You are on page 1of 9

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/7027443

Shame, blame, and contamination: A review of


the impact of mental illness stigma on family
members

Article in Journal of Family Psychology July 2006


DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.239 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

119 1,763

3 authors, including:

Patrick Corrigan Amy Watson


Illinois Institute of Technology University of Illinois at Chicago
452 PUBLICATIONS 19,727 CITATIONS 85 PUBLICATIONS 3,894 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Patrick Corrigan on 28 December 2013.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 20, No. 2, 239 246 0893-3200/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.239

Blame, Shame, and Contamination: The Impact of Mental Illness and


Drug Dependence Stigma on Family Members
Patrick W. Corrigan Frederick E. Miller
Illinois Institute of Technology Evanston Northwestern Healthcare and Northwestern
Amy C. Watson University
University of Illinois, Chicago

Family members of relatives with mental illness or drug dependence or both report that they
are frequently harmed by public stigma. No population-based survey, however, has assessed
how members of the general public actually view family members. Hence, the authors
examined ways that family role and psychiatric disorder influence family stigma. A national
sample (N 968) was recruited for this study. A vignette design describing a person with a
health condition and a family member was used. Family stigma related to mental illnesses,
such as schizophrenia, is not highly endorsed. Family stigma related to drug dependence,
however, is worse than for other health conditions, with family members being blamed for
both the onset and offset of a relatives disorder and likely to be socially shunned.

Keywords: stigma, family, mental illness, drug dependence, other health conditions

The New Freedom Commission (Hogan, 2003) high- Then we examine how family members in various roles
lighted stigma as a major barrier to the mental health goals parents, children, spouse, and siblingsinteract with
of Americans and recommended concerted efforts to change family stigma.
public opinion and to diminish prejudice. Although the New
Freedom Commission report was not explicit about family
stigma, it clearly recognized that barriers to family partici- Public Stigma Applied to People With Mental
pation significantly impede mental health care. This paper Illness and Drug Dependence Disorders
addresses the complexity of what is called family stigma.
Stigma not only harms many people with mental illness or Common stereotypes about people with mental illness
drug abuse or both but also injures family members who are seem to parallel those with drug dependence and include
associated with these individuals. Goffman (1963) called dangerousness and blame (Angermeyer, Matschinger, &
this courtesy stigma, the prejudice and discrimination that is Corrigan, 2004; Link, Phelan, Bresnahan, Stueve, & Pesco-
extended to people not because of some mark (e.g., mental solido, 1999). Generally, research shows that psychiatric
illness, disorder) that they manifest but rather because they disorders are viewed as more blameworthy than physical
are somehow linked to a person with the stigmatized mark. health conditions such as cancer and heart disease (Corrigan
Surveys have shown that family members with relatives et al., 1999; Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson, 1988). Research
who have mental illness or drug dependence disorders re- has focused on stereotypes related to attributions about
port significant experience with family stigma. However, to personal responsibility and blame (Corrigan, 2000). Inves-
our knowledge, no survey based on a national sample has tigators have found that research participants who blame
been conducted to determine whether the public, in fact, relatives for the onset of the relatives mental illness or drug
endorse stigmas about family members. The goal of this dependence are more likely to react angrily to those rela-
study is to examine family stigma in a sample drawn from tives, to withhold help, to avoid them socially, and to
the general adult public. We first review the stigma expe- support coercive mental health services (Corrigan et al.,
rienced by families: blame, shame, and contamination. 1999, 2000; Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan, & Ku-
biak, 2003; Corrigan, & Miller, 2004). Research that com-
pares the public stigma of mental illness to drug dependence
consistently shows that persons with drug dependence are
Patrick W. Corrigan, Institute of Psychology, Illinois Institute of judged to be more responsible for their disorder (Corrigan et
Technology; Amy C. Watson, School of Social Work, University al., 1999; Link et al., 1999; Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson,
of Illinois, Chicago; Frederick E. Miller, Department of Psychiatry
and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern Healthcare and Northwest-
1988). The difference between perceptions of drug depen-
ern University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
1
Patrick W. Corrigan, Institute of Psychology, Illinois Institute of In this article, we distinguish the stigma experienced by people
Technology, 3424 South Slate Street, Chicago, IL 60616. E-mail: with psychiatric disorders from family stigma by labeling the
corrigan@iit former primary stigma.

