You are on page 1of 10

Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Industrial Marketing Management

How co-branding versus brand extensions drive consumers' evaluations of new


products: A brand equity approach
Ali Besharat ⁎
University of South Florida, College of Business Administration, Department of Marketing, 4202 E. Fowler Avenue, BSN 3403, Tampa, FL 33620-5500, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Current research into co-branding and brand extensions indicates that these marketing strategies benefit
Received 31 October 2008 firms, yet marketing literature examines the concepts only independently. This article reports the findings of
Received in revised form 3 July 2009 two studies, conducted among 256 students, that compare the effectiveness of co-branding versus brand
Accepted 2 December 2009
extension strategies. The comparison of these strategies, both individually and concurrently, considers
Available online 19 March 2010
consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions toward a new product (i.e., Bluetooth-
Keywords:
enabled sunglasses). The first study reveals that the presence of at least one high-equity brand in co-
Co-branding branding strategy suffices to leverage consumers' evaluations of a new product. However, the findings of the
Brand extension second study indicate no significant differences between co-branding and brand extensions in terms of
Brand equity consumer evaluations of an identical product.
Signaling theory © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction within and beyond an original product, such as lower cost advertising
and promotions, therefore usually generalize to co-branding strategy
When Apple launched the iPhone, an Internet-connected smart (Tauber, 1988; Volckner & Sattler, 2006).
phone, its rivals in the cell phone industry worked furiously to introduce Various examples of co-branding span a wide range of product
similar prototype models and extend their product lines. Yet they faced categories. Kellogg, a cereal company, co-branded with ConAgra to
a considerable barrier, in that the iPhone's advanced features dramat- launch Healthy Choice cereals in 1994. Coca-Cola co-branded with
ically increased phone users' expectations, to the point that meeting Diebels, a German beer producer, to market a new fruit beer called
such criteria was unrealistic in the short term. Nonetheless, Google, Dimix in 1998. Nike and Apple jointly launched Nike Plus sport shoes,
which earns profits on the basis of advertising that appears on its online which feature both brands' logos and a technology that enables
products and services, partnered with T-Mobile to introduce a first- communication between the shoes and a runner's iPod. Even Ferrari,
generation cell phone, G1 in August 2008. The astonishing 1.5 million the infamous Italian sport car manufacturer, and Fila, a sport goods
preorders reported in the press indicate that T-Mobile made a smart manufacturer, found the idea appealing and co-branded a series of
move to co-brand with Google to challenge the dominant iPhone. sport clothing and shoes from 2001 to 2004. Philips's cool skin electric
Unstable environments, dynamic markets, intense competition, and shaver is co-branded with Nivea, an international skin and body care
high costs to enter new markets force companies in various industries to brand. This shaver dispenses Nivea for Men shaving lotion or gel.
adopt nontraditional, innovative branding strategies, including co- No study empirically investigates how such combinations of brands,
branding, in an attempt to exploit their existing brand equity (Desai & with their unique brand equities, in co-branding strategy influence
Keller, 2002). This strategy usually provides a tool for differentiation consumers' judgments of the participating brands and the new product.
that leverages brands through the transfer of positive associations, such Prior studies instead study co-branding between one unknown brand
as brand-quality, image, or awareness, from one brand to another and one established brand or two well-known brands, without taking
(McCarthy & Norris, 1999; Simonin & Ruth, 1998; Washburn, Till, & into consideration the possible combinations of their brand equities
Priluck, 2000). Co-branding strategy is defined when two brands jointly (Rao & Ruekert, 1994; Simonin & Ruth, 1998; Voss & Gammoh, 2004).
appear on the logo and/or package of a new product (Grossman & Till, For example, if Sony, as a high-equity brand, intends to engage in co-
1998). Because the ultimate goal of co-branding is to launch a new branding with another brand, is its best choice another high-equity
product, it sometimes is referred to as a special case of brand extension brand, like Nike, or would a low-equity brand produce similar consumer
(Park, Jun, & Shocker, 1996). The financial benefits of extending a brand judgments and market positioning for the new product? If there are no
significant differences in attitudinal factors, perceptions, and future
buying intentions, a high-equity partner such as Sony should select a
⁎ Tel.: +1 813 974 6181 (Office); fax: +1 813 974 6175. low-equity co-branding partner, because it would enjoy more power
E-mail address: abeshara@usf.edu. during contract negotiations and likely more profit premiums.

0019-8501/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.indmarman.2010.02.021
A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249 1241

Furthermore, though researchers investigate the advantages and marketed by a well-known and an unknown brand, they tend to
disadvantages of brand extension and co-branding strategies, they assume the unknown brand shares values and images with the well-
typically consider only one strategy, without assessing consumers' known brand.
evaluations of alternative strategies (Aaker & Keller, 1990; James, Research on co-branding also explores the antecedents and
2005). In particular, no work offers a paired comparison of co- moderators of favorable co-branding evaluations. For example,
branding and brand extensions with identical new products. This Simonin and Ruth (1998) reveal that positive existing attitudes
study addresses this gap by investigating whether co-branding toward each participating brand lead to favorable judgments about a
strategy, relative to brand extension strategy, significantly alters new product. They also identify product fit, or the extent to which the
consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions product categories of the partnering brands appear compatible, and
toward a new product. For example, if consumers generally favor co- brand fit, or the degree of consistency between the images of the
branding to brand extensions for the same product, companies need participating brands, as antecedents of co-branding evaluations.1 The
to seek partners that can help them leverage their brand equities by country-of-origin image of the participating brands also has a
boosting customers' evaluations in the market. significant impact on attitudes toward cross-border brand alliances.
In the next section, this article describes the conceptual back- That is, when customers perceive overall compatibility between the
ground and theories relevant to the study of co-branding and brand countries of origin of the allied brands, they adopt a positive attitude
extension strategies. A synthesis of previous research leads to a set of toward the new co-branded product launched internationally. Brand
hypotheses. The next section describes the research method and familiarity may moderate customers' judgments about co-branding
presents the results, followed by a discussion of the findings with strategy too. When customers are less familiar with partnered brands,
some managerial implications and further research directions for co- the effect of their prior attitudes decreases (Simonin & Ruth, 1998),
branding literature. and more familiar brands contribute more to their co-branding
evaluations (Baumgarth, 2004).
2. Defining the study scope Some researchers extend such findings to other contexts or end
users. Dickinson and Barker (2007) suggest that nonprofit organiza-
2.1. Brand extension definition tions can ally with commercial partners in cause-related marketing to
acquire financial resources. Co-branding concept also may apply to
With brand extension strategy, companies use an established cultural and artistic products, such as those introduced by multiple
brand to launch new products. This approach represents one of the nonprofit art organizations (d'Astous, Colbert, & Fournier, 2007).
most frequently employed branding strategies (Aaker & Keller, 1990). Bengtsson and Servais (2005) even examine how co-branding, as a
However, the failure rates of brand extensions in many fast moving or network of partnerships and trust, may enable industrial markets,
high-tech industries are close to 80% (ACNielsen, 1999), which has whose end users are industrial buyers rather than individual
prompted extensive study of the potential determinants of brand customers, to increase their profitability.
extension success (Bottomley & Doyle, 1996).

