You are on page 1of 24

Development and Psychopathology 17 ~2005!

, 1007–1030
Copyright © 2005 Cambridge University Press
Printed in the United States of America
DOI: 10.10170S0954579405050479

Family interaction and the development


of borderline personality disorder:
A transactional model

ALAN E. FRUZZETTI,a CHAD SHENK,a and PERRY D. HOFFMAN b


a
University of Nevada, Reno; and b Mt. Sinai College of Medicine, New York

Abstract
Although no prospective epidemiological studies have evaluated the relationship between family interactions and
the development of borderline personality disorder ~BPD!, there is considerable evidence for the central role of
family interactions in the development of BPD. This paper describes the role of family interactions or processes,
especially those that might be regarded as invalidating or conflictual, negative or critical, and the absence of more
validating, positive, supportive, empathic interactions, in the development of BPD. Perhaps more importantly, the
proposed model considers how these parental and family behaviors transact with the child’s own behaviors and
emotional vulnerabilities, resulting in a developmental model of BPD that is neither blaming of the family member
with BPD nor of her or his parents and caregivers, and has important and specific implications for both prevention
and intervention.

Borderline personality disorder ~BPD! is one we will discuss a variety of issues pertaining
of the most complex and difficult disorders to to the development of BPD, including classi-
understand and to treat. With high lifetime fication problems, types of models, biological
rates of suicide, hospitalization, and other treat- and genetic factors, the role of parental and
ment utilization, BPD presents an enormous caregiver responses to a child’s developing
public mental health problem in addition to emotional experience, and several parent and
the considerable suffering that those with BPD, family interaction factors, resulting in the de-
and their loved ones, endure. The develop- scription of a comprehensive, transactional
ment of empirically validated etiological mod- model of the development of BPD.
els would be invaluable in developing early
prevention or intervention programs for BPD.
However, like many psychological disorders, Problems With Current
its relatively low prevalence in the general Conceptualizations of BPD
population ~as low as 0.3–0.7%; Lenzen-
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Men-
weger, Loranger, Korfine, & Neff, 1997! make
tal Disorders—4th Edition ~DSM-IV; Ameri-
prospective epidemiological studies that eval-
can Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 650!
uate its etiology from multiple perspectives
defines BPD as “a pervasive pattern of insta-
prohibitive due to costs. Nevertheless, it is
bility of interpersonal relationships, self-
important to develop comprehensive models
image, and affects, and marked impulsivity
consistent with available data. In this paper,
that begins by early adulthood and is present
in a variety of contexts.” The DSM-IV delin-
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Alan E. eates nine formal characteristics of BPD. To
Fruzzetti, Department of Psychology 298, University of receive a diagnosis of BPD using the DSM-IV,
Nevada, Reno, NV 89557; E-mail: aef@unr.edu. an individual must meet a clinical threshold in

1007
1008 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

any of five or more of these nine criteria. Thus, an onset in adolescence or early adulthood
the focus of the diagnosis of BPD is on the and are stable over time, yet studies examin-
presence of any five of these nine criteria and ing the predictive validity of the diagnosis of
that these problem criteria are associated with BPD in adolescence suggest that the diagnosis
significant impairment in the person’s daily is not stable over time ~Garnet, Levy, Mat-
functioning. tanah, Edell, & McGlashan, 1994; Levy et al.,
Many difficulties with a formal classifica- 1999!. These problems are likely the result of
tion system such as the DSM-IV have been a flawed classification system, poor under-
elaborated. For example, researchers and theo- standing of the etiology of BPD, or both. For
rists have criticized the diagnostic system and these reasons, many researchers and theorists
pointed out the lack of empirical support for are now looking to dimensional, rather than
arbitrary diagnostic thresholds ~Morey, 1988!, categorical, conceptualizations of BPD.
the unreliability of many diagnoses due to clin- Dimensional models of BPD based on sta-
ical heterogeneity within diagnostic catego- tistical techniques such as factor analysis and
ries ~Widiger & Sanderson, 1995!, and its lack hierarchical modeling have begun to identify
of theoretical principles to guide its structure aspects of BPD that are predictive of the com-
~Follette & Houts, 1996!. In addition, it has ponent behaviors that are used to define and
been estimated that at least two-thirds of peo- diagnosis the disorder ~e.g., suicide attempts,
ple diagnosed with BPD also meet criteria for parasuicide, impulsivity!. This approach avoids
one or more Axis I diagnoses ~Fabrega, Ul- the nosological problems noted earlier, and
rich, Pilkonis, & Mezzich, 1992!. Other noted increases the predictive validity of the dimen-
problems with diagnostic classification sys- sions assessed, which in turn better informs
tems in general, and with the DSM in particu- treatment. In a review of the literature on BPD,
lar, include a lack of a clear distinction between Skodol et al. ~2002! characterized BPD in two
normal and abnormal personality disorders broad dimensions: impulsive aggression and
~Livesley, Schroeder, Jackson, & Jang, 1994!, emotion dysregulation ~e.g., affective instabil-
a high percentage of comorbidity between di- ity!. Impulsive–aggression and emotion dys-
agnostic categories ~Oldham et al., 1992!, and regulation have recently received the most
its failure to consider context ~especially rela- attention from scientists representing a wide
tionships or ongoing transactions! in determin- variety of theoretical positions ~Critchfield,
ing diagnostic threshold ~Fruzzetti, 1996!. Sanford, Levy, & Clarkin, 2004; Depue & Len-
Diagnostic problems due to heterogeneity are zenweger, 2001; deVegvar, Siever, & Trest-
abundant in BPD specifically: because there man, 1994; Keenan, 2000; Linehan, 1993;
are nine diagnostic criteria for this disorder, Links, Heslegrave, & van Reekum, 1999;
with only five criteria required for diagnosis, Soloff et al., 2003!, although the validity of
it is possible for there to be hundreds of dif- these dimensions to detect reliably and pre-
ferent iterations or variations of the BPD di- dict a diagnosis of BPD has not been estab-
agnosis. Thus, the classification system allows lished. Current evidence supports impulsive–
for any two individuals diagnosed with BPD aggression and emotion dysregulation as key
to have as few as one diagnostic criterion in mediators and precursors of future suicidal
common. Useful research on subtypes of BPD behavior; evaluating these variables further
is ongoing, but the many differing constella- may be useful in understanding the develop-
tions of problems that all meet BPD criteria ment of BPD.
pose a problem for researchers and clinicians
alike. These kinds of problems raise questions
about the validity of this diagnostic paradigm Factors Affecting the Development of BPD
for BPD and other disorders.
The issue of diagnostic validity is even more Many distal factors have been identified in the
problematic when a diagnosis of BPD is as- etiology of BPD. Genetic and biological fac-
sessed with children and adolescents. The DSM tors, histories of sexual and physical trauma
states that personality disorders typically have in childhood, and familial characteristics such
Family interaction and BPD 1009

as problematic interactions between parents Apter, Gidon, Har–Even, & Avidan, 1998!.
and children have been shown to be relevant. One biological explanation for the expression
Although an extensive review of this litera- of impulsive–aggressive behaviors maintains
ture is beyond the scope of this paper, we will that reduced serotonergic activity, specifically
briefly consider the literature on several fac- in the 5-hydroxytryptophan ~5-HT! system,
tors that are relevant to building a model of inhibits a person’s ability to modulate or con-
the development of BPD. trol destructive urges ~Atre–Vaidya & Hus-
sain, 1999; Figueroa & Silk, 1997; Paris et al.,
2004; Rinne, Westenberg, den Boer, & van
Genetic and biological factors
den Brink, 2000; Skodol et al., 2002!. These
Researchers have studied both the genetic in- results have been replicated with group differ-
fluence on meeting diagnostic threshold for ences found among BPD populations and non-
BPD and the degree of inheritance for pheno- clinical controls. Blunting of serotonergic
typic traits associated with BPD. In a study of activity in this same system may also contrib-
personality disorders using a sample of 221 ute to difficulties with emotion regulation
Norwegian twins, Torgersen et al. ~2000! found ~Depue & Lenzenweger, 2001; Krakowski,
a 35% concordance rate between monozy- 2003!. Thus, there appear to be differences in
gotic twins and diagnostic threshold for BPD. serotonergic functioning between those diag-
This rate dropped to 7% for dizygotic twins nosed with BPD and control subjects.
and the diagnosis of BPD, suggesting a ge- A considerable amount of biological re-
netic role in BPD development. With regard search on emotion regulation and dysregula-
to phenotypic expressions of BPD, heritabil- tion has focused on hormonal and physiological
ity rates for emotion dysregulation and impul- correlates following acute environmental
sivity have been reported to be 41 and 30%, stressors. For instance, the hypothalamus–
respectively, for monozygotic twins, pituitary–adrenal ~HPA! axis is one system
and with rates of 12 and 23%, respectively, that is often studied to note differences in emo-
for dizygotic twins ~Livesley, Jang, & Vernon, tion regulation as they pertain to varying lev-
1998!. These studies suggest that for any one els of cortisol. It is generally understood that
person diagnosed with BPD there is a small to exposure to environmental stress initially in-
moderate chance that their children will ex- creases levels of cortisol in the HPA system,
hibit these particular traits, whether the chil- which contributes to the excitation of behav-
dren meet full criteria for the disorder or not. ioral responses to stress ~e.g., fight or flight!
Although these studies do appear to support a that have a regulatory function on emotion
genetic association between BPD and the spe- ~Diamond & Aspinwall, 2003; Figueroa & Silk,
cific behaviors associated with BPD, critics 1997!. Frequent increases in cortisol over the
maintain that the familiality of BPD per se has course of time may affect the 5-HT system by
not been definitively established ~e.g., Dahl, blunting serotonergic activity, thereby linking
1993; White, Gunderson, & Zanarini, 2003!. prolonged exposure to environmental stress to
Regardless of the transmission of BPD per se, symptoms of BPD through biological media-
these studies do seem to indicate that genetics tors ~e.g., HPA, 5-HT systems!. Data of this
are a modest contributing factor to certain traits kind support models that integrate biological
~e.g., impulsive–aggression and emotion dys- systems and describe the elicitation of psycho-
regulation! that are relevant to the develop- pathology as a result of dysfunction across
ment of BPD. these systems ~Depue & Lenzenweger, 2001!.
Impulsive–aggression and emotion dysreg- Vagal tone is another physiological corre-
ulation have received a significant amount of late of an individual’s response to stress that is
attention from biological researchers as well. currently a focus among BPD and emotion
Impulsive aggression has been implicated as a regulation researchers. Research on emotion
significant predictor of future suicide at- regulation in children suggests that children
tempts in both adults ~Brodsky, Malone, Ellis, with a high vagal tone are more adept at reg-
Dulit, & Mann, 1997! and adolescents ~Stein, ulating their emotions and physiological re-
1010 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

