. It does lead us to think of the way
the popular ideology of romance and attractiveness empowers men in
certain situations. It suggests that the ideology of romance was a potent
part of these mens experience; the world of heterosexual romance and
sexuality was very salient to them and very easily evoked. Pryors experiment spells out how easily men may be induced to adopt the positional
identity of an attractive man relating to a less attractive, less prestigious
woman. The experimental conditions created a particular sort of gendering space, a particular privileging space, that was very potent not only for
the woman but also for the men.
Many of the women in our studies had trouble handling such situations. Della, when her professor praised her attractiveness, felt embarrassed and subsequently tried to avoid him. Retreat was the only solution
she could conceive that would not risk provoking his further comment.
After Sam treated her badly, Annette was probably less quick to reveal her
attraction to a man and perhaps less likely to pursue one of Sams prestige. That the women struggled to manage such situations tells us not
that they were intrinsically victims but rather that the playing eld of the
encounters was unequal. The women had relatively few resources for
contesting such genderings.

The Discourse of Sexual Harassment

Ex

Since the time of our studies of romance on college campuses, there have
been organized efforts by individuals and groups to use legal and quasilegal means to stop unwanted sexual advances. Now there are discourses
of sexual harassment and other sources of remedy beyond the individual womans attempts to counter-insult or dissuade a man from involving
her in unwanted relations. The language of these policies and laws, and
the debates over what precisely constitutes sexual harassment, have informed the relatively new discourse. They have at least created some new
categoriesbeyond prick, jerk, and the likefor interpreting and
naming the parties to the type of gendering insults analyzed in this
chapter.13 Were we to return to the same campuses today and interview
students about the incident simulated in the video, we would no doubt
nd reference to this discourse. Perhaps women would tell us about
incidents in which they did not respond with embarrassment, unex-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

164

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

pressed anger, or guilt, but instead rebuked the man for sexually harassing them. Perhaps we would hear, in other words, about cases where the
discourse of harassment was brought into the incidents themselves.
The availability of this discourse certainly does not eviscerate the
popular ideology of romance and attractiveness. Nor does it save women
from the type of gendering insults discussed here. The Anita
Hill/Clarence Thomas hearings were one potent testimony to womens
continuing vulnerability, even in the process of pressing claims of sexual
harassment, to degradation through discourses of desire (see Eisenhart
and Lawrence 1994). Nonetheless, there is little doubt that legal and
quasi-legal remedies for sexual harassment have altered the resources
available for contesting this form of sexism.14 The ideology of romance
and attractiveness has the force of a cultural inertia, born especially from
its potential for constructing pleasure and desire, but it cannot be assumed that inertia grants it an automatic monopoly on meaning. Instead
its place must be held or dislodged through social effort and struggle.
Women now have an alternative discourse, and sometimes the backing
of an enforcement apparatus, through which they may try to refuse or
defuse situations of the type we have described.
The discourse of sexual harassment affords a woman a new resource to
turn the playing eld to her advantage, redening the incident from one
of her inappropriate and degrading gendering into one of his negative
gendering, of outlawing and ostracizing the man. A woman in a situation
like Cindys could now try to alter the computer trainers actions by
threatening to denounce him as a sexual harasser. The emerging discourse of sexual harassment gives women a resource for disrupting the
construction of spaces that give the privileged positions to men. They
can try to convert these spaces into ones where men are reminded of
their liability to sanctions by university hearing boards, corporate committees, and government agencies. Had Eva Haleys encounter with the
bikini-clad pinup happened in the late 1990s, not in 1985, she might
have been moved to counter-attack. Instead of feeling devalued because
she failed to meet certain standards of physical appearance, she might
have tried to turn the parts managers ofce into a place that discomforted him, because the space he had created did not meet standards of
acceptability.15 She might have hung an antisexual harassment poster
over his pinup calendar.
How useful these resources will turn out to behow effectively they

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

The Sexual Auction Block

165

Ex

am

Co

py

will aid women to defuse insulting genderings and disrupt gendering


spacesremains to be seen. Besides the dangers of insult to the victims
of sexual harassment in hearings, there is intense struggle over the very
discourse itself.
David Mamets play Oleanna provides an instructive example. The
play, a drama with only two actors, takes place in a professors ofce. In
the rst scene an anxious undergraduate, a student in one of the professors courses, arrives for an appointment. She is confused about her grade
and generally insecure about her educational progress. She wants suggestions for improvement. The egocentric male professor, absorbed in
contemplating his successful career and in buying a house, virtually
ignores her wishes. He takes the opportunity, instead, to instruct her on
his quasi-radical academic analyses of the failures of American higher
education.
In the next scene the student returns for another appointment, this
time armed with a more authoritative way of guring the conict between them. She has in the interim joined a womens group on campus,
and she has learned not only that she has rights but also ways of asserting
those rights. The communication between student and professor worsens, and she eventually makes a charge that sets in motion the college
apparatus for investigating sexual harassment. The originally ambiguous
tension between them has become socially claried in the terms of a
gendered conict, not limited to his ofce, but grown campus-wide. He
is (re)positioned, cast as a suspected sexual harasser, and eventually he
loses his tenure, his job, his new house, and even his wifes support.
Mamets play not only shows how sexual harassment might intervene
between a woman and the insults of her position but also illustrates a
contestation of the discourse of sexual harassment and the identities it
affords. Audience discussions, which followed the performances we saw
and those we read about, often denigrated the woman student for evoking the specter of sexual harassment and falsely accusing the professor.
The discussants had the ideathough it was more often implied than
made explicitthat recourse to charges of sexual harassment is both
unfair and counterproductive because it is too easily abused. Our concern here is not to debate Mamets play but rather to let it dramatize the
simple point that these discourses are still under heavy dispute. Whether
and how they will survive is still in question.
Equally intriguing is the question of just how salient the discourse of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

166

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

sexual harassment will become in the construal of gender relations.


Could it become even more salient than the ideology of romance and
sex? A world of gender relations gured on a central narrative of men as
predators who use their power to exploit women sexually is imaginable
and indeed has already been imagined. It is the emerging and monstrous
world, the panic-world, portrayed in the writings of such controversial
and self-proclaimed feminists as Roiphe (1993).

Romance and Attractiveness and Other Fields of Power

Ex

am

Co

The popular ideology of romance is thoroughly entangled with other


important collective structures that affect and effect gendering. The discourse of sexual harassment may offer an additional means of interpreting male/female encounters and relations, but it sheds little light on and
gives little relief from a veritable obsession that some of us have with our
own attractiveness. Capitalism has stimulated the fetishization of attractiveness, this central notion in the popular model of romance, and encouraged the commodication of gender itself (Haug 1986; Willis 1991).
The advertisements of the cosmetics and apparel industries alone, not to
mention the ways attractiveness, romance, and sexuality are harnessed to
sell other products, clearly intensify and shapeand are intensied and
shaped bythe pivotal role accorded attractiveness in the gured world
of romance.16
Romantic practices are ever confounded with other powerful systems
of privilege that affect the playing eld of cross-gender relations. Clearly
women and men are differentially advantaged as participants in the
world of romantic and sexual relations according to race, class, age, and
sexual orientation. How is it, for example, that some forms of appearance
and ways of acting have come to be considered more attractive than
others? How is it that the media represent images of attractiveness that
are ageist, constructing younger forms as beautiful; racist, putting a premium on white features; and classist, promoting those forms of beauty
that depend upon wealth? These race, class, and age privileges are not
determined by the gured world of romance and attractiveness, but are
mediated and potentiated by it.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

IV

Ex

am

Co

py

The Space of Authoring

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

Authoring Selves

am

Bakhtins vision of self-fashioning, which we call the space of authoring, resonates with what the case studies have told us about identity
formation. The cases belie any simplistic notion that identities are internalized in a sort of faxing process that unproblematically reproduces the
collective upon the individual, the social upon the body.1 Bakhtins concepts allow us to put words to an alternative vision, organized around
the conictual, continuing dialogic of an inner speech where active identities are ever forming.

Dialogism

Ex

In explicating the interconnections of Bakhtins seminal contributions to


literary analysis and criticism, to linguistics, and to anthropology, Michael Holquist (1990) draws out a central organizing principle, which
he names dialogism. The gured world of dialogism is one in which
sentient beings always exist in a state of being addressed and in the
process of answering.2 People coexist, always in mutual orientation
moving to action; there is no human action which is singularly expressive. Holquist provides a helpful exposition:
Dialogism begins by visualizing existence as an event, the event of
being responsible for (and to) the particular situation existence assumes as it unfolds in the unique (and constantly changing) place I
occupy in it. Existence is addressed to me as a riot of inchoate potential
messages, which at this level of abstraction may be said to come to
169
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

170

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

individual persons much as stimuli from the natural environment come


to individual organisms. Some of the potential messages come to me in
the form of primitive physiological stimuli, some in the form of natural
language, and some in social codes, or ideologies. So long as I am in
existence, I am in a particular place, and must respond to all these
stimuli either by ignoring them or in a response that takes the form of
making sense, of producingfor it is a form of workmeaning out of
such utterances. (1990: 47)

Co

In the making of meaning, we author the world. But the I is by


no means a freewheeling agent, authoring worlds from creative springs
within. Rather, the I is more like Lvi-Strausss (1966) bricoleur, who
builds with preexisting materials. In authoring the world, in putting
words to the world that addresses her, the I draws upon the languages,
the dialects, the words of others to which she has been exposed. One is
more or less condemned, in the work of expression, to choices because
heteroglossia, the simultaneity of different languages and of their associated values and presuppositions, is the rule in social life. In Bakhtins
words:

am

Languagelike the living concrete environment in which the consciousness of the verbal artist livesis never unitary. It is unitary only
as an abstract grammatical system of normative forms, taken in isolation from the concrete, ideological conceptualizations that ll it, and in
isolation from the uninterrupted process of historical becoming that is
a characteristic of all living language. Actual social life and historical
becoming create within an abstractly unitary national language a multitude of concrete worlds, a multitude of bounded verbal-ideological and
social belief systems; within these various systems (identical in the
abstract) are elements of language lled with various semantic and
axiological content and each with its own different sound. (1981: 288)

Ex

It is important to remember that languages are, for Bakhtin, not only


abstract semiotic systems but inevitably and inextricably also ideological
and lived perspectives on the world:
all languages of heteroglossia, whatever the principle underlying them
and making each unique, are specic points of view on the world,
forms for conceptualizing the world in words, specic world views,
each characterized by its own objects, meanings and values. As such
they all may be juxtaposed to one another, mutually supplement one
another, contradict one another and be interrelated dialogically. As

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

171

py

such they encounter one another and coexist in the consciousness of


real people . . . As such, these languages live a real life, they struggle
and evolve in an environment of social heteroglossia . . . They may all
be drawn in by the novelist for the orchestration of his themes and for
the refracted (indirect) expression of his intentions and values. (Bakhtin 1981: 291292)

Co

In such a diverse and contentious social world, the author, in everyday


life as in artistic work, creates by orchestration, by arranging overheard
elements, themes, and forms, not by some outpouring of an ineffable and
central source. That is, the author works within, or at least against, a set
of constraints that are also a set of possibilities for utterance. These are
the social forms of language that Bakhtin summarized: dialects, registers,
accents, and speech genres. We discuss speech and other expressive
genres in more detail later, for they embody Bakhtins closest analogue to
what we call gured worlds. For the time being, we will gloss speech
genres as conventional styles of speaking whose performances presume
and depend (for their very meaningfulness) upon a gured world, which
Bakhtin (1981) called a chronotope.3

am

Dialogic Selves
In the gured world of dialogism the vantage point rests within the I
and authoring comes from the I, but the words come from collective
experience. Words come already articulated by others:

Ex

As a result of the work done by all these stratifying forces in language,


there are no neutral words and formswords and forms that belong
to no one; language has been completely taken over, shot through
with intentions and accents. For any individual consciousness living in
it, language is not an abstract system of normative forms but rather a
concrete heteroglot conception of the world. All words have the taste
of a profession, a genre, a tendency, a party, a particular work, a particular person, a generation, an age group, the day and hour. Each word
tastes of the context and contexts in which it has lived its socially
charged life; all words and forms are populated by intentions. Contextual overtones (generic, tendentious, individualistic) are inevitable in
the word.
As a living, socio-ideological concrete thing, as heteroglot opinion,
language, for the individual consciousness, lies on the borderline between oneself and the other. The word in language is half someone

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

172

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

elses. It becomes ones own only when the speaker populates it with
his own intention, his own accent, when he appropriates the word,
adapting it to his own semantic and expressive intention. Prior to this
moment of appropriation, the word does not exist in a neutral and
impersonal language (it is not after all out of a dictionary that the
speaker gets his words!), but rather it exists in other peoples mouths,
in other peoples contexts, serving other peoples intentions: it is from
there that one must take the word, and make it ones own. (Bakhtin
1981: 293294)

Co

The mixture of the perspective of the I and the words of others


creates the contours of Bakhtins contribution to our ideas of selves and
identities. Here again,

am

an active understanding . . . establishes a series of complex interrelationships, consonances and dissonances with the word and enriches
it with new elements. It is precisely such an understanding that the
speaker counts on. Therefore his orientation toward the listener is an
orientation toward a specic conceptual horizon, toward the specic
world of the listener; it introduces totally new elements into his discourse; it is in this way, after all, that various different points of view,
conceptual horizons, systems for providing expressive accents, various social languages come to interact with one another. The speaker
strives to get a reading on his own word, and on his own conceptual
system that determines this word, within the alien conceptual system of
the understanding receiver; he enters into dialogical relationships with
certain aspects of this system. The speaker breaks through the alien
conceptual horizon of the listener, constructs his own utterance on
alien territory, against his, the listeners, apperceptive background.
(Bakhtin 1981: 282)

Ex

It is not only being addressed, receiving others words, but the act of
responding, which is already necessarily addressed, that informs our
world through others. Identity, as the expressible relationship to others,
is dialogical at both moments of expression, listening and speaking.
Reminiscent of Mead (1934), Bakhtin insists that we also represent
ourselves to ourselves from the vantage point (the words) of others, and
that those representations are signicant to our experience of ourselves.
In Holquists rendition of Bakhtins views:
It will be remembered that the time of the self is always open, unnished, as opposed to the time we assume for others, which is (relative
to our own) closed, nalizable. And yet, in order to be known, to be

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

173

py

perceived as a gure that can be seen, a person or thing must be put


into the categories of the other, categories that reduce, nish, consummate. We see not only our selves, but the world, in the nalizing categories of the other. In other words, we see the world by authoring it.
(Holquist 1990: 84)

Ex

am

Co

The meaning that we make of ourselves is, in Bakhtins terms, authoring the self, and the site at which this authoring occurs is a space
dened by the interrelationship of differentiated vocal perspectives on
the social world. In Bakhtins vision, the self is to existence as the pronoun I is to language. Both the self and I designate pivotal positions
in the stream of (language) activity that goes on always.4 In explaining
what an I is, position, rather than content, is important. Suppose one
tries to dene I by summarizing the characteristics of everybody one
has heard use the term in the past week. One can imagine a prototypical
tree, but can one imagine a prototypical I? In Bakhtins system the self is
somewhat analogous to I. The self is a position from which meaning is
made, a position that is addressed by and answers others and the
world (the physical and cultural environment). In answering (which is
the stuff of existence), the self authors the worldincluding itself and
others.
The authoring self is invisible to itself. The phenomenology of the self
is, in Bakhtins terms, characterized by openendedness. Because the
self is the nexus of a continuing ow of activity and is participating in
this activity, it cannot be nalized. It cannot step outside of activity as
itself; the self as it reects upon its activity is different from the self
that acts. In Bakhtins view, the self-process must be dialogic. It consists,
as told by Holquist (1990: 22, 28), in relating, orchestrating, the I-for-itself (the center) and the not-I-in-me (the noncenter). The other is
authored, captured, and nalized in language as though the other were
not a subject just as open-ended as the self. And, by the same token, in
answering the other as its necessary counterpart, the self represents
(and thereby nalizes) itself through a collective language. Reiterating
Holquist: And yet, in order to be known, to be perceived as a gure that
can be seen, a person or thing must be put into the categories of the
other, categories that reduce, nish, consummate. The self authors itself, and is thus made knowable, in the words of others.
If, to be perceptible by others, we cast ourselves in terms of the other,
then we do that by seeing ourselves from the outside. That is, we assume

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

174

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

a position which Bakhtin names outsideness or transgredience


(Bakhtin 1981, 1990; Holquist 1990). In earlier chapters we described
selves authored via cultural resources, such as the life stories of Alcoholics Anonymous, that have become aspects of history-in-person. Putting
ones self in the texts, the genres, of AA achieves outsideness. Coming
to realize how they can be read as romantic gures, the women described
in Chapters 5 and 7, in effect, achieved outsideness.

Bakhtin and Vygotsky

Ex

am

Co

Wertsch (1985a, 1991) and others (Emerson 1983b; Holquist 1981,


1990) have argued that Bakhtins work complements that of the sociohistorical school of psychology and that it in fact remedies serious lacunae
in that schools treatment, especially of the social conformation of activity.5 We, too, see such an integration as productive, indeed necessary to
the kinds of study we outline here.
Vygotskys vision of psychology, seventy years later, remains one of
the few to attempt a true integration of culture and mental life. What
Bakhtin has to offer this still revolutionary psychology is a much keener
appreciation of the ties of cultural forms to social others and to the
fundamentally social nature of dialogism. By looking carefully, one can
discern a philosophy of dialogism in Vygotskyespecially if one pays
attention to his recognition that consciousness forms and re-forms in
activitybut the theme is dim and underdeveloped (see Wertsch
1985b). Vygotsky attended much more to the issues of human development and, in particular, to the role of cultural forms in that development.
Again, one can discern in Bakhtin aspects of change and development
and the importance of cultural forms in mediating those changesBakhtin writes of the development of an authorial stance and of art and
literature as providing possibilities for reorchestrating the selfbut the
theme is dimmer and less developed than in Vygotskys work. Development and dialogism are the critical themes to be intertwined here.

Inner Speech as a Site of Subjectivity


Vygotsky and Bakhtin gured language, words, speech as the key means
of subjectivity and consciousness, and both held inner speech to be the
key intra-mental node, where social speech penetrated the body and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

175

Ex

am

Co

py

became the premiere building block of thought and feeling. Vygotsky put
forward a law: every function in the childs cultural development
appears twice: rst, on the social level, and later, on the individual level;
rst, between people (interpsychological) and then inside the child (intrapsychological) (Vygotsky 1978: 57).
This maxim entails that inner speech is preceded in ontogeny by external speech, where children talk as though speaking were simply part
of an activity, repeating words and gestures they have heard from others
(see Chapter 3). Vygotsky thus reversed the Piagetian formulation (that
speech begins for oneself), and argued that speech begins for others
and then eventually is directed toward oneself. Speech directed toward
oneself begins out loud (as so-called egocentric speech), but eventually becomes silent, inner speech, a speech whose formal characteristics
(such as ellipsis, deletion of the subject, focus on sense rather than
meaning) become differentiated from those of social speech (Vygotsky
1986; Voloshinov 1986). The possibility of directing speech to oneself is
equally, for Vygotsky, the possibility of achieving at least a modicum of
control over ones own behavior. One can at least have a voice in directing ones own actions.
As we discussed in Chapter 3, Vygotsky emphasized the ability of
humans to use mediating devices, especially symbols, to modify their
own mental environment and so direct their own behavior. These devices were generally rst taken up in interaction with others, and then
only gradually taken into ones self-activity. The resulting complex (device plus behaviornot forgetting context) could eventually become
habitual or fossilized, in Vygotskys term (1978: 68), be moved out of
awareness, possibly with no observable trace of the mediating means;
the act, subsuming its exterior development, became seemingly an
essential characteristic of the person.6
Vygotskys attention to the achievement of freedom via discipline, that
is freedom from the random stimuli of the environment and discipline by
the direction of oneself through signs, emphasized the signicance of
cultural forms. Cultural forms were the creation of the collective, and
through these forms a second world could be articulated. By constructing an unseen, a spoken if not imaginary, world for its agentswhich as
scientic concepts could achieve maximum distance from the directly
experienced physical stimulihumans vitiated external exigency.
Vygotskys appreciation of the collective dimensions of human life also

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

176

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

extended to the social. Individuals have access to the cultural legacy of


the collective through others. Social interaction is the context in which
cultural forms come to individuals and individuals come to use cultural
forms. Learning is situated, to use the term of scholars such as Lave
and Wenger (1991); the cultural forms that come to inhabit the individual depend upon the place, the social position, from which the individual engages with others in activities, in practice. At least at rst, and, we
suspect, in many cases later on as well, others support or censure use of
particular cultural forms in practice. With the helpful encouragement
and gentle rephrasings of oldtimers, newcomers to Alcoholics Anonymous meetings gradually begin to tell their stories. In the interchange
that surrounds the telling, they are softly directed toward the generic
forms of the organization.
Vygotskys theories of education and pedagogy made explicit his ideas
about the distribution of neophytes activities, even their thinking and
feeling, over those with whom they interacted. Vygotsky summed up this
absolutely social character of human learning through his concept of the
zone of proximal development, the ZOPED. In most U.S. schools, students are measured for acquired and inherited skills and abilities by tests
in which help from others is considered cheating. The ZOPED seeks to
determine what the individual can do with the support of others, in the
social context of learning. Perhaps an AA member can/will tell the story
of her life as an alcoholic only with the support of other AA members.
The story lies within her zone of proximal development, if not yet within
her sole capacity.
Yet Vygotsky was himself guilty (as are we all, more often than not) of
focusing too exclusively upon the facilitation of skills and abilities that
took place in the ZOPED.7 He often omitted its negative aspectboth
the censuring and extinction of behaviors irrelevant to the learning
task and the shaping and inculcation of only those skills and actions t
for the social position the neophyte was accorded. To reverse the title of
Foucaults famous book, the ZOPED was also a place to punish and
discipline.
While the social was clearly important to Vygotsky, we miss the elements of power, status, stratication, and ownership that Bakhtin emphasized. In Vygotskys vision cultural forms became fused with mental
life within social interaction, but he avoided attention to the conictual
in social life or to the oppressive nature of many institutions in which

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

177

Ex

am

Co

py

humans interact these cultural forms (Wertsch, Minick, and Arns 1984).
And so we miss the social struggles and conicts that drive aspects of
inner speech. Vygotsky recognized differences between inner speech
(speech for ones self), and social speech (speech for others), and he
thus recognized a tension, for example, in moving from inner to social
speech. But the tension lay in such contrasts as those between the
sense of a word or phrase for ones self, the connotations and memories
of the contexts of the words use, and the meaning of a word or phrase
for generalized others. Bakhtins view of the social and the fusion of the
social with cultural forms was much more complex.
Bakhtin was keenly aware that speech formssocial languages,
speech genreswere neither neutral with regard to values and world
views nor a simple means of expressing thoughts, leaving off the subtexts
of power and stratication. Vygotsky quite appropriately emphasized the
potential of words as tools, as bootstraps by which one could pull
oneself up to another form of behavior,8 but he ignored the conicts and
struggles, the whole history of contested practices now emblematized
and brought along with the tools. Vygotskys tools, according to Bakhtins
vision of human expression, came marked with social division and the
often incompatible perspectives attached to them.
To appreciate the difference between the two it is also necessary to
recall Bakhtins insistence on dialogism, addressivity and answerability.
Vygotskian psychology, especially its development by the Kharkov line
of the sociohistorical school now referred to as activity theory, emphasized the situatedness of thought and feeling and their developmental
courses over the lifetime. But, where Bakhtin emphasized the alwayscontinuing activity of producing meaning in dialogue, activity in the
sociohistorical school refers to any historically specic, collectively developed or conventionalized, social endeavor such as work. Where Bakhtin emphasized a global self-fashioning, or authoring of the self, the
tasks that Vygotsky considered, such as remembering to do something
(Vygotsky 1978) or controlling ones emotions in a particular situation
(Vygotsky 1987b), were more varied and more everyday.
It is true that, writing with and against the formalists, Vygotsky (1986)
did discuss monologic versus dialogic forms of (inner) speech, but he
simply did not give inner speech the dialogic quality that Bakhtin did.
Inner speech was, for Vygotsky, only possible because of social speech,
and thus dialogical in its developmental origins. But Vygotsky gave more

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

178

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

emphasis to the semantic, representational potential of mature linguistic


practice and its potential for semiotic mediation, and thus gave less
attention to what Bakhtin continued to emphasize, the pragmatic aspects
of languagehow it was used, how it communicated power and authority, how it was inscribed with status and inuence.9

Authoring the Self, Orchestrating Voices

Ex

am

Co

Vygotsky wrote little about the self, at least as far as we are aware. If we
extrapolate from his position, especially his emphasis on semiotic mediation, however, we nd no incompatibilities with the writings of Bakhtin
on the subject.10 In fact, the implications of Vygotskys work seem to
complement Bakhtins position. In Bakhtins account of self-authoring,
the I-for-myself realizes itself explicitly in words and categories, naming the I-for-others and the other-in-myself. As we have argued,
following Vygotsky, such symbols for self may become important in attempts to control or modify ones behavior. The AA member who comes
to identify herself as a non-drinking alcoholic may use the tokens of that
identity, such as the number of colored chips she has collected, to tell
herself parts of her personal story, to keep herself from taking a drink.
The categories of the other not only nalize the self, putting closure on
what is actually open-ended, but also guide the future self in its activity.
They direct the trajectory of the self-process, becoming part of what we
call history-in-person.
Bakhtin has more to offer. For him, the voices, the symbols, are socially inscribed and heteroglossic. Often the voices are in conict. The
AA member may hear the voices of other members, but she also hears
the voices of her friends who still drink: Oh, come on, have a drink.
And it is unlikely that the member will be able to avoid situations in
which these voices, which are reminders of earlier selves, promote divergent actions. These voices will have to be put together in some way. The
orchestration of such voices, which Bakhtin calls self-authoring, thus is
a complicated matter indeed. Or, in Vygotskys terms, semiotic mediation, made over into a matter of voice, looks less like a smooth steering
mechanism and more like a self-contest.
Consider Sandy, one of the college women introduced in Chapter 5. In
her talking diary interviews we discern several voices, some of which
Sandy identied with and others with which she was at odds, the voices

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

179

of critics. Sandy was upset by the critical talk. She had come to the
university from another part of the country and, from what she said,
found her fellow students ideas of attractiveness unfamiliar:

Co

py

In my hometown, I was pretty much respected in the community and


accepted for what I am, or was, in that community. [I was] basically
your nonconformist, and I dressed to suit me. But when I came down
here I . . . got the impression that here I was a sloppy little girl and I
didnt have any class or I didnt have any style . . . I have some preppie
clothes, and sometimes I wear them but I dont feel that what you wear
puts you in a certain circle, and all of a sudden I felt that I was put
either to one side or to the other side . . . and I didnt have a choice
because it was all around me . . . and I didnt like that . . . And it really
really bothered me.

am

Sandy went on to say that the same forces made her feel that she was
not a lady just because she cursed when she got angry. As she reported
her thoughts and feelings, the criticisms were remembered in (took on)
the voices of her peers. She seemed to reexperience the comparison of
herself to an ideal through the questions and criticisms of her peers.
These critics had become an internal interlocutor, which invidiously
compared her to the ideal and to whose charges she formulated answers
and defenses (Holland 1988).11
Much of Bakhtins most fascinating work is devoted to styles of
authorship in literary genres. It was characteristic of the novel, as Bakhtin conceived it, that the authors position was articulated not through
explicit statements but through the juxtaposition, the orchestration, of
the voices and performances of the novels characters:

Ex

The author does not speak in a given language (from which he distances himself to a greater or lesser degree), but he speaks, as it were,
through language, a language that has somehow more or less materialized, become objectivized, that he merely ventriloquates.
The prose writer as a novelist does not strip away the intentions of
others from the heteroglot language of his works, he does not violate
those socio-ideological cultural horizons (big and little worlds) that
open up behind heteroglot languagesrather he welcomes them into
his work. The prose writer makes use of words that are already populated with the social intentions of others and compels them to serve his
own new intentions, to serve a second master. Therefore the intentions
of the prose writer are refracted, and refracted at different angles, de-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

180

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

pending on the degree to which the refracted, heteroglot languages he


deals with are socio-ideologically alien, already embodied and already
objectivized. (Bakhtin 1981: 299300)

am

Co

py

We see this same sort of authoring in inner speech.


An excerpt from Lachicottes interviews with psychiatrists and other
mental health workers provides an example. Public mental health care is
a profession involving at least four vocations: the medical or psychiatric,
the psychological or psychotherapeutic, the sociological or social work,
and the administrative or managerial. Because people are trained in at
least the rudiments of each vocation, and because they work in teams
drawn from these vocations, each professional learns and personalizes
the discourse (and, as Bakhtin would say, the volitional-emotional content) appropriate to all the vocations. That is, the vocations become
voices, situated vocal-images within inner speech, and thus possible
ideological standpoints for responding to the problems that confront
practitioners. The authoring of mental health care, as an activity and an
understanding that are ones own, is composed, in Bakhtins terms, by the
relations among these situated voices.
In order to elicit accounts that might retrieve that process of authorship, our interview confronted each professional with a hypothetical case
summary, a description of a womans psychiatric problems in the context
of her life. We asked how the professional would plan treatment of such
a case. As we will explain in Chapter 9, mental health workers have
trouble treating the type of mental patient represented in the case summary. The following excerpt from an interview shows how a particularly
articulate therapist tried to create an authorial position for herself in
relation to the hypothetical character. Each ideological-linguistic voice
available to the therapist is noted in brackets.

Ex

[social] Find out what her resources are. A girl like this is going to
need outside resources. Does she have any? A lot of times, surprisingly
enough, they have. Surprisingly enough, they very often may have
friends that youre unaware of, that theyre unaware of!
[therapeutic] But, work out with her, again, trying to get her to take
as much control as she can of her life. What are we going to do?
and, again, set up the rules. Set up the consistency and rules. Try to
understand that just because her illness, her manifestation of illness

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

181

Co

py

tends to be less pleasant than your chronic schizs, doesnt make her any
less safe.
So what does she want to do about the divorce? Does she want to
divorce this guy? [social] Usually, right off, they dont have any money.
Where is she going to live? So, with her, I would start off with much
more practical considerations. Where are you going to live? What are
we going to do? What are the options? Where can you go? Can you go
to your family? [and administrative] Because if shes going to go to
South Carolina to live with her family, then were not going to do
anything with this lady except get her to South Carolina.
[medical/therapeutic] So a patient like this, shes very easily depressed and very unstable. Shes going to need stability before she needs
anything else, so my rst priority with her would be to nd some way
to get her stabilized.
[social] I dont mean psychiatrically stabilized, I mean socially stabilized. [medical/therapeutic] This is somebody who, youre not going to
x the psychiatric problems, for the next fteen years, at least, even if
shes cooperative. And I dont mean that in a mean way, but that the
reason shes unstable is that obviouslyher mom spent the last year in
a hospital suffering from psychotic depression[and social] basic lack
of security. Husband losing a job; shes going to bounce off the walls
and be dead.

Ex

am

For virtually all the people interviewed, the womans case was a hard
problem. Few expressed condence that any treatment they could devise
would prove effective, and most accounts reect that difdence. Many
lack an authorial center, wandering substantially from task to task and
discursively crossing the vocations conventionally responsible for these
tasks. Others take an authorial stance that directly reports, without
adopting or valuing any one interrelationship, the steps each vocation
might take and the reasons for their failure.
The therapist quoted was an exception, and it is not surprising that
she was considered an expert with borderlinesthe diagnosis most often
evoked by the hypothetical case. Throughout the case recitation she
relates the vocations with little commentary. She presents each with
respect, as a serious participant, but gives clear weight to the sociological
standpoint. She begins and ends from that perspective, she interrupts
speaking from the other vocations to return to social concerns, and, at
times, she mixes (in Bakhtins term, hybridizes) the other vocations

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

182

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

discourse with the social. The formal ways of relating social languages,
evident in this therapists speech, provide a powerful means to outsidenessa way of standing apart from immediate experience that yet
denes it as ours by the very arrangement that relates it. It is utterly
characteristic of self-identication under a regime of heteroglossia.
Mental health workers have long been exposed to heteroglossia, a
cacophony of different languages and perspectives. They are faced with
the choice of either taking on these different languages and perspectives willy-nilly or developing a more or less stable authorial stance, a
voice that over time speaks categorically and/or orchestrates the different
voices in roughly comparable ways. A rst step toward an authorial
stance, one already mastered by this therapist, is the creation of internally persuasive discoursesexternal or authoritative speech that has
been married to ones own:

Ex

am

Internally persuasive discourseas opposed to one that is externally


authoritativeis, as it is afrmed through assimilation, tightly interwoven with ones own word. In the everyday rounds of our consciousness, the internally persuasive word is half-ours and half-someone elses. Its creativity and productiveness consist precisely in the fact
that such a word awakens new and independent words, that it organizes masses of our words from within, and does not remain in an
isolated and static condition. It is not so much interpreted by us as it is
further, that is, freely, developed, applied to new material, new conditions; it enters into interanimating relationships with new contexts.
More than that, it enters into an intense interaction, a struggle with
other internally persuasive discourses. Our ideological development is
just such an intense struggle within us for hegemony among various
available verbal and ideological points of view, approaches, directions
and values. The semantic structure of an internally persuasive discourse is not nite, it is open; in each of the new contexts that dialogize
it, this discourse is able to reveal ever newer ways to mean. (Bakhtin
1981: 345346)

Here one sees a close approach in Bakhtins thought to Vygotskys notion


of mediation. The creation of an internally persuasive discourse mediates
the reorganization and extension of social speech into new forms of
inner speaking. It changes the nature of subjectication.
In a situation of heteroglossia different languages and perspectives
come inscribed with differing amounts of authority, which suggest how

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

183

Co

py

they might be orchestrated. Nonetheless, especially in situations where


the usual authorities are unsettled (see Chapter 9), there is little agreement about how to orchestrate these various voices. As the case of Susan
in Chapter 5 shows, the development of an authorial stance, in any
gured world, can take a long time. Sandy, as well, struggled with the
criticisms she encountered from her peers. Sorting out and orchestrating
voices is much more than sorting out neutral perspectives in some rationalists argument; the voices, after all, are associated with socially
marked and ranked groups (the in-crowd, ones sorority sisters) and
even with particularly potent individuals (Mom, ones brother, a hated
classmate).
A Bakhtinian space of authoring is then very much a particular
zone of proximal development, and one that is extremely important in
an explication of the development of identities as aspects of history-inperson. Bakhtin does not take development as the center of his concerns,
as does Vygotsky. Yet he does write about differences between the neophyte, given over to a voice of authority, and the person of greater experience, who begins to rearrange, reword, rephrase, reorchestrate different
voices and, by this process, develops her own authorial stance.12 Again,
in Bakhtins words:

Ex

am

This processexperimenting by turning persuasive discourse into


speaking personsbecomes especially important in those cases where
a struggle against such images has already begun, where someone is
striving to liberate himself from the inuence of such an image and its
discourse by means of objectication, or is striving to expose the limitations of both image and discourse. The importance of struggling with
anothers discourse, its inuence in the history of an individuals coming to ideological consciousness, is enormous. Ones own discourse
and ones own voice, although born of another or dynamically stimulated by another, will sooner or later begin to liberate themselves from
the authority of the others discourse. This process is made more complex by the fact that a variety of alien voices enter into the struggle
for inuence within an individuals consciousness (just as they struggle with one another in surrounding social reality). (Bakhtin 1981:
348)

Brown and colleagues (1993) provide a fascinating glimpse of this


process in their examination of how American girls (ages 1115) use the
medias representations of sexuality. They describe three different styles

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

184

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

of usage, three different dispositions toward the discourse presented in


the popular media: uninterested, intrigued, and resisting. Members of
the rst, and youngest, subgroup remain uninterested in sexual content
and avoid reference to it. Girls of the second, and largest, subgroup are
intrigued by the representations of sexuality. They readily take up the
medias images into their own talk, but in a relatively naive manner. The
discourse provides a model for their own experimentation with sexual
activity, but the model is still idealized, and provides a kind of retreat, a
place of safety unfreighted with the contest of real partners. It is this
attachment of identied others to the images and voices of the media
that distinguishes the third, and oldest, subgroup, which the authors call
resisting.
The critical stance of these older girls toward depictions of sexuality
developed within social interaction, with the realization of heterosexual
activity: at least for some, as more personal experience is gained, the
fantasy painted by the media is tarnished by the reality of imperfect
bodies, distant boys, and lack of control, and the media image is resisted
(Brown et al. 1993: 180).
Bakhtin (1981) described how one makes the authority of discourse
into ones own word (into internally persuasive speech) by rst personifying the alien word, attaching to it the real limitations of specic
others. As one girl said: Guys I know are so afraid of getting it [AIDS]
. . . because theyre so scared, theyre more likely to use rubbersthat
helps with the pregnancy problemcould it be a blessing in disguise?
(192). Far from the tepid waters of Blue Lagoon! The girls have used this
technique to authorize their own understandings of sexuality, opposing
to the idealized blandishments of television and magazine not only the
boys of their own acquaintance but the foibles of people in the news:
First its Jim Baker and recently Gary Hart. All some stupid sex scandals
that no one really pays attention to. Its all been one bore after another
(189). It is not that girls nd a voice independent of the medias discourse; they make a voice by taking a stance toward the variety of media
and personal voices in their worlds.

From Inner Speech to Social Speech


We can now continue our argument about joining Bakhtins dialogism to
Vygotskys developmentalism. To reiterate, Bakhtin brings to Vygotskys
rather abstract concern with the social roots of psychological activity a

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

185

py

differentiated conception of sociality, both of its cultural means (sociolects and speech genres) and of its actual, localized articulation (the
sociolinguistics of utterance). Vygotsky highlighted the ways in which
language and other cultural forms are socially interacted and, in many
cases, taken in and made into personal tools for affecting ones own
behavior. Bakhtin reminded us that these forms are by no means taken in
as socially neutral:

Co

It might be said . . . that in the makeup of almost every utterance


spoken by a social personfrom a brief response in a casual dialogue to
major verbal-ideological works (literary, scholarly and others)a signicant number of words can be identied that are implicitly or explicitly admitted as someone elses, and that are transmitted by a variety of
different means. Within the arena of almost every utterance an intense
interaction and struggle between ones own and anothers word is being
waged, a process in which they oppose or dialogically interanimate
each other. The utterance so conceived is a considerably more complex
and dynamic organism than it appears when construed simply as a
thing that articulates the intention of the person uttering it, which is
to see the utterance as a direct, single-voiced vehicle for expression.
(Bakhtin 1981: 354355)

Ex

am

Vygotsky saw the escape from the tyranny of environmental and internal stimuli as coming through the gift of collective symbols wielded
toward the self. In Bakhtin, escape from being ventriloquated by rst one
and then another authoritative voice comes through the orchestration
of and adoption of stances toward these voices, arrangements that are
themselves suggested (perhaps) by texts. Indeed, the complementarity is
apparent here. Had he studied Alcoholics Anonymous, Vygotsky might
have emphasized the life story as a sign by which an AA member comes
to turn himself from drinking and eventually toward a new habitual way
of responding to messages inviting him to drink. Bakhtin would have
weighted the life story more as a text that provides the AA member with
a model for responding to the multiple and conicting voices that speak
to him about drinking and what it means that he is drawn to drink. Both
enable us to see that consciousness and subjectivity are thoroughgoing
products of the collective life, but their paths to that vision take us to
different vantages on self-fashioning. Vygotsky leads us to look at a more
asocial, developmental effect of cultural forms, as mediating devices of
consciousness. Bakhtin leads us to capture the ongoing social struggles,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

186

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

and the continuous social demands, the responsibility of answering,


that follow along with the symbolic gift.
The task of answering can be understood in the relation between
inner and social speech. Both Vygotsky and Bakhtin (Voloshinov 1986)
conceive inner speech as the major site where social speech and other
aspects of day-to-day existence are brought into history-in-person, into
bodily reactions and responses. Yet we have seen that subjectication,
the trafc of social speech to inner speech, is not a simple or direct
process. It does not result in a facsimile of the social reproduced upon
the body. Inner speech, though parented by social speech, is not a clone
of social speech.13 Nor does the translation of bodily reactions and responses into social speech belie the difference between the two. The
constraints of answering are not so stringent that sameness is produced and dialogue superseded.
The social shaping of objectication, that is, the social responsiveness
of the movement from inner speech to social speech, is given more
signicance and reported more vividly by Bakhtin than by Vygotsky.
Vygotsky approached the issue of objectication mainly through his
distinction between the sense of words and symbols and their meaning. Inner speech works primarily through the sense of words and
events; social speech depends more upon meaning.
Vygotsky argued that children rst learn to use words and, by implication, other symbols in interactive contexts. They develop a sense of a
word that is bound up with these contexts of its use. Over time, from
comparison of the various contexts in which the word is used, they
gradually infer the meaning or cross-contextual referents of the word.
Meaning is a part of sense, the stable or systematic aspect, the signication, of a word or symbol. Sense is contextualized, connotational; meaning is decontextualized, denotational. Vygotsky put the distinction in
this way:
A words sense is the aggregate of all the psychological facts that arise in
our consciousness as a result of the word. Sense is a dynamic, uid, and
complex formation which has several zones that vary in their stability.
Meaning is only one of these zones of the sense that the word acquires
in the context of speech. It is the most stable, unied, and precise of
these zones. In different contexts, a words sense changes. In contrast,
meaning is a comparatively xed and stable point, one that remains
constant with all the changes of the words sense associated with its use
in various contexts . . . Isolated in the lexicon, the word has only one

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

187

meaning. However, this meaning is nothing more than a potential that


can only be realized in living speech, and in living speech meaning is
only a cornerstone in the edice of sense. (1987c: 275276)

py

Luria provides a concrete example of this distinction:

Co

The Russian word ugol (coal, charcoal) has a denite and clear
meaning. It involves a black substance, most often having its origin in
wood, that is derived from the carbonization of trees. It involves a
substance having a denite chemical composition, the chief constituent
of which is the element C (carbon). However, the sense of the word
ugol can vary greatly for different people in different situations. For
the housewife, ugol is associated with something used for heating
the samovar or something used for heating the stove . . . For the artist
who wishes to use ugol (charcoal) for making a sketch, it is associated with the means he/she has for outlining a picture. And for the little
girl who has smudged her dress with coal, the word ugol carries an
unpleasant sense. It is something that has spoiled her dress, something
that evokes an affective feeling. (1981: 45)

am

Vygotsky implied that the process of moving from inner speech to


social speech reverses the proportional weight given sense over meaning.
One may (indeed must) personalize symbols, using them to direct ones
own behavior, but to affect otherswhose histories of usage undoubtedly diverge widelymeaning must predominate. Thus Vygotsky tended
to reestablish a kind of gulf between the social/meaningful and the personal/sensible.
Voloshinov (1986), Bakhtins collaborator, made the fusion of the social and the personal far more complete:

Ex

Psychic experience is something inner that becomes outer and the


ideological sign, something outer that becomes inner. The psyche enjoys extraterritorial status in the organism. It is a social entity that
penetrates inside the organism of the individual person. Everything
ideological is likewise extraterritorial in the socioeconomic sphere,
since the ideological sign, whose locus is outside the organism, must
enter the inner world in order to implement its meaning as sign.
(1986: 39)

Or, more briey: there is no such thing as experience outside of embodiment in signs . . . It is not experience that organizes expression, but the
other way aroundexpression organizes experience (85).14
Not only does the speaker depend upon the words of others to author

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

188

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

his own experience, he also must answer to, respond to, the social
relations between himself and his addressee in speaking socially:

py

Even if a word is not entirely his, constituting, as it were, the border


zone between himself and his addresseestill, it does in part belong to
him . . . If . . . we take the implementation of word as sign, then the
question of proprietorship becomes extremely complicated. Aside from
the fact that word as sign is a borrowing on the speakers part from the
social stock of available signs, the very individual manipulation of this
social sign in a concrete utterance is wholly determined by social relations. (1986: 86)

Co

. . . expression-utterance is determined by the actual conditions of the


given utteranceabove all by its immediate social situation. (85)
The word is oriented toward an addressee, toward who that addressee
might be: a fellow-member or not of the same social group, of higher
or lower standing (the addressees hierarchical status), someone connected with the speaker by close social ties . . . or not. (85)

am

The immediate social situation and the broader social milieu wholly determineand determine from within, so to speakthe structure of an utterance. (86)

Ex

Utterances, that is, are constructed between socially related and thus
positioned persons. Since utterances organize experience (as the source
of the psyche itself), we are strongly affected by the position we are cast
into within interactions. Voloshinov (1986: 87) gives an example of
hunger and its expression. How hunger is expressed and therefore experienced depends upon ones social group and position, and upon the
immediate situation. Suppose you are a guest in an expensive hotel (here
we modify Voloshinovs example somewhat). Imagine that you enter
hungry and are kept waiting for your dinner order. Imagine how you
might express/experience the hunger. Or imagine yourself a political
protester, jailed and deprived of food long past mealtime. Would the
expression/experience of hunger be different from that in the expensive
hotel? Or imagine you are a host, deferring your own meal in order to
provide food for your guests. How would you express/experience your
own hunger in that situation? For Bakhtin and Voloshinov, moving from
inner to outer speech is a process in which the immediate social situation

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

189

Co

py

profoundly shapes the experience from within. Their position is reminiscent of the situational determinacy we discussed in Chapter 1. We see
peoples behavior/experience/expression conforming (in the sense of being created together) with the social situation.
In short, the task of answering to others is a signicant one. Bakhtins insistence on dialogism, the always present, always operating, always demanding job of being in dialogue with others, with ones environment, forces attention to the present situation and entails its
importance in the space of authoring. We are reminded how unlikely it
is that ones identities are ever settled, once and for all. Dialogism makes
clear that what we call identities remain dependent upon social relations and material conditions. If these relations and material conditions
change, they must be answered, and old answers about who one is
may be undone.