239
240 CORRIGAN, WATSON, AND MILLER

dence and mental illness is expected to emerge when family different samples. Almost half of one sample (N 281),
stigma is the dependent measure. comprised mostly of mothers, reported some concern about
being blamed for their childrens mental illness. Typically,
Stigma and Family Members blame is attributed to bad parenting skills; for example, the
mothers incompetence led to the child developing a mental
The themes of blame and shame are seen in surveys of illness. Results from a second sample (N 180) reported by
families of individuals with psychiatric disorders in which Struening et al. (2001) reported the same concerns though at
family members discuss their experience with family a lower rate: about 10% of mothers experienced being
stigma. Large-scale studies have shown that between a blamed. Siblings and spouses are often blamed for family
quarter and a half of family members believe that their members who mismanage their illness. In describing causal
relationship with a person with mental illness should be kept attributions about human behavior, Weiner (1995) distin-
hidden or is otherwise a source of shame to the family guished between onset and offset attributions. As applied to
(Angermeyer, Schulze, & Dietrich, 2003; Phelan, Bromet, health conditions, onset attributions answer questions re-
& Link, 1998; Ohaeri & Fido, 2001; Phillips, Pearson, Li, garding how a set of symptoms started. Offset attributions
Xu, & Yang, 2002; Thompson & Doll, 1982; Shibre et al., reflect the regular recurrence of symptoms (e.g., the treat-
2001; Wahl & Harman, 1989). Shame seemed to be linked ments a person must participate in to experience a cure).
to blaming the family for the members psychiatric disorder. Siblings and spouses are often blamed for a relatives dis-
Findings from a group of 178 family members showed that ease offset; namely, they fail to help the person with mental
about 25% worried that other people might blame them for illness adhere to treatment such that the person unnecessar-
their relatives mental illness (Shibre et al., 2001). ily relapses. A study of 164 siblings hinted at this stigma;
Blame and shame seem to lead to discrimination in the survey participants were concerned about relatives with
form of social avoidance. Three large studies reported that mental illness remaining adherent to treatment regimens and
about a fifth to a third of family members reported strained perceptions that relapse was somehow the participants fault
and distant relationships with extended family or friends or (Greenberg, Kim, & Greenley, 1997). Unlike the kind of
both because of a relative with mental illness (Oestman & responsibility experienced by parents, sibling blame seems
Kjellin, 2002; Shibre et al., 2001; Struening et al., 2001; to mirror public expectations that family members who are
Wahl & Harman, 1989). However, another study found a somehow currently associated with adult children with men-
much smaller rate, with only 10% of a sample reporting tal illness (e.g., siblings) or who have opted to live with the
occasional avoidance by a few people (Phelan et al., 1998). adult (e.g., spouses) have greater responsibility for current
Note that all the studies we reviewed examined family status. This is evident in the reduced shame experienced by
stigma from the perspective of the family, that is, whether family members who do not live with the relative with
family members perceive members of the general public mental illness as compared to those who do (Phelan et al.,
stigmatizing them because of their relatives with mental 1998).
illness. Hence, the first goal of this study was to conduct a The child of a person with mental illness is often viewed
survey using a national sample to determine how a subset of as contaminated by the parents mental illness. One inves-
the American public actually views family stigma. In addi- tigation attempted to test this finding using a more carefully
tion to descriptive statistics representing the endorsement of controlled vignette experiment (Mehta & Farina, 1988).
family stigma, our survey also included a comparison be- Results showed that students portrayed in the vignettes as
tween the family stigma of mental illness and a physical having a father who is depressed, alcoholic, or an ex-convict
health condition for which patients are frequently blamed: were viewed as having more difficulty than the other
emphysema (Chapple, Ziebland, & McPherson, 2004). Con- groups. Another study illustrated the complexity of contam-
sistent with research on primary stigma (Weiner et al., ination on children, in this case, of parents with alcoholism
1988), we expected the family stigma of mental illness to be or mental illness (Burk & Sher, 1990). A sample of 570
more severe than that related to emphysema. Also note that adolescents was more likely to rate teenagers with stigma-
these previous studies limited their research to the family tized parents as more socially negative than teens with
stigma that stems from a family member with mental ill- parents who do not abuse alcohol and do not have a mental
ness; we were unable to find any studies examining how illness.
family members of people with drug dependence disorders
experience family stigma. An additional goal of this study A Comparison of Family Stigma and Primary
was to examine family stigma for drug dependence disor- Stigma
ders. Consistent with the research on primary stigma, family
stigma due to drug dependence is hypothesized to be worse Finally, the data in this paper provide an answer to a
than for mental illness. fundamental question about family stigma: how bad is it?
One way to address this question is by comparing the family
How Stigma Varies by Family Role stigma applied to mental illness and drug dependence versus
that experienced by emphysema. A second question,
Family stigma may vary by family role: parent, spouse, though, is how bad is family stigma for a specific health
sibling, or child (Corrigan & Miller, 2004). Struening et al. condition compared to corresponding primary stigma? We
(2001) examined this question in terms of parents in two answer this question by comparing responses made by the
FAMILY STIGMA 241