sponses to an environmental stressor when Trauma


compared to children who have a low vagal
tone ~Calkins, Smith, Gill, & Johnson, 1998; Another factor receiving significant attention
Gottman & Katz, 2002; Katz & Gottman, 1995; in models of BPD is exposure to childhood
Porges, Doussard–Roosevelt, & Maita, 1994!. sexual abuse ~CSA!. CSA history in BPD pop-
Although researchers have found consis- ulations has been reported to be as high as
tent differences in serotonergic functioning for 75% in both inpatient and outpatient samples
BPD patients, specific causal pathways for the ~Battle et al., 2004; Silk, Lee, Hill, & Lohr,
phenotypic elicitation of problems associated 1995!, and there is evidence that CSA preva-
with BPD are still not clear. Furthermore, the lence is higher in BPD than in other disorders.
specificity of reduced serotonin to impulsive For example, a history of CSA has been shown
or aggressive behaviors is a major problem to discriminate between BPD populations and
for this model as well. For example, many depressed, non-BPD populations for both ad-
people with major depressive disorder also olescents and adults ~Horesh, Sever, & Apter,
have reduced serotonergic activity ~Golden & 2003; Ogata, Silk, & Goodrich, 1990!. Be-
Gilmore, 1990! yet do not demonstrate impul- cause of high rates of CSA among those diag-
sive or aggressive behaviors comparable to nosed with BPD, many researchers have
those with BPD ~e.g., parasuicide, and other suggested that CSA is etiologically linked to
impulsive behaviors!. In turn, pharmacologi- the onset of BPD ~Guzder, Paris, Zelkowitz,
cal agents that target serotonergic functioning & Feldman, 1999; Links & Munroe Blum,
appear to have limited efficacy when treating 1990; Norden, Klein, Donaldson, Pepper, &
BPD ~Soloff, 2005!, especially when com- Klein, 1995; Ogata, Silk, Goodrich, et al., 1990;
pared to the treatment of depression, although Trull, 2001; Wagner & Linehan, 1997; Za-
the proposed mechanisms responsible for each narini, 1997!. Closer examination, however,
disorder are similar. Research has also shown suggests a more complicated relationship be-
that it is difficult to correlate biological mea- tween CSA and the development of BPD.
sures of serotonin with clinical measures of CSA itself does not appear to be the mech-
impulsivity and aggression ~Stein et al., 1996!, anism through which BPD develops. In a pro-
although this later point may be more meth- spective study examining the factors that
odological than ontological ~Critchfield et al., predict future suicidal behaviors, Yen et al.
2004!. Finally, it appears that significant and ~2004! found that CSA was significantly asso-
chronic environmental stressors, such as phys- ciated with future suicidal behavior. However,
ical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect common the mediating variable of “affective stability”
to BPD populations, at times plays a signifi- ~similar to what we are calling emotion dys-
cant role in moderating ~eliciting or exacer- regulation! was most predictive of future
bating! the pathological functioning of the suicidal behaviors ~excluding previous para-
biological correlates associated with BPD. suicidal behavior!. In addition, these research-
What seems clear from biological research ers found that major depression was not
on the development of BPD is that it is essen- predictive of suicidal behaviors. In a similar
tial not to assume that an individual’s biolog- study, Brodsky et al. ~1997! found that CSA
ical make-up develops in isolation from social was significantly related to lifetime number
and developmental factors. Indeed, biological of suicide attempts. However, they found im-
differences in children, adolescents, or adults pulsivity to be the mediator between CSA and
may represent considerably more than linear prediction of future suicidality. Mediational
differential biological development. Biologi- effects of emotion dysregulation have been
cal differences may also result from social found with adolescent populations as well. In
and family responses to individuals over time a sample of adolescents exposed to physical,
that shape individuals’ biology, or from more sexual, and0or emotional abuse, Shields and
complicated transactions between individual Cicchetti ~1998! found that trauma was signif-
temperamental factors and social and family icantly associated with behavioral problems
processes ~see below!. in adolescents. However, when emotional la-
Family interaction and BPD 1011

bility or dysregulation was added to the regres- lation skills, which then reduced the production
sion equation, maltreatment status per se no of cortisol in their bloodstreams. Infants from
longer predicted child behavior problems. That high-trauma environments do not seem to learn
is, in sophisticated regression analyses, emo- these self-regulatory skills, which likely con-
tion dysregulation was more predictive of child tributes to the higher average levels of corti-
problems than maltreatment per se. Thus, al- sol in the bloodstream. This suggests that early
though CSA may be a significant risk factor and frequent exposure to traumatic events in
for the development of BPD, the constellation childhood may have a significant influence on
of difficulties associated with BPD appears to biological processes that affect the ability to
be mediated by the development of emotion regulate emotional arousal and distress
dysregulation, and perhaps other difficulties. ~Keenan, 2000!. Thus, although a person may
In addition, the simple fact remains that more or may not have a genetic predisposition to
than 90% of victims of CSA do not develop the development of BPD ~or to traits associ-
BPD, so CSA or other early traumas can per- ated with BPD!, the biological functioning of
haps best be understood as more distal risk a person at risk for BPD can be shaped and
factors in the development of BPD. developed through traumatic events ~Figueroa
Further complicating, and potentially con- & Silk, 1997!, and likely also through the
founding, the relationship between CSA and successful development of self-regulation
BPD is the fact that physical abuse and emo- skills. Thus, for example, the occurrence of
tional abuse and neglect co-occur at high rates chronic abuse may lead to higher baseline emo-
with CSA. Although researchers employ vary- tional arousal, which could make the develop-
ing definitions of sexual, physical, and emo- ment of problematic means of coping ~e.g.,
tional abuse ~and emotion neglect!, all of these parasuicide, substance use! more likely. That
factors are associated with the development of is, the combination of chronically high emo-
BPD ~e.g., Trull, 2001!. Moreover, some evi- tional arousal, poor impulse control, and prob-
dence suggests that sexual and physical abuse lems regulating emotion ~i.e., not learning
per se are not the most important factors in ordinary self-regulatory emotion skills; cf.
determining negative consequences of these Fruzzetti, Shenk, Mosco, & Lowry, 2003!, may
events; rather, parental and caregiver re- provide a situation in which dysfunctional cop-
sponse to the disclosure of the abuse ~validat- ing patterns and other problematic behaviors
ing or invalidating of the report! may mediate are easily and frequently reinforced.
the effects of the abuse ~Horwitz, Widom, A prospective study by Johnson, Cohen,
McLaughlin, & White, 2001!. Brown, Smailes, and Bernstein ~1999! re-
As mentioned above, research has also ex- ported that children who experienced child-
amined the impact that various types of trauma hood abuse or neglect were four times more
have on biological functioning and on the de- likely to be diagnosed with a personality dis-
velopment of psychopathology in both infants order during young adulthood than children
and adolescents. For example, abnormalities who were not abused. Data such as this sug-
in cortisol functioning frequently have been gests a strong relationship between trauma and
observed in samples with prior exposure to personality dysfunction, although the causal
trauma. When an individual is exposed to stress pathways remain unclear. Most children who
or trauma, cortisol is secreted by the adrenal have been abused will not develop any person-
glands. For example, researchers found that ality disorder ~Binder, McNiel, & Goldstone,
maltreated adolescents had higher levels of 1996! or even experience long-term psycho-
cortisol than nonmaltreated children through- logical difficulties of any kind when the sam-
out daily functioning, which in turn, were as- ple is examined prospectively ~Horwitz et al.,
sociated with more internalizing behavior 2001!. In fact, less than 10% of children with
problems ~Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2001!. Simi- a CSA history go on to meet criteria for BPD
larly, Ramsay and Lewis ~1994! found that as adolescents or adults. Kendall–Tackett, Wil-
infants from low-trauma environments natu- liams, and Finkelhor ~1993!, in a review of
rally habituated to stressors by learning regu- the literature on abuse during childhood, noted
1012 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

a number of variables that minimized the lon- ious dimensions of personality dysfunction
ger term effects of trauma on these children. ~Zweig–Frank & Paris, 1991!, whereas these
These factors included absence of force, shorter same parenting styles may not be predictive
duration of abuse, having a nonrelative abuser, of depression ~Carter, Joyce, Mulder, & Luty,
absence of pretrauma family problems, the 2001!.
child being able to externalize blame to appro- In addition to high rates of childhood abuse,
priate others, and maternal support. Zanarini et al. ~1997! reported that 92% of the
This raises the important question of what borderline patients surveyed reported ~retro-
social and family processes, associated with spectively! having experienced biparental ne-
abuse or not, may influence development to- glect and emotional denial before the age of
ward BPD and problems with impulse control 18, with emotional denial being a significant
and emotion dysregulation versus toward more predictor of the diagnosis of BPD. Conversely,
normative functioning. other studies have shown that the diagnosis of
BPD actually statistically predicts the pres-
ence of parental neglect in this sample ~Battle
Family interactions
et al., 2004!. Researchers in this area con-
Physical, sexual, and emotional abuse and their clude that abuse alone ~like other factors! is
sequellae, of course, occur in a family context neither necessary nor sufficient for the devel-
even when not perpetrated by a family mem- opment of BPD, and that contextual features
ber. As noted, children who receive parental such as specific parent–child relationships and
support ~e.g., believing the child’s report of interactions ~along with other factors such as
abuse, protecting the child without becoming the parents’ relationship! are also key compo-
overprotective, not expressing high levels of nents in the development of BPD. In other
anger! recover more quickly from abuse than words, the specific etiology of BPD appears
children who do not receive these types of to be complex and not linear: abuse or trauma,
parental support after an abuse incident ~Ever- biological predispositions, environmental
son, Hunter, Runyon, Edelsohn, & Coulter, events, and ongoing parent–child and other
1989, 1991!. Thus, parental and other care- social interactions are not regarded as inde-
giver responses ~family interactions! play an pendent causal factors in the development of
important role in mitigating against the ef- BPD, but rather are a set of factors of strong
fects of abuse. In contrast, the lack of emo- influence ~e.g., Meehl, 1977! that interplay in
tional involvement, support, and validation complex ways.
may actually potentiate the effects of abuse The effects of neglectful or uninvolved par-
and be related more generally to the develop- enting on their children are familiar to devel-
ment of BPD or related problems. opmental and clinical psychologists. Stemming
Specifically, neglect, emotional underin- from Baumrind’s ~1967, 1991! conceptualiza-
volvement, and invalidation by caretakers ap- tion of parenting styles, data have suggested
pear to contribute to the development of BPD. consistently that children and adolescents ex-
Prospective studies ~Johnson, Cohen, Gould, posed to this type of parenting more likely
Kasen, Brown, et al., 2002! have shown that develop significant behavioral and psycholog-
parental emotional underinvolvement toward ical difficulties ~Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg,
children impairs their ability to socialize ef- & Dornbusch, 1991; Steinberg, Lamborn,
fectively, which then increases their chances Darling, Mounts, & Dornbusch, 1994!. Re-
of engaging in suicidal behaviors. In this same searchers have also begun to recognize the
study, this type of parenting was associated detrimental effects of parental uninvolvement
with risk for suicide attempts after parental specific to BPD populations. For example,
psychopathology was statistically controlled, Hooley and Hoffman ~1999! found that rela-
thereby illustrating the importance of the par- tively high levels of emotional involvement
enting relationship in the etiology of BPD. by family members were significantly associ-
Other studies also suggest that low parental ated with better clinical outcomes at a 1-year
involvement is a significant risk factor for var- follow-up for patients diagnosed with BPD.
Family interaction and BPD 1013

