You are on page 1of 15

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237733277

"Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia" in Women with


Right-Hemisphere Syndrome: A Psychoanalytic
Perspective on Neurological Body Knowledge
Disorders

Article January 2014


DOI: 10.1080/15294145.2005.10773494

CITATIONS READS

9 32

4 authors, including:

Catherine Morin
French Institute of Health and Medical Resea
59 PUBLICATIONS 1,510 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Catherine Morin
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 15 September 2016
Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 2005, 7 (2) 47

Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere


Syndrome: A Psychoanalytic Perspective on Neurological Body
Knowledge Disorders

Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl,


and Dominique Mazevet (Paris)

Disorders of discourse and behaviorin particular anosognosia and somatoparaphreniamay be associated with body schema
neurological disorders in the right-hemisphere syndrome (RHS). From a psychoanalytic point of view, the content of the patients
discourse may be considered as an alteration of the unified conception an individual has of him/herselfthat is, of what Lacan
called specular image. After presenting the specificities of the Lacanian conception of specular image, we consider its heuristic
value in the RHS field, based on three detailed observations of women with anosognosia and hemineglect. Each of the three
patients personified her paralyzed hand as being or belonging to a daughter, whether real or imagined. We propose considering
rephrase to this symptom as a disclosure of the normally hidden object of the demand. We briefly discuss how these observations indirectly
clarify? shed light on the mindbrain problem.

Right-brain lesions are known to alter what neuro- mate, cumbersome, or supernumerary object, or per-
scientists (see Gallagher, 1986; Paillard, 1999) sonification of the paralyzed limbs. This assimilation
currently refer to as body schemathat is, a non- to an object or to a person is often also colored with
conscious body representation that allows us to hatred or disdain; this led Critchley (1962) to put
automatically adjust our gestures to space (Coslett, forward the term misoplegia.
1998). The most common symptom of this disturbance While neurologists are principally interested in the
is hemineglecta lack of interest to the left hemispace knowledge we have of our bodies, psychoanalysts are
and/or the left hemibody (Benton & Sivan, 1993). concerned with narcissismthat is, the libidinal inter-
Certain right hemispheric lesions also produce specific est we invest in these bodiesa difference that, at least
discursive and behavioral disorders that concern body at first sight, seems to seriously limit the scope of
representation (see Critchley, 1953; Feinberg, 1997; exchanges between the two disciplines. Indeed, the
Hcaen & de Ajurriaguerra, 1952). While asomato- first psychoanalytic study of the human subjects rela-
gnosia (unawareness of the paralyzed hemibody) is a tionship with his body is Freuds seminal 1914 article.1
rare and transient symptom, anosognosia (apparent Freud stated that, as an infant, the child first enjoys his
unawareness of or indifference to left hemiplegia; see various body parts, including body orifices (auto-
Marcel, Tegnr, & Nimmo-Smith, 2004) is much more erotism), and then secondarily attaches his libido to his
frequent and persistent. Hemineglect, asomatognosia, ego. Freud only indicated that a new psychical ac-
and anosognosia may be accompanied by productive tion should permit that change. In his 1914 article,
symptoms called somatoparaphrenia by Gertsmann Freud only allusively mentions the unifying concept
(1942): assimilation of paralyzed limbs to an inani- that every person has of both his body and his identity

Catherine Morin: CNRS LEAPLE (UMR 8606), Paris; Service de Mdecine Physique et Radaptation, Groupe Hospitalier Piti-Salptrire, Paris.
Stphane Thibierge: CNRS LEAPLE (UMR 8606), Paris; UFR Sciences Humaines et Arts, Universit de Poitiers. Pascale Bruguire: Service de Mdecine
Physique et Radaptation, Groupe Hospitalier Piti-Salptrire, Paris; Service de Psychiatrie Adultes, Groupe Hospitalier Piti-Salptrire, Paris. Pascale
Pradat-Diehl and Dominique Mazevet: Service de Mdecine Physique et Radaptation, Groupe Hospitalier Piti-Salptrire, Paris; INSERM E0349, Paris V.
Correspondence to: Dr. Catherine Morin, Service de Mdecine Physique et Radaptation, Groupe Hospitalier Piti-Salptrire, 47 Bd de lHpital,
75651 Paris Cedex 13, France (email: catherine.morin@chups.jussieu.fr).
Acknowledgement: Our thanks are due to Mary Shaffer, who reread the English.
1
Following the example of Allan Sheridan (1977), we employ the masculine pronouns each time the human subject is designated: this corresponds to the
French tradition of using the masculine pronouns to indicate either a male person or a person characterized independently of his/her gender.
48 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

(a unity comparable to the ego cannot exist in the ated by a regard that gives us access to a mirror <gaze?>
individual from the start). In addition, while representation of ourselves (Lacan, 1977a); this regard
asomatognosia and anosognosia were known at his is oriented by linguistic lines of force. Lacans reflec-
ref? time, Freud never alluded to these phenomena. Unlike tion had been stimulated by Wallons psychological
Freud, Schilder (1999) insisted that we mentally con- observations. In 1931, Wallon (1994) wrote how the not in refs
ceive our body as a unity. In 1935, he proposed the notion of its own body develops in the child between
term body image to encompass the heterogeneous the age of 6 and 24 months. Wallon observed that, at
aspects (in his words, physiologic, libidinal, and socio- that time, after first treating its own body as if it were
logic) of this unity.2 He considered anosognosia as a made of distinct parts each with a personal life, the
disorder of body image and assumed that it was due to child discovers his image in the mirror and displays a
what he called organic repression. But he realized jubilant activity in front of this image. The child also
that this concept was not fully adequate, since he turns toward the accompanying adult, seeking ac-
considered the nature of the psychosexual aspects of knowledgement and assurance that this image is his
this unawareness of paralysis to be an open question. It own. In The Mirror Stage as Formative of the Func-
was Lacan (1977a) who proposed a well-founded tion of the I as Revealed in the Psychoanalytical Expe-
theory that illuminates the question left unexplained by rience, Lacan (1977a) reread Wallons observations
Freud (1914)that is, how autoerotism is replaced by (1931) in terms of identification: for him, during the
narcissism. Lacan claimed that body image has a mirror phase, the body passes from a real state (frag-
prominent role in creating the necessary illusion of the mented body) to an imaginary register (virtual image).
unity of the self, a representation he called specular Lacan underlined that this identification is crucial for
image. Like Schilder, Lacan used the term image in a subjectivity. In its Lacanian sense, identification
sense that differed from the neurological one; for him, literally the acquisition of what is usually referred to as
image refers both to self-knowledge and to narcissism. an identityis defined as the transformation that hap-
Moreover, Lacan proposed that the limits of the knowl- pens in a subject when he assumes an image. The
edge we can have of our whole world are linked to this human subject then identifies himself with the image
image. This position makes it possible to establish a of a complete body, erected as a whole (imaginary
true dialogue between psychoanalysis and neurology identification). Identification also operates in the sym-
in the field of the right-hemisphere syndrome (RHS). bolic register.3 Insofar as the adult indicates and recog-
In this article, we first present the Lacanian concept of nizes the body as belonging to that particular child,
the role of body representation in organizing subjectiv- wordsand in particular a personal nameare at-
ity and underline the points that distinguish it from the tached to that image (Lacan, 1966b). This validation
concurrent Freudian concepts. Based on our previous through a proper noun makes the infant a human
studies and on three detailed observations, we then subject, who recognizes his body as a whole, similar in
propose a Lacanian reading of the psychiatric-like its form to others bodies, while it is his own body
disturbances of discourse or behavior that may accom- recorded in filiation and sexual belonging.4 Lacan
pany the neurological disorders of body schema. (1962) called this complex structure specular image
(see also Thibierge, 1999b). Winnicott (1982) also
Self-representation of the human body in insisted later upon the crucial role of the gaze of the ok as changed ?
psychoanalysis: the notion of specular image mother in the structuring of what he called the self.
Lacan was hardly unaware of the neurological cor-
The mirror phase and specular image relates of body representation, as shown by his refer-
ence to Jean Lhermitte (Lacan, 1966a, p. 185) and his
In 1936 and 1949 (see Guillerault, 2003, pp. 267272), qualifying the cerebral cortex as the intra-organic
Lacan proposed that our self-representation is medi- mirror (1977a). He insisted on the discordance, at the
2
The term body image is thus most confusing. After having been
used interchangeably with that of body schema in neurological literature, 3
Lacan (1966b) revisited the terms ideal ego and ego ideal coined
it currently has a specific neurological meaning: unlike body schema, it by Freud (1923); Lacan assimilated the former to the virtual form of body
refers to a semantic knowledge both of the names of the parts of the body image and the latter to the symbolic traits that represent the subject in the
and of their spatial relations with clothes or with other bodies; left-brain register of language.
lesions may alter this knowledge, causing autotopoagnosia (Buxbaum & 4
As recently shown by Thibierge (1999a, 1999b), psychosisin par-
Coslett, 2001). This neurological meaning has very little to do with the ticular, the so-called delusional misidentificationsstrikingly illustrates
psychoanalytic meaning of the term. In addition, one may find in the the crucial importance of this symbolic identification: in Capgras and
literature on eating disorders (Stein et al., 2003) and disabilities (Keppel & Frgoli syndromes, the name is not attached to body image and does not
Crowe, 2000) casual utilization of the term body image to refer merely to refer to the variants of one and the same singular image. As a result, names
the physical body appearance. and images proliferate into an infinite series of reduplications.
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 49