sample toward people with mental illness or drug depen- specific disorder rather than a generic category. We chose emphy-
dence disorder against the same research participants atti- sema on the possibility that its connection with smoking might
tudes about the family member. increase the level of blame associated with it (Chapple, Ziebland,
& McPherson, 2004).
Family roles were limited to four dominant ones found in
Methods previous research on family stigma (Corrigan & Miller, 2004):
parents, children, siblings, and spouses. Some evidence suggests
The data for this study come from the Family Stigma Survey that family stigma may vary by the gender of the family role; for
collected by Time-Experiments for the Social Sciences (TESS; example, mothers may be stigmatized more harshly than fathers
NSF Grant 0094964, Diana Mutz and Arthur Lupia, Investigators). (Corrigan & Miller, 2004; Lefley, 1992). Hence, vignettes ran-
TESS uses a national online research panel recruited by Knowl- domly varied the gender of the family member. In like manner,
edge Networks (KN). KN recruits for its sample via list-assisted gender of the person with the health disorder was also randomly
random digit-dialing techniques on a sample frame consisting of varied by vignette. We decided not to vary other sociodemograph-
the entire United States telephone population. Recruits are pro- ics of vignette participants because earlier research, for the most
vided free WEB-TV access in return for completing surveys that part, failed to show them to be relevant in stigmatizing people with
are sent to them via e-mail weekly. psychiatric disorders (Pescosolido et al., 1999).
For this study, KN randomly identified and solicited 1,307 To capture the effects of these many variations on several
individuals from its overall panel for the Family Stigma Survey outcomes by means of direct questioning would be cumbersome
from March 26, 2004 to April 8, 2004; 74% completed the survey and time-consuming. This dilemma can easily be managed, how-
(N 968). The sample was 51.9% female, with a mean age of 47.0 ever, by using a factorial survey design (Rossi & Nock, 1982)
years (SD 16.5, range 18 95). The sample was 72.5% White, similar to those employed by previous national probability surveys
11.7% Black, 11.0% Mexican Americans, and 4.8% other. Of the of attitudes toward persons belonging to stigmatized groups (Link
sample, 15.8% had less than a high school education, 32.1% were et al., 1999; Phelan et al., 2000). In vignette experiments that
high school graduates, 27.8% had completed some college, and incorporate factorial survey designs, respondents are presented
24.4% had a bachelors degree or higher. Postsurvey stratification with descriptions of a fictional person who varies across theoret-
weights were used to adjust sample demographics to values con- ically relevant dimensions (i.e., type of health condition, hire
sistent with the 2000 U.S. Census. Variables used to determine versus promotion decision, gender, age, and ethnicity). The differ-
stratification weights include gender, age, race/ethnicity, geo- ent versions of the vignette that are created in this way are
graphic region in the United States, and level of education. Data randomly assigned to respondents. Respondents then answer ques-
reported in this paper represent weight-corrected cases. Despite tions about the described person in terms of relevant outcome
efforts to attain a true probability sample, there are limits to the KN variables. In addition to manipulating and then measuring the
approach. Most prominent of these are limiting the sample to effects of several independent variables at one time, this factorial
phone-bearing households that do not have the Internet but wish to survey design also allowed the strengths of the experimental
do so via WEB-TV. method to be brought to bear in our study. By experimentally
manipulating one of the individuals characteristics (e.g., whether
Vignette Conditions NAMEs health condition is psychotic disorder, drug dependence,
or emphysema) while holding all others constant, we were able to
Each respondent was randomly assigned to read a vignette that attribute any differences in attitudes and behavioral intentions
varied across four conditions: disease of the person with the specifically to variation in that characteristic. Because respondents
disorder, role of the corresponding family member, gender of the were assigned randomly to vignettes, differences in responses
person with the disorder, and gender of the family member. One could be attributed to variations in the stimulus rather than to
such vignette follows: variations in respondents characteristics. The profiles required to
[John Smith/Joan Smith] is the [father/mother/son/daughter/ accomplish a factorial survey design can be produced using
brother/sister/husband/wife] of [Frank/Fran] Smith, a 30-year-old distribution-generating algorithms available in standard statistical
[man/woman] with [schizophrenia/drug dependence/emphysema]. packages.
[Frank/Fran] lives with [his or her] family and works as a clerk at
a nearby store. [Frank/Fran] has been hospitalized several times
because of [his or her] illness. The illness has disrupted [his or her] Dependent Measures
life significantly.
The quality of specific terms used to describe health conditions After reading the vignette, respondents were instructed to re-
can influence the reaction of respondents. For example, problems spond to 14 items using seven-point Likert scales (e.g., 7
related to psychiatric disorder are broader than the idea of strongly agree). Seven of the items were about the person with the
mental illness alone and include areas such as drug dependence health disorder, while seven were about the family member. The
(Martin, Pescosolido, & Tuch, 2000). We addressed this problem first seven items were from the short form of the Attribution
by providing respondents with types of mental health problems as Questionnaire, which has been shown to be a reliable and valid
listed in the DSM. Moreover, we adopted labels from the measure of primary stigma (Corrigan et al., in press; Corrigan et
MacArthur Mental Health Module of the 1996 General Social al., 2003, 2002). For example, it is [Franks/Frans] own fault that
Survey (GSS) for the two psychiatric conditions in order to facil- [he or she] is in the present condition. These studies include
itate comparison with previous research (Pescosolido, Monahan, confirmatory factor analyses which support the reliability and
Link, Stueve, & Kikuzawa, 1999). Mental illness was schizophre- content validity of measures. The items selected from the short
nia and drug dependence was drug dependency. Based on form of the Attribution Questionnaire represented the single item
earlier research by Weiner et al. (1988) on attributions across that loads most into the seven factor solution of the confirmatory
health conditions, we decided on emphysema as the comparison factor analysis.
physical health disorder. Consistent with the labels of the GSS The selection of seven items reflecting family stigma was based
MacArthur Module, we decided on a label that represented a on our review of relevant content areas from three sources. First,
242 CORRIGAN, WATSON, AND MILLER