2.3. Consumer-based brand equity


2.2. Co-branding definition

Customer-based brand equity receives extensive attention in


Because no globally accepted definition of co-branding exists
marketing literature (Keller, 1993), including investigations into the
(Leuthesser, Kohli, & Suri, 2003), several other terms appear
effects on brand preferences, purchase intentions (Cobb-Walgren,
interchangeable, including brand alliance, composite branding (Park
Ruble, & Donthu, 1995; Van Osselaer & Alba, 2000), and brand
et al., 1996), ingredient or composite branding (Leuthesser et al.,
partnerships (Rao et al., 1999; Rao & Ruekert, 1994; Washburn, Till, &
2003), multi-branding (DiPietro, 2005), and joint or dual branding
Priluck, 2004). There are many ways to measure the brand equity of a
(Levin & Levin, 2000; Rao, Qu, & Ruekert, 1999; Rao & Ruekert, 1994).
company; some researchers assess it at the firm level, others at the
Research that attempts to define co-branding (Abratt & Motlana,
product level, and some at the consumer level. This study conceptua-
2002; Baumgarth, 2004; Washburn et al., 2000) implicitly suggests
lizes brand equity from the customer's point of view and thereby
three criteria:
measures the existence and magnitude of any associations consumers
have with a brand (Keller, 1993). Two underlying dimensions—brand
• Co-branding should be accompanied by a long-term agreement and
awareness (recall and recognition of a brand) and brand image
cooperation.
(overall associations a brand has)—thus emerge to measure the latent
• The name of both brands should appear on the product, logo, or
construct, consumer-based brand equity. Evidently, brands with high
product package.
levels of awareness and favorable and unique associations represent
• The primary objective is to launch a new product in a new or
high-equity brands (Keller, 2003; Yoo & Donthu, 2001).
existing market.

Previous research indicates the benefits of co-branding for 2.4. Co-branding theories
participating brands. For example, co-branding may facilitate the
transfer of positive brand associations from one brand to another. Co-branding research has used various theories such as information
McCarthy and Norris (1999) argue that co-branding also provides asymmetry (Voss & Gammoh, 2004; Rao et al., 1999), information
quality signals to customers about a new product in the market, such integration (Simonin & Ruth, 1998), concept combination (Levin &
that quality perceptions of one partner brand influence quality Levin, 2000), associative learning (Washburn et al., 2004), and
perceptions about another brand. Customers also perceive an average associative network memory models (Samu, Krishnan, & Smith, 1999)
quality host brand that partners with a high-quality brand more to explain underlying mechanisms for this strategy. The research
favorably. Similarly, Park et al. (1996) suggest that two well-known inquiries in this article can be explored using two theories: associative
brands can achieve a better attribute profile when one of them learning (Shimp, 1991) and information asymmetry (signaling) theory
extends into a new product category. Levin and Levin (2000) further (Spence, 1974).
assert that co-branding provides a legitimate context for influencing
impressions about the image of one brand through a transfer from the 1
In a replication though, Baumgarth (2004) does not find a significant role of product fit
second brand. When people encounter a co-branded product in co-branding evaluations.
1242 A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249

2.5. Information asymmetry (signaling) theory their existing brand attitudes. This integration process is similar to any
other sponsorship or celebrity endorsement literature (Dickinson &
The basic tenet of information asymmetry theory is that different Barker, 2007). Therefore, co-branding strategy that combines two high-
parties in a transaction hold different information regarding the equity brands should evoke a better value perception because the
transaction (Spence, 1974). A highly reputable brand might help mitigate combination adds additional information (Washburn et al., 2004). On
the information difference between the seller and customer by sending the other hand, low-equity brands, according to an instrumental
quality signals (Rao & Ruekert, 1994). The brand signal acts as an learning theory, will not provide enough information to people to
indicator that reduces the likelihood of a bad outcome for the buyer leverage their attitude. Simonin and Ruth (1998) also show that the
(Montgomery & Wernerfelt, 1992), because the threat of future monetary attitude toward a co-branded product is accessed in the memory by
and reputation losses usually prevent high-equity brands from behaving sufficiently strong associations and cues from each of the constituent
opportunistically and producing low-quality products. Otherwise, con- brands. If the association links in the memory are not strong enough or
sumers will punish the brand by choosing not to repurchase, engaging in do not exist, customers are not able to leverage their judgment about co-
negative word of mouth, or even boycotting the brand. branding strategy when no positive additional information is accessible
Rao and Ruekert (1994) propose that brand alliances also provide (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981). In other words, adding a low-equity
credible marketplace signals. Even when an unknown brand partners brand to co-branding strategy will not help the other brand to improve
with a highly reputable one, consumers can assume the overall quality the customers' judgments. Accordingly, for any particular product,
of the product, because the presence of the second brand name signals people should hold a greater positive attitude toward co-branding
sufficient quality. Empirical support for this theory comes from, for strategy having two high-equity brands than co-branding strategy
example, Levin et al. (1996), who find that well-known, branded having a low-equity brand paired with a high-equity brand.
ingredient enhances product evaluations for both unknown and well-
known brands. In their investigation of two types of ingredient H1a. Consumers' attitudes for a product having two high-equity
branding strategies—branding the target attribute ingredient with a brands (high-high) are significantly greater than consumers' attitudes
new name as a self-branded ingredient (e.g., Tide with its own for the same product produced by a high-equity brand and a low-
EverFresh bath soap) or with an established, well-respected name as a equity brand (high–low).
co-branded ingredient (e.g., Tide with Irish Spring bath soap)—Desai
and Keller (2002) find that the co-branded ingredient, relative to a H1b. Consumers' attitudes for a product having two high-equity
self-branded ingredient, evokes better quality perceptions. brands (high–high) are significantly greater than consumers' atti-
tudes for the same product produced by a low-equity brand and a
2.6. Associative learning theory high-equity brand (low–high).