time of the mirror phase, between the infants ability to ment in sexual life or in the stages of bodily education,
identify with a whole form and his organic motor that makes some of them erogenous (Lacan, 1977c):
immaturitythat is, his lack of knowledge and mas- the mouth is the area where food and love are de-
tery of his own body. He also referred to the phantom- manded of the Other, the eye and the ear are the areas
limb phenomenon (see Ramachandran & Hirstein, where the Others demand or desire is expressed
gaze?
1998) to illustrate the normal unawareness that charac- through regard or voice, the anus is the area where the
terizes our own intuitive body representation. How- Other exerts his demand. Above all, the corporeal
ever, according to Lacan, normal unawareness extends aspects of this symbolization of our relationship to the
far beyond failing to acknowledge organic deficien- Other are repressed. The fundamental property of the
cies, and perhaps the term ignorance would be more object is thus to be lacking, not to be present, in body
convenient: we often ignore the role of the symbolic imagethat is, not to be representable at all.6 It should
traits that were attributed to us from and before our be emphasized that it is precisely insofar as body
birth in making our body image a unique whole form image lacks the object that this image may gain con-
regarded as one and the same. We also usually ignore sistency, and our or others visible bodies may arouse
the relationships between our narcissistic attachment our desire. The primordial lack of the object, which
clarify this sentence?to body image and our unconscious determination by may appear as an irreducible loss, is designated by
or something what Lacan called object. psychoanalysis as castration (Lacan, 1977c).
missing?
The lack of the object has a kind of positive
symbolic representation, under the guise of the phal-
The Lacanian concept of object lusbut as a purely symbolic element, a signifier
(Lacan, 1977b). For example, the normal representa-
While Freud (1914) only took note of the fact that we tion of the human body as standing, symmetric and
love our body, Lacan specified the two aspects of this erect implies interference between this phallic signifier
libido attached to the body. For him, on the one hand, and the imaginary body representation. As underlined
the human subject is captivated by the form of the by Lacan in his seminar on anxiety (1962), any other
human body, and on the other, his body is represented positive appearance of the lost object has disorganiz-
for an Otherthat is, it is necessarily experienced, in a ing effects. These effects extend from the varieties of
positive or negative light, as an object of desire for feelings of strangeness, which have been described in
others. Lacan qualified these fundamental aspects of classical psychiatry (see Thibierge, 1999b), to deper-
the psychic correlates of body representation as real sonalization. An important point in this seminar is that
insofar as they are not reducible to its imaginary or Lacan considered that all the circumstances and liter-
symbolic aspects: what I represent for the Other and ary fictions to which Freud (1919) attributed an un-
his desire is precisely what I cannot either master or canny nature involved an undue appearance of the
have a clear knowledge of.5 In other words, a narcissis- object in the field of reality. The failure of the castra-
tically invested body represents the subjects question tion process, the presence of the object, is obvious in
regarding his ability to please, to suit to the Others psychotic verbal hallucinations (voice-object) and
demand or desire. This impalpable x is what Lacan scopic (regard-object) persecutions (Czermak, 1986). gaze?
called object (Lacan, 1962, 1977c). Lacan (1977c) Contrary to our intuitive apprehension of having a
showed that the suitable object for this desire or for unified and autonomous self, Lacanian psychoanalysis
this demand may be thought of under four fundamental thus emphasizes that human subjectivity is heterogene-
aspects: the breast and the feces (oral and anal objects ous and Other-dependent. The term specular image
gaze? corresponding to the Others demand), the regard and designates the intertwining of three levels or three
the voice (scopic and oral objects corresponding to the registers: (i) the object, which is not any thing or
Others desire). This notion of object, which Lacan person in the external world but delimits the modalities
considered his major contribution to psychoanalysis, of the subjects value and position in the Others eyes;
does not contradict but is more precise than that pro- (ii) the body image; and (iii) the signifiers, which
posed by Freud (1905). According to Lacan, it is the represent him in the symbolic order.
symbolic value of body areas, not just their involve- The consequences of this heterogeneity go far be-
yond the avatars of self-representation. As emphasized
5
Despite being possibly personified in the eyes of a given subject by a by Thibierge (1999b), our body image provides the
variety of human characters, the Other, as indicated by its capital initial, is
not a fellow creature, but is intrinsically foreign to the subject. For 6
This object has no more recognizable meaning than a letter of the
example, God may be considered as the figure of the Other in believers alphabet, for example, when used in an algebraic function. This led Lacan
minds. to propose the term objet a.
50 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

ground pattern of our entire external-world representa- tion and introjection (two ways of putting the object
tion. As a result, what we ordinarily call perception inside) in mourning provides a striking illustration of
consists of recognizing structures that are familiar the difference between mourning subjects as being
that is, congruent with our body representationwhile whether they have or have not taken into account the
our deliberate observations are guided by the search loss of the object. According to Abraham and Torok,
for the lacking, lost object. what is put insidein their words, what is swal-
lowedin normal mourning is the loss (introjection),
while pathological mourning consists of swallowing
Implications of the Lacanian concept the lost object itself (incorporation), so that the object
of the object for mourning, spatial cognition, is still present, psychically not lost.
and pathological object/body image
relationships.
Spatial cognition and body image
The Lacanian concept of the object holds that the
efficiency of the object is something whose lack (and While Kaplan-Solms and Solms (2002) consider that
not whose presence) makes us psychically living sub- space is integrated into the narcissistically invested
jects. It differs from the contemporary Freudian con- body, Lacan proposes that narcissism results in our
cept of both the subjects relationship to his body seeing the external world according to our body image.
image and his object cathexis. This difference has The external space is thus structured according to the
implications in three areas that are addressed in this pattern of our body image. Like the stability of body
article, namely mourning, spatial cognition and patho- image, this process is efficient only on the condition
logical object/body image relationships. that the objects loss has been symbolized.