we reviewed the common themes that describe the primary stigma emphysema, p .001) and offset of the disorder (drug
of mental illness and drug dependence used in prior research dependence-schizophrenia, p .0001; drug dependence-
(Corrigan, 2005). Although this information largely influenced the emphysema, p .001), more likely to be contaminated (drug
first seven items relevant to how the public views the person with dependence-schizophrenia, p .0001; drug dependence-
the health disorder, we also considered it in developing items
reflecting family stigma. Second, we reviewed the common themes
emphysema, p .001), more ashamed of afflicted family
that family members have used to describe their experience with member (drug dependence-schizophrenia, p .0001; drug
family stigma (Corrigan & Miller, 2004). Third, we conducted a dependence-emphysema, p .001), and less competent in
focus group of family members to augment our list of items their family role (drug dependence-schizophrenia, p
reflecting family stigma. During a 60-minute session, seven mem- .0001; (drug dependence-emphysema, p .001). Fami-
bers of families with a person with psychiatric disorder (57.1% lies of people with drug dependence and schizophrenia
female, including and across all four family roles) answered ques- were viewed as more pitiable (p .05) than those with
tions about their general understanding of stigma and prejudice. emphysema.
They also responded to examples of stigma applied to their family Additional analyses examined whether survey partici-
member with psychiatric disorder and to examples of stigma pants endorsed any specific family attitudes higher than
applied to them as family members. Analyses of the responses of
focus group participants endorsed the themes of blame, shame, and
others. Results of a within-Group ANOVA for the subgroup
contamination found in our literature review. In addition, a content of survey participants randomized to the schizophrenia vi-
analysis of transcripts of the focus group yielded additional family gnettes, with the seven family stigma items as dependent
stigma items, such as onset responsibility (family member to variables, was significant, F(6, 1842) 108.19, p .001.
blame for person getting disorder), offset responsibility (family Subsequent contrasts showed the group most agreed with
member to blame for person relapsing), pity, contamination (ill- withholding pity (p .05), viewing the person as incom-
ness could rub off), shame, incompetence (the family member was petent in his or her family role (p .05) and as socially
not very good as a parent, sibling, spouse, or child), and avoidance avoiding the family member (p .05) compared to the
(the respondent would not want to socialize with the family mem- remaining four items. The within-Group ANOVA for the
ber). For example, [John/Joan] bears some responsibility for [his group assigned to drug dependence was also significant,
or her] [insert relationship] originally getting ill.
Items assessing primary stigma were always presented to re-
F(6, 1950) 52.42, p .001. Subsequent contrasts showed
search participants before family stigma items to prime stereotypes withholding pity (p .05) and contamination (p .05) as
related to the health condition. Items within each domain (i.e., the two items most highly endorsed by survey participants.
primary stigma and family stigma) were presented in random
order. How Does Family Stigma Vary With Family Role?

