You are on page 1of 7

Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793

Tool-use and tool-making by captive, group-living orangutans


(Pongo pygmaeus abelii) at an artificial termite mound
Masayuki Nakamichi
Laboratory of Ethological Studies, Faculty of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Suita 565-0871, Japan

Accepted 16 July 2003

Abstract
The present study examined the use and making of tools to obtain foodstuffs in artificial-mound holes by five captive,
group-living Sumatran orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii). Three adult orangutans frequently stripped leaves and twigs from
a branch provided (tool-making), and then inserted the tool into a hole to obtain foodstuffs (tool-using). A 5-year-old female
juvenile usually used the tools that adult orangutans had previously used, but rarely made tools herself. A 2-year-old male infant
did not use any tools. The adult orangutans tend to leave one to several leaves at the top of the branch than to leave many leaves
on the branch or to strip all leaves. It seemed likely that tools with appropriate leaves are easier to insert into holes and obtain
more foodstuffs, compared with branches with many leaves or sticks without any leaves. When the orangutans were unable to
insert a tool into a hole, they usually modified the tool and/or changed their tool-using technique, such as changing how they
grasped the tool. These findings are discussed from the perspectives of the orangutans behavioral flexibility regarding tool-use
skills and hierarchical organization in food-processing techniques.
2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Pongo pygmaeus abelii; Orangutan; Tool use; Tool making; Tool modification; Tool learning

1. Introduction in captivity have been reported to intentionally throw


sticks into the foliage of trees, which the gorillas
Many observations have shown that wild chim- could not climb due to electric wire, to knock down
panzees (Pan troglodytes) are consistent and habitual leaves and seeds, and to pull tree branches toward
tool-users and tool-makers (see review by McGrew, themselves using sticks (Nakamichi, 1999). These
1992). On the other hand, there have been no pre- tool-using behaviors might reflect a hitherto unknown
vious reports on tool-using behavior in wild gorillas aspect of the cognitive ability of this species. Thus,
(Gorilla gorilla) or bonobos (Pan paniscus), despite observations of tool-using and tool-making among
long-term field studies (Fossey, 1983; Kano, 1992). great apes, both in the wild and in captivity, are in-
There have been many reports that captive gorillas in dispensable for understanding cognitive ability from
zoos use objects as tools, such as by throwing missiles, an evolutionary perspective.
or sponging and probing with sticks (Boysen et al., There have been only a few observations of tool
1999; Parker et al., 1999). Moreover, some gorillas use among wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) (Van
Schaik et al., 1996; Fox et al., 1999; Peter, 2002),
Tel.: +81-6-6879-8129; fax: +81-6-6879-8010. while free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans have been
E-mail address: naka@hus.osaka-u.ac.jp (M. Nakamichi). reported not only to use objects as tools in a variety

0376-6357/$ see front matter 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2003.07.002
88 M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793