Mourning, desire, and castration Object and body image


In the concept developed after Freud by Karl Abraham Thibierge (1999a), following Czermak (1986) and
(1949) and Melanie Klein (1940), love is considered a based on the clinical phenomena of Frgoli and Cap-
maturation of libido: libido is meant to concern only gras delusions, recently proposed to reduce the various
partial objects that the subject considers part of his psychotic syndromes to the avatars of one and the same
own body, while love addresses the whole object, trouble: a nonentangling of body image and naming,
perceived as external by the subject. In this Kleinian with, as a result, the inconsistency of body image and
view, mourning consists of the subject reinstalling the the persistence in the foreground of the positived
lost object inside for the moment. object. Moreover, Thibierge also suggested, based
In the Lacanian concept, desire and not love is what upon classical neurological descriptions of disorders of
makes us living subjects. What interests us in the body image (Hcaen & de Ajurriaguerra, 1952) that
external world is the unceasingly disappointed hope of neurological body schema disorders could be associ-
finding the lost object once again. This lack of object, ated with or result in body image alterations, thus
castration, is the operation that permits us to be psychi- bringing the normally repressed object to the fore
cally alive, desiringthat is, in a certain sense, neu- (Thibierge, 1999a).7 According to this view, two differ-
rotic subjects. In this view, normal mourning consists ent pathologies would thus share a common pair of
of passing through a new version of castration, as one symptoms: a nonunified body representation and the
experiences the events of life. The ability to pass intrusive presence of the object in the patients psychic
though mourning is thus linked to castration, to repres- reality. This suggests that the unification of body repre-
sion of the corporeal coordinates of the object, and to sentation is normally tightly linked with the repression
desire. Pathological mourning does not consist so of the object. In other words, the stability of body image
much of a pathological attitude (introjection, projec- and the psychic presence of the object would be mutu-
tion, love, or hatred) toward an external object, as of ally exclusive.
revealing a fundamental failure to take (or to be more
precise, to have taken) into account the primordial 7
It is interesting to note that, based on a different point of view,
nature of the lack of object. Abraham and Toroks Gallagher and Voever (in press) also gather psychotic symptoms and RHS published?
(1972) concept of the difference between incorpora- under a common labelthat is, disorders of embodiment.
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 51

The specificity of psychic symptoms of right anosognosia.8 These patients seemed uninvolved in
vs. left hemispheric lesions: psychological, their own enunciation, a typical sentence being They
Freudian, and Lacanian perspectives say that I am performing better. Their self-portraits
were skewed and asymmetric. Not only was the left
Indeed, anosognosia, asomatognosia, and somatopara- part of the body often incomplete or lacking, due to
phrenia simultaneously undermine the patients body body schema alteration, but some patients added su-
knowledge, the unified representation he has of him- pernumerary details when asked to complete their
self, and, as particularly clear in somatoparaphrenia, half-portraits, or they produced completely disor-
his narcissism. ganized portraits (Morin, Pradat-Diehl, Robain, et al.,
2003; Morin, Thibierge, & Perrigot, 2001). These
traits strongly suggest that body schema disorders are
Anosognosia associated with an alteration of specular image, and
not at all with the maintenance of an ideal I-image.
The specificity of psychic reactions to right-brain le- Obtaining knowledge of reality through perception
sions has already been underlined by Gainotti (1972): or cognition normally consists of recognizing struc-
while catastrophic reactions, depression, and mourning tures that are organized around the absence of the
are observed in left-brain lesions, apathy and indiffer- object (Thibierge, 1999b), as stated above. According
ence are observed in right-brain lesions. Anosognosia to this concept, taking paralysis into account and real-
of left hemiplegia is the most striking symptom in a istically anticipating its consequences implies being
series of characteristics for which professional care- able to refer this paralysis to the virtual image of a
givers have one label: he is a lefty (cest un gauche symbolically incomplete body. It also implies passing
in French). Lefties display a variety of behavioral or through a mourning processthat is, through a new
discursive symptoms. They are unrealistic, at risk of version of the primordial loss of the object. In the
falling due to their not taking into account the precau- absence of body schema disorders, right hemiplegic
tionary advice given by their therapists, not committed patients, like left hemiplegic and other handicapped
all Morin et al to their rehabilitation, and so forth (Morin, Taillefer, et patients with no brain lesions, have access to such an
cites identified by al., 2001). Anosognosia ceaselessly raises an image and experience mourning, as attested to by their
second author
irresolvable contradiction: although its close links with discourses and self-portraits (see above). This seems
right hemispheric lesions are well known, reliable au- not to be the case in neurological patients with body
thors continue to underline the psychological aspects schema disorders, who may behave as if they were not
of this symptom (Kaplan-Solms and Solms, 2002; paralyzed and deny or ignore a pessimistic medical
Prigatano & Weinstein, 1996; Ramachandran, 1994), a prognosis regarding the recovery of arm function.
possibility that Babinski himself considered in his Kaplan-Solms and Solms (2002) consider, from a psy-
seminal 1914 article. Ramachandran (1994) and choanalytic point of view, that contrary to aphasic
Kaplan-Solms and Solms (2002) consider anosognosia patients, patients with RHS experience a failure of the
as a manifestation of Freudian repression, an interpre- process of mourning.
tation that will be discussed below. Prigatano and
Weinstein (1996) consider that anosognosia involves a
denial process, which occurs in patients whose psychic
rigidity prevents them from accepting any alteration of
their ideal ego. This interpretation implies that
anosognosia should result in maintaining the ideal 8
In the absence of body schema disorders, patients used undetermined
grammatical subjects when designating themselves as ill or handicapped
image of these patients. Indeed, when comparing the people, a typical sentence being I am affected mainly because you depend
discourses and self-portrait drawings of right versus upon everybody (Morin & Salazar-Orvig, 1996). Their typical self-por-
left stroke patients (Morin, Pradat-Diehl, Robain, traits were unclothed and lacked both hands and a mouth. Such alterations
Bensalah, Perrigot, 2003; Morin & Salazar-Orvig, may also be observed not only in paraplegic (Morin, Pradat-Diehl, Robain,
et al., 2003), but also in elderly, chronically ill, or depressed patients (see
1996), we obtained results that run counter to this Clment et al., 1996; Morin & Bensalah, 1998). This indicated that, like
hypothesis. Our observations showed that, while pa- many other stressful life events, hemiplegia by itself obliges patients to
tients with left hemispheric lesions maintained a self- mourn their ideal image, momentarily replaced by an impersonal, sexless,
representation with a phallic value, able to structure and lacunary representation. The nonrepresentation of the asymmetry of
the hemiplegic body indicates that this goes hand in hand with the
their ideal ego, this was not at all the case in right- repression of the specific and concrete aspects of the loss caused by
brain-injured patients with body schema disorders and hemiplegia.
52 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