Results Table 1 also includes the mean and standard deviations of


participant responses to family stigma items organized by
The research questions guiding this paper suggest a three- family role and health condition. Results of a 4 3 (family
step approach to analysis. First, we conducted a series of role by health condition) MANOVA with the seven family
descriptive analyses to examine how the public endorses stigma items as dependent variables yielded significant
family stigmata for families that have members with one of main effects for family role, F(21, 2805) 10.18, p .001,
these three health conditions. This included an inferential as well as a significant interaction, F(42, 5628) 2.53, p
analysis to examine how family stigma varied by disorder. .001. Subsequent 4 3 ANOVAs were then conducted for
Second, we examined how stigma varied by family role. the seven family stigma items individually. Significant main
Third, we determined how family stigma compared to pri- effects for family role were found for onset blame (F
mary stigma by comparing the mean score on selected 2.45, p .05), contamination (F 8.09, p .001), offset
primary stigma items to the scores on family stigma items. blame (F 3.95, p .05), and withholding pity (F 1.35,
ns). Subsequent post hoc Tukeys tests examined pairwise
How Does Family Stigma Vary by Health differences. Results showed that parents and spouses are
Condition? viewed to be more responsible for the onset (p .05) of the
persons schizophrenia, drug dependence, and emphysema
Mean and standard deviations of responses to the seven than children and siblings. Schizophrenia, emphysema, and
overall family stigma survey items is summarized in the drug dependence were likely to contaminate children (p
Overall row of Table 1. Results of a oneway MANOVA .05) more than other family roles. Parents are viewed as
with the seven items as dependent variables were signifi- more responsible for the persons schizophrenia or drug
cant, F(14, 1886) 13.31, p .001. Subsequent oneway dependence relapse than children (p .05). Generally,
ANOVAs showed that all seven items differed significantly siblings were the least pitied of the four groups.
across the three health conditions (F ranges from 3.95, p
.05 to 54.18, p .0001). Post hoc Tukeys test examined How Does Family Stigma Compare to Primary
the differences between pairs of health conditions. Results Stigma?
suggest that families of people who are drug dependent are
viewed in the most stigmatizing manner, that is, these The final question examined in this paper was how the
families are viewed as more responsible for onset (drug public endorses family stigma compared to primary stigma.
dependence-schizophrenia, p .0001; drug dependence- Means and standard deviations of participant responses to
FAMILY STIGMA 243

Table 1
Differences in Courtesy Stigma Rated by Health Conditions
Health condition
Schizophrenia Drug dependence Emphysema
Survey items M SD M SD M SD
1. Family member bears some responsibility for person originally getting ill.
Overall 1.98 1.29 2.67 1.44 2.22 1.38
Parent 2.35 1.47 2.99 1.44 2.46 1.45
Child 1.82 1.13 2.03 1.26 1.87 1.24
Sibling 1.90 1.19 2.47 1.33 2.15 1.33
Spouse 1.79 1.26 3.11 1.46 2.31 1.39
2. Persons illness could rub off on family member.

Overall 2.12 1.41 3.03 1.72 1.89 1.25


Parent 2.00 1.40 2.14 1.46 1.78 1.26
Child 2.65 1.44 4.17 1.73 2.20 1.38
Sibling 1.99 1.41 2.49 1.32 1.83 1.15
Spouse 1.77 1.24 3.40 1.61 1.80 1.18
3. When person relapses, it may be family members fault.

Overall 2.22 1.25 2.40 1.33 2.12 1.25


Parent 2.39 1.29 2.55 1.37 2.07 1.14
Child 2.00 1.20 1.95 1.16 2.10 1.30
Sibling 2.28 1.28 2.38 1.26 2.30 1.32
Spouse 2.22 1.22 2.69 1.41 2.05 1.26
4. Family member should feel ashamed about persons illness.

Overall 1.87 1.22 2.72 1.44 1.77 1.16


Parent 1.86 1.21 2.64 1.34 1.74 1.06
Child 1.93 1.17 2.88 1.60 1.65 0.96
Sibling 1.94 1.37 2.56 1.37 1.75 1.10
Spouse 1.74 1.14 2.81 1.46 1.91 1.47
5. was not a very good family member to person with illness.

Overall 2.48 1.38 2.72 1.38 2.47 1.43


Parent 2.32 1.42 2.85 1.44 2.32 1.34
Child 2.67 1.36 2.45 1.34 2.35 1.48
Sibling 2.63 1.32 2.68 1.28 2.72 1.45
Spouse 2.33 1.36 2.87 1.40 2.54 1.47
6. I would not want to socialize with family member.

Overall 2.32 1.24 2.69 1.34 2.16 1.29


Parent 2.38 1.32 2.67 1.34 2.17 1.25
Child 2.28 1.14 2.56 1.28 2.23 1.34
Sibling 2.39 1.35 2.58 1.48 2.31 1.34
Spouse 2.25 1.18 2.93 1.26 1.95 1.24
I would NOT be likely to pity family member.a

Overall 3.91 1.63 3.62 1.51 4.26 1.64


Parent 3.40 1.46 3.66 1.52 4.10 1.64
Child 3.82 1.58 3.26 1.50 4.00 1.70
Sibling 4.60 1.59 4.10 1.46 4.77 1.51
Spouse 3.99 1.73 3.49 1.45 4.21 1.61
Note. Findings are listed as a whole (overall) and subdivided by family role: parents, children,
siblings, and spouse.
a
This item is reverse scored from the original survey item. The original stated: I would be likely
to pity family member.