Thus, although it is important to consider factors. To make the distinction further, it may
temperament and biological, trauma, and fa- be useful to compare several different types of
milial variables in the development of BPD, models of behavior, and distinguish them from
viewing any one of these variables in isolation the transactional model that we propose.
does not provide an adequate account of the
development of BPD. We must consider the
Individual difference models
interplay among all of these factors to under-
stand the development of BPD, and to identify Individual difference models in developmen-
relevant mechanisms of change that are im- tal psychopathology generally focus on bio-
portant for successful intervention. Next, we logical or genetic explanations of problem
will discuss a transactional approach to con- behaviors. In such models, conditions suffi-
ceptualizing BPD that incorporates all of these cient for the manifestation of the disorder
factors into a model of the development of reside within the individual, regardless of learn-
BPD. ing history or developmental processes. In such
a model, the behaviors of BPD would be man-
ifested irrespective of family or social envi-
Transactional and Other Types of Models
ronment, according to factors solely within
Transactional models have long been apart of the person. As noted earlier, to date there is
developmental psychology. For example, Bron- little evidence that the problems of BPD are
fenbrenner’s ~1979! ecological model has been solely ~or even largely! the result of genetic or
useful in identifying the contextual influences individual biological factors irrespective of so-
exerted on a developing child or adolescent cial environmental factors.
beyond traditional models that only look at Temperament is often a key component of
the immediate family context. Ecological mod- individual difference models. It has been
els such as this have influenced the way de- widely studied for many years, especially as a
velopmental theorists approach the issue of factor in the development of psychopathology
the development of psychopathology. For in children and adolescents ~Frick, 2004!. De-
example, leading theorists and researchers scriptors of problematic temperament in the
in adolescence are embracing contextual0 clinical literature include “moody,” “diffi-
transactional models of pathology ~Steinberg cult,” and “ill-mannered,” which has made the
& Avenevoli, 2000! that incorporate the recip- measurement of temperament challenging ~La-
rocal interplay between environmental and bio- hey, 2004!. Despite widely varying conceptu-
logical development. Transactional analyses alizations of temperament, a clear link between
of individuals and their environment have been individual components of temperament and
influential in the field of developmental psy- psychopathology has been established. How-
chopathology as well, where a trend toward ever, the specificity with which temperament
ecological–transactional models of pathology contributes to psychopathology has yet to be
is also apparent ~Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2002; elucidated.
Lynch & Cicchetti, 1998!. Temperament is often regarded as a biolog-
When discussing the development of psy- ical or genetic component that can predispose
chopathology, it is important to make a dis- someone to experience psychopathology, which
tinction between a biological or behavioral is contingent upon environmental factors
predisposition and a present disposition, to a throughout the life span. This concept has been
subsequent event or process. A predisposition described as “goodness of fit” ~Seifer, 2000!,
is an early causal factor that is independent of whereby the relation of temperament to psy-
other factors and not essentially changed over chopathology is mediated through the familial
time. Present disposition describes a current experiences of the child. For example, Calkins,
state biologically, or current tendency to act in Dedmon, Gill, Lomax, and Johnson ~2002!
a particular way. A disposition may be a prox- demonstrated that children who were more
imal “cause” that was influenced by other fac- easily frustrated ~a common measurement of
tors and is in an ongoing transaction with other temperament! during a particular task had more
1014 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

intense physiological reactions and less abil- Interactional models


ity to regulate their emotions compared to
children who were not as easily frustrated. With interactional models, a necessary level
Data such as this suggest that temperament on one factor, in combination with a particu-
is a key component of emotional reactivity lar event ~or other factor! interact to result in a
and physiological arousal related to emotion. particular condition. The factors are static and
These data also suggest that children living essentially unrelated. Such models are often
in an environment in which parents soothe referred to as diathesis stress models, and are
and instruct their children how to manage their common models for diseases. An important
emotional responses are more likely to be ef- dimension here is that the presence of the first
fective at managing emotion without resort- factor ~also called a risk factor, condition, or
ing to aggressive or other dysfunctional diathesis! is not typically considered norma-
behaviors. tive. Consider, for example, being genetically
As mentioned above, models focusing on predisposed to certain allergic reactions ~e.g.,
biological or genetic factors as sufficient causes ragweed!. If a person never comes in contact
of individual psychopathology in general, and with ragweed, he or she will never become
BPD in particular, appear limited. Although congested, sneeze, and so forth from ragweed,
individual temperament and biology appear to regardless of this predisposition. If the predis-
play important roles in to the development of position is present and the person is exposed
various psychological problems, their roles ~it may take several exposures! the individual
likely are in more complex interaction or trans- becomes symptomatic. In this case, neither
action with multiple other variables in the de- the genetic0biological predisposition ~exist-
velopment of BPD. ing even prior to exposure to ragweed! nor the
environmental event itself ~presence of rag-
weed! are sufficient to cause the allergic reac-
Environmental models tion. Rather, both factors, in combination, cause
the sneezing. Moreover, the predisposition and
These models typically maintain that some the stressor are static: avoiding ragweed would
kinds of stressful or traumatic events or pro- not diminish the predisposition, and the level
cesses are sufficient to explain a particular of predisposition has no impact on the amount
disorder. For example, a sufficient amount of of ragweed present. As such, the predisposing
anoxia will result in brain damage, the nature factor and the stress factor are orthogonal.
of the damage depending on age and develop- Currently, interactional models are a popular
ment. No matter how healthy an individual means of understanding, at least retrospec-
infant, child, or adult may be, neurological tively, available data that show high rates of
impairment will result from oxygen depriva- family distress and emotion vulnerability in
tion of a particular duration. Childhood phys- adults with BPD, but these models may be
ical and sexual abuse may also be considered quite limited theoretically.
examples of trauma, of course. But, as noted
above, although high rates of childhood phys- Transactional models
ical and sexual abuse have consistently been
reported ~e.g., Zanarini, Gunderson, & Marino, An alternative to an interactional model, of
1989!, only a small minority of people who course, is a transactional model, wherein two
have been victims of childhood abuse have ~or more! factors transact, or influence each
the pervasive difficulties found in BPD. More- other reciprocally, resulting in a particular con-
over, a significant percent of those with BPD dition ~for an individual! or relationship style
did not have childhood physical or sexual abuse ~for a parent–child or spouse dyad!. Transac-
experiences. Thus, the available evidence does tional models are common ways of understand-
not support these factors ~or other environmen- ing the development of all kinds of behavioral
tal factors in this type of model! as sufficient repertoires in social interactional situations
to explain the development of BPD. ~Fruzzetti, 1996, 2002; Fruzzetti & Iverson, in
Family interaction and BPD 1015

press!, including severe problems such as BPD. placate the child, getting very upset when the
Unlike interactional models, in transactional child does tend toward a tantrum, putting a lot
models a person exhibits a particular behavior of effort into trying to keep the child from
~normative or not! that has some impact on the becoming unhappy or throwing a tantrum!.
person social or family environment ~which may Thus, it is important to distinguish be-
itself be normative or not!. The person may be tween predispositions ~current action tenden-
predisposed to this behavior, or it may simply cies that are the direct result of genetic or
reflect his or her current disposition. People in temperament factors in the individual! and cur-
the social and family environment respond, rent dispositions ~current propensities to act
shaping the individual, who responds, again af- in a particular way, the reasons or causes not
fecting others in the social and family environ- implied nor known definitively!. Unless we
ment, and so forth. Thus, a transactional process have clear evidence of predispositions ~ab-
ensues, with each part influencing the other ~re- normal factors present entirely in the person,
ciprocal influence between the individual and essentially irrespective of learning or life ex-
her or his social environment!. Of course, rel- periences!, it may be more accurate to refer to
evant aspects of genetics, biological strengths current dispositions.
and limitations, and previous learning and so- The distinction between dispositions and
cialization, are all instantiated during the predispositions is important in understanding
transaction. the development of BPD, and may account in
For example, in a transaction between a part for the variety of etiological pathways
parent and a child in a grocery store, the child that have been proposed. As adults, people
might be ~normatively! tired and hungry one with BPD show dispositions to affective dys-
day, and ask for a candy bar in the candy aisle regulation, interpersonal chaos, cognitive
in a whiny kind of way. The parent, also nor- dysregulation, impulsivity, and so forth. Im-
matively tired, might respond brusquely, and portantly, evidence suggests that many of these
the child might get upset and noisier. The par- behaviors can be moderated in both adoles-
ent, perhaps feeling embarrassed, might give cents and adults ~e.g., Miller, Wyman, &
the child the candy bar even though she or he Huppert, 2000; Robins & Chapman, 2004!.
has never done this before; the child becomes Currently, there is insufficient evidence to sup-
quiet, even content, having received the candy; port BPD as a function solely of either indi-
and subsequently the parent feels some relief vidual differences or environmental factors,
~an example of Patterson’s “coercive family and interactional models may similarly be
process;” e.g., Patterson, 2002!. The next time problematic. Alternatively, a transactional
they are in the grocery store, the child may get model may hold some promise.
whiny and ask for a candy bar even if she or
he is not as tired or hungry. In addition, the
Emotion Regulation and BPD
parent may give the child a candy bar in those
circumstances even if he or she is not tired Linehan and colleagues have developed a trans-
either. It is likely that each person has influ- actional model of the development of BPD,
enced ~in this case, reinforced! the other’s be- also called a biosocial theory, which provides
havior in similar situations in the future. Notice the theoretical basis for dialectical behavior
that if this kind of transaction were to con- therapy ~Linehan, 1993!. This biosocial theory
tinue it could result in the child being “dis- is, in essence, a modern contextual behavioral
posed” to tantrums ~i.e., having an abnormally theory, or transactional model, that is com-
low threshold for tantrums!, at least in certain pletely compatible with a developmental
situations. The child may also develop height- psychopathology perspective. The model em-
ened sensitivity to others and may not develop phasizes ~a! the role of temperament in the
skills to tolerate distress in general. Of course, child, including the possible roles of genetics
this pattern also could result in the parent be- and early biological development; ~b! the role
ing “disposed” to high reactivity regarding the of parenting and other family or caregiver re-
child ~i.e., having a low threshold for trying to sponses to the child, as well as the overall
1016 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

quality of the family environment; ~c! the individual’s low threshold for reacting ~emo-
person–environment transaction over time; ~d! tionally and socially! and therefore increase
the resulting continuum of individual disposi- subsequent dysregulation tendencies in rela-
tions and range of behaviors from health to tionships. Thus, if we are able to understand
disorder; and ~e! a modern contextual behav- the factors that contribute to the development
ioral analysis, including operant and classical of chronic and pervasive emotion dysregula-
conditioning, of key behaviors ~including cog- tion, we will understand the development of
nition, biology, emotion, temperament, and at- BPD.
tachment styles, along with overt behavior! to The development of emotion regulation, of
explain the development and maintenance of course, is a normative developmental process,
BPD. What follows is an elaboration and ex- and includes many component behaviors. For
tension of this model. example, Gross ~1998, p. 275! suggests that
In this model ~Fruzzetti & Iverson, in press; normative emotion regulation includes “pro-
Linehan, 1993!, chronic and pervasive emo- cesses by which individuals influence which
tion dysregulation is considered the core fea- emotions they have, when they have them,
ture and core difficulty in BPD ~and related and how they experience and express these
disorders; Fruzzetti, 2002! rather than a “symp- emotions.” Thompson ~1994! notes that emo-
tom” of the disorder. Emotion dysregulation tion regulation processes are in the service of
is a state of negative or aversive emotional the individual’s long-term goals and not nec-
arousal that is sufficiently high to disrupt cog- essarily in the service of short-term goals ~such
nitive and behavioral self-management: the in- as relief from negative arousal!. Emotion dys-
dividual may lose track of important long- regulation often includes such a high level of
term goals, experience diminished abilities to experienced aversive emotional arousal that
solve problems or engage in complex cogni- the individual may engage in problematic be-
tive tasks, and engage in behaviors increas- haviors simply to escape from these short-
ingly designed only to reduce negative arousal, term unpleasant private experiences ~e.g.,
irrespective of long-term consequences. Thus, substance use, angry outbursts, verbal aggres-
emotion dysregulation is hypothesized to sion, extreme social withdrawal or isolation!.
provide a framework from which the other Thus, emotion dysregulation in general is
behaviors of BPD may be understood. The predicated on the following factors ~Fruzzetti
characteristic behaviors and patterns of BPD & Iverson, in press!: ~a! vulnerability to neg-
are understood either to be problematic at- ative emotion, specifically high sensitivity, high
tempts to regulate dysregulated emotion, reactivity, and slow return to baseline, which
problematic attempts to prevent or truncate influence emotional arousal at any given
dysregulated emotions, or natural consequences moment ~cf. Linehan, 1993!; ~b! deficient
of dysregulated emotion. emotion-relevant skills or competencies that
For example, impulsive behaviors such as allow a person to choose situations in which
parasuicide ~self-injury! or substance abuse in he or she can act effectively; manage social
BPD most often function to facilitate an es- interactions effectively; be aware of relevant
cape from high levels of aversive arousal ~or stimuli; discriminate more relevant from less
to prevent escalating arousal from becoming relevant stimuli; identify, label, tolerate, and
highly aversive!. Chaotic relationships and express private experiences accurately; and
fears of abandonment, in contrast, result nat- manage arousal in ways that are consistent
urally when an individual is chronically dys- with long-term goals and values; and ~c! prob-
regulated: such a person would naturally, when lematic responses of others ~especially part-
dysregulated, put significant demands on oth- ners and parents! to expressions of emotion,
ers, often making relationships difficult. Fre- wants, thoughts, and goals; these responses of
quently this results in others minimizing, others are an integral part of emotion dysreg-
avoiding, or ending a relationship, causing sub- ulation because demands of others can initiate
sequent fears of ~real! abandonment. Of course, arousal and responses of others can reduce or
abandonment may further contribute to the exacerbate arousal, and social situations are
Family interaction and BPD 1017