Development

Ex

am

It is not just the present condition of dialogue that is important to Bakhtin. There is history in the person, which also signicantly shapes social
activity. Voloshinov emphasizes, perhaps overemphasizes, the determination of utterance by the social situation. But he also postulates a feature of inner speech that he calls a stabilized social audience: Each
persons inner word and thought has its stabilized social audience that
comprises the environment in which reasons, motives, values, and so on
are fashioned (1986: 86).15 This stabilized social audience can be specic persons or more idealized ones.16 It is in this sense of an idealized
audience (like Meads generalized other) that Bakhtin and Voloshinov
speak of ideology: any cognitive thought whatever, even one in my
consciousness, in my psyche, comes into existence . . . with an orientation toward an ideological system of knowledge where that thought will
nd its place. My thought, in this sense, from the very start belongs to an
ideological system and is governed by its set of laws (1986: 35).
Ideology is not, however, an abstractum, held in the mind as a set of
propositions or in habit as a set of tropisms. It is instead a habituated,
gured world. So, though Bakhtin does not often speak to the point,17
there must be periods (and these periods may even be the majority)
during which ones authorial stance becomes stable, or, in our terms, an
identity becomes habituated, usual, common. These topics return us to

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

190

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

Vygotskys concerns with development, consciousness, and control. The


space of self-authoring can be illuminated by Vygotskys concept of the
zone of proximal development.
For Joas (1985), resuming Meads position in terms of voices, voices
that are very alive in inner speech may, at some point, fade in their
insistence on being answered. The authorial stance that one takes, the
orchestration that one makes, may become stabilized. The behavior that
was meaningful or indexical with respect to the identity has become
habituated, fossilized in Vygotskys terms, automatic. The complexes of
behavior associated with and mediated by the identity have become second nature, and their production has moved out of awareness. The relation of the self to a signicant audience (which is the identitys reference)
has become stabilized and the orchestration is one that has endured.18

Development of Identity as Problematic

Ex

am

Yet, despite the ordinary force of habituation, a developmental perspective must problematize development. As our ethnographic studies have
made clear, identity formation should not be taken for granted. As we
have seen, many actors are not sufciently engaged by the world of
romance, say, or that of Alcoholics Anonymous that they ever form much
of an identity relevant to the particular gured world, at least not for a
long time. Sandys concern with her peers remarks about her anomalous
behavior and lack of attractiveness faded with time. Instead, her life
became focused around a particular friend. She and the friend moved
away from campus life, and Sandy eventually quit school. Years later, in
the follow-up interview, we discovered that she had only recently returned to the university and to the world of romance and attractiveness
that she had eschewed.
Similarly, some participants in AA never identied enough to attend
meetings faithfully or to produce an interpretation of their lives that
accorded with the genre performed in the meetings. It is folly to assume
that members of a voluntary group, or even members of an involuntaryan ethnic or racialgroup are uniform in their identities (see
Rouse 1995). There may be far less to participation than meets the eye.
In other cases there is more to participation than might be suspected.
As we shall see in Chapters 10 and 12, many of the Naudadan women
who participated in the Tij festival developed identities and senses of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Selves

191

Ex

am

Co

py

themselves that were important to them. Nonetheless, those aspects of


identity associated with the critical commentary voiced at Tij and with
the social world gured by Tij songs were dependent upon the recognition of others. Since Tij was only a day-long festival, conned to a small
space in the year, it might seem that these womens identities had little importance to the broader public world. In fact, the identications
founded in Tij readily turned the women to new forms of action and
combined with other gures of femininity to produce a broader realm of
social and political activity. The subterranean (re)production of identities, within inner speech or rst in the putatively closeted venues of
specialized practices, the force of history-in-person, may erupt in social
interaction with surprising and powerful results.
We conceive the space of authoring, then, as a broad venue, where
social languages meet, generically and accentually, semantically and indexically, freighted with the valences of power, position, and privilege.19
Such a large concept is needed if we are to understand more particularly the places each of us occupies, and if we are to develop notions
of authorship, of social and personal agency, that do justice both to Vygotskys keen sense of persons-in-history and to Bakhtins heteroglossic (and, we might add, heteropraxic) social worlds. Such a concept
is needed to do justice to the complexities of self-fashioning in everyday worlds.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Co

py

Mental Disorder, Identity,


and Professional Discourse

Ex

am

Identity is produced socially and culturally from the generic personae


and scenery of our gured worlds as they are positioned in the hierarchies of power and privilege that relate elds of activity. In these landscapes, or rather in the movement across them, are wrought the bodily
and mindful impersonations of the people, ourselves included, whom we
come to know in our lived worlds. That is to say, identity responds to
both the imaginary and the embodied communities in which we live. My
Hamlet is not Hamlet, nor Oliviers Hamlet, though it has much to do
with both. Identity is mediated, a product of both dis-stance and instance, just as, Voloshinov and Bakhtin argued, human utterance (and
we extend this to all activity) is somehow generic (it is language) and
somehow specic (it is talking) and is realized as meaningful action only
to the extent that it is both.
Let us continue our consideration of the intricacies of mediation
through which human agency produces itself. The capacity of self-management, Vygotskys great concern, is the paradoxical and nal mediation of human experience. Through its development within people, in
their inner speaking and proactivity, the power of language and image to
suspend the immediate and thus resituate persons nds its own locus of
reection. In the commentaries of inner speaking, ones own activity is
represented and reenacted (or pre-presented and pre-enacted) and thus
becomes the object of evaluation and manipulation.
Yet this autonomy of the self, or self-agency, depends upon ones capacity to produce the means that organize ones activity. There is a second half-step of personalization hidden here. It is not enough merely
192
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

193

am

Co

py

to direct ones behavior intimately. Rather, one must be able to produce


the means of signication personally as well. The relevant situation of
semiotic mediation must be freed from its domination by discourse;
others words must become internally persuasive speech. This feat,
however, is itself a kind of social product and a form of social location
created by the encounter of manifold discursive practices. This place is
Bakhtins space of authorship. It is not simply a natural artifact of peoples invariant self-consciousness, but an achievement of different degrees in practice.
The power of discourse to dene persons, Foucaults notion of subjectication, had its rst articulation in his works on madness (1965, 1979).
We will return to it to show the pertinence of his rendition to any human
agent and also to diversify its intentions for that agent. We will try to
recover from Foucault a social world whose various practices, professional disciplines, and everyday congeries are not so regimentedwhose
forms of power, though surrendering none of their formative primacy,
produce something less than an epistemic totality. Rather, they work
often at cross-purposes, in what Engestrm (1987) calls, in straightforwardly Marxian terms, contradiction. We will try to show how one
person plays and is played within these diverse eldswithin a space
of authoring, to make at last a repertory of potential identities and, from
that repertory, something more than Foucaults subjectthough less
than an imperial self.

The Project

Ex

The Triangle Mental Health Survey, from which the case we report here is
drawn, had two purposes.1 The rst was to reconstruct the ways in
which people who suffer from severe and persistent mental disorders
learn to survive economically and socially. It was a study of subsistence
strategies, of personal economiessomething familiar to many anthropologists (such as Spradley 1970). We recruited people who were, so to
speak, early in the career of their illness, those to whom the potentially
damaging effects of mental illness were yet new. We followed each participant for two years, doing a formal interview every six months.
The rst, third, and fth of these interviews were broad and long
survey instruments. We collected information on employment history,
the everyday experience of work, job seeking, working and nancial

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

194

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

relationships, and the context of nancial decisions. We also collected


extensive information on broader social network and interpersonal relationships, psychiatric status, and dealings with the mental health system.
This information was compiled for analysis by multivariate statistical
techniques, particularly by a method called event history analysis. Again,
the objective was to weigh the conditions that inuence the choices
people make and the changes in their lives after those choices.
Still, the interpretable models, both of measurement and of the social
world, that make statistical analysis possible derive from ethnographic
inquiry, from rigorous, naturalistic description, and from the professional, critical discourse that is its commentary. Hence the real core of
the inquiry concerned our descriptive, narrated understanding of our
participants economic and social lives. We devoted the second and
fourth interviews and parts of the surveys to what may best be called
discussion. We sought a picture of subsistence more complete than
xed-format items could elicit, a picture that would take account of our
informants own conceptions of the events of their socioeconomic and
sociomedical worlds.
We also interviewed persons important in our informants lives (signicant others), staff members of the local mental health centers, and
hospital social workers who dealt with the centers. With signicant others, who were usually family members, the interviews centered on the
informant. With staff, rules of condentiality often precluded our speaking of the informant. Instead, these interviews plumbed the usual practice and conception of treatment of mental illness in the local area. Both
these types of interview included extensive discussion. Our purpose was
to re-create the perspective of representatives of groups that play important roles in the process by which the social career of a mentally ill
person is constructed.
In order to recruit our participants, to follow them in their peregrinations, and to prepare for the interviews, we had to do much observation
and much natural as opposed to interview talk. We spent hours in
the hospitals, in the mental health centers, in peoples homes, neighborhoods, and communities.
The information we collected from all these sources was vital to the
second objective of the study: to understand how participants created
new identities in the face of illness often thought to be destructive of the
self. This objective is tied to economic adaptation, especially in a society

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

195

Disorders and Persons

Co

py

that so emphasizes work. The making of identity is the precondition, the


context, an outcome, and a means to evaluate the facts of subsistence. It
is what we as social beings are about.
I want to bring all these sources of information to bear upon a single
case, to answer not the projects question but the more complicated one
posed earlier: How do we understand a persons subjectivity and his
agency? The case is that of the man we have called Roger Kelly. Over two
and one-half years I interviewed Roger six times and talked with him
much more frequently. I also interviewed his wife and his psychiatrist. I
will briey describe how Roger tried to regure his life in the face of its
massive disruption by mental disorder. Mental disorder creates conditions that approximate what Vygotsky would call an experiment, albeit a
natural one: situations in which the ordinary courses of activity and the
forms of psychological functioning are disrupted. Under such conditions, the arrangements of self-management, the social constituents of
identity, are revealed.

Ex

am

Psychiatric practice is a kind of subworld, a demimonde, with many


kinds of activities and many differing characters who perform them, all
brought together to manage the social disorders of mental illness. It
refers to more than what psychiatrists do about illness. Instead psychiatric practice encompasses all that psychiatrists and allied health professionals, social service administrators, police and social workers, patients,
and their friends and families do about mental illness. Here I will focus
mainly on one range of activity, diagnosis, and one aspect of the gured
world, psychiatric classication or nosology. There are several traditions
and vocations for managing mental disorder, some medical, some psychological, and some sociological. The efforts to unite these traditions
under the hegemony of medical psychiatrywhat is sometimes called
medicalizationhave not yet erased their differences. The eld is still
strongly contested at certain points. Moreover, American psychiatry differs from European and other ethnopsychiatries.2 Let us begin, however,
as if psychiatric classication were a simple thing.
Four broad groups of mental disorders listed by the fourth edition
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, or DSM-IV
for short (APA 1994), were represented among the participants in our

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

196

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

study:3 (1) the schizophrenias and other psychotic disorders, (2) major
affective disorders, (3) anxiety disorders, and (4) personality disorders.
The rst category, schizophrenias, designates severe disturbances of cognition, especially social cognition, and of perception. The extreme or
orid manifestations of schizophrenia, once called hebephrenia and
now simply disorganized schizophrenia, are the prototypic gures of
the mad person. This group is also known as thought disorders, in contrast with the second large category, affective disorders. Affective disorder
is characterized by severe disturbances of mood or emotionality, and the
most important variants are major depressive disorder (major depression) and bipolar affective disorder (manic-depression).
There is considerable overlap between these two categories when they
are attributed to people by diagnosis on the basis of symptoms. Persons
with schizophrenia have frequent (and even characteristic) disturbances
of emotional expression, called inappropriate (silly) or at (monotonous or uninected) affect. Similarly, persons with affective disorders
often exhibit some psychotic behaviors: delusions, hallucinations, and
confused thinking. Psychiatrists distinguish the two by judging the relative pervasiveness and often the persistence of the two symptomatic
types. It is not uncommon, as we shall see in Rogers case, for a person to
receive a diagnosis of thought disorder on one occasion and affective
disorder on another, especially when different diagnosticians weigh the
evidence.
The last two groups differ from the rst two by the presumed severity
of their effects and by their causes, their etiology. Anxiety disorders (in
which for convenience I also include dissociative, somatoform, and adjustment disorders) and personality disorders are widely conceived to be
less disabling and often less enduring conditions. They are also thought
to arise more from psychological and social (situational) causes than
from physiological dysfunction. Experience and learning, especially under conditions of extreme psychological stress or trauma, are at the root
of these disorders;4 they are simply secondary or contingent to schizophrenias and affective disorders, whose causes are ordinarily thought to
be neurological.
Anxiety disorders span much of what used to be termed neuroses in
the psychodynamic literature. Persons with anxiety disorders often respond favorably to certain medications, the widely used anxiolytics or
minor tranquilizers (distinguished from the antipsychotics or major

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

197

Ex

am

Co

py

tranquilizers, like Thorazine), of which Xanax and Valium are the best
known. Hence, and despite the larger role accorded to environmental
factors in their etiology, anxiety disorders are considered to have a neurological substrate, to involve a physiological impairment. They are arrayed as specic, limited or episodic, clinical syndromes with the
thought and affective disorders on what is called Axis I in the DSM.
Personality disorders are yet again different. They are stable but
maladaptive patterns of relation to ones environment (and oneself)
whose etiology is regarded to be primarily developmental in the psychological sense, a matter of learning during infancy and early childhood.
They are global disorders of peoples cognitive, affective, and practical
organizations, that is, their personalities as this discourse denes
them. Their formation precedes, coexists with, and may outlast the various clinical disorders. Beginning with the third edition of the DSM, the
personality disorders were assigned to a second axis, afrming their
distinction from other disorders. They are gured, in the abstract form of
the diagnostic space, to vary independently of the clinical disorders of
Axis I.
Ideally, any diagnosed person should be described along both axes.
Practically, however, psychiatrists focus on one aspect or the other and
accord one or the other precedence in treatment. It is unusual for a
person with schizophrenia to be diagnosed with a personality disorder,
and rarer still for such a disorder to be given much time in treatment.
Schizophrenia is taken to be the more important problem because it is
the more serious illness. In contrast, an anxiety disorder that coexists
with one of the severe personality disorders may receive little attention
beyond a drug prescription.5
This system of psychiatric classication is a phenomenological or descriptive system. The types of disorder are differentiated according to
observed constellations of symptoms called syndromes. The ideology of
psychiatry maintains that people are distinct from their illnesses, insofar as the illnesses are presumed to originate from somatic dysfunctions,
that is, diseases of the brain. Still, because the illnesses are noticeable
from the ways people behave, psychiatric categories inevitably identify
personae, recognizable modes of activitywhat I call signatures of
practice. There is an understandable confusion, an elision, that pervades
not only popular discourse but also the practical and informal discourse
of professionals. Persons diagnosed with schizophrenia become schizo-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

198

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

phrenics; those diagnosed with borderline personality disorder, borderlines.6


Each diagnostic type has instructions, so to speak, for the making of a
kind of person, and these instructions are communicated within the
interchange of staff members and their charges. It is not usually direct
transmission, though that may occur when therapists teach clients about
symptoms and their interrelations in order to encourage self-monitoring
and to minimize, by early detection, the effects of exacerbations (active
episodes of chronic illness). It is more strongly the unspoken concerns,
the ways questions are posed, and the themes of psychological instruments, that is, the expectations embedded in the means of gathering
information, that allow people to infer the symptomatic activities that
constitute the therapeutic identity.
Only prejudice and misunderstanding make us think that persons we
consider mentally ill are not as aware of and attentive to the social
framing of communication as normal people. We attune to one another by voicing, inection, and accent, by spatial orientation and arrangement, by timing, rhythm, and pace, through the contexts and intertexts of speech and gesture. All the mechanics of practical
communication apply to convey a sense of ones own position. Moreover,
there is a growing popular literature on psychiatric disorders in which
people may read about the basics of (disordered) behavior that constitute
such therapeutic identities. Roger, whose life we consider here, made
frequent use of both therapeutic and popular resources to understand
and, I believe, to produce the forms of his own behavior and their place
within a world of related gures.
Roger has had many diagnoses in his psychiatric history, but has
nally received the one that is almost characteristic of debatable cases:
borderline personality disorder. The name of this disorder describes it
accurately. Something that is borderline exists between more settled or
more clearly dened things. Borderline personality disorder is a most
unusual category, whose history includes a wide range of earlier diagnosesodd forms of schizophrenia, affective disorders, and earlier personality disorderswhich are preserved in its diverse symptomatic prole.
Its historical course records the attempt to assimilate what was originally a psychotherapeutic identity into a medical discourse. The notion
of the borderline comes from the psychoanalytic or Freudian tradition,
where it designated those people whose volatile cognitive and interper-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

199

am

Co

py

sonal behaviors fell between the recognized psychotic and neurotic


forms of organization. Even today, professionals can largely agree about
the ways borderlines behave in therapy. Yet, as American psychiatry
moved from its largely psychodynamic orientation of the 1950s to a
medical or somatic model of illness and treatment in the 1960s and
1970s, this psychotherapeutic understanding of the borderline came into
contact with ambiguous or intermediate forms of thought and mood
disorder that derive from the medical psychiatric tradition (for more on
borderline disorders see Lachicotte 1992; Stone 1986).
What borderline behavior signied was thoroughly unsettled by this
mixture, and new hierarchy, of discourses. What kind of disease or disorder produces the borderline syndrome, and what therapeutic forms
best treat it, are still undecided paradigmatic questions, in Kuhns sense
(1962). They refer to broad differences in the ways the object world of
the discipline is construed. Professionals talk about borderlines far more
than any other kind of disorder, and their mixed feelings about the
people called borderlines reect in part the uncertain status of psychiatric diagnosis. This uncertainty is not limited to borderline disorders; it is
general to the entire nosology. Psychiatry has a descriptive system, but
the discipline wants something more. It wants to connect its various
categories by a general theory of neurological causation, or etiology, but
cannot do so. Hence, odd categories like borderline personality disorder
persist. The very uncertainty epitomized by this diagnosis gives the people who hold it a kind of power, a freedom, in dealing with professionals.

The Case

Ex

Roger at our rst meeting was a rather short and rotund man of thirtythree. He looked the part of a prosperous middle-class salaried worker.
He was well kempt and well dressed and showed few of the irregularities
of self-care that were apparent among other patients. Roger did not have
obvious thought disorders, behavioral abnormalities, or disturbances of
mood. His affectemotional tone of speech and activityseemed, on
observation, full and appropriate to his situations. It was hard to see why
Roger was on the ward at alluntil you spoke with him. Roger had a bad
case of alien voice. He spoke loudly, with little inection, yet with a force
and speed that intimated agitation or excitement. His tone of voice often
did not match his other means of expression, either gestural or facial.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

200

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

There was something at odds about Roger. Although a college graduate, he had been consistently underemployed during the ten years he had
worked. He was married, throughout the time I met with him, to a
psychiatric nurse. I rst interviewed him during his fourth psychiatric
hospitalization.
The previous year had been a hard one for Roger. He had suffered
several setbacks at work, including a transfer that he regarded as a demotion. His marriage had gone through a shaky period after four years of
relative tranquillity. According to his own assessment, Roger turned suicidal because he could not handle the stress of his job, of his and other
peoples expectations:

am

Yeah, I justracing thoughts and fearful thoughts and you feel closed
in and I cant do this job another minute, and yeah they had me on
something called the mini-stacker. And these big boxes would come
down with hundreds of parts in them. I had to count all the parts;
theyre in bags, some of them werent in bags. Then I had to put them
on another conveyer belt, punch in a few things, and it would take
them to the racks and stick them up in stock. But some days youd
stand around an hour. Your technician is looking at you the whole time.
And I dont know what to do. And hell give me some racks to ll and
Ive never done them before. And it just created a lot of tension, him
looking at me all the time. You know, and other days youve got so
much on that conveyer belt, its backed up around the corner someplace. [It changes so much] and I like something steady.

Ex

Three times that year he made attempts on his own life and each time
was hospitalized on the psychiatric ward of the local hospital. These
were short stays. Once in the hospital Roger stabilized quickly and soon
left to return to work.
At our rst interview Roger gave the impression of someone who had
found an explanation of his problems that he could accept: Its a major
psychiatric illness . . . A sort of thinking and feeling disorder. I cant
always trust what Im thinking and feeling. He remembered, in a garbled way, the names the doctor had told him: Obsessive-compulsive
disorder. Bipolar. Schizoid-affective personality disorder. But he preferred to call his illness neurotic depression.
After this discharge Roger was placed on medical disability by his
employer. As time went on he bought into the therapeutic system in a big

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

201

Co

py

way. He told me at our second interview: Maybe Im really sick. Im


trying not to face that fact, but it looks like Im really sick, you know. I
may have this disability for several years; I dont know. Each time I
asked, he would say that his meds (medications) were the best regimen
he had had so far. He took an active interest in psychosocial therapies
as well, attending weekly sessions with his therapist, with his support
group, and, from time to time, with his wife in couples therapy. He told
me a year after we rst met: I have a 200-page notebook upstairs that
has notes on sessions with every doctor, social worker Ive ever seen. It
helps.
It was as if Roger had taken on his recovery as a job, and he worked
quite hard at it:
I think all the time. You know, How can I go back to work? and How
can I be less fearful and less depressed? and Should I exercise more at
the spa? Should I change vitamins? Should I eat different foods?
and Should I talk to myself and be positive all the time? You know, all
the techniques Ive read about, so I incorporate a few of them, but not
all of them. Theres so many ways that I dont know, you know, what Ive
read. Ive read probably thirty, forty books, but I cant remember, you
know, what was what. I probably should stop reading those things.

Ex

am

About the reading he confessed: Thats my major activity. Self-help


books, trying to change my personality better, and things like that. But
this therapeutic labor met with little success. Each of his attempts to
complete the taskto return to real employmentended quickly: I
dont know [what will happen]. Ithe book store had four days open for
a cashier job in January. I worked two days and I got very panicky
attacks, fearful. I had to leave, so I only completed two days out of four.
Id like to go back to work part-time . . . but I dont know if I can last
more than a couple of days.
Eventually Roger settled into a routine of television, volunteer work,
and the rounds of therapy. He applied for federal income maintenance
(SSDI), which he was granted after a years wait. Two years from our rst
interview he told me: Im only 35. I mean I wikind of wish I was 60,
and I was done with all . . . It would make things simpler, you know, Im
disabled. Ill just live out my last few years and do a little good works and
try to enjoy my day. But when youre only 35 theres pressure to get back
in there and try it again. Ive tried four or ve times. Ive been up in the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

202

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

hospital ve times now. How many more times do I have to try? When I
last talked to Roger, eight months after he admitted this secret wish, to be
older, he was still planning a return to college, a plan he had conceived
over a year before. He was better now and thought once again that he
could move back into a regular life.
Throughout our acquaintance Roger professed a mental illness without hesitation. His ideas about this illness, however, altered. Not only did
he learn more about psychiatric illness in general; his conception of the
nature of his own illness changed. By the second interview he had
adopted manic-depression, a combination of racing thoughts and
enduring depression: how about just a guy feels lousy and theres no
known reason? The lack of apparent reason for both his cognitive and
affective states clinched the argument for him: Im starting to believe
Im very manic-depressive. Over the weekend I was quite suicidal, and
there was no apparent reason. My wife was hereshes a psychiatric
nurse, so thats as good as anything. And she said, Youre recycling too
fast or something. You need another antidepressant. It had to be something within him, not outside and visible. At this point Roger focused
more upon his symptoms than upon any causal model of his illness. It
was enough for him that his illness was a medical or somatic condition
and not, as his parents seemed to suggest, a problem of character or will.
By the third interview, one year after the rst, Roger had again
modied his ideas about his psychological problems: Ive always
thought they were mainly chemically related and I still think they are.
But there is some personality. When youre growing up, I believethat
affected the way I . . . my behavior, my thinking patterns. That has not
changed. Its a combination of personality and chemical, thats what I
feel, more chemical though. After discharge from his baseline hospitalization Roger had begun working with a new psychiatrist. This doctor
told me that he had changed Rogers diagnosis and the emphasis of his
therapy. Previously Roger had carried a primary diagnosis of either bipolar disorder or schizoaffective disorder, depressed type.7 His new psychiatrist decided, putatively on the basis of psychological testing, that
Roger was basically borderline, that is, personality disordered. He had
told Roger his conclusion, and Roger had begun to take it to heart.
The next interview, six months later, found Roger still learning his
roles. In fact he reverted to speaking about his racing thoughts and
twisted, negative thoughts, which he carefully referred to mania and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

203

Ex

am

Co

py

depression and not to any character disorder. He was pleased about a


new cognitive therapy he had begun to learn and a new support group
for manic-depressives he was attending. His medication was once again
effective, especially the Prozac recently prescribed for him. Roger was
quite upbeat and full of plans to return to normal life. He spoke of a piece
of land he and his wife had recently purchased in a resort area, of their
thoughts of relocating there. He did not expect, nor had he for a long
time expected, a cure. He told me, with a sophistication that bespoke his
continual reading: No, youre always a manic-depressive. You never
recover. You have a chemical imbalance thats always there in your synapses and all that, your serotonin uptake. Its always got to be regulated.
Still, at this time Roger was optimistic about his ability to manage the
illness.
I encountered a chastened Roger six months later at our fth interview. He had just returned from a three-day hospitalization instigated by
a severe bout of suicidal thoughts. When I asked about the cause of
this incident, Roger, in his rather oddly earnest and naive way, began to
report problems of character: I would say . . . borderline personality
disorder. That means a manipulator, a liar, social behavior is sometimes
you get abnormalthings like that. I go to massage parlors occasionally.
My wife knows about it. Shes very upset. So part of my personality is like
that. And, alongside the biochemical model of the previous interview,
he recounted different causes: My parents were highly critical of me
growing up. I had to be perfect, get straight As, so I was very hard on
myself, so some aspects of my personality probably never developed as
much as they should have. I stuntstunted, stunted growth, thats what
happened.
He sometimes mixed the two sorts of understandings: My mothers
some traits of it. I think its genetic, a lot of that stuff. Its how youre
raised, according to that book, by your parents. Whether they uh, reward
you or punish you, theyre critical or loving. Theyre there when you
need them. Breastfed, too early, not enoughI mean theres lot of factors
involved in that, sort of like chemistry. Theres a lot of factors. Roger had
been reading again and was attempting to piece his ideas together.
What amazed me, after the countless times I had heard staff complain
about borderlines, was that he showed no resentment, shame, or anger at
the dread label of borderline personality. To Roger, one psychiatric condition was equivalent to another. But, as he reminded me, Im not really a

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

204

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

full blown borderline . . . No. No, I got a book on it. I let my doctormy
psychiatristhe said its a good book, but I havent been sexually abused
as a child, I remember, nothing like that.
In the two years from our rst interview to our fth, then, Roger
consistently identied himself as a mentally ill participant in the world of
mental health care. But his ideas of his illness had deepened and broadened in the ways predicated by an ever fuller involvement in that world.
Rogers nal report of the extent to which his illness affected his life is no
surprise: It permeates you. Its everything, like friendships . . . its in me.
It affects everything. Everything from my relationship to God, who wont
heal me, to my wifetry to cheat on her, to my parentscall them
twice a day. I mean my best friend, I told his girlfriend he was seeing
somebody else, and, I mean Ive really screwed up lately. I uh, its just
its in my life. It affects my thinking and feeling, and my behavior. Its me.
Im who I am.

Public and Private Politics

Ex

am

As Roger left the public world centered upon work and the day-to-day
accounting of common interaction, he entered into different means for
the making and interpretation of behavior. These psychiatric means are
not completely alien to the everyday, so inuential has medical practice
become. But the more removed one becomes from the ordinary arenas of
interaction, the more salient are these special technologies of the self.
Not all people who nd themselves so secluded agree with, or even
attend to, the psychiatric line. But their opposition or indifference leaves
them in the same ambit as those, like Roger, who accept it wholeheartedly. The power of social circumscription, what has been called role
constriction, sets persons with psychiatric disorders apart, despite their
wishes and sometimes considerable efforts to counteract it.8
There were other reasons for Rogers willingness to adopt psychiatric
ideas. Rogers inability to work was the central fact of his predicament in
the eyes of others. It was both the great puzzle and the great complication of his everyday life. Work is, in many social venues of the United
States, the central claim to personal competency and personal worth.
When work is denied us, that failure requires explanation, and the
explanation comes to serve as defense for our endangered personhood.
Roger devoted an inordinate amount of discursive labor to this subject,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

205

am

Co

py

and his explanation reveals much about his local as well as our societys
world.
His mental illness obviously entered into a longstanding interchange
among Roger, his wife, and his family. His wife told me: Theres always
been a big competition in the family. And he was the one that went to
four years of college, and you know, everybody wanted him to be a
lawyer, and heres his brother who didnt go to any college and took a few
courses at IBM and has a very good position in IBM. Rogers failure
required justication if he was to reclaim some place of value among his
close associates. He received little help or understanding of his efforts, as
he himself admitted: Im very closely tied to my parents. I dont know
why. They neverIm probably trying to get what I never got from them.
I dont knowlove and understanding. All I got was criticism and you
can do better . . . They wonder why Im not at work now. They dont
understand mental illness. They think, you know, if you get off the pills,
get off this, everythingll be OK. This sense of competitiveness in economic achievement, the emphasis on self-control, the giving and withholding of affection as motivation toward achievement, are stereotypical
features of American middle-class family life. Rogers quite obvious
failure to meet these standards, and the consequent disapproval he met
from his family, was a lasting dilemma in his social life and for his
self-understanding.
If from his family Roger met expectations that he found hard to fulll,
he encountered an opposing pressure from his wife (and his psychiatrist). She told me:

Ex

When he sees a job in the paper that he really would want, I have to
think in my mind, is he really going to be able to perform that type of
work, and yet I dont want to be the one to be the bad guy and say
Roger, I dont think you can really do that. You know, I think he ought
to look into job opportunities as hes been doing, but I think theres
some things that hes just never going to be able to do. And until he
goes and applies for these jobs and realizes hes not qualied hes, you
know, its hard for him to accept that.

She saw her task as helping Roger lower his expectations about employment, in line with her notions of the functional impairment that mental
disorders cause. From her stance as a psychiatric nurse, she had adopted
in her domestic relations both the separation between Roger and his

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

206

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

illness and the power of illness over person that is the popular way of
guring this separation. But this toleration too was a problem for Roger.
With his family, the distress that mental disorder caused Roger disappeared in the disguise of Rogers defective will. With his wife, Rogers
own thoughts, feelings, and interests disappeared within the manifestations of illness.
It is not illness itself but the interrelations of various elds of practice
that determine the possibilities of self-expression. It is easy to guess what
impelled Rogers conation of identity work and work in its usual sense.
How Roger used psychiatric notions strategically in his everyday relations is rather more interesting. From his local world, shaped by the
counterpositions and alliances among his wife, psychiatrist, parents (and
silently, his brother), and employer, Roger crafted a response, which was
at once his identied place and his proper labor.
It was not a singular response. When speaking about his parents or his
employers and co-workers, Roger usually emphasized his manic-depression. Both medical psychiatry and much of popular psychological discourse conceive this illness, bipolar affective disorder, to be primarily a
somatic disorder, caused by a dysfunction of neurotransmission. Susceptibility to the illness is also thought to be genetically based. Thus having
manic-depression is not ones own fault. Roger opposed its unwilled
disruption of his own capacities to his parents and co-workers interpretation that he lacked the willpower or the energy to change his misbehavior. He borrowed the authority of medicine to answer other peoples
claims against him. At the same time, the persuasiveness of this psychiatric identity increased as it provided Roger with a way to legitimate his
past activity and to maintain the relationships he valued. It is appropriate, when one is aficted by a disease that incapacitates, for ones therapy
to become ones entire work. So Roger justied his withdrawal from the
world of employment by his identity as manic-depressive.
This construction drew authority from explicitly biological models of
bipolar disorder to oppose Rogers parents ethnopsychology of will.
Unlike the latter, which locates disorder as misbehavior, within the realm
of self-control, Rogers adopted understanding blamed no person, only
his neurophysiology. It both signaled and consequently encouraged the
restitution of his relationships, with himself, his wife (who shared this
assessment of his disorder), and even his parents. Moreover, it explained
the propriety of Rogers disability and the promise of his (chemo)thera-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

207

Ex

am

Co

py

peutic regimen. In his discourse manic-depression came up commonly


with talk of work, of disability, and (however paradoxically) of hope.
When speaking of his wife and his psychiatrist Roger usually took a
different line. He emphasized his borderline persona. After all, Rogers
wife did not doubt his incapacity or the necessity of his treatment. If
anything she went too far: her fatalism and her obsession were even
greater than Rogers. Rogers life apart from his illnesswhat he wanted
to do and what he could do, not what he was made to do by diseasewas
nearly lost in their dealings. Hence Roger changed the terms of interpretation. The personality disorders, to the extent that they are learned,
matters of experience, remain within the scope of will. We can unlearn
what we have learned, if we want to. Roger drew upon this sense about
personality disorders to oppose his wifes (or his psychiatrists) reduction
of him to his disease.
By becoming borderline at the proper times and places, Roger empowered a sense of his agency, his active self-expression, without letting
go of the legitimate disabilities of illness. Borderline personality disorder is peculiarly adapted to this kind of double posture because of its
undecided position in psychiatric knowledge. It carries still the signs of
schizophrenia, of affective disorder, and of character disorder, in a continually rebalanced mix. And the powerfulness attributed to its volatility,
the fact that its balance may come apart at any moment, served Roger
well. It went far to ensure that he, the actor Roger, would not be
dismissed or overlooked. So Roger reproduced a sense of personal power
through his borderline identity.
The association of willfulness that colors the personality disorders
does two kinds of discursive work here. It differentiates and reasserts
Rogers self-activity, his autonomy, in the face of his wifes presumption
(and cultivation) of his debility, and yet it leaves that activity somehow
unwilled, beyond everyday self-management, and hence in the realm of
therapy. Personality disorders (like the classical neuroses) double the
self through its division into impaired (misbehaving) and intact (helpseeking) parts. They thereby afford inculpable opportunities for strategic
actionwhich Roger pursued.
This talk, like the actions it describes, is directed to his wife. When
Roger addressed his parents through the notion of the borderline, he did
so in a way that did not accede too much to their view of his problems as
willful. Instead, he indirectly reassigned culpability for that ill-formed

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

208

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

will to his parents, according to the developmental etiologies conventional to these personality disorders. Rogers control over his social misbehavior was thus vitiated, his actions recast into a reenactment of an
originally pathological relation of parents to child. Work has little to do
with this settling of personal relationships, and Rogers talk of it did not
coincide with his borderline discourse. Its absence may also have been
strategically signicantthough such an attribution is always chancy.
Work disability is ordinarily associated with only the most severe personality disorder, which Roger did not avow. His borderline identication could not serve him, and was not invoked by this crucial problematic.
Hence Rogers bipolar and borderline identities participated in different social and imaginative landscapes. They were differently addressed
and differently answerable, in Bakhtins terms, yet they were not thoroughly separable. A gure in the realm of popular psychology united
them: the idea of stress: [My doctor] said depression and borderline
personality disorder. Thats what he labeled it as. And I cant handle
stress. We agree on that. Though Roger clearly described the conditions
of his work and personal life in terms any reader would call stressful, he
did not use the word itself until the third interview (just quoted). The
context was one in which he was asked to represent his doctors understanding of his illness. Stress provided the gurative grounds of their
agreement. Over the next year Roger increasingly used the notion of
stress as a point of interchange not only between the differing understandings afforded by his dialogue with psychiatry but also between his
(ever more psychiatrically informed) conceptions and those of his parents and friends: [My parents] say, You cant take . . . well, they nally
realized I cant take stress, and they think Im a little lazy. Thats what
they think.
This usage is not idiosyncratic. Just as, in contemporary Euroamerican
social life, a common medium for material interchanges has developed in
the form of money, so too a common medium for psychic interchanges
has developed under the rubric of stress. The concept takes a negative
mode, somewhat like impedance, resistance, or load in engineering,
and it predicates a differential between inner and outer, personal and
public economies. In the metaphorical world of architectonics (using
Bakhtins Kantian terminology) the person or individual is conceived
as a integral system of balances among constituents, as conditioned by its
environments. When changes of state occur, such as when an inter-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

209

Ex

am

Co

py

change with the environment takes place, the equilibrium of the system
is necessarily disrupted.
In Rogers words: And, like I said, you dont know what feeling is
normal, so its a little bit of both, really. But mainly irrational thinking.
Then I get under stress. Im thinking irrationally and feeling fearful. So
its really a combination that kicks in under stress. Stress is the (retrospective) measure of effort requisite to reestablish the (interior) balance
of the person, the cost of living in alterable circumstances. Projectively, it
is the measure of an environments potential disequilibrating effect. The
notion was popularized in psychology by Selye (1956), though it has
been extended from its original and calculable physiological settings to
less measurable, qualitative changes of psychological state.9
Stress talk is a genre of both popular and professional psychologies,
whose chronotopeBakhtins phrase for the denitive gured world of a
form of discourseis the abstract landscape just described. Different
persons, different identied systems, are presumed to have differing susceptibilities to stress. (Roger said: My stress tolerance is very low. I dont
know why my stress tolerance is so low. Ive never gured that out.)
This susceptibility may come from either genetic causes or historical
(developmental) ones. Likewise, different environments predicate differing stresses, for any number of reasons. In order to calculate actual
stress, one must know both functional components: systemic capability and environmental energetics. At this abstract level, the gured world
of stress unites rst the biological and sociological conditions of mental
illnesses and then the different forms of illness as so many products of
these generic conditions. Stress warrants that, no matter what variant
illnesses one has, there exists a common ground for them in the relation
of body to social world. As Roger told me: And the other thing is
sensitivity to stress, and depression, and fear, and obsession. Theyre all
tied in. When I get stressful, depression, fear, and obsessional thinking
kick right in, so I cant take much stress. It also warrants, by extension,
the integrity of a personI cant take much stressinhabited by the
personae named by these illnesses.

Artifacts and Associates


Roger used various tools to revise and establish his new identications
and understandings of himself. These mediating devices included the
various behavioral templates provided by accounts of diagnostic catego-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

210

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

ries (borderline, manic-depressive, obsessive-compulsive, and so on), by


therapeutic regimens (cognitive chaining and replacement, de-escalation), and by Rogers own rehearsals (the therapy book) and others
renditions (self-help and support groups). Apart from these specics,
there was the structure, the means of organization provided by the token of the activity itself. Performing therapy, attending self-help groups,
reading both popular and professional treatisesthese activities express,
by their very sensibility, a dispositional lesson. They signal to ourselves
as well as others where we are headed and with whom we travel. They
are the means we use to reproduce the desire for particular identities. But
there is a price.
Access to these activities and to the specic identities they produce is
a matter of social situation and negotiationof, in a word, politics. By
participating one adopts specic afliations, disafliations, and relations
of self-production. Rogers course was not smooth. He did not settle on
one psychiatric identity but maintained several versions of himself: as
bipolar, as obsessive-compulsive, as borderline. These were tied to the
activities and agents from which they were mastered. As either the situational or the institutional relations among these activities and agents
changed, so did Rogers identications (or at least the work needed to
maintain them). The change of psychiatrists, of therapeutic groups, of
the relations between wife and family, wife and psychiatrist, psychiatrist
and group, even abstract changes in the authority of psychiatric diagnoses, all had direct effects upon Rogers self-conception. Bakhtin would
say that, even as means of the imagination, semiotic devices remain
others words and acts, others speaking, others voices. By using them
one gains the material toward identity because they come socially identied. At the same time that you inform yourself, you open yourself
(even in your own thoughts and feelings) to the social world.
Yet the world of inner speaking, of the imagination and consciousness,
is not a simple copy of the social world. We have neither the time nor the
resources to do analytic justice to Rogers history of self, to how prior
self-concepts and self-understandings shaped the course of his self-reformation. What Rogers case shows, however, is that ones personal agency
is not the creation of a self that is always uniquely ones own. Rather,
agency takes shape in what we call the space of authoring. This space is
formed, both within us and outside us, by the very multiplicity of persons, who are identiable positions in networks of social production,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

211

Ex

am

Co

py

and of worlds of inner activity that are also scenes of consciousness.


When we act, whether that act is instrumental or imaginative, we
move through this space guratively. None of us is occupied singularly: we are not possessed by one identity, one discourse, one subjectposition. Each act is simultaneously a social dynamic, social work, a set
of identications and negations, an orchestration or arrangement of
voices. And our sense of self comes from the history of our arrangements, our styles of saying and doing through others. The freedom
that Bakhtin calls authorship comes from the ways differing identications can be counterposed, brought to work against one another, to
create a position, our own voice, from which we work.
Roger opposed several identities drawn from psychiatric discourse to
those cast for him by his wife, family, co-workers, psychiatrists, even by
himself. We believe he did not do so either to destroy or deny or to
assert or take up any one identityto say that he was truly one person
or another. Rather, he orchestrated these differing identities, differing
voices, to shape a place for his own activity. When speaking of his family
and friends, and of work, Roger counterposed his manic-depression, and
thus signally the authority of the persons, his wife and psychiatrist, and
the situations of therapy that were its expressive source. When contesting the connes of these last associations, he drew upon his borderline
persona, referring to the strong (though in this case negative) voices of
psychiatrists and the popular psychological literature, to the disorder of
everyday impulse that wrecks the discipline of therapy. His agency was
no one personage but the ensemble of persons that lived around and
within him.
It is therefore entirely conceivable that Roger saw no conict between
identities founded upon the two types of disorder, that they simply coexisted for him, manifest at different times in differing forms. The gure of
stress, which is the popular and professional point of some resolution of
these differences, seems, in Rogers case, an afterthought. Or, stress is not
so much the reality as the possibility of unity; not so much the source,
the original, from which different forms of personal disorder arise as the
point of exhaustion into which they collapse. Hence it is not really the
resolution, but rather the recognition of these different gurations of
personhood, these psychiatrized identications. Rogers case teaches
us one nal lesson: there is no necessary integrity to self-understanding,
but there are degrees of relatedness among the partial selves that respond

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

212

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

to sets of our associates, our multiple others (see Gergen 1994). That is a
story we will have to leave to another time.

Ex

am

Co

py

Michel Foucault has taught us that historical and political actions, on the
grand scale, created rst insanity and then mental illness from madness, a notion of behavioral and cognitive aberration hardly divorced
from everyday life. We must begin to amend his vision at the point where
it seems whole, at the point where the variety of the forms of life, which
after all was one of Foucaults motives, becomes subsumed in the habitual ordering of these forms. When it is forgotten that such an order must
be reproduced, when it is acknowledged that the reproductions themselves are identical, then one must think again. Foucault must always be
brought to contend against himself.
It is also a mistake to concentrate too much upon the course of discipline, for example, of interchanges that move only from therapist to
client, however fully explored. As Rogers and my other case studies
show, people participate in several discourses and elds of activity relevant to their psychological identities. They attend, besides the formal
therapeutic activities of hospitals and mental health centers, self-help,
support, and advocacy groups that are more loosely connected to the
institutional psychiatric apparatus. The mass media also provide a substantial and less regular means toward psychological self-formation. Selfhelp and popular psychological literature and televised talk shows and
documentaries take the discursive and practical subjects of psychiatry
and medicine into a realm where they associate and contend with forms
of subjectivity and agency created and preserved under different social
and cultural worlds.10 And, while these means are one important vehicle
of the medicalization of everyday life, they are also an opportunity for
the transguration of the properly medical, for what Bakhtin called the
carnivalization of authoritative discourse. We will return to this interplay
of gured worlds in Chapter 11.
Finally, if one penetrates even further into the underlife (Goffman
1961) of psychiatry, one reaches common parlance and the cultural
subworlds that persons with mental disorders create among themselves
and with consociates of other practical elds. What we call the everyday,
or everyday practice, is a reality that is everywhere diverse and at the
same time substantially whole. That is to say, it is composed within the
interrelations of many specic practices. Any one instance of the every-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

213

Ex

am

Co

py

day is both peculiar in the practical arrangement of the moment and


locale and generic in its dependence upon the institutional (extralocal)
and historical conditions of the discursive and practical elds.11 Rogers
and all our worlds are local, as Bakhtin would insist, only to the extent
that they seek the general, and vice versa. Our identities are formed
within, and serve to mediate, this dialogic movement between extension and intention in social life, between worlds at play and ways of
self-discipline.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

10

Co

py

Authoring Oneself as a
Woman in Nepal

Ex

am

In the spring of 1990 a peoples pro-democracy movement overthrew the


one-party panchayat system in Nepal, which had been in place for thirty
years. Political parties again became legal, and in the spring of 1991
elections ofcially restored the multiparty or bahudal system of government. In the interval between these events, several remarkable incidents
took place in Naudada, the Hindu mixed-caste hill community where
Skinner and Holland did ethnographic research.1 Before the elections,
women from one of the eastern hamlets (gaon) began a procession toward Tarara Gaon, the administrative center and gathering place of the
community, a place renowned for its tea shops where men came to drink
and gamble. Along the way women from other gaons joined them, and
together they marched, shouting their demands: Men, stop your drinking and gambling! Let women inherit property! Give women equal
rights! Dont marry off your daughters so young! In another incident, a
group of women brandishing sticks entered some of the local teashops to
break drinking glasses, bottles of distilled alcohol (raksi), and the equipment used to make the homemade brew. A third incident took place after
a woman was beaten by her drunken husband. A group of about fteen
women, armed with sticks, descended on him and threatened to beat
him if he ever again abused his wife.
Both the womens banding together to voice demands at odds with
patriarchal power and their retaliatory acts against men were unusual,
perhaps unprecedented, events for this area of Nepal. On the surface they
were surprising because, in Naudada, women as well as men voiced
notions of the good woman that resonated with Brahmanical teach214
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

215

Co

py

ings about how women should be and act.2 Women enacted this gured
world and its identity of the good Hindu woman in many ways in their
everyday lives.
But a closer look suggested that women also felt anger and resentment toward the ways this narrativized world dened them as women.
Disrupting currents were also evident in relational aspects of identity.
Skinners research showed how young girls and women developed a
sense of their position relative to others in the social world, a sense that
as women they were entitled to, or denied, access to certain spaces and
activities. There were movements into, and thus contestations with, the
positional identity associated with the good Hindu woman. The
womens protests exemplied these contestations of self and of gender
relations. The womens complex and contested self-understandings developed and were produced in particular social, historical, and cultural
sites or activities, and their development allowed good Naudadan
women to rapidly transform into angry, active protesters who called their
treatment into question through their words and deeds.