the seven primary stigma items are summarized in Table 2 Tukeys test showed that primary stigma for drug depen-
by health condition. Results of a oneway MANOVA were dence was the worst. The sample rated the person with drug
significant, F(14, 1890) 99.56, p .001. Subsequent dependence as more dangerous (p .05), fearful (p .05),
ANOVAs for each of the seven items were all significant; F blameworthy (p .05), anger arousing (p .05), and likely
ranged from 7.85, p .05 to 319.02, p .001. Post hoc to be avoided (p .05). They were also rated as less pitiable
244 CORRIGAN, WATSON, AND MILLER

Table 2
Differences in Primary Stigma Rated by Health Conditions
Health condition
Drug
Schizophrenia dependence Emphysema
Survey items M SD M SD M SD
1. I feel NO pity for the person with
mental illness.a 3.36 1.45 3.77 1.63 3.35 1.52
2. The person with mental illness is
likely to be dangerous. 3.85 1.30 4.29 1.24 1.91 1.24
3. I feel scared of the person with
mental illness. 3.37 1.40 3.54 1.48 2.33 1.60
4. It is the persons own fault that
he/she is in the present condition. 1.90 1.17 4.71 1.47 3.05 1.61
5. I feel angry toward the person
with mental illness. 2.06 1.18 3.60 1.48 1.96 1.27
6. If you knew him/her, how likely
is it that you would NOT help the
person with mental illness.a 3.02 1.19 3.24 1.35 2.68 1.18
7. If you know him/her, how likely
is it that you would stay away
from the person with mental
illness? 3.37 1.32 4.26 1.62 2.17 1.31
a
These items are reverse scored from the actual survey item. The original stated: I feel pity for
the person with mental illness. and If you knew him/her, how likely is it that you would help the
person with mental illness?

(p .05) and worthy of help (p .05). The person with ence significant family stigma. They report being blamed
schizophrenia was rated as more dangerous (p .05), for the onset of their relatives disorder, held responsible for
fearful (p .05), and likely to be avoided (p .05) than the relapse, and being viewed as an incompetent family member
person with emphysema. Results showed that the person (Corrigan & Miller, 2004). This has led to feelings of shame
with emphysema was viewed as more responsible (p .05) and contamination. Our study was guided by the following
for his or her disorder than the person with schizophrenia. question: How does a sample of the American public actu-
Table 3 represents the mean and standard deviation of the ally view family members of people with mental illness or
total scores for primary stigma and for family stigma. Re- drug dependence? Several interesting trends emerged. First,
sults of a 3 x 2 ANOVA suggested that primary stigma was the public does not seem to highly endorse family stigma of
endorsed more highly than family stigma, F(1, 939) 409, mental illness or drug dependence. A second way to deter-
p .001. Post hoc Tukeys tests examined the differences mine the depth of family stigma is to compare it to primary
between primary and family stigma across the health con-
stigma using the same vignette. Results suggest the sample
ditions. In all cases where paired tests yielded significant
in this study was less likely to endorse stigmatizing attitudes
results, survey participants endorsed primary stigma more
than family stigma. In terms of schizophrenia, participants about family members compared to people who directly
rated primary stigma greater. Total score was also ranked experience the health conditions. Survey participants, on
more highly for drug dependence. average, produced higher overall stigma scores for people
with drug dependence disorders and with schizophrenia
Discussion than other family members. Survey participants were also
more likely to avoid people with schizophrenia and with
Surveys of family members of people with mental illness drug dependence disorders compared to their family.
or drug dependence conclude that family members experi- A second question asked the following: Despite the low
level of family stigma, does the public discriminate among
Table 3 health conditions? Results suggest that families with a rel-
Selected Within-Group Differences in Courtesy and ative with drug dependence disorder are viewed most
Primary Stigma Across Health Conditions harshly. Compared to the vignettes of people with schizo-
phrenia and emphysema, results showed that family mem-
Health condition
bers with relatives who were drug dependent were blamed
Drug more for the onset of their relatives conditions and for
Schizophrenia dependence Emphysema
relapses, although this latter difference was not significant
Survey items M SD M SD M SD between the vignettes on drug dependence and those on
Primary stigma 2.99 0.68 3.91 0.78 2.49 0.80 schizophrenia. Family members in the vignette on drug
Courtesy stigma 2.41 0.81 2.84 0.84 2.40 0.81 dependence were viewed as more likely to be contaminated
FAMILY STIGMA 245