Figure 1. The transactional model: emotion dysregulation a invalidating response cycle; *includes out of control
behavior.

the most common in which demands are placed features of this model. We will discuss the
on individuals with BPD to which they re- model in some detail, considering both emo-
spond with dysregulated emotion. tion vulnerabilities and invalidating family re-
According to Linehan ~1993!, people with sponses, and finally describe the transaction
BPD have extreme difficulties with emotion between these two reciprocal factors in the
regulation, as evidenced by ~a! extreme diffi- development of BPD.
culties compared to norms in changing or mod-
ulating the physiological arousal associated with
Emotion vulnerability
emotion ~emotional lability!, ~b! extreme dif-
ficulties orienting or reorienting attention or cog- What we refer to as emotion vulnerability is
nitive processing ~cognitive dysregulation!, ~c! defined and determined by three factors: emo-
extreme difficulties compared to the norm in- tion sensitivity, emotion reactivity, and slow
hibiting mood-dependent actions ~impulsiv- return to baseline arousal ~cf. Linehan, 1993!.
ity!, ~d! abnormally high likelihood of escalating
or blunting emotions ~escape or avoidance!, and Emotion sensitivity. Emotionally vulnerable
~e! dispositions to organize behavior in the ser- individuals are more likely to have high sen-
vice of internal or mood-dependent goals ~es- sitivity to ~low threshold for discriminating!
cape behaviors, interpersonal insensitivity! the emotionally relevant things in their world.
rather than longer term goals. Because they discriminate or notice ~with or
Emotion dysregulation ~for which BPD may without cognitive awareness! emotionally rel-
be considered the prototype! is hypothesized evant stimuli in their world more readily, they
to develop as a result of transactions between have a lower threshold for reacting in a vari-
a person with emotion vulnerabilities and ety of situations, compared to norms. In con-
parents, partners, or others that respond in “in- trast, people with lower sensitivity may be
validating” ways. Figure 1 displays the core viewed as even tempered, or possibly disinter-
1018 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

ested or disengaged; if a person does not no- may have transacted with their emotional sys-
tice emotionally relevant stimuli, he or she tem ~incipient vulnerabilities! to result in in-
cannot react to them. creased emotion vulnerability ~sensitivity,
reactivity, slow return to baseline! and affect
Emotion reactivity. People high in emotion dysregulation over time. Alternatively, some
reactivity demonstrate more intense emo- adolescents and adults with BPD may have
tional responses to emotionally relevant stim- been, as infants and children, extremely sen-
uli when these stimuli are noticed. Emotionally sitive and reactive, with a slow return to base-
vulnerable individuals will respond more line arousal ~highly vulnerable!, such that even
quickly and0or with greater intensity across a normatively healthy parenting could have
variety of situations, again compared to norms. maintained or even exacerbated their emo-
Of course, high reactivity can include reacting tional vulnerability and increased their dispo-
with a wide variety of emotional responses sition to dysregulation. Notice that in either
~e.g., sadness, shame, anger, fear!. case the individual was vulnerable to the ac-
tual family environment in which she or he
Return to arousal baseline. For some people lived: we may not be able to identify ~post
it takes a longer period of time to return to hoc! whether there was initial temperamental0
baseline after becoming emotionally aroused. biological vulnerability, normatively problem-
People who return to baseline quickly are less atic parenting, or both, during the devel-
vulnerable to the next emotionally relevant opmental transactional process. Thus, many
event in their life ~that they discriminate, at combinations of emotional vulnerability in in-
least!; in contrast, those who return very slowly fancy and childhood and invalidating parent-
are likely to remain at least partially emotion- ing environments could lead to BPD, or other
ally aroused when the next emotionally rele- disorders. In principle, even very high emo-
vant event occurs, which may make subsequent tion vulnerability in a child living in a validat-
reactions more likely to be problematic. ing family environment could lead to normative
In this model, only the combination of all emotional functioning, or even high resil-
three of these factors ~high sensitivity, high ience, as adults. This hypothesis is consistent
reactivity, and slow return to baseline! makes with some models within behavior genetics as
a person vulnerable to chronic emotion dys- well ~cf. Scarr & McCartney, 1983! that ad-
regulation or BPD. Only the combination of dress the transactions between what an indi-
factors is likely to keep a person’s arousal vidual brings to her or his environment, what
high regularly and interfere with the develop- the environment brings, and what behaviors
ment of normative regulatory behaviors. How- are selected in those contexts. Much longitu-
ever, the development of BPD requires that dinal research is needed to understand and test
the emotionally vulnerable person also lack these processes to create a more comprehen-
sufficient skills to manage these vulnerabili- sive, and empirically validated, model.
ties successfully, and transact with and in an
invalidating environment: emotion vulnerabil-
Invalidating family environments
ities are not the disorder per se.
What is uncertain is when individuals who Although the word invalidating is often em-
have problems with emotion regulation and ployed in psychology, in this model it is used
meet criteria for BPD as adolescents or adults to describe a particular set of responses, in
first begin to demonstrate emotion vulnerabil- context, by parents or others in the social and
ity that is not normative. For example, some family environment, to an individual’s behav-
adolescents and adults with BPD may have ior ~including verbal or other expressions of
had normative emotion functioning, or tem- emotion, want, pain, etc.!. Thus, invalidating
perament, as infants or young children. For is not consonant with “bad” but more a de-
these individuals, chronic, pervasive, aver- scription of the inaccuracy and0or judgmental
sive, and0or invalidating responses ~including quality ~rejection of something valid! demon-
neglect! when they were infants and children strated in response to the person. Describing a
Family interaction and BPD 1019

response as invalidating depends less on the the same. For example, in a recent study of cou-
topography of the response in isolation than ple interactions ~Fruzzetti, 2005!, traditionally
its relevance and function regarding the defined “negativity” ~rated by blind observers!
person’s experience or behavior to which it was entered first in a regression model to pre-
responds. For example, if a child sees a sweet dict relationship satisfaction. Then validating
dessert and says “I’m hungry” and the parent and invalidating behaviors were entered as a
replies, “No, you’re not hungry” or “you don’t set, which significantly improved the model.
want that,” we would consider the parent to be Similarly, the relationship between behaviors
invalidating the child’s hunger and experience rated ~by observers! as invalidating and re-
of wanting the sweets. A whole range of to- ported by subjects as negative or conflictual is
pographies would be invalidating: “You’re al- modest ~r ⫽ .29; Fruzzetti, Shenk, Lowry, &
ways wanting something, you ungrateful little Mosco, 2005!. Moreover, validating responses
snot” ~in a nasty tone! might be more obvi- are not necessarily present just because inval-
ously negative, whereas “oh, my love, you idating responses are absent, and vice versa:
don’t want that big dessert now, do you?” ~in the correlation between validating and invali-
a loving voice! may be nicer, but is still inval- dating behaviors ~observer ratings! in one re-
idating. Similarly, misperceiving the child’s cent study was about ⫺.34 ~Fruzzetti et al.,
emotional experience ~whether due to paren- 2005!. Yet, invalidation may be a core part of
tal preoccupation with other stimuli or inaccu- the transaction in the development of emo-
rate expression by the child! may lead to tional and behavioral difficulties. For exam-
mislabeling of the child’s emotion, and “mis- ple, in a recent study of adolescents and their
matched” responding that invalidates her or parents, Shenk and Fruzzetti ~2005! found that
his actual emotional experience. This type of observed parental invalidation was highly re-
invalidating response is similar to what some lated to adolescent reports of family distress,
clinicians may call “empathic failure.” adolescent distress and psychopathology, and
Whereas validating responses legitimize the adolescents’inability to identify and label emo-
child’s ~or adult’s! valid experiences, includ- tions, and related to parent reports of child be-
ing emotions, desires, sensations, thoughts, and havior problems. Similarly, Schneider and
so forth, invalidating responses delegitimize Shipman ~2005! found that lower levels of ob-
valid experiences ~directly maintain the expe- served maternal validation and higher levels of
rience is wrong or invalid, or that the child observed maternal invalidation for their
should not have that experience or behavior! children’s expression of sadness were associ-
or simply fail to acknowledge their existence ated with depression in those children. Thus,
and0or legitimacy ~indirectly maintaining the validating and invalidating behaviors, although
experience is wrong or invalid!. Validating they may at times overlap with more common
responses are not necessarily warm or posi- constructs ~e.g., support or positivity for vali-
tive, and do not necessarily convey agree- dating and conflict, criticism, or negativity for
ment, compliance, or approval; they do convey invalidating behavior!, seem to represent a more
legitimacy and acceptance of the other’s ex- distinct conceptualization of this part of family
perience or behavior, at least minimally. Thus, interactions ~Fruzzetti, 2005; Fruzzetti &
validating responses acknowledge or legiti- Fruzzetti, 2003; Fruzzetti & Iverson, 2004, in
mize only valid behaviors; criticizing or point- press!, one that may be important in under-
ing out problems with faulty behaviors is quite standing the development of chronic emotion
different ~e.g., “no, you can’t wear your san- dysregulation and BPD.
dals to school today because it is cold and
snowing outside” is invalidating a problem-
atic or essentially invalid request; telling the Understanding Invalidating Responses
child she is stupid for wanting to wear sandals
would be, of course, invalidating!. Let us consider more specifically some inval-
Thus, invalidation and criticality or nega- idating processes and the effects of chronic
tive expression overlap, but are not necessarily and pervasive invalidation developmentally.
1020 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