Growing Up Female in Hindu Nepal

Ex

am

Skinners research in Naudada focused on childrens roles in the coproduction and reproduction of gender and caste identities and relations.
Skinner examined the organization, contexts, and content of socialization activities in which children participated and the words and actions
of thirty boys and girls, aged eight to eighteen, as they responded to and
co-created these structures, activities, and cultural knowledge. We will
concentrate here on the ten girls she followed most closely, and on what
they conveyed to her about their lives in daily encounters, tape-recorded
interviews, and, for the literate ones, written accounts.
The girls often portrayed themselves as good workers, good girls, and
obedient and honorable daughters. These identities relevant to work,
gender, and family were central ones for females in Naudada, and were
gured by the Brahmanical model of the expected life path for women.
When Skinner asked them to describe themselves, most girls recited the
good habits they had and the bad ones they avoided. A typical response
included most of the following characteristics and behaviors: I am a
good girl. I love those who are smaller than me. I obey my parents. I
never tell a lie; I always speak the truth. I get along with others. I dont

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

216

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

talk unnecessarily with boys, and I dont quarrel or scold others. I dont
steal from others elds. I stay in the house and do my work. I do my
studies. I only get angry if someone scolds me.
Parbati, a thirteen-year-old Damai girl (Damai is a lower caste considered untouchable),3 described herself this way: I am a very good girl. I
dont fool around with others . . . I am very good . . . I dont speak that
much also. I will say Namaskar, didi (Greetings, older sister) on the
spot when I meet you. I am like that. I am very good. I dont even like to
fool around much. In my being, I am very good. I do my own work by
myself. And Muna, a Damai girl of fourteen, said: I dont speak lies, and
when somebody speaks a lie against me, I get furious, but I dont try to
pick ghts . . . I dont get angry . . . I have lesser quarrels and I dont
speak nonsense. These self-descriptions did not vary much by caste or
socioeconomic status. Here is Maiya, an eleven-year-old Chetri girl: I
am a good, nice girl. I dont get mad at anyone. I dont ght. I dont steal
. . . I dont deceive anyone and dont lie.
The girls responses had a certain canned quality. They were recitations that varied little in content or format. In these self-descriptions the
girls were rehearsing themselves in relation to the gured world of the
expected life path of the good Hindu woman. In daily life and in rituals
experienced in childhood and beyond, girls were presented with a variety of messages related to what was expected of them as women. Most
socialization contexts promoted the notion of a good woman, one who
follows the ideal life path as set out in Brahmanical writings and teachingsthe dominant ideology of gender identities, roles, and relationships in Naudada as in other Hindu areas of Nepal and India. The model
presumed that Hindu women, except in cases of bad luck or bad morals,
would follow a particular life path (see Bennett 1976, 1983; Caplan
1985; Das 1979; Skinner 1989, 1990; Stone 1978). The generic womans
roles in this gured world were family-centered, and the favored characteristics were those which supported the patriarchal family.
In the storyline envisioned in this world, a girl is rst a daughter and
a sister. She grows up surrounded by relatives who urge her to cultivate
the habits that will mark her as a good daughter, wife, and daughter-inlaw. From their teachings and scoldings she learns to be a hard worker,
obedient and respectful to elders, and shy, gentle, and virtuous. In the
next stage of the life path she is married soon after she reaches puberty.
With marriage she becomes a daughter-in-law, a wife, and eventually a

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

217

Ex

am

Co

py

mother. She is hard-working, faithful and devoted to her husband, obedient and helpful to her mother-in-law, and a respectable woman in the
community. With the birth of children, especially sons, she acquires a
valued identitywhat some have called the central or prototypical identity of women: that of mother (Stone 1978). A woman who bears sons
proves herself to be a good woman with a good fate. Finally, as a motherin-law, a position accorded higher status than other female roles in the
family, she can direct and control the lives of her daughters-in-law. At the
end of the life path, ideally, a woman never becomes a widow. A good and
virtuous woman, through ritual observance and daily practice, ensures
the health and long life of her husband and dies before him.
Women in Naudada were interpreted and evaluated against this
narrativized world with its ideal woman and her life course. Those who
deviated from the life path of the good woman were judged to be aberrant and problematic in some way. Women who withheld their labor
from domestic and agricultural tasks, who gossiped about their husbands shortcomings, committed adultery, contradicted their mothers-inlaw, did not give birth to sons, or were widowed were the subjects of
gossip. Good women labored for the benet of their households. Good
women never talked unnecessarily or wandered about to malinger, gossip, or irt.
Within this gured world, women who deviated from the life path did
so because they were intrinsically bad or the victims of bad luck. A
woman who did not have children, for example, was presumed to be
barren, and this condition was seen as a punishment for her misdeeds in
a previous life. Widows, too, had fallen off the expected life path. They
had the bad fate to outlive their husbands. Widows were to be pitied, but
they were sometimes feared as witches.
Talk about bad or sinful women was part of daily conversation, a
great deal of which included evaluations of self and others in relation to
this gured world. Devi, a Chetri woman in her late twenties, talking
with Skinner about women they both knew, explained that one was a
nathi (a irt or a woman who goes with men other than her husband).
Another was a papini (female sinner) because she did not feed her grandchildren while her daughter-in-law was working long days in the elds.
One of Devis neighbors was a bad woman (naramro aimai) because
she was very jealous of others and mistreated her father, who lived with
her. The many negative types that commonly gured in such conversa-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

218

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

tionsthe boksi (witch), radi (widowwith the connotation of bitch


and whore), aputri (barren woman), alacchini (a woman who brings bad
luck to her household), sande aimai (a woman who acts masculine, like a
bull)were all characters who had gone awry in the gured world of
good womens lives.
By the age of four or ve, and in some cases even younger, girls knew
the meaning of chori (daughter), sasu (mother-in-law), sauta (co-wife),
and other terms that designated kin roles and relations. By seven or
eight, they also knew words for women who had deviated or threatened
to deviate from the expected life path and could begin to elaborate the
meaning of these words in relation to this gured world (Skinner 1990):
they knew these types were considered bad, and they knew why they
were bad.
The appropriation of these terms began very early, especially in cases
where the words were used as insults. Even children of four or ve knew
that radi (widow) was not a good word to say. It was one of the most
common insults used for any female regardless of her age (and radiko
chora, or widows son, was often used as a term of abuse for males). It
was used often in scolding and cursing. While the younger girls knew
only that a radi was a bad thing to be called, the girls of eight or so knew
why a radi was bad. They knew it was bad for a woman to outlive her
husband. Likewise, from scoldings the younger girls knew only what
behaviors they were supposed to avoid, whereas the older girls had acquired a broader understanding of the proper comportment of daughters
in relation to the gured world. The older girls also talked frequently of
life as a daughter-in-law and wife and knew the codes of behavior they
would be expected to follow and the characteristics they would be expected to exhibit in these roles.
These evaluative terms for women indexed a moral universe, a gured
world, and served as cultural resources in the process of identity formation (Skinner 1989, 1990). Girls developed understandings of gender-related terms and the discourse of evaluation in specic sites and activities
such as everyday events of gossiping and scolding; and they used these
terms to identify themselves and others as particular kinds of moral
actors in the gured world of domestic relations in Naudada. They scrutinized their own and other womens behavior through the lens of this
discourse. They described themselves, both to themselves and others, in
ways that dened them as good daughters or good women, and they
attempted to avoid or dispute the negative labels.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

219

Ex

am

Co

py

This evaluative discourse created an inner voice of censure and divided women from one another. Aruna, a Damai girl of fourteen, reported that anyone, even a close girlfriend, would say a girl was bad if
she went to a boys house. Kamala, a fourteen-year-old Chetri girl, complained that she could not show her anger because others would say,
How angry that daughter seems to be. Tika, a Damai girl of fteen,
described herself as being on a straight line. Then she talked about a
woman who had run away with her husbands brother: It is better to
take a straight line than to do such a scandalous thing. A good wife stays
with her husband; a bad one leaves and goes with another. Kamala,
talking about the same event, said others were saying that this woman
was a kukurni (female dog) and had spoiled the prestige/honor (ijjat) of
her family.
It appears that these girls not only knew the expected life path and the
behaviors that counted as those of a good woman but also embraced
these concepts and behaviors as ways to dene themselves. They were
gured by this world, valuing themselves and others in its terms. They
desired to be and be seen by others as good selves. Their appreciation of
the gured world was not a product of its intrinsic persuasiveness, however. It was not so much that they were drawn to the image of the good
woman as that they were backing into it to avoid negative evaluations. For example, Kamala and her friends experienced many scoldings in which they heard evaluations of themselves and others. They
cared about what other people thought of them, and they did not wish to
dishonor their families. They tried to avoid behaviors that would categorize them as a bad type. In self-censoring their actions this way, they
backed into attachments to good and valued identities, and developed
claims to being good daughters and future daughters-in-law. Kamala said
she and her friends often heard women talking about what made a good
wife and daughter-in-law:
A good wife never criticizes her husband, and she never complains
about her husband to other people . . . A good daughter-in-law always
gets up early in the morning, and she does all the work she has to and
obeys her mother-in-law. She obeys her husband. She does not run
away to her natal home. She never steals to eat, and she does not steal
corn that grows in anothers eld. Daughters-in-law are not good if they
do not obey their mothers-in-law or if they move about here and there.
They do whatever they like. They steal, and they spend their time at
others houses. They go to festivals sometimes.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

220

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

Kamala thought that, if she had to get married, she would be a good
daughter-in-law, but she recognized that it would be very difcult: One
has to work very hard. We all have to act good. We are not supposed to
quarrel, lie, or steal. At home, we may not obey our parents all the time,
but we must obey at our mother-in-laws. A friend of Kamalas added
that women whose husbands beat them should just take it.
As portrayed so far, the Naudadan girls seem to have been totally
absorbed in all that the gured world promoted for women in their
domestic life path. The reproduction of patterns of gender relations practiced by previous generations seemed assured. From the comments we
have quoted, we might assume the girls accepted the life path and were
committed to its views of marriage and their roles as daughters-in-law
and wives, or, at least, that they acquiesced to their fate and silently
complied with the dominant ideology, as some authors have suggested
South Asian Hindu women do (see Dhruvarajan 1989). Their identities
as gured by this world seem to be their only views of self.
This picture of their self-understandings becomes more complex,
however, when we consider other things the girls said and did. Although
they laid claim to being good girls and good daughters and said that if
they married they would try to be good daughters-in-law and wives, they
also questioned the expected life path, were critical of the unequal treatment of men and women, expressed anger and sadness about the way
they were treated because they were female, and schemed to avoid marriage. They were gaining a sense of their position vis--vis others, noting
their relative lack of privilege and power. They were developing relational identities along with the gured identities.
Maiya, like many of the other girls, compared her life to that of her
brothers. In her rst interview, in response to the question Are you
happy to be a girl? Maiya replied that she would rather be a boy because
sons dont have to work hard. Girls have to carry water and cut grass,
while the sons can just sit and eat. Sunita, a ten-year-old Bahun, said she
had argued with her parents against the idea that only boys should go to
school. She told her parents, You are sending boys [to school], why not
send girls? Tika also complained: If I were a son, I would have been
allowed to study. Since I am a daughter, I did not get to study.
Muna, when asked what she thought about the future, replied: In the
future, now when I see others get married and have ghts in their new
home, um . . . why do we give daughters to anothers house? These

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

221

py

thoughts play in my mind. I think about it . . . If I am not able to study


and not able to work, um, if I am not treated well after I am married in
my new house. When I think that this may happen to me in the future, I
fear it and am very worried. In trying to express her sadness and sense
of injustice, Muna sang a song that included these lines:

Co

You are forced to leave your fathers house,


Stars oating in the sky . . .
The stream of tears has oated the stars in the sky.
Why do we have to wash the walls of others?
Mothers lap of love is empty.
Stars oating in the sky.

am

In her song Muna voiced her dread and sadness over having to leave her
natal home and the mother she loved. In her husbands home she would
be the servant of her in-laws and work for them. Munas singing in
response to a question about her life was not unusual. The girls frequently resorted to song to express some sense of their lives, often a
sense that reected a critical view of their position as women in a patriarchal society. A particularly strong example is a song sung by Naudadan
schoolgirls in 1986 at the annual cultural performance at the local high
school:

Ex

Being enslaved and repressed, dont remain in isolation.


We have to tear and throw out this web of exploitation.
If the husband keeps a co-wife, it is all right.
But if a woman takes another man, she must pay a price.
Even to eat, you have to ght hard.
Even to survive, you have to sell your very life.
Wake up, oh sister, and recognize this sinner man.
Lets unite to destroy this tradition.

Naudadan girls accounts, evaluations, narratives, and actions revealed images, understandings, and feelings about themselves that varied within and across contexts. The ways girls talked about themselves
and their lives and the ways they presented and represented themselves
were not consistent.4 Sometimes girls and women gave voice to culturally dominant notions of what the proper Hindu woman should be,
portraying themselves in terms of the ideal Hindu woman who accepts
her submissive and relatively powerless role vis--vis men. But they also,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

222

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

at least in some contexts, took another stance; they authored different


senses of themselves through an emotional commentary of sadness, anger, and resistance about the position that the gured world put them in
as women.
They were aware of the hardships and injustices that came with being
a good woman, and they gave extensive reective and critical commentaries, both in discourse and in song, on the relative lack of status
and privilege of that position. They denounced the treatment women
received at the hands of some men (and sometimes other women such as
mothers-in-law) and castigated the legal system that gave men inheritance and marital rights that women were not granted. The girls Skinner
interviewed were at times critical of the expected life path; they sometimes rejected it or tried to nd acceptable alternatives to it. In the
interviews all but two stated that they did not want to get married, and a
number had plansusually involving going to school and becoming a
teacher or ofce workerfor avoiding or delaying marriage.
The evaluative discourse used in everyday activities of Naudadan
lifein scolding and gossiping, in the telling of hardship talesgradually had been appropriated by the girls; at the same time they had inferred the gured world of the expected life path and womens domestic
lives (construed in these activities of everyday evaluation). Words such
as alacchini (a woman who brings bad luck to her family) and naramro
chori (bad daughter), and behaviors such as doing household chores and
avoiding being with boys evoked for them the gured world and the
meaning of being a good daughter, daughter-in-law, and wife as dened
within it.
But the words and behaviors also evoked memories of the actual episodes of scolding, gossiping, and hardship that the girls had experienced.
The expected life path that was symbolized in ritual and indexed in
everyday discourse was constituted as natural, as ahistorical and timeless. It was the path for the generic woman. Attention to the grounding
of this gured world in historically particular conditions and in the
various social spaces of Naudada was not invited. The girls had been
encouraged almost daily by their parents and others to develop a view of
themselves as traveling along the taken-for-granted life path and as accountable in that gured world. They had clearly developed such an
identication, one readily expressed in certain contexts, but they also
retained a denite sense of injustice. Girls often told of situations in
which they were shamed, hurt, angered. While developing an under-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

223

Ex

am

Co

py

standing of the meanings underlying the Brahmanical cultural model of


womens lives, the Naudadan girls were also developing a sense of the
hardships (dukha) associated with the actions required of them in this
gured world. How had they arrived at the point where they both embraced and rejected the expected life path?
Here Bakhtin and the sociohistorical school of psychology are helpful.
Its theory does not insist upon a coherent and integrated self. Instead it
guides us to look at the sites and practices in which identities and selves
are formed and to consider that the existence of a transcendent selfa
self that is independent of contentis contingent upon the existence of
cultural resources, such as the AA personal story (Chapter 4) or the
means of accounting oneself a good woman, for creating that self and
for the continuing social work that reminds one of ones self.5 It leads us
to specic spaces of authoring; it also recognizes what we observed in
Naudada. Children appropriated conventions of meaningto interpret
and evaluate their own behavior and that of others, but their sense of the
contexts or sites in which those more abstract meanings were being
distilled was distinct from those conventions. The girls seemed to have
developed a salient identication with the gured world of womens
lives: a desire to be a good girl, a good daughter, a good woman.
Yet they were also developing a sense of gender as entitlement, a knowledge of their gendered position and how it limited their access to certain
spaces, activities, and resources.
Naudadan girls distilled the common, public meanings associated
with specic sites and activities and identied with them, (re)producing
subjectications of these gured worlds in inner speech. At the same
time, they were developing embodied senses associated with their positionality in these sites and activities. In the realm of inner speech, these
different senses of self intersected and, at times, conicted. From this
internal and heteroglossic dialogical process, girls developed both good
selves and oppositional, angry selves.

Sites and Activities of Identity Formation


In Naudada there were a number of activities in which gender-marked
terms and gender-specic rules were expressed: scoldings that children
overheard, received, or gave; gossip sessions that they overheard and
sometimes contributed to; exchanges of complaints and worries about
ones life, often in the form of dukha stories (stories of hardships and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

224

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

suffering); domestic scenes in which girls observed and heard womens


problems; the telling of their worries for the future; the singing of songs
about womens lives; and the conduct of rituals that marked girls transition from one status to another (see Skinner 1989). Most of these activities were fairly common, occurring almost daily, and they, more than the
less frequent rituals, were what the girls talked about in the interviews.6
We will discuss three activities: scolding, domestic scenes, and the annual Tij festival.

Co

Scolding Activities

Ex

am

Scolding and cursing others were ubiquitous, everyday activities. Children were the frequent targets of scolding for transgressions and for
problems they caused their parents, other adults, and one another. Such
scolding generally began with an insult. Radione term for widow
was a common insult to hurl at any female, even a toddler. Nathi, loose
woman, was another. Radi! the scolder would scream. Then the girl
would be told the rules that she had violated. A typical scolding might
run as follows: Radi! What a daughter you are! After we have given you
food, this is all the work you have done! What have you done the whole
day? Why are you sitting there when the cattle need fodder and theres
no water in the house? Did I raise a lazy daughter?
Aruna, a Damai girl of thirteen, said her mother always scolded her,
saying radi when she did not do her work.7 Kamala, a Chetri girl of
fourteen, related that mothers sometimes called their daughters nathi,
scolding them, saying, Dont walk with boys this way. Muna said: I
feel very sad . . . when my father and mother scold me when I am not
doing any work, and get angry with me, when they say something and
scold, I feel very sad and unhappy. I feel sad when they scold my friends.
The incident that opens Chapter 6 presents a dramatic example of
scolding and its effects. To recapitulate, Skinner was recording songs one
day when three girls who were visiting her ran afoul of Tila Kumari, the
Bahun woman who lived in the house above hers. Tila Kumari walked
down to Skinners porch and yelled at the three Damai girls for eating
fruit and cutting fodder from her property:
You stupid girls. You are joking with us. Why do you cut grass from our
elds? Are you not ashamed? . . . Everyone has seen the guavas here,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

225

py

but only you have eyes for them . . . Go to your husbands or fathers
eld to eat. You have no fear to cut the grass in anothers eld. After
telling your parents, I will weed your hair. You have no shame to show
those teeth of yours. I will take your molar teeth out. I will complain
like the daughter of Hari. Do not do unnecessary things here.

Ex

am

Co

The girls were upset and angry, particularly since Tila Kumari also went
to their houses and berated them and their families. They brought up the
scolding in a number of later interviews. As quoted earlier, Muna said
this about the scolding: We hadnt done anything. I cant gure out why
they made such a scandal out of it . . . I felt like crying. I couldnt
retaliate. I do not do such things. I thought, who was she to tell someone
like my mother such things? We did not get angry because we had not
done anything wrong. Thats why I felt bad and wanted to cry.
Evidently Muna heard implications about behavior she should avoid,
but she also heard and felt other things. For her, the context of the
knowledge was highly charged. If we can judge by her interviews, Muna
thought of the episode not only in terms of the contentthe dos and
donts of how to actbut also in terms of her own feelings about the
scolding. Muna recounted her process of reacting to the scolding. She
rehearsed seeing and hearing Tila Kumari, and she composed arguments
to defend her actions. She described feelings of pain, sadness, and a sense
of being unjustly accused. Tila Kumari became a dialogic other, envisioned and heard as a particular person, but also perhaps as a representative of the higher-caste Bahuns, who sometimes accused and insulted the lower castes.8 By replaying this event Muna was forming a
notion of her liability to the public meaning of certain acts, but at the
same time she was developing a sense of mistreatment and a sense of her
positioning as a lower-caste woman by a higher-caste woman.
Another example of the distinction between a developing knowledge
of the meaning of acts in the gured world and a personalized sense of
ones own positional identity can be seen in the way girls attained an
understanding of radi. Girls heard mothers-in-law and husbands call
daughters-in-law and wives radi for a variety of perceived offenses. Often
the girls themselves were the targets of scoldings in which radi was the
insult or epithet. Over time they abstracted the common meaning of the
term and came to know that a radi was a deviant type, one who had left
the life path and failed to be a good woman. Radi evoked and was under-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

226

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

stood in relation to the Brahmanical cultural world of the expected life


path. But the girls also embodied personal senses of the word that were
more contextualized.9 When they heard the term radi they remembered
lived situations in which they were scolded unjustly by parents, humiliated by boys, screamed at by Tila Kumari, or saddened by the taunts and
insults of strangers.
Scolding involved the expression of generic concepts, meanings, and
evaluations commonly applied to womens behavior in Naudada. The
activity of scolding was a potential source not only of the public meanings that the girls were appropriating to talk about their own and other
womens behaviors in Naudada but also of their embodied sense of the
treatment they received in the positions afforded women. On the one
hand, they were developing an understanding of themselves as participants in a narrativized, gured world organized around the life trajectory
of a good woman. They were developing the disciplines and characteristics that could earn them praise for being good daughtersdisciplines and characteristics naturalized to stand outside of history, outside of time, and in isolation from caste or other local social groupings
in Naudada. On the other hand, they retained vivid memories of abrupt
encounters with these expectationsmemories of the particular people
who scolded them, of their feelings of shame and anger, of bodily sensations associated with their sense of having been unjustly accused. Where
their parents were involved, they retained a question of how their
mother or father could treat them like this: Why am I expected to do all
of this work and my brother is not? Thus subsequent reminders could
evoke for them both the cultural meaning and a sense of injustice, anger,
or other emotions not accounted for in the gured world of the good
Hindu woman.

Ex

Domestic Scenes and Their Telling

Girls also witnessed scenes from everyday life that centered about gender
and kin relations. They were admonished as they went about their
household and agricultural chores to be good, obedient, hardworking,
and honorable daughters. At the same time, as daughters in the natal
home, they were given fewer rights and privileges than their brothers;
they saw their brothers getting more and better food and clothes, and
they often had to work in the elds while their brothers went to school.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

227

Ex

am

Co

py

They were teased by their parents and others about being married away
beyond nine mountains, while their brothers, they knew, would stay at
home. They observed cases in which parents were in such a rush to
marry off their daughter that they chose a man who would cause her
many problems. They saw that an incoming daughter-in-law was expected to work long days at arduous tasks without complaint while the
mother-in-law sat idle or went out of her way to scold the newcomer.
They heard daily the expectations for wives: be respectful, virtuous,
and hardworking. The reward for all this servitude was supposed to be
the husbands affection and support for the wife and her children and the
wifes increasing status in the household. But girls knew of numerous
cases in the community in which women were abused or forsaken by
their husbands. They talked of marriage as an uncertain fate. A womans
husband might turn out to beat her and lose the family property through
drinking and gambling. If the wife scolded him, he might bring in a
co-wife, who would take away not only his affection but also scarce
resources.
Devi, a Chetri woman, often told her life story to the girls of Naudada.
Devis husband once had a good job with a local development agency. He
owned land, a house, and other wealth. But he drank the locally brewed
liquor to excess and gambled away all of the money and property. Devi
was powerless to stop him. He sold not only his wifes gold jewelry but
her pots and pans as well. He would drink until late at night, then come
home and beat her. Finally he deserted her and had not been seen for
years. But Devi still told her story, recounting to attentive girls the times
she would hide with a baby at her breast in the terraces below her house
until her drunken and enraged husband fell asleep.
While bad treatment was not the norm in Naudada, a girl heard about
and witnessed enough of it to make her worry that her own future
might include similar hardships. Everyday exhortations and evaluations
of womens behavior directed her to accept her roles without complaint
and to strive to obtain the favor of kin in both her parents and her
husbands home, but her observations provided her with images of the
pain possibly awaiting her on this life path. The expected life path may
have given her one means of envisioning her life, but it did not address
all the ways she thought and felt about her position as a female.
The Naudadan girls had clearly developed an identication with the
ideal woman of the gured world of womens lives; yet they also retained

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

228

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Songs and Activities of the Tij Festival

py

senses from their experiences of what it meant to be a girl. These individual senses were gured publicly and objectied in critical commentary
that was produced and passed on by groups of women. This commentary,
as a cultural resource, provided an alternative meaning and a means of
both expressing and understanding ones gender identity.

am

Co

In Naudada and elsewhere in South Asia (see Raheja and Gold 1994;
Skinner et al. 1991, 1994), songs have been a primary way for women
to express their experiences and their awareness of their positioning
within the gured world of domestic life. The Tij festival is part of a
larger ritual complex called TijRishi Pancami, during which women
fast, do puja (worship), and ritually bathe to protect their husbands
health and life and to purify themselves from sins connected with menstruation (see Bennett 1983). What interests us here is not the ritual
complex but the songs that the women compose and perform for the
festival.10
Tij songs are a specialized genre of folksong that extends womens
critical commentary from inner speech and private conversations among
two or three women to a public setting where hundreds of people gather
to observe the women sing and dance. Tij songs portray and comment
upon the shared feelings of women in their sometimes problematic positions as wives, daughters, and daughters-in-law. A dukha (hardship) Tij
song composed and performed by Naudadan girls in 1991 resounds with
a daughters sense of the favored treatment her brothers receive:

Ex

[Daughter:]
I rose in the morning to pick owers,
But did not pick them because they were covered with dew.
Parents just keep the daughters to do work at home,
But not even a small piece of the courtyard is given [to
daughters].
[Parents:]
The small piece of courtyard is needed to dry the paddy,
Go, daughter, to your husbands house to get your property.
[Daughter:]
We have to go empty-handed [to our husbands home],
The brothers fence in their property.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

229

Brothers clothes are so many that they rot away in a box,


But when they have to give us a single cloth, tears come to their
eyes.

Co

py

These verses give voice to a daughters resentment that she has no


rights to inherit her parents property. Other Tij songs expressed girls
fear of marriage and the treatment they might receive at the hands of
their future husbands and in-laws, their criticism of their parents for
marrying them far away or into a poor or bad family, and their feelings
toward mothers-in-law who mistreat them.
Many songs were about the husband-wife relationship. Women sang
of their own hardships in marriage and those of their friends and sisters.
One song that moved Devi to tears recalled her own experience with a
drunken husband:

am

Whatever money he has, he runs to the bazaar,


He searches for a place to drink alcohol.
If he has ve or ten rupees, he goes to the bazaar.
He forgets his family and household and drinks alcohol.
Waiting in the kitchen the rice becomes cold,
Waiting for the husband the night is almost gone.
How much rotten alcohol is there in Thakalis hotel!11
The husband came with a stick at midnight.
If I say, Do not drink alcohol, he threatens to bring a co-wife.
The new wife will give [him wine] and pleasure,
And she will give orders, and bring the water to wash his feet.

Ex

A second type of Tij songs, political (rajniti) songs, became predominant in Naudada following the peoples movement against the one-party
panchayat system. Rajniti songs thoroughly dominated the Tij festival of
1991. In them girls and women criticized various political parties and
government ofcials, chronicled their abuse and exploitation of women
and the poor, and demanded equal rights for women and the poor. In
1986 Naudadan girls composed the following song verses, which represented a more explicit critical commentary and call for equal rights:
You [parents] compel the daughter to work hard.
How can we abolish this exploitation?
When the daughter is educated, they call her a prostitute.
They believe such matters.
Our society wont allow women to be educated.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

230

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

When we think of this, it makes us angry.


They not only select the husband,
And donate us to him like the gift of a cow.
We have no right to select a boy whom we like.
No money is in our hand to spend.
Even when we are sick, we women have to work.
They say we are lazy and trying to cheat them.
A minor mistake they blow out of proportion.
We women have to live like animals.
How long can we tolerate this kind of custom?
We must become determined to abolish this condition!

Co

In the 1991 Tij festival one group of singers took on the problem of
alcohol, urging men to stop drinking the local brew (raksi). They took an
active political stance, asserting that only with communism would liquor
cease to ow and women get equal rights and better treatment:

Ex

am

Our brothers of Ward 2,


It is our request that you dont drink raksi.
In Ward 2 of our village,
Dont drink raksi in this area.
Brothers, keep this in your mind,
Remove raksi and gambling from here.
Drunkards are treated like animals,
People give them grass to eat.
The dignity of our village is gone,
Youre not allowed to sell raksi in the shops.
If you do, youll not be allowed to keep a shop.
After drinking raksi, you people walk around talking nonsense.
The oxen [the drunkards] are used to plow the elds.
From now on, you stop playing cards and drinking.
Move forward to bring the revolution.
You wasted grain making raksi.
You killed the poor people.
Raksi was free in the panchayat period.
Star Beer was started in the Congress period.
Communism is needed to stop raksi,
We can get rights only when communism comes.

In Tij, womens experiential and personal senses of being female, developed in other contexts, were expressed publicly, cast into a critical,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

231

Ex

am

Co

py

cultural form. These songs provided a cultural resource and an alternative perspective on womens lives, an alternative way of understanding
the life path and oneself as female.12 Women who had experienced the
problems associated with being daughters, daughters-in-law, and wives
joined together in Tij to censure those who had created difculties for
them (the mother-in-law, husband, father) and, increasingly, to criticize the social and political system that allowed or brought about these
abuses.
In day-to-day situations Naudadan women were constrained in what
they could say and do. Because of power relations in Naudada, the
groups of women who composed Tij songs were a restricted group, circumscribed to meet within a brief period, once a year. But in the context
of Tij they came together and drew upon their individual experiences to
evoke, through song, strong, shared feelings. As individuals they had
little power to change others ideas, but as a collectivity, even for a brief
time, they could create new songs, develop alternative perspectives, and
express usually outlawed meanings of what it was to be female.
These critical Tij songs, composed and sung as long as the oldest
women could remember, acted as cultural resources to organize more
strident opposition to the patriarchal aspects of society in Nepal and
helped create a potentially resistant group. Rituals can overow their
boundaries (Bakhtin 1984b), and this happened in the Naudadan situation. The events chronicled at the beginning of this chapterthe
womens procession for equal rights and the posse that threatened
local distillers and wife-beaterswere the stuff of Tij songs. The understandings of womens lives developed through the critical commentary
voiced at the Tij festival were being pushed forward into more and more
public venues and were thus gaining signicance for a re-visioning of
selves and a reshaping of gured worlds.13
Naudadan girls participation in the sites and activities we have discussed gave rise to emotionally charged visions of and rehearsed dialogues about themselves as women. These selves were contradictory and
contested, selves that Bakhtin (1981, 1984a) would have called heteroglossic. The girls were developing a nuanced understanding of the
gured world of Hindu womens lives and an emotional attachment to
an identication of themselves in that world. The girls were also developing an embodied personal sense of themselves in their position as

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

232

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

female, as the victims of frequently unfair treatment. Though left unaccounted for by the public meanings associated with the expected life
path of Hindu women, these personal senses were of concern to the girls.
At the annual Tij festival, women expressed a critical commentary on
their position in the world of domestic relations of Naudada, and, in
1991, after the countrys rst multiparty election for many years, on the
mistreatment of women by the government. These Tij songs, especially
the ones addressed to relatives, echoed girls reactions to the scoldings
they were receiving and the domestic scenes they were witnessing. Unsurprisingly, some of the girls used these songs as a cultural resource to
express their sense of their lives.
This case study from Naudada reveals ways in which women not only
appropriated the identity afforded to them by the gured world but
also formed personal senses and, especially in practices related to the
Tij festival, personal perspectives that contested the expected life path.
Womens selves in this formulation were part of a dialogic process,
emerging in social interactions and sites where different voices coexisted,
and part of dynamic, changing social formations tied to competing interests in the sociohistorical context of their emergence. We will return to
this case in Chapter 12 to examine both the new world of gender relations created in Tij songs and the tentative acting out of that emerging
world by some of the womens groups.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Ex

am

Co

py

Making Worlds

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

11

Co

py

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds,


and Fantasy Resources

Ex

am

Specialized discourses and practices, each predicating a gured world


and each realized in a variety of positional identities, exist in all societiesthough they are especially profuse in large-scale societies. These
discourses and practices lead to a great diversity of options for, as well
as constraints on, peoples activity. Peoples lives take shape among the
identications, gured and relational, that are arranged within the space
of their activity. We have argued, following Vygotsky, that these social
forms, these discourses and practices, are not simply the context but the
content of inner life, albeit in some way transformed. The interpersonal
becomes the intrapersonal in a literal way; the forms of speaking and
interacting inhabit us to make inner speech and inner action. They
are the mediating devices of our thinking, feeling, and willing.
We have extended Bakhtins notion of voices, that is, of the social
identication and valuation of these cultural resources and artifacts. Dialects, genres, styles of dress, ways of holding and moving the bodyall
are socially inected forms that have both substantive and indexical
value, telling our social category, our relational position, and our group
afliations. These social forms and cultural resources become personal in
the arrangement or orchestration of these voices. Within this arrangement exists whatever reality there is to the self, as a collocation of social
persons (Harr 1984) that endures, at least for a time. Concurrently,
social interactivity becomes personied, so that its forms assimilate the
character of the people from and by whom they are reproduced.1 This
process itself is a kind of dynamic, uniting the intimate and the social
sites of cultural production, and it is within this double-sided context,
235
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

236

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Play Worlds and Artifacts of Fantasy

py

set within a larger historical and institutional context, that new gured
worlds and new identitiesboth gured and positionalemerge.

Ex

am

Co

Play occupies a central place in Vygotskys theories; it constitutes ontogenetically the completion within the organism of the capacity for symbolic mediation (Vygotsky 1978, 1987a). All games are jeux desprit. In
play, more than in other human activity, aspects of the natural world, and
by extension, of social worlds that have come to seem natural, are suspended and made subject to pretense. Play is the form of activity that
proceeds in ignorance of any constitutive condition other than a cultural
and conventional design. It draws upon recognized genres of speech and
activity, but it takes the player beyond the immediate setting. Play happens through the world in which it is observably set. Its real setting is
imaginary; it answers only to a gured world. Each generation, however,
comes to play historically and socially; one learns the bounds of play, the
scaffolding, from ones predecessors and fellows and passes them on to
ones successors. It cannot be otherwise for Vygotsky, for it is play that,
by its use of collectively produced artifacts, realizes what is only a species
competence. Each of us learns to inhabit the cultural world through play
when it is instituted within us. The social practices of acting otherwise
become the grounds for our thinking otherwise. The mastery we gain
over our play is mastery over our imagination (see Lachicotte 1992).
Play is also the medium of mastery, indeed of creation, of ourselves as
human actors. Without the capacity to formulate other social scenes in
imagination, there can be little force to a sense of self, little agency. In
play we experiment with the force of our acting otherwise, of our projectivity rather than our objectivity. And through play imagination becomes
embodied, proprioception as much as conception, experienced in activity, moved as much as mapped. Through play our fancied selves become
material.
It is the opening out of thought within the activity of play, what we
might call the cultural production of virtualities, that allows for the
emergence of new gured worlds, of regured worlds that come eventually to reshape selves and lives in all seriousness. Vygotsky and Bakhtin
both attended to this process, but Bakhtin was especially alert to its
place in larger social structures.2 He showed that genres and other forms

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

237

Ex

am

Co

py

of specialized practice, informal and formal, come to inhabit ones speech


and thought on an everyday basis (Voloshinov 1986; Bakhtin 1981,
1984a). Within the relation of interplay that denes and partially suspends specic activity from the everyday (what Bakhtin called behavioral ideology), people are subject to the regulation, which we call the
guring, that governs the practice. They learn to act as roles in play; they
are inhabited by the discourse so that, as Foucault demonstrated, the
instituted relations of the practice are disseminated within. This inhabitation is the hallmark of Foucaults disciplines (1979), of Habermass
colonization of the life-world (1984). But it is only half the story.
The work of Foucault, Habermas, and other contemporary students of
agency and subjectivity can be reconceived as the development of Webers (1978) category of traditional or habitual action.3 We nd the habitual a useful touchstone for understanding what these social theorists
have contributed and so, too, what they have missed. Habit or habituation forcefully communicates the bodily aspect of activity and thinking,
yet it overlooks our ability to fantasize, to envision other worlds, to
create other worlds by recombining elements from those we know. In
Chapter 6 we characterized positional identities as more or less conscious, more or less habitual, moving sometimes out of awareness, toward fossilization, and at other times toward consciousness and susceptibility to manipulation. As we see it, play, especially socially organized
play, works in both directions of this process. Were this not the case,
were inhabitation the only means of reproducing practices and practitioners, then we would have Webers iron cage not only around us but
within us.
Bakhtin had another word to say, which he took, most appropriately,
from the author of the word made esh, Franois Rabelais. He directed
attention to the ways play frees people from the generic forms that govern their actions. This moment of freeing is at the heart of his famous
notion of the carnival, or better of the process itself, carnivalization
(Bakhtin 1984b).4 From its very abstraction and mimicry, which make
representation itself thematic and thus ironize everyday usage, play
moves these identities and the gured worlds in which they are embedded closer to consciousness. Only in its second moment, through the
continued rehearsal of its activities, does play that has been popularized
move, with its gures and their contexts, toward habituation.
We can use Bakhtins notion of dialogism (discussed in Chapter 8) to

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

238

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

understand how carnivalization becomes a real force. Dialogism pictures


social and cultural activity as a manifold phenomenon, of a variety
shaped by the juxtaposition of incommensurate voices not only within
but also between gured worlds. Bakhtins dialogism sets everyday social
life in what might be called an inter-world. This postmodernesque
phrase describes the condition of a sociality whose generality derives
from the convention of many gured worlds. (The model of a language,
composed within its dialects and other sociolects, is the one Bakhtin
most often used to represent this condition.) In our everyday lives we
encounter and enter into many specialized practices, and these practices exist in various degrees of interrelation. Activity predicated upon a
gured world is never quite single, never quite pure. It is dialogized,
gured against other possible positions, other possible worlds. Our habitual identities bump up against one another. And this is just Bakhtins
point. The space of freedom that is the space of play between these
vocations is the space of the author.
Each person constitutes a potential convocation of numerous practitioners, that is, each of us has a repertory. In the heart of inner speech,
according to Bakhtin, these socially identied, vocal images are orchestrated. They are played out. It is not imaginatively necessary to accommodate all voices at any given time. Under most situations ordinary
relations of authority prevail and establish that one (or several) vocations arrest the dialogue. There are situations, however, whether they be
scenes of practice or scenes of consciousness, in which conventional
relations are disturbed. Other sources of social valuation abide where
realms of interactiongames, arts, ritualsare established in the partial
suspension of the ordinary course of events.
These arenas of play, the power of carnival, have much to do with
social experimentation as well as social reproduction (see Bakhtin
1984b; Vygotsky 1978, 1987b; Huizinga 1955, 1956). Here people create
new orchestrations from the play of inner speaking and seek to convert
them interactively to new imagined practices, new virtualities. If inhabitation is the one moment of social reproduction when one gured world,
one set of active identities, comes to dominate us as authors, then dishabitation is its complement. We unlearn bodily in the remove from
dominant to emerging world, so that we return to the everyday, perhaps,
with an altered subjectivity, an altered sense of who we are. The art of
play has a spectrum of effects: new genres are created and recorded in the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

239

Co

py

durable media, old ones are regured, and new worlds and new identities
are created.
Or it can have such effects. It is the irony of play that its freedom is as
contained as its discipline. Both depend upon the authority and power
which establish the possibility that is also the boundary, the space of
possibilities, that holds play apart from ordinary life. It is no automatic
thing, either the inscription or the dissemination, either discipline or
novelization (as Bakhtin would have it). We must turn once again to the
life of communities, to their institutional and historical coproduction
with the persons who populate them and the cultural forms that express
themto understand both how play worlds are realized and how real
worlds are played out. The media through which new worlds are envisioned have no life of their own, and the people have no own to their
life apart from one another. A particular historical casethe medieval genre and practice called courtly lovewill illustrate the large social
movements that create new gurations of human life. It will show how
a play world may be taken, ultimately and profoundly, into the real
world, to shape the sites of everyday activity.