Animals make connections among various events that take place in According to the associative learning theory, well-known and
their environment (Shimp, 1991). Both instrumental learning and familiar brands typically have developed a series of associations in the
classical conditioning represent examples of associative learning consumers' minds. When a brand is allied with an almost unknown or
(Shimp, 1991). In the former, the connection may include a reward, less familiar brand, people tend to extend the associations' network of
whereas in classical conditioning, the connection is temporal. These the first brand to the second partner (Washburn et al., 2004). Since
theories can help explain brand equity effects. For instance, buyers can customers always expect high-equity brands to deliver on their
be conditioned to form favorable impressions about a constituent promises regarding the characteristics and quality of their products,
unknown brand in co-branding when they see it allied with a high- customers perceive it to be very uncommon for a high-equity brand to
equity partner (Levin, Davis, & Levin, 1996). If learning occurs during threaten its reputation and put its brand value into risk by delivering a
this stage, people tend to associate their perceptions and emotions low-quality product. Therefore, consumers typically use brand values
about a well-established brand with a second brand. With instrumental of the participating brands as an information source to make their
learning, consumers develop positive feelings toward a particular brand judgments about a co-branded product, especially when the attri-
because of their positive past experiences with the brand. butes of a new product are not testable before a product trial (Rao
et al., 1999). In other words, when customers recognize a high-equity
3. Hypotheses brand in co-branding strategy, they may use it as a simple decision
heuristic to choose their option among other potential alternatives
3.1. Consumer judgments of co-branding strategies (Janiszewski & Van Osselaer, 2000).
Rao and Ruekert (1994) believe that one of the major objectives of
According to the signaling theory, familiarity with a high-equity, co-branding strategy is to leverage the brand value of the participat-
well-reputed brand provides a consumer with favorable information ing brand(s). That is, co-branding strategy implies a synergistic
about the desired quality and should prompt a positive consumer alliance by which the sum is greater than the constituent elements.
attitude (Van Osselaer & Alba, 2000). Van Osselaer and Alba (2000) Therefore, the higher the aggregated brand equity of the partnering
emphasize that consumers base their predictions on product cues, so brands in co-branding strategy is, the stronger the signal about the
they are accurate to the extent that consumers have properly learned quality of a new co-branded product will be. Accordingly, one should
the relationship between the cues and performance. Existing research expect that the strength of quality cues associated with a co-branded
also shows that people may judge a brand based upon external cues product having two high-equity brands will be greater than the
such as brand equity when the brand's attributes are presented strength of quality signals being sent out by a high-equity brand
ambiguously (Levin & Levin, 2000). By making an alliance with paired with a low-equity partner.
another high-equity brand, a new high-equity partner can build
positive associations that lead to an enhanced attribute profiles (Park H2a. Consumers' quality perceptions for a product having two high-
et al., 1996). equity brands paired together (high-high) are significantly greater
Attitudes and beliefs about a co-branded product in the market are than quality perceptions for the same product produced by a high-
mainly formed by the integration of customers' attitudes and evalua- equity brand and a low-equity brand (high–low).
tions about each participating brand in the alliance (Simonin & Ruth,
1998). Ultimately, companies, by having co-branding strategy with H2b. Consumers' quality perceptions for a product having two high-
well-known brands, are able to integrate original brand attitude with equity brands paired together (high–high) are significantly greater
A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249 1243

than quality perceptions for the same product produced by a low- tion, increase product associations, or enhance the overall signal about
equity brand and a high-equity brand (low–high). the presence of attributes or unobservable quality (Rao et al., 1999; Rao &
Ruekert, 1994). Furthermore, as Hillyer and Tikoo (1995) posit in their
One of the major differences between a high-equity and a low-equity conceptual paper, strong brand equity combinations might enhance
brand is the fact that a company with a high brand value can charge brand evaluations in an alliance because people process more positive
premiums prices for its products, without losing customers (Keller, information about the brands. This rationale suggests that in co-branding
2003). As it was expressed previously, brand values can be used as strategy, companies are able to provide customers more information
information cues for consumers' judgments and preferences (Aaker & than when they launch a new product independently. The additional
Keller, 1990). As a result, customers are typically more inclined to pay information may serve as a positive decision cue to customers (Van
high premiums for branded products as opposed to unknown brands, Osselaer & Alba, 2000). In addition, Washburn et al. (2004) argue that co-
even if they are unaware of the product attributes. Co-branding is branding strategy increases the brand equity of each participating
argued to have a synergistic role on the brand strategy of a company brands compared to that of each brand prior to the alliance. Therefore,
(Rao & Ruekert, 1994). That is, the brand equity of co-branding strategy co-branding strategy enhances the value perception and improves the
is greater than the sum of to the brand equities of its constituent brands. price judgment about each constituent brands after the pairing (Simonin
Alternatively, Washburn et al. (2004) show that the brand equities of & Ruth, 1995). Integrated information and signals from co-branding
various co-branding strategies fall in order from highest to lowest as strategy, regardless of the value of each partner brand, may evoke more
follows: high–high, high–low (or low–high), and low–low. favorable attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions toward
Simonin and Ruth (1995) argue that the attitude toward co- the new product than would brand extension strategy.
branding strategy is an important determinant of the price judgment
about the co-branded product. Therefore, the higher the attitude H7. Consumers' attitudes toward a new product in co-branding
toward co-branding strategy is, the greater the judgment about the strategy are greater than attitudes for each constituent brands
price of a co-branded product will be. Accordingly, co-branding launching the same product independently.
strategy that has two high-equity brands provides additional
information and evokes better value perceptions about a product H8. Consumers' quality perceptions toward a new product in co-
than co-branding strategy with a lower brand equity combination, branding strategy are greater than quality perceptions for each
given the same product. constituent brands launching the same product independently.

H3a. Consumers' purchase intentions for a product having two high-


H9. Consumers' purchase intention toward a new product in co-
equity brands (high–high) are significantly greater than purchase
branding strategy are greater than purchase intentions for each
intentions for the same product produced by a high-equity brand and
constituent brands launching the same product independently.
a low-equity brand (high–low).

H3b. Consumers' purchase intentions for a product having two high- 4. Research methodology
equity brands (high–high) are significantly greater than purchase
intentions for the same product produced by a low-equity brand and a 4.1. Participants
high-equity brand (low–high).
The combined participants for both experiments consisted of 256
However, low-equity brands cannot induce favorable attitudes undergraduate business students, who received extra credit in a
and quality perceptions (Van Osselaer & Alba, 2000) and price marketing course offered by a public, southern U.S. university. The
judgment (Simonin & Ruth, 1995). Washburn et al. (2004) believe participants ranged somewhat in age (18–22 years 76.1%, 23–26
that a low-equity brand may serve as a discounting cue, which may 15.3%, 27–30 3.5%, and 35+ years 2.7%, no response 2.4%), with a
cause consumers to rely less on the information retained from it. mean age of 22.8 years and a standard deviation of 2.9. They were
Therefore, consumers likely exhibit lower attitudes, quality percep- distributed nearly equally in gender (male 45.9%, female 54.1%). Only
tions, and purchase intentions in response to a pairing of two low- 3 subjects left the questionnaire completely blank and were excluded
equity brands, because they lack any positive signals about product from the sample. In addition, very few cases involved missing data
characteristics and quality. (less than 1%); in these cases, the mean substitution method assigned
the mean of the group to those with missing variables.
H4. Consumers' attitudes for a product having two low-equity brands
(low–low) are significantly less than attitudes for the same product 4.2. Pretests
produced by two high-equity brands (high–high).
To determine the appropriate product for a hypothetical co-branding
H5. Consumers' quality perceptions for a product having two low- strategy, various combinations were tested (e.g., swimsuit and watch,
equity brands (low-low) are significantly less than quality perceptions sport shoes and vitamin products, DVD players and GPS devices, and
for the same product produced by two high-equity brands (high–high). sunglasses and cell phones). In each combination, respondents (n = 42)
initially read a story describing how two product categories have
H6. Consumers' purchase intentions for a product having two low- formed a new product. Then, they saw the new product on an
equity brands (low–low) are significantly less than purchase intentions anonymous website to get a sense how the new product looks. Subjects
for the same product produced by two high-equity brands (high–high). were told that because of some privacy issues, they were not allowed to
see prices and brands of each new product in the study. For instance, in a
sunglasses and cell phone scenario, the experiment indicated that cell
3.2. Consumer evaluations of co-branding versus brand extensions phone technology had paved the way for Bluetooth technology, while
sunglasses design and quality had increased to allow the technology to
Simonin and Ruth (1998) argue that preexisting attitudes influence be embedded into sunglasses. Then, they were asked to assess the
overall evaluations of brand alliances, because consumers integrate new features and the design of the product on a pre-designed website. On the
information into their existing beliefs. Because of a synergy effect, the website, respondents were able to rotate the product and read some
partners in co-branding strategy may each provide additional informa- descriptive information about the basic attributes of the product.
1244 A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249