Somatoparaphrenia A Lacanian approach to anosognosia


and somatoparaphrenia in three cases
Like anosognosia, somatoparaphrenia has been the
subject of many different psychological interpreta- The three case observationsDT, MN, DSwere ok as added?
tions. Halligan, Marshall, and Wade (1995) and gathered in a rehabilitation unit devoted to stroke
Feinberg (2001) consider it to be a rationalization that hemiplegia. Rehabilitation activities provide an oppor-
allows the patient to cope with the traumatic failure of tunity for neurological and psychological observation,
his body schema, or to express feelings regarding his since patients may be observed over long periods, and
own present situation. Other authors consider the numerous transferential relationships involved in
somatoparaphrenia as completely meaningless produc- therapeutics offer privileged and nonintrusive access
tions (Laplane, 1998). Nevertheless, these productions to various everyday-life psychic manifestations such as
display recurrent themes, which are stable in a given depression, mourning, unawareness of hemiplegia,
patient and may be very similar from one patient to delusional phenomena, and so on. Among the three
another. Feinberg (2001) thus noticed that female pa- patients presented here, one patient (DS) had psycho-
tients very often personified their hands as their hus- therapeutic interviews with P.B., a Freudian psycho-
bands. Delahousse (1972) and Thibierge (1999a) have analyst. C.M. was the referring neurologist of MN and
proposed that somatoparaphrenia might be linked to DT and had two research interviews with DS. All
the undue appearance of the object (with its strange- observations were worked through in discussions with
ness, its bodily links, its extraneous hostile or S.T., a Lacanian psychoanalyst. All three patients ex-
persecutory characteristics) in the very place where a hibited intense hemineglect in cancellation and copy-
major loss (that of hand command) has occurred. Due ing tests. DT and MN were seen during the first 3
to the relationships of object repression and knowledge months after their stroke, and their discourse and
(described above), anosognosia might also be linked to behavior were observed by their therapists. DS was
the undue psychic presence of the object. This hypoth- seen much later; therefore, a part of the data com-
esis is supported by our previous observations (Morin, mented on here are reported self-observations that
Durand, et al., 2002; Morin, Pradat-Diehl, Mazevet, et went back to the early weeks following her stroke.
al., 2003; Morin, Taillefer, et al., 2001; Morin,
Thibierge, & Perrigot, 2001). In these cases, various
reflections of the object appeared in the discourse or Case 1: DT
behavior of patients with left hemineglect,
anosognosia, or asomatognosia. A patient (PR) with a DT, aged 67, widowed, five daughters and two sons,
very incapacitating left hemineglect (Morin, Taillefer, was seen 1 month after a massive left hemiplegia due
et al., 2001) thus described his problems in terms not of to a cardiac embolism in the right sylvian artery (Fig-
space-processing disorders, but of a variety of disor- ure 1a). She had not only hemineglect and
ders that he mentioned in one and the same series: on anosognosia, but also asomatognosia: when shown her
the one hand, body image disorders (having holes in own hand, she said she saw a bandage (which she
his body), and on the other, problems with the oral actually wore), or the nails that her daughter has just
objectnot only precipitation in eating and speaking, varnished. When her left hand was placed in her right
but also unceasing and urgent demands to his wife hand, and she was asked what it was, she answered I
(Morin, Taillefer, et al., 2001). Two patients with left do not see what you mean.
asomatognosia and hemineglect attributed oral charac- When telling the story of her life, she insisted on her
teristics to their left hand (Morin, Durand, et al., 2002): being abandoned by her two parents, having had to
patient QR (who also said that his hand, like himself, fight to bear her fathers name, and having married a
had worked too much) explained that he had seen a man who turned out to be unable to support their
left arm passing and that he had felt like biting this numerous children. If she had to do it over, she said,
arm. Patient DN, a right-handed man, kissed his inter- she would manage so that she, and not her husband,
locutors hand because I cant shake hands and put was in command.
forward the same reason for having drawn enormous In the very first days of her illness, DT said to her
lips besides his self-portrait. We present here three children that her paralyzed hand was the hand of a man
case observations (see Morin, Pradat-Diehl, Mazevet, and that her hand did not like her any more. In the
et al., 2003) in which another avatar of the object, a rehabilitation department, she did not speak of her
specifically feminine one (Freud, 1933; Lacan, 1974), hand, she spoke to it. She called her a bad girl, ca-
appears under the guise of a daughter-arm. ressed, beat, or kissed it (because I like it anyway),
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 53

talked about the love of a mother for the children she


feeds. She also wrote on the left part of the sheet: Our
children are the most beautiful thing in the world.
This self-portrait gives the impression of an endless
motherly reduplication: not only does DTs body con-
tain that of her daughter, but the circles she drew
around the bodywhich, she explained, were meant to
represent her coatcall to mind a uterine wrapping.
DTs state did not improve; she never recovered
either the ability to walk or her sense of balance, and
her discharge to a nursing home was prepared. She did
not complain about her hemiplegia: according to DT,
her grandson had helped her to make a few steps in the
hospitals park and the nurses were responsible for her
not walking because they used to tie her to her arm-
chair. She complained, rather, of the pains her left
hand felt: it cannot bear the chrome of the armchair, it
produces static electricity, when I touch it after touch-
ing the wheelchair, it transfers synthetic [sic] electric-
ity to it, she said while caressing her hand. When
asked what her pains felt like she answered it burns, it
Figure 1. Case 1, DT: (a) Schematic representation of brain lesion from is like when you suffocate, when you swallow and the
CT scan; (b) self-portrait (original size 21 29.7 cm).
food goes down the wrong pipe. This strange re-
hid it under her shawl, so it would not be too cold, for sponse seems to establish equivalence between the
it to be able to recover. At night my hand starts mouth and the hand. Indeed, other oral-sphere disor-
moving because it is quite warm under the blankets; ders were observed. DT changed her eating habits:
during the day, it does not want to move, it behaves now, she said, she mixed salty and sweet food, to the
oppositely to us, it plays during night and rests in the dismay of her children. They were also surprised that
daytime, it is a lazybones. I dont touch it because it is their mother, who never went out without her dentures,
painful. It probably feels abandoned. I tell it I wish it now did not care about her dental prosthesis being
would come back. . . . In the night, it scratches me, it maladjusted to her jaw. On the contrary, she claimed
comes stealing under me and scratches me. . . . It is that this prosthesis had never fitted properly, though
probably angry with me for not taking care of it . this did not prevent her from eating steak.
When asked whom this hand might remind her of, DT became depressed, did not recognize her doctors
she answered she did not want to pronounce the name any more, and began to speak with negative formulae
of that man. She soon admitted that it was her hus- in a melancholic tone: Everything is empty inside, it
bands name. This husband had more bad than good is if there were only bones, only a carcass. She viewed
aspects. her therapists as persecutors, claiming that one of the
When asked to draw herself (Figure 1b), she added nurses hated her and kept on repeating her name all day
two little legs to the hand located to the left of the sheet long.
and seemed puzzled thereafter: Why have I made All DTs discursive symptoms may be understood
double legs? Once the drawing was finished and she as expressing a specular-image lesion. The specular
was asked to indicate the right and left hand, she image is broken and interferes with a foreign image:
declared: as for the left hand I dont see it, it is dead. DTs hand is not hers, it is dead; it is also something
As she had not drawn her mouth, C.M. suggested that ever-living, a child who was never separated from his
DT examine whether anything was lacking. DT then mothers body. This discourse gives the impression of
added eye make-up, although she herself never used uncanniness in the sense of the term proposed by
make-up: it was her daughter who used make-up. Freud (1919). As stated above, this may attest to the
When asked to draw the mouth, she eventually drew it appearance of the object in the patients psychic real-
and declared: The mouth is useful for speaking and ity. A pathological presence of the oral object may also
singing, I dont miss that. be suspected from DTs attitude toward her dental
When commenting on her drawing, DT mentioned problems and her comments about not drawing her
the breasts that are not useful any more and then mouth.
54 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