Courtly Love

Ex

am

In the area of southern France that was called Languedoc, or more


broadly the Occitans, there arose during the eleventh century a new form
of poetry and of entertainment.5 It was fostered by the upper nobility and
practiced by them, their retainers, and their courtiers. The poets were
called the troubadoursafter one name for their verbal arts, the trobar
and the entertainment was known, if it had any one name, as n amors.
The genre has been intensively explored by medievalists, and its status is
still intensely debated (Newman 1968; Boase 1977; Kay 1990; Bloch
1991). Its reality, as a way of behavior and even as a literary phenomenon, has been questioned (Robertson 1962, 1968; Donaldson 1965; Benton 1968). The balance of opinion today holds that courtly love (as it has
come to be known) was indeed a specic form of both artistic performance and courtly behavior (Frappier 1972; Boase 1977; Paterson 1993).
It was an activity of social life in the courts whose participants included
the poets, other members of the retinue, and the principals, lord and
lady. Indeed, there was seemingly little separation between performers
and audience, for the poet could be a court follower, or the lord him-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

240

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

selfthe rst well-known troubadour was Guilhem IX, Duke of Aquitaineor even the lady (Paden 1989). Courtly love was a convivial
pastime, a genre of fanciful conversation and play-acting, or at least of
diversion (Huizinga 1956; Stevens 1973).
Much of the skepticism directed toward courtly love as a social phenomenon with real effect derives from its pretense, its status as a kind of
game (Howard 1966) played by the powerful after their serious work of
oppression had concluded for the day. What difference could such afterhours ephemera make in the materially hard and often brutal life of those
desperate ages? Our answer reveals much about the processes by which
new gured worlds emerge and the kinds of effects their emergence
brings. Cultural historians have traced the slow dissemination and transformation of courtly love (see Rougemont 1956; Smith and Snow 1980),
which amounts to a domestication, from medieval court practice into the
ordinary world of contemporary romance, where it is part of the lives
of, for example, the college women discussed in Chapters 5 and 7. We
live today a gured world of heterosexuality that owes much of its interpretation to the casual spectacles of old courtship.
The scene of courtly love is a familiar one. The principal characters
were two: a lover and his beloved.6 The lovers inhabited their own special world, apart from the social realities that ordinarily dened them. In
some versions this displacement was taken to extremes, as in the cave
where Tristan lived with Iseult, free in their fantastic and solitary absorption with each other. All acts within this exhaustively meaningful world
carried emotional charge. Each lover addressed and focused upon the
other. Predisposition and proaction, the movements of the passions and
the essence of emotion, united them. So much is romantic love, then and
now, the gure of rapture.
To make this supercharged fantasy manifest, obstacles to love were
taken as the very precondition of it. Only by overt movement, the deeds
achieved to overcome impediments, could love be securely signied. The
private world had to become articulate; talk was the necessary service of
adoration. Yet talk was also cheap, indeed speech was seen as the very
bed of falsehood. The false lover and the talebearer, the maledictor, were
the greatest villains in the gured world of courtly love.7 Love had to be
proved in ways that surpassed speech. Hence great travailthe bodys
sacrice and truth, the constant striving of lover to lover, their continuous attractionproved and sanctied the bond. In the play world that

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

241

Ex

am

Co

py

was courtly love, this rule held: the greater the effort, the more dire the
obstacle, the nearer to surety and revealed truthand the greater the
reward. It was preeminently the part of the beloved, the lady, to set
obstacles of her own, though there would be plenty set by the world
outside, because intimacy with her was the reward given.
Inevitably romantic love was also about prestige. Each lover was the
token of the other. But all this exertion was not entirely other-directed.
From the accomplishments that proved love also accrued the authority
that enforced belief. Each lover remade his or her life in front of the
audience and was valued accordingly, for courage, for sincerity, for that
gift of self which required return, and above all for competence. All
other things being equal, the greater ones prestige, the greater were ones
chances of attaining a tting lover.
But that statement must be qualied. For one thing, love was about
gaining elevation through learning and instruction. The woman employed her desirability toward this goal, that her lover become worthy of
her. Here again is a reason for all those trials of love, dragons slain,
competitors bested. The man did not simply instance an already worthy
character. Rather he acquired that character, spurred by his love, informed by his lovers care and cleverness, and tempered through action.
In that achievement the beloved gained her glory. How much more telling was the skill that created worth than the lesser wisdom that only
recognized it. Thus the beloved, for all her seeming passivity, had the
power in courtly love. She was exalted and served, yet that service was
only the realization of her subtle action and the proof of her love for her
courtier.
Courtly love turned the public world of medieval Occitania outside in,
as does modern romantic love the contemporary world of work. Worth
and power subsisted there in the attractions and the stations of the
private, after-hours court, not in the acts and ofces of the days work.
But what sort of vision was this inverted world? What relation did the
play world of courtly love bear to the political-economic, social, and
cultural forms of life in the Occitans of the eleventh and twelfth centuries, or in the western medieval communities that later became acquainted with the traditions of courtly love? Historical research (Duby
1983, 1994; Jaeger 1985) suggests that the scene fantasized through the
artifacts of courtly lovethe poetry, performances, contests, and ceremonieshad an ambivalent relation to the real worlds of its partici-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

242

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

pants. For instance, many writers have remarked that the relation of the
male lover to the female beloved inected and therefore satirized the
relation of servant (courtier) to lord. It has been read both ways, as
elevating the beloved to a kind of potentate and as derogating the lordly
power by giving it feminine form.
The beloved was called domna, literally a feminized form of lord (domnus), a title that signied a kind of political power, a sovereignty over
those pledged to her. This fact has led several scholars (for example,
Jackson 1960) to propose that courtly love was fostered, if not created,
by the powerful women who sometimes ruled lands in the Occitans.
Eleanor of Aquitaine, the granddaughter of Guilhem IX and wife of
Henry Plantagenet, the Angevin king of England, is the prototypical
case: several writers have suggested that her courts at Poitiers were havens for troubadours and home to the entertainments called courts of
love.8 Although there is little evidence that Eleanor actively promoted
courtly lovethere were other powerful, though less grand, domnae
whose sponsorship is better attested (see Paterson 1993). Nonetheless,
most of the supporters of courtly love were men, and it was their wives
(as much as any other actual beloved) who were being addressed in the
poems. What are we to make of their support?
The support of the male nobility was essential, and not simply for the
place and sustenance they gave the poets. Courtly love was a double
profanation, rst of feudal forms of authority and second of the religious
teachings on relations between men and women. Its elevation of women
to higher position, even if in play, made powerful enemies among the
clergy, and therefore needed, all the more, the protection of power.
In the Occitans the cultural elements that shaped the generic forms
of the courtly artsMoorish lyric poetry; erotic Latin poetry along the
model of Ovid; the secular poetry of peripatetic monks, ex-religious
and peri-religious court followers, clerks; and perhaps other elements
as wellwere brought together with social conditions that allowed them
to ourish.9 There existed in the Occitans, rst, a feudalism founded
upon alliance between lords and fee-for-service between lord and retinue, which was less rigid and hierarchical than a vassalage system
founded upon the control of land and labor bound to land; second, a
polity where provincial magnates played off the contending interests of
great dynastskings of France, England, Castile, and Aragon, the German emperorto build their own substantial realms; third, a conten-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

243

am

Co

py

tious and diverse, sometimes heretical, religious life and establishment;


and fourth, a relatively open and egalitarian tradition of gender relations,
at least among the nobility and perhaps among the bourgeois as well
(Paterson 1993). These political and social conditions became motive,
means, and occasion to create the new practice.
It was in the interest of the Occitanian nobility to recruit people to
their retinue and ofces, and, for that purpose, to foster the arts that both
provided the enticements and accrued the courtly prestige that attracted
followers and allies. The troubadours spread praise of the lords largess
as widely as praise of their ladys beauty. They echoed a double entreaty
and a hope of paired reward, material and intimate, that bound the
household together. In this regard, though the lord acted more by his
license and even his absence, he was as much a principal of courtly
love as his lady. Conversely, the lady, though she often stayed in the
domestic sphere, became a full partner in a realized, intimate polity: the
household as court of power and affect (see Duby 1994; Paterson 1993).
It is small wonder that the poems seem to address each patron in the
guise of the other.
Courtly love, then, was rst a game of etiquette, and also a discipline
of politicized personal relations. It dramatized for its devotees (members of the retinue and allied lesser nobility) the hope of advancement
through service, and for its patrons (the higher nobility) the means of
delity and allegiance. Its rehearsal established the common dispositions, the inner activity proper to a house of its kind.

The Publicization of Courtly Love

Ex

If proponents of courtly love used it as a kind of propaganda, to spread


news of the pleasures and rewards available to those who would support them, they faced strong opposition to and even co-optation of these
means of political persuasion. This counteraction came from many
fronts. The French, adherents of the Capetian kings whose land bordered Occitania to the north, ridiculed the effete character of this courtly
diversion and boasted of their own prowess in arms and of the code of
chivalry told in the great Carolingian epics. They were forced to share
their inheritance of that imperial glory with the Hohenstaufen rulers in
Germany, who now held the imperial seat of Rome, the so-called Holy
Roman Empire. The Angevins, who under Henry II held not only the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

244

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

crown of England but, through his marriage to Eleanor, the Duchy of


Aquitaine itself, adopted the Arthurian heritage for their own. The kings
of Castile and Aragon laid claims to the deeds of the Reconquista, the
long struggle against the Moors, and these Spanish kingdoms shared
much economic, linguistic and demographic interchange with the Occitans. Indeed it is hard to separate the two regions, especially at the
Catalonian and Pyrenean boundaries. All these powers were active in
Occitania, seeking dominion or hegemony in alliance. And all were actively moving against the others toward that end.
Perhaps for this reason, though not for it alone, their respective court
apparatuses began to adapt courtly love to their own purposes (Heer
1963). In the twelfth-century Arthurian cycles, especially the tales of
Chrtien de Troyes, the precepts of the courtly lover reached in the gure
of Lancelot a union with the peerless man of arms. So potent did the
Arthurian tradition become, reworked by the new courtliness, and so
capacious did its imagined realm of Arthur become, that French and
Imperial courts too discovered their own part in it. Of the great thirteenth-century Arthurian romances, those in prose are mainly of French
sources, and the epics, Parzival and Tristan, are German. These works
realized the conjunction of the courtier and the warrior that was the
idiom of later chivalry (see Painter 1967; Duby 1983; Jaeger 1985). Even
the Spanish works took on this chivalrous manner, though they remained involved in the Reconquista, not in Arthur.
Oddly, the Occitanian world remained unmoved by the new chivalry.
Its vision of the court remained more urbane and everyday, almost academic in its devotion to the arts. There persisted a lyric tradition of
courtly love, appropriate to the quiet times at court, to an everyday
evenings entertainment. It too spread out from Occitania, sometimes
with troubadours attached to the retinues of heiresses who married foreign powers. Two daughters of Eleanor of Aquitaine exemplify this
means of transplantation. One, Eleanor (Leonora), married Alfonso VIII,
king of Castile, and their court hosted many troubadours. The custom
was already established in Castile, though that land was at the edge of the
troubadours ambit (Paterson 1993). With the advent of Leonora the
Castilean court became a much more popular stop on the circuit traveled
by poets who were not attached to a single patron. The second, Marie,
married the French Count of Champagne and, on his early death,
attained some power in that rich county. Her court at Troyes (where

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

245

Ex

am

Co

py

Chrtien wrote) became the most famous center of courtly love in the
north.
The north of Europe remade and learned courtliness in the warlike
dress of imperial dreams. Of foreign lands, Italy best absorbed the amusements and manners of courtly love. What is now the west of Italy, along
the Provence/Savoy border, was then the eastern reach of Occitanian
society and culture. The trobar was well known there from the early
twelfth century (Paterson 1993), adopted by a nobility that was to become ever more urban and ever more commercial. In the following centuries the great poets of the Lombard coast and plain, Dante, Petrarch,
and Boccaccio, were sophisticates, and sometimes critics and satirists, of
the art of love. Still, they were its heirs and their works, vehicles of its
devices. It was a woman transplanted from this urbane Italy to the courts
of Paris, Christine de Pizan, who would at the end of the fourteenth
century present a new model of courtly behavior, a modied etiquette for
gender relations, that sought to counter a misuse of courtly love (see
McLeod 1976; Willard 1984).
During its adaptation and dissemination beyond Occitania to the
north, especially in its marriage to chivalry, courtliness took on a decidedly masculine bent. Even in its earliest forms courtly love was produced
largely by men, and it had only one female character of note. But that
character was central, her token (the woman addressed) was near at
hand, and the image of her power differed greatly from the customary lot
of even the noblest women found in other contemporary ideologies (Paterson 1993). However, as the domestic court of the troubadours was
transformed, replaced by the grand courts of kingdom and legend, the
presence of the domna faded. Her customage became less important, and
her station, not her person, became the thing. Epics have small place for
idiosyncrasy. One consequence of courtly love, or rather its transmuted
appropriation, is the woman on the pedestal, the woman whose only
realization lies in the activity of her lover (Kay 1990; Bloch 1991). Another is her opposite, the gure of lechery known from later and cynical
romances. Ironically, this veneration of the abstract belovedwho thus
displaced Jesus or Mary as object of worshipoffended the church as
much as the taste for pleasures of the body (Denomy 1947).
To be sure, the church had opposed courtly love from its inception, for
its vanity and frivolousness, its celebration of worldly merit, and its
parody of male and religious authority. Given the often strained relations

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

246

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

between church and nobility, clerical condemnation may have only


added to the attractiveness of this most secular of doctrines. The teachings of love were countered by clerical treatises (Jaeger 1985), to little
effect. Churchmen, who were the lettered people of the day, even played
a role in memorializing and disseminating the troubadours folderol.
Andreas Capellanus, author of the treatise De Arte Honeste Amandi, was
reputedly a chaplain at Maries court of Troyes.10
Only with the crusades against the Cathars (1209), known as the
Albigensian crusades, was the church able to move against the troubadours and their patrons and followers in Occitania. The crusaders and
their propagandists claimed that the worldly, frivolous, and blasphemous
pursuits of love reected, and brought the innocent to acceptance of, a
heresy they painted as lost to the devil and the world of the esh. That
portrait was hardly true of Catharism, or of courtly love. The heresy was
actually world-renouncing, and its effects on courtly love, if negligible,
were to make it more ideal (Rougemont 1956; Boase 1977). Catharist
love would replace the domna with a perfect (the Catharist ascetic), a
woman to adore, certainly, but one denitively removed from worldly
pleasures and the promise of worldly reward. Yet that replacement too
was blasphemous, and only condemned the practice more deeply.
The crusades were an effective campaign, if one (intentionally) erroneous in its interpretation of Occitanian culture. Occitanian courts and
sovereignty were terribly disrupted, lands were usurped by the crusaders (mostly northern French), and the inquisition was established. A
living for the troubadours became harder to procure. Poetry became
more ideal, more orthodox, or at least more obscure (Boase 1977).11
Occitanian courtliness, as a living, not literate, practice, necessarily
changed. The play of n amors ended.
The death of a lived worldan embodied, that is, socially enacted way
of realizing gured worlds in practiceis not necessarily the end of the
gured worlds that developed within it. Court life in Occitania was
irreparably disrupted during the crusades. Courtly love moved on, becoming a literary project or a performance to be attended, ever more a
spectacle and ever less a mode of conversation and conviviality. It became the stuff of manuals and treatises, manuscripts and later books.
Considering English only, it was part of Chaucer, Gower, Malory, and
Spenser. It was played out in the Forest of Arden and on the streets of
Shakespearean Italy. Jacobean, Restoration, Augustan, and Georgian; ro-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

247

Imagined Communities

Co

py

mance, lyric, epic, and epistolarythe image of love changing somewhat, but always remaining, whatever the tradition or form, the service
of a courtier to win the favor of a lady. The Victorians, aristocracy and
middle classes, were fascinated by it, by the cult of Arthurian chivalry,
by a medievalism in which courtliness was the nostalgic centerpiece.
Today the gures of the troubadours live in high literature and the most
mundane and banal romances, in soap operas and grand opera, in advertisements, and in the talk and dispositions of college students. Courtly
love, albeit in modied form, has been absorbed into the contemporary
gured world of romance. It participates in a lived world far removed
from the noble households of High Medieval Occitania.

Ex

am

Benedict Anderson, in his book Imagined Communities (1983), provides


a second example of the development and proliferation of a gured
worldthat of modern nationalism. He emphasizes not so much the
gured world of nationalism as the imagined community created in
relation to that world. His work has important implications for the possibilities for positional as well as gured identities that are dependent
upon the mass circulation of cultural resources.
By an imagined community Anderson means a potent and effective
sense of commonality, of membership in a categorical social bodya
social body that exists despite the absence of direct or even indirect
social intercourse among its (putative) members. Such an imagined community is developed and continued through common participation in
activities that gure for people their identication with others who also,
elsewhere or nearby, perform similar acts. The sense of abstract community is acquired and maintained through the use of common cultural
artifacts that have acquired indexical value. Anderson is clearly not talking about actual cooperation or conjoint work, where people are involved in a face-to-face community of practice. Rather he is talking about
solitary or modular activities, which most individuals or small groups
perform separately.
Andersons classic example is reading the newspaper, and the lesson of
it is less what happens during the activity than the notion of a readership
that accrues from it. The common information, convened in the single
medium whose use becomes emblematic, creates for those for whom it

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

248

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

has become a marker an index of relational identity, a sort of kinship


with others imagined to be reading the same stories in the same paper in
similar places. Particular newspapers, the New York Times, say, or the
Nation, come to realize a theory of (and thus to signify) the persons who
read them. And so is formed, of this common perception, an imagined
community. The media mark, in their users minds, those who use them
with a particular relational identity.
Anderson is not particularly concerned with the gured world of nationalism. He speaks mostly of the meta-effect of a manufactured and
generic (and to that extent formless) sense of community. However, the
media do carry specic instructions, scripts, or guidelines for the enactment of the various gures of a specic nationalist world: they provide
narratives for potential adherents. Many of the stock characters of these
nationalist dramas are the stuff of school primers and songs, folktales of
the state-to-be. Though the writings of the more serious and quotidian
media, newspapers and journals, make mostly ironic or condescending
reference to this mythos, still it is the touchstone, the root knowledge of
political culture. (The greater the sense of a trial of state, during periods
of war or political dissension, the closer will the quotidian prescript
approach the mythic model.)
American versions of these narratives include revolutionary heroes
(Washington, Marion), lesser but exemplary martyrs (Nathan Hale) and
patriots (Paul Revere, Patrick Henry), revered statesmen (Franklin, Jefferson, Madison), followers whose devotion gives them a collective grandeur (the soldiers at Valley Forge, the Minutemen), and worthy allies
(Lafayette, Grasse). In opposition to these are the traitors (Benedict Arnold), the loyalists caught in blind servitude to colonial masters, the
colonizers themselves (King George, Pitt), and the occupying troops
(Cornwallis and so on) and foreign minions (the Hessians), who inevitably commit brutal acts of oppression. There are the activities performed
by these iconographs, repeated locally and largely: the Tea Parties and
Midnight Rides, the Resolves and Declarations, Crossings and Camps.
These are the origin myths; they are followed by stories of the new
countrys institution, its early history and trials of statehood and its
ultimate salvation by second-, third-, and later-generation patriots and
citizens.
Alongside these political gures, and sometimes in the same character
(such as Franklin), exist culture heroes, who emblematize the popu-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

249

Ex

am

Co

py

lace as a nation, an ethnos distinct from (though attached to) the governing apparatus. American mythohistory is also replete with such gures,
heroes and antiheroes, from founding fathers to latter-day personages
gured according to such narrativesSacco and Vanzetti, Rosa Luxemburg, the suspected Communists pursued by McCarthy in the 1950s,
and, as contemporary conservatives might have it, liberals and other
advocates of political correctness in todays politics. All combine to
provide the forms for emulation or rejection, so that an action, such as
voting for a particular candidate or physically attacking immigrants, can
be gured to protect the country from Communist traitors or to make
sensible ones identity as a patriot.12
Calhoun (1991) relates Andersons imagined communities to the general condition of sociality in modern society, to the fact that many relationships we moderns have with one another are indirect, conducted at a
distance by means of generalized media. He also reiterates an important
caution: it is important not to get too enamored of the media as the
tutelary spirits of postmodern societies. The stories and images they
provide are learned, retold, analyzed, and revalued in the contexts of our
everyday communities. There exist embodied traditions of usecommentaries, practical guides, rules of thumb, and outright regulations
that make the effect of the gured worlds of our mass media anything
but automatic or axiomatic. The social practice of the public houses,
salons, clubs, and associations that fostered newspaper literacy cannot
be erased from the story of nationalisms. Calhoun argues, and we concur, that there is also no necessary fascination with these imaginary
worlds, just as there need be no necessary alienation from our embodied
lives as the social power of such imaginaries grows. From the sociological or social psychological vantage, it is a mistake to rely upon some
universalistic, usually psychodynamic, apparatus to explain the impact
of cultural forms on personal life. It is better by far to attend to the social
history of their use and to the specic identities that form in relation to
them.
Nationalism and courtly love, to the extent that they are popularized,
are locally gured worldsimagined or play worlds of a sortthat have
come of age. Their specic visions have been publicized, moved from the
local, highly circumscribed, conditions of their original production to a
broader purview, their audience grown from a coterie to a true public.
(By this standard, it is harder to imagine a more successful gured world

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

250

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

py

than courtly love.) Imagined worlds are then potentiary, cultural resources that help us envision new identities that could give shape to the
afliations and disafliations we live by day to day. Like courtly love for
Occitanian courts, looking outward, they can oppose the positions assigned to us by other powers. Or, looking inward, they give content and
form to the positions we seek for ourselves.

Counter-Worlds

Ex

am

Co

The movement from play world to gured world, from a world without
a public to a world with communities, imagined or otherwise, is often
accomplished by the guring of the opposition to this publicization.
Our brief accounts of courtly love and nationalism revealed glimpses of
opposition and barriers to the emergence of these worlds, and of the
development of tools of insult and derogation for threatening or mobilizing action against those who supposedly endanger the course of emergence. Often these villains are imagined as allied with an opposing
gured world. In that counter-world, motives are askew and actions are
opposed to the course of events appropriate to the worlds topos. For
courtly love the counter-world was that of the illiberal court, whose
lord closets the lady and keeps a retinue of maledictors, talebearers,
and opportunists. For the nationalism of the twentieth-century United
States, the counter-world, as construed by conservative talk-show hosts,
is one of secular humanists and multiculturalists whose liberal social
and economic policies dilute and weaken the family and undercut the
country itself. These are portraits given to struggle and mobilization,
scenarios to fear, worlds into whose mismaking all of us will be condemned.
These counter-worlds, though undoubtedly gured as we have used
the term, rarely posit what a lived world should be. Instead they show us
what should not be, what threatens us, and they position the persons
presumed to inhabit them as relationally inferior and perhaps beyond the
pale of any imagined community we would ever want to join. The portrayal of these counter-identities, transgressors of principles and values, often becomes a genre itself, a concatenation of powerful artifacts
left to suggest an awful specter. Typically one gure, say Stalin, or one
transmogried event, the making of the Gulag, becomes a metonym for

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

251

Codevelopment

Co

py

the entirety. Their rhetorical use is, rst, to censure activities by claiming
they lead to the nightmare world and, second, to motivate an amorphous
support for the activities of the propagandists, which are presented as the
antidote to the actions of those censured.13 Villainy, however, is also in
the eye of the beholder, and so persuasion must be sought elsewhere.
Inuential persons or associations may be recruited to perform or endorse performance of the vision; the gured world will be allied to those
forms of life already known to be popular. And the opposite views will be
linked to despised people or to already established negatives. These tactics are familiar in the material churned out daily in the conduct of
modern politics.

Ex

am

This political-cultural process, however, is less a matter of choice, of


the marketplace of ideas, than a matter of production, the labor of
constituting Bourdieus habitus. The contention of gured worlds is not
one that simply presumes its audience. Instead a public is located, cultivated, created from a cultural system become desire. At this point again
the notions of Vygotsky and Bakhtin are important. Figured worlds move
through us as spoken discourse and embodied practice. By continual
rehearsal they are transgured within the activity of inner speech into
the vocal images, the virtual voices that are the resources of pro(to)action. The interplay of a persons identities is thus open to and dependent
upon a eld of continuing social discourse and everyday interaction. The
realities of countercultures and movements of resistance remind us
that the negotiation of authority is not a matter of pure power or rank.
Yet the person inside is always political, in the sense that the persuasion
of a voice, the valence of identication through it, must alter as the social
position of its speakers moves relative to the other member voices in the
ensemble, the repertory of intimate actors.
But, by the same circuit that social production takes through us, into
inner speaking and activity, the political itself becomes personal. That is
the inevitable conclusion toward which Bakhtin and Vygotsky draw us.
The pathetic fallacy of romanticism, that human mood and thought
are mirrored in the external world, here has a kind of realization. The
cultural production of virtualities, of new gured worlds, is thus simulta-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

252

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

neously a new personication, a repopulating of the cultural media


through which humans associate. Whether it is Kabyle houses, Occitanian court life, or the songcraft of Hindu women in rural Nepal, a
gured world enters the very landscape of social life. Only so can the
public constituency of its practice, the imagined community, be stably
reproduced.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

12

Co

py

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

Ex

am

The imagined communities discussed in Chapter 11 are often the outcomes of large-scale, spectacular social movements. Anderson (1983)
takes us through the colonial revolutions of South and Central America
on the way to modern nationalism, and surely other historical dramas
could be added to that story. But there also exist quieter means of social
reconstitution, where a public and its habitus take shape together before
the great events happen. E. P. Thompsons great history of the English
working class (1963) showed that seemingly ordinary forms of communal life can produce the grounds for large social upheaval. Another example of the quiet formation of a new public and the realization of an
alternative vision of social and political life is the cultural production of
the Tij festival in rural Nepal.
In 1990 we (Skinner and Holland) went to Naudada, Nepal, for Tij,
an annual festival for women that takes place in late August or early
September.1 We were struck, as Skinner had been in 1986, by the incongruities between the public part of the festival and the rituals that preceded and followed it. The portions of the ritual complex that called for
womens fasting for the health of their husbands or bathing to cleanse
away the pollution of menstruation were in accord with Brahmanical
texts and the patriarchal ideologies and practices that shaped life in large
parts of Nepal. Other portions did not: as noted in Chapter 10, the
womens groups that sang and danced in public on the day of Tij had
composed songs that explicitly criticized male privilege in the family.
In 1991, when we returned to continue our research, our appreciation
of the signicance of Tij grew. The 1991 celebration differed markedly
253
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

254

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

The Tij Festival

Co

py

from those of 1990 and 1986. The songs of the Tij groups had shifted
from criticism of womens positions in the family to criticism of the
government and its treatment of women. Songs that identied womens
problems in terms of abusive husbands, vulnerability to sharing resources with a co-wife, or parents favoring of sons over daughters had
given way to songs that blamed womens troubles on the former government and on the political party that had recently come to power.2 Increasingly strident verses called for equal rights for women, lower castes,
and the poor.
In addition to an important space of authoring gendered identities
(discussed in Chapter 10), Tij contained, or rather opened up, an imagined space where women envisioned and rehearsed, at least for several
weeks of the year, a gured world that rearranged gender relations.
Through the medium of Tij songs and the collective activity of their
song groups, the Naudadan women brought to life an atmosphere of
possibility. In the play of Tij, womens groups composed and performed
songs through which alternative worlds were imagined and experienced,
through which alternative femininities were given shape.3

Ex

am

In some older literature the Tij festival is described as a part of the


TijRishi Pancami complex (Bennett 1983; Bouillier 1982; Goodman
1981; but see Bista 1969). Such accounts emphasize the relation of the
rituals to Brahmanical cosmology and ideology. They include the dancing and singing on Tij Day, but contextualize these activities within the
religious observancesfasting, performing puja (worship), and ritual
bathingthat, in addition to the feast on the night before Tij, take place
over a four-day period.
Interpreted in light of Hindu religious texts, the rituals call for an
emulation of the ideals of womanhoodrepresented by the devotion of
Parvati to her husband, Shiva, and of the acts of atonement performed by
the wives of a group of Hindu holy men called the Rishi. By fasting on Tij
Day and dedicating the fast on the following day to their husbands,
women act to ensure long lives for their husbands. By ritually bathing on
the day of Rishi Pancami, they act to absolve themselves of sins associated with menstruation.
Bennett (1983) described the Tij portion of TijRishi Pancami as a

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

255

Ex

am

Co

py

time of licensea complete reversal of what is expected during the rest


of the year, a deviation from the Hindu ideal of womanly behavior
(225). Women are supposed to be shy and embarrassed, yet the dancing
they do on Tij Day, representing Parvatis seduction of Shiva, is considered erotic and highly suggestive. Bennett interpreted the sexuality expressed at the festival as being licensed by the ascetic rituals (the fasting
before and during and the ritual bathing after) that bracket the performance and therefore regulate it. She argued that these rituals, done on behalf of husbands, could be viewed as controlling womens sexuality and
reafrming patrilineal principles. She suggested that the village women
who perform TijRishi Pancami rituals have accepted the restrictions
placed on them by the dominant ascetic and patrilineal ideology of Hinduism (234). The ritual complex including Tij was reproducing a femininity valued in the world gured by Brahmanical doctrine.
In our research, we found that religious observances provided only a
part of the picture (see Holland and Skinner 1995a). Bennetts research
was conducted in the 1970s in Narikot, an area much closer to Kathmandu. It is possible that the women of Narikot were more subject to the
guidance and supervision of Bahun priests than were those of Naudada.
In Naudada the women were not constrained to adopt or even to hear
the priests renditions of the ritual complex, and many were, according to
the most learned priest in Naudada, at least partially ignorant of the
textual interpretation of the fasting and the ritual bathing.4
For the women who sang at Tij, the emotional and attentive center of
the festival we witnessed was not in the rituals. It was in the bringing together of groups of women, and it was in the making of the songs. Song
composition and rehearsals began long before Tij Day. Several weeks
before the public events of the festival and before any of the other rituals,
groups of girls and young women gathered at night to compose new
songs, sing old ones, and dance. Traveling about at night and getting
together with women friends, especially to dance, were unusual activities for women in Naudada. At times other than Tij a woman who engaged in such activities risked being labeled immoral. On the day of Tij
itself the women adopted a more serious and composed demeanor, but at
the rehearsals they took the opportunity to engage in a little bawdiness
and playfulness.
The girls and young women in the Tij groups, particularly the eight to
twelve core singers and composers, referred and related to one another

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

256

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

according to kinship ties, usually glossed by the terms didi (older sister)
and bahini (younger sister). Although friends from other gaons (hamlets,
neighborhoods) sometimes joined in, as well as mothers and in-married
women, the members of a given group were mostly of the same gaon,
related by consanguineal ties, and between ten and twenty-ve years old.
Often they were also members of the same parma, or cooperative labor
exchange group. Women organized and carried out much of the agricultural work in Naudada. They accomplished this work through labor
exchange with a set of more or less permanent partners.5 The groups of
didi/bahini produced songs at the nighttime rehearsals and also in their
parma groups as they worked in the elds.
In the rehearsal sessions there was, in addition to the somewhat carnivalesque atmosphere, an edge of anticipated competition. Tij groups
knew that their songs and dancing would be compared to those of other
groups that performed on the day of Tij. They had a sense of which songs
would be of interest to the audience and which would not. God songs,
for example, were said to be uninteresting.6 Although the upcoming
performance did not overshadow the rehearsal sessions, which were enjoyed in their own right, the anticipation of having their songs and
dancing appreciated lent excitement to the groups preparations.
Another emotional ingredient of the festival was extremely powerful
in shaping the atmosphere of Tij. It came from the return to Naudada of
women who had recently married out. Women from Naudada were usually married to men unknown to them, whose homes took hours or even
days of walking to reach. At Tij the young married women got a respite
from the work and tension in their husbands homes; they were permitted to return to their natal homes.7 Before the roads to Gorkha and
Pokhara were nished and before bridges spanned the larger rivers, trips
home were especially formidable. Then, as well as during the period of
our eldwork, Tij might be the only time the married didi/bahini were
able to make the difcult journey back home.8 And only then were they
able to tell anyone about the situation they faced in their husbands
household. At their maita (natal home) they found some relief from the
constant work and the strains of being a daughter-in-law. Among their
didi/bahini they could nd listeners sympathetic to their ordeals.9 Coming home was a strong emotional event for both the women who returned and their didi/bahini who remained in the gaon.
The returning womens stories often became the inspiration for new
songs. Their news of incidents outside of Naudadafor example, homi-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

257

Ex

am

Co

py

cides in their husbands communitiesbecame the events reported upon


in ghatana (incident) Tij songs.10 And, even more strikingly, their ordeals
in their husbands homes supplied images for dukha songs (songs of
sadness, hardship, and suffering). The dukha songs sung on Tij Day
resonated with the special atmosphere created by the return of the married sisters.
Tij commentatorsthat is, practically every person in Nepal to whom
we expressed our interest in Tijcalled the festival a time of license. But
they differed from Bennett in their emphasis on the songs rather than the
dancing. Many told us the festival is a time when women can pour out
their thoughts and feelings in Tij songs. They likened Tij to Gai Jatra, a
festival in which men put on skits that satirize unusual peoplesuch as
holy men and touristsas well as government ofcials, development
workers, and doctors. Tij is a time, they said, when women can express
sentiments and ideas that they usually keep hidden. Dukha songs were
said to spring from experience, from the man (the seat of thought and
emotion, located in the chest).
Together, then, the groups of girls and women in Naudada made up
new songs and rehearsed old ones that they wanted to sing again. Their
excitement mounted as the day of performance drew near. On Tij Day
the women and older girls spent most of the morning bathing and making themselves their most beautiful. They put on their best saris, preferably red or pink (colors associated with auspiciousness and fertility), and
adorned themselves with jewelry and makeup. The married women wore
all the markers of that status: the red vermilion powder in the part of
their hair, a red hair braid, red glass bangles, and a style of necklace worn
only by married women. They then gathered at the political and religious
center of Naudada where there was a tati (gathering place) big enough to
accommodate the people attending the festival.
In the early afternoon the women constituting the core of the performing group paraded in as a team. They spent a few minutes listening to
other singers who had already established themselves at the tati. They
then situated themselves in the best unclaimed space and began to sing
and dance. One member of the group danced in the center while her
friends sang and clapped along with her. Other women and girls surrounded the core groups and joined in the verses and clapping. Since
each verse was repeated twice, onlookers could sing the repeated verse
even if they had not heard the song before. Contemporary songs were all
sung to the same tune and cadence, and the movements and gestures of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

258

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

the dancer evoked the meaning of the lyrics. As many as four groups
performed simultaneously.
People of all ages and castes came from the surrounding hamlets to
view the performance. Men encircled the womens groups or stood on
the periphery of the crowd, watching, listening, and sometimes tape-recording the songs. They commented on the beauty of the women and the
quality of the songs. The women of each group hoped that their singing
and dancing would draw the largest crowd, a sign that their lyrics had
been judged the best. The dukha songs were especially moving. Women
both young and old nodded their heads and exclaimed in agreement
with the sentiments expressed by the singer, and sometimes even wept as
the images evoked memories of sad events in their own lives or the lives
of their friends. A woman who became visibly agitated while listening to
one song about a drunken husbands mistreatment of his wife told us
later that her man started churning when she heard the verses. They
brought back vivid memories of her own husband, who would often
come home drunk late at night and beat her.
The TijRishi Pancami complex, we learned, did collectively represent
the feminine ideals gured by the Hindu stories that priests read and
reread to families throughout the year in Naudada. Yet it also did something else in the rehearsal and performance of Tij songs, replete as these
were with womens feelings about their lives and their friendships with
one another. Tij provided, for a brief period each year, a space for the
gathering of the didi/bahini groups and a time for them to reect,
through song, upon their lives as daughters, daughters-in-law, and wives,
and increasingly as political actors. The Tij festival and the month-long
preparation for it provided a space for play; in their production of Tij
songs women could represent themselves as kinds of actors in social
scenes other than those gured by Brahmanical texts and rituals. They
could together think and act otherwise. As we shall see, in envisioning
a novel world of gender relations where women were not the social and
ritual inferiors of men, they were agents in bringing about this world and
agents in reshaping themselves.

Imagining Alternative Worlds and Alternative Femininities


As the two Tij dukha songs we presented in Chapter 10 show, Tij lyrics
provided a perspective on womens difculties, hardships, and problems.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

259

Ex

am

Co

py

The actors in the world of domestic relations envisioned in Tij songs


were women and their unkind and sometimes abusive husbands, mothers-in-law, fathers, and co-wives. These dukha songs portrayedpoignantly, if we may judge by the tears of listenersthe unhappy situations of
their female protagonists. But the songs did more than portray suffering:
they implied and often stated that these women deserved a better lot in
life. Through the verses they jointly produced, women assessed their
status and their relative worth within the world of domestic relations.
They often included in their dukha songs a more general social and
political analysis, to the effect that women were unfairly underprivileged. The songs criticized differences in the treatment of sons versus
daughters and husbands versus wives. They opposed the gender privilege instituted in Nepalese law, carried out in social practice, and interpreted through religious texts and teachings. They criticized womens
limited right to inherit land, womens lesser access to education, and
womens constant vulnerability, under polygyny, to the division of their
husbands resources and affections with a co-wife.
Through the cultural form of Tij songs and the atmosphere created
around the festival, a gured world and subjectivity were developing
together. Girls and women were producing a gured world in which
womens modes of being and acting differed signicantly from those
promoted by Brahmanical texts and Hindu religious rites, and they were
developing identities meaningful in this alternative world: not as the
passive and submissive good Hindu woman, but as an aggrieved and
wronged character vis--vis others in the domestic sphere.
Renu, a member of the Chetri caste (one of the two highest castes in
Naudada), who was in her mid-twenties in 1991, provides an example of
these dual processes. Renu lived in her maita in one of the hamlets that
made up the community of Naudada. She had never attended school, but
her parents arranged for her to marry an educated man. According to
Renu, her husband soon came to have contempt for her because she was
uneducated. This, coupled with his attraction to drink, forced her to
leave her husbands home and return to her maita to live and nd support. After her return she composed several songs for Tij, which she, her
friends, and her female relatives performed for the festival. Her songs
seemed to be a medium she deftly used to objectify her thoughts and
emotions. The following song is one Renu composed for the 1991 Tij
festival:

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

260

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

I worked all day on the at lands of Salyantar.


On that long road, no one was there.
Though I say no one, my husband was walking with me.
He didnt say a single word on this long road.
I walked ahead and my husband walked behind.
My husband did not scold me.
If he didnt like me, why did he marry me?
If I walk ahead and behind, why did he turn his eyes away from
me?
When I went inside and out, my mother-in-law didnt want to
see me.
Though it was a time of drought, my tears created a lake.
You knead our in a plate and [separate] oil from water.
If my husband doesnt like me, he will return the sidur.
By fate, you are [as] the brother and I am [as] the sister.
Then Ill say Im unmarried and go to the river.

Ex

am

These lines expressed Renus distress at her husbands and mother-inlaws contempt for her. Her husband was so angry he would not speak to
her, not even to scold; her mother-in-law could not bear to look at her.
She worried that her husband would take away the vermilion powder
(sidur) that women wear in their hair as a symbol of being married. If
he did this, she threatened to go to the river, a very literal reference
to suicide: over the period of Skinners research several Naudadan
women had committed suicide by throwing themselves in rivers swollen
by monsoon rains.
In a song Renu composed the year before, in 1990, she sang of a
drunkard husband who squandered the familys wealth and property on
alcohol. She criticized the inheritance system that keeps property in the
patriline and relegates women to being outsiders in both their husbands house and their maita.
...
Listen to the description of the drunken husband.
Rising in the morning, he goes down to the hotel.
Who will do the household chores?
The hotel girl has probably made the tea.
The raksi has nished all the money.
The household wealth has all gone to the hironi [a cinema
rolehere, the hotel girl].

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

261

am

Co

py

The most fertile land is all nished because of his drinking


raksi.
Whatever money you have, it is not enough,
Two or even four bottles of raksi is not enough for you.
If I say, Dont drink, he replies, Im not drinking your fathers
[property].
The most fertile land is gone and still he does not know.
The best land is gone because of the drunkard husband.
How will we spend our lives?
The whole day the husband king drinks a jar,
You dont need to return home after drinking there in the
evening.
In Pokhara bazaar [there is] an electricity line,
The household property is not mine.
The housewife is an outsider,
All the household property is needed [for raksi].
If this wife is not enough, you can get another,
The head of the cock will be caught [that is, with two wives
hell have problems].
Why do you hold your head? Go sell the buffalo and pigs,
If you dont have enough money, you will even sell your wife.
After selling his wife, hell become a jogi [here: a beggar
without a wife].
I, the daughter, will go stay at my maita.
If I want a man, I can nd one just like you,
For my parents reputation, I have to stay with you.

Ex

Renu had become expert at producing dukha songs of a certain type,


and her two songs chronicled and criticized the suffering and difculties
she had experienced. By her account, these songs owed forth from her
man. She transgured the thoughts and feelings stored and churning
in her man in these vocal images, which in turn were taken up by other
women as a way of shaping and evoking their thoughts and feelings
about their own lives.
Renu, like other women in Naudada (Skinner 1989, 1990), had made
Tij songs a tool for authoring herself as a woman in this society. The
songs provided a perspective on her life, an understanding of herself as
female. These songs, these cultural forms, and the way they were produced and used in womens interaction with one another in the Tij song
groups affected womens understanding of themselves in the lived world

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

262

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

of family and domestic relations among Hindu caste groups in central


Nepal. The women became, through the medium of Tij songs, critical
commentators on their lives in families and their various roles as daughters, sisters, daughters-in-law, and wives.
Over the course of our research (Skinner rst attended Tij sessions in
Naudada in 1986), we watched young girls rst hover on the periphery of the composition and practice sessions and then gradually take on
a larger role. Over the years they became procient in (re)producing
critical commentaries, and they developed, or so we infer from their
descriptions of their concerns and choices in life, identities or self-understandings informed by this critical stance on womens positions within
the family (see Holland and Skinner 1995a). During the same years the
groups underwent a heuristic development as well.
Our understanding of the Tij festival as a ground for play, as a creative
space for envisioning other worlds and subjectivities, deepened in our
1991 research. We realized that Tij was even more dynamic than we had
imagined. We learned that the Tij groups could compose another type of
song in addition to the dukha songs and that the previous boundaries on
the time and space of Tij groups were being breached. In 1991 those
members of Tij groups who identied themselves as students and educated women moved their groups to sing about the world of political
parties and of antigovernment activity. These songs were not considered
dukha songs but rather rajniti (political) songs.
In 1990, as mentioned in Chapter 10, a peoples movement for democracy succeeded in abolishing the panchayat, or one-party, system of government. Under the new constitution Nepals government was transformed from an absolute to a constitutional monarchy. Political parties
became legal and gained strength. These changes ushered in a feeling of
freedom in political expression and a call for human rights. Beginning in
1990, after the revolution, and especially in 1991, after the establishment
of the multiparty system and general elections, rajniti songs were much
more widely sung in the Tij festivals.
In 1990 a majority of the songs in Naudadas festival referred to the
world of family relations. In these songs the didi/bahini attributed
womens suffering to their bad treatment in the family. They disparagingly compared the treatment of daughters with that of sons; they called
attention to the harsh treatment of daughters-in-law and wives by mothers-in-law and husbands. In contrast, a majority of the songs in 1991
criticized the government, old and new, for policies and practices that

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

263

Ex

am

Co

py

harmed women and other groups. Although complaints about the lot of
women were similar to those of the earlier dukha songs, the 1991 rajniti
songs were sung against party politicians and members of the ruling
class, not against mothers-in-law, husbands, brothers, and other kin.11
We learned that these rajniti songs were not new. Several people,
mostly men from Gorkha, Chitwan, Lamjung, Pokhara, and Naudada,
told us of composing or of hearing women sing antigovernment songs at
Tij festivals decades before the pro-democracy movement of 1990. These
earlier songs told of corrupt practices, brutality, and exploitation encouraged by those in power during the panchayat period. A few people even
remembered critical songs that might have been sung during the earlier
Rana regime. Our own recordings from 1990 included an antigovernment song, and Skinners collection from 1986 revealed several songs
critical of local politics. It became clear that this kind of song, abundant
in 1991, had been sung, though sparingly, for a long time. After the
political changes it was less dangerous to sing them, and they proliferated in great number.
Although the Tij groups remained the same in 1991, we found a different set of participants stepping forward to lead the composition of rajniti
songs. Kamala, a thirteen-year-old, belonged to the same caste as Renu,
but unlike Renu she attended school. In 1991 she was in the eighth
grade. Kamala strongly identied herself as a student and was clearly
developing, as were many of her school-going friends, a view of herself as
an educated and a politically active person. Like Renu she was recognized as an expert at song composition, but her songs were oriented to a
different eld of action than Renus songs. Kamala composed political Tij
songs that were critical of regional and national politics and that called
for equal rights for women and the poor. The following song, produced
and performed by Kamala and her Tij group, condemns the old political
system for exploiting women and calls for a new political party that will
give women equal rights under the law:
Oh, dominated sisters of Nepal,
We have so much tyranny.
The panchas ate the esh and also the blood,
At last we have the multiparty system.
The thirty-year panchayat reign gave so much trouble to
women,
They drank liquor by selling unmarried girls.
They sold our innocent sisters,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

264

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

am

Co

py

And lled their bellies with liquor and chicken.


For even a small thing, they call a meeting,
Panchas took bribes, their reign is like this.
The administration was like this,
The panchas dominated women.
Now this type of rule cannot be tolerated.
Women will no longer tolerate what they did in the past.
Understanding these things, we moved forward,
We fought in the peoples movement.
Rise up, women, now we dont have to fear,
Clapping cannot be done by a single hand.
On the day of Baishak 29, 2048,
There will be a general election.
There are so many parties in the multiparty system,
At last we need the party that benets us.
A party called the Democratic party was established,
It deleted the name panchayat and deceived the people.
If this party wins, it will be the same as before,
Women will not have rights and they will have to weep all of
their life.
There is a party called Congress,
If this party wins, women will have to suffer more.
There is another party called Communist,
If this party wins, women will get rights.
The symbol of Communists is the sickle and hammer,
Women, lets publicize the Communist symbol.
Though I want to stop my pen, it doesnt stop,
Goodbye to my respected listeners.

Ex

In song, ofcials of the former panchayat system (the panchas) were


accused of selling women as prostitutes to India, taking bribes, and
exploiting the people. The singers assailed two political parties as abusers of womens rights, and promoted the Communist party as the only
one that would bring justice to women.
Also in 1991, another group of women in Naudada combined some
lines from a published Tij song with some they had written themselves to
create a song entitled Tij Is Our Festival, which compared their status
as Nepalese women with that of other women in the world:
Tij is our festival, Tij is celebrated in a gathering,
This is the time to tell our hearts happiness and sadness.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

265

Co

py

This happiness and sadness are pressing fully on our hearts,


I have to stay with my eyes full of tears.
Other countries women have accomplished so much,
Our countrys women are involved in dancing.
We only do household work.
Why did you panchas walk about wearing wristwatches?
Women have reached the moon and climbed Mt. Everest.
What have women not done in this world!
They go to school and also to the battleeld,
If they get the chance women can do anything.
The girl child grows by mothers love,
Another child cries upstairs.
Until the mothers love goes to the daughter,
This heart wont have any peace.

am

This rajniti song recognizes Tij as a time when women can sing about
their heartfelt feelings. But instead of focusing on the domestic problems and relationships portrayed in dukha songs, it orients its performers toward national political parties and the international arena of economic development and it takes up the question of womens roles in this
broader world.
In 1993 Kamala told Skinner about a song she and her group had
performed for the 1992 festival. In this song, which Kamala and her
group called Tyranny over Women, women demanded equal access to
education and jobs and a recognition that women are as capable as men
in developing the country:

Ex

Listen sisters, listen society,


Today I am going to speak about tyranny over women.
The male and female born from the same womb,
Do not have equal rights.
The son gets the ancestral property at the age of fourteen,
Whereas the daughter has to get married when she is only
twelve.
Parents engage in great trickery,
Sending their daughter weeping to her husbands house.
Parents send the son to school,
Whereas they are afraid to provide education to the daughter.
Father bought books and pens for my younger brother,
Whereas he wove a basket for me, the daughter.
My name is Kamala, who has studied only to the eighth grade,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

266

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Co

py

But who has a great desire for further education.