Types of invalidating responses and likely If a parent or caregiver responds in an invali-


developmental consequences for children dating way to behavior X, saying something
like, “don’t do X” and the child does more of
Invalidation of emotions, thoughts, wants, and X, it is likely that the parent would respond
other internal or private behaviors. In an in- with increasingly aversive behaviors ~includ-
validating family environment the child’s com- ing increased invalidation!, contributing to fur-
munication of her or his valid ~actual! internal ther emotion vulnerability and, ultimately, to
or private experiences ~thoughts, feelings, emotion dysregulation.
wants, etc.! are met often by erratic, inappro-
priate, or extreme responses. This can include Invalidation of overt or public behavior. In
~but is not limited to! ~a! not accepting or addition to invalidating private experiences
refuting the accuracy or veracity of the person’s such as emotional experiences, wants, and
self-description; ~b! treating the person’s valid thoughts, parents or others can also respond
responses ~emotions, thoughts, wants, etc.! to with invalidation to many public behaviors in
events or situations as invalid, inappropriate, a manner that likely disrupts normative devel-
flawed, and so forth; ~c! dismissing or trivial- opment. Invalidating behavior in its essence
izing opinions, thoughts, feelings, wants, and punishes behavior that has some validity or
so forth; ~d! criticizing and0or punishing these legitimacy, so not all forms of criticism, neg-
descriptions; ~e! pathologizing normative re- ative feedback, and so forth, are considered
sponses; ~f ! normalizing problematic or patho- invalidating. For example, telling a child that
logical or abnormal responses; and0or ~g! he was wrong to hit a friend or sibling, per-
attributing the person’s normative and “legit- haps even scolding the child for it, is critical
imate” feelings, thoughts, wants, and so forth, and may be negative and unpleasant, but would
to socially unacceptable characteristics ~e.g., not be invalidating if the focus remained on
a “disorder” such as BPD, paranoia, intent to the specifics of hitting, its consequences, and
manipulate, lack of motivation, immaturity!. so forth. However, extremely critical or aver-
An interesting phenomenon relevant to the sive family environments are likely also to be
effects of invalidation on emotional develop- highly invalidating, and have a number of del-
ment is described in experimental research in- eterious consequences ~Biglan, Lewin, & Hops,
vestigating thought and emotion suppression 1990! on both internal experiences and public
that can result from being told not to think or behavior. Avoiding aversives can become a
feel certain things ~cf. Cioffi & Holloway, main motivation for a child’s behavior, and
1993; Wegner & Gold, 1995!. A “rebound” she or he may not be able to do so via effec-
effect has been described in which trying not tive means. The child may instead develop
to think about a particular thing or trying not dysfunctional escape or avoidance behaviors
to feel a particular emotion leads, paradoxi- ~“numbing” out, self-injury, depression, sub-
cally, to focusing more attention on the un- stance use, aggression, bingeing or purging,
wanted thought or feeling and increasing the dangerous, sensation-seeking, or other prob-
intensity of the experience rather than decreas- lem behaviors!. High levels of aversive re-
ing it. For example, telling a small child not to sponding thus may have the generalized effect
spill her or his full glass of milk may lead the of producing erratic behavior, a common prob-
child to focus more on not spilling ~rather lem in BPD.
than simply drinking successfully!, perhaps Moreover, complex developmental reper-
increasing the likelihood of spilling. Thus, in toires, such as intense and sustained engage-
some invalidating environments being told, ment in problem solving, which involve both
“Don’t be angry” or “you should not be sad” cognitive attention and behavioral control, may
~or other invalidating responses! may lead the not be learned in invalidating environments
child to pay more attention to the undesired because such complex behaviors often require
private experience, which increases its inten- consistent feedback that validates small im-
sity and increases the probability of the child provements over time ~shaping! rather than
engaging in associated problematic behavior. consistently calling attention to shortcomings
Family interaction and BPD 1021

~invalidating progress, effort, desire, frustra- cess!. Again, the pervasiveness of invalidat-
tion, etc.!. Without the ability to solve com- ing responses is what defines the parents as
plex problems, children ~and later, teens and invalidating, not normative miscues and occa-
adults! must rely on others to solve them, lead- sional invalidating responses.
ing to what is socially often considered pas-
sivity, neediness ~e.g., fears of abandonment, Invalidation of a sense of self and self-initiated
“enmeshment”!, and social manipulation. Fi- behavior. The concept of “self” is utilized
nally, high levels of invalidation, aversive crit- across a wide range of theoretical perspec-
icism, and punishment for engaging in ordinary, tives. For our purposes here, a person’s self
developmentally appropriate, activities pro- includes the private context of her or his emo-
vide the opportunity for a wide range of with- tions, thoughts, wants, and overt public behav-
drawal, escape, and avoidance behaviors to be iors. This includes the individual’s perspective,
negatively reinforced. That is, by creating reg- knowing what one feels, thinks, wants, and so
ular situations of high negative emotional forth ~e.g., Koerner, Kohlenberg, & Parker,
arousal, especially in the absence of good prob- 1996!, unfettered by environmental constraints.
lem solving and coping skills, these kinds of This view of the development of the self
dysfunctional escape responses ~e.g., sub- has important implications for invalidating en-
stance use, self-injury, eating disordered be- vironments. If a whole class of private expe-
haviors, dissociation! may be the only “skills” riences are systematically invalidated ~wants,
a child or adolescent may have in response to feelings, etc.! in childhood, the developing
chronic high negative emotion. person might have enormous difficulty identi-
fying or trusting ~knowing! what he or she
Minimizing difficulties. Expectations for ma- wants. For example, the very experience of
ture ~appropriate to age! behavior, when cou- “I” may be largely related to normal valida-
pled with support ~both instrumental and tion during language development. Kohlen-
emotional! is a common recipe for effective berg and Tsai ~1991! suggest that validation of
parenting. However, when a parent minimizes a whole class of private behaviors ~e.g., “I
the difficulties that a developmentally appro- want ice cream,” “I see the dog,” “I am hot”!
priate task or situation present to a child, the help the child to know both what she or he
parent is unlikely to provide either the instru- feels in language that is consistent with the
mental help or the emotional support to help verbal community, and to know the difference
the child master the task. This may occur more between “I want0feel0am” and “he0she wants0
frequently if a child is quite different in tem- feels0is” that is a separate person. This is con-
perament and “natural” abilities from the par- sistent with considerable research on emotion
ent: the parent may not easily comprehend socialization, but more clearly highlights the
how something so “simple” ~perhaps the par- role of invalidation per se, when the develop-
ent easily mastered this type of task as a child! mental process is problematic. Thus, in ex-
could be difficult for the child, and may sub- tremely invalidating environments, parents or
sequently invalidate the child on many levels caregivers do not teach children to discrimi-
~e.g., “you’re not trying hard,” or “you are nate effectively between what they feel and
just doing this to be contrary to me,” or “look, what the caregivers feel, what the child wants
it’s very easy; what’s the matter with you?”!. and what the caregiver wants ~or wants the
This poses a particularly vexing problem for child to want!, what the child thinks and what
some parents because there may be a thin line the caregiver thinks. The probability of an in-
between validating the child’s potential ~e.g., validating response may depend on whether
“you can do it,” said in an encouraging way, the parent is paying attention to the child and
without suggesting that failure in task will his or her developmental needs versus the
result in judgment of the child! and invalidat- parent’s own emotion, or other distracting stim-
ing the child’s experience ~e.g., “you can do uli, and children show greater abilities in emo-
it,” said in an insistent way, suggesting that tion regulation and the development of self
acceptance and support is contingent on suc- when the parent attends to the child and
1022 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

responds in a validating way ~Fruzzetti & tory! and is not under aversive control in the
Fruzzetti, 2005!. present or historically ~the person does not
The lack of consistent validating responses, engage in the behavior because of its rewards
along with at least intermittent invalidating from others, nor to avoid aversive responses
responses, does not provide appropriate ~nor- from others!. Thus, in a sense, these intrinsic
mative! discrimination training or socializa- or “self behaviors” come out of a context that
tion of emotion and other private experiences. is largely the person ~healthy self ! at that mo-
Such a maladaptive process naturally leads to ment ~observing one’s own thoughts, feelings,
several of the problems described by individ- desires, etc.!. A successful individual engages
uals with BPD, such as a sense of emptiness in a considerable amount of intrinsic or self-
~not “knowing” one’s private experiences at initiated behavior, which allows one to trans-
all!, boredom ~not knowing what one wants or act with the social environment well ~fostering
feels, or what to do to experience nonaversive high rates of activity and involvement in the
stimulation!, or fears of abandonment ~a valid world in general, allowing others to shape ap-
fear if one must rely on others to interpret the propriate responses, encouraging adaptive be-
world!. haviors specifically that provide a sense of
It is important to note again that such in- autonomy and mastery, etc.!. Invalidating en-
validation may not be malevolent, and the form vironments, by reinforcing escape and with-
of the parent’s behavior per se may vary con- drawal behaviors, and meting out aversive
siderably: the form may be harsh and aversive responses at high rates, no doubt punish many
or may be pleasant and0or normative. For ex- intrinsic or self-initiated behaviors as well.
ample, a child sensitive to warm temperatures This may lead to tendencies toward depres-
may say “the bath water is too hot” or have an sion and anxiety, social isolation, emotion
extreme pain response even if the water is lability, self-invalidation, ostensibly “self-
only just above room temperature. Invalida- sabotaging” behaviors, or pervasive hopeless
tion occurs when the parent or caregiver re- thinking, and so forth, all common behaviors
sponds to the child by forcing the child into among individuals with BPD.
the tub, saying, “no, the temperature is fine,
honey” or when the caregiver spanks and yells
at the child for being “difficult.” What makes Consequences of pervasive invalidating
the response invalidating is that it does not responses
acknowledge the child’s experience as ~possi-
bly! valid, the caregiver does not respond as The consequences for an emotionally vulner-
though it is valid ~e.g., the caregiver could able child living in invalidating environments
say, “OK, let’s add some more cold water” are many, including wide-ranging negative ef-
even if the temperature seems fine to her or fects on emotional and social development and
him!, nor does the caregiver support or help functioning. Below we will highlight some of
the child to tolerate or adjust to the water. Of the major effects of chronic, pervasive inval-
course, if the bath water initially is hot enough idation for vulnerable individuals.
to burn the child and damage the child’s skin,
in the future the child, ironically, may be more Heightened emotional arousal. One immedi-
painfully sensitive even to lukewarm water. ate effect of invalidation is heightened emo-
Thus, invalidation may increase the child’s tional arousal ~Swann & Schroeder, 1995!. In
sensitivity, which makes invalidation more situations of chronic invalidation, over time
likely in the future ~see Figure 1!. the individual’s baseline level of arousal may
In another sense, however, an important increase ~thereby increasing emotion sensitiv-
developmental process regarding intrinsically ity!, and the person may become activated to
motivated behavior also may be disrupted by avoid situations that commonly result in fur-
chronic invalidation. Intrinsic or “self-initiated” ther negative arousal. However, if important
behavior comes out of the “context” of the attachment figures are also the source of in-
individual ~including her or his learning his- validation, this may result in more ambiva-
Family interaction and BPD 1023

lent, approach–avoid relationships common in would likely be “sad.” Alternatively, in the