Subjects in the pretest ranked their perception about the degree of the between-subjects factorial design, such that the participants (n = 84)
compatibility between each set of product categories forming all four are randomly assigned to a treatment condition (for each condition,
new products. The categories of sunglasses and cell phones categories n = 21). In each treatment, the first brand belonged to the sunglasses
were chosen to construct a new fictitious product, Bluetooth-enabled category, whereas the second brand pertained to the cell phone
sunglasses, in this particular study. The results of the first pretest category. Participants in every treatment condition hear that two
indicated that this combination seemed realistic to the subjects and that participating brands have decided to introduce high-tech Bluetooth-
the participants perceived the two product categories to have good enabled sunglasses, which will enable users to listen to their favorite
product fit (Simonin & Ruth, 1998). music on their cell phone (or any other Bluetooth-enabled devices)
A second pretest served to classify each product category while they are wearing the sunglasses. Then, subjects were able to see
according to its consumer-based brand equity and thereby construct the product's image showing paired brands' names on the product as
distinct co-branding combinations. The participants in this pretest well as read the general descriptive information about the new
(n = 36) evaluated the brand equity of eight brands from each product in an anonymous website. The price of the co-branded
category, using nine-item measures drawn from Yoo and Donthu's product was $100 and was kept fixed for all the treatments.
(2001) study, which employ seven-point Likert scales. The items After reading the treatment-specific scenario, participants
come from a validated instrument in the literature, but this study tests responded to a set of questions designed to measure their attitudes,
their reliability again to confirm their relatedness to the brand equity quality perceptions, and purchase intentions toward the new co-
construct (Cronbach's alpha = .836). The items and scales used to branded product. As a check on the manipulations, each participant
measure the brand equity in this study are listed in the Appendix A. also responded to a set of questions developed to measure the brand
Participants used the brand equity scales to indicate the value of each equity of the co-brand and the partner brands independently. These
brand and revealed Ralph Lauren (mean = 5.24) has the highest brand brand equity measures come from Yoo and Donthu (2001) and
equity in the sunglasses category, whereas Blackberry (mean = 5.08) employ a seven-point Likert scale. Finally, the respondents completed
ranks first in the cell phone category. Police (mean= 2.88) and Hitachi the last section of the questionnaire to indicate their demographic
(mean = 3.19) are the lowest equity brands in the sunglasses and cell factors (e.g., age, gender, race, major, income) and general habits in
phone categories, respectively. Therefore, four different co-branding terms of listening to music and wearing sunglasses (e.g., how often
combinations appear in the experiment pertaining to the launch of a they listen to music and wear sunglasses, how likely they would be to
new fictitious product, Bluetooth-enabled sunglasses: Ralph Lauren– listen to music while wearing sunglasses).
Blackberry (high–high equity), Ralph Lauren–Hitachi (high–low),
Police–Blackberry (low–high), and Police–Hitachi (low–low).
5.2. Statistical results
4.3. Measures
The results confirm that the manipulation is successful. That is, the
4.3.1. Consumer attitudes brand equity ratings of co-branding fall in order from highest to
The operationalization of this variable uses four items, each lowest as follows: high-high (mean = 5.86), high–low (mean = 5.04),
measured with seven-point semantic deferential scales. The items low–high (mean = 4.93), and low–low (mean = 3.22).2 Also, the
come from Maheswaran and Sternthal (1990). In support of the constituent brands, selected to represent high brand equities in both
reliability of the interitem test, the Cronbach's alpha (.811) exceeds product categories, show higher brand equities than partner brands,
.70, the standard cut-off point. The items and scales used to measure assumed to have low brand equities (sunglasses: mean difference= 2.29,
this construct appear in the Appendix B. p b .05; cell phone: mean difference= 2.47, pb .05). Because the objective
of this experiment is to detect significant differences among various co-
4.3.2. Product quality perceptions branding strategies for variables such as attitudes, quality perceptions,
Four items, each measured on a seven-point semantic differential and purchase intentions, appropriate statistical analysis tools include
scale, assess product quality perceptions. Lim, Darley, and Summers separate analyses of variances (ANOVAs) or a MANOVA (multivariate
(1994) provide the items, which produce a Cronbach's alpha of .643, analysis of variance). MANOVA's data assumptions are satisfied, in that
below the .70 standard but still acceptable for an evolving construct the dependent variables are moderately correlated, the data are
such as quality perceptions. An additional analysis of this latent multivariate normally distributed for each treatment, and the equality
construct to consider the possibility of hidden constructs, using of the variance/covariance matrices (Box's test) is available for all
principle component analysis (i.e., items excluded from the reliability treatments.
measure one at a time) reveals no improvement in the Cronbach's The statistical results reveal significant differences in the means
alpha. The Appendix B lists the items that measure this construct. across the treatments (F = 24.3, p b .05). To determine which
dependent variable causes these mean differences, three separate
4.3.3. Purchase intentions analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) use each dependent variable as the
To measure this variable, the study uses three items, each model's dependent variable, such that the remaining dependent
measured on a seven-point semantic differential scale, derived from variables provide the covariates for that model. 3
existing literature on purchase intentions (Baker & Churchill, 1977; No significant mean differences occur in consumers' attitudes in the
Kilbourne, 1986). The Cronbach's alpha (.866) indicates the items high–high versus high–low (mean difference= .234, p N .05) or the
provide good indicators of purchase intentions. Again, the items and high–high versus low–high (mean difference = .576, p N .05), meaning
scales appear in the Appendix B. that it fails to support H1a and H1b. That is, consumers' attitudes toward
a product branded by two high-equity brands are not significantly
5. Experiments different than those for a product that combines a high-equity brand
with a low-equity brand (whether high–low or low–high). Table 1 lists
5.1. Study 1 the results of the multiple comparisons for the attitude construct.

The first study investigates the effects of different levels of brand 2


The result of this manipulation test also confirms the findings of a study by Washburn
equity combinations on consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions, and et al. (2004).
purchase intentions for a new product. It employs a 2 (sunglasses 3
The Bonferroni method is chosen to adjust the confidence level for multiple
brand equity: high or low) × 2 (phone brand equity: high or low) comparisons.
A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249 1245

Table 1
Pairwise comparisons of means for brand attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions.