of contexts, but also to perform human-like behav- (Indah, captive-born, 11-year-old), a juvenile female
iors with human artifacts frequently (Galdikas, 1982; (Karen, captive-born, 5-year-old), and one infant
Russon and Galdicas, 1995; Russon, 1999). Observa- male (Satu, captive-born, 2-year-old). The four older
tional and experimental studies on captive orangutans orangutans had lived together in a group for at least
have also demonstrated that their capacity for tool use three years before the start of the present study. The
is as high as that in chimpanzees (Lethmate, 1982; remaining orangutan (Satu) was born in the group.
Visalberghi et al., 1995). Compared with chimpanzees, The five orangutans usually came out in a grassy
however, there is little information available on the outdoor enclosure (approximately 200 m2 ) between
spontaneous tool-using and tool-making behaviors of 8:30 and 9:00 h, and stayed there until they returned
orangutans. In particular, few studies have dealt with to their indoor cages at around 17:00 h. There was the
how orangutans modify tools or change their behav- artificial mound (180 cm high and 500 cm round) in
ior after failing to use tools and little information is the center of the enclosure. The mound, constructed of
available about the development of tool-use skills in concrete, had five holes, each 38 mm in diameter and
immature orangutans. 5681 cm from the ground. These gave access to plas-
To enhance the psychological well-being of captive tic pipes of varying length from 43 to 76 cm. Some
chimpanzees, some zoological parks have provided of the holes were filled with foodstuffs such as mus-
artificial mounds, designed to simulate the earth tard, BBQ sauce, applesauce, peanut butter slurry etc.
mounds made by African termites for which wild up to 10 to 20 cm from the bottom, every morning be-
chimpanzees fish by inserting grass or twig tools into fore the orangutans came out. The mound was set up
the holes (Nash, 1982). Such a man-made mound in the enclosure at least three years before the start of
has been provided for the group-living Sumatran the present study.
orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) at the San Diego Many fresh branches (approximately 50100 cm
Zoo. Adult orangutans in this group stripped leaves long and 0.52 cm in diameter at the thick end) with
and small twigs from branches (i.e. tool-making) and small twigs and leaves were provided every morning.
inserted them into holes to obtain foodstuffs in the These branches consisted of figs of several species
mound holes (i.e. tool-using). These tool-using and (Ficus spp.), eugenia (Syzygium spp.), mulberry
tool-making behaviors by orangutans, which could (Morus alba), and hibiscus (Hibiscus rosa-sinensia).
provide important opportunities for understanding the The orangutans ate the leaves and seeds from these
cognitive abilities of this species, should be analyzed branches, and also used them as tools to obtain the
with precision. foodstuffs in the mound.
The purpose of the present study was to investi- Observations were usually conducted for one to
gate tool-using and tool-making behaviors to obtain three hours immediately after the orangutans came out
foodstuffs in artificial-mound holes by the orangutans in the enclosure, since they were more likely to show
ranging in age from 2 to 37 years at the San Diego tool-using and tool-making behaviors during this time
Zoo. I answered the following questions by ana- of the day. The orangutans were observed from the
lyzing video-recorded data: How and how often do public space for a total of 44.5 h (19 days of observa-
orangutans use and make tools? Do orangutans mod- tions).
ify tools or change their behavior after failing to use Tool-using and tool-making behaviors by orangutans
tools? Can immature orangutans make and use tools? were recorded with an 8-mm videotape recorder.
Based on this videotaped data, I recorded those
branches which they selected to obtain the food-
2. Materials and methods stuffs in the mound, i.e. new branches or sticks. New
branches had not previously been used as a tool or
Five Sumatran orangutans living at the San Diego eaten by orangutans, and thus still had at least several
Zoo were observed from late August to mid-December leaves and/or small twigs. Sticks had few or no leaves
1997: one full-adult male (Clyde, captive-born, 21- since orangutans had already used them to obtain
year-old), one full-adult female (Josephine, wild-born, foodstuffs or had eaten the leaves. I also recorded
37-year-old, estimated), one young-adult female whether or not orangutans stripped off leaves or twigs
M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793 89

when they used a new branch as a tool. When an one juvenile orangutans showed both tool-using and
orangutan was not able to insert the tool into the hole, tool-making behaviors to obtain foodstuffs in the
for example, because there were too many leaves on mound, whereas the remaining 2-year-old male infant
the branch, this was regarded as an unsuccessful act. did not, although he did insert his index finger into
On the other hand, when the tool was inserted into the hole, wiped the inside of the hole and then put it
the hole, this was regarded as a successful act. I also in his mouth to lick.
recorded the behaviors of tool-users after unsuccess- To obtain foodstuffs from within the mound, the
ful acts. 5-year-old juvenile female (Karen) was recorded to
pick up sticks 27 times (93% of the total acts of
picking up materials to obtain foodstuffs), while she
3. Results selected new branches only twice (P < 0.01 on a bi-
nomial test). Thus, she customarily used tools that had
3.1. General patterns of tool-using and tool-making been previously used by other orangutans, while she
behavior rarely selected new branches from which the leaves
and twigs needed to be stripped. The adult female
Table 1 summarizes tool-using and tool-making (Josephine) and young-adult female (Indah) picked
behaviors by the orangutans. The three adult and up new branches as often as sticks (Ps > 0.10 on