Parents, dont take me out of school,
See if I can study well or not.
Parents, if you provide me with an education, I wont fail,
And after study, I can live by myself.
Parents, provide me with an education at any cost,
And later when I hold a job, I will repay you.
We women are also energetic and want justice,
We also have the right to hold a job.
A red ribbon tied around black hair,
We women are always deprived in Nepal.
Women have even climbed Mt. Everest and reached the moon,
Women have done so many things in this world.
Women of other countries are pilots,
We Nepalese women will be happy if we get the chance to be
great women.
Therefore, women of Nepal, this is not the time to be silent,
Lets ght to obtain our rights.

Ex

am

This song is a clear example of what Bakhtin (1981) would call a


hybridizationa mixture of two voices within a single utterance.
Kamala as author combined the voice of dukha songs with themes of the
rajniti songs. Her verses draw from the gured world of dukha songs,
criticizing family relations that fashion inequalities between sons and
daughters. But mixed with this voice from the domestic realm is a voice
from the world of politics, and explicit talk about ghting to obtain equal
rights. Through her hybridized form, which brings different worlds into
contact with each other, Kamala illuminates a potential link between the
worlds imagined by the dukha and rajniti songs. The voice of the dukha
songs frames the move into the world of politics and offers a vision of
how the gured world of dukha songs could lead to a world outside the
household, a world of politics, and to new positions for women. From
Bakhtins perspective it is possible to view all rajniti Tij songs and the
political world of gender relations they embody as a hybridized discourse, drawing from the gured world of dukha songs and from the
discourse of politics spoken and practiced among the educated in their
world of schools and party-afliated groups (see Skinner and Holland
1996).12

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

267

Ex

am

Co

py

Political Tij songs thus were involved in constructing yet another


gured world in which women act, one different from both the
Brahmanical world and the world envisioned by the dukha songs.
Through symbols used in song composition, women created stories and
visions of better treatment, and in turn, the narrativized world in which
their visions had meaning. Through the play allowed in Tij, women were
producing new imagined priorities and relationships. They were suspending the natural world, where women were subordinate to men, to
envision a world where women were powerful and educated, equal to
men, capable of achieving any feat, and deserving of the same opportunities as men. The actors in the social scenes depicted in political Tij songs
were educated women, political activists, rebels, revolutionaries.
The women in Naudada, over the course of scores of Tij festivals, built
a vision of an alternative world. Through both dukha and rajniti songs,
they constructed counter-worlds to the ones they knew. They imagined
other gured worlds in which there was a possibility, and a moral valuing, of equal rights for women. In the Brahmanical world depicted in
Rishi Pancami, a ritual bathing that occurs two days after the Tij festival,
women are a problem. They menstruate, and their menstrual blood is
polluting. In the dukha songs of the Tij festival, women are portrayed not
as being problems but as having problems; wives are unjustly underprivileged relative to husbands, and daughters unfairly disadvantaged
relative to sons. In the rajniti songs, women (and womens problems)
are redeployed in a broader political arena, where corrupt government
ofcials and unjust laws keep them from having equal rights and opportunities in work, education, and politics. In the special atmosphere and
space of the Tij festival and the rehearsal sessions preceding it, the
women created and participated in imagined worlds where injustice
against women was recognized and deplored.
The Tij festival has been a site where women, through symbolic mediation and play, have been able collectively to envision a world in which
women deserve better treatment, rights to inherit land, more say in
decisions about family resources, and greater access to education. The
pivot of the songs enables them to enter a different place and become a
different I.13 The Tij groups of Naudada were composing and performing songs that had become a means of symbolic bootstrapping, a medium
through which a different world could be envisioned and experienced,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

268

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

and new dispositions and new sensibilities could be formed. These novelties were not so much the results of free play as the outcomes of
situated cultural production. It is not only womens increasing participation in education but also an emergent form of song production, in
which Tij songs are written down (often by individual authors) for rehearsals and for publication, that has opened up the space of Tij to the
world of the educated and vice versamaking possible the revolution of
rajniti songs.
The years and years of Tij songs have been important at both the
individual and collective levels. During some period in the past, perhaps from their inception, Tij songs became a medium for describing
womens worlds and the position of women in those worlds. At some
point womens Tij groups began to articulate a critical commentary on
the world of domestic relations and, much less frequently until the peoples movement, on the world of the state. Although Tij was timeboundthe practice sessions constrained to a few weeks, the public
performance lasting only one daythe festivals provided a space for
women to objectify, and thus the possibility to disinhabit, the familiar
domestic world and, if only for a brief time, inhabit a new one. To
relegate Tij to the category of rituals of rebellion or rituals of reversal, in which people are portrayed as stepping outside the boundaries of
everyday life then returning unchanged with the boundaries reafrmed,
is to miss the power of play-worlds like Tij to transform selves and
boundaries. After Tij, though women returned to the domestic world,
they re-created, by rehearsing the Tij songs to themselves, the altered
subjectivities and the sense of the unnaturalness of the social and political relations that kept them unequal to men.
Over the period of our research this time-bounded world began to
expand. Tij groups became less circumscribed in time, their compositions less controllable. Their songs became available both in handwritten
versions and in published songbooks, outside the time and locale of the
festival (earlier, singing Tij songs after the festival had been labeled a
sin). The relations between women in many Tij groups also became more
multi-stranded: some of the women began to relate to one another not
just as sisters and friends but also as political actors and, in one case, as a
literacy teacher promoting equal rights. Women in these groups also
expanded the scope of their agency in the worlds of domestic relations
and political action. For instance, the women who participated in the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

269

Ex

am

Co

py

march and the posse described in Chapter 10 cast their demands and
sought redress in ways novel to both the world of political action and the
world of domestic relations. The inuence of these Tij groups has grown
apace, extended beyond Tij Day by songbooks that have carried their
songs to other arenas of activity and broader expanses of time.
In the space of the Tij festival, women were authoring new worlds
and, in turn, new selves. Different constructions of being female were
evolving along with novel imagined worlds. As they appropriated political song texts as tools of self-identication, Kamala and the other women
producing these songs were developing ways of conceptualizing themselves as politically aware actors and activists in a political world.
Through the medium of the festival, alternative worlds, identities, cultural forms, and senses of agency were codeveloping. This was a path
toward a new world of gender relations and gendered identities in
Naudada.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

13

Co

py

Identity in Practice

Ex

am

We have attempted to articulate the relation of person and society in a


way that makes light of neither social life nor the world of the psyche. At
the same time, we reject a dichotomy between the sociological and the
psychological. Person and society are alike as sites, or moments, of
the production and reproduction of social practices. But there is a substantiality to both sites. We object to an anti-essentialism that rotely
rejects any sense of durability or predisposition in social life. Forms of
personhood and forms of society are historical products, intimate and
public, that situate the interactivity of social practices. It is in this doubly
historical landscape that we place human identities. We take identity to
be a central means by which selves, and the sets of actions they organize,
form and re-form over personal lifetimes and in the histories of social
collectivities.
Identity is one way of naming the dense interconnections between the
intimate and public venues of social practice. In the cultural studies of
the person described by Hall (1996), these connections are understood
as a suturing of the person to social position, via psychodynamic processes of identication. We move toward a different metaphor, that of
codevelopment. Improvisational responses to social and cultural openings and impositions elaborate identities on intimate terrain, even as
these identities are worked and reworked on the social landscape. In
order to set forth our own guring of this connection between the social
and the intimate, we have followed closely Vygotsky (and the sociohistorical school), Bakhtin, and Bourdieu. We have often set aside the volumi270

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

271

Ex

am

Co

py

nous literature on identity; instead of arriving at our point about identity after another lengthy critique of extant versions, we provide an alternative formulation, one grounded in practice and activity theories, to be
sure, but also in our own eldwork. Our focus is on what we call, in
homage to Bakhtin, the space of authoring, and it directs our effort to
understand identity in practice.
Practiced identities are constructs that can be described by reference
to several contexts of activity. The rst context of identity is the gured
world. The notions of cultural, intentional, virtual, or imaginary
worlds are quite common across the disciplines of contemporary social
science. They are the frames of meaning in which interpretations of
human actions are negotiated. What we add is the peculiarly practical
notion that such worlds are also socially identied. Their gures are
not gures of speech or expression as these are ordinarily conceived: as
disembodied conveyances, or neutral media, of interchange. Rather the
parts of gured worlds are gures in Bakhtins sense; they carry disposition, social identication, and even personication just as surely as
they carry meaning. Thinking, speaking, gesturing, cultural exchange
are forms of social as well as cultural work. When we do these things we
not only send messages (to ourselves and others) but also place ourselves in social elds, in degrees of relation toafliation with, opposition to, and distance fromidentiable others.
The second context of identity, then, is positionality. It is less a separate
second context than a (separable) counterpart of guration. Positionality has to do with more than division, the hereness and thereness of
people; it is inextricably linked to power, status, and rank. Social position has to do with entitlement to social and material resources and so to
the higher deference, respect, and legitimacy accorded to those genders,
races, ethnic groups, castes, and sexualities privileged by society. It is
the context of identity that was most developed by constructivism. But
the activity of positioning refers back to the cultural lay of the land, to
gured worlds, and not only to some species-given universal frame. It is
true that people never inhabit only one gured world. It is true that
the more durable social positionssuch as gender, race, ethnicity, and
classhave probably been cultivated in almost every frame of activity.
Positions marked by these enduring divisions, and their expectations of
privilege (or not), are features of the worlds we describe, whether that of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

272

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

AA, of romance, or of mental disorder. But they are prominent features in


some worlds, highlighted less in others, and gured differently in terms
of the symbolic capitals particular to each world.
The third context of identity, the space of authoring, is Bakhtins rendition of the normal world faced by any person or collective. The world
must be answeredauthorship is not a choicebut the form of the
answer is not predetermined. It may be nearly automatic, as in strictly
authoritative discourses and authoritarian practices (thus nearing Bakhtins monology), or it may be a matter of great variability and most
signicant to a single persons address. In either case authorship is a
matter of orchestration: of arranging the identiable social discourses/practices that are ones resources (which Bakhtin glossed as
voices) in order to craft a response in a time and space dened by
others standpoints in activity, that is, in a social eld conceived as the
ground of responsiveness. Human agency comes through this art of improvisation; the space of authoring also includes Vygotskys zone of
proximal development. The voices that make up a space of authoring
are to an author as Vygotskys instructing adults are to the neophyte:
they do not so much compel rote action as extend, through their support, the competencies, the answerability, of persons to operate in such
a diverse yet powerful social universe. The histories that give shape to
spaces of authoring, besides being both personal and public, are thus
also compulsory and liberatory, in degrees that vary greatly.
This vision of human activity makes a mockery of any notion of individual authorship, if that means a kind of independent or autonomous
creativity. But it lls personal authorship with social efcacy, for identities take us back and forth from intimate to public spaces. The fourth
context of identity is that of making worlds: through serious play, new
gured worlds may come about, in the peculiarly Bakhtinian way that
feeds the personal activities of particular groups, their signatures, into
the media, the cultural genres, through which even distant others may
construe their lives. Vygotskys understanding of play is crucial to this
argument. Just as childrens play is instrumental in building their symbolic competencies, upon which adult life depends, so too social play
the activities of free expression, the arts and rituals created on the
margins of regulated space and timedevelops new social competencies
in newly imagined communities. These new imaginaries build in their
rehearsal a structure of disposition, a habitus, that comes to imbue the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

273

py

cultural media, the means of expression, that are their legacy. The course
of publicization is markedly complicated (as suggested by the spectacularization of courtly love, and by the new divisions of women signaled by rajniti songs), but this concretion of a novel gured world
brings us back to the rst of the four contexts.

Climbing Houses

Ex

am

Co

In Chapter 1 we related an incident involving caste identities in Nepal


that lent itself equally well to a cultural or culturalist interpretation of
action and to a social or constructivist view. Now that we have joined
aspects of the two perspectives, let us return to the incident to discuss a
signicant piece neglected by either framework alone.
Recall that Gyanumaya, a lower-caste woman, arrived at the house
where Skinner was living in Naudada. Calling down from the secondoor balcony, Skinner invited her to come in through the kitchen and up
the stairs to be interviewed. But instead Gyanumaya scaled the outside of
the house and climbed through an opening in the railing that enclosed
the balcony. Since neither Naudadans in general nor Gyanumaya in particular was accustomed to climbing the sides of houses to reach the
second oor, something called for explanation.
In the world of human behavior gured by those we call culturalists,
cultural conceptions or beliefs, in this case Hindu understandings of
pollution and purity, move people. They are powerful enough to make a
person go to great lengths to observe cultural tenetseven if she is
disadvantaged or discomforted in the process. Hence a culturalist might
propose the following reason for Gyanumayas climb: subscribing to the
religious tenets she absorbed in childhood, Gyanumaya may have seen
herself as a moral actor who, because of her caste, carried an impurity
that would pollute the household she was being invited to enter, especially as the route Skinner suggested led through the kitchen, taking her
near the hearth. So Gyanumaya found a way to compromise between
refusal of Skinners invitation and pollution of a higher-caste household,
and reached the balcony by another route.
In a constructivist interpretation, the events at Skinners house would
have been, rst and foremost, social dramas in which participants commanding different levels of power and privilege in the local community
were actively negotiating the idiom of social relations that was to pre-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

274

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

dominate. A Chetri (higher-caste) woman was already seated on the


balcony when Gyanumaya arrived. Perhaps a glance from that woman
positioned Gyanumaya as one who had no right to enter the kitchen of a
house clearly occupied by higher-caste persons. Gyanumaya would, in
this account, be compelled by social power, not cultural logic; she
would be constrained in her access to the houses space by her subordinate social position, not by her embrace of a cultural imperative that just
happened to disadvantage her. Although the constructivist perspective
includes the possibility of resistance to what Goffman called the interaction order, in this incident Gyanumaya accepted (or at least chose not to
contest openly) the position afforded her by the Chetri woman, did not
enter through the kitchen, and so avoided making a social claim that she
probably would not have been able to defend.
Research in Nepal, our own and that of others, leads us to appreciate
both the culturalist and the constructivist perspectives on Gyanumayas
actions but to accept neither as sufcient or comprehensive. Caste identities in Nepal are extensively developed, both guratively in talk and
other forms of expression and relationally in practice. Cultural elaborations, many unmarked for the social position that they privilege, abound.
Religious teachings, folk stories, images, rituals, and holy texts provide a
wealth of interpretations in which caste is portrayed as a rightful ordering of the social world. Other gurings, less prevalent, objectify the caste
system as wrong.
Someone like Gyanumaya would have had many experiences with the
cultural resources, discourses and practices, that justify caste. She would
have learned early in life to gure herself and others as inhabitants of a
culturally constructed world of caste. She no doubt could interpret interactions between persons of different castes according to the discourses of
caste and imagine herself and others inspired by the motives (such as
maintaining purity) of this gured world. As a child she had probably
also had many experiences in which particular castes were associated
with the particular localities in Naudada that placed them and the particular activities and acts that marked their presence: particular languages, ways of speaking, ways of carrying the body, ways of holding the
body when in the presence of persons of different castes, ways of dressing, moral evaluations, and so on.
For her, her own caste identity no doubt was embodied and, as
Bourdieu has insisted, powerfully affected her behavior through habitual

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

275

Ex

am

Co

py

associations that she might not have been able to verbalize. Her body
carried a sense of caste, and of her caste position in respect to others
about her, wherever she went. At the same time, she both knew and
could generate gurings of caste and possessed an embodied sense of
how caste was practiced in Naudada. She could probably tell herself (and
others) about herself as a person of a particular caste, as well as consciously monitor and edit her own behavior to t an image of proper
caste behavior. She was, in sum, highly susceptible to guring herself
and being gured by others according to caste, and to positioning herself
and being positioned as a lower-caste personone prohibited from entering the kitchens of higher-caste persons. She lived in a web of constraints organized around caste and was probably propelled at times by
purely ideological constructions; at other times by an almost purely tactical social reckoning of what it would cost her to refuse the caste
position afforded her in a given situation; and, probably a majority of the
time, by a mixture of the two.
Both a culturalist view, which credits an asocial emphasis on cultural
logics, and a constructivist view, which emphasizes the calculus of social
position by actors, make too little of the kind of agency we have seen
throughout the cases described in this book. This is our major disagreement with both frames of interpretation. For us, the incident of the
woman who climbed up the house became an invitation to consider not
only the means by which her behavior was collectively orchestrated but
also the ways in which creativity is collectively enabled. Even hemmed in
by caste constraints, Gyanumaya managed, in a strikingly inventive way,
to circumvent the obstacles to her participation in the interviewing. She
improvised from the resources at hand a rather spectacular entry to the
house. By focusing only on the social constraints, we would have missed
the signicance of her improvised departure from a routine path. By
ignoring the constraints, we would have missed the forces that made the
path obligatory and the pointedness of her deviation.
Or take the case of Roger (Chapter 9). Foucault has presented a compelling picture of scientic discourse, its contribution to a constructed
behavioral normality, and the specically modern impetus to discipline
those who deviate from this norm. Roger was one such person, who had
come under the purview of the medical establishment and was subjected
to its categorization of mental disorders and to the treatments prescribed
to manage these disorders. But Roger twisted the categories applied to

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

276

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

his case. He used what his caregivers told him and popular texts about
mental disorders as ways to gure an identity or rather identities, as ways
to interpret and organize his actions in the social contexts important to
him. He was having difculties with both his parents and his wife; the
histories of negotiations around his problems with them had created
dilemmas that troubled him. Roger appropriated different diagnoses of
his mental problems to create distinctive standpoints within these two
dilemmas. With his parents, he elaborated an identity organized around
manic-depression; with his wife, an identity around borderline personality disorder. Roger assimilated the textual artifacts and categories of psychiatry to an everyday part of his life, improvising several new identities through which he would craft new answers to his dilemmas and
(concomitantly) new positions for his parents and his wife. He used
these resources opportunistically, as a Lvi-Straussian bricoleur would,
yet without regulation by an overarching cultural logic or an institutionally prescribed blueprint.
A more collaborative improvisation, and one of clear importance, was
evident in the Tij festival in Naudada, Nepal (Chapter 12). A particular
sort of Tij song, rajniti or political songs, predominated in the festival of
1991, virtually supplanting the dukha or suffering songs that had predominated in earlier festivals. Rajniti songs were responsive to the Peoples Movement of 1990 and the resulting change from the kings oneparty system to a multiparty system. They were quite different in content
from the dukha songs: instead of being about womens mistreatment by
husbands and mothers-in-law, the songs of 1991 were more often about
the atrocities of the former government and the ill-treatment of women
and poor people by political parties. In one regard, however, rajniti songs
were rather simple improvisations on a third, less frequent type of song.
These ghatana or event songs typically described newsworthy incidentssuicides, landslides, plane and bus accidents. Formally, rajniti
songs were mere variations, a type of ghatana song that took political
events and incidents as its subject matter. The improvisation allowed
women to redirect their critical commentary from the domestic hearth
and eld to the arena of government and party politics.

The Signicance of Improvisation


Why celebrate these moments of resourcefulness? Why pay attention to
these improvisations that piece together existing cultural resources op-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

277

Ex

am

Co

py

portunistically to address present conditions and problems? Why pay


attention to them instead of analyzing the webs of constraints that limit
peoples activities and life possibilities? Webs like those imposed on Gyanumaya in the name of caste, on Hindu women in Naudada in the name
of the life path of the good woman, or on Roger in the name of personal
competence: surely they overpower these improvisational moves and
reveal their feebleness and insignicance?
To put it simplistically, there are two answers. We attend to improvisation because we speak from a critical perspective, in a manner akin to
that of Haraway (1988). We take an evaluative stand toward gross oppression, exploitation, and the small kinds of relentless and irredemptive
strictures that Foucault detailed. Yet we neither anticipate an impending
liberatory revolution nor favor schemes of intervention that would impose policies and programs that only a select few recognize as benecial.
The remaining option is to document and support those local openings
and social movements which seem liberatory.
The second answer concerns theoretical comprehension. Improvisations command our attention because they may be excluded only at the
risk of missing the back-and-forth of engagement. Even within grossly
asymmetrical power relations, the powerful participants rarely control
the weaker so completely that the latters ability to improvise resistance
becomes irrelevant. Tactics such as Rogers, for example, by which the
terms for controlling patients and clients are taken up by their subject for his own purposes, are more than problematic for mental health
practitioners. The borderline confounds both the ideology and the
practice of psychiatry by refusing to accept the boundaries of the practitioner-client relationship (see Lachicotte 1992). The borderline diagnosis itself and the commentary produced around it are historical constructions responsive to the perturbations caused (in part) by the particular
improvisations of a particular type of client that the practice has not
learned to control.

A Second Form of Agency


If improvisations are signicant means of renovation, which even the
most powerful and hegemonic of social regimes cannot preclude, they
are even more signicant when paired with another form of human
agency: agency through self-directed symbolizations. In developing our
picture of identity in practice we drew upon two theorists: Bakhtin and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

278

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

Vygotsky. Together they afford us a sober view of the possibilities for


human agency. They tell us wherealong the margins and interstices of
collective cultural and social constructionshow, and with what
difculties human actors, individuals, and groups are able to redirect
themselves. Though an overemphasis on social constraints leaves little
room for human agency, many accounts make the opposite error: they
neglect the cultural and social contexts that inform the playing eld to
which human action is directed and by which it is shaped. Vygotsky and
Bakhtin explicitly addressed the issues of agency that were such a part of
the intellectual eld of post-revolutionary Russia. Yet, through the socially mediated ways in which they cast such forms of intimate action
as inner speech and the dialogical interplay of self and others, they
avoided the asocial and acultural guring of the world of human action
that still predominates among many of those who explore psychological processes.
Vygotsky and Bakhtin have allowed us to elaborate a theory of human
action that also takes advantage of Bourdieus shift to the study of culture
in practice and his correlative appreciation of embodiment and rejection of the conception of culture as rules. The two Russian theorists
fascination with the relation of language and other means of expression
to consciousnesstaking into account both everyday genres and such
specialized forms as literary and artistic worksled them to devise conceptual tools that we have used to augment practice theorys understanding of human action. Improvisations crafted in the moment are one
of the margins of human agency. Self-directed symbolizations are a second means by which a modicum of agency is made possible.
In Bourdieus work, identity seems principally embodied; he pays
much less attention to objectications of identity or self-understandings.
For our work, the more interesting cases of identity are those which are
objectied, for they are the ones more likely to mediate sustained agentive action. The positional aspects of identity, such as the discomfort a
woman may feel on entering a space she has experienced before as for
men only, are most often embodied, and may be unmediated by any
linguistically realized apprehension. But to grasp the potential for ennoblement, for moving beyond such mute positioning, we must consider
how the symbolization of identities can come about and be used to direct
the behavior of others and oneself.
Bourdieu (1977a) makes improvisation the predominant form of

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

279

Ex

am

Co

py

agency. He argues conclusively that it is fruitless to make up a set of


cultural rules to account for peoples behavior. The material and social
conditions of activity vary in a plenitude of ways; even in highly restrictive environments, it is difcult to avoid unusual combinations of people and things. Rules that handle all possible combinations in realworld settings are impossible to devise. Instead, Bourdieu suggests, our
bodies are repositories of a complex set of associationsof actions
(movements), gures (categories), and contexts (environments)sedimented from experiencing concrete instances of their combination, their
work together. More complicated experiences, sequences of joint action
coordinated among actors as a relation to particular qualities, such as
honor, produce sensitivities toward a set of culturally devised games or
contexts of action like our gured worlds, where there are general, dispositional motives and acts and ways of faring well and faring poorly.
Agency lies in the improvisations that people create in response to particular situations, mediated by these senses and sensitivities. They opportunistically use whatever is at hand to affect their position in the
cultural game in the experience of which they have formed these sets of
dispositions.
Bakhtin and Vygotsky, too, emphasize that we have our existence not
in repose but in practice. Bakhtin takes the culturally specic games that
Bourdieu elucidates (which compare to Bakhtins notion of speech genres), gives them voice, and directs them to a more general, even universal, condition of human activity. If we are alive, says Bakhtin, then we are
engaged in answering what is directed to us. We are always engaged in
the activity of making sense of what is happening as one who will respond. We are always authoring the meaning of action. If, for Bourdieu,
we can best understand cultures and the people who enact them by
considering behavior in the space of practice, then, for Bakhtin, we can
best understand languages and the selves constructed in and through the
words and voices of others by attending to the space of authoring. Improvisation using the resources at hand, and the limited agency improvisation entails, characterize both spaces. Furthermore, both spaces are
shot through with the activity of social positioning. Bourdieus action,
Bakhtins utterance, take place within an always present, partially durable construction of stratied social differences, positions, languages, and
other cultural media of action.
Vygotskys work, in comparison with that of Bourdieu and Bakhtin,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

280

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

often appears both socially naive and simplistic. Vygotsky neglected the
social forms and constraints to which the others attend so carefully. He
was nearly silent on the kinds of human domination that they reveal. He
focused instead on a fantastic, seemingly utopian and liberatory power
granted by symbols and the human ability to play with symbols: the
power to create worlds, effective contexts of action, that may never exist
apart from the pivot of imagination. Yet it is Vygotskys focus that enables
us to link Bourdieus space of practice with Bakhtins space of authoring
and so helps us envision this second kind of agency.
Human life is inexplicable without our abilities to gure worlds, play
at them, act them out, and then make them socially, culturally, and thus
materially consequential. This collective ability to take imaginary worlds
seriouslythe sort of fetishization that makes certain pieces of paper
over into moneyis the magic that anthropologists as well as others
have tried to capture in the concept of culture. Vygotsky was fascinated
by the human ability to escape the immediate control of environmental
stimuli and instead organize behaviors, thoughts, and feelings in relation
to imagined stimuli. Children early in their lives exhibit the ability and
the desire to enter into worlds that they imagine, to treat objects around
them as (regured) objects from those worlds, and to put aside, at least
temporarily, feelings, interests, and concerns that are irrelevant to the
play world. The play worlds that children create are akin to the more
complex culturally constructed or gured worlds that they enter into as
they grow older.
Human play, Vygotsky emphasized, develops the childs capacity for
semiotic mediation and makes it a part of the childs intimate world.
Through play people acquire the key cultural means by which they
escape, or at least reduce, the buffeting of whatever stimuli they encounter as they go through their days. Vygotsky stressed that only part of the
signicance of language, signs, and symbols was caught by their representational potential or their capacity for stating rules. Like Austin, he
believed that people do things with words, and that representation is
only one action, albeit an important one, that words make possible.
People use symbols: to organize and manage their own and others behavior, to intervene in what Bakhtin would call the worlds addressivity, to objectify, or concretize even in their literal absence, other contexts of activity. This is the reason we nd it important to think about
how cultural resources are used; for example, how the life narratives

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

281

am

Co

py

described in Chapter 4 are used in Alcoholics Anonymous to evoke and


shape members conceptions of themselves as non-drinking alcoholics.
Remembered stories, re-creations of ones own story, can be purposively
used to redirect oneself away from the impulse to take a drink, to interpose new actions, and to cast oneself as a new actor in a new social play.
Bourdieu calls attention to the taken-for-granted or out-of-awareness
associations between the important categories of social division and social spaces, body postures (hexis), the uses of colors, and so on. These
connections are especially important for understanding how relational
(positional) aspects of identity work. But worlds are gured through
language and images as well, and these means of objectication make
possible the kind of re-cognition that Kondo experienced when she
saw her reection (Chapter 6) and even the deliberate efforts of persons
and groups to direct their own behavior. Through the transguration
that commentary provides, people may even increase their awareness of,
and thus their capability to inhibit, such automatic responses as the
deferential body postures assumed in the presence of higher-status people. Vygotskys formulations, in short, direct us to attend to peoples
collective ability to imagine themselves in worlds that may yet be
scarcely realized, and to the modest ability of humans to manage their
own behavior through signs directed at themselves. These important
aspects of human agency need to be considered in tandem with the
kinds of improvisation that Gyanumaya, Roger, or the Tij groups accomplished. They both enable the creation of new worlds and new identities
and make us appreciate how gured (objectied) identities become important tools with which individuals and groups seek to manage one
another and their own behavior.

Ex

The Social and Political Work of Identities


Vygotsky recognized that tools of self-management are not the products
of personal invention. Instead they are cultural or collective resources
rst experienced by children and neophytes in social interaction. People
learn to use these signs historically, rst as parts of behavioral routines,
then as signs meaningful to others, and nally signs for directing their
own actions, managing their own feelings, and organizing their own
thoughts.
This is a basic process in the intimate formation of an identity. One

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

282

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

learns the identity of inscribed actsthe signs or markers of culturally


constructed identity, whether they be the display of particular skills, the
enactment of certain motives, the cultivation of ways of speaking, the use
of certain expressions, the display of certain emotions, or the wearing of
distinctive clothesin like manner. They are rst parts of an undistinguished behavioral routine, second a means to affect others, and third,
if the identity forms to this extent and especially if objectied in the
gured world, means to evoke ones own sense of who one is and so
organize ones behavior. The process Vygotsky describes is a developmental one that proceeds by converting signs into what we have called
heuristic devices: signal relations turned toward self-understanding.
Identities have, to our minds, principally this import: they are social
forms of organization, public and intimate, that mediate this development of human agency.
It is a process of personal formation that occurs via cultural resources
enacted in a social context. The person makes herself over into an
actor in a cultural world; she may even, over time, reach the point of
being able to evoke the world and her sense of herself within it without
the immediate presence of others. But the cultural gurings of selves,
identities, and the gured worlds that constitute the horizon of their
meaning against which they operate, are collective products. One can
signicantly reorient ones own behavior, and can even participate in the
creation of new gured worlds and their possibilities for new selves, but
one can engage in such play only as part of a collective. One can never
inhabit a world without at least the gural presence of others, of a social
history in person. The space of authoring, of self-fashioning, remains a
social and cultural space, no matter how intimately held it may become.
And, it remains, more often than not, a contested space, a space of
struggle.1
The way in which identities take an intimate form (which we discussed in Chapter 8) is still a political process. As we have shown, the
social value of various identitiesthe symbolic capital they command
is often contested interpersonally through the attempted denigration of
the discourses that posit them or the acts that sign them. Even in the
intimate venue of inner speaking and the imagination, however, the
signs and tools of self-formation are themselves likely to be inscribed
with social position. Bakhtins inspired notion of a dialogic self predicates the condition of self-formation in that space of authoring where

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

283

Ex

am

Co

py

(re)forming senses of self are addressed, even challenged, by the socially


inscribed voices that one (re)generates in inner speech. His notion of
authorship, in other words, often depends upon personalizing the contestations of identity that mark social speech.
Our discussion of the beginnings of the gured world of romance in
medieval Europes play world of courtly love (Chapter 11) looked back
to a collective or public space of authoring. Even though it was play,
many of the powerful found courtly love objectionable and unacceptable. Among its many transgressions, courtly love modeled the relation
of male lover to female beloved as the feudal tie of vassal to lord, an
inversion of the prevailing gender hierarchy that also mocked the bonds
of authority. Opponents wanted no space where gender and power relations were transgured in such a radically different way. Some of the
church hierarchy even attempted to brand proponents of courtly love as
heretics. Other writers inveighed against the effeminacy of its practitioners, and all but called for their conquest. Both groups saw their dreams
of eradication of this source of social heresy attempted (if not entirely
realized) in the Albigensian crusades.
A more recent struggle over acts and discourses, recounted in Chapter
7, similarly disputes the value of the identities they organize and the
standing of the people and groups of whom the identities are taken as a
sign. Judging by the amount of energy and attention directed to the
contestations over the discourse of sexual harassment, something must
be collectively at stake. The rancor aroused by the testimony of Anita
Hill in the Clarence Thomas hearings (Eisenhart and Lawrence 1994),
and even the ink spilled over the lesser case of J. Donald Silva, a University of New Hampshire professor who was charged by female students
with sexual harassment (Bernstein 1994), attest to the power of issues of
sexual harassment. These are trials, whether of a metaphorical or literal
variety, over the social positioning of specic acts and individuals. Silva
was accused of sexual harassment for acts such as giving out in his
classes the following example of a simile: Belly dancing is like jello on a
plate with a vibrator under the plate. He was at rst unrepentant and
deant. Months later, he said on a radio talk show: It makes one begin
to think one might be all the things that these people say that you are.
You begin to think of yourself as really a, you know, probably a . . . a . . .
a . . . a very, you know, evil teacher . . . which is frightening.2 And, as we
pointed out with the example of Mamets play Oleanna, there are also

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

284

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Ex

am

Co

py

trials over the authority to be given to women who try to wield this new
weapon and, thus by Bakhtins rule, even to the discourse itself.
The critical discourses of Tij songs (see Chapters 10 and 12) provide
another example of an identity politics that is both public and intimate
(see Holland and Skinner, 1995a). Several of the songbooks published
by various Communist parties around the time of the Tij festival of 1991
included forewords that invited readers to gure the world according
to a Marxian analysis of gender relations espoused by the sponsoring
parties. They did so in a manner that was not necessarily politic. They
branded the analyses of gender relations given in the older dukha songs
as correctly critical but as failing to understand the true source of
womens oppression in class dynamics. The rajniti songs published in the
books were designed to remedy that failed critique. The domestic world
gured in dukha songs and the village women marked by that world
were devalued, and a new division between them and their younger,
more educated, and more politically active sisters was opened up.
Because Tij songs are such important resources of inner speaking, this
division was felt and rehearsed personally and reproduced (in some parts
of Nepal, though not in Naudada) in interpersonal relations among the
group members. The feminist wings of the parties have grown sensitive
to these ill effects, and now seek to disseminate songs that unite the
communities that practice rajniti and dukha singing. These politics of
identication concerning (types of) people, the cultural resources they
produce, and the acts that are taken to signify particular identities characterize the social space of authoring. They provide generic and personied models, the voices that are re-created in inner speech, for the
complex interchange that makes up particular identities.
The social and political work of identication, in brief, reveals two
closely interrelated lessons. First, identities and the acts attributed to
them are always forming and re-forming in relation to historically specic contexts. They come to bear the marks of these contexts and their
politics. Identities and their cultural resources, their semiotic tools in
Vygotskys terms, are constantly being generated, sometimes censored or
suppressed. But they are responses to, develop in, and so include the
dilemmas set by the struggles, personal crises, and social recruitment
under which they form. The salience and authority of Rogers psychiatric identities grew as he regured a place for himself in the web of
familial and institutional relations disrupted by his mental disorder. Tij

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

285

Ex

am

Co

py

groups, especially the schoolgirls within them, responded to the new


openings made by the peoples movement of 1991 by expanding, in
rajniti songs, the scope of womens social and political worlds and the
identication of womens places in them. In doing so they inadvertently
created a split within the groups and within each of the members.
The second lesson is that identities form on intimate and social landscapes through time. The distinction of particular acts as indexical of an
identity, and the expansion of identities to include broader or different
ranges of acts, depend upon the work of the groups involved in the
relevant eld of activity, and the work of their allies and opponents. The
historical moment of this workthe conjuncture of the allied personal
and social histories that determines whether the identities posited by any
particular discourse become important and are worked into the everyday
lives of peoplecannot be simply dictated by the discourse, the cultural
form itself.3
Forming an identity on intimate landscapes takes time, certainly
months, often years. It takes (and makes) personal experience to organize a self around discourses and practices, with the aid of cultural resources and the behavioral prompting and verbal feedback of others. It
takes the heuristic developments of disposition and savoir faire to imagine the world and to identify with the gured world. Conceiving oneself
as an agent whose acts count in, and account for, the world cannot
happen overnight.
Forming an identity on social landscapes also takes timepublic and
institutional time. The dim images of fantasy worlds and the actors that
populate such worlds cannot be devised and translated into practice
from one week to the next. The work of reproduction and regeneration
of public life moves heuristically as well, through a kind of historically
contingent hybridization (as Bakhtin might call it). Cultural resources
take form from elements that may have been generated in other contexts
and brought together in the mix of struggles that may lack any logical
connection to the world being improvised in social play. Nor do these
worlds necessarily endure the transguration beyond the local sites of
their production. No constituency may coalesce to work them into different places, different landscapes of activity. Or an opposition may successfully choke them off. Just as an identity may never gain any prominence among a persons organizations of behavior, a fantasized world
may never gain any purchase on the sociohistorical landscape. Its gur-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

286

Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds

Subjectivities across Figured Worlds

py

ings and the identities afforded by them may simply not be taken up, as
Andrew refused to learn the world according to Alcoholics Anonymous
and to gure himself in the shapes of its narratives. Worlds and identities
can be eetinga moments playor they can eventually become as central to personal and public life as the (once courtly) world of romance.

Ex

am

Co

If persons and gured worlds are in the making over months, years,
decades, even centuries, then so must be the groups with which people
afliate. Critics of the concepts of culture successfully argued, during the
period we call the critical disruption (see Chapter 2), that social positions cannot be disregarded in any cultural guring of the world of
human action. Accounts that conceived cultures as shared webs of meaning, in which all are equally suspended regardless of such structured
social divisions as gender, class, and race, were called into serious question. This recognition of the importance of social position in shaping
peoples experience led to experiments in ethnography. This trend was
marked, for example, in gender studies descriptions of womens cultures
and socialities, of gendered worlds; in British cultural studies emphasis on subcultures, whether based on gender, race, class, or generation;
and in Bourdieus even more ambitious concepts of class (and potentially
other forms of) habitus.
Yet, as proponents of these approaches have noted, perspectives predicating clearly differentiable arrays of social forces, even those privileging
class dynamics as a denitive social and cultural divider, have proven too
limiting. Social positions cut and cut again across one another. Womens
experience in the social life of the United States is not only different for,
say, black women and white women; each of these groups is further
divided by class differences, by differences of sexual orientation, by ethnicities, by the gamut of social dividers that have become common in
postmodern sociologies.
Our use of identitiesinformed by these two dependent, but noncoincident processes, guring and positionalityleads to another way of
conceptualizing personhood, culture, and their distributions over social
groups. Figured worlds and their situated realizations, rendered collectively and personally as spaces of authoring, are socially animated by
groupings that may not be reied as social groups. The politics of partici-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Identity in Practice

287

Ex

am

Co

py

pation in gured worldsfor example, who may enter the activities of


an AA chapter and how members gain or lose place among themselves
may not reproduce a group according to the categories of currency in
social positioning. Furthermore, these same politics may bring together
persons who share little else: few means of guring common action and
few of the common markers of social position. As Bourdieus elds are
meant to be, though in an even less structured and durable form, our
spaces of authoring are games peculiar to themselves.4 Yet, as the examples of courtly love and nationalisms attest, these spaces have the potential to expand, and their players may become social groupings and categories of newer currency.
Likewise persons may be stretched over times and spaces, fully active
in some worlds, perhaps scarcely formed in others. Bakhtins dialogism
lives within this understanding. These elds and spaces of human activity work in the recognition of an otherness that reaches beyond their
specic polities to other ways of guring activity. The worlds address is
both generic and specic, and only in the transit between the two aspects can we craft an answer to it. So we cannot deny what we might
call the power of culture. And this is as true for a single person as it is
for the collective subjects; the sites of consciousness lie within a thoroughly social world. Figured worlds, the politics of social positioning,
and spaces of authoring are our attempts to conceptualize collective and
personal phenomena in ways that match the importance of culture in
contextualizing human behavior with the situating power of social position. Identities are our way of guring the interfaces among these dimensions of collective life; our way of naming the places where society organizes persons and persons in turn reorganize, albeit in modest steps,
societies; the pivots of our lived worlds.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Notes

1. The Woman Who Climbed up the House

Ex

am

Co

1. All persons names and many place names are pseudonyms to preserve
the anonymity of those who helped us in our research.
2. McCall and Simmons (1978) represent an important Meadean vein that
inspired us originally. That literature now includes an expanded and
elaborated vision of the social and cultural bases for possible identities.
Besides social roles, cross-cutting master statuses of race, ethnicity,
class, gender, and sexuality are important bases (McCall 1987; Stryker
1987), as are salient cultural conceptions of personality (e.g., an aggressive person).
3. In the United States, at least, the center of gravity of cultural studies is
currently in the humanities. This book is part of an effort to extend
cultural studies through more sociological and ethnographic work.
4. This tradition was centered at Marburg (see Holquist 1990). Bakhtins
early works (1990, 1993) developed a phenomenological aesthetics (or
better, perhaps, an aesthetic phenomenology) through a technical, philosophical language and argument more or less faithful to the Marburg
position (see Chapter 9).
5. Self and identity are loaded terms. In Chapter 2 we place our use of
them in the context of the literature. For the present, let us borrow Harrs
(1984) notion of the self as the (theoretical or presumptive) collocation of
a personal repertory. The self is the place from which persons view or
sense the world and reect upon it. There is no presumption that a person
has only one important sense of self; or an impetus to consistency across
these different senses of self. We are not concerned here with the psychodynamic dilemmas that occupied Erikson but rather with the selves
that are assembled from, and in relation to, cultural resources.
6. This book is a collective product, including the analyses of the experiences we describe, but from time to time we speak from the perspective of
one person.
7. The incident happened in 1986. Note that, although Holland had already

289
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

290

read the convincing theoretical treatises of Bourdieu (1977a) arguing for a


shift away from culturalist views in the eld, she still rst went through a
culturalist interpretation.
Hegemony is a much debated concept. We refer here to something like
the hegemonic practices discussed by R. W. Connell (1987: 183): Hegemony means . . . a social ascendancy achieved in a play of social forces
that extends beyond contests of brute power into the organization of
private life and cultural processes. Ascendancy . . . achieved at the point
of a gun, or by the threat of unemployment, is not hegemony. Ascendancy
which is embedded in religious doctrine and practice, mass media content, wage structures, the design of housing, welfare/taxation policies and
so forth, is. Hegemony is also not total domination. What is hegemonic
is contested and variable according to situation.
One cannot, Lutz says, reduce the social claim to a contingent or secondary matter. Rather, social status is one of the things emotion is about
among the Ifaluk.
The extent to which womens ways of speaking in the United States are
characteristic of a womens culture or of womens position in society is
disputed; see Chapter 7.
Here we have set up ideal types. Some constructivists emphasize positioning to the point that they convey a radical constructivist perspective (see,
e.g., Davies and Harr 1990; Harr and Van Langenhove 1991).
The school was another such place (see Skinner and Holland 1996). Also,
caste restrictions were not observed in the more illicit arenas of drinking
and gambling.
Others have made similar arguments, e.g., Begona Aretxaga (1993: 231):
As an anthropologist I am interested in cultural formations of meaning
and their articulation through personal experience because it is at this
intersection at which cultural constructions blend together with unique
personal (or collective) experience that models of feeling are shaped and
new meanings created.

10.

11.

12.

Ex

13.

am

9.

Co

py

8.

Notes to Pages 1119

2. A Practice Theory of Self and Identity

1. This denition is roughly built from characteristics shared by the many


meanings of self in the anthropological literature. It alludes to a fuller
denition given by Ito (1987): Self has (1) a reexive quality, the ability to
distinguish, evaluate, and objectify self and other; and (2) a dynamic
quality, the apperceptive ability to understand, interpret, manipulate, and
incorporate sensory judgments. Self is both executor and object of judgments, discriminations, creativity, and order. Besides Ito, Skinner (1990),

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 1922

4.

Ex

5.

am

Co

3.

White (1992), Harris (1989), and Fogelson (1979) review the anthropological uses of self, identity, person, and individual.
Nepal is home to many groups, whose history and social position vary
widely. Their cultural practices, including self-related discourses, also
vary. Skinners and Hollands research was carried out in a different area of
Nepal with people who, for the most part, were not Newar.
Concepts that might be translated as self in South Asia, including Nepal, are complex. A Sanskrit term, atma or atman, is sometimes translated
as self or soul. This term, deriving from Hindu religious and philosophical texts and discourses, is sometimes used by nonspecialists to
speak of an aspect of themselves that continued on in a cycle of death and
rebirth. In Naudada, where Skinner and Holland did their research among
mixed-caste Nepali speakers, people made more use of concepts having to
do with what we will later dene as self-in-practice. Skinner (1990: 21)
writes: There is no noun I heard used to indicate this notion of an
empirical self [self-in-action], but there are pronouns (e.g., aphu, which is
used as a reexive pronoun to refer to ones own self, mero or my, ma or
I), that an individual would use to refer to herself as an actor in the world
and to reect on her actions. Man was also an important concept in
Naudada (see Chapter 12); it is often glossed as heart/mind. The man is
where thoughts and feelings are stored and churn about. It is located in
the center of the chest. McHugh reported that Gurungs, an ethnic group
living primarily in another region of the country, used the term sae to
mean the seat of will, memory, and emotion . . . conceived as a kind of
entity located in the chest (1989: 82). She found the concept to be an
important one that individuals use to articulate their own will, thoughts,
and emotions.
For reviews see Harris (1989); Marsella, DeVos, and Hsu (1985); Levine
(1982); Shweder and Bourne (1984); White (1992).
The slight differences between the culturalist position we describe here
and that set out in Chapter 1 are artifacts of the opposition of culturalists
to constructivists, rather than to universalists. In Chapter 1 we assumed
the effects of the critical disruption that gave birth to constructivism.
A proposition that psychologists such as Markus and Kitayama (1991),
Kitayama, Markus, and Lieberman (1994), and Miller (1988) have set out
to test through quasi-experimental designs.
This was why Hallowell argued that self was a better focus for cross-cultural research than ego or personality: The term self . . . seem[s] to
connote a concept that remains closer to the phenomenological facts that
reect mans self-awareness as a generic psychological attribute. It retains
the reexive connotation that is indicated when we say that a human

py

2.