by the disorder, more shameful, and more likely to be A third goal of this study was to determine whether
avoided socially. family stigma varies by role. Does the public view parents,
Family stigma of people with mental illnesses like siblings, children, and spouses differently? Results suggest
schizophrenia was less deleterious. We chose emphysema a difference in perception, but the specific difference de-
as a physical health condition because of the perception that pends on the type of stigma. Results showed that adults in
people suffering with this disorder are more blameworthy a family with an immediate relationship with the person
because of a past smoking history (Chapple et al., 2004). with a health conditionparents and spousesare more
And, in fact, the primary stigma related to blame was more likely to be viewed as responsible for the health condition.
highly endorsed for emphysema than schizophrenia. How- This effect was found for all three conditions, although
ever, no difference was found in blaming family members parent and spouse blame was significantly worse for the
for emphysema compared to schizophrenia. In fact, the only relative with drug dependence. Parents were also viewed as
family stigma item that differed significantly across these significantly more responsible for the relapse for children in
two groups was for pity, with the schizophrenia family the two psychiatric vignettes on psychiatric conditions.
members viewed as more pitiable than the emphysema Children were more likely to be viewed as contaminated by
group. all three disorders than the other groups, with drug depen-
Feeling shame because of mental illness or public stigma dence once again showing statistically more contamination
might have significant impact on illness career (Pescosolido than schizophrenia or emphysema. In all, these findings
& Boyer, 1999). Research has shown that people with suggest that the public distinguishes between the role of
greater stigma are later to admit their illness, less likely to parents and children in terms of stigma and judges family
begin treatment, and more likely to drop out of treatment role most harshly in families that have a person with drug
prematurely (Corrigan, 2000). Shame and self-blame are dependence.
frequently the stigma that lead to diminished illness career. Despite these positive findings, this study is limited. First,
Clinicians may opt to adjust treatments so that stigma does the technology used to recruit and test research participants
not become a barrier to participation. For example, clini- resulted in some bias in the sample. Research participants
cians could avoid words that are likely to elicit stigma. were required to be phone users with sufficient interest in
These include references to onset and offset responsibility web-mail to be willing to sign up for KN efforts. This group
and to feelings of shame. was obviously not an unbiased subset of the population.
Combined with our earlier findings, these results suggest Second, the measures used in the study were mostly single
that families with a relative who is dependent on drugs are items with no clear information about their psychometrics.
viewed in a stigmatizing manner by the public while those This problem could be partially overcome when future
with a relative with mental illness are not. What might studies are able to replicate these findings using the same
account for the difference between this public survey and measures.
the perceptions of families with mental illness? The differ- Advocacy groups have sought to erase family stigma and
ence may represent a history effect, that is, family stigma associated discrimination, that is, they are to blame for their
has diminished over the five plus years since these family loved ones illness and should be kept away from the person
surveys were completed (Phelan et al., 1998; Wahl & Har- with mental illness, especially during treatment. Efforts by
man, 1989). This seems unlikely given other recent data that parental advocacy groups like the National Alliance for the
suggest that primary stigma has actually worsened over the Mentally Ill (NAMI), the largest group of this kind in the
past four decades (Phelan, Link, Stueve, & Pescosolido, United States, have diminished the stigma and promoted
2000). Alternatively, the disparity between family percep- appropriate affirmative action: increase parental involve-
tions and public report may represent the effects of social ment in treatment. Antistigma efforts have included protest,
desirability. Members of the general public are unwilling to asking participants to suppress their negative attitudes about
endorse the family stigma that, in fact, exists and to risk a group; education, contrasting the myths of mental illness
social disapproval. with the facts; and contact, decreasing stigma by fostering
Social scientists have tested a variety of implicit mea- interactions between a person with mental illness and a
sures that measure stigma without the influence of the group where such stereotypes might exist. Most of the
desirability effect (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, research on stigma change has focused on decreasing pri-
1995; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Future research should mary stigma. However, primary stigma seem like adequate
include implicit measures to determine if the low rate of candidates for also diminishing family stigma.
family stigma in terms of mental illness is still evident. The
difference between public perceptions and family reports
may represent self-stigma. Namely, families with a relative References
with mental illness may internalize prejudice (Corrigan &
Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Corrigan, P. W. (2004).
Miller, 2004; Corrigan & Watson, 2002) which, in turn, Familiarity with mental illness and social distance from people
diminishes the self-esteem and self-efficacy of family mem- with schizophrenia and major depression: Testing a model using
bers. If self-stigma accounts for family reports of family data from a representative population survey. Schizophrenia Re-
stigma, then future research should find a significant asso- search, 69, 175182.
ciation between internalized stigma and a family members Angermeyer, M. C., Schutze, B., & Dietrich, S. (2003). Courtesy
perception of how others stigmatize him or her. stigma: A focus group study of relatives of schizophrenia pa-
246 CORRIGAN, WATSON, AND MILLER