Case 2: MN
MN was a Greek painter who, at the age of 69, suffered
a left hemiplegia due to an intracerebral hematoma
(Figure 2a). The story she told of her childhood had a
very classical oedipal tone: although her father was
bitterly disappointed that his first child was a girl, he
very much appreciated her intelligence, had serious
discussions with her alone, and encouraged her to
attend a painting class. She had poor relations with the
brother who was born two years after her, but she was
very much interested in the next child, a girl 5 years
younger than herself, whom she unceasingly looked
after. Figure 2. Case 2, MN: (a) Schematic representation of brain lesion
She did not have any children, since, she said, she from CT scan; (b) self-portrait (original size 21 29.7 cm); (c)
had preferred to devote her life to artistic creation. She drawing of the left paralyzed arm in its armrest (original size 21 29.7
cm).
also mentioned that her husband, a rigid mathemati-
cian, strongly encouraged her in this artistic exclusiv- armrest designed for her paralyzed arm). Like her
ity; according to her family doctor, MN used to young sister, who, when a teenager, borrowed MNs
complain a lot about her husband not understanding clothes, the leaf-daughter was appearance-conscious
her. Her figurative paintings were devoted either to the and tyrannical. She asked for arm bracelets and gowns
Parisian landscapes she saw in her walks or to nostal- and used to demand that people phone to see how she
gic representations of Greece or of her mother and was doing. She spoke with the sisters voice. That
other feminine figures. voice had no hallucinatory characteristics: it produced
Due to left hemineglect, MN ignored people stand- routine comments about the hospital or family life.
ing on her left, and she often presented with the left These comments did not interfere with the patients
arm of her spectacles over but not fitted behind the ear. thoughts; they were neither insulting nor mysterious.
Her gaze was permanently directed rightwards and Indeed, MN clearly stated that she had no hallucina-
downwards, and she could not keep her gaze focused tions: she did not really believe, she said, that her hand
on the midline when asked. Her constant knowing and was a true child. It was rather like a film scenario
gaze? blissful little smile and her oblique regard made the addressed to her husband, and it consoled her. How-
staff uneasy. During the first interview (12 weeks after ever, MN took the logical inferences of this scenario
stroke), she said she had not noticed her hemiplegia very far: she explained that she wondered whether her
until her husband told her she was paralyzed (not a daughter would not have psychological problems due
very nice word); she herself thought she had broken to her nonnatural birth and her very special relation-
her leg or her arm. She hoped that that would be ship to her mother, or that this girl would never leave
temporary, adding that she had always had problems MN since she would be buried with her. MN was asked
with her left side (left pleurisy, left-heel trauma, left to draw a self-portrait. Her drawing (Figure 2b) bore
otitis.). She now felt that her left arm was lifeless signs of left hemineglect: the left half of the body was
and sometimes she felt like speaking to it: we have sketchy, the left half of the wheelchair was lacking,
done such a lot of things together, how can you leave and both legs were situated in the right part of the
me alone? It is, she said, like a child. Throughout drawing. When CM suggested that she draw her leaf-
the following weeks, she began to speak with her daughter-arm, MN made a separate realistic represen-
therapists and her doctor of having given a name to her tation of her left arm lying on the armrest (Figure 2c).
hemiplegic hand, this name meaning leaf , in her However, perhaps due to the relative emptiness of the
mother language. When she was seen again 20 weeks left part of the drawing, her self-portrait might seem to
after stroke, she explained to C.M. that this is because contain a kind of small double inside the right part.
leaves grow green each spring again. She also called After 30 weeks, MN had not recovered the ability to
her hand her leaf-daughter and said she treated it as walk. She then bitterly reproached her physiotherapist
she treated the little sister she had so much looked after and her doctor (C.M.) for telling her that she probably
when they were children: this sister was her living would not be able to walk more than a few steps (you
doll. The leaf-daughter had a symbolic birth date should not speak like that to an ill person). She said
(the day the stroke occurred) and a cradle (the that she would not commit suicide, for religious rea-
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 55

sons. However, she did not exhibit any signs of depres-


sion; she continued to initiate unrealistic projects (to
go with a friend to the restaurant, provided he is not
ashamed to be seen with a woman using a cane), fell
because of trying to get up without help, and said that
she had got up to follow a doctor into his office.
This case clearly suggests that there is a common
phenomenon in somatoparaphrenia and anosognosia:
calling her paralyzed hand Leaf may be considered a
manifestation of anosognosia for paralysis, insofar as
the patient explained that leaves continuously turn
green again; but it also consists of qualifying a human
limb as a vegetable that is either dead or alive accord-
ing to the season. Such a qualification removes this
limb from the body image. Like the expression living
doll, this refers to animate/inanimate things. As in
DTs case, it may indicate the appearance of the object.
This appearance of the object in the left paralyzed hand
could be considered one of those psychosexual as-
pects of the unawareness of left hemiplegia hypoth-
esized by Schilder (1999). Figure 3. Case 3, DS: (a) Schematic representation of brain lesion from was RMN: ok
magnetic resonance imaging; (b) self-portrait and written account of as changed?
The fact that MN was consoled by the presence of DSs early illusions (original size 21 x 29.7 cm). Translation: My left
her daughter-hand, instead of mourning her lost abili- arm was naked and tucked [in the fetal position]. It had white traces,
ties, is not without a connection to the appearance of like paste marks left by bandages. Indeed, it was my daughters arm.
the object in the psychic reality of the patient. Accord- She had wanted a cuddle with me and we had remained joined together;
the staff refused to believe me and insisted that it was my arm. I alone
ing to Abraham and Toroks formula (1972), MNs knew the truth, which, however, seemed obvious.
mourning is of the incorporation form: the object (the
little sister) is incorporated, and the daughter-hand is sometimes told her therapists that she would be able to
positively present in the patients body. That gives walk alone. The interview was proposed by DSs
evidence of the nonsymbolization of the loss, of the speech therapist, who had been puzzled by a strange referral?
absence of true mourning. incident: while they were speaking of MNs daughters,
MN had suddenly stopped and said she would intro-
duce one of her daughters to her speech therapist. She
had then caught her left arm, caressed it, and begun to
Case 3: DS
speak to it: Mimi, say good morning, are you here!
DS was a brilliant bank manager, divorced, who lived Faced with her therapists surprise, she had eventually
with her three daughters. At age 40, she sustained an recognized she was wrong but did not seem embar-
extended right hemispheric ischemia due to the rupture rassed by her strange behavior.
of a sylvian aneurysm (Figure 3a). This caused a During her two interviews with C.M., DS never
complete sensorimotor hemiplegia with hemineglect spoke of the incident with the speech therapist. When
and anosognosia. She was seen several months after recounting her first memories of the poststroke period,
her stroke, during the difficult rehabilitation process she emphasized that she was not able to find a time
that she attended for over two years as an outpatient. when she would have noticed her paralysis. In the
DS was very determined in her rehabilitation and kept intensive care unit, the nurses would not allow her to
addressing a variety of demands to her therapists and get up and stand alone in the room, because they were
fellow patients. She did everything to charm her thera- afraid that she might fall. DS disagreed with them
pists, and her attitude aroused identifications and mili- because I said that it was not true and that I was
tant sympathy. She eventually insisted and succeeded perfectly able to stand up. When asked about possibly
in resuming a job, despite pessimistic medical and having had strange impressions, she mentioned having
psychological prognoses. C.M. saw her 8 months after experienced illusions at that time. These illusions
the stroke, when she was still using a wheelchair. consisted of thinking that her arm was not her arm but
Although DS clearly explained to C.M. that she was her daughters arm, but DS did not consider these
hemiplegic, she presented mild anosognosia: she thoughts as strange:
56 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