Table 1
Numbers (%) of various acts performed by orangutans at the San Diego Zoo, CA, USAa
Clyde (21, m)b Josephine (37, f) Indah (11, f) Karen (5, f)

Picking up materials to obtain foodstuffs


New branches 57 (64.0) 43 (41.0) 13 (43.3) 2 (6.9)
Sticks 32 (36.0) 62 (59.0) 17 (56.7) 27 (93.1)
Stripping leaves/twigs from new branches
Yes 39 (68.4) 37 (86.0) 11 (84.6) 2
No 18 (31.6) 6 (14.0) 2 (15.4) 0
Making the tool
Stripping all leaves/twigs 5 (12.8) 1 (2.7) 2 (18.2) 1
Leaving leaves at the top of the branch 15 (38.5) 31 (83.8) 7 (63.6) 0
Stripping leaves/twigs inappropriately 19 (48.7) 5 (13.5) 2 (18.2) 1
Which end of the tool was inserted first?
Distal (thin) end 658 (95.8) 583 (99.1) 85 (96.6) 238 (100)
Proximal (thick) end 29 (4.2) 5 (0.9) 3 (3.4) 0 (0)
Inserting the tool into a hole
Successful 563 (82.0) 553 (94.0) 86 (97.7) 221 (92.9)
Unsuccessful 124 (18.0) 35 (6.0) 2 (2.3) 17 (7.1)
Behavior after unsuccessful attempts
Modify tool/change technique for inserting the tool 103 (90.4) 29 (82.9) 2 14 (82.4)
Perform again in the same manner 7 (6.1) 3 (8.6) 0 2 (11.8)
Leave the mound 4 (3.5) 3 (8.6) 0 1 (5.9)
Details of modify tool/change technique for inserting the tool
Modify the tool 27 (26.2) 6 (20.7) 0 0 (0)
Change technique for inserting the tool into a hole 66 (64.1) 14 (48.3) 0 11 (78.6)
Change tool 7 (6.8) 7 (24.1) 2 1 (7.1)
Change hole 4 (3.9) 3 (10.3) 0 3 (21.4)
a The percentages in parenthesis are shown as the ratio of the number of acts in a sub-category to the number of total acts in the main

category. For example, Clyde picked up new braches at 64.0% of the total acts of picking up materials to obtain foodstuffs.
b Age in years, m: male, f: female.
90 M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793