Treatments comparison Mean difference Standard error Significance

Consumer Quality Purchase Consumer Quality Purchase Consumer Quality Purchase


attitude perception intention attitude perception intention attitude perception intention

Ralph Lauren-Blackberry versus Ralph Lauren-Hitachi 0.234 0.015 1.063 0.227 0.261 0.379 1.000 1.000 .033
(High-High) (High-Low)
Ralph Lauren-Blackberry versus Police-Blackberry 0.576 0.154 1.244 0.226 0.269 0.379 0.077 1.000 0.009
(High-High) (Low-High)
Ralph Lauren-Blackberry versus Police-Hitachi 0.973 1.080 1.304 0.233 0.272 0.382 0.043 0.037 0.006
(High-High) (Low-Low)
Ralph Lauren-Hitachi versus Police-Blackberry 0.341 0.169 0.321 0.219 0.254 0.380 0.740 1.000 1.000
(High-Low) (Low-High)
Ralph Lauren-Hitachi versus Police-Hitachi 0.194 0.538 0.382 0.217 0.248 0.371 1.000 0.200 1.000
(High-Low) (Low-Low)
Police-Blackberry versus Police-Hitachi 0.147 0.369 0.061 0.223 0.255 0.384 1.000 0.916 1.000
(Low-High) (Low-Low)

Similarly, no significant difference emerges for the quality percep- to gain predictive cues. Hence, the associations of one constituent
tions in the high–high versus high–low (mean difference = .015, brand in the joint branding may inhibit the access to any possible
p N .05) nor high–high versus low–high (mean difference = .154, associations of the second ally. That is, when one of the constituent
p N .05). Unlike the predictions of H2a and H2b, quality perceptions brands is well-known, the synergy effects may not be applicable to co-
for products with two high-equity brands do not differ from those for a branding strategy. In other words, the inclusion of only one high-
product with one high-equity and one low-equity brand (see Table 1). equity brand in co-branding strategy has a ceiling effect and provides
However, in support of H3a and H3b, significant differences for the sufficient predictive cues to construct consumers' attitudes. This is a
mean values of purchase intentions are observed. As Table 1 illustrates, compelling reason why H1a and H1b are rejected.
people are more willing to buy a co-branded product having two high- On the other hand, low-equity brands do not provide enough
equity brands than to buy the same product having only one high-equity predictive cues to compete. Therefore, a DA model may explain the
constituent brand (high-low condition: mean difference = 1.063, effects of two low-equity brands in co-branding strategy. Simonin and
p b .05; low–high condition: mean difference = 1.244, p b .05). Ruth (1998) believe that attitude toward a co-branded product, when
The tests of H4, H5, and H6 involve comparing the low–low brand associations are accessible from the memory, is the outcome of the
equity condition with the high–high combination for the three additive information acquired from each partner brand. Nevertheless,
variables of interest. These tests also rely on sets of ANCOVAs for a low–low equity co-branding decision may still fail to provide strong
each variable. Customers' attitudes differ significantly for the high– brand associations to customers to construct a favorable attitude.
high versus low–low treatments, and, specifically, they express Therefore, addition of a low-equity brand to another low-equity
significantly less attitudes toward low brand equity combinations partner does not evoke a synergy effect and ultimately does not
(mean difference = .973, p b .05). So H4is supported. A significant change the customers' attitudes toward a new product. This notion
difference emerges for quality perceptions in support of H5 (mean provides a promising rationale in support of H4.
difference = 1.08, p b .05). Consumers' quality perceptions of a In general, consumers do not expect high-equity brands to produce
product that combines two low-equity brands are significantly low-quality products, even if a firm has allied with a low-equity brand.
lower than those for a product having two high-equity brand Van Osselaer and Alba (2000) believe that there is a direct link between
combinations. Finally, the mean values of purchase intentions differ the learning and the brand equity. That is, high-equity brands predict
significantly between low–low brand equity combinations and high– product quality for consumers even without learning about the
high brand equity combinations (mean difference = 1.304, p b .05). attributes' information. Similarly, Van Osselaer and Janiszewski (2001)
This result supports H6: A product that consists of two low-equity by using an adaptive learning model explain how brand equity may
brands induces significantly lower purchase intentions than does a influence the quality perceptions about a product. According to them, an
product with two high-equity brands (see Table 1). adaptive-learning model leads to a blocking effect. That is, customers
cease any additional information processing after they find a justifiable
6. Discussion cue (e.g., high brand equity) predicting the quality. Therefore, findings
of the current research suggest that having at least one high-equity
Consumer attitudes do not necessarily change for different forms brand in co-branding strategy provides sufficient predictive cues to
of co-branding strategy, having at least one high-equity brand. This customers to judge the quality of the new product. This justification
intuitive finding can be explained with a connectionist model perhaps explains why H2a and H2b are rejected.
(Janiszewski & Van Osselaer, 2000). Janiszewski and Van Osselaer However, low-equity brands may not provide additional information
(2000) argue that there are two distinctive types of models to explain to enhance the overall signal to the consumer about the presence of
brand associations. The first model, Direct Association (DA) model, is attributes or unobservable quality associated with them (Rao et al., 1999).
based on the spreading-activation model (Keller, 1993) in which Specifically, when attributes of a new product are not testable before a
declarative knowledge is represented as a network of nodes product trial, an experienced product, people do not have any information
connected by links in the memory. This model assumes that each source to judge the quality of a co-branded product (Washburn et al.,
cue can gain predictive values that are independent and additive. The 2004). H5 is supported because having two high-equity brands in co-
second model, Least Mean Square (LMS) connectionist model, branding strategy can significantly enhance the strength of quality signals
assumes that cues compete to acquire predictive values. Unlike a DA compared to the pairing of two low-equity brands.
model, the LMS model explains why some additional associations Customers are typically more willing to pay higher prices for
bundled with a current set of associations may not enhance (or even branded products than for unknown brands (Keller, 2003). Brands
sometimes be detrimental to) the positive predictive cues of an generally serve two objectives. First, they can be used as extrinsic cues to
existing brand. When one of the partner brands in co-branding has a represent an aggregate of information about a product to infer quality
high brand equity, brand names in co-branding strategy may compete perceptions (Rao & Ruekert, 1994; Keller, 1993). Second, brands
1246 A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249

positively influence customers' internal reference prices. Biswas and 7. Discussion