6.

7.

291

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

10.

11.

Ex

12.

py

9.

individual becomes an object to himself, that he identies himself as an


object among other objects in his world, that he can conceive himself, not
only as a whole, but in terms of different parts, that he can converse with
himself, and so on (1955b: 80; see Skinner 1990).
He called these invariant characteristics orientations. In a manner similar
to Neissers (1988) delineation of different forms of self-knowledge, Hallowell (1955a) identied ve such invariants: self-orientation, object-orientation, spatio-temporal orientation, motivational orientation, and normative or moral orientation.
Shweder (1991) muddies the universalist-culturalist waters by asking us
to problematize what we consider natural. He proposes that demons and
other constructions we deem unnatural may, in fact, be parts of the natural environment that we do not acknowledge because of our cultural
lenses. He chides Obeyesekere for not considering the possibility that the
malevolent ancestral spirits his informants tell him about do exist and
can get into ones body, . . . and that the cultural representation of their
existence and a persons experience of their existence lights up an aspect
of reality that has import for the management of the self (346347).
Sahlins (1995) also takes Obeyesekere (1992) to task for a kind of ethnocentrism.
For a more hopeful vision see Haraway (1988), who disavows radical
forms of the social-constructivist criticism of science and opts for a successor science project that offers a more adequate, richer, better account
of a world in order to live in it well, one that has simultaneously an
account of radical historical contingency for all knowledge claims and
knowing subjects, a critical practice for recognizing our own semiotic
technologies for making meanings, and a no-nonsense commitment to
faithful accounts of a real world (579).
Especially relevant here is the construction of the psychological in
modernization theory. Such theoriesof achievement motivation, modernizing personalities, and the likewere complicit in rationalizing and
depoliticizing processes of colonization.
They include, e.g., images of the other in National Geographic (Lutz and
Collins 1993), life-story genres in Alcoholics Anonymous (Cain 1991 and
Chapter 5), specialist discourses on the self in the anthropology and
psychology of the 1950s, and performances such as the Balinese cockght
made famous by Geertz (1973a).
Kenneth Gergen, a social psychologist, pursues other aspects of social
constructionism related to our work: Narratives of the self are not personal impulses made social, but social processes realized on the site of the
personal (1994: 210). His pointabout the guring of self within social

Co

8.

Notes to Pages 2227

am

292

13.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 2831

relationshipsis like Bakhtins notion of dialogism, which we consider in


Chapter 8.
Dennett (1988) argues that the self is an abstractum, a ctionbut a
convenient one. He suggests an analogy between selves and centers of
gravity. A chair, say, has no tangible, but only a virtual, center of gravity.
Still, the gurative reication is useful in describing the behavior of chairs
in many circumstances. Likewise, self is a popular ction, a gurative
reication, by means of which we account for our and others actions.
Dennetts notion is in a sense constructivist, in that the self is a dependent
gure of our ethnopsychological discourse. Yet it has a kind of persistence and a transitive, cross-situational quality, indeed almost an inevitable or natural aspect. It is difcult to imagine how to interact with, or
speak about, people without such a ction of integrity. Harr (1979,
1984) has presented the most extensive argument for a ctive notion of
self, which is at the same time a virtually necessary consequence of the
deictic character of social interactivity. See the discussion of Bakhtin in
Chapter 8.
Studies critical of psychology and psychological anthropology are an obvious result of the critical disruption. Works by psychologists (such as
Henriques et al. 1984 and Parker 1992) and the writings of the critical
psychologists (Tolman and Maiers 1991) parallel critical works by
anthropologists writing on psychological topics. Scheper-Hughess (1992)
analysis of the Brazilian folk idiom of nervios is an example, as are
Lutzs (1988) critiques of scientic discourses on emotion and research
(in press) on the effects of U.S. military funding in the 1950s on subsequent directions of psychological research. Critical psychological anthropology, critical psychology, and critical social psychology (Wexler
1983; Gergen 1984, 1994) are important because they disallow innocentseeming and apolitical views of the conceptual tools and discourses that
researcher/practitioners use. They have a more specic import for theories of the self, suggesting a revised theoretical orientation that admits
power relations.
Although, as Scheper-Hughes (1992) points out, such theory mostly
awaits inventiona task we undertake in this book.
Reication of this sort not only creates an impression of immunity to
social and historical change but also overlooks a crucial heterogeneity.
Reication obviates the recognition that social conditions themselves
may be changing. Markus and Kitayama (1991) point out that scientic
terms for independent self-processes, such as self-disclosure, are recent.
They interpret this as a matter of delayed discovery. Our guess is that the
creation of more and more terms that assume independent selves is both

Ex

16.

am

15.

Co

py

14.

293

17.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

19.

20.
21.

22.

py

Ex

23.

responsive to and creative of historical conditions (see Gergen 1991 for


an analogous point about concepts of mental deciency). The discourses
and practices that construct selves as independent may be more powerful
in the United States now than they were fty years ago. Holland and
Kipnis (1994) argue that both modes of self-organization, sociocentric
and egocentric, are evident in American society. Perhaps independence is
a more pervasive mode at present, but to read pervasiveness as essence
obscures recognition that the relative salience of the two modes is a matter of tension, if not struggle.
Efforts to study members of the same social position simply denedas
women, sayhave proven problematic. Social positions are multidimensional, and their context is too important to ignore. One solution has
been to focus on historically and socially constituted groups relevant to
the context in question, avoiding any assumptions about the groups homogeneity (or representativeness).
Seminal examples are Rosaldo (1980) and Crapanzano (1980). AbuLughod (1986), Kondo (1990), and White (1991) extend (and criticize)
these. A recent example is Holland and Skinner (1995a). For more discussion, see Holland (1997).
Others use this concept or emphasize a similar idea, e.g., Ewing (1990);
McHugh (1989); Mines (1988); Parish (1991).
Whether the psychodynamic account of history-in-person, to which Hall
alludes, and our sociohistorical account can contribute to each other
remains to be seen. Bakhtin (see Voloshinov 1987) and Vygotsky had
complex and complicated relations with psychodynamic theory, and kept
their distance from it (van der Veer and Valsiner 1991). They give us little
help with this question. But we have little reason to exclude the classic
psychodynamic dilemmas as sources of contingencies with which the
authoring self must contend.
See Wertsch (1985b) and Clark and Holquist (1984) for biographical
sketches of Vygotsky and Bakhtin, respectively.
Bourdieus emphasis on embodiment at the expense of symbols has
helped move anthropologists away from the notion of culture as beliefs
as linguistically mediated, symbolically encoded conceptions and convictions about the worldand toward a notion of culture as dispositional,
out-of-awareness orientations toward the world (see Strauss 1992). With
semiotic mediation, Bourdieu and the sociohistorical school diverge in
emphasis. Bourdieu alludes to symbolic mastery in Outline of a Theory
of Practice (1977a: 83), but does not develop the concept. In The Logic of
Practice (1990b) he says enough to indicate that his idea of symbolic
mastery is of a representational rather than a heuristic sort. His symbolic

Co

18.

Notes to Pages 3135

am

294

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 3839

26.

27.

py

Ex

28.

Co

25.

mastery is a rationalist view of symbolic mediation and does not capture


what Vygotsky was describing. For a Vygotskian semiotic mediator, the
important fact is its investment within the pragmatics of communication,
which includes its representation. Heuristic development is replete with
symbolic formsforms whose meaning is not solely, or even principally, representational.
Training wheels is Hubert Dreyfuss metaphor (personal communication).
By distributed over others we mean both that ones use of the tools depends upon the collaboration of others and that the ability to sustain
ones own behavior under ordinary conditions is continually reconstituted by collectively produced practices. Beckers (1963) study of marijuana users in the early 1950s provides a good example: neophytes were
often dependent upon collective interpretations to experience pot smoking as having a noticeable effect and as being pleasurable. And even the
old hands, during bad trips or under intense social criticism, had to
turn to their fellows for reinterpretations and reasons to continue (see
Chapter 5).
There are major arguments regarding the relationship of Leontiev and
Vygotsky (see van der Veer and Valsiner 1991; Kozulin 1986). Van der
Veer and Valsiner (1994: 5) state that in the literature interest in
Leontevs activity theory spilt over to Vygotsky (as Leontev himself
claimed direct heritage from Vygotskys and Lurias cultural-historical theorya claim much disputed and proven questionable . . .). Though we
recognize the need for accurate attributions of intellectual genealogy, we
believe their conceptions are compatible and more usefully taken in tandem than alone.
Leontievs position differs from the way we render culture. What is important here is his distinction between the necessity of three versus two terms
in accounts of behavior.
Bourdieu develops ideas about specic practices less than activity
theorists develop ideas about specic activities. Sometimes he writes of
games (e.g., 1990b); game as he uses the term is somewhat equivalent
to activity.
The relationship of Bakhtins work to his associates in the Bakhtin circle
is a matter of much debate (Clark and Holquist 1984; Todorov 1984;
Titunik 1987; Holquist 1990; Morson and Emerson 1990). We follow
Todorov (1984) and refer to Bakhtin-plural, that is, his circle inclusive,
when we say Bakhtin. We agree with Todorovs defense of shared authorship as eminently Bakhtinian. Nonetheless, for purposes of citation, we
list only the author under whose name the work was originally published.

am

24.

29.

295

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

296

Notes to Pages 4052

am

Co

py

30. Similar processes of development occur for groups; we primarily address


persons here for convenience.
31. Although we use identity here as a cover term to refer to the senses and
understandings that one has, no matter how symbolically (un)developed
they are, it will later become clear that our prototypic identity is one that
is symbolized.
32. Holland and Reeves (1994) describe perspective not only as the angle
from which one views the world (through ones position in activity) but
also as ones take on activity, the understanding one comes to have of
activity (from ones position in it). They emphasize that the objectication and development of a perspective are collective, historical, and contingent achievements. In this view, perspectives are always at least an
incipient, though often as yet inarticulate or unsymbolized, identity.
33. Especially in practice theory, these aims are conceived to take form out of
awareness. They nonetheless constitute a point of view. See Bourdieu
(1977a; 1990b).
34. For Bourdieu, social and material conditions can change rapidly, affecting the interrelations of positions in the eld of production. Forms of
behavior are modied in response to the altered conditions, but the habitus is slower to change and the lag between the two produces hysteresis
(1977a: 78).
35. To what extent the various identities are organized coherently depends
upon the collective resources that exist and the persons successful use of
them. In the prototypical life stories of Alcoholics Anonymous, for example, participants are exposed to a symbolic means that orders the gamut of
their different identities. They are encouraged to be, rst and foremost,
non-drinking alcoholics. This identication supersedes all others, even
religious identities or potential identities as drug users.

3. Figured Worlds

Ex

1. The anthropological examples are vast: every ethnography tries to capture at least one signicant gured world. Two fascinating examples are
Luhrmanns work with witches in contemporary Great Britain (1989)
and Lancasters account of machismo and male sexualities in Nicaragua
(1992).
2. Vygotsky and the sociohistorical school made much of the childs dependence upon social interaction as both the source of cultural symbols for
imagination and the initial support for enacting the symbols.
3. An important pivot into gured worlds is through discourses. In any
conversation or text a great deal of taken-for-granted information is omitted. This talk, the particular material and social conditions in which it is

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 5355

297

5.

Ex

6.

am

4.

Co

py

being used (Holland and Cole 1995), and the more generalized notion of
discourse are made more determinant in relation to an underlying cultural model (Quinn and Holland 1987). Cultural model directs attention to the ways in which individuals come to know and sense gured
worlds. Dened cognitively, cultural models consist of schemas (mental/emotional knowledge structures) that guide attention to, draw inferences about, and evaluate experience. They also provide a framework
for organizing and reconstructing memories of experience (DAndrade
1995). The types of cultural models most closely related to gured worlds
are what Leavitt (1996) calls prototypical social situations or scenarios.
Lutz (1988) and Lutz and Abu-Lughod (1990) argue that emotions have
meaning in relation to such simplied social scenes, imagined courses
of social interaction, or what Quinn and Holland (1987) and Holland and
Kipnis (1994) call prototypical event sequences. We believe that types of
people are identied and have meaning in relation to gured worlds.
DAndrade (1981) calls this kind of abstraction, through which persons
form (generalized) cultural knowledges, guided discovery, and contrasts its slow and natural course with the articial means of specialized and highly regulated knowledges. The latter are particularly dependent upon verbal commentary and formal instruction. Vygotskys zone of
proximal development casts a different light upon this distinction, since
it takes the heart of instruction, the verbal commentary of elders, right
into everyday discovery in the form of inner speech.
Harr and others have used the concept of storylines in a way that is
relevant to gured worlds. Storylines seem to be the taken-for-granted
unfolding of particular activities such as instruction. There are many
storylines associated with gured worlds. One might consider what we
have called the central narratives in the world of romance and the world
of domestic relations in Nepal storylines that are especially important.
These arrests recall what Bakhtin (1981) called centripetal social
forces: those forms of (meta)practice (e.g., translinguistics) which asserted the presumptive unity of language, in the face of an always overwhelming diversity of speaking practices. The point, for Bakhtin as for
Crapanzano, is the dialogicality of social activity, the mutual interplay of
specic instance and generic means, neither of which exists apart from
the other.
This is not an unusual occurrence in anthropological research. Holland,
in studying Shango in Trinidad, traveled around the island to many different feasts and celebrations, learning from participants about the African
powers that manifested at the feasts. In the world of Shango, gaining
knowledge of this sort was a meaningful act. To become expert in Shango
it was necessary to enter into a reciprocal relationship with the powers.

7.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

10.

Ex

11.

py

9.

Eventually, at one of the feasts, a power manifested and directed Holland


to give a Thanksgiving (a celebration of lesser scale than a feast); that is,
to enter into a more proper relationship with the powers. All of us were
more or less drawn into the gured worlds we studied and began to form
identities in those worlds.
In later chapters we describe womens resistance to the treatment they
received and their criticisms of the world of romance.
These commonsensical exemplars of activities are reminiscent of
Wittgensteins chosen illustrations of language games. In either case,
the rhetorical point calls attention to the concepts scopethat it refers
not to some specialized or technical form of life, but to the virtual worlds
entirety.
See also Wulffs (1995) concept of microcultures. Other concepts we
draw upon to explicate gured worldse.g., elds, activities, power
systems (Lancaster 1992), discourses, communities of practice
posit collective complexes of meaning and action. However, unlike older
notions of culture indicated by such phrases as American culture or
Hindu culture, these concepts are partial and modest. Their proponents
make no claim that such complexes of meaning and action are essential or
ubiquitous, that they make up a way of life or are even necessarily tied
to a particular (ethnic) group. Rejecting the earlier assumption that these
complexes of conventions are equally accepted by all who participate in
them (Amit-Talai and Wulff 1995), they place these conventions in practice, looking at the ways in which people use them to organize, dispute,
negotiate, and often impose or resist sets of joint activities. These newer
means for conceptualizing lived worlds attend to relations of power and
control. They posit that cultural frames of interpretation and evaluation
are seldom socially neutral, and that, even if they were, such neutrality would itself be a historical (and potentially mutable) product of political life.
Engestrm pays a great deal of attention to the social-theoretical dimension of activity theory. Wertsch has also developed and extended this
perspective. He emphasizes ways in which broader systems of privilege
enter into activities, such as reading groups in schools (1991). Many
proponents of activity theory, however, omit these connections; see Holland and Reeves (1994).
As a eld is partly a space of possibles, against which actual social
positions are matched and valued, it also explicitly includes the imaginative, as-if character of gured worlds.
Bourdieu (1985a) acknowledges Webers concept as suggestive in his
thinking.

Co

8.

Notes to Pages 5659

am

298

12.

13.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 5960

299

Ex

am

Co

py

14. We have Americanized references to the academic world.


15. For the sake of contrasting eld and gured world, we downplay one
of Bourdieus main points: that status is multidimensional, the value of its
various dimensions itself constituting a source of struggle.
16. Field, as Bourdieu has used it in his sociological studies of French intellectual production, encompasses the interconnections among related
gured worlds. Bourdieu is careful to point out that semi-autonomous
eldsof academic and artistic production, e.g.are embedded and
limited in social space by the eld of power and the political-economic
system of class relations (see esp. Bourdieu 1993). The embedding notion
is important for us as well. We are suggesting that gured worlds could be
described as nodes that together make up the elds that Bourdieu analyzes.
17. Bourdieu in other works attends to the effects of elds of power on life in
the gured worlds embedded in them. He attends, for instance, to the
ways day-to-day school activities (e.g., grading exams) transmute signs of
privilege or elite background into intelligence and thus into a symbolic
capital that gures in the reproduction of elite status and access to power
and wealth (see Bourdieu and Saint-Martin 1974). He also offers a powerful commentary on the ways status pervades linguistic practicesuch as
how ones sense of the (lesser) value attributed to ones dialect or style of
speaking can lead to silences in, or withdrawal from, interpersonal encounters (Bourdieu 1977b).
18. Bourdieus writings on elds (collected in Bourdieu 1993) are also
suggestive for gured worlds in other ways. They remind us that the
lived worlds of romance, Alcoholics Anonymous, or gender relations in
Naudadaas only somewhat independent sites of cultural production
intersect with and are affected by social actions carried out in the name of
other lived worlds. Holland and Skinner (1995a) reveal the intersection
between the lived worlds of domestic gender relations and political activity in a community in Nepal; Holland and Reeves (1994) argue that students perspectives on classroom activities were not formed by what the
teacher said but by discourses coming from outside the classroom.
19. It is worth reiterating how gured world departs from the older concept
of a groups culture. It adheres more specically to activity and therefore
does not imply that the works and persons created and re-created by a
particular group will exhibit the same principles across all settings. Nor
does it imply that a particular gured world incites the same level of
participation from all people of a country or an area. With todays transportation and communication, participants from many places can mingle
in a number of activities that realize gured worlds across many notional,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

21.

22.

py

Ex

23.

social, and political boundaries. Moreover, participants in these worlds


are identied with and knowledgeable about them to greater and lesser
degrees (see Chapter 5) and develop different perspectives on them (see
Chapter 7).
This discussion of cultural artifacts resonates with the approach to cultural forms taken in cultural studies. There the focus has shifted from
culture as some sort of whole to cultural textswhere text is a term
with a very broad meaning. The texts of cultural studies still include
written representations, but they also encompass lms, television programs, travel advertisements, and popular song genres. For example, in
Lutz and Collinss (1993) analysis of photographs of non-Westerners
in National Geographic or Schade-Poulsons (1995) work on rai music in
Algeria, cultural forms are analyzed for the theory of the task (their
purpose) and theory of the person (who will read them) that they
embed. See Johnson (198687) for a critique of those who analyze
texts (cultural forms) without regard for their collective history and
present use.
The properties of artifacts apply equally whether one is considering language and genres of speech and action or the more usually noted forms
of artifacts such as tables and knives. What differentiates a word such
as table or a social routine such as setting the table from a table is
the relative prominence of their material and conceptual aspects. The
difference remains relative, however, and not essential to the artifact. The
word table has no existence apart from its material instantiation (as a
conguration of sound waves, hand movements, or writing), and every
table, in addition to its duration over time as a physical object, has,
equally indicative of its artifactuality, a past embedded in collective memory so that it embodies an order imposed by thinking human beings.
On this model of cognition in psychology, especially educational psychology, see Lave (1988).
Words themselves do not carry meaning (Davies and Harr 1990: 57).
They must be related to a conceptual frame, or in our terms, a gured
world.
As Inden (1990: 23) called the three orientations of persons in practices.

Co

20.

Notes to Pages 6266

am

300

24.

4. Personal Stories in Alcoholics Anonymous

1. For this eldwork, Cain attended open meetings of three separate AA


groups in a small city in North Carolina in 1985 and 1986. All names
have been changed and identifying information disguised or omitted.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 68103

301

Co

py

2. As we shall see, personal knowledge of this sort, gained in the absence of


an identication with a gured world, is both supercial, poorly attached
affectively and conatively to ones actions, and mechanically and laboriously produced, step by step, from rules and maxims.
3. These two aspects are analytical distinctions, not requirements for membership. Ofcially, the only requirement for membership is a desire to
stop drinking.
4. Here cultural model is used in the sense of the taken-for-granted
standard scenarios, what Bakhtin calls chronotopes (time-spaces),
that inform the narrative possibilities within a gured world.
5. To protect the condentiality of the AA members Cain spoke with, we
have avoided including information from which outsiders might be able
to recognize them. Preventing other AA members from recognizing them
is more difcult. Much of the information is known to other members
already, but some of the information from the interviews may not be.
Where we suspect this may be the case, we have omitted the information.
6. In the rst part of the interview Cain asked the Who am I? questions of
Kuhn and McPartland (1954).

5. How Figured Worlds of Romance Become Desire

Ex

am

1. The names of the universities and all personal names are pseudonyms.
Some potentially identifying details in the quotes have been changed.
Although we sometimes use the term American, the sample is small and
from the Southeast. There were differences between the black and white
womens interpretations of romantic relationships, but they are not relevant here (see Holland and Eisenhart 1990).
2. See Chapter 3 on the concept of cultural model and its relation to the
notion of a gured world. Here, as in Chapter 4, we sometimes use cultural model in the more specic sense of the taken-for-granted standard
social scenarios that form backdrops of meaning for a gured world.
3. Because few men were interviewed in the earlier study at SU, we describe the model as one held by women. Women at Bradford used similar
sorts of terms and seemed to rely upon similar ideas about attractiveness
and the respective roles of men and women, but were more likely to
emphasize self-determination and self-protection in their interpretation
of male/female relationships (see Holland and Eisenhart 1990). We did
not happen to have any women in our study who identied themselves as
lesbian. Later studies of the world of romance for gay and lesbian couples
on one of the campuses suggest both similarities with and differences
from the one we describe (Reidel, personal communication).

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

302

Notes to Pages 107128

Ex

am

Co

py

4. Both Della and Karla were embarrassed and offended by being cast into
the world of gender relations in what they considered to be inappropriate
situations by men they considered inappropriate partners. While most of
the interpretations of them as potential romantic/sexual partners were not
taken amiss, these were. The women were learning of their vulnerability
to romantic/sexual typing regardless of their wishes to be so typed (see
Holland and Eisenhart 1990).
5. Markus and Nurius (1987) argue that individuals form personalized images of themselves in various situationse.g., visions of themselves receiving a Nobel Prize. These visions motivate actions that seek to realize
these possible selves. There also is evidence that some images of possible
selves are negatively evaluated, so that one works to avoid their realization.
6. As mentioned earlier, the African-American women at Bradford were concerned about maintaining self-determination and control. Since knowledge about ones behavior and feelings can be used by others to manipulate one, having a steady boyfriendas opposed to going out with a
number of boyfriendsbecomes a way to control information.
7. Susans implied commentary, carried partly through her sarcastic tone,
calls to mind the space of authoring selves (see Chapter 8).
8. Susans process of learning is a good example of the appropriation and
personalization of social discourse (see Chapter 8).
9. This notion of fossilization as habituation is also pertinent to Bourdieus
conception of the habitus, and of the processes of embodiment that
ground learning in the habitus. It suggests, for instance, that nonverbal
(proprioceptive) forms of practice interact with verbally mediated practice in the reproduction of the habitus more intimately, and perhaps more
convertibly, than Bourdieu seems to think.
10. In other words, expertise is far more than cognition. It involves emotions
that are, in Rosaldos (1984: 143) words, embodied thoughts, thoughts
seeped with the apprehension that I am involved.

6. Positional Identities

1. One reader of this chapter, a man whod grown up in Nepal, remarked


that this sort of extreme scolding is unusual in his part of the country. Part
of Tila Kumaris anger and willingness to express it seemed to relate to her
higher-caste status (Bahun) relative to the girls (Damai).
2. Harr and van Langenhove (1991) and Davies and Harr (1990) propose
a framework that intersects in some regards with ours. Their concept of
positionings does not analytically separate the gurative from the positional as we do. Nonetheless, Harr and von Langenhove briey discuss

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 130136

5.

6.

7.

py

Ex

8.

Co

4.

peoples relative power to position themselves and others. Their conversational dominance and conversational charisma relate to our notion
of positionality.
Brown (1980) comes to a similar conclusion about Tenejapa Maya
women. Women tend to use linguistic patterns of negative politeness (as
Brown calls strategies such as indirection, formality, and deference) more
frequently than men in many different situations. Brown concludes that
women, because of their position of lesser privilege than men, often take
special care not to cause offense. Their sense of their social position is
evidently important across several gured worlds.
Skinner learned that, in the schools of Naudada, students who thought of
themselves as progressive indicated their rejection of the hierarchy of
castes by accepting food and water from one another regardless of caste.
Others who picture social and cultural life as organized around semi-independent spheres include, besides Bourdieu (1993), Connell (1987),
whose idea of gender regimes combines development in local institutions and settings, themselves intricately related, with their ties to state
interventions, to make up a gender order.
Fordham primarily emphasizes race and gender. She does say that race
undercuts class in African-American communities, so that two dissimilar class structures, Euro- and African-American, are created.
Gregor tells of a woman who taunted the men back; she became the target
of a rumor that she had entered the mens house. Although the men,
individually, were reluctant to carry out the threat of gang rape, they
persuaded one another that the act was necessary and carried it out.
Lave and Wenger (1991) argue that social position species knowledge in
what they call communities of practice. Knowledge differs for the different positions of such communities and must, in a sense, be reconstructed
when one moves from one position to another. For instance, knowledge
of an academic topic must be recongured when one moves from student
to teacher, because the social claims that one makes in the expression of
knowledge cannot be extricated from its content to create any invariant
form of meaning.
Knowledge display is not the only skill with signing import that is learned
in exclusive activities. Brenneis (1990) makes the point that emotional
experiences are also collectively developed in certain activities. Those
who are barred from these activities are unlikely to develop the emotional
experiences to the same degree of facility as those who fully participate. If
expressing a certain emotion competently is treated as a claim to a certain
positional identity, then one is unlikely to be able to claim that identity if
one has been barred from the formative activity.
The situation is more complex and uid than we have indicated. The

am

3.

9.

10.

303

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

12.

13.

py

Ex

14.

relationship between index and position is to some extent a transitive


one. Signs and people rub off on one another. The sign mediates who
the person is, and the person mediates the meaning of the sign. In U.S.
high schools, for example, clothing styles worn by popular people attain
the aura of the popular people.
One benet of analyzing conversations according to the social actions of
positioning that are taking place is the illumination of the social scientists
own conversations with people in her study. Our interviews with Karla,
Della, and the other women were subject to the same processes we describe here. Thus, in Karlas case, the interview situation was such that an
image of her was produced, a representation of a woman who was active
in structuring her romantic relationships. See Crapanzano (1980), who
describes a common process in which the persona collaboratively produced in interaction is attributed to the other as essential characteristics.
While it is more convenient at this point to pose these arguments in
relation to individuals, it is perhaps even more signicant that counterpositionings are also produced collectively. Willis (1981), in his ethnography of working-class boys in an English school in the 1970s, describes the
cultural practices they produced, practices that created subject positions
different from those offered them by the discourses of the school. In
Chapter 12 we look at this process in relation to the womens groups of
Naudada who produced songs for an annual womens festival.
That is, if we ignore the way Vygotskian ideas have been appropriated
into educational work in the United States. As van der Veer and Valsiner
(1994: 6) put it, in the educational applications of Vygotsky, a very
curious oversight can be observedthe role of the social other (teacher,
more capable peer, parent, etc.) is presented as always helpful, concerned
about the future advancement of the child, etc. The (very real) possibility
that under some circumstances educational interference . . . might be purposefully harmful, promote ignorance and be potentially detrimental in
other ways, is not considered.
See Ledermans account of women in Mendi, highland Papua New
Guinea, who explain by rote why they fail to attend public meetings:
Thats mens business, I had garden work to do (1984: 100). Even
women who were active leaders of public opinionone, in particular,
who stayed near her husband in public discussions and told him what to
sayseemed to have little reaction to being disqualied, because of gender, from taking the oor themselves.
In William Faulkners Absalom Absalom! Thomas Sutpen spends a lifetime
reacting to a scene that objectied how others saw him. As an adolescent
Sutpen travels with his family from his native western Virginia to resettle

Co

11.

Notes to Pages 137142

am

304

15.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 143151

305

am

Co

py

in the eastern area of the state. Though he feels superior to the black
slaves who work the plantations of the region, he nds outin a scene
that epitomizes for him the relations of power dening (and conning)
his place in the Antebellum Souththat he, a poor white, is not regarded
as very different from them. He goes to the big house of the plantation
for which his father works, to deliver a message to the owner. The black
butler who responds to his knock denies the positional claim he has
innocently made by going to the front door and sends him, in a condescending manner, to the back. From this revelatory experience, a moment
of recognition like Kondos glimpse in the butchers case, Sutpen determines a lifelong course of action that he follows with singleness of purpose (to tragedy).
16. This mutually constitutive relationship between the semiotic mediator
and the target of the mediation can also be more complex. Kulick (1993)
describes a highland Papuan man who used a genre usually associated
with women to mediate, in fact to accentuate, an understanding of his
message.
17. In effect, this statement repeats in more personal terms Bakhtins point
about the social valuation of language-images, voices, and other media
of guration (see Chapter 8). It is not simply that peoples activity is
motivated and shaped by the social valuation of the forms of its expression. It is equally that the real activity of people changes the forms valuation. There is no abstract process of reproduction, like a psychologized
notion of generalizability, which removes the form from its social context of use. There are only real iterations in specic instances.

7. The Sexual Auction Block

Ex

1. In this chapter rst-person pronouns refer to Holland and her associates.


2. Here we focus on discourses of romance. Other gendering discourses are
those of parenting, reproductive medicine, and any culturally constructed
world in which gendered types have taken-for-granted interrelations and
greater or lesser value.
3. Many of the tellers had a vivid, seemingly emblematic, memory of at least
one such experience, a memory still evocative of mixed and unresolved
feelingsanger, embarrassment, guilt, and helplessness. Their stories,
like those of the women in the study, were readings of incidents according to the gured world of romance and attractiveness. More recent ones
often refer to the incidents as possible cases of sexual harassment.
4. Although the main portion of our studies focused on women, our studies
of gendered marked terms included men (Holland and Skinner 1987).

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

306

Notes to Pages 151163

Ex

am

Co

py

5. By taken-for-granted scenario we mean the situation that constituted the


standard of meaning. Participants to a discussion assumed that particular
cases t the taken-for-granted case. Unless faced with explicit indication
to the contrary, students assumed that they and their compatriots would
be attracted to romantic and sexual partners who were of roughly equivalent attractiveness.
6. Harr and Van Langenhove (1991) point out that subject positions in
discourses are generally interrelated, such that the claiming of position
for oneself or another suggests a distribution of positions to all who are
party to the eld of interaction.
7. The very description of the woman as foxy, with its overtones of approachability and sexual invitation, colluded in the double-edged construal of the situation. Plausible deniability is built into the mens prologue, and is a presumption of their power to dene the gender regime of
the situation.
8. See Holland (1988) on essentialized and enacted attractiveness. The
idea that attractiveness is in the person is much fostered by the industries that benet from peoples anxiety about social worth, as gured
according to appearance.
9. As with the symbolic capital of honor among the Kabyle, attractiveness is
claimed and validated through face-to-face interaction (Bourdieu 1977a).
Its reication as physical attribute is a characteristic form of misrecognition (mconnaissance).
10. Bourdieu contrasts the insult, in which individuals try to impose their
point of view of the divisions of the social world and their position within
it, with ofcial nomination. An insult is made by an individual who
takes the risk of reciprocation. An ofcial nomination is backed up by
force (Bourdieu 1985b).
11. Here we are dealing only with cross-gender relations. Such images
also have powerful, and somewhat independent, effects upon male-male
transactions.
12. The 90 percent gure given by the narrator is misleading when compared
to the statistics given by Pryor et al. (1993). A large group of men was
assessed by a scale designed to measure attitudes toward women and
likelihood to harass. Two subgroups, those who scored high and low on
the scale, were selected for the experiment; 90 percent of those who
scored high engaged in harassment in the experimentnot 90 percent of
the whole sample. Of those scoring low on the scale, 36 percent engaged
in harassment.
13. On the campuses we studied, other changes have occurred as well:
womens studies programs have been instituted and expanded, and gay
rights activists have become much more visible.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 164177

307

8. Authoring Selves

Co

py

14. Alarm and retaliation over sexual harassment charges belie this point.
See Richard Bernsteins (1994) Guilty If Charged in the New York Review of Books. Bernstein is not reviewing a book, but rather ridiculing
sexual harassment charges and their prosecution at the University of New
Hampshire (see Chapter 13).
15. Michael Lynch (1991) makes a similar point regarding laboratories and
the spaces where science is done. Space, in the way he uses it, is not
simply a neutral place on some ideal geophysical (or even psychological)
grid. Rather space is haunted by the activities that take place in it, and
hence by the dispositions and emotional tones of their performers.
16. These processes of commodication are surely implicated in the degree
to which attractiveness is thought of as an essential characteristic of a
person.

Ex

am

1. The fax metaphor comes from Strauss (1992).


2. Bourdieus (1977a, 1990b) practice theory and a stream of the culturalhistorical school, activity theory, also reect this conception; see Chapter 3.
3. James Wertsch (1991) has employed Bakhtins notion of speech genres to
build a social developmental understanding of mind and consciousness.
Our accounts have a similar intent. Wertsch too wants to combine Bakhtin and Vygotsky in order to develop an account of human activity as
mediated by specic sociocultural tools and contexts. He too emphasizes the interconnection of social and psychological activity through the
conceptual means of dialogue, inner speech, and voices. Our accounts
diverge, however, at several points, as we shall see.
4. Lee and Hickman (1983) also remind us that the deictic position that is
the I is meaningless without a counterpart, the you or other.
5. Vygotskys ideas also have been grafted to Bakhtins, to expand Bakhtins
vision of literature and art. See, e.g., Holquists (1990) appropriation of
Vygotskys zone of proximal development.
6. Vygotskys insistence on a genetic method was motivated by his conceptualization of this process of development. A simple focus on outcome was
insufcient, for outcome was likely to be a fossilization of a complex built
around a mediating device, not an essential characteristic.
7. Van der Veer and Valsiner (1994) argue that Vygotskys apparent disregard
for conict and the possibility that mentors can be harmful or have negative inuences on their charges is a product of the selective translation
and appropriation of his work.
8. In trying to nd a microcosm within mental life on a par with Marxs

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

10.

py

Ex

11.

identication of the commodity within economic life, Vygotsky (1986)


settled upon the word. Wertsch (1985a) spells out why this choice, even
though word for Vygotsky also included phrases, was an unfortunate
one. Too bad Vygotsky evidently did not read the arguments of Bakhtin
and his circle that identied utterances as just such an element of analysis
(Voloshinov 1986).
Around these differences we also diverge from Wertsch (1991). Wertsch
addresses the variety of social languages primarily through speech genres
rather than other, traditional sociolects: dialects, registers, accents. He
details the differing ways speech genres divide the worldits objects,
relationships, and transformationsand examines how actors (children)
come to apprehend and accord these differing world views place and time
by the activity pertinent to them. His account is well tailored to his primary topic, the development of cognitive resources and skills from the
appropriation of differentiated social forms, but it neglects (relatively)
what we wish to emphasize, processes of self-identication. When speakers appropriate social languages, they address themselves not just to the
object-world conceived there, nor just to the other speakers present, but
to the socially identied voice of the genre itself. Voice expresses a
social position, a politics by afliation or disafliation that moves toward
or away from forms of agency.
Lee and Hickman (1983) extrapolate from Vygotskys insistence on the
psychological importance of language to build a Vygotskian view of the
emergence of self through the metapragmatic functions of language. Despite the number of, as yet unconnected, steps to be taken between their
extrapolation and Bakhtins conceptualization, we do not discern any incompatibilities.
In his analysis of rebellious working-class schoolboys, Willis (1981) surmised that they had internalized the position of the school adults, and
thus the larger society, as an internal interlocutor that questioned and
criticized their behavior. Luttrell (1996) shows similar internalized dialog
for women in adult literacy classes. They had developed images of themselves against others in the gured worlds of school and success.
He also, edging close to Vygotskys notion of mediating devices, discusses
the potential of literature and art to model different possibilities of orchestrations. Holquist (1990) elaborates this aspect of Bakhtin by incorporating Vygotskys zone of proximal development. Holquist applies the
ZOPED concept not to the individual, as Vygotsky tended to do in reference to schooling, but to the collective. Artists, in this vision, are able
to develop outsideness, to the extreme position that Bakhtin labeled
transgredience, and by virtue of that advantage, are able to create a
vision beyond what is currently understood. It is Bakhtins version of the

Co

9.

Notes to Pages 178183

am

308

12.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 186191

avant-garde. Their visions become templates for possible futures; their art
provides texts of possibilities. We shall take up this world-forming aspect
of cultural expression in Chapters 11 and 12.
In Vygotskys account inner speech becomes differentiated from social
speech somewhere in the rst decade of the lives of most of us. The
person is not simply a robot of the collective in either Vygotskys or
Bakhtins view. In the Vygotskian view even the total neophyte, the infant and subsequent child, is active. Valsiner (1993) describes the crossgenerational transit of cultural forms not as a cultural transmission, which makes the child simply the recipient of the lifeways of the
parental generation, but rather as a bi-directional exchange, where the
child is likely to produce behavior outside the cultural repertoire of
the parent.
Voloshinov posits two extremes, or ideal types, of experience, which conform to the poles of expression (socioeconomy/organism). One, entirely
shaped by common parlance and social speech and thus perfectly ideological, he called we experiences. The second, I experiences, approaches the organismic, and is virtually unarticulated. I experiences,
because they cannot be put into words, are chaotic, unorganized.
Reference to this stabilized audience sets Bakhtin apart from many of the
social constructivists, such as Harr, who give no bases for peoples durability despite multiple resistance to discursive positionings.
Here he species that we are always communicating in inner speech with
an addresseeif no particular person, then we assume as our addressee a
contemporary of our literature, our science, our moral and legal codes
(1986: 86).
It can be argued, however, that Bakhtins notion of monological or
authoritative discourse implicitly requires a stable, even juridical or ritual, audience. This assertion would be partly the converse of Bakhtins
argument (1981) that heteroglossia arises and ramies through the social
division of labor.
Stability of social relations and material conditions is a necessary condition of the stability of the authorial stance.
Languages are not just verbal or scriptural. We have already discussed
other means of communicating social relations, including spatial congurations, positions and decorations of the body, silence and nonsilence.
Communication via these means need not coincide with the communication that proceeds linguistically, via choice of social language and dialect.
Indeed, the force of social stratication, of social hierarchy, is often objectied in the disjunction across differing media of expression. Dialogues
happen via these metacommunicative means as well, in ways that are
important to the development of identity.

15.

16.

17.

Ex

18.

am

14.

Co

py

13.

309

19.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

310

Notes to Pages 193204

9. Mental Disorder, Identity, and Professional Discourse

Ex

am

Co

py

1. Fieldwork for the study was done from 1986 to 1991 by a research team at
the University of North Carolina School of Medicine headed by Sue E.
Estroff. I in the account is Lachicotte; we in this section includes not
only his coauthors but also his colleagues on the research team.
2. See, e.g., Siegler and Osmond (1974) on medicalization in psychiatry;
Millon and Klerman (1986) on classication of psychopathology in the
DSM; Grob (1973, 1983, 1991) for the classic histories of American mental health care; Stone (1986) on differences in European traditions; Szasz
(1961, 1970) on the antipsychiatry movement.
3. The discussion adheres more to the language of the third edition, the one
in use at the time of our study. The fourth edition, for instance, substitutes
the term mood disorders for what we call affective disorders. The fact that
psychiatry has its own manual says something about its peculiar history.
All other medical specialties use the International Classication of Diseases, now in its tenth edition.
4. Two recent and excellent monographs examine the social and cultural
constructions of traumatic disorders: Hackings study (1995) of multiple
personality and Youngs work (1996) on post-traumatic stress disorder.
5. The nature of the personality disorders is perhaps the most debated topic
of modern American medical psychiatry. See Millon (1981, 1985, 1990)
for a comprehensive treatment. Nuckolls (1992a, 1992b) draws upon
Millon in his own work on personality disorders.
6. It is unrealistic to call this confusion a kind of error, if by that we make
the diagnosis the basic gure. The personae precede, in most cases, the
formal diagnosis. It might be more appropriately called the medicalization of a psychological discourse. This cultural world is replete with discourses related in various degrees of sub- and superordination. Though
medical psychiatry, ordained by the DSM-III and now IV, is hegemonic, it
has itself a compilation of earlier and contemporary discourses, not all of
which are well subsumed or even regulated (see Lachicotte 1992).
7. Schizoaffective disorder is a form of schizophrenia whose dominant
symptomatic expression features mood disturbancesdepression or mania or both. Schizoaffective disorder, like borderline personality, is a specic kind of intermediate or liminal category of the nosology.
8. The notion of role constriction comes from Kai Erikson (1957, 1962) and
from the medical sociological tradition founded in role theory (Parsons
1951, 1972). The classic account of the social formation of mental patients is Goffman (1961). Labeling theory (Scheff 1966) was another
limited but inuential view of the social foundations of mental disorder;
for criticism of it see Townsend (1976); Gove (1970, 1980).

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 209221

311

Co

py

9. For generalization of Selyes work see, e.g., Coelho, Hamburg, and Adams
(1974); Monat and Lazarus (1977). According to Marsh (1992) this generalization entailed changing Selyes pathogenic model of stress to a transactional model. The latter describes the interrelations of organism and
environment dynamically, so that the stress present in the local ecosystem
is a function less of the nature of the components (such as a pathogen)
than of their interactions. It is analogous to the change from pathogenic
to ecoimmunological theories of disease. Hateld (1987) calls this basic
perspective a stress-diathesis model.
10. Cain (1995) studied patients undergoing treatment in mental health facilities in Costa Rica, where neither psychiatry nor alternative treatments
(such as Neurotics Anonymous) have established authority to the same
extent as in the United States. The persons she followed tacked back and
forth, sometimes chaotically, among the explanations of these various
schools.
11. Compare Habermass examinations (1975, 1984, 1987) of the general
forms of communicative practice in relation to authoritative, often statesponsored, discourses. Our view owes a more obvious debt to Bourdieu.

10. Authoring Oneself as a Woman in Nepal

Ex

am

1. Much of the material reported here comes from Skinners eldwork in


Naudada in 198586. We also include some information from joint research by Skinner and Holland in 1986, 1990, and 1991.
2. We prefer to use the more specic Brahmanical instead of Hindu to
refer to ideologies and practices promoted by Vedic literature and by most
Bahun (that is, Brahman) priests. For aspects of the Brahmanical model
for womens lives see Bennett (1976, 1983); Caplan (1985); Das (1979);
Skinner (1989, 1990); Stone (1978).
3. The people of Naudada divided themselves into several jat (caste/ethnic
groups): Bahun and Chetri, considered the two highest or big (thulo)
castes and together constituting a slight majority of the population; Newar and Magar, ethnic groups in the middle of the caste hierarchy; and the
lower or small caste (sano jat) groups, including Damai, Sunar, Kami,
and Sarki. Only a minority of the members of the lower-caste groups
seemed to support themselves primarily by the crafts and services conventionally associated with them (Kami, blacksmithing; Sunar,
goldsmithing; Sarki, leatherworking; Damai, tailoring and music). Nonetheless, most families, especially among the Damai, still supplied the
traditional services to particular Bahun or Chetri families in return for a
portion of the upper-caste families harvests.
4. We are not the rst to notice seemingly inconsistent stances in what

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

7.

8.

9.

Ex

10.

py

6.

people say and do; see Strauss (1990). Our interpretations are compatible
with Strausss position, but we add a temporal dimension by always considering peoples acts and self-understandings within the context of heuristic development. For a psychoanalytic perspective see Ewing (1990)
and Obeyesekere (1981).
The possibility that somewhere there exist the encompassing resources
and consistently effective social work required to produce a single, transcendent self seems very small. As described in Chapter 4, the personal
story of Alcoholics Anonymous posits a self for alcoholics that crosses
many situations and contexts. Yet, as Hank says about his own story,
there was a life, too.
One important exception to the pattern of not talking much about rituals
concerned Tij, which we discuss later in this chapter and in Chapter 12.
Two people from other areas of Nepal were appalled when they heard
of this scolding, saying they had never been scolded in such a way when
they were children and had never even heard it in their villages. They associated such scolding with poorer and lower-caste and lower-class families. In Naudada not all parents scolded their children this way, but parents from all castes did so. Some parents said they did not know how to
scold, distinguishing themselves from those who did such things, but
even they could be heard scolding their children in such a way when they
were angry.
Holland (1988) describes these sorts of representations for oneself as
epitomized scenes. Willis (1981) writes of an internal interlocutor.
For other references on the dialogic self see Abu-Lughod (1990).
This notion of sense is, in Vygotskys conception, the currency of inner
speaking. When extended to the embodiment of all means of expression
(as inner activity), it becomes like Bourdieus understanding of practical
sense, sens pratique (see Chapter 8).
We have done extensive research on Tij songs as a form of womens
critical commentary: see Holland and Skinner (1995a, 1995b, 1996);
Skinner (1990); Skinner et al. (1994). For other accounts see Enslin
(1992, 1998); Raheja and Gold (1994).
Thakali is an ethnic group whose members run tea shops and hotels
where various kinds of liquor are available.
See Egnor (1986) and Raheja and Gold (1994) for other accounts of
South Asian womens songs as expressions of alternative visions of femininity and as vehicles for the production of both selves and cultural
meanings.
Sites and activities other than Tij, such as schools, literacy classes, the
activities of political workers, and womens organizations, are also impor-

Co

5.