binomial tests), while the adult male (Clyde) picked changing how they grasped the tool (i.e. grasping the
up new branches more often than sticks (P < 0.05 on more distal end of the branch), by changing the hand
a binomial test). in which the tool was grasped (i.e. right to left hand
When picking up new branches, all of the three or vice versa), or by changing one-hand to two-hand
adults stripped some leaves and twigs from the branch grasping (i.e. grasping the distal end of the tool in
at least two-thirds of the time before inserting them one hand and the proximal end in the other). Only
into the holes. When stripping leaves and twigs from Josephine pushed the leaves at the top of the branch
new branches, Josephine and, to a lesser degree, Indah into the hole with the extended index finger of her
were more likely to leave one to several leaves at the free hand: these leaves made it more difficult to insert
top of the branch (i.e. good tool) than to leave many the branch but also made the tool more effective for
leaves on the branch or to strip all leaves (bad tool). obtaining more foodstuffs from the hole. Clyde and
It seems likely that good tools are easier to insert into Josephine were also recorded to change branches and
holes and obtain more foodstuffs, compared with bad holes after unsuccessful acts. Karen was recorded to
tools. None of the orangutans were observed to chew modify the tool only once (7%) after unsuccessful acts,
or bite the leaves from the tip of the tool when they while she changed her technique, such as by changing
put it into their mouth to obtain foodstuffs. Josephine how she grasped the tool, 11 times (79%).
made good tools significantly more often than Clyde
(2 = 16.36, P < 0.01). 3.2. Josephines special tool
Clyde, Josephine and Indah striped leaves and twigs
from branches with their fingers or picked them off Josephine was once observed to make a special tool:
from the distal (thin) to proximal (thick) end of the she chewed a 120-cm long (estimated) stick for ap-
branch, while grabbing the distal end of the branch proximately 40 cm from the distal end for 30 s, and
in the fist, in 29 of 39 trials (74.4%), in 34 of 37 crumpled it, which effectively made a sponge at the
trails (91.9%) and in all 11 observed trials (100%), end of an 80 cm handle. She pushed the sponge into the
respectively. It seemed likely that leaves and twigs hole, by grasping the middle of the stick and pushing
were more easily detached from the stem when they it into the hole, then withdrew the sponge by extract-
were stripped from the distal to proximate end than in ing the stick, and kept it in her mouth for 1025 s to
the opposite direction. suck out the liquid. While she sucked the sponge the
All four orangutans inserted tools into holes with the fifth time, her 2-year-old son (Satu) pulled the stick.
distal end first in at least 95% of acts. They sometimes As a result, the stick detached from the sponge, which
tried to insert the stick with the distal end first, even still remained in Josephines mouth. She continued
though this end seemed to be too limp to be inserted sucking the sponge for another 18 s, and then pushed
compared to the opposite end. it from her mouth. Thereafter, she inserted the stick
The rate of successful acts of inserting the tool into into the same hole seven more times.
the hole for Clyde was significantly lower than those
for each of the three females (Table 1): Josephine
(2 = 42.48, P < 0.01), Indah (2 = 14.26, 4. Discussion
P < 0.01), Karen (2 = 16.28, P < 0.01). There
were no significant differences among the three fe- The results of the present study demonstrate that
males: Josephine and Indah (2 = 2.00, P > 0.10), adult orangutans habitually made and used tools to
Josephine and Karen (2 = 0.41, P > 0.10), Indah obtain foodstuffs, a 5-year-old juvenile female was
and Karen (2 = 2.78, P > 0.10). also a habitual tool-user but not a tool-maker, and a
After more than 80% of unsuccessful acts, they 2-year-old infant male did not use tools. These results
again tried to insert the tool into the hole after modi- are largely consistent with a finding in the wild. Ac-
fying the tool and/or changing their technique. Their cording to Fox et al. (1999), a wild 3- to 4-year-old
tools were modified by stripping obstructive leaves juvenile female orangutan frequently observed her
and twigs from the branch so that it could enter the mother break off a branch and strip it to fish for ants
hole smoothly. They also changed their technique by in tree holes, but did not do so herself. However, she
M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793 91