Blair (1991) note that customers generally construct an internal
reference price based on their past experience with a stimulus. One The extant literature suggests that co-branding strategy may
element of past experience is recognition of a brand name. According to improve a firm's existing product mix (Desai & Keller, 2002), help the
the associative learning theory, even when customers have not had any firm to enter into new markets (Abratt & Motlana, 2002), add new
sort of direct experience with a product, merely exposure to a brand attributes to current brand associations (Park et al., 1996), or reduce
name gives them a certain degree of familiarity (Shimp, 1991). total cost of advertising (Samu et al., 1999). However, findings of this
Grewal, Krishnan, Baker, and Borin (1998) similarly show that study counter-intuitively show that this strategy compared to the
brand values positively influence customers' internal reference prices. brand extension strategy does not leverage the consumer attitudes,
That is, the more positive the brand reputation is, the higher the quality perceptions, and purchase intentions toward a new product.
buyers' expectations about the price would be. Washburn et al. (2004) Least Mean Square (LMS) connectionist and Direct Association (DA)
show that the brand equities of various co-branding strategies fall in models (Janiszewski & Van Osselaer, 2000) can explain why co-
order from highest to lowest as follows: high–high, high–low (or low– branding strategy under various brand equity combinations does not
high), and low–low. Therefore, by integrating the findings of Grewal have any advantage over the brand extension strategy in terms of
et al. (1998) and Washburn et al. (2004), one should expect that the customers' perception and evaluations of a new product.
price judgment of customers about a new co-branded product will The LMS connectionist model asserts that additional associations
also fall in order from highest to lowest as follows : high–high, high– for a second partner brand added to the current set of associations
low (or low–high), and low–low. Grewal et al. (1998) indicate that may not boost the positive predictive cues of an existing brand.
there is a positive relationship between value perception and purchase Therefore, some of new information or quality associations shared
intention of a product. Since the price of the co-branded product was with a second brand in co-branding strategy may be blocked
identical for all the experiment treatments ($100), the value (Janiszewski & Van Osselaer, 2000). A high–high equity co-branding
perception of respondents as well as their purchase intention for a strategy may provide the same amount of predictive cues to
given product should also comply with the same order. This is a customers as brand extension by either constituent brand may.
legitimate reasoning why hypotheses H3a, H3b, and H6 are supported. Similarly, when consumers' evaluations of a high–low (or low–high)
equity co-branding strategy are compared to those of each partner
6.1. Study 2 brand independently, the ceiling effect may occur (Van Osselaer &
Janiszewski, 2001). That is, customers will stop processing new
This experiment attempts to test H7, H8, and H9 empirically by information after a certain cue predicts an outcome of an entity.
determining whether co-branding strategy might be more successful Hence, when people see a high-equity brand allied with a low-equity
than brand extension. It compares the effectiveness of these two brand, they base their processing merely on the high-equity partner
alternative strategies for the variables of interest (attitudes, quality because it provides sufficient predictive cues about the outcome.
perceptions, and purchase intentions) for an identical product. The The findings also suggest that a low-equity brand, by itself, is not
experiment consists of four, between-subjects, randomized designs, less favorably evaluated than co-branding strategy with a high–low
each with three levels. The first level features co-branding, whereas equity combination. This fact can be explained by the current body of
the two remaining levels correspond to the individual constituent literature identifying the disadvantages of adopting co-branding
brands that that form co-branding strategy. Participants (n = 130) strategy. For instance, Janiszewski and Van Osselaer (2000) in their
were randomly assigned to a single brand condition to evaluate a new fourth study show that co-branding strategy may sometimes dilute (or
product (Bluetooth sunglasses). The questionnaire booklet contains hurt) the constituent brands' associations. Similarly, Farquhar (1994)
the same scenario as in Study 1, but with single constituent brands. argues that brand alliances create asymmetries that risk diluting brand
associations. Hence, combining a high-equity brand with a low-equity
6.2. Statistical results brand may inhibit some positive associations that would have been
activated by the high-equity partner. Consequently, the interference
A global MANOVA test contrasts each treatment, that is, both co- by one partner brand's associations with associations of another ally
branding and the single constituent brands. Similar to the Study 1, the may lead to a negative synergy, whereby the sum of the parts is less
assessment of the MANOVA assumptions shows that the dependent than the sum of individual effects. In other words, a low-equity brand
variables are multivariate normally distributed for each treatment, may serve as a discounting cue in co-branding strategy, which may
and equality of the variance/covariance matrices is available for all of cause consumers to be less inclined to appreciate the value of a high-
the treatments. equity brand paired with a low-equity one (Washburn et al., 2004).
Insignificant results of the global MANOVA test (F = 2.217, p N .05) Various studies have shown that the strength of brand associations
show that none of the dependent variables significantly differ between depends on the extent to which predictive cues are accessible from
co-branding and corresponding brand extensions. Specifically, co- the memory (e.g. Keller, 1993). Therefore, if brand's associations are
branding, regardless of the brand equity combination, does not improve retrievable from the memory, they may be processed independently
consumers' attitudes toward the new product compared with brand and additively (Van Osselaer & Janiszewski, 2001; Janiszewski & Van
extensions by each of the constituent brands (F = 3.615, p N .05). This Osselaer, 2000). However, Van Osselaer and Alba (2000) believe that
finding contradicts H7: Consumers' attitudes toward a new product are low-equity brands do not provide enough predictive cues to
just as favorable in co-branding strategy as in brand extension strategy, customers. Therefore, even if the direct association (DA) model is
regardless of the level of brand equity. Similarly, the results do not involved in the evaluation of a low–low equity co-branding decision,
support H8, such that consumers' quality perceptions in co-branding still the lack of strong brand associations may not form a greater
strategy do not differ significantly from their quality perceptions in positive evaluation toward a new product compared to brand
brand extension strategy, regardless of the level of the brand equity extension by either low-equity constituent brands.
(F = 4.127, p N .05). Finally, co-branding, regardless of the level of brand Overall, two strategies, co-branding and the brand extension, are
equity, does not increase customers' purchase intentions relative to equally perceived in terms of consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions,
brand extension by each individual brand, so H9 must be rejected and purchase intentions. Therefore, the value of brands pairing in co-
(F = 2.119, p N .05). Table 2 shows the detail results of the mean branding strategy does not imply that possibly a more favorable
comparisons between co-branding and individual brand extensions for judgment about a new product will be formed for co-branding versus
the attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchases intentions constructs. the brand extension. These findings will help managers who dominantly
A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249 1247

Table 2
Direct comparison of co-branding strategy to the brand extension based upon mean of consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions.

Treatments Comparison Mean Difference Standard Error Significance

Consumer Quality Purchase Consumer Quality Purchase Consumer Quality Purchase


attitude perception intention attitude perception intention attitude perception intention

Ralph Lauren-Blackberry Ralph Lauren (High) 0.360 0.430 0.726 0.241 0.214 0.387 1.000 0.112 0.193
(High-High) Blackberry (High) 0.034 0.032 0.353 0.236 0.216 0.386 1.000 1.000 1.000
Ralph Lauren-Hitachi Ralph Lauren (High) 0.220 0.234 0.032 0.197 0.179 0.346 0.796 0.580 1.000
(High-Low) Hitachi (Low) 0.264 0.043 0.231 0.200 0.184 0.323 0.570 1.000 1.000
Police-Blackberry Police (Low) 0.317 0.269 0.373 0.169 0.168 0.289 0.192 0.338 0.597
(Low-High) Blackberry (High) 0.355 0.045 0.726 0.164 0.166 0.275 0.098 1.000 1.000
Police-Hitachi Police (Low) 0.433 0.149 0.187 0.180 0.204 0.354 0.054 1.000 1.000
(Low-Low) Hitachi (Low) 0.202 0.238 0.225 0.180 0.198 0.346 0.801 0.588 1.000