DS: It was not strange for me because I said the daughter. I said That is not my daughter . . .
truth, but all the doctors told me it was not true. Always the same, by the way. Youd think the
Therefore, I deduce that it was perhaps me who other ones impress me less.9 And I said That is
did not have my head on straight. not my daughter.
C.M.: Yes, what did you think of it, then? After that interview, C.M. asked DS whether she
DS: I said that it was not my arm. I said that I was would like to write anything about these strange illu-
convinced that I fell asleep with my daughter in sions. She then dedicated her remembrances from
my arms. Thus, my daughter had put her arm elsewhere to C.M. and accompanied them with a
around me. Thus it was her arm. Yes and I told drawing (Figure 3b). In her text, DS more or less
everybody that it was not my arm since it was maintains that her illusions were true: The staff
hers. And all doctors said: no, D, indeed, it is refused to believe it and maintained that it was my arm.
your arm! I alone knew the truth, which, however, seemed obvi-
C.M.: . . . for you, it was her arm? Was it an idea of ous. The drawing is a self-portraitwith a mirror
yours or whenever you looked at it . . . you had lateralizationintended to allow C.M. to understand
the impression that it was Mimis arm? what DSs early illusions looked like. Due to left
DS: No, it was in my head. It was an idea I had. hemineglect, the left leg is missing, while the left arm
C.M.: When you looked at your arm, what did you is called Mimis arm and shows traces of paste left
see? by bandages.
DS later accepted psychotherapeutic help. This help
DS: Well, I saw an arm. But between hers and mine
was proposed because she encountered difficulties in
...
coping with the family problems caused by her illness.
C.M.: An arm is an arm in some ways . . . but, for The interviews mostly concerned her neurotic relation-
example, the clothes . . . Had you noticed that it ships with her daughters, sisters, and mother. It was
was the same sweater or anything like that? only when questioned about her bodily feelings that
DS: No, because I think it was naked. DS spoke to her therapist of a left side you have not
C.M.: It was naked? any awareness of, the feeling that the left side does not
DS: Yes exist, perhaps because you loose the sensitivity, which
C.M.: And thus, it would have been wrapped around is much more important than not being able to move,
you? and a side you cannot mourn because it does not exist.
She also once mentioned that she sometimes had the
DS: (putting her left hand on her right shoulder)
feeling she had lost her arm or forgotten her left
Yes . . .
arm at home. When asked about the anxiety possibly
C.M.: Do you know when this disappeared? aroused by this impression, she answered that she was
DS: The feelings regarding the fact that I thought it not anxious but annoyed and, laughing, added that her
was my daughter? I think it came about because physiotherapist would be displeased at her. DS did not
they told me that it was not true. seem to think of any possible link between her daugh-
C.M.: Since otherwise, you had the feeling it was ter-arm illusions and her problems with the feminine
true? members of her family.
DS: Ah yes!! DSs case is very illustrative. It shows that, despite
C.M.: And when, for example, you saw your daugh- the apparent remission of a spectacular symptom
ter. That arm of hers. (somatoparaphrenia), a body image pathology may
persist: the arm remains outside the body image, left
DS: Ah! I never told her, I didnt.
at home. Above all, one could not find a better
C.M.: Well, I understand, but in your idea, if it were illustration of the fact that identifying the object and
there around you, would it mean she had another recognizing reality are incompatible: the daughter is
one, or how did that work? not recognized in the reality area insofar as she is
DS: No, it meant that she had held me so strongly in <please clarify>
identified as a too-present object (incorporated child,
her arms that it had left an imprint . . . to be more unlost part of the patients body).
precise.
C.M.: That is to say, she had kept her arm and you
kept the imprint . . .
DS: And my older daughter told me that during the 9
DS later told her therapist that Mimi, her youngest daughter, was her
baby love, whereas she called the older girls my dear.
first week after my coma I did not recognize my
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 57

Discussion Morin, Taillefer, et al., 2001), the body schema disor-


der is associated with an alteration of specular image,
A Lacanian perspective on daughter- insofar as that object that should, due to repression, be
somatoparaphrenia lacking and give rise to desire intrudes on the psychic
reality of the patient. It is necessary to underline that
One may find in the literature, from the early 1920s up demand and not desire objectsas is the case in psy-
to today, several observations of male or female pa- chosisthus appear in the fore. This is obvious in the
tients who personified their left hemiplegic arm as, or case of the oral object, and also in daughter-
considered it as belonging to, a child. Except for the somatoparaphrenia, if one recalls the Freudian as-
three female cases reported by Lan, Delahousse, similation of the child to a gift the girl demands to
Christiaens, Fontant (1969; see, above, the psychoana- compensate for her penis privation (Freud, 1933).
lytic comments on this case by Delahousse, 1972), by Such an undue psychic presence of the object might
Bisiach, Rusconi, and Vallar (1991), and by Rama- also account for anosognosia, insofar as it would,
chandran (1994), these observations do not provide a together with the alteration of body image, undermine
detailed account of the patients words. Moss and the very structure that organizes normal knowledge.
Turnbull (1996) have described a case of somatopara- This interpretation avoids the contradictions that, in
phrenia and anosognosia in a boy who hated his our opinion, weaken the interpretation of anosognosia
paralyzed arm, protested against physiotherapy ses- put forward by Kaplan-Solms and Solms (2002).
sions, and asked that his mother give him her arm. This These authors consider that anosognosia is due to
case, like our observations, shows that the unconscious repressionthat is, in our eyes, takes place among the
sharing of body parts in the motherchild relationship normal neurotic psychic processes. On the other hand,
may openly appear in RHS. they insist that right-brain damage specifically under-
The observations presented here may shed some mines the whole-object cathexis, the means bysource?
(KS&S?)
light on the characteristics of the strange children-arms which we normally transform infantile narcissistic li-page numbers?
that patients speak of in somatoparaphrenia. On the bido into mature and realistic object love; right-brain
one hand, they belong to women whose body image is damage thus results in a failure of normal mourning.
not unified any more; on the other, they have traits that Indeed, as stated above, it is questionable whether
call to mind an animate/inanimate object (leaf-child, patients with a failure of normal mourning may be
living doll). They share these traits with the other considered as being in the field of repression. It is
avatars of the left arm in RHS, insofar as this arm may therefore difficult to understand how a neurotic pro-
be described as both independent from the body (a cess might be particularly efficient in a pathology that
passing arm, an arm attached by glue) and endowed attacks precisely the neurotic structure of the patient.
with its own personal life (it worked too much, it is However, in our opinion, the fact that psychoanalysts
angry at not being taken care of). All these figures, as may propose interpretations that are not perfectly co-
the children incorporated in the mothers body, are herent (see also discussion above of the repression/
among those that, for Freud (1919), give rise to the unawareness problem in the field of anosognosia) is a
feeling of uncaniness and which, according to Lacan sign of the contradictions and paradoxes that arise
(1962), are evidence of the appearance of the normally when attempting to characterize the psychic structure
repressed object in the foreground. The fact that of RHS patients. On the one hand, a few of Kaplan-
daughter-arms specifically appear in female cases Solms and Solms cases exhibit paranoiac or melan-
strongly supports this hypothesis: the object status of cholic manifestations, and we ourselves observed a
children in women is well-known (Freud, 1933; Lacan, quasi-melancholic evolution in one case (DT).10 On the
1974). Indeed, we have published the observation of a other hand, the discourse (see above) of MN and DS
male patient (Morin, Thibierge, & Perrigot, 2001) who and their behavior obviously attest to their neurotic
addressed his hand as he would have spoken to a child psychic structure. On the one hand, MN and DS use
(go and have a walk with your sister!). However, this words that make the separate components of neurotic
man explained that when commanding his hand, he structure directly audible, which is not the case ordi-
intended to behave like his physiotherapist behaved narily. But on the other, their words seem to escape
with him. It is noticeable that such an authoritarian, from an isolated psychic territory. This is clearly
masculine style implies a separation rather than an illustrated in the case of DS: this patient never sponta-
incorporation of the childish hand.
It may be said that, in the cases presented here and
10
DTs expression I do not miss that may even remind us of the
formula proposed by Czermak (1986) for characterizing psychotic syn-
in our previous studies (Morin, Durand, et al., 2002; dromes: the lack itself is lacking.
58 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