made her own tool and probed tree holes at the age through trial and error. To evaluate comprehension of
of 5 to 6 years. Although new branches as well as a causal relation, it can be useful to examine perfor-
used sticks were usually available near the mound mance in novel tasks in which the potential tools can
in the morning, the 5-year-old female in the present be used to perform different acts (Visalberghi et al.,
study preferred using tools that had been previously 1995; Vizalberghi and Tomasello, 1998). In the present
used by adults rather than making tools from new study, adult orangutans who had been presented with
branches, which were less elaborate than those made the artificial mound for at least three years were ob-
by adult orangutans. These findings indicate that the served to use and modify probing tools, but no in-
use of tools that have been previously used by adults formation was available on the early stages of when
may be an important step for juvenile orangutans to they acquired this tool-using and tool-making behav-
acquire appropriate tool-using and tool-making skills. ior. Moreover, although it was difficult to insert new
The special tool (i.e. stick with a sponge) made by branches into the holes, in 31, 14 and 15% of the time
the adult female in this study (Josephine) consisted of that the adult male, full-adult female and young-adult
two parts and each had its own function; i.e. the stick female in this study picked up a new branch, respec-
was used to push the sponge into the hole and to ex- tively, they inserted the new branch into a hole with-
tract it again, while the sponge, which could not be out stripping any leaves or twigs (see Table 1). Based
used without the stick, was used to soak up more food. on these facts, the idea that the orangutans learned an
Moreover, the adult orangutans including Josephine association between the use of a tool with leaves and
stripped leaves from new branches, but tended to leave its outcome may be more parsimonious than the idea
one to several leaves at the top of the branch to ob- that they understand the functional value of the leaves
tain more foodstuffs. Such a tool with leaves can also for obtaining more foodstuffs.
be considered a single tool consisting of two parts, Russon (1998) observed food-processing tech-
each with its own function. Therefore, we may say niques of ex-captive juvenile orangutans that had been
that Josephines special tool and the tools with ap- reintroduced to the wild in Kalimantan, Indonesia,
propriate leaves at the top resemble man-made tools, and concluded that the orangutans showed hierarchi-
such as spoon and fork, which have functionally dis- cal organization when obtaining difficult foods. She
tinct parts. Wild chimpanzees habitually use tools for suggested that hierarchization generates complex,
ant- and termite-fishing (McGrew, 1992). Each tool for higher level cognition by re-using existing cogni-
fishing seems to consist of only one part, and thus has tive units to build new cognition structures (Russon,
only one function. Since it is well known that chim- 1998). Byrne and Byrne (1991) also pointed out dex-
panzees use a variety of objects as tools (McGrew, terous food-processing techniques in wild mountain
1992), the question of whether or not chimpanzee tools gorillas, whose diet is dominated by a few herbaceous
have functionally distinct parts should be addressed species. Since these herbs are typically defended by
systematically. However, this is beyond the scope of stingers, hooks, spines or fibrous outer cases, multiple
the present study. processing steps are needed, where each step must be
More interestingly, the orangutans did not bite off completed before the next can begin, and sequences
the leaves left at the top of the tool when they put the of 4 to 6 steps are normal. In the present study, the
tools in their mouth to obtain the adherent foodstuffs. orangutans sometimes failed to insert the tool into
Instead, they just wiped the leaves between their lips the hole. However, in more than 80% of these unsuc-
or pressed them between their tongue and the roof cessful trials, they modified the tool or changed their
of their mouth. Josephine also sucked the sponge at technique to successfully insert the tool into the hole,
the top of her special tool, but did not bite off it. such as by stripping obstructive leaves and twigs from
These observations suggest that the orangutans may the branch or grasping the more distal end of the
understand the functional value of the leaves to obtain branch. These acts may reflect hierarchical organiza-
more foodstuffs. Thus, they may understand causality tion in orangutan food-processing techniques. Russon
between the use of the tool with leaves and its out- (1998) also offered evidence that in orangutans, the
come. On the other hand, it is also probable that the cognition that governs motor actions was hierarchi-
orangutans learned of an association between them cally organized to correct errors.
92 M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793