associate positive outcomes with co-branding strategy. Given all the show that people are more inclined to purchase a co-branded product
benefits recognized for co-branding strategy including the improve- that has two high-equity brands than the same product having at least
ment of brand image, awareness, transfer of positive associations, cost one low-equity constituent brand. This issue is critical to the profitability
reduction in manufacturing and advertising, enriching the attributes' of the firms. Purchase intention is the ultimate outcome of the
profile, and etc., one should know that co-branding strategy does not consumers' judgment and is the significant predictor of an actual
improve the consumers' judgment about a product when it is compared purchase behavior. Therefore, making an alliance with a low-equity
with a sole brand extension. brand is not a win–win scenario for a well-known brand. A high-equity
brand that intends to involve itself in co-branding strategy with a low-
equity partner should make a trade-off between the profit premiums
8. General discussion and conclusion and financial incentives (if any) and the amount of monetary value it
may lose. Second, unlike the common wisdom about the overall synergy
The intense competition in the dynamic marketplace and high cost effects of co-branding strategy, co-branding strategy between two
of investments involved in entering new markets have forced unknown brands will not enhance the consumers' perceptions and/or
companies to adopt innovative brand strategies such as brand extension purchase intentions to compete with any other co-branding strategies
and co-branding (Desai & Keller, 2002). The extant literature has having at least one high-equity partner. It means that managers who
discussed co-branding strategy as a key marketing technique for the have the propensity to form co-branding strategy should be extremely
firms. It is proposed that this strategy can enable firms to strengthen the cautious about the value of their own brands as well as those of any
overall quality, corporate reputation, and awareness of the participating potential partners they may consider. If brand values of both partners,
brands. This strategy is known as an efficient way of positioning new relative to those of other competitors in the same industry, are
products and a potential source of competitive advantage in the market. considered low, this strategy will not be promising.
Despite the prevalent assumptions in regard to advantages of co- Study 2 addresses another research inquiry. To date, the existing
branding strategy, the current research extends the growing body of co- branding literature has treated co-branding and the brand extension
branding literature by examining the effectiveness of co-branding as strategies as two independent entities. Although some scholars have
well as brand extension strategies in terms of consumers' judgments argued that co-branding is a special case of brand extension (Park et al.,
with the brand equity approach. 1996), they failed to test the advantages of each strategy over one
Study 1 investigates whether various co-branding strategies, pairing another in terms of the consumers' judgments. The second study in this
partners with either equal or non-equal brand values, may differ in paper explores the challenges managers may encounter: should a brand
terms of consumers' perceptions about a new product. Particularly, ally itself with a second brand to introduce a new product in the market
study 1 explores whether synergy effects are observable for any brand or should it launch the product independently? May the association of a
partnerships or whether they are merely limited to the specific type of high-equity brand be diluted by pairing with a low-equity brand such
alliances. The findings suggest that high-equity brands do not benefit that the overall consumers' evaluations are negatively influenced? The
from co-branding strategy for a new product in terms of consumers' findings of this paper suggest that no significant statistical differences
attitudes and quality perceptions. In other words, consumers do not hold exist between co-branding and the brand extension in terms of
a different attitude and quality perception toward a high-equity brand consumers' attitudes, quality perceptions, and purchase intentions for
allied with a low-equity partner than they hold toward the same brand an identical product. This fact alerts brand managers to the con-
paired with another high-equity one. This paper reveals that co- sequences of their decisions for co-branding. In particular, when a firm
branding strategy is not fruitful under every condition. This issue decides to introduce a new product to the market, regardless of its brand
promises appealing managerial implications. If a high-equity brand value, it should extend its own brand rather than searching for a partner
seeks co-branding strategy to promptly respond to the market brand with which to co-brand. Because any strategic alliance entails
turbulence, technological limitations, or market entry barriers, it some behavioral uncertainties and requires additional monitoring, it is
would be better off to make an alliance with a low-equity partner. This not worth engaging in co-branding strategy unless it produces
decision not only does not dilute the consumers' quality perceptions and significant market share or generates cash flow. Obviously, according
attitude towards the high reputable brand, but also offers several to the findings, companies will not benefit from co-branding strategy in
advantages. First, a high-equity brand because of its significance in the terms of consumers' evaluations of a product in the short-run.4 Building
market may demand more profit premiums from a low-equity partner. positive brand attitudes and favorable quality perceptions requires time
Second, a well-known brand has more power in negotiation, meaning and effort. If a company is seeking a quick boost in sales or improved
that it can ask for financial incentives (e.g., manufacturing facilities, consumers' perceptions of a product by making an alliance with another
advertising budget, sales personnel, and etc.) as well as non-financial brand, this research does not suggest any significant outcome for this
incentives (e.g., training, access to networks of suppliers, access to particular decision.
customer databases, and etc.) from a low-equity brand.
Nevertheless, co-branding strategy, composed of at least one low- 4
Washburn et al. (2004) suggest that brand partners in co-branding strategy will
equity brand, has its own drawbacks. First, the results from the study 1 have higher brand equity ratings after than prior to the pairing.
1248 A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249

9. Limitations and further research Appendix A. Customer-based brand equity scale

Despite the contribution of this research to both branding literature


and managerial practices, it contains several limitations that suggest
opportunities for further research. First, the main source of criticism of this
research is perhaps the methodology used. It involves a laboratory setting
where an experiment is carried out. The pretests and actual experiments
are all scenario-based. Although respondents, right after reading a
treatment's scenario, are directed to a website having a simulated 3D
image of the Bluetooth-enabled sunglasses to mimic an actual purchase
experience, this situation is not still realistic. External validity can be
enhanced by future research that tests hypotheses in a retail environment
setting. A more realistic shopping context would allow respondents to
evaluate a product by systematically searching the detail features and
options, yet at the same time, actual customers will be invited to a realistic
shopping environment. Contributing to the artificiality of this research is
also the use of a fictitious product. Although during a pretest respondents
reported that the product seemed realistic and found it to be practical, a
few sample of the respondents do not represent either the general public's
interest or a real world situation. Further research inquires are warranted
to use a real co-branded product in the experiment.5
Second, another limitation pertains to the sampling frame of this
study. Sample groups are limited to only undergraduate students
majoring in business. The demographic profiles of these subjects are
almost homogenous. This fact may create a bias in the responses that
students may provide. Although student samples can be a part of the
potential target market for the new product in this study, they do not
represent a heterogeneous sample of real customers who may be Appendix B. Scales used to measure attitudes, quality perception,
older, have higher income levels, or are less technology savvy. Future and purchase intentions
research should definitely use a probability sampling (e.g., cluster
sampling) for a group of customers to control for the bias associated
with a convenience sample.
Third, the generalizability of this study is also limited by the product
categories and brands chosen. The products categories, sunglasses and
cell phone, are selected according to the level of compatibility between
them and a new product. A different basis for product fit, besides the
physical attributes fit, could produce different results. Future research
should explore circumstances under which two brands with either
similar or dissimilar product categories form an atypical co-branded
product. In addition, future scholars should examine various combina-
tions of attribute profiles for a co-branded product under which one
brand may dilute the second partner. For instance, do customer evaluate
two utilitarian brands more favorably or two hedonic ones? Do
customers search for alignable attributes or non-alignable attributes
for product categories involved in co-branding strategy?
Finally, findings of this study are limited only to co-branding
context. Future research should examine consumers' evaluations in
different contexts such as product bundling or co-branding between
service providers. Another statistical tool, such as conjoint analysis,
might also help future researchers to predict the actual preferences
for co-branded products more effectively.

Acknowledgements

The author sincerely appreciates the insightful critiques from the


anonymous reviewers. The author also thanks Anand Kumar, Uday
Murthy, and Patricia Nickinson for their invaluable comments on
previous versions of this manuscript.