Notes to Pages 223231

am

312

11.
12.

13.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 235242

313

py

tant in this regard (see Enslin 1990, 1998; Skinner and Holland 1996;
Skinner et al. 1998), but the development of this critical commentary in
Tij antedates by far its development in these other sites (Skinner et al.
1994).

11. Play Worlds, Liberatory Worlds, and Fantasy Resources

Ex

am

Co

1. In a way more convenient to the human sciences of today than the


Hegelian dialectic of self-consciousness, Vygotsky and Bakhtin socialize
the person and impersonate sociality. They have made being thought
throughdifferentiating the individuum of inner speech in order to
return to association more fully heldthe way of being human. Those
who fail to attend to this achievement may be lost in postmodernity.
2. Because he insisted on the original interpersonal, interactive context of all
cultural forms that become intrapersonally signicant, Vygotsky by no
means closed off attention to social dynamics and structures. But, at least
as his ideas have been transported to English-speaking academics (van der
Veer and Valsiner 1994), he did not develop these aspects as did Bakhtin.
3. Perhaps it would be more accurate to say that such theorists combine
habit and Wert-rationalitt.
4. The play of carnival may at rst appear chaoticas if social order had
been uprooted and dispelled. But as one continues to observe carnival, it
becomes clear that its activity is more a counter- than an antigenre (see
Lancaster 1996 for another view).
5. The term amour courtois was rst used by Paris (1883). In this section
we rely heavily on secondary sources, esp. Boase (1977) and Paterson
(1993).
6. Our account follows Andreas Capellanus (1941), a late twelfth (or early
thirteenth) century codication of the genre. The Occitanian poems included this kind of programmatic lovers world but also exhibited more
varied and mundane romantic landscapes. Although virtually all lovers
are depicted as male, there are a few exceptions. These may occur in
poems composed by female troubadours, or male poets may have created
female personae from which to speak. See Paden (1989); Kay (1990);
Paterson (1993).
7. Courtly love was ordinarily adulterous love, the love of an already married woman. Hence the vulnerability of lovers to false, and sometimes
true, witness (Lewis 1936; Boase 1977).
8. These were mock trials (of dubious authenticity) at which cases concerning breaches of the proper conduct of love were adjudicated (Neilson
1967). Eleanors daughter Marie was said to sponsor such amusements

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

11.

12.

13.

py

10.

(Wilcox 1930; Lewis 1936; Kelly 1952). One of the more popular forms
of the trobar, the tenso, did take the form of a debate over questions of love
(or other topics).
Various literary and intellectual sources have been named as origins of
courtly love. The Hispano-arabic thesis (Dawson 1935; Denomy 1947;
Daniel 1975; see Boase 1977) is perhaps the best supported. Lewis (1936)
championed Ovid; Denomy (1947), neoplatonism; Rougemont (1956),
Catharist doctrine; and Briffault (1965), Marianism and older fertility
cults.
In this treatise Capellanus goes so far as to discuss the love of nuns, which
was to his mind the most chaste and unattainable of all such loves
though it was often not held in such high regard by popular reputation.
The cult of Mary, Mariolatry, may have beneted from this development,
for, some have argued, the devices that live on in ecstatic religious poetry
resemble the praise of the troubadours for their beloved.
Such gured worlds and their associated imagined communities are objects of controversy. The gured world of race relations is another extremely contested social venue in contemporary America. It has heroes
and villainsMartin Luther King Jr. and Orville Faubus, Malcolm X and
Lyndon Johnson, Stokely Carmichael and Bull Connorand events
evoked by the names of towns and citiesLittle Rock, Montgomery,
Selma, and so on. But who is villain and who is hero? What is the signicance of these events? The accent, tone, and value of gure and scene
vary according to the positions of the persons one asks.
In the cultural studies literature their propagations have been called panics (Hall et al. 1978).

Co

9.

Notes to Pages 242255

am

314

12. Making Alternative Worlds in Nepal

Ex

1. Tij means third day. The festival occurs on the third day of the bright
fortnight of the Nepali month of Bhadra.
2. We collected verses from and/or information about roughly 1,000 songs.
For 26 songs, in both English and Nepali, see Skinner, Holland, and
Adhikari (1994). For more detailed analyses see that article and Holland
and Skinner (1995a).
3. For more on the complexities of the Tij festival and its relationship to
the larger social and political context see Holland and Skinner (1995a,
1995b); Skinner et al. (1994).
4. Women sometimes called upon priests to guide the puja that is part of the
TijRishi Pancami complex, but not to explain the meaning of the fasting
and the ritual bathing as interpreted through the textual accounts of the

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Notes to Pages 256267

8.

9.

10.

11.

Ex

12.

py

7.

Co

6.

Hindu gods, goddesses, and holy men. The most learned priest said no
one consulted him; they lacked interest.
Although men were invited to participate in mela (work parties), they
were not usually permanent members of a parma group.
In spite of the religious themes of the ritual complex and its dedication to
husbands well-being, the overwhelming majority of Tij songs were neither about gods and goddesses (deuta songs) nor about devotion to husbands. Only a very small proportion of the songs we collected from older
women and of those in two other collections of Tij songs that we saw were
deuta songs (see Skinner et al. 1994). We heard no deuta songs on Tij Day.
Women with several children were less likely to return to their natal
home for Tij. Those whose parents had died were especially unlikely to
return.
Married women were also allowed to travel home during Tihar, for a
ceremony in which the sister carries out rites that protect the brother
from death in the coming year. When a sister was still residing in the natal
home, however, the married sister was not always invited. There was not
the same anticipation that all the younger married daughters would return home, nor was there license for women to gather together for singing
and dancing. Unlike Tij, Tihar was a family affair, conned to each household, and not a matter of the didi/bahini of the gaon.
In the Tamang womens life narratives described by March (1988),
women emphasized the right to speak in their natal familys home and the
injunction to silence in their husbands familys home.
Tragic events that happened in the ten gaons nearest the tati (public
space) where the Tij groups performed were not sung about on Tij Day.
No group prepared a ghatana song, for example, about the deaths of
Naudadans run over by a truck a few weeks before the 1991 festival. We
were told that such songs would cause the relatives too much pain.
Holland and Skinner (1995a) detail how this shift from local, familial
relations to the world of antigovernment activity and party politics was
able to occur so rapidly.
Hybrid forms, as thoroughly dialogized speech, refract the voices that are
juxtaposed. They not only create a novel social standpointhere a distinctly feminine (perhaps even a feminist) political agentbut also
change the social positions of the element voices. The articulation and
reproduction of the hybrid regures the space of authorship, multiplying
the possible ways of identifying activity. The Tij songbooks that have
proliferated in Nepal since 1990 recognize the changed shape of both
womens authority and political opportunity.
The song pivot also exists in womens intimate activity: Skinners inform-

am

5.

13.

315

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

316

Notes to Pages 282287

13. Identity in Practice

py

ants not only sang to themselves, they even sang some of their answers to
her questions in the words of Tij songs. The world of Tij has long transcended the temporal and spatial bounds of the festival.

Ex

am

Co

1. Holland and Lave (forthcoming) collect a number of recent studies that


explore local contestations as they relate both to long-term, historical
struggles (as in Northern Ireland) and to the production of subjectivities
in these histories of intense struggle.
2. Silva was interviewed in the fall of 1994 by Christopher Lydon, host of
The Connection, a show aired by WBUR, one of Bostons public radio
afliates. On Silva see Bernstein (1994).
3. One can assimilate this point, we suppose, to the fact of intertextuality
among discourses. But, should we move to Bakhtins language, and call it
multivocality under the condition of heteroglossia, we come closer to
our point. Utterance and human expression are fully materialized, social
actions, works whose shaping is open to the complex of political-economic, social, cultural, and psychological productions of any activity.
4. Of course these games are socially constrained. But cultural gurings
need not form a transcendent logic that produces by its reductions only
one gured world.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

References

Ex

am

Co

Abu-Lughod, Lila. 1986. Veiled Sentiments: Honor and Poetry in a Bedouin Society.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
1990. The Romance of Resistance: Tracing Transformations of Power
through Bedouin Women. American Ethnologist 17(1): 4155.
Alcoholics Anonymous. 1967. What Happened to Joeand His Drinking Problem. . . . New York: Alcoholics Anonymous World Services [hereafter
AAWS].
1968a. AA for the Woman. New York: AAWS.
1968b. It Happened to Alice! . . . How She Faced a Drinking Problem. New
York: AAWS.
1976. Alcoholics Anonymous: The Story of How Many Thousands of Men
and Women Have Recovered from Alcoholism. 3rd ed. New York: AAWS.
1979a. Time to Start Living: Stories of Those Who Came to AA in Their
Later Years. New York: AAWS.
1979b. Too Young? Teenagers and AA. New York: AAWS.
1979c. Young People and AA. New York: AAWS.
1980. Dr. Bob and the Good Oldtimers: A Biography with Recollections of
Early AA in the Midwest. New York: AAWS.
American Psychiatric Association. 1987. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders. 3rd ed. rev. [DSM-III-R]. Washington: APA.
1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed.
[DSM-IV]. Washington: APA.
Amit-Talai, Vered. 1995. Conclusion: The Multi Cultural of Youth. In AmitTalai and Wulff, eds., 1995, 223233.
Amit-Talai, Vered, and Helena Wulff, eds. 1995. Youth Culture: A Cross-cultural
Perspective. New York: Routledge.
Anderson, Benedict. 1983. Imagined Communities. London: Verso.
Andreas Capellanus. 1941. The Art of Courtly Love. Trans. John J. Parry. New
York: Columbia University Press.

317

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

318

References

Ex

am

Co

py

Aretxaga, Begona. 1993. Striking with Hunger: Cultural Meanings of Political


Violence in Northern Ireland. In The Violence Within: Cultural and Political
Opposition in Divided Nations, ed. K. Warren, 219-253. Boulder: Westview.
Asad, Talal, ed. 1973. Anthropology and the Colonial Encounter. London: Ithaca
Press.
Bakhtin, Mikhail M. 1981. The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays by M. M. Bakhtin. Ed. M. E. Holquist, trans. Caryl Emerson and Michael Holquist. Austin:
University of Texas Press.
1984a. Problems of Dostoevskys Poetics. Ed. and trans. C. Emerson. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
1984b. Rabelais and His World. Trans. Helene Iswolsky. Bloomington:
Indiana University Press.
1986. Speech Genres and Other Late Essays. Ed. Caryl Emerson and
Michael Holquist, trans. Vern W. McGee. Austin: University of Texas Press.
1990. Art and Answerability. Ed. Michael Holquist and Vadim Liapunov,
trans. Vadim Liapunov. Austin: University of Texas Press.
1993. Toward a Philosophy of the Act. Ed. Vadim Liapunov and Michael
Holquist, trans. Vadim Liapunov. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bateson, Gregory. 1972. The Cybernetics of Self: A Theory of Alcoholism. In
Bateson, Steps to an Ecology of Mind, 309337. New York: Ballantine.
Becker, Howard S. 1963. Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance. New
York: Free Press.
Bennett, Lynn. 1976. Sex and Motherhood among Brahmins and Chhetris of
East-Central Nepal. Contributions to Nepalese Studies 3: 152.
1983. Dangerous Wives and Sacred Sisters: Social and Symbolic Roles of
High-caste Women in Nepal. New York: Columbia University Press.
Benton, John F. 1968. Clio and Venus: An Historical View of Medieval Love. In
Newman, ed., 1968, 1942.
Bernstein, Richard. 1994. Guilty If Charged. New York Review of Books 41(1
and 2): 1114.
Bista, Khem Bahadur. 1969. Tj ou la fte des femmes. Objets et Mondes 9:
718.
Bloch, Maurice. 1985. From Cognition to Ideology. In Power and Knowledge:
Anthropological and Social Approaches, ed. R. Fardon, 2148. Edinburgh:
Scottish Academic Press.
Bloch, R. H. 1991. Medieval Misogyny and the Invention of Western Romantic Love.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Boase, Roger. 1977. The Origin and Meaning of Courtly Love: A Critical Study of
European Scholarship. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
Boltanski, L., and L. Thevenot. 1983. Finding Ones Way in Social Space: A
Study Based on Games. Social Science Information 22(4/5): 631680.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

319

Ex

am

Co

py

Bouillier, Vronique. 1982. Si les femmes faisaient la fte: A propos des


ftes fminines dans les hautes castes Indo-Nepalaises. LHomme 22: 91
118.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977a. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Trans. Richard Nice.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1977b. The Economics of Linguistic Exchanges. Social Science Information 16(6): 645668.
1984. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste. Trans. Richard Nice. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
1985a. The Genesis of the Concepts of Habitus and Field. Sociocriticism 2(2): 1124.
1985b. The Social Space and the Genesis of Groups. Theory and Society
14: 723744.
1988. Homo Academicus. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.
1990a. In Other Words: Essays towards a Reexive Sociology. Trans. Matthew Adamson. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.
1990b. The Logic of Practice. Trans. Richard Nice. Palo Alto: Stanford
University Press.
1993. The Field of Cultural Production: Essays on Art and Literature. New
York: Columbia University Press.
Bourdieu, P., and J. C. Passeron. 1977. Reproduction in Education, Society and
Culture. Trans. Richard Nice. London: Sage.
Bourdieu, Pierre, and Monique de Saint-Martin. 1974. Scholastic Excellence
and the Values of the Educational System. In Contemporary Research in the
Sociology of Education, ed. J. Eggleston, 338371. London: Methuen.
Brenneis, Donald. 1990. Shared and Solitary Sentiments: The Discourse of
Friendship, Play, and Anger in Bhatgaon. In Lutz and Abu-Lughod, eds.,
1990, 113125.
Briffault, Robert. 1965. The Troubadours. Ed. L. F. Koons. Bloomington: Indiana
University Press.
Brown, Jane D., A. B. White, and L. Nikopoulou. 1993. Disinterest, Intrigue,
Resistance: Early Adolescent Girls Use of Sexual Media Content. In Media,
Sex and the Adolescent, ed. B. S. Greenburg, J. D. Brown, and N. L. BuerkelRothfuss, 177195. Cresskill, N.Y.: Hampton.
Brown, Penelope. 1980. How and Why Are Women More Polite: Some Evidence from a Mayan Community. In Women and Language in Literature and
Society, ed. Sally McConnell-Ginet, Ruth Borker, and Nelly Furman, 111
136. New York: Praeger.
Butler, Judith. 1993. Bodies That Matter. New York: Routledge.
Cain, Carole. 1991. Personal Stories: Identity Acquisition and Self-understanding in Alcoholics Anonymous. Ethos 19: 210253.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

320

References

Ex

am

Co

py

1995. Wandering the Streets without Direction: The Search for Meaning in Mental Illness in Costa Rica. Ph.D. diss., University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill.
Calhoun, Craig. 1991. Indirect Relationships and Imagined Communities:
Large-scale Social Integration and the Transformation of Everyday Life. In
Social Theory for a Changing Society, ed. P. Bourdieu and J. S. Coleman, 95
121. Boulder: Westview.
Cameron, D., F. McAlindon, and K. OLeary. 1988. Lakoff in Context: The
Social and Linguistic Function of Tag Questions. In Women in Their Speech
Communities: New Perspectives on Language and Sex, ed. J. Coates and
D. Cameron, 7493. New York: Longman.
Caplan, Patricia. 1985. Class and Gender in India: Women and Their Organizations
in a South India City. London and New York: Tavistock.
Caughey, John L. 1984. Imaginary Social Worlds: A Cultural Approach. Lincoln:
University of Nebraska Press.
Clark, Katerina, and Michael Holquist. 1984. Mikhail Bakhtin. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Clement, D. C. 1982. Samoan Cultural Knowledge of Mental Disorders. In
Cultural Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy, ed. A. J. Marsella and
G. M. White, 193215. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Clifford, James. 1988. The Predicament of Culture: Twentieth-century Ethnography,
Literature and Art. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Coelho, George V., David A. Hamburg, and John E. Adams, eds. 1974. Coping
and Adaptation. New York: Basic Books.
Cole, Michael. 1985. The Zone of Proximal Development: Where Culture and
Cognition Create Each Other. In Wertsch, ed., 1985, 146161.
1996. Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Connell, R. W. 1987. Gender and Power: Society, the Person and Sexual Politics.
Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.
Conrad, Peter, and Joseph W. Schneider. 1980. Deviance and Medicalization:
From Badness to Sickness. St. Louis: C. V. Mosby.
Crapanzano, Vincent. 1980. Tuhami: Portrait of a Moroccan. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
1990. On Self Characterization. In Cultural Psychology: Essays in Comparative Human Development, ed. J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, and G. Herdt,
401423. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Csordas, Thomas. 1994. The Sacred Self. Berkeley: University of California Press.
DAndrade, Roy. 1981. The Cultural Part of Cognition. Cognitive Science 5:
179195.
1992. Schemas and Motivation. In Human Motives and Cultural Models,

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

321

Ex

am

Co

py

ed. R. G. DAndrade and C. Strauss, 2344. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.


1995. The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Daniel, Norman. 1975. The Arabs and Medieval Europe. London: Longman.
Das, Veena. 1979. Reections on the Social Construction of Adulthood. In
Identity and Adulthood, ed. S. Kakar, 89104. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Davies, B., and Harr, R. 1990. Positioning: The Discursive Production of
Selves. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior 20: 4363.
Dawson, Christopher. 1935. The Origins of the Romantic Tradition. In
Dawson, Medieval Religion and Other Essays, 121154. London: Sheed and
Ward.
Dennett, Daniel. 1988. Why Everyone Is a Novelist. Times Literary Supplement
(16 Sept.): 1016, 10281029.
Denomy, Alexander J. 1947. The Heresy of Courtly Love. New York: MacMullan.
Dhruvarajan, Vanaja. 1989. Hindu Women and the Power of Ideology. Granby,
Mass.: Bergin and Garvey.
Donaldson, E. T. 1965. The Myth of Courtly Love. Ventures 5: 1623.
Dreyfus, Hubert. 1984. What Expert Systems Cant Do. Raritan 3(4): 2236.
Duby, Georges. 1983. The Knight, the Lady and the Priest: The Making of Modern
Marriage in Medieval France. Trans. B. Bray. New York: Pantheon.
1994. Love and Marriage in the Middle Ages. Trans. Jane Dunnett. Cambridge: Polity.
Dumont, L. 1980. Homo Hierarchicus: The Caste System and Its Implications.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Eckert, P., and S. McConnell-Ginet. 1992. Think Practically and Look Locally:
Language and Gender as Community-based Practice. Annual Review of Anthropology 21: 461490.
Edgerton, Robert B. 1967. The Cloak of Competence: Stigma in the Lives of the
Mentally Retarded. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Egnor, Margaret T. 1986. Internal Iconicity in Paraiyar Crying Songs. In Another Harmony: New Essays on the Folklore of India, ed. Stuart Blackburn and
A. K. Ramanujan, 294344. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Eisenhart, Margaret, and Nancy Lawrence. 1994. Anita Hill, Clarence Thomas,
and the Culture of Romance. Genders 19: 94121.
Emerson, Caryl. 1983a. Bakhtin and Vygotsky on Internalization of Language.
Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition 5(1):
913.
1983b. The Outer Word and Inner Speech: Bakhtin, Vygotsky, and the
Internalization of Language. Critical Inquiry 10(2): 245264.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

322

References

Ex

am

Co

py

Engels, Frederick. 1968. The Part Played by Labor in the Transition from Ape to
Man. In The Origins of the Family, Private Property and the State, ed. Eleanor
Leacock, 251264. New York: New World Books.
Engestrm, Yrjo. 1987. Learning by Expanding: An Activity Theoretical Approach
to Developmental Research. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit Oy.
1990. Learning, Working and Imagining: Twelve Studies in Activity Theory.
Helsinki: Proemta-Konsultit.
1992. Interactive Expertise: Studies in Distributed Working Intelligence. Dept. of Education, Research Bulletin 83. Helsinki: University of
Helsinki.
Enslin, Elizabeth. 1990. The Dynamics of Gender, Class, and Caste in a
Womens Movement in Rural Nepal. Ph.D. diss., Stanford University.
1992. Collective Powers in Common Places: The Politics of Gender and
Space in a Womans Struggle for a Meeting Center in Chitwan, Nepal. Himalayan Research Bulletin 12: 1126.
1998. Imagined Sisters: The Ambiguities of Womens Poetics and Collective Actions. In Selves in Time and Place: Identities, Experience, and History in Nepal, ed. Debra Skinner, Alfred Pach III, and Dorothy Holland,
269299. Lanham, Md.: Rowman and Littleeld.
Erikson, Erik. 1963. Childhood and Society. 2nd ed. New York: Norton.
1968. Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York: Norton.
Erikson, Kai T. 1957. Patient Role and Social Uncertainty: A Dilemma of the
Mentally Ill. Psychiatry 20: 263272.
1962. Notes on the Sociology of Deviance. Social Problems 9: 307314.
Ewing, Katherine P. 1990. The Illusion of Wholeness: Culture, Self, and the
Experience of Inconsistency. Ethos 18(3): 251278.
Fabian, Johannes. 1983. Time and the Other: How Anthropology Makes Its Object.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Faulkner, William. 1936. Absalom, Absalom! New York: Modern Library.
Favret-Saada, J. 1980. Deadly Words: Witchcraft in the Bocage. Trans. C. Cullen.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fillmore, Charles. 1975. An Alternative to Checklist Theories of Meaning. In
Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, ed.
C. Cogne et al., 123131. Berkeley: University of California Press.
1982. Towards a Descriptive Framework for Spatial Deixis. In Speech,
Place, and Action, ed. R. J. Jarvella and W. Klein, 3159. New York: Wiley.
Fogelson, R. D. 1979. Person, Self, and Identity: Some Anthropological Retrospects, Circumspects, and Prospects. In Psychosocial Theories of the Self, ed.
B. Lee, 67109. New York: Plenum.
Fordham, Signithia. 1993 Those Loud Black Girls: (Black) Women, Silence,
and Gender Passing in the Academy. Anthropology and Education Quarterly
24(1): 332.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

323

Ex

am

Co

py

Foucault, Michel. 1965. Madness and Civilization: A History of Insanity in the Age
of Reason. Trans. Richard Howard. New York: Random House.
1978. The History of Sexuality: An Introduction. Trans. Robert Hurley.
New York: Pantheon.
1979. Discipline and Punish: The Birth of the Prison. Trans. Alan Sheridan.
New York: Random House.
Frappier, Jean. 1972. Sur un procs fait lamour courtois. Romania 93: 145
193.
Gal, Susan. 1987. Language and Political Economy. Annual Reviews in Anthropology 18: 345367.
Gearing, F., T. Carroll, L. Richter, P. Grogan-Hurlick, A. Smith, W. Hughes, B.
Tindall, W. Precourt, and S. Topfer. 1979. Working Paper 6. In Toward a
Cultural Theory of Education and Schooling, ed. F. Gearing and L. Sangree,
938. The Hague: Mouton.
Geertz, Clifford. 1973a. Deep Play: Notes on the Balinese Cockght. In Geertz,
The Interpretation of Cultures, 412453. New York: Basic Books.
1973b. Thick Description: Toward an Interpretive Theory of Culture.
In Geertz, The Interpretation of Cultures, 332. New York: Basic Books.
Gergen, Kenneth. 1984. An Introduction to Historical Social Psychology. In
Historical Social Psychology, ed. K. J. Gergen and M. M. Gergen, 336.
Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
1991. The Saturated Self: Dilemmas of Identity in Contemporary Life. New
York: Basic Books.
1994. Realities and Relationships: Soundings in Social Construction. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Gibson, James J. 1979. The Senses Compared as Perceptual Systems. Boston:
Houghton Mifin.
Goffman, Erving. 1961. Asylums: Essays on the Social Situation of Mental Patients
and Other Inmates. Garden City, N.Y.: Anchor.
Goodenough, Ward. 1994. Toward a Working Theory of Culture. In Assessing
Cultural Anthropology, ed. R. Borofsky, 262273. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Goodman, James C. 1981. Shivas Scarlet Women. ASIA (Sept.Oct.): 2025.
Gove, Walter R. 1970. Societal Reaction as an Explanation of Mental Illness: An
Evaluation. American Sociological Review 35: 873884.
1980. Labeling and Mental Illness: A Critique. In Labeling Deviant
Behavior, ed. Walter R. Gove, 53109. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Greeneld, Patricia M. 1984. A Theory of the Teacher in the Learning Activities
of Everyday Life. In Everyday Cognition: Its Development in Social Context,
ed. B. Rogoff and J. Lave, 117138. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University
Press.
Gregor, Thomas. 1977. The Mehinaku: The Drama of Everyday Life in a Brazilian
Indian Village. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

324

References

Ex

am

Co

py

Grob, Gerald N. 1973. Mental Institutions in America: Social Policy to 1875. New
York: Free Press.
1983. Mental Illness and American Society. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
1991. From Asylum to Community: Mental Health Policy in Modern America. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Gumperz, John, and Dell Hymes, eds. 1972. Directions in Sociolinguistics: The
Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Habermas, Jrgen. 1975. Legitimation Crisis. Trans. Thomas McCarthy. Boston:
Beacon.
1984. The Theory of Communicative Action, vol. 1: Reason and the Rationalization of Society. Trans. Thomas McCarthy. Boston: Beacon.
1987. The Theory of Communicative Action, vol. 2: Lifeworld and System: A Critique of Functionalist Reason. Trans. Thomas McCarthy. Boston:
Beacon.
Hacking, Ian. 1995. Rewriting the Soul: Multiple Personality and the Sciences of
Memory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Hall, Stuart. 1996. Introduction: Who Needs Identity? In Questions of Cultural
Identity, ed. S. Hall and P. Du Gay, 117. London: Sage.
Hall, Stuart, C. Critcher, T. Jefferson, J. Clarke, and B. Roberts. 1978. Policing the
Crisis: Mugging, the State and Law and Order. New York: Holmes and Meier.
Hallowell, A. I. 1955a. The Ojibwa Self and Its Behavioral Environment. In
Hallowell, Culture and Experience, 172182. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
1955b. The Self and Its Behavioral Environment. In Hallowell, Culture
and Experience, 75110. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Hannerz, Ulf. 1983. Tools of Identity and Imagination. In Identity: Personal and
Socio-cultural, ed. Anita Jacobson-Widding, 347360. Atlantic Highlands,
N.J.: Humanities Press.
Haraway, D. 1988. Situated Knowledges: The Science Question in Feminism
and the Privilege of the Partial Perspective. Feminist Studies 14: 575600.
Harr, Rom. 1979. Social Being: A Theory for Social Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.
1984. Personal Being: A Theory of Individual Psychology. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Harr, R., and L. Van Langenhove. 1991. Varieties of Positioning. Journal for
the Theory of Social Behavior 21: 391407.
Harris, G. G. 1989. Concepts of Individual, Self, and Person in Description and
Analysis. American Anthropologist 91: 599612.
Harvey, David. 1996. Justice, Nature and the Geography of Difference. Cambridge,
Mass.: Blackwell.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

325

Ex

am

Co

py

Hateld, Agnes B. 1987. Coping and Adaptation: A Conceptual Framework for


Understanding Families. In Families of the Mentally Ill: Coping and Adaptation, ed. A. B. Hateld and H. P. Leey, 6084. New York: Guilford.
Haug, W. F. 1986. Critique of Commodity Aesthetics: Appearance, Sexuality and
Advertising in Capitalist Society. Trans. Robert Bock. Minneapolis: University
of Minnesota Press.
Heer, Friedrich. 1963. The Medieval World: Europe, 11001350. Trans. Janet
Sondheimer. New York: Mentor.
Henriques, Julian, W. Hollway, C. Urwin, C. Venn, and V. Walkerdine. 1984.
Changing the Subject: Psychology, Social Regulation and Subjectivity. London:
Methuen.
Hill, Jane. 1995. Junk Spanish, Covert Racism and the (Leaky) Boundary between Public and Private Spheres. Pragmatics 5(2): 197212.
Hochschild, Arlie. 1983. The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Holland, Dorothy C. 1988. In the Voice of, in the Image Of: Socially Situated
Representations of Attractiveness. IPrA Papers in Pragmatics 2(1/2): 106
135.
1997. Selves as Cultured: As Told by an Anthropologist Who Lacks a
Soul. In Self and Identity: Fundamental Issues, ed. R. Ashmore and L. Jussim,
193221. New York: Oxford University Press.
Holland, D., and M. Cole. 1995. Between Discourse and Schema: Reformulating a Cultural-historical Approach to Culture and Mind. Anthropology and
Education Quarterly 26(4): 478489.
Holland, D., and J. Crane. 1987. Adapting to an Industrializing Nation: The
Shango Cult in Trinidad. Social and Economic Studies 36(4): 4166.
Holland, Dorothy, and Margaret Eisenhart. 1981. Womens Peer Groups and
Choice of Career. Final report. Washington: National Institute of Education.
1990. Educated in Romance: Women, Achievement and College Culture.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Holland, Dorothy C., and Andrew Kipnis. 1994. Metaphors for Embarrassment
and Stories of Exposure: The Not-so-egocentric Self in American Culture.
Ethos 22(3): 316342.
Holland, Dorothy, and Jean Lave, eds. Forthcoming. History in Person: Enduring
Struggles and the Practice of Identity. Santa Fe: SAR Press.
Holland, Dorothy C., and Naomi Quinn, eds. 1987. Cultural Models in Language
and Thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Holland, Dorothy, and James R. Reeves. 1994. Activity Theory and the View
from Somewhere: Team Perspectives on the Intellectual Work of Programming. Mind, Culture and Activity 1: 824.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

326

References

Ex

am

Co

py

Holland, Dorothy, and Debra Skinner. 1987. Prestige and Intimacy: The Cultural Models behind Americans Talk about Gender Types. In Holland and
Quinn, eds., 1987, 78111.
1995a. Contested Ritual, Contested Femininities: (Re)forming Self and
Society in a Nepali Womens Festival. American Ethnologist 22: 279305.
1995b. Not Written by the Fate-writer: The Agency in Womens Critical
Commentary in Nepal. Folk: The Journal of the Danish Ethnographic Society
37: 103133.
1996. The Co-development of Identity, Agency, and Lived Worlds. In
Comparisons in Human Development: Understanding Time and Context, ed.
J. Tudge, M. Shanahan, and J. Valsiner, 193221. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Hollway, W. 1984. Gender Difference and the Production of Subjectivity. In
Henriques et al., 1984, 227263.
Holquist, Michael. 1981. Politics of Representation. In Allegory in Representation, ed. S. Greenblatt, 163185. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
Press.
1990. Dialogism: Bakhtin and His World. New York: Routledge.
Howard, Donald R. 1966. The Three Temptations: Medieval Man in Search of the
World. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Huizinga, Johan. 1955. Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play Element in Culture.
Boston: Beacon.
1956. The Waning of the Middle Ages: A Study of the Forms of Life, Thought
and Art in France and the Netherlands in the XIVth and XVth Centuries. Garden
City, N.Y.: Doubleday.
Hutchins, Edwin. 1986. Mediation and Automatization. Quarterly Newsletter
of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition 8(2): 4758.
1987. Myth and Experience in the Trobriand Islands. In Holland and
Quinn, eds., 1987, 269289.
Inden, Ronald. 1990. Imagining India. Oxford: Blackwell.
Irvine, Judith T. 1980. How Not to Ask a Favor in Wolof. Papers in Linguistics
13: 350.
1985. Status and Style in Language. Annual Reviews in Anthropology
14: 557581.
1989. When Talk Isnt Cheap: Language and Political Economy. American Ethnologist 16(2): 248267.
Ito, K. L. 1987. Emotions, Proper Behavior (hana pono) and Hawaiian Concepts
of Self, Person, and Individual. In Contemporary Issues in Mental Health
Research in the Pacic Islands, ed. A. B. Robillard and A. J. Marsella, 4571.
Honolulu: University of Hawaii, Social Science Research Institute.
Jackson, W. T. H. 1960. The Literature of the Middle Ages. New York: Columbia
University Press.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

327

Ex

am

Co

py

Jaeger, C. Stephen. 1985. The Origins of Courtliness: Civilizing Trends and the
Formation of Courtly Ideals, 9391210. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Joas, Hans. 1985. G. H. Mead: A Contemporary Re-examination of His Thought.
Trans. Raymond Meyer. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Johnson, Richard. 198687. What Is Cultural Studies Anyway? Social Text:
Theory/Culture/Ideology 16(Winter): 3880.
Joravsky, David. 1989. Russian Psychology: A Critical History. Oxford: Blackwell.
Kay, Sarah. 1990. Subjectivity in Troubadour Poetry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kelly, Amy. 1952. Eleanor of Aquitaine and the Four Kings. London: Cassell.
Kitayama, S., H. Markus, and C. Lieberman. 1994. The Collective Construction
of Self-esteem: Implications for Culture, Self, and Emotion. In Everyday
Conceptions of Emotion, ed. J. A. Russell, et al., 523570. Dordrecht: Kluwer
Academic.
Kondo, D. K. 1990. Crafting Selves: Power, Gender, and Discourses of Identity in a
Japanese Workplace. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kozulin, Alex. 1986. Vygotsky in Context. In Vygotsky, Thought and Language,
trans. Alex Kozulin, xilvi. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Kuhn, Manford, and Thomas S. McPartland. 1954. An Empirical Investigation
of Self-attitudes. American Sociological Review 19(1): 6876.
Kuhn, Thomas. 1962. The Structure of Scientic Revolutions. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Kulick, Don. 1993. Speaking as a Woman: Structure and Gender in Domestic
Arguments in a New Guinea Village. Cultural Anthropology 8(4): 411429.
Lachicotte, William S. 1992. On the Borderline: Profession, Imagination and
Authority in the Practice of Mental Health Care. Ph.D. diss., University of
North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Ann Arbor: University Microlms.
Lakoff, Robin. 1975. Language and Womans Place. New York: Harper and Row.
Lancaster, Roger N. 1992. Life Is Hard: Machismo, Danger, and the Intimacy of
Power in Nicaragua. Berkeley: University of California Press.
1996. Gutos Performance: Notes on the Transvestism of Everyday
Life. Presented at the Department of Anthropology Colloquium, University
of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, March 28.
Lave, Jean. 1988. Cognition in Practice: Mind, Mathematics and Culture in Everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lave, Jean, and Etienne Wenger. 1991. Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral
Participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Leavitt, John. 1996. Meaning and Feeling in the Anthropology of Emotions.
American Ethnologist 23: 514539.
Lederman, Rena. 1984. Who Speaks Here? Formality and the Politics of Gender
in Mendi, Highland Papua New Guinea. In Dangerous Words: Language and

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

328

References

Ex

am

Co

py

Politics in the Pacic, ed. D. L. Brenneis and F. R. Myers, 85107. New York:
New York University Press.
Lee, Benjamin. 1985. Intellectual Origins of Vygotskys Semiotic Analysis. In
Wertsch, ed., 1985, 6693.
Lee, Benjamin, and Maya Hickman. 1983. Language, Thought, and Self in
Vygotskys Developmental Theory. In Developmental Approaches to the Self,
ed. B. Lee and G. Noam, 343378. New York: Plenum.
Leontiev, A. N. 1978. Activity, Consciousness, and Personality. Englewood Cliffs,
N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
LeVine, R. A. 1982. Culture, Behavior, and Personality: An Introduction to the Comparative Study of Psychosocial Adaptation. Chicago: Aldine.
Lvi-Strauss, Claude. 1966. The Savage Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Lewis, C. S. 1936. The Allegory of Love: A Study in Medieval Tradition. Oxford:
Clarendon Press.
Luhrmann, Tanya M. 1989. Persuasions of the Witchs Craft: Ritual Magic in Contemporary England. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Luria, Alexander R. 1979. The Making of Mind: A Personal Account of Soviet
Psychology. Ed. Michael and Sheila Cole. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
1981. Language and Cognition. Ed. J. V. Wertsch. New York: Wiley.
Luttrell, Wendy. 1996. Becoming Somebody (or Not): The Schooling of Gendered Selves. In The Cultural Production of the Educated Person: Critical
Ethnographies of Schooling and Local Practice, ed. B. A. Levinson, D. E. Foley,
and D. C. Holland, 93117. Albany: SUNY Press.
Lutz, Catherine. 1987. Goals, Events, and Understanding in Ifaluk Emotion
Theory. In Holland and Quinn, eds., 1987, 290312.
1988. Unnatural Emotions: Everyday Sentiments on a Micronesian Atoll
and Their Challenge to Western Theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
1990. Engendered Emotion: Gender, Power, and the Rhetoric of Emotional Control in American Discourse. In Lutz and Abu-Lughod, eds., 1990,
6992.
1995. The Gender of Theory. In Women Writing Culture, ed. Ruth
Behar and Deborah A. Gordon, 249266. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
In press. The Psychological Ethnic and the Spirit of Permanent War:
The Military Production of Twentieth-Century American Subjects. In The
Invention of the Psychological: The Cultural History of Emotions in America, ed.
J. Pster and N. Schnog,. New Haven: Yale University Press,.
Lutz, C., and Abu-Lughod, L. 1990. Introduction: Emotion, Discourse, and the
Politics of Everyday Life. In Lutz and Abu-Lughod, eds., 1990, 123.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

329

Ex

am

Co

py

eds. 1990. Language and the Politics of Emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge


University Press.
Lutz, Catherine A., and Jane L. Collins. 1993. Reading National Geographic.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lykes, M. B. 1985. Gender and Individualistic vs. Collectivist Bases for Notions
about the Self. Journal of Personality 53(2): 357383.
Lynch, Michael. 1991. Laboratory Space and the Technological Complex: An
Investigation of Topical Contextures. Science in Context 4: 5178.
MacAndrew, Craig, and Robert B. Edgerton. 1969. Drunken Comportment: A
Social Explanation. Chicago: Aldine.
March, Kathryn. 1988. Talking about Not Talking. Presented at the 87th
American Anthropological Association Annual Meeting, Phoenix.
Markus, H. R., and S. Kitayama. 1991. Culture and the Self: Implications for
Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224253.
Markus, Hazel, and Paula Nurius. 1987. Possible Selves: The Interface between Motivation and the Self-concept. In Self and Identity: Psychosocial
Perspectives, ed. Krysia Yardley and Terry Honess, 157172. Chichester:
Wiley.
Marriott, McK. 1976a. Hindu Transactions: Diversity without Dualism. In
Transaction and Meaning: Directions in the Anthropology of Exchange and Symbolic Behavior, ed. B. Kapferer, 109142. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study
of Human Issues.
1976b. Interpreting Indian Society: A Monistic Alternative to Dumonts
Dualism. Journal of Asian Studies 36: 189195.
Marsella, A., G. DeVos, and F. L. Hsu, eds. 1985. Culture and Self: Asian and
Western Perspectives. New York: Tavistock.
Marsh, Diane T. 1992. Families and Mental Illness: New Directions in Professional
Practice. New York: Praeger.
Marx, Karl. 1975/1845. Theses on Feuerbach. In Early Writings, ed. Rodney
Livingstone and Gregor Benton, 421423. New York: Vintage.
McCall, George. 1987. The Structure, Content and Dynamics of Self: Continuities in the Study of Role Identities. In Self and Identity: Psychosocial Perspectives, ed. K. Yardley and T. Honess, 133145. Chichester: Wiley.
McCall, George J., and J. L. Simmons. 1978. Identities and Interactions: An Examination of Human Associations in Everyday Life. Rev. ed. New York: Free Press.
McConnell-Ginet, Sally. 1988. Language and Gender. In Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey, ed. F. J. Newmeyer, 7599. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
1989. The Sexual (Re)production of Meaning: A Discourse-based Theory. In Language, Gender and Professional Writing, ed. F. W. Frank and P. A.
Treichler, 3550. New York: Modern Language Association.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

330

References

Ex

am

Co

py

McDermott, R. P. 1974. Achieving School Failure: An Anthropological Approach to Illiteracy and Social Stratication. In Education and Cultural Process: Toward an Anthropology of Education, ed. G. D. Spindler, 82118. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
McHugh, E. 1989. Concepts of the Person among the Gurungs of Nepal.
American Ethnologist 16: 7586.
McLeod, Enid. 1976. The Order of the Rose: The Life and Ideals of Christine de
Pizan. London: Chatto and Windus.
Mead, G. H. 1934. Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Miller, J. G. 1988. Bridging the Content-Structure Dichotomy: Culture and the
Self. In The Cross-Cultural Challenge to Social Psychology, ed. M. H. Bond,
267328. Newbury Park: Sage.
Millon, Theodore. 1981. Disorders of Personality. DSM III: Axis II. New York:
Wiley.
1985. Personality and Its Disorders: A Biosocial Learning Approach. New
York: Wiley.
1990. Toward a New Personology: An Evolutionary Model. New York:
Wiley.
Millon, Theodore, and Gerald L. Klerman, eds. 1986. Contemporary Directions in
Psychopathology: Towards the DSM-IV. New York: Guilford.
Mines, M. 1988. Conceptualizing the Person: Hierarchical Society and Individual Autonomy in India. American Anthropologist 90: 568579.
Monat, Alan, and Richard S. Lazarus, eds. 1977. Stress and Coping: An Anthology.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Morson, Gary Saul, and Caryl Emerson. 1990. Mikhail Bakhtin: Creation of a
Prosaics. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.
Murray, D. W. 1993. What Is the Western Concept of the Self? On Forgetting
David Hume. Ethos 21: 323.
Neilson, William A. 1967. The Origins and Sources of the Court of Love. Rpt. of
1899 ed. New York: Russell and Russell.
Neisser, Ulric. 1988. Five Kinds of Self Knowledge. Philosophical Psychology
1(1): 3559.
Newman, F. X., ed. 1968. The Meaning of Courtly Love. Albany: State University
of New York Press.
Nuckolls, Charles W. 1992a. Toward a Cultural History of the Personality Disorders. Social Science and Medicine 35(1): 3747.
1992b. Notes on a Defrocked Priest: The Cynosure in South Indian
Shamanic and American Psychiatric Diagnosis. In Ethnopsychiatry, ed. Atwood D. Gaines, 6984. Albany: SUNY Press.
OBarr, William, and Bowman Atkins. 1980. Womens Language or Powerless
Language? In Women and Language in Literature and Society, ed. Sally

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

331

Ex

am

Co

py

McConnell-Ginet, Ruth Borker, and Nelly Furman, 93110. New York:


Praeger.
Obeyesekere, G. 1981. Medusas Hair: An Essay on Personal Symbols and Religious
Experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
1992. The Apotheosis of Captain Cook. Princeton: Princeton University
Press.
Ortner, Sherry. 1984. Theory in Anthropology since the Sixties. Comparative
Studies in Society and History 26(1): 126166.
Paden, W. D., ed. 1989. The Voice of the Trobairitz: Perspectives on the Women
Troubadours. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Page, Heln. 1994. White Public Space and the Construction of White Privilege
in U.S. Health Care: Fresh Concepts and a New Model of Analysis. Medical
Anthropology Quarterly 8: 109116.
Painter, Sidney. 1967. French Chivalry: Chivalric Ideas and Practices in Medieval
France. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Paris, Gaston. 1883. Lancelot du Lac, II: La conte de la charrette. Romania 12:
459534.
Parish, S. 1991. The Sacred Mind: Newar Cultural Representations of Mental
Life and the Production of Moral Consciousness. Ethos 19: 313351.
1994. Moral Knowing in a Hindu Sacred City. New York: Columbia University Press.
Parker, I. 1992. Discourse Dynamics: Critical Analysis for Social and Individual
Psychology. New York: Routledge.
Parsons, Talcott. 1951. The Social System. New York: Free Press.
1961. Social Structure and the Development of Personality. In Studying Personality Cross-culturally, ed. B. Kaplan, 165200. Evanston: Row, Peterson.
1972. Denitions of Health and Illness in the Light of American Values
and Social Structure. In Patients, Physicians and Illness, ed. E. G. Jaco, 2d
ed., 107127. New York: Free Press.
Paterson, Linda M. 1993. The World of the Troubadours: Medieval Occitan Society,
c. 1100c. 1300. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Peterson, John H., Jr. 1988. The International Origins of Alcoholics Anonymous. Presented at 12th International Congress of Anthropological and
Ethnological Sciences, Zagreb.
Price, Laurie. 1987. Ecuadorian Illness Stories: Cultural Knowledge in Natural
Discourse. In Holland and Quinn, eds., 1987, 313342.
Pryor, John B., C. M. LaVite, and L. M. Stoller. 1993. A Social Psychological
Analysis of Sexual Harassment: The Person/situation Interaction. Journal of
Vocational Behavior 42: 6883.
Quinn, Naomi. 1992. The Motivational Force of Self-understanding: Evidence