On the other hand, the orangutans in the present (CRES), for his kind permission to perform this re-
study inserted their tools into holes with the distal search at the San Diego Zoo and to H. Fitch-Snyder,
end first in almost all cases. Even immediately after behavior specialist at CRES, for making the neces-
acts in which they could not insert the tool into the sary arrangements for my research. I also thank M.
hole due to fraying of the distal end, they did not try Bate and the other keepers at the San Diego Zoo for
to insert the tool from the opposite end (i.e. the prox- their kind help during the present observations. A.
imal end). As discussed above, orangutans commonly Toth taught me the names of the trees in the enclo-
show hierarchically organized food-processing tech- sure and those of branches given to the orangutans.
niques. However, the orangutans in the present study Two anonymous referees made helpful comments. I
persevered in inserting the tool with the distal end am also grateful to J. Moore, D. Tuzin, A. Mitsuta
first, and the reason for this perseverance is unknown. and the staff of the Laboratory of Ethological Stud-
These results suggest that the orangutans behavioral ies and Department of Ethology, Faculty of Human
flexibility regarding tool-use skills or hierarchical Sciences, Osaka University, for their help and encour-
organization in food-processing techniques may be agement. This work was supported by a grant from
context-dependent. the Ministry of Education, Sciences, and Culture of
Wild Borneo orangutans Pongo pygmaeus abelii Japan.
have rarely been observed to display tool-using skills
despite long-term field studies (Galdikas, 1989), while
Sumatran orangutans (P. p. abelii) in a restricted area References
are habitual tool-users: they use sticks to fish for ants
Boysen, S.T., Kuhlmeier, K., Halliday, P., Halliday, Y.M., 1999.
from tree holes and to extract seeds from hard-husked Tool use in captive gorillas. In: Parker, S.T., Mitchell, R.W.,
fruit (Fox et al., 1999; Van Schaik et al., 1996). Based Miles, H.L. (Eds.), The Mentality of Gorillas and Orangutans.
on their finding that the wild orangutans that use tools Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 179187.
are the most gregarious and socially tolerant among Byrne, R.W., Byrne, J.M., 1991. Hand preferences in the skilled
the known study populations, Van Schaik et al. (1999) gathering tasks of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. berengei).
Cortex 27, 521546.
proposed that some level of gregariousness and some Fossey, D. 1983. Gorillas in the Mist. Houghton Mifflin Company,
level of tolerance during foraging are required for so- Boston.
cial transmission. In the wild, orangutans lead rather Fox, E.A., Sitompul, A.F., van Schaik, C.O., 1999. Intelligent
solitary lives, while in captivity they can live in social tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. In: Parker, S.T.,
groups, like the orangutans at the San Diego Zoo in the Mitchell, R.W., Miles, H.L., (Eds.), The Mentality of Gorillas
and Orangutans. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
present study. Such captive, group-living orangutans pp. 99116.
can offer important opportunities for conducting both Galdikas, B.M.F., 1982. Orang-utan tool use at Tanjung Putting
uncontrolled observations and experimental manipu- Reserve, Central Indonesian Borneo (Kalimantan Tengah). J.
lations, and thus promote a better understanding of Hum. Evol. 10, 1933.
their cognitive abilities. In particular, by examining the Galdikas, B., 1989. Orangutan tool use. Science 243, 152.
Kano, T., 1992. The Last Ape: Behavior and Ecology of Pygmy
performance of group-living orangutans in novel tasks Chimpanzees. Stanford University Press, Stanford.
including tool-using and tool-making, we can evalu- Lethmate, J., 1982. Tool-using skills of orangutans. J. Hum. Evol.
ate the importance of gregariousness and tolerance for 11, 4964.
social transmission, as proposed by Van Schaik et al. McGrew, W.C., 1992. Chimpanzee Material Culture. Cambridge
(1999), and thus gain more information about how University Press, Cambridge, p. 277.
Nakamichi, M., 1999. Spontaneous use of sticks as tools by captive
much sociality influences the process of building new gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates 40, 487498.
cognition structures in orangutans. Nash, V.J., 1982. Tool use by captive chimpanzees at an artificial
termite mound. Zoo Biol. 1, 211221.
Parker, S.T., Kerr, M., Markowitz, H., Gould, J., 1999. A survey
Acknowledgements of tool use in zoo gorillas. In: Parker, S.T., Mitchell, R.W.,
Miles, H.L., (Eds.), The Mentality of Gorillas and Orangutans.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 188193.
I wish to express my sincere thanks to D. Lindburg, Peter, H.H., 2002. Tool use to modify calls by wild orang-utans.
the Center for Reproduction of Endangered Species Folia Primatol. 72, 242244.
M. Nakamichi / Behavioural Processes 65 (2004) 8793 93

Russon, A.E., 1998. The nature and evolution of intelligence in Van Schaik, C.P., Deaner, R.O., Merrill, M.Y., 1999. The conditions
orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Primates 39, 485503. for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of
Russon, A.E., 1999. Orangutans imitation of tool use: a cognitive material culture. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 719741.
interpretation. In: Parker, S.T., Mitchell, R.W., Miles, H.L., Visalberghi, E., Fragaszy, D.M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., 1995.
(Eds), The Mentality of Gorillas and Orangutans. Cambridge Performance in a tool-using by common chimpanzees (Pan
University Press, Cambridge, pp. 117146. troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo
Russon, A.E., Galdicas, B., 1995. Constraints on great apes pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Com.
imitation: model and action selectivity in rehabilitant orangutans Psychol. 109, 5260.
(Pongo pygmaeus) imitation. J. Comp. Psychol. 109, 517. Vizalberghi, E., Tomasello, M., 1998. Primate causal understanding
Van Schaik, C.P., Fox, E.A., Sitompul, A.F., 1996. Manufacture and in the physical and psychological domains. Behav. Processes
use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Naturwissenschaften 42, 189203.
83, 186188.

You might also like