5
The data for this article are collected on March, 2007. Before the data collection
phase, a careful market analysis was carried out such that a real co-branded product
that satisfies the criteria of both studies is selected. No real product that may meet the
expectations of co-branding and brand extensions was found. Ironically, this product is
not a fictitious product anymore. Recently, Oakley, sunglasses manufacturer, has allied
with Motorola to launch their Bluetooth-enabled sunglasses. Therefore, future
research efforts with a new pool of respondents can enhance the external validity of
the current study by using the real product.
A. Besharat / Industrial Marketing Management 39 (2010) 1240–1249 1249

References Lim, J. S., Darley, W. K., & Summers, J. O. (1994). An assessment of country of origin
effects under alternative presentation formats. Journal of the Academy of Marketing
Science, 22(3), 274−282.
Aaker, D. A., & Keller, K. L. (1990). Consumer evaluations of brand extensions. Journal of
Maheswaran, D., & Sternthal, B. (1990). The effects of knowledge, motivation, and type
Marketing, 54(1), 27−41.
of message on ad processing and product judgments. Journal of Consumer Research,
Abratt, R., & Motlana, P. (2002). Managing co-branding strategies: Global brands into
17(1), 66−73.
local markets. Business Horizons, 45(5), 43−50.
McCarthy, M. S., & Norris, D. G. (1999). Improving competitive position using branded
ACNielsen (1999). New product introduction-successful innovation/failure: Fragile
ingredients. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 8(4), 267−285.
boundary. New York, AC Nielsen BASES and Ernst & Young Global Client Consulting.
Montgomery, C. A., & Wernerfelt, B. (1992). Risk reduction and umbrella branding.
Baker, M., & Churchill, G. A. (1977). The impact of physically attractive models on
Journal of Business, 65(1), 31−50.
advertising evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 14(4), 538−555.
Park, C. W., Jun, S. Y., & Shocker, A. D. (1996). Composite branding alliances: An investigation
Baumgarth, C. (2004). Evaluations of co-brands and spill-over effects: Further empirical
of extension and feedback effects. Journal of Marketing Research, 33(4), 453−466.
results. Journal of Marketing Communications, 10(2), 115−131.
Raaijmakers, J., & Shiffrin, R. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological
Bengtsson, A., & Servais, P. (2005). Co-branding on industrial markets. Industrial
Review, 88(2), 93−134.
Marketing Management, 34(7), 706−713.
Rao, A. R., & Ruekert, R. W. (1994). Brand alliances as signals of product quality. Sloan
Biswas, A., & Blair, E. (1991). Contextual effects of reference prices in retail adver-
Management Review, 36(1), 87−97.
tisements. Journal of Marketing, 55(3), 1−12.
Rao, A. R., Qu, L., & Ruekert, R. W. (1999). Signaling unobservable product quality
Bottomley, P. A., & Doyle, J. R. (1996). The formation of attitudes toward brand
through a brand ally. Journal of Marketing Research, 36(2), 258−268.
extensions: Testing and generalizing Aaker and Keller's model. International Journal
Samu, S., Krishnan, H. S., & Smith, R. E. (1999). Using advertising alliances for new
of Research in Marketing, 13(4), 365−377.
product introduction: Interactions between product complementarity and promo-
Cobb-Walgren, C. J., Ruble, C. A., & Donthu, N. (1995). Brand equity, brand preference,
tional strategies. Journal of Marketing, 63(1), 57−74.
and purchase intent. Journal of Advertising, 24(3), 25−40.
Shimp, T. A. (1991). Neo-Pavlovian conditioning and its implications for consumer
d'Astous, A., Colbert, F., & Fournier, M. (2007). An experimental investigation of the use
theory and research. Handbook of consumer behavior. New York: Prentice Hall.
of brand extension and co-branding strategies in the arts. Journal of Services
Simonin, B., & Ruth, J. (1995). Bundling as a strategy for new product introduction:
Marketing, 21(4), 231−240.
Effects on consumers' reservation prices for the bundle, the new product, and its
Desai, K. K., & Keller, K. L. (2002). The effects of ingredient branding strategies on host
tie-in. Journal of Business Research, 33(3), 219−230.
brand extendibility. Journal of Marketing, 66(1), 73−93.
Simonin, B. L., & Ruth, J. A. (1998). Is a company known by the company it keeps?
Dickinson, S., & Barker, A. (2007). Evaluations of branding alliances between non-profit
Assessing the spillover effects of brand alliances on consumer brand attitudes.
and commercial brand partners: The transfer of affect. International Journal of
Journal of Marketing Research, 35(1), 30−42.
Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 12(1), 75−89.
Spence, A. M. (1974). Market signaling. Boston: Harvard University Press.
DiPietro, R. B. (2005). The case against multibranding strategy. Cornell Hotel and
Tauber, E. M. (1988). Brand leverage: Strategy for growth in a cost-control world.
Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 46(1), 96−99.
Journal of Advertising Research, 28(4), 26−30.
Farquhar, P. H. (1994). Strategic challenges for branding. Marketing Management, 3(2),
Van Osselaer, S. M. J., & Alba, J. W. (2000). Consumer learning and brand equity. Journal
8−15.
of Consumer Research, 27(1), 1−16.
Grewal, D., Krishnan, R., Baker, J., & Borin, N. (1998). The effect of store name, brand
Van Osselaer, S. M. J., & Janiszewski, C. (2001). Two ways of learning brand associations.
name and price discounts on consumers' evaluations and purchase intentions.
Journal of Consumer Research, 28(2), 202−223.
Journal of Retailing, 74(3), 331−352.
Volckner, F., & Sattler, H. (2006). Drivers of brand extension success. Journal of
Grossman, R. P., & Till, B. D. (1998). The persistence of classically conditioned brand
Marketing, 70(2), 18−34.
attitudes. Journal of Advertising, 27(1), 23−31.
Voss, K. E., & Gammoh, B. S. (2004). Building brands through brand alliances: Does a
Hillyer, C., & Tikoo, S. (1995). Effect of co-branding on consumer product evaluations.
second ally help? Marketing Letters, 15(2), 147−159.
Advances in consumer research, 22, 123−127.
Washburn, J. H., Till, B. D., & Priluck, R. (2000). Co-branding: Brand equity and trial
James, D. (2005). Guilty through association: Brand association transfer to brand
effects. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 17(7), 591−604.
alliances. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 22(1), 14−24.
Washburn, J. H., Till, B. D., & Priluck, R. (2004). Brand alliance and customer-based
Janiszewski, C., & Van Osselaer, S. M. J. (2000). A connectionist model of brand-quality
brand-equity effects. Psychology & Marketing, 21(7), 487−508.
associations. Journal of Marketing Research, 37(3), 331−350.
Yoo, B., & Donthu, N. (2001). Developing and validating a multidimensional consumer-
Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand
based brand equity scale. Journal of Business Research, 52(1), 1−14.
equity. Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1−22.
Keller, K. L. (2003). Strategic brand management: Building, measuring, and managing
brand equity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Ali Besharat is a PhD candidate of marketing in the University of South Florida, USA. His
Kilbourne, W. E. (1986). An exploratory study of the effect of sex role stereotyping on research interests include the role of affect/cognition in consumer information processing,
attitudes toward magazine advertisements. Journal of the Academy of Marketing investors' psychology, and brand management. Besides numerous proceedings in leading
Science, 14(4), 43−46. conferences such as American Marketing Association, Academy of Marketing Science,
Leuthesser, L., Kohli, C., & Suri, R. (2003). Academic papers 2 + 2 = 5? A framework for Society of Consumer Psychology, and Society of Marketing Advances, he has two papers
using co-branding to leverage a brand. Journal of Brand Management, 11(1), 35−47. under the review of European Journal of Marketing and Journal of Consumer Psychology.
Levin, I. P., & Levin, A. M. (2000). Modeling the role of brand alliances in the assimilation
of product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9(1), 43−52.
Levin, A. M., Davis, J. C., & Levin, I. (1996). Theoretical and empirical linkages between
consumers' responses to different branding strategies. Advances in consumer research,
23, 296−300.

You might also like