BPD. same circumstances, the child might instead
glare at the offending sibling and clench her
or his fists. In this situation, a normative label
Cognitive and attentional dysregulation. High
would probably be “angry,” not sad. In an
levels of emotional and psychophysiological
invalidating environment, the more accurate
arousal interfere with cognitive processing in
label would less likely be applied, and the
general. An invalidating environment may be
person teaching the labeling would base the
sufficiently chaotic to affect normative atten-
label on cues outside the child ~perhaps only
tional control development in general. In par-
on environmental cues, or only on how the
ticular, a person’s ability to self-focus, become
caregiver feels!. Consequently, the child
aware of private ~thoughts and feelings! and
transacting with others in an invalidating en-
public events may not be reinforced by an
vironment will less likely integrate both envi-
invalidating, chaotic, family environment.
ronmental events and private experiences into
the label ~and explanation! for the emotion.
Emotion skill deficits. Because of a lack of Of course, this problematic labeling could be
validation or a surfeit of invalidation a person’s somewhat circumscribed ~e.g., only certain
verbal labels for her or his emotions may not feelings might be invalidated! or highly gen-
develop in a way that is consistent with others eralized ~many negative feelings, along with
in the verbal community. Moreover, consis- wants and desires, and other self-initiated be-
tent and accurate verbal labeling and good haviors, could be ignored and0or invalidated
emotion socialization is essential in our in a more pervasive manner!. The inaccuracy
language-based culture for others to respond of the labels for private experiences automat-
to our experience and situation effectively. ically reduces the likelihood that good coping
From a developmental perspective, a person responses will be learned to manage or reg-
requires outside focus and labeling from ulate emotions because emotions must be
others that is consistent and accurate ~corre- discriminated and labeled accurately for ap-
sponding with norms within the culture! to propriate coping strategies to be learned.
discriminate and label new phenomena. No-
where is this more evident than in discriminat- Secondary emotions. In addition to problems
ing and labeling private phenomena that are with mislabeling emotions, maladaptive emo-
not directly observable to others. tional responding to cues may also be learned.
For example, a child learns to label her or Regular invalidation of primary emotions ~those
his experiences as hot or cold when others that are normative, justified, and healthy; Green-
consistently notice the environmental condi- berg & Safran, 1989! can lead children to learn
tions corresponding to the child’s behavior to respond with secondary emotions ~problem-
~shivering, sweating! and apply an accurate atic emotions that are not justified or norma-
label ~e.g., accurate empathy!. If the child is tive in the situation!. For example, routine
shivering and the ambient temperature is 808F, invalidation of primary emotions such as sad-
an attentive caregiver will assess the child’s ness or disappointment may lead children to
health ~perhaps she or he also has a fever, feel angry or ashamed ~common secondary emo-
aches, etc.! and likely will label this phenom- tions! in situations in which sadness or disap-
enon as “sick” instead of simply “cold.” This pointment might be more primary. This further
way, the child learns to scan the environment contributes to emotion skill deficits ~above! and
for certain cues ~outside temperature! that, may elicit further invalidation, resulting in
together with her or his private experience higher rates of emotion dysregulation.
~feeling cold, etc.!, inform the label and the
explanation for the phenomenon. Analogous Emotion dysregulation. Emotion regulation is,
processes occur with emotions. If a child were of course, at the heart of this model of BPD,
hit by an older sibling and cried, looked down as noted earlier. Not only are basic skills in
quietly with eyes closed, a normative label discriminating and labeling emotion not learned
1024 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

or are compromised ~above! in the presence emotional inhibition. Alternatively, on those


of pervasive invalidation, but also an indi- occasions in which inhibition did not result in
vidual’s ability to regulate or manage strong diminished arousal, they might become aware
emotions may never be learned. This may be of it, or express these strong feelings, but do
understood in part as it relates to poor identi- so only past the point at which they could no
fication or labeling of emotion: if a particular longer inhibit, a point too high for the person
feeling or desire is not discerned at all, or to regulate them independently. This would
discerned but mislabeled, the most effective lead to what might seem to others to be wildly
coping skills to manage that feeling will not changeable, unpredictable displays: the expres-
be trained or learned. Even if the feeling ~or sion of moderate emotion would be unlikely
other private experience! is accurately identi- ~it would be inhibited, looking much like sat-
fied and labeled, the caregiver~s! may still not isfaction or a lack of arousal!, and would mean
provide effective strategies or skills for cop- that untended moderate arousal could often
ing ~e.g., the parent may still minimize the escalate into higher, more unpleasant, arousal
difficulties associated with coping and hence that could no longer be inhibited. These kinds
not provide instruction and support!. of “unpredictable” behaviors could easily be-
Thus, in an invalidating family environ- come a context for further invalidation, with
ment, emotion management skills are simply others viewing the borderline individual’s be-
not taught, shaped, encouraged, modeled, and havior as crazy, manipulative, out of control,
so forth, to the extent necessary for success- unpredictable, lazy, and so forth.
ful emotional development. Instead, the child
learns ineffective repertoires to manage dis-
Passivity in problem solving. Invalidation may
tress by necessity. Unfortunately, some of the
also include minimizing the difficulty of solv-
most destructive behaviors that borderline cli-
ing problems ~“just do it”! by failing to appre-
ents demonstrate likely were learned because
ciate and validate either the inherent difficulty
more skillful ways of managing emotion were
in a task or its difficulty under a heightened
not available. That is, rather than learning ef-
state of emotional arousal ~anxious apprehen-
fective coping skills for tolerating distress, bor-
sion, shame, etc.!. If a child does not learn
derline individuals more likely learned
how to solve small ~developmentally appro-
problematic means of managing strong emo-
priate! problems, and does not develop self-
tions, either by escalating and demanding that
efficacy in approaching problems to be solved,
others manage the situation ~and hence, exter-
the child will increasingly turn to others to
nally regulate the individual’s emotions!, by
solve those problems. Over time, the child
escaping these experiences through impulsive
may not develop entire repertoires and instead
acts ~e.g., high sensory behaviors like cutting,
rely on others in many instrumental situa-
sexual activity! that could override high neg-
tions. If the child or adolescent ~or adult! also
ative emotional arousal, or by numbing or
inhibits emotion associated with these situa-
reducing high arousal with alcohol or other
tions ~i.e., suppresses rather than expresses
substance use.
the emotion accurately!, he or she may seem,
In addition, not having the skills to iden-
and indeed be called, manipulative, lazy, and
tify, label, and manage strong emotions may
so forth, which of course, would further inval-
result in oscillation between emotional inhibi-
idate the person’s actual ~valid! emotion and
tion and extreme emotional experiencing and
motivation.
expression. Borderline clients may not have
had experience successfully managing or reg-
ulating their emotions, so naturally would be Self-invalidation. Of course, another conse-
intermittently reinforced for ignoring rising quence of invalidation is that individuals might
negative emotion ~or not discriminating it ini- not learn to trust their emotional responses as
tially! because sometimes they would reregu- valid when parents and caregivers regularly
late, either by chance, or someone would invalidate them. That is, if one’s private expe-
facilitate it externally. This would reinforce riences are pervasively invalidated, one’s own
Family interaction and BPD 1025

experience does not predict or seem to corre- to validate a person who is self-invalidating.
spond with the responses of others, and thus Emotionally dysregulated individuals may use
we may learn not trust our own experience escape behaviors such as anger and aggres-
~we refer to this phenomenon as “gaslight- sion or shame and withdrawal to titrate their
ing” in reference to the 1940 and 1944 films; arousal ~intentionally or not!, making stable,
Fruzzetti, 1995!. Furthermore, self-invalidation reciprocal relationships difficult. Further-
may also be reinforced because it may lead more, individuals who have difficulties ~a!
to diminished criticism, aggression, and in- identifying their desires and preferences, ~b!
validation by others ~Hops, Biglan, & Sher- self-validating what they want, ~c! asserting
man, 1987!. A vicious cycle often ensues: a what they want, and ~d! managing negative
person is pervasively invalidated, he or she feelings such as disappointment, fears of aban-
does not learn emotion skills very well, and donment, and so forth, may appear unpredict-
mislabels, inaccurately expresses, and0or self- able in relationships. Consequently, they may
invalidates frequently; others ~even poten- develop extreme interpersonal styles, oscillat-
tially validating ones! see the person as chaotic ing between nurturing and giving to others
and unpredictable, perhaps emotionally ex- and feeling that they are being exploited. BPD
treme, and further invalidate the person; individuals may indeed have problems trust-
this leads to more self-invalidation. Self- ing others. Similarly, an acquaintance being
invalidation, of course, is highly associated “nice” to someone with much self-loathing
with various forms of individual psychopa- ~negative self-construct! may result in a very
thology ~e.g., depression! in addition to quixotic, negative, response, at which point
BPD, although in various models other labels the person likely withdraws or becomes crit-
may be employed. For example, Swann and ical, reinforcing the negative self-view and
colleagues ~e.g., Giesler, Josephs, & Swann, keeping relationship patterns chaotic. Thus,
1996; Swann, 1997! have demonstrated that following immersion in an invalidating envi-
when a person’s negative “self-construct” is ronment, borderline individuals may later con-
invalidated with a positive evaluation from tribute toward “selecting” further invalidating
another, the individual may try to demon- environments as a result of a combination of
strate or “prove” that his or her construct multiple skill deficits and familiarity, reinforc-
is correct ~i.e., paradoxically exhibit more of ing the cycle of invalidation from others, self-
the behavior being invalidated!. Thus, a per- invalidation, and dysfunctional coping.
son who sees herself as unreliable may pro-
ceed to do something in fact unreliable, if
another person comments on her consistency. Factors that make invalidation more likely
This may not be to sabotage the relationship,
but rather ~according to Swann! to bring eval- Experience or behavior is unexpected. Even
uations from others in line with her self- among biological relatives there are signifi-
construct. It is clear how pervasive invalidation cant differences in temperament. When a
from others can result in chronic self- child’s private experiences are very different
invalidation and the creation of enduring neg- from those of a parent or caregiver, the parent
ative self-constructs, and how the self- may not even imagine that the child’s emo-
verification process in turn can maintain tion, sensations, or wants are what they are.
negative self-views and self-invalidation even This may be exacerbated if one or more sib-
in reasonably compensatory relationships. This lings are similar in temperament to the parent;
is one factor that may make interventions with this might make it even more likely that ordi-
individuals with BPD particularly difficult, nary ~albeit significant! differences would be
and make validation a particularly important missed and misunderstood ~invalidated!. Fur-
feature of treatment. thermore, invalidation would be even more
likely if one child displays extreme emotion
Social and interpersonal dysregulation. Fi- vulnerability and siblings are both normative
nally, as suggested above, it may be difficult and similar to parent~s!.
1026 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

Behavior puts unwanted demands on another. Transactions and Reciprocity Between


Invalidation may be considerably more likely Emotion Vulnerability and
when recognition of a child’s vulnerability or Invalidating Responses
valid needs would put significant demands
on parents or on the care giving system. One The problems of BPD are chronic and severe.
model of this systemically is the increased Although it may be possible for someone to
prescription of stimulants as active ~but not be born with clear precursors to BPD, there is
pathological! children enter the school sys- very limited evidence to support this. Simi-
tem. Alternatively, many parents have strong larly, although very harsh and problematic fam-
implicit rules, for example, that all children ily environments clearly result in myriad
should receive the same amount of attention. problems for the children who grow up in
However, if one child genuinely needs more them ~both as children and later as adoles-
than the others do to foster healthy develop- cents and adults!, evidence suggests that such
ment, parents may opt to minimize the need family environments do not specifically lead
~invalidate the child! rather than break the to BPD. Therefore, in this model, the combi-
rule. In addition, parents may struggle with nation of emotion vulnerability and an invali-
their own psychological difficulties such as dating family environment provide the requisite
depression, anxiety, substance abuse, or other precursors, and as these factors transact over
problems, highly prevalent in the families of time, each leads to a worsening in the other.
those with BPD ~e.g., Trull, 2001!. These par- Much research is needed to clarify and sup-
ents may have few emotional resources left port this model.
to give to needy children, having enormous For example, a child born with an extreme
needs themselves. This sets up a situation in ~e.g., very sensitive, reactive! temperament is
which the children’s needs can easily be in- likely to be different ~statistically! in impor-
validated, which of course ~paradoxically!, in- tant ways from her or his parents or caregiv-
creases their needs for nurturance, support, ers. Moreover, such a child is likely to be
and validation. more emotionally vulnerable and needier than
an average child, and therefore puts very high
demands on caregivers. If the child’s environ-
Other person has insufficient ability to help ment is less than optimal, and the caregivers
or understand. This situation is similar to struggle with their own difficulties ~depres-
the one above, except that perhaps no parent sion, substance abuse, problems in living!, they
or caregiver may have the ability to give a may already engage in high rates of invalidat-
certain child what is “needed.” Colic in ing behavior ~perhaps neglectful, inattentive,
children provides an example: for many chil- harsh, demanding, critical, etc., as well as poor
dren with colic, no amount or type of parental attention to and frequent misunderstanding of
soothing really makes a difference. Fortu- the child’s experiences!, along with low rates
nately, colic is typically time-limited, and is of validating behavior. The increased burdens
readily diagnosed, so it is less likely that par- and demands of a needy child may stress par-
ents “blame” children for it. However, “emo- ents or caregivers a great deal more, exacer-
tional colic” may be a useful metaphor for bating their own distress and their invalidating
some extremely vulnerable children, for whom tendencies, which in turn further destabilize
no available remedies for their suffering are the child and facilitate the child going down a
found, even among caring, stable parents problematic developmental pathway: the vul-
who may even seek help and guidance from nerable child becomes more vulnerable over
experts. Over time, such caregivers can “burn time, and the invalidating family environment
out,” have fewer resources to give, and becomes more invalidating over time. With-
even they may begin to invalidate the child out some significant change in this system,
for her or his own suffering by minimizing the chronic and serious problems are likely.
child’s distress or blaming her or him for the However, the form of invalidation may be
distress. subtle, which makes research difficult, requir-
Family interaction and BPD 1027