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

332

References

Ex

am

Co

py

from Wives Inner Conicts. In Human Motives and Cultural Models, ed.
R. G. DAndrade and C. Strauss, 90126. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Quinn, Naomi, and Dorothy Holland. 1987. Culture and Cognition. In Holland and Quinn, eds., 1987, 340.
Quinn, Naomi, and Claudia Strauss. 1994. A Cognitive Cultural Anthropology. In Assessing Cultural Anthropology, ed. R. Borofsky, 284297. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Raheja, Gloria G., and Ann G. Gold. 1994. Listen to the Herons Words: Reimagining Gender and Kinship in North India. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Reidel, Brian. 1994. Personal communication.
Reiter, R. R., ed. 1975. Toward an Anthropology of Women. New York: Monthly
Review Press.
Robbins, R. H. 1973. Identity, Culture and Behavior. In Handbook of Social
and Cultural Anthropology, ed. J. J. Honigmann, 11991222. Chicago: Rand
McNally.
Robertson, D. W. 1962. Some Medieval Doctrines of Love. In Robertson, A
Preface to Chaucer: Studies in Medieval Perspectives, 391503. Princeton,
N. J.: Princeton University Press.
1968. The Concept of Courtly Love as an Impediment to the Understanding of Medieval Texts. In Newman, ed., 1968, 118.
Robins, Lee N. 1980. Alcoholism and Labelling Theory. In Readings in Medical
Sociology, ed. D. Mechanic, 180198. New York: Free Press.
Robinson, David. 1979. Talking out of Alcoholism: The Self-help Process of Alcoholics Anonymous. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Rogoff, Barbara. 1990. Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. New York: Oxford University Press.
Roiphe, Katie. 1993. The Morning After: Sex, Fear, and Feminism on Campus.
Boston: Little, Brown.
Rosaldo, Michelle Z. 1980. Knowledge and Passion: Ilongot Notions of Self and
Social Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1984. Toward an Anthropology of Self and Feeling. In Culture Theory:
Essays on Mind, Self and Emotion, ed. R. A. Shweder and R. A. LeVine, 137
157. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rougemont, Denis de. 1956. Love in the Western World. Trans. M. Belgion. New
York: Pantheon.
Rouse, Roger. 1995. Questions of Identity: Personhood and Collectivity in
Transnational Migration to the United States. Critique of Anthropology
15(4): 351380.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

333

Ex

am

Co

py

Sahlins, Marshall D. 1995. How Natives Think: About Captain Cook, for Example. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sanday, Peggy R. 1990. Fraternity Gang Rape: Sex, Brotherhood and Privilege on
Campus. New York: New York University Press.
Sanders, W. B. 1980. Rape and Womans Identity. Sage Library of Social Research
no. 106. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Schade-Poulson, Marc. 1995. The Power of Love: Ra Music and Youth in
Algeria. In Amit-Talai and Wulff, eds., 1995, 81113.
Scheff, Thomas J. 1966. Being Mentally Ill: A Sociological Theory. Chicago: Aldine.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy. 1992. Hungry Bodies, Medicine, and the State: Toward
a Critical Psychological Anthropology. In New Directions in Psychological
Anthropology, ed. T. Schwartz, G. M. White, and C. A. Lutz, 221247. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schur, Edwin. 1980. The Politics of Deviance: Stigma Contests and the Uses of
Power. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall.
Schwartz, Gary, and Don Merten. 1968. Social Identity and Expressive Symbols:
The Meaning of an Initiation Ritual. American Anthropologist 70: 111731.
Scott, Robert A. 1969. The Making of Blind Men: A Study of Adult Socialization.
New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Selye, Hans. 1956. The Stress of Life. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Shepel, Elina. 1995. Teacher Self-identication in Culture from Vygotskys Developmental Perspective. Anthropology and Education Quarterly 26: 425
442.
Shotter, John. 1982. Consciousness, Self-consciousness, Inner Games, and Alternative Realities. In Aspects of Consciousness: Awareness and Self-awareness, vol. 3, ed. G. Underwood, 2761. London: Academic Press.
Shweder, R. A. 1991. Thinking through Cultures: Expeditions in Cultural Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Shweder, R. A., and E. J. Bourne. 1984. Does the Concept of Person Vary Crossculturally? In Shweder and R. A. LeVine, eds., Culture Theory: Essays on
Mind, Self, and Emotion, 158199. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Siegler, Miriam, and Humphrey Osmond. 1974. Models of Madness, Models of
Medicine. New York: Macmillan.
Skinner, Debra. 1989. The Socialization of Gender Identity: Observations from
Nepal. In Child Development in Cultural Context, ed. J. Valsiner, 181192.
Toronto: Hogrefe and Huber.
1990. Nepalese Childrens Understanding of Themselves and Their
Social World. Ph.D. diss., University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Skinner, Debra, and Dorothy Holland. 1996. Schools and the Cultural Produc-

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

334

References

Ex

am

Co

py

tion of the Educated Person in a Nepalese Hill Community. In The Cultural


Production of the Educated Person, ed. B. A. Levinson, D. E. Foley, and D. C.
Holland, 273299. Albany: SUNY Press.
Skinner, Debra, Dorothy Holland, and G. B. Adhikari. 1994. The Songs of Tij: A
Genre of Critical Commentary for Women in Nepal. Asian Folklore Studies
53: 257303.
Skinner, Debra, Alfred Pach III, and Dorothy Holland, eds. 1998. Selves in Time
and Place: Identities, Experience, and History in Nepal. Lanham, Md.: Rowman
and Littleeld.
Skinner, Debra, Jaan Valsiner, and Bidur Basnet. 1991. Singing Ones Life: An
Orchestration of Personal Experiences and Cultural Forms. Journal of South
Asian Literature 26: 1543.
Smith, N. B., and J. T. Snow, eds. 1980. The Expansion and Transformations of
Courtly Literature. Athens: University of Georgia Press.
Smith, Paul. 1988. Discerning the Subject. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press.
Spiro, Melford E. 1982. Collective Representations and Mental Representations
in Religious Symbol Systems. In On Symbols in Anthropology, vol. 3: Other
Realities, ed. J. Maquet, 4572. Malibu: Undena.
1993. Is the Western Conception of the Self Peculiar within the Context of the World Cultures? Ethos 21: 107153.
Spradley, James. 1970. You Owe Yourself a Drunk: An Ethnography of Urban Nomads. Boston: Little, Brown.
Stevens, John. 1973. Medieval Romance: Themes and Approaches. London:
Hutchinson.
Stone, Linda. 1978. Cultural Repercussions of Childlessness and Low Fertility
in Nepal. Contributions to Nepalese Studies 5(2): 736.
Stone, Michael H., ed. 1986. Essential Papers on Borderline Disorders: One Hundred Years at the Border. New York: New York University Press.
Strauss, Claudia. 1990. Who Gets Ahead? Cognitive Responses to Heteroglossia in American Political Culture. American Ethnologist 17(2): 312328.
1992. Models and Motives. In Human Motives and Cultural Models, ed.
R. G. DAndrade and C. Strauss, 120. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Stryker, Sheldon. 1987. Identity Theory: Developments and Extensions. In Self
and Identity: Psychosocial Perspectives, ed. K. Yardley and T. Honess, 89103.
Chichester: Wiley.
Szasz, Thomas S. 1961. The Myth of Mental Illness: Foundations of a Theory of
Personal Conduct. New York: Dell.
1970. The Manufacture of Madness: A Comparative Study of the Inquisition
and the Mental Health Movement. New York: Harper and Row.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

335

Ex

am

Co

py

Tannen, Deborah. 1990. You Just Dont Understand: Women and Men in Conversation. New York: Morrow.
Thompson, E. P. 1963. The Making of the English Working Class. New York:
Pantheon.
Thomsen, Robert. 1975. Bill W. New York: Harper and Row.
Titunik, I. R. 1987. Translators Introduction. In V. N. Voloshinov, Freudianism:
A Critical Sketch, ed. I. R. Titunik and N. H. Bruss, trans. I. R. Titunik, xv
xxv. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Todorov, Tzvetan. 1984. Mikhail Bakhtin: The Dialogical Principle. Trans. Wlad
Godzich. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Tolman, C. W., and W. Maiers, eds. 1991. Critical Psychology: Contributions to an
Historical Science of the Subject. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Townsend, J. M. 1976. Self-concept and the Institutionalization of Mental Patients: An Overview and Critique. Journal of Health and Social Behavior
17(3): 263271.
Traube, Elizabeth. 1992. Dreaming Identities: Class, Gender, and Generation in
1980s Hollywood Movies. Boulder: Westview.
Trosset, Carol S. 1986. The Social Identity of Welsh Learners. Language in
Society 15(2): 165192.
Turner, Terrence. 1969. Tchikrin: A Central Brazilian Tribe and Its Symbolic
Language of Bodily Adornment. Natural History 78: 5059.
Valsiner, Jaan. 1993. Bi-directional Cultural Transmission and Constructive
Sociogenesis. In Sociogenesis Reexamined, ed. W. de Graff and R. Mair, 47
70. New York: Springer.
van der Veer, Ren, and Jaan Valsiner. 1991. Understanding Vygotsky: A Quest for
Synthesis. Oxford: Blackwell.
1994. Reading Vygotsky: From Fascination to Construction. In The
Vygotsky Reader, ed. R. van der Veer and J. Valsiner, trans. T. Prout and R. van
der Veer, 19. Oxford: Blackwell.
Van Gennep, Arnold. 1960/1909. The Rites of Passage. Trans. M. B. Vizedom and
G. L. Caffee. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Voloshinov, V. N. 1986. Marxism and the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
1987. Freudianism: A Critical Sketch. Ed. I. R. Titunik and N. H. Bruss,
trans. I. R. Titunik. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. 1930. The Primitive Man and His Behavior. In Etiudy po isotorii
povedeniya, ed. L. S. Vygotsky and A. R. Luria, 54121. Moscow-Leningrad:
Gosudarstvennoye Izdatelstvo. (In Russian.)
1960. Development of Higher Psychical Functions (Razvitie vyshhih
psihicheskih funktsii). (In Russian.) Moscow: Izd. APN.
1971. The Psychology of Art. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

336

References

Ex

am

Co

py

1978. Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes.


Ed. M. Cole et al. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
1984. Child Psychology. In The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. 4.
Moscow: Pedagogika. (In Russian.)
1986. Thought and Language. Rev. ed. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
1987a. Imagination and Its Development in Childhood. In The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. 1: Problems of General Psychology, trans.
Norris Minick, ed. Robert W. Rieber and Aaron Carton, 339349. New York:
Plenum.
1987b. The Problem of Will and Its Development in Childhood. In
Collected Works, vol. 1, 351358.
1987c. Thinking and Speech. In Collected Works, vol. 1, 39285.
Weber, Max. 1978. Economy and Society. Ed. G. Roth and C. Wittich. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Weiner, Annette B. 1976. Women of Value, Men of Renown: New Perspectives in
Trobriand Exchange. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Wertsch, James V. 1981. The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology. In
Wertsch, ed., 1981, 336.
1985a. The Semiotic Mediation of Mental Life: L. S. Vygotsky and M.
M. Bakhtin. In Semiotic Mediation: Sociocultural and Psychological Perspectives, ed. Elizabeth Mertz and R. J. Parmentier, 4971. Orlando: Academic
Press.
1985b. Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind. Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard University Press.
1991. Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action.
Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Wertsch, James, ed. 1981. The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology. Armonk,
N.Y.: M. E. Sharpe.
1985. Culture, Communication, and Cognition: Vygotskian Perspectives.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wertsch, James V., Norris Minick, and Flavio J. Arns. 1984. The Creation of
Context in Joint Problem-solving. In Everyday Cognition: Its Development in
Social Context, ed. B. Rogoff and J. Lave, 151171. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Weston, Kath. 1990. Production as Means, Production as Metaphor: Womens
Struggle to Enter the Trades. In Uncertain Terms: Negotiating Gender in
American Culture, ed. Faye Ginsburg and Anna Lowenhaupt, 137151. Boston: Beacon.
Wexler, P. 1983. Critical Social Psychology. Washington: Falmer.
White, G. M. 1991. Identity through History: Living Stories in a Solomon Islands
Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

References

337

Ex

am

Co

py

1992. Ethnopsychology. In New Directions in Psychological Anthropology, ed. T. Schwartz, G. M. White, and C. Lutz, 2146. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1993. Emotions Inside Out: The Anthropology of Affect. In Handbook
of Emotion, ed. M. Lewis and J. Haviland, 2939. New York: Guilford.
Wilcox, John. 1930. Dening Courtly Love. Michigan Academy of Science, Arts
and Letters 12: 313325.
Willard, Charity C. 1984. Christine de Pizan: Her Life and Works. New York:
Persea Books.
Williams, Raymond. 1977. Marxism and Literature. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Willis, Paul. 1981. Learning to Labor: How Working Class Kids Get Working Class
Jobs. New York: Columbia University Press.
Willis, Susan. 1991. A Primer of Everyday Life. London: Routledge.
Wood, D. J., J. S. Brunner, and G. Ross. 1976. The Role of Tutoring in Problem
Solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 17: 89100.
Wulff, Helena. 1995. Interracial Friendships: Consuming Youth Styles, Ethnicity and Teenage Femininity in South London. In Amit-Talai and Wulff, eds.,
1995, 6380.
Young, Alan. 1996. The Harmony of Illusions. Berkeley: University of California
Press.

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py

Credits

Ex

am

Co

Some of the material in Chapter 1 was originally published in Dorothy


Holland, The Woman Who Climbed up the House: Some Limitations of
Schema Theory, in New Directions in Psychological Anthropology, ed.
Theodore Schwartz, Geoffrey White, and Catherine Lutz (Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 1992). It is used here with permission from
Cambridge University Press.
Some of Chapter 2 is drawn from Dorothy Holland, Selves as Cultured: As Told by an Anthropologist Who Lacks a Soul, in Self and
Identity: Fundamental Issues, ed. R. Ashmore and L. Jussim (New York:
Oxford University Press, 1997). It is used here with permission from
Oxford University Press.
Chapter 4 is a modied version of Carole Cain, Personal Stories:
Identity Acquisition and Self-understanding in Alcoholics Anonymous,
Ethos 19 (1991). It is used here with permission from Ethos.
Chapter 5 is a modied version of Dorothy Holland, How Cultural
Systems Become Desire: The Case of American Romance, in Human
Motives and Cultural Models, ed. Roy DAndrade and Claudia Strauss
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992). It is used here with
permission from Cambridge University Press.
Portions of Chapter 10 are modied from Debra Skinner and Dorothy
Holland, Contested Selves, Contested Femininities: Selves and Society
in Process, in Selves in Time and Place: Identities, Experience, and History
in Nepal, ed. Debra Skinner, Alfred Pach III, and Dorothy Holland (Lanham, Md.: Rowman and Littleeld, 1998). They are used here with permission from Rowman and Littleeld Publishers.
Some segments of Chapter 12 were originally published in Dorothy
Holland and Debra Skinner, Contested Ritual, Contested Femininities:
(Re)Forming Self and Society in a Nepali Womens Festival, American
Ethnologist 22, no. 2 (1995). They are used here with permission from
American Ethnologist.
339
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

py
Co
am
Ex
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Fictitious names are in quotation marks.

Angevins, 242244
Answering/answerability, 169, 172173,
177, 186, 189, 208
Anthropology, 4, 7, 1517, 2032, 38, 54,
99100, 127, 131, 169, 280
Anxiety disorders, 196197
Aquitaine, 240, 242, 244
Aretxaga, Begona, 290n13
Arnold, Benedict, 248
Arns, Flavio J., 177
Arthurian romances, 244, 247
Artifacts. See Cultural artifacts
Asad, Talal, 23
Atkins, Bowman, 12, 130
Attractiveness, 5657, 59, 104, 106, 112,
129130, 137, 145147, 152163, 166,
190
Austin, John L., 280
Authoring. See Self-authoring; Space of
authoring

Ex

am

Co

AA. See Alcoholics Anonymous


AA for the Woman, 74, 89
Abu-Lughod, Lila, 297n3
Academia, 49, 5859, 283
Activity/activities, 3839, 41, 5657, 60, 65,
236238, 271272
Activity theory, 39, 41, 177
Addressivity, 169174, 177, 188, 208, 280
Affective disorders, 196, 198, 202, 206207
Affordance, 26, 6162, 64, 134, 136137,
162, 164
African Americans, 101, 132, 314n12
Agency, 45, 7, 38, 4042, 46, 49, 272, 285;
improvisation as, 1518, 45, 63, 270,
275277; in world of romance, 115121;
and mental illness, 192193, 211212;
symbolization as, 277281
Albigensian crusades, 246, 283
Alcoholics Anonymous (AA), 78, 4244,
46, 5153, 6162, 6465, 6697, 98, 133,
174, 176, 178, 185, 190, 223, 272, 281,
286287, 296n35
Alcoholics Anonymous (Big Book), 69,
7679, 82, 89
Alcoholism, 6671, 7779, 83, 8788, 9697
Alcohol use in Nepal, 214, 230231, 258,
260
Alternative worlds, 236239; of courtly love,
240243; and nationalism, 247249; of
gender in Nepal, 258269
Althusser, Louis, 33
American Psychiatric Association, 43, 195
American Samoa, 19, 30
Amit-Talai, Vered, 57
Anderson, Benedict, 46, 49, 247249, 253;
Imagined Communities, 247

py

Index

Bahudal system, 214, 262


Bahun caste, 9, 11, 128, 130, 140, 220,
224225, 255, 311n3
Bakhtin, M. M., 56, 8, 15, 3236, 39, 41,
45, 111, 118, 128, 141142, 169174,
176189, 191, 192193, 208213, 223,
231, 235239, 251, 266, 270272,
278280, 282, 284285, 287, 295n29,
297n6, 308n12, 309n17, 313n1; Problems
of Dostoevksys Poetics, 6
Basnet, Bidur, 3
Bateson, Gregory, 70
Becker, Howard S., 100, 122, 295n25
Behavioral environments, 5455
Behaviorist psychology, 39

341
Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index
Chetri caste, 9, 11, 14, 216217, 219, 224,
227, 259, 274, 311n3
Chivalry, 243244
Chrtien de Troyes, 244245
Christine de Pizan, 245
Chronotopes, 171, 209
Class, 58, 286
Clement, D. C., 19, 30
Clifford, James, 23
Codevelopment, 45, 122, 251252
Cognitive salience, 105, 110, 115116, 118,
120122, 163
Cole, Michael, 37, 6163, 297n3
Collective meanings, 5, 22
Colonialism, 2324
Communism, 230, 249, 264, 284
Communities of practice, 5657, 60
Competency, 98, 117119, 121
Connell, R. W., 133, 146, 290n8
Conrad, Peter, 67
Constructivist perspective, 8, 1017, 2628,
4344, 5556, 62, 64, 131, 133, 273275
Context-sensitive rules, 13
Cooley, Charles H., 99
Cornwallis, Charles, 248
Counter-worlds, 250251, 267
Courtly love, 4546, 141, 239247, 273,
283, 286287
Crane, J., 16
Crapanzano, Vincent, 25, 55, 297n6, 304n11
Critical disruption, 2326
Csordas, Thomas, 22
Cultural artifacts, 15, 17, 28, 50, 235,
300nn2021; and materiality, 6064; and
positional identity, 127, 129130, 137
Cultural forms, 5, 8, 26, 31, 33, 45, 65, 285,
300n20; in Alcoholics Anonymous, 73,
88; and authoring selves, 173177, 185;
songs of Nepali women as, 231, 261; of
courtly love, 239; of nationalism, 249
Cultural heroes, 248249, 314n12
Cultural-historical school of psychology, 5
Cultural identity, 7
Culturalist perspective, 8, 1011, 1315,
2023, 27, 55, 131, 273275
Cultural logic, 9, 11, 1315, 17, 274
Cultural meaning systems, 11, 99100,
116118, 120121
Cultural means, 5355, 185

am

Co

Bennett, Lynn, 216, 228, 254255, 257


Benton, John F., 239
Bergson, Henri, 138
Bernstein, Richard, 283, 307n14
Bill W., 7677
Bi-polar affective disorder, 196, 202, 206
Bista, Khem Bahadur, 254
Bloch, R. H., 20, 239, 245
Boase, Roger, 239, 246
Bob, Dr., 7678
Boccaccio, Giovanni, 245
Boltanski, L., 152
Bootstrapping. See Symbolic bootstrapping
Borderline personality disorder, 4243, 59,
181, 198199, 202204, 210, 277
Bouillier, Vronique, 254
Bourdieu, Pierre, 13, 1718, 3839, 45, 56,
5860, 87, 104, 128129, 135138, 143,
144, 146, 158, 251, 270, 274, 278281,
286287, 294n23, 295n28, 299nn1618,
306n10; Homo Academicus, 59
Bourne, E. J., 21, 2829
Boyfriend-back-home strategy, 113
Bradford University, 101
Brahmanical texts, 142, 214216, 223, 226,
253255, 258259, 267
Brenneis, Donald, 303n9
Brown, Jane D., 183184, 303n3
Bruner, J. S., 83
Bunin, Ivan Alekseyevich, 6
Butler, Judith, 33

py

342

Ex

Cain, Carole, 51, 7576, 8889


Calhoun, Craig, 249
Cameron, D., 130
Capellanus, Andreas (Andr le Chapelain),
313n6; De Arte Honeste Amandi, 246,
314n10
Capitalism, 166
Caplan, Patricia, 216
Carnivalization, 212, 237238, 313n4
Caste, and Nepali women, 911, 1314,
1617, 126128, 130131, 134, 138, 140,
142143, 216217, 219220, 224225,
227, 254255, 258259, 262, 273276,
311n3
Cathars, 246
Caughey, John, 49
Chaucer, Geoffrey, 246

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index

py

Dreyfus, Herbert, 116120


Drinking. See Alcoholism
Drug therapy, 196197
Drug use, 100101
DSM (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders), 43, 195, 197
Duby, Georges, 241, 243244
Dukha songs, 223224, 228229, 257259,
261263, 266267, 276, 284
Dumont, L., 2122, 29
Durkheim, Emile, 87, 99
Eckert, P., 131
Edgerton, Robert B., 67
Egocentric self, 2931
Egocentric speech, 175
Eisenhart, Margaret, 53, 56, 98, 101102,
112, 145, 164, 283
Eleanor of Aquitaine, 242, 244
Embodiment, 138143
Engels, Frederick, 35
Engestrm, Yrjo, 56, 193, 298n11
England, 242, 244, 246247, 253
Entitlement, 127, 133, 139, 162
Erikson, Erik, 7, 289n5
Erikson, Kai, 310n8
Essentialism, 2729, 131, 270
Essential self, 27
Ethnographic present, 23
Ethnographies of personhood, 3132
Ethnopsychiatry, 195
Ewing, Katherine P., 29
Exclusion/inclusion, acts of, 134135
Experience, 187188, 309n14
Expertise, 111122, 137
External speech. See Social speech

Co

Cultural models, 30, 55, 71, 88, 102103,


121, 297n3, 301n4
Cultural patterns, 16
Cultural productions, 2829, 4546, 5859,
235236, 251, 253
Cultural resources, 4, 31, 45, 50, 174, 235,
280; and improvisation, 18, 274, 276;
songs of Nepali women as, 231232; and
identity formation, 284285
Cultural studies, 47, 2526, 32, 270,
289n3, 300n20
Cultural symbols, 6, 15, 18, 2324, 3538,
4950, 143, 175, 178, 185187
Cultural work, 271
Cultural worlds, 3, 78, 41, 5455, 64, 66,
118, 120122, 127, 236. See also Figured
worlds

Ex

am

Damai caste, 9, 134, 142, 216, 219, 224,


311n3
DAndrade, Roy, 55, 121, 297n4
Dante Alighieri, 245
Das, Veena, 216
Davies, B., 63, 302n2
Democracy movement (Nepal), 214, 229,
232, 262263, 276
Dennett, Daniel, 293n14
Denomy, Alexander J., 245
Depressive disorder, 196
Development, 174177, 185186, 189191
Developmental psychology, 5, 3538, 4950,
84, 100, 118, 138, 174, 177, 184, 282
Deviance, 67
Dhruvarajan, Vanaja, 220
Diagnosis, 43, 196198
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM), 43, 195, 197
Dialogic self, 171174, 282
Dialogism, 8, 15, 169171, 174, 177, 184,
189, 232, 237238
Diffusion, identity, 7273
Discursive theory, 26, 62, 285
Discussion meetings (AA), 77, 8284
Disorganized schizophrenia, 196
Dispositions, 137140, 143, 160161
Domna, 242247
Donaldson, E. T., 239
Dramatized worlds, 53
Dr. Bob and the Good Oldtimers, 79

343

Fabian, Johannes, 23
Fantasy, 4951, 236241, 285
Faulkner, William: Absalom, Absalom!
304n5
Favret-Saada, J., 25, 5556, 59
Feminist theory, 5, 7, 16, 2425, 44
Feudalism, 242, 283
Feuerbach, Ludwig, 35
Field of power, 58
Fields, 5860, 299nn1618
Figurative identity, 127128, 141
Figurative worlds, 53

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index
Gendering insults, 153155, 159
Gender labeling, 160
Gender-marked lexicons, 43, 102103,
151153, 155, 157, 160, 163
George III, 248
Gergen, Kenneth, 6, 212, 292n13
Getting the upper hand, 112, 117
Ghatana songs, 257, 276
Gibson, James J., 61
Goffman, Erving, 212, 274, 310n8
Gold, Ann G., 228
Goodenough, Ward, 57
Goodman, James C., 254
Good woman, life path of, 41, 5354, 140,
214223, 226, 255, 277
Gorkha, Nepal, 256, 263
Gower, John, 246
Grasse, Franois-Joseph-Paul de, 248
Greeneld, Patricia M., 84
Gregor, Thomas, 25, 51, 133134
Guided discovery, 297n4
Guilhem IX, 240, 242
Gumperz, John, 126
Gurung people, 291n3

am

Co

Figured worlds, 4143, 4546, 4953,


235236, 271, 280, 299n19; and cultural
means, 5355; beyond imagination,
5557; and power/privilege, 5760; and
artifacts/materiality, 6063; paradoxical
possibility of self-control, 6365; of
Alcoholics Anonymous, 6697; of
romance, 98122, 146147; recruitment
to, 99, 104, 110; and positional identity,
126132; of dialogism, 169, 171; of
Nepali women, 214232, 253269; and
play worlds, 236239; of courtly love,
239247; of nationalism, 247250;
counter-worlds as, 250251; and
codevelopment, 251252; subjectivities
across, 286287
Fillmore, Charles, 52
Films, 62
Fin amors, 239246
Folksongs. See Tij Songs
Food, 130, 134
Fordham, Signithia, 131132, 136
Forms. See Cultural forms
Fossilization, 37, 117118, 141, 175, 190,
237, 302n9
Foucault, Michel, 2326, 28, 32, 57, 62, 64,
176, 193, 212, 237, 275, 277
Frame semantics, 52
France, 55, 5859, 141, 239247, 250, 252
Franklin, Benjamin, 248
Frappier, Jean, 239
Freud, Sigmund, 99, 107, 198
Friendship, 105106

py

344

Ex

Gai Jatra, 257


Gal, Susan, 12
Games, 4951, 63, 119, 143, 236
Gay relationships, 301n3
Gearing, F., 127, 135
Geertz, Clifford, 24
Gender, 2325; and positional identity, 126,
131, 133134, 140, 143; and symbolic
violence, 147163; and sexual
harassment, 148, 163166, 283; and
Nepali women, 214232, 253269. See
also Attractiveness; Courtly love;
Romance, world of
Gendered identities, 146148, 150
Gendering, 147166

Habermas, Jrgen, 237


Habit/habituation, 235238
Habitus, 1718, 45, 58, 128, 138, 251, 272,
286, 302n9
Hale, Nathan, 248
Hall, Stuart, 7, 3233, 270
Hallowell, A. I., 22, 54, 291n7
Hannerz, Ulf, 42
Harassment. See Sexual harassment
Haraway, D., 277, 292n10
Harr, Rom, 44, 63, 235, 289n5, 293n14,
297n5, 302n2, 306n6
Harvey, David, 28, 32
Haug, W. F., 166
Heart, in self-discourse, 1920
Hebephrenia, 196
Heer, Friedrich, 244
Hegemony, 290n8
Henry, Patrick, 248
Heteroglossia, 170171, 178180, 182, 231
Heuristic development, 18, 33, 4046, 52,
65, 137, 262, 282, 285, 295n23
Hickman, Maya, 307n4, 308n10
Hierarchy, 58, 128, 131

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index

py

Inclusion/exclusion, acts of, 134135


Inden, Ronald, 42
Independent self, 30
Indexicality, 127129
India, 21, 29, 220, 228, 264
Inhabitation, 235238
Initiation rituals, 7273
Inner speech, 174178, 180, 184189, 235,
238, 283284, 309n13
Insulting gendering, 153160
Intentional worlds, 54
Interdependent self, 30
International Classication of Diseases, 310n3
Interviews, 101, 147, 180181, 193194
Irvine, Judith T., 12, 128
Iseult, 240
It Happened to Alice! 75, 89

am

Co

Higher Power, in Alcoholics Anonymous,


6970, 76
Hill, Anita, 164, 283
Hill, Jane, 134
Hinduism, 12, 21, 5354, 127128, 140,
214223, 226, 231232, 252, 253255,
258259, 262, 267, 273, 277
History-in-person, 8, 18, 46, 98, 111, 174,
186, 191
Hitting bottom, 6970, 79
Hochschild, Arlie, 3738
Holland, Dorothy, 911, 1517, 30, 43,
5253, 5556, 6061, 63, 98, 101102,
112, 121, 137, 142, 145, 147, 151, 159,
179, 214, 253, 255, 262, 266, 284, 289n7,
294n17, 296n32, 297nn3,7
Hollway, W., 28
Holquist, Michael, 169170, 172174,
308n12
Homogeneity, in social and cultural theory,
122
Honor, 129, 136
Howard, Donald R., 240
Huizinga, Johan, 238, 240
Hume, David, 30
Hutchins, Edwin, 37, 100
Hybridization, 181, 266, 285
Hymes, Dell, 126

345

Ex

Identication, as process of subjectivity, 33


Identity, 34; theories of, 49; perspectives
on, 912; altered, 18; practice theory of,
1946; heuristic development of, 4042;
and gured worlds, 49, 60, 63, 65; and
activity, 5657; in Alcoholics Anonymous,
66, 68, 7076, 8788, 9697; in world of
romance, 9899, 105111, 115116,
119122, 144; positional, 125143, 146,
148, 163, 235; and self-authoring, 169,
172, 183, 190191; and mental illness,
192213; of Nepali women, 214232,
253269; contexts of, 271273;
social/political work of, 281286
Identity diffusion, 7273
Identity in practice, 270287
Identity reconstitution, 7273, 75
Illness models, 6869, 71
Imagined communities, 46, 247250
Improvisation, 1518, 45, 63, 270, 275277

Jackson, W. T. H., 242


Jaeger, C. Stephen, 241, 244, 246
Jefferson, Thomas, 248
Joas, Hans, 190
Joravsky, David, 35
Kabyle people, 129, 135136, 252, 306n9
Kami caste, 142, 311n3
Kant, Immanuel, 6, 208
Kathmandu, Nepal, 255
Kay, Sarah, 239, 245
Keeping one man up front, 112
Kharkov school, 39, 41, 177
Kipnis, Andrew, 30, 294n17, 297n3
Kitayama, S., 30, 293n17
Kondo, D. K., 30, 138139, 141142, 281
Kozulin, Alex, 60
Kuhn, Manford, 68, 199
Kulick, Don, 305n16
Labeling theory, 310n8
Lacan, Jacques, 28, 33
Lachicotte, William S., Jr., 3, 180, 199, 236,
277
Lafayette, Marie-Joseph-Paul de, 248
Lakoff, Robin, 130
Lancaster, Roger N., 129
Lancelot, 244
Language, 170174, 178180, 182, 192,
278280
Lave, Jean, 5657, 176, 303n8

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index
Meaning systems, 11, 99100, 116118,
120121
Mediating devices, 3538, 50, 6162, 67, 71,
88, 96, 175, 185, 209, 235. See also
Semiotic mediation
Mehinaku people, 51, 133134
Menstruation, 253, 267
Mental health workers, 180182
Mental illness, 41, 43, 192213, 272,
275277
Merleau-Ponty, Maurice, 2223
Merten, Don, 68, 7273, 7576
Microcultures, 298n10
Miller, J. G., 20, 22, 31
Minick, Norris, 177
Mnemonic objects, 36
Motivation, 98, 100, 113, 121
Murray, D. W., 2930

Co

Lawrence, Nancy, 164, 283


Leavitt, John, 297n3
Lederman, Rena, 304n14
Lee, Benjamin, 51, 307n4, 308n10
Leonora of Castile, 244
Leontiev, A. N., 3839, 41, 56, 295n26
Lvi-Strauss, Claude, 170, 276
Levy-Bruhl, Lucien, 36
Lexicons. See Gender-marked lexicons
Life stories. See Personal stories
Linguistic behavior, 1113, 130131
Linguistic choice, 126128; expressing
deference, 127
Linguistics, 169, 178
Logic, cultural, 9, 11, 1315, 17, 274
Logic of emotion, 37
Love. See Courtly love; Romance, world of
Luria, Alexander R., 35, 37, 187
Luttrell, Wendy, 308n11
Lutz, Catherine, 12, 71, 127, 290n9,
297n3
Lykes, M. B., 136
Lynch, Michael, 307n15

py

346

Ex

am

MacAndrew, Craig, 67
Machismo, 129130
Madison, James, 248
Major depressive disorder, 196
Malory, Thomas, 246
Mamet, David: Oleanna, 165, 283
Manic-depression, 4243, 196, 202203,
206207, 210211
March, Kathryn, 315n9
Marie of Champagne, 244, 246
Marion, Francis, 248
Markus, Hazel R., 30, 293n17, 302n5
Marriott, M., 21, 29
Marsh, Diane T., 311n9
Marx, Karl, 3435, 64; Kapital, 34
Marxism, 57, 64, 193, 284
Mary, Virgin, 245, 314n11
Mauss, Marcel, 87
McCall, George J., 289n2
McConnell-Ginet, Sally, 13
McDermott, R. P., 127
McHugh, E., 291n3
McLeod, Enid, 245
McPartland, Thomas S., 68
Mead, G. H., 4, 99, 172, 189190
Meaning, 5, 22, 170, 173, 177, 186187

Narrativized identities, 4345, 127


Narrativized worlds, 53
Nationalism, 46, 49, 247250, 287
Natural self, 2022
Naudada, Nepal, 9, 11, 13, 17, 44, 5354,
59, 62, 125, 128, 130131, 134, 138140,
142143, 190, 214232, 253269,
273277, 284
Neisser, Ulric, 292n8
Nepal, 3, 718, 1921, 4145, 5354,
5960, 62, 64, 88, 125128, 130131,
134135, 138140, 142143, 190191,
214232, 252, 253269, 273277, 284
Neuroses, 199, 207
Newar people, 1921
Newman, F. X., 239
Nurius, Paula, 302n5
OBarr, William, 12, 130
Obeyesekere, Gananath, 2223, 292n9
Objectication, 45, 17, 39, 142143, 186,
278, 281
Occitania, 239, 241247, 250, 252
Orchestration, 178184, 190, 211, 235, 238,
272
Ortner, Sherry, 17
Ovid, 242
Oxford Group Movement, 6869
Paden, W. D., 240
Page, Heln, 134

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index

py

Psychological anthropology, 293n15


Psychological tools, 60
Psychology, 4, 20, 26, 28, 34, 38, 63, 174,
177, 180, 184, 209, 212, 293n15
Psychoses, 196, 199
Psychotherapy, 180181
Quinn, Naomi, 52, 55, 100, 121, 297n3

Rabelais, Franois, 237


Radi, (widows), 54, 140, 217218, 224226
Raheja, Gloria G., 228
Rajniti songs, 229230, 262, 266268, 273,
276, 284
Rape, 157
Reconstruction, identity, 7273, 75
Reeves, James R., 296n32
Reication, 293n17
Reiter, R. R., 23
Relational identity. See Positional identity
Resources. See Cultural resources
Revere, Paul, 248
Rishi Pancami, 228, 254255, 258, 267
Robertson, D. W., 239
Robins, Lee N., 67
Robinson, David, 87
Rogoff, Barbara, 135
Roiphe, Katie, 166
Role constriction, 310n8
Role theory, 310n8
Romance, world of, 8, 41, 4346, 53, 5657,
59, 65, 98122, 129130, 137, 140141,
144166, 174, 178179, 183184, 190,
272, 286
Rosaldo, Michelle Z., 302n10
Ross, G., 83
Rougemont, Denis de, 240, 246
Rouse, Roger, 190

Ex

am

Co

Painter, Sidney, 244


Panchayat system, 214, 229, 262264
Parish, S., 1920
Parsons, Talcott, 99, 310n8
Parvati, 254255
Parzival, 244
Passeron, J. C., 144
Paterson, Linda M., 239, 242245
Personality disorders, 196199, 202204,
207208, 210, 277
Personal stories, 43; in Alcoholics
Anonymous, 6697, 174;
reinterpretations of past in, 8897; of
Nepali women, 227232, 256269
Personhood, 45, 3132, 204, 211
Perspectives, 4446, 182183, 296n32
Peterson, John, 6869, 79
Petrarch (Francesco Petrarca), 245
Piaget, Jean, 175
Pinup calendars, 159160, 164
Pitt, William, 248
Play/play worlds, 4951, 63, 236239,
272273, 283
Pollution, 911, 1314, 16, 127, 134, 142,
253, 267, 273
Position. See Social position; Subject position
Positional identity, 125143, 146, 148, 163,
235
Positionality, 4345, 58, 146, 148,
271272
Postmodernism, 16, 32, 44, 249, 286
Poststructuralism, 16
Power, 5760, 125, 127, 273274
Practice. See Communities of practice;
Identity in practice; Self-in-practice
Practice theory, 17, 1946, 55
Prestige, 5960, 113, 121, 157158, 162
Price, Laurie, 17
Privilege, 5760, 125, 131, 133, 136, 142,
271, 273
Privileging spaces, 160163
Productions. See Cultural productions
Prociency, 117118, 120
Propositionality, 128
Pryor, John, 148, 158, 162163
Psychiatry, 180, 195199, 204206, 208,
211212
Psychoanalytic theory, 33, 198
Psychodynamic theory, 78, 2223, 33, 100,
199, 270, 294n21

347

Sacco, Nicola, 249


Said, Edward, 132
Salience. See Cognitive salience
Samoa, 1920, 26, 3031
Sanday, Peggy R., 162
Sanders, W. B., 157
Sarki caste, 142, 311n3
Savoir faire, 98, 111116, 119, 122
Scaffolding, 8384
Scheff, Thomas J., 310n8
Schizoaffective disorder, 202, 310n7

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index
Skinner, Debra, 3, 911, 1314, 17, 43,
5354, 60, 102, 125, 137138, 140, 142,
145, 151, 214218, 222224, 228, 253,
255, 260263, 265266, 273, 284
Smith, N. B., 240
Smith, Paul, 29
Snow, J. T., 240
Social afliation, 127128
Social audience, 189
Social categories, 130131
Social constructivism. See Constructivist
perspective
Socially constructed self, 2628
Social position, 5, 1112, 14, 16, 33, 4445,
176, 294n18; and positional identity,
126128, 132139; in world of romance,
144, 155, 158; and identity in practice,
270271, 274275, 286287
Social psychology, 4, 6, 35
Social situation, 4, 9, 1112, 14
Social speech, 174175, 182, 184189, 283,
308n9
Social work, 134135, 271, 281286
Sociocentric self, 2931
Sociohistorical school of psychology, 5,
3338, 45, 5657, 60, 64, 138, 174, 177,
223, 270, 294n21
Sociology, 4, 6, 38, 127, 180181, 286
Southern University, 101
Soviet Union, 34, 64, 278
Space of authoring, 63, 169, 190, 193, 210,
272, 282284, 286287. See also
Self-authoring
Speakers meetings (AA), 77, 82, 87
Speech, 174178, 180, 182, 184189
Spenser, Edmund, 246
Spiro, Melford E., 2930, 99, 105, 116, 118,
120
Spradley, James, 3738, 193; You Owe
Yourself a Drunk, 67
Stalin, Joseph, 250
Status, 5859, 103104, 125, 131, 136, 158,
178
Stereotypical associations, in social
positioning, 130131
Stevens, John, 240
Stone, Linda, 216
Stone, Michael H., 199
Storylines, 297n4

Ex

am

Co

Schizophrenias, 196198, 207


Schneider, Joseph S., 67
Schur, Edwin, 67
Schwartz, Gary, 68, 7273, 7576
Scolding, 224226
Scott, Robert A., 71
Self, 289n5, 290n1, 291nn3,7, 293n14;
discourses/practices of, 1928; talking
about, 1920; universal versus culturally
specic, 2023; critical disruption
concerning, 2326; socially constructed,
2628; emerging directions in study of,
2833; sites of, 2930;
Bakhtinian-sociohistoric view of, 3342;
case studies, 4246
Self-authoring, 32, 63, 169191; and mental
illness, 192213; by Nepali women,
214232. See also Space of authoring
Self-censorship, 60, 138
Self-control, 4344, 46, 6365
Self-directed symbolizations, 277281
Self-fashioning, 4, 29, 43, 60, 169, 177, 185,
191, 282. See also Self-authoring
Self-in-practice, 1920, 28, 3032, 3840,
46, 60
Self-objectication, 5
Self-production, 2829
Self-understanding, 34, 8, 65, 282; in
Alcoholics Anonymous, 66, 68, 7075,
82, 8788, 9697; in world of romance,
118, 121; and mental illness, 210211
Selye, Hans, 209
Semiotic mediation, 3538, 40, 137, 143,
178, 182, 192193, 280, 294n23. See also
Mediating devices
Sexism. See Sexual harassment; Symbolic
violence
Sexual harassment, 148, 163166, 283
Sexuality. See Courtly love; Romance, world
of
Shakespeare, William, 246
Shango cult, 1516, 297n7
Shepel, Elina, 64
Shiva, 254255
Shotter, John, 53
Shweder, R. A., 21, 2829, 54, 292n9
Silva, J. Donald, 283
Simmons, J. L., 289n2
Situated learning, 5657

py

348

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

Index
Universalist perspective, 2023, 27
Universal self, 2122

py

Valsiner, Jaan, 3, 6, 304n13, 307n7, 309n13


Van Gennep, Arnold: Rites of Passage, 72
Van Langenhove, L., 302n2, 306n6
Van der Veer, Ren, 6, 304n13, 307n7
Vanzetti, Bartolomeo, 249
Voices/vocal images, 128, 180, 183184,
190, 235, 272
Voloshinov, V. N., 3436, 39, 41, 141, 175,
186189, 192, 237, 309n14; Marxism and
the Philosophy of Language, 6
Vygotsky, L. S., 56, 8, 32, 3438, 40, 4951,
58, 6164, 84, 100, 111, 117119, 137,
141142, 174178, 182187, 190191,
192, 195, 235236, 238, 251, 270, 272,
278282, 284, 295nn23,26, 304n13,
307nn78, 308nn8,10,12, 309n13,
313nn12; The Psychology of Art, 6

Co

Strauss, Claudia, 55, 312n4


Stress, 208209, 311n9
Subjectication, 3233, 40, 182, 193
Subjectivity, 78, 15, 18, 19, 2526, 3132,
42, 238; and inner speech, 174178; in
mental illness, 195, 212; of Nepali
women, 259, 262; and identity in practice,
286287
Subject position, 1218, 46
Sunar caste, 911, 311n3
Symbolic bootstrapping, 38, 4244, 177, 267
Symbolic capital, 129, 152, 155, 157158,
160162, 272, 282
Symbolic interactionism, 146
Symbolic mastery, 294n23
Symbolic violence, 144, 146163
Symbolizations, self-directed, 277281
Symbols. See Cultural symbols
Syndromes, 197

349

Ex

am

Tannen, Deborah, 131


Thakali people, 229, 312n11
Therapeutic identity, 198, 210212
Thevenot, L., 152
Thomas, Clarence, 164, 283
Thompson, E. P., 253
Thomsen, Robert, 76
Thought disorders, 196
Thurnwald, Richard, 36
Tij festival, 45, 190191, 224, 228232,
253269, 276, 281, 284285
Tij songs, 43, 221, 228232, 253, 255269,
284
Tij-Rishi Pancami ritual complex, 228,
254255, 258, 267
Todorov, Tzvetan, 295n29
Tools, 3536, 38, 50, 6062, 64, 140
Tools of identity, 4243
Too Young? Teenagers and AA, 77, 89
Tranquilizers, 196197
Traube, Elizabeth, 62
Tristan, 240, 244
Trosset, Carol S., 135
Troubadours, 239, 242247
Troyes, France, 244246
Turner, Terrence, 127
Twelve Steps, 7577, 87
Tylor, Edward, 141

Washington, George, 248


Weber, Max, 59, 237
Weiner, Annette B., 23, 49
Wenger, Etienne, 5657, 176, 303n8
Wertsch, James V., 6, 37, 56, 174, 177,
307n3, 308nn89
Western concept of self, 21, 2930
Weston, Kath, 154
What Happened to Joe, 75
White, G. M., 31
Willard, Charity C., 245
Willis, Paul, 166, 304n12, 308n11
Witchcraft, 5556, 59
Wolof, Senegal, 12, 14
Womens rights movement (Nepal),
214215, 229, 231, 262265, 267268
Wood, D. J., 83
Words, social valence of, 52, 171174, 177,
182, 186, 188
Wulff, Helena, 298n10
Young People and AA, 6970, 74, 77, 7982
Zone of proximal development, 176, 183,
190, 272, 308n12

Copyright 1998 The President and Fellows of Harvard College

You might also like