tients. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 38, 593 Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear
602. and loathing: The role of disturbing behavior, labels, and
Burk, J. P., & Sher, K. J. (1990). Labeling the child of an alco- causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with
holic: Negative stereotyping by mental health professionals and mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 208
peers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 156 163. 223.
Chapple, A., Ziebland, S., & McPherson, A. (2004). Stigma, Mehta, S. I., & Farina, A. (1988). Associative stigma: Perceptions
shame, and blame experienced by patients with lung cancer: of the difficulties of college-aged children of stigmatized fathers.
Qualitative study. British Medical Journal, 328, 1470 1473. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 192202.
Corrigan, P. W. (2000). Mental health stigma as social attribution: Oestman, M., & Kjellin, L. (2002). Stigma by association: Psy-
Implications for research methods and attitude change. Clinical chological factors in relatives of people with mental illness.
Psychology-Science and Practice, 7, 48 67. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 494 498.
Corrigan, P. W. (2005). On the stigma of mental illness: Implica- Ohaeri, J. U., & Fido, A. A. (2001). The opinion of caregivers on
tions for research and social change. Washington, DC: Ameri- aspects of schizophrenia and major affective disorders in a ni-
can Psychological Association. gerian setting. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
Corrigan, P. W., Lurie, B., Goldman, H., Slopen, N., Medasani, 36, 493 499.
K. M., & Phelan S. (in press). How adolescents perceive the Pescosolido, B. A., & Boyer, C. A. (1999). How do people come
stigma of mental illness. Psychiatric Services. to use mental health services? Current knowledge and changing
Corrigan, P. W., Markowitz, F. E., Watson, A. C., Rowan, D., & perspectives. In A. V. Horwitz & T. L. Scheid (Eds.), The
Kubiak, M. A. (2003). An attribution model of public discrimi- sociology of mental health and illness (pp. 392 411). New York:
nation towards persons with mental illness. Journal of Health Cambridge University Press.
and Social Behavior, 44, 162179. Pescosolido, B. A., Monahan, J., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., &
Corrigan, P. W., & Miller, F. E. (2004). Shame, blame, and Kikuzawa, S. (1999). The publics view of the competence,
contamination: A review of the impact of mental illness stigma dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with
on family members. Manuscript submitted for publication. mental health problems. American Journal of Public Health, 89,
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., Uphoff-Wasowski, 1339 1345.
K., Campion, J., Mathisen, J., et al. (1999). Predictors of partic- Phelan, J. C., Bromet, E. J., & Link, B. G. (1998). Psychiatric
ipation in campaigns against mental illness stigma. Journal of illness and family stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24, 115126.
Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 378 380. Phelan, J. C., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Pescosolido, B. A. (2000).
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., Uphoff-Wasowski, Public conceptions of mental illness in 1950 and 1996: What is
mental illness and is it to be feared? Journal of Health and Social
K., Campion, J., Mathisen, J., et al. (2000). Stigmatizing attri-
Behavior, 41, 188 207.
butions about mental illness. Journal of Community Psychology,
Phillips, M. R., Pearson, V., Li, F., Xu, M., & Yang, L. (2002).
28, 91102.
Stigma and expressed emotion: A study of people with schizo-
Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, L. P.,
phrenia and their family members in China. British Journal of
Uphoff-Wasowski, K., et al. (2002). Challenging two mental
Psychiatry, 181, 488 493.
illness stigmas: Personal responsibility and dangerousness.
Rossi, P. H., & Nock, S. L. (Eds.) (1982). Measuring social
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293310.
judgments: The factorial survey approach. Beverly Hills, CA:
Corrigan, P. W., & Watson, A. C. (2002). The paradox of self- Sage.
stigma and mental illness. Clinical Psychology-Science and Shibre, T., Negash, A., Kullgren, G., Kebede, D., Alem, A.,
Practice, 9, 3553. Fekadu, A., et al. (2001). Perception of stigma among family
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. members of individuals with schizophrenia and major affective
(1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobstrusive disorders in rural Ethiopia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 299 303.
Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027. Struening, E. L., Perlick, D. A., Link, B. G., Hellman, F., Herman,
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled D., & Sirey, J. A. (2001). Stigma as a barrier to recovery: The
identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. extent to which caregivers believe most people devalue consum-
Greenberg, J. S., Kim, H. W., & Greenley, J. R. (1997). Factors ers and their families. Psychiatric Services, 52, 16331638.
associated with subjective burden in siblings of adults with Thompson, E. H., & Doll, W. (1982). The burden of families
severe mental illness. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 67, coping with the mentally ill: An invisible crisis. Family Rela-
231241. tions, 31, 379 388.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cogni- Wahl, O. F., & Harman, C. R. (1989). Family views of stigma.
tion: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Re- Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 131139.
view, 102, 4 27. Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a
Hogan, M. F. (2003). New freedom commission report: The pres- theory of social conduct. New York: The Guilford Press.
idents new freedom commission: Recommendations to trans- Weiner, B., Perry, R. P., & Magnusson, J. (1988). An attributional
form mental health care in America. Psychiatric Services, 54, analysis of reactions to stigmas. Journal of Personality and
14671474. Social Psychology, 55, 738 748.
Link, B. G., Phelan, J. C., Bresnahan, M., Stueve, A., & Pescoso-
lido, B. A. (1999). Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, Received October 5, 2004
causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Revision received February 24, 2005
Public Health, 89, 1328 1333. Accepted March 29, 2005

You might also like