neously spoke of her asomatognosia to her therapist. recognition to mental state understanding. Indeed, the
She was, however, able to speak of her arm, even to relationships between individual brains and familial or
say that it did not exist or it had stayed home. Indeed, social symbols and images raise fascinating problems.
one may wonder whether this arm left at home This is particularly true if one considers the relation-
might not be understood as expressing precisely the ships between identification and neural development:
belongs both ambiguous status of the object, which both belongs to for example, only a specific brain developmental
to, or add is:
and is his lost the subject and his lost maternal Heim, the Other. One phase, during which the infants brain is not com-
? could thus propose that these patients, regardless of pletely mature, seems available for the identification
their structure, openly express a truth that is normally process. The concerns of neurodevelopmental research
repressed (in the present case, that children may never and psychoanalysis have, however, little in common.
cease to be a part of the mothers body in the mothers Moreover, there is currently no scientific knowledge
unconscious). It is doubtful whether the therapist that allows us to assume that the laws that govern the
would be justified in putting this truth in a harsh light: efficiency of social and familial symbols and images
DSs unconcerned attitude when speaking to her thera- either are identical to or directly parallel those of
pist of her forgetting her left arm at home clearly neural functioning. Such a parallelism is, however,
indicates that this does not pose a problem for herin implicitly assumed by Decety (2002): Mental pro-
other words, that it is not a personal truth. This is the cesses, and particularly thosenumerous in Homo
reason why we ordinarily take the position neither to specieswhich are universal, must be described in
interpret this type of discourse in psychotherapeutic terms that make evident that they are achievable by one
dialogue nor to correct its aberrations in medical inter- brain. We believe that a crucial problem is hidden in
views. the expression achievable by one brain, which im-
plies that the mind only lives in individual brains.11

Is there consistency between psychical


and neuronal location? The implications of the Lacanian perspective
in RHS for enlightening the mindbrain problem
The fact that patients may confuse their arm with the
arm of someone else could be interpreted quite differ- Our psychoanalytic reading of pathological observa-
ently if put in relation with the notion that the self and tions makes clear that, although individual brain le-
other body representations in the brain are partially sions may have very destructive psychic effects, this
overlapping (Decety & Sommerville, 2003). Accord- does not automatically imply that only one brain is
ing to this line of thinking, our observations might be sufficient to create a normal subject. These pathologi-
interpreted in terms of erroneous body representations cal observations indirectly show that our normal certi-
arising due to poststroke neuronal reorganization. tude of having an autonomous self implies both our
However, this would not explain why, in the RHS, the separation from and our dependence on the Other.
paralyzed arm may be assimilated not only to existing They offer the opportunity for a true dialog between
children but also to fictitious children or to an inani- psychoanalysis and neurology. On the one hand, psy-
mate object. Therefore, we insist that the logic of the choanalysts may learn something from these neuro-
RHS patients discourse may be found in the symbolic logical observations. Even if our separation/
and imaginary coordinates of their self-representation dependence relationship to the Other relies on a sym-
and not only in the involvement of given brain struc- bolic operation, this operation cannot but result in
tures. This argument does not remove the problem of neuronal inscriptions, a fact that psychoanalysts ordi-
the consistency between psychic symptoms being asso- narily do not need to take into account. When neuro-
ciated and their appearing when brain areas near to one logical lesions occur to alter these inscriptions, our
another are either activated or lesioned. Decety and dependence on the Other and its representatives
Sommerville (2003) claimed that this consistency ex- (nurturer, child), which ordinarily implies a psycho-
ists: Consistent with research and theoretical claims analytic construction, appears in full light. On the
from developmental and social psychology, represen- other hand, our observations are instructive for
tations of aspects of the self both overlap with repre- neuroscientists: they show that our mental life does not
sentations of other and are distinct from such only depend on the structure of our neuronal nets, but
representations. Common and distinct cognitive repre-
sentations of self and other extend along many dimen- 11
We take the liberty of translating un cerveau as one brain, since
sions of self and other processing from action Decety did not write le cerveau but un cerveau.
Daughter-Somatoparaphrenia in Women with Right-Hemisphere Syndrome 59

also on the words and images that make us dependent Buxbaum, L. J., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Specialised structural
on the Otherthat is, that take their source outside our descriptions for human body parts: evidence from
individual brains. This makes the wish to unify neuro- autotopoagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18: 298306.
science and psychoanalysis (see, for example, Kandel, Clment, J. P., Marchan, F., Boyon, D., Monti, P., Lger, J. M.,
1999) questionable, insofar as these two scientific & Desrouesn, C. (1996). Utilization of the Draw a Person
Test in the elderly. International Psychogeriatrics, 8: 349
fields necessarily differ in their points of view. 364.
Kaplan-Solms and Solms work (2002) clearly illus- Coslett, H. B. (1998). Evidence for a disturbance of the body
trates this necessary heterogeneity of points of view: in schema in neglect. Brain and Cognition, 37: 527544.
Kaplan-Solmss psychoanalytic interviews with RHS Critchley, M. (1953). Disorders of body image. In: The Parietal
patients, she considers the patients words in terms of Lobes. London: Arnold, pp. 225255.
hate and love toward the object, and notalthough Critchley, M. (1962). Clinical investigation of disease of the
aware of the lesional origin of the disorderin terms parietal lobes of the brain. Medical Clinics of North
of neuronal dysfunction. Conversely, when Marshall, America, 46: 837857.
,names added: Halligan, Fink, Wade and Fracowiak (1997) study the Czermak, M. (1986). Signification psychanalytique du syn-
ok? drome de Cotard. In: Passions de lobjet, ed. J. Clims. Paris:
functional anatomy of a hysterical paralysis in a female
patient who had sustained a psychic trauma, they rea- Editions de lAssociation Freudienne, pp. 205236.
Decety, J. (2002). Naturaliser lempathie. Lencphale, 28: 9
son in terms of brain circuitry but do not address either 20.
the nature of the trauma or the mechanism through Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. (2003). Shared representations
which their patient went from a psychic trauma to a between self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience
pathological activation of orbito-frontal and cingulate view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7: 527533.
cortex whenever she tried to move her leg. The psy- Delahousse J. (1972). Considrations sur lattitude anosognos-
choanalytic analysis of the patients words and the ique de lhmiplgique gauche. In: Comptes rendus du 70e
neuropsychological or the neuroanatomical study of Congrs de Psychiatrie et de Neurologie de Langue Fran-
the brain thus imply two different points of views.12 aise, ed. P. Warot. Paris: Masson, pp. 10821088.
These points of view, although not contradictory, can- Feinberg, T. E. (1997). Some interesting perturbations of the
not be held simultaneously. self in neurology. Seminars in Neurology, 178: 129135.
Feinberg, T. E. (2001). Altered Egos: How the Brain Creates
the Self. New-York: Oxford University Press.
Freud, S. (1905). Three Essays on the Theory of Sexuality.
REFERENCES Standard Edition, 7: 125243.
Freud, S. (1914). On narcissism: An introduction. Standard
Abraham, K. (1949). A short study of the development of the Edition, 14: 67102.
libido, viewed in light of mental disorders. In: Selected Freud, S. (1919). The uncanny. Standard Edition, 17: 219
Papers on Psycho-Analysis, ed. E. Jones. London: Hogarth 256.
Press, pp. 418501. Freud, S. (1923). The Ego and the Id. Standard Edition, 19: 3
Abraham, N., & Torok, M. (1972). Introjecterincorporer: deuil 47.
ou mlancolie. Nouvelle Revue de Psychanalyse, 6: 111 Freud, S. (1933). New Introductory Lectures on Psychoanaly-
122. sis. Standard Edition, 22: 81111.
Babinski, J. (1914). Contribution ltude des troubles men- Gainotti, G. (1972). Emotional behaviour and hemispheric side
taux dans lhmiplgie organique crbrale (anosognosie). of the lesion. Cortex, 8: 4155.
Revue Neurologique (Paris), 1: 845848. Gallagher, S. (1986). Body image and body schema: A concep-
Benton, A., & Sivan, A. B. (1993). Disturbance of body tual clarification. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 7: 550
schema. In: Clinical Neuropsychology, ed. K. M. Heilman & 554.
E. Valenstein. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 123 Gallagher, S., & Voever, M. (in press). Disorders of embodi- published?
140. ment. In: The Oxford Reader in Philosophy and Psychiatry,
Bisiach, E., Rusconi, M. L., & Vallar, G. (1991). Remission of ed. J. Radden. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Gertsmann, J. (1942). Problems in imperception of disease and
Neuropsychologia, 29: 10291031. of impaired body with organic lesions. Archives of Neurol-
ogy and Psychiatry, 48: 890913.
Guillerault, G. (2003). Le miroir et la psych. Dolto, Lacan et le
stade du miroir. Paris: Gallimard.
12
Admitting that neuroscientists and psychoanalysts are each assigned Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C, & Wade, D. T. (1995). Unilat-
to a specific point of view that limits their understanding of the patients eral somatoparaphrenia after right hemispheric stroke: A
mind should not leave us unsatisfied. That no therapeutic or scientific
discipline has a holistic understanding or mastery of human subjectivity
case description. Cortex, 31: 173182.
contributes to ensuring that the minds of patients and citizens cannot be Hcaen, H., & de Ajurriaguerra, J. (1952). Mconnaissances et
manipulated too efficiently. hallucinations corporelles. Intgration et dsintgration de
60 Catherine Morin, Stphane Thibierge, Pascale Bruguire, Pascale Pradat-Diehl, Dominique Mazevet