ing intensive observation and longitudinal ization and validation. Parents with deficits
follow-up. Similarly, a child’s vulnerabilities in identifying and describing their children’s
toward negative emotion may not start out as emotions and wants could be targeted for help
extreme, and even if they are they may not be in these domains, and their children could
public in many situations, further complicat- receive additional coaching and support from
ing measurement and research. The possibil- other caregivers to try to compensate for pa-
ity that extreme differences in both children’s rental deficits or overall problematic transac-
and parent’s behaviors can still lead to prob- tions. Similarly, interventions with distressed
lems with emotion dysregulation and BPD is parents could focus on reducing their dis-
consistent with available ~typically retrospec- tress, especially in situations in which they
tive! data, and is therefore a strength of this are likely to invalidate, or fail to validate,
model. However, considerably more study is their child.
needed to validate both the essential transac- Finally, another advantage of this model is
tional nature of the model and the specific that there is no inherent blame placed either
component parts of the model. on the child ~or later adolescent or adult! or on
This model does have significant heuristic the parents or caregivers. There are many path-
value in developing early interventions with ways that may lead to the problems associated
troubled families and0or children with ex- with BPD, and it may be impossible ~and ir-
treme emotional vulnerabilities. For exam- relevant! to determine which part came first,
ple, parents in troubled parent–child dyads the child’s emotional vulnerability or the
can learn the importance of trying to under- parent’s invalidation. At the point of discov-
stand and validate problematic child behav- ery of the emotion regulation problems or bor-
iors ~especially private ones!, in addition to derline personality traits it may be sufficient
helping children change them and learn to to implement relevant interventions to alter
regulate their emotion. For example, parent the transaction, which would likely result in
training programs could be expanded to in- improved well being for those on both sides
cluded greater emphasis on emotion social- of the transaction.

References
American Psychiatric Association. ~1994!. Diagnostic and Brodsky, B. S., Malone, K. M., Ellis, S. P., Dulit, R. A., &
statistical manual of mental disorders ~4th ed.!. Wash- Mann, J. J. ~1997!. Characteristics of borderline per-
ington, DC: Author. sonality disorder associated with suicidal behavior.
Atre–Vaidya, N., & Hussain, S. M. ~1999!. Borderline American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1715–1719.
personality disorder and bipolar mood disorder: Two Bronfenbrenner, U. ~1979!. The ecology of human devel-
distinct disorders or a continuum? Journal of Nervous opment: Experiments by nature and design. Cam-
and Mental Disease, 187, 313–315. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Battle, C. L., Shea, M. T., Johnson, D. M., Yen, S., Zlot- Calkins, S. D., Dedmon, S. E., Gill, K. L., Lomax, L. E.,
nick, C., Zanarini, M. C., et al. ~2004!. Childhood & Johnson, L. M. ~2002!. Frustration in infancy: Im-
maltreatment associated with adult personality dis- plications for emotion regulation, physiological pro-
orders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal cesses, and temperament. Infancy, 3, 175–197.
personality disorders study. Journal of Personality Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C.
Disorders, 18, 193–211. ~1998!. Maternal interactive style across contexts: Re-
Baumrind, D. ~1967!. Child care practices anteceding three lations to emotional, behavioral, and physiological
patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development,
Monograph, 75, 43–88. 7, 350–369.
Baumrind, D. ~1991!. Effective parenting during the early Carter, J. D., Joyce, P. R., Mulder, R. T., & Luty, S. E.
adolescent transition. In P. A. Cowan & E. M. Heth- ~2001!. The contribution of temperament, childhood
erington ~Eds.!, Family transitions ~pp. 111–163!. neglect, and abuse to the development of personality
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. dysfunction: A comparison of three models. Journal
Biglan, A., Lewin, L., & Hops, H. ~1990!. A contextual of Personality Disorders, 15, 123–135.
approach to the problem of aversive practices in fam- Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. ~2001!. The impact of
ilies. In G. R. Patterson ~Ed.!, Depression and aggres- child maltreatment and psychopathology on neuro-
sion in family interaction ~pp. 103–129!. Hillsdale, endocrine functioning. Development and Psychopa-
NJ: Erlbaum. thology, 13, 783–804.
Binder, R. L., McNiel, D. E., & Goldstone, R. L. ~1996!. Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. ~2002!. A developmental
Is adaptive coping possible for adult survivors of child- psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Jour-
hood sexual abuse? Psychiatric Services, 47, 186–188. nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6–20.
1028 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

Cioffi, D., & Holloway, J. ~1993!. Delayed costs of sup- Fruzzetti, A. E., & Fruzzetti, A. R. ~2003!. Borderline
pressed pain. Journal of Personality & Social Psychol- personality disorder. In D. Snyder & M. A. Whisman
ogy, 64, 274–282. ~Eds.!, Treating difficult couples: Helping clients with
Critchfield, K. L., Sanford, J. I., Levy, K. N., & Clarkin, coexisting mental and relationship disorders ~pp. 235–
J. F. ~2004!. The relationship between impulsivity, 260!. New York: Guilford Press.
aggression, and impulsive–aggression in borderline Fruzzetti, A. E., & Iverson, K. M. ~2004!. Mindfulness,
personality disorder: An empirical analysis of self- acceptance, validation and “individual” psychopathol-
report measures. Journal of Personality Disorders, ogy in couples. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, &
18, 555–570. M. M. Linehan ~Eds.!, Mindfulness and acceptance:
Dahl, A. A. ~1993!. The personality disorders: A critical Expanding the cognitive–behavioral tradition ~pp.
review of family, twin, and adoption studies. Journal 168–191!. New York: Guilford Press.
of Personality Disorders, 7~Suppl. 1!, 86–99. Fruzzetti, A. E., & Iverson, K. M. ~in press!. Intervening
Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. ~2001!. A neurobe- with couples and families to treat emotion dysregula-
havioral dimensional model. In W. J. Livesley ~Ed.!, tion and psychopathology. In D. K. Snyder, J. Simp-
Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, son, & J. Hughes ~Eds.!, Emotion regulation in families.
and treatment ~pp. 136–176!. New York: Guilford Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Press. Fruzzetti, A. E., Shenk, C., Lowry, K., & Mosco, E. ~2005!.
deVegvar, M.-L., Siever, L. J., & Trestman, R. L. ~1994!. Defining and measuring validating and invalidating
Impulsivity and serotonin in borderline personality behaviors: Reliability and validity of the Validating
disorder. Progress in Psychiatry, 45, 23– 40. and Invalidating Behaviors Coding Scale. Unpub-
Diamond, L. M., & Aspinwall, L. G. ~2003!. Emotion lished manuscript, University of Nevada, Reno.
regulation across the life span: An integrative per- Fruzzetti, A. E., Shenk, C., Mosco, E., & Lowry, K. ~2003!.
spective emphasizing self-regulation, positive affect, Emotion regulation. In W. T. O’Donohue, J. E. Fisher,
and dyadic processes. Motivation and Emotion, 27, & S. C. Hayes ~Eds.!, Cognitive behavior therapy:
125–156. Applying empirically supported techniques in your
Everson, M. D., Hunter, W. M., Runyon, D. K., Edelsohn, practice ~pp. 152–159!. New York: Wiley.
G. A., & Coulter, M. L. ~1991!. Maternal support Fruzzetti, A. R., & Fruzzetti, A. E. ~2005!. Child-contingent
following disclosure of incest. In S. Chess & M. E. parenting: Effects on children’s emotion regulation
Hertzig ~Eds.!, Annual progress in child psychiatry and self-development. Unpublished manuscript.
and child development, 1990 ~pp. 292–306!. Philadel- Garnet, K. E., Levy, K. N., Mattanah, J. J. F., Edell, W. S.,
phia, PA: Brunner0Mazel. & McGlashan, T. H. ~1994!. Borderline personality
Everson, M. D., Hunter, W. M., Runyon, D. K., Edelsohn, disorder in adolescents: Ubiquitous or specific? Amer-
G. A., & Coulter, M. L. ~1989!. Maternal support ican Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1380–1382.
following disclosure of incest. American Journal of Giesler, R. B., Josephs, R. A., & Swann, W. B. ~1996!.
Orthopsychiatry, 59, 197–207. Self-verification in clinical depression: The desire for
Fabrega, H., Ulrich, R., Pilkonis, P., & Mezzich, J. E. negative evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
~1992!. Pure personality disorders in an intake psychi- ogy, 105, 358–368.
atric setting. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, Golden, R. N., & Gilmore, J. H. ~1990!. Serotonin and
153–161. mood disorders. Psychiatric Annals, 20, 580–586.
Figueroa, E., & Silk, K. R. ~1997!. Biological implica- Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. ~2002!. Children’s emo-
tions of childhood sexual abuse in borderline person- tional reactions to stressful parent–child interactions:
ality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, The link between emotion regulation and vagal tone.
71–92. Marriage and Family Review, 34, 265–283.
Follette, W. C., & Houts, A. C. ~1996!. Models of scien- Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. ~1989!. Emotion in
tific progress and the role of theory in taxonomy de- psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 44, 19–29.
velopment: A case study of the DSM. Journal of Gross, J. J. ~1998!. The emerging field of emotion regula-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1120–1132. tion: An integrative review. Review of General Psy-
Frick, P. J. ~2004!. Special selection: Temperament and chology, 2, 271–299.
childhood psychopathology integrating research on Guzder, J., Paris, J., Zelkowitz, P., & Feldman, R.
temperament and childhood psychopathology: Its pit- ~1999!. Psychological risk factors for borderline pa-
falls and promise. Journal of Clinical Child and Ad- thology in school-age children. Journal of the Amer-
olescent Psychology, 33, 2–7. ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Fruzzetti, A. E. ~1995!. The Closeness–Distance Family 38, 206–212.
Interaction Coding System: A functional approach to Hooley, J. M., & Hoffman, P. D. ~1999!. Expressed emo-
coding couple and family interactions. Unpublished tion and clinical outcome in borderline personality dis-
manuscript, University of Nevada, Reno. order. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1557–1562.
Fruzzetti, A. E. ~1996!. Causes and consequences: Indi- Hops, H., Biglan, A., & Sherman, L. ~1987!. Home ob-
vidual distress in the context of couple interactions. servations of family interactions of depressed women.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 55,
1192–1201. 341–346.
Fruzzetti, A. E. ~2002!. Dialectical behavior therapy for Horesh, N., Sever, J., & Apter, A. ~2003!. A comparison of
borderline personality and related disorders. In T. Pat- life events between suicidal adolescents with major
terson ~Ed.!, Comprehensive handbook of psychother- depression and borderline personality disorder. Com-
apy: Vol. 2. Cognitive–behavioral approaches ~pp. prehensive Psychiatry, 44, 277–283.
215–240!. New York: Wiley. Horwitz, A. V., Widom, C., McLaughlin, J., & White,
Fruzzetti, A. E. ~2005!. Beyond support and criticism: H. R. ~2001!. The impact of childhood abuse and
The role of validating and invalidating responses in neglect on adult mental health: A prospective study.
couple and family interaction. Unpublished manuscript. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 184–201.
Family interaction and BPD 1029

Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E., & Bern- Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. ~1998!. An ecological-
stein, D. P. ~1999!. Childhood maltreatment increases transactional analysis of children and contexts: The
risk for personality disorders during early adulthood. longitudinal interplay among child maltreatment, com-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 600– 606. munity violence, and children’s symptomatology. De-
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Gould, M. S., Kasen, S., Brown, velopment and Psychopathology, 10, 235–257.
J., & Brook, J. S. ~2002!. Childhood adversities, inter- Meehl, P. E. ~1977!. Specific etiology and other forms of
personal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts dur- strong inference: Some quantitative meanings. The
ing late adolescence and early adulthood. Archives of Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 2, 33–53.
General Psychiatry, 59, 741–749. Miller, A. L., Wyman, S. E., & Huppert, J. D. ~2000!.
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. ~1995!. Vagal tone protects Analysis of behavioral skills utilized by adolescents
children from marital conflict. Development and Psy- receiving dialectical behavior therapy. Cognitive and
chopathology, 7, 83–92. Behavioral Practice, 7, 183–187.
Keenan, K. ~2000!. Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor Morey, L. C. ~1988!. Personality disorders under DSM-III
for child psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Sci- and DSM-III-R: An examination of convergence, cov-
ence and Practice, 7, 418– 434. erage, and internal consistency. American Journal of
Kendall–Tackett, K. A., Williams, L. M., & Finkelhor, D. Psychiatry, 145, 573–577.
~1993!. Impact of sexual abuse on children: A review Norden, K. A., Klein, D. N., Donaldson, S. K., Pepper,
and synthesis of recent empirical studies. Psycholog- C. M., & Klein, L. M. ~1995!. Reports of the early
ical Bulletin, 113, 164–180. home environment in DSM-III-R personality disor-
Koerner, K., Kohlenberg, R. J., & Parker, C. R. ~1996!. ders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9, 213–223.
Diagnosis of personality disorder: A radical behav- Ogata, S. N., Silk, K. R., & Goodrich, S. ~1990!. The
ioral alternative. Journal of Consulting and Clinical childhood experience of the borderline patient. In P. S.
Psychology, 64, 1169–1176. Links ~Ed.!, Family environment and borderline per-
Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. ~1991!. Functional analytic sonality disorder ~pp. 85–103!. Washington, DC:
psychotherapy: Creating intense and curative thera- American Psychiatric Association.
peutic relationships. New York: Plenum Press. Ogata, S. N., Silk, K. R., Goodrich, S., Lohr, N. E., Wes-
Krakowski, M. ~2003!. Violence and serotonin: influence ten, D., & Hill, E. ~1990!. Childhood sexual and phys-
of impulse control, affect regulation, and social func- ical abuse in adult patients with borderline personality
tioning. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147,
Neurosciences, 15, 294–305. 1008–1013.
Lahey, B. ~2004!. Commentary: Role of temperament in Oldham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Hyler,
developmental models of psychopathology. Journal of S. E., Rosnick, L., & Davies, M. ~1992!. Diagnosis of
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 88–93. DSM-III-R personality disorders by two structured in-
Lamborn, S. D., Mounts, N. S., Steinberg, L., & Dorn- terviews: Patterns of comorbidity. American Journal
busch, S. M. ~1991!. Patterns of competence and ad- of Psychiatry, 149, 213–220.
justment among adolescents from authoritative, Paris, J., Zweig–Frank, H., Ng Ying Kin, N. M. K.,
authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Schwartz, G., Steiger, H., & Nair, N. P. V. ~2004!.
Development, 62, 1049–1065. Neurobiological correlates of diagnosis and underly-
Lenzenweger, M. F., Loranger, A. W., Korfine, L., & ing traits in patients with borderline personality dis-
Neff, C. ~1997!. Detecting personality disorders in a order compared with normal controls. Psychiatry
nonclinical population: Application of a 2-stage for Research, 121, 239–252.
case identification. Archives of General Psychiatry, Patterson, G. R. ~2002!. The early development of coer-
54, 345–351. cive family process. In J. B. Reid & G. R. Patterson
Levy, K. N., Becker, D., Grilo, C. M., Mattanah, J. J. F., ~Eds.!, Antisocial behavior in children and adoles-
Garnet, K. E., Quinlan, D. M., et al. ~1999!. Concur- cents: A developmental analysis and model for inter-
rent and predictive validity of the personality disorder vention ~pp. 25– 44!. Washington, DC: American
diagnosis in adolescent inpatients. The American Jour- Psychological Association.
nal of Psychiatry, 156, 1522–1528. Porges, S. W., Doussard–Roosevelt, J. A., & Maita, A. K.
Linehan, M. M. ~1993!. Cognitive–behavioral treatment ~1994!. Vagal tone and the physiological regulation of
of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guil- emotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in
ford Press. Child Development, 59, 167–186, 250–283.
Links, P. S., Heslegrave, R., & van Reekum, R. ~1999!. Ramsay, D. S., & Lewis, M. ~1994!. Developmental change
Impulsivity: Core aspect of borderline personality dis- in infant cortisol and behavioral response to inocula-
order. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13, 1–9. tion. Child Development, 65, 1491–1502.
Links, P. S., & Munroe Blum, H. ~1990!. Family environ- Rinne, T., Westenberg, H. G. M., den Boer, J. A., & van
ment and borderline personality disorder: Develop- den Brink, W. ~2000!. Serotonergic blunting to meta-
ment of etiologic models. In P. S. Links ~Ed.!, Family chlorophenylpiperazine ~m-CPP! highly correlates with
environment and borderline personality disorder. sustained childhood abuse in impulsive and autoag-
Progress in psychiatry series ~pp. 3–24!. Washington, gressive female borderline patients. Biological Psy-
DC: American Psychiatric Association. chiatry, 47, 548–556.
Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., & Vernon, P. A. ~1998!. Robins, C. J., & Chapman, A. L. ~2004!. Dialectical be-
Phenotypic and genetic structure of traits delineating havior therapy: Current status, recent developments,
personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, and future directions. Journal of Personality Disor-
55, 941–948. ders, 18, 73–89.
Livesley, W. J., Schroeder, M. L., Jackson, D. N., & Jang, Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. ~1983!. How people make
K. L. ~1994!. Categorical distinctions in the study of their own environments: A theory of genotype–
personality disorder: Implications for classification. environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424– 435.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 6–17. Schneider, R. A., & Shipman, K. L. ~2005!. Maternal
1030 A. E. Fruzzetti, C. Shenk, and P. D. Hoffman

emotion socialization and risk for child depression. Swann, W. B. ~1997!. The trouble with change. Psycho-
Unpublished manuscript. logical Science, 8, 177–180.
Seifer, R. ~2000!. Temperament and goodness of fit: Im- Swann, W. B., & Schroeder, D. G. ~1995!. The search for
plications for developmental psychopathology. In A. J. beauty and truth: A framework for understanding re-
Sameroff, M. Lewis, et al. ~Eds.!, Handbook of devel- actions to evaluations. Personality & Social Psychol-
opmental psychopathology ~2nd ed., pp. 257–276!. ogy Bulletin, 21, 1307–1318.
Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. Thompson, R. A. ~1994!. Emotion regulation: A theme in
Shenk, C., & Fruzzetti, A. E. ~2005!. The impact of pa- search of definition. Monographs of the Society for
rental validating and invalidating responses on ado- Research in Child Development, 59, 24–52.
lescent emotion regulation: A comparison of clinic Torgersen, S., Lygren, S., Oien, P. A., Skre, I., Onstad, S.,
and non-clinic samples. Unpublished manuscript. Edvardsen, J., et al. ~2000!. A twin study of personal-
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. ~1998!. Reactive aggression ity disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 41, 416– 425.
among maltreated children: The contributions of at- Trull, T. J. ~2001!. Structural relations between borderline
tention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clini- personality disorder features and putative etiological
cal Child Psychology, 27, 381–395. correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 471–
Silk, K. R., Lee, S., Hill, E. M., & Lohr, N. E. ~1995!. 481.
Borderline symptoms and severity of sexual abuse. Wagner, A. W., & Linehan, M. M. ~1997!. Biosocial per-
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1059–1064. spective on the relationship of childhood sexual abuse,
Skodol, A. E., Siever, L. J., Livesley, W. J., Gunderson, suicidal behavior, and borderline personality disorder.
J. G., Pfohl, B., & Widiger, T. A. ~2002!. The border- Progress in Psychiatry, 49, 203–223.
line diagnosis II: Biology, genetics, and clinical course. Wegner, D. M., & Gold, D. B. ~1995!. Fanning old flames:
Biological Psychiatry, 51, 951–963. Emotional and cognitive effects of suppressing
Soloff, P. H. ~2005!. Pharmacotherapy in borderline thoughts of a past relationship. Journal of Personality
personality disorder. In J. G. Gunderson ~Ed.!, Under- & Social Psychology, 68, 782–792.
standing and treating borderline personality dis- White, C. N., Gunderson, J. G., & Zanarini, M. C. ~2003!.
order: A guide for professionals and families ~pp. Family studies of borderline personality disorder: A
65–82!. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 11, 8–19.
Association. Widiger, T. A., & Sanderson, C. J. ~1995!. Towards a
Soloff, P. H., Meltzer, C. C., Becker, C., Greer, P. J., dimensional model of personality disorders in DSM-IV
Kelly, T. M., & Constantine, D. ~2003!. Impulsivity and DSM-V. In T. A. Widiger ~Ed.!, The DSM-IV
and prefrontal hypometabolism in borderline person- personality disorders ~pp. 433– 458!. New York: Guil-
ality disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, ford Press.
123, 153–163. Yen, S., Shea, M. T., Sanislow, C. A., Grilo, C. M., Skodol,
Stein, D., Apter, A., Gidon, R., Har–Even, D., & Avidan, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., et al. ~2004!. Borderline per-
G. ~1998!. Association between multiple suicide at- sonality disorder criteria associated with prospec-
tempts and negative affects in adolescents. Journal of tively observed suicidal behavior. American Journal
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychi- of Psychiatry, 161, 1296–1298.
atry, 37, 488– 494. Zanarini, M. C. ~Ed.!. ~1997!. Role of sexual abuse in the
Stein, D. J., Hollander, E., DeCaria, C. M., Simeon, D., etiology of borderline personality disorder ~Vol. 49!.
Cohen, L., & Aronowitz, B. ~1996!. m-Chloro- Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
phenylpiperazine challenge in borderline personality Zanarini, M. C., Gunderson, J. G., & Marino, M. F. ~1989!.
disorder: Relationship of neuroendocrine response, be- Childhood experiences of borderline patients. Com-
havioral response and clinical measures. Biological prehensive Psychiatry, 30, 18–25.
Psychiatry, 40, 508–513. Zanarini, M. C., Williams, A. A., Lewis, R. E., Reich,
Steinberg, L., & Avenevoli, S. ~2000!. The role of context R. B., Vera, S. C., Marino, M. F., et al. ~1997!. Re-
in the development of psychopathology: A conceptual ported pathological childhood experiences associated
framework and some speculative propositions. Child with the development of borderline personality disor-
Development, 71, 66–74. der. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1101–1106.
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., Zweig–Frank, H., & Paris, J. ~1991!. Parents’ emotional
& Dornbusch, S. M. ~1994!. Over-time changes in neglect and overprotection according to the recollec-
adjustment and competence among adolescents from tions of patients with borderline personality disorder.
authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 648– 651.
families. Child Development, 65, 754–770.

You might also like