la somatognosie. Paris: Masson. Morin, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., Robain, G., Bensalah, Y., &
Kandel, E. R. (1999). Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: Perrigot, M. (2003). Stroke hemiplegia and specular image:
A new intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. Lessons from self-portraits. International Journal of Aging
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156: 505524. and Human Development, 56: 141.
Kaplan-Solms, K., & Solms, M. (2002). Clinical Studies in Morin, C., & Salazar-Orvig, A. (1996). Paroles de patients
Neuro-Psychoanalysis. Introduction to a Depth Neuropsy- hmiplgiques: discours et position subjective. Sciences
chology. London: Karnac. Sociales et Sant, 14: 4778.
Keppel, C., & Crowe, S. F. (2000). Changes to body image and Morin, C., Taillefer, C., Vallat, C., Helsly, N., Thibierge, S., &
self-esteem following stroke in young adults. Neuropsycho- Pradat-Diehl, P. (2001). Quest-ce quun gauche? Annales
logical Rehabilitation, 10: 1531. de Radaptation et de Mdecine Physique, 44: 192204.
Klein, M. (1940) Mourning and its relation to manic-depressive Morin, C., Thibierge, S., & Perrigot, M. (2001). Brain, body
states. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 21: 125 image and language: A psychoanalytic perspective. Journal
153. of Mind and Behaviour, 22: 6989.
Lacan, J. (1962). Langoisse. Unpublished seminar, Paris. Moss, A. D., & Turnbull, O. (1996). Hatred of the hemiparetic
Lacan, J. (1966a). Propos sur la causalit psychique. In: Ecrits, limbs (misoplegia) in a 10 year old child. Journal of Neurol-
ed. J. Lacan. Paris: Seuil, pp. 151193 ogy Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 61: 210211.
Lacan, J. (1966b). Remarques sur le rapport de Daniel Lagache. Paillard, J. (1999). Body schema and body image: A double
In: Ecrits, ed. J. Lacan. Paris: Seuil, pp. 647684. dissociation in deafferented patients. In: Motor Control
is this Lacan, J. (1974). RSI. Unpublished seminar, Paris. Today and Tomorrow, ed. G. N. Gantchev, S. Mori, & J.
actual title?
Lacan, J. (1977a). The mirror stage as formative of the function Massion. Sofia: Academic Publishing House, pp. 197214.
of the I as revealed in psychoanalytic experience. In: Prigatano, G. P., & Weinstein, E. A. (1996). Edwin A.
Jacques Lacan. Ecrits: A Selection, ed. A. Sheridan. Lon- Weinsteins contribution to neuropsychological rehabilita-
don: Tavistock/Routledge, pp. 17. tion. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 6: 305326.
Lacan, J. (1977b). The signification of the phallus. In: Jacques Ramachandran, V. S. (1994). Phantom limbs, neglect syn-
Lacan. Ecrits. A Selection, ed. A. Sheridan. London: dromes, repressed memories, and Freudian psychology. In-
Tavistock/Routledge, pp. 281291. ternational Review of Neurobiology, 37: 291333.
Lacan, J. (1977c). The subversion of the subject and the dialec- Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W.(1998). The perception of
tic of desire in the Freudian unconscious. In: Jacques Lacan. phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. Brain, 121: 1603
Ecrits. A Selection, ed. A. Sheridan. London: Tavistock/ 1630.
Routledge, pp. 292325. Schilder, P. (1999). The Image and Appearance of the Human
Initial? Lan, E., Delahousse, J., Christiaens, ?., & Fontant, M. (1969). Body. Studies in the Constructive Energies of the Psyche.
De lanosognosie la fabulation concernant lhmicorps New-York & London: Routledge.
paralys chez lhmiplgique gauche. Annales Mdico-Psy- Sheridan, A. (1977). Translators note. In: Jaques Lacan.
chologiques, 127: 672679 Ecrits: A Selection. London: Tavistock/Routledge, pp. vii
Laplane, D. (1998). Ltrange en neurologie. Etudes Psycho- xii.
thrapiques, 17: 2332. Stein, D., Orbach, I., Shani-Sela, M., Har-Even, D., Yarulasky,
Marcel, A. J., Tegnr, R., & Nimmo-Smith I. (2004). Anoso- A., Roth, D., Meged, S., & Apter, A. (2003). Suicidal
gnosia for plegia: Specificity, extension, partiality and disu- tendencies and body image and experience in anorexia ner-
nity of bodily unawareness. Cortex, 40: 1940. vosa and suicidal female adolescent inpatients. Psycho-
Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Wade, D. T., & therapy and Psychosomatics, 72: 1625.
Fracowiak, R. S. J. (1997). The functional anatomy of Thibierge, S. (1999a). Pathologie de limage du corps. Etude
hysterical paralysis. Cognition, 64: 18. des troubles de la reconnaissance et de la nomination en
Morin, C., & Bensalah, Y. (1998). Self-portrait in adulthood psychopathologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
and aging. International Journal of Aging and Human De- Thibierge, S. (1999b). Limage et le double. La fonction
velopment, 46: 4570. spculaire en pathologie. Paris: Ers.
Morin, C., Durand, E., Marchal, F., Timsit, S., Manai, R., Wallon, H. (1931). Comment se dveloppe chez lenfant la
Pradat-Diehl, P., & Rancurel, G. (2002). Asomatognosie et notion du corps propre. Journal de Psychologie, 28: 705
troubles de loralit. Une lecture psychanalytique. Annales 748.
de Radaptation et de Mdecine Physique, 46: 1223. Winnicott, D. W. (1982). Mirror role of mother and family in
Morin, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., Mazevet, D., Gautherin, D., Weill- child development. In: Playing and Reality, ed., D. W.
Choulamountry, A., Dubail, M., & Robain, G. (2003). Un Winnicott & C. Winnicott. London: Routledge, pp. 111
enfant dans le bras. Un trouble fminin de limage du corps 118.
en neurologie? Bulletin de l Association Lacanienne Inter-
nationale, 101: 1322 .

You might also like