You are on page 1of 8

The Journal of Pain, Vol 8, No 9 (September), 2007: pp 700-707

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Psychological Flexibility and Traditional Pain Management


Strategies in Relation to Patient Functioning With Chronic Pain:
An Examination of a Revised Instrument
Lance M. McCracken and Kevin E. Vowles
Pain Management Unit, Royal National Hospital for Rheumatic Disease and University of Bath, Bath, United
Kingdom.

Abstract: Recent developments in cognitive behavioral theory emphasize the role of “psychological
flexibility” in adaptive functioning. Psychological flexibility includes processes of acceptance, mind-
fulness, values, and cognitive defusion. The present study was intended to investigate aspects of
psychological flexibility in relation to the functioning of patients with chronic pain. Two hundred
sixty patients seeking treatment for chronic pain completed a battery of measures, including an
expanded version of an instrument assessing responses to pain that reflect both psychological
flexibility and traditionally conceived “pain management strategies” (ie, pacing, relaxation, positive
self-statements). Initial psychometric evaluation of the expanded instrument yielded 2 reliable sub-
scales, as hypothesized. Both subscales were correlated with measures of emotional functioning and
psychosocial disability, although psychological flexibility achieved larger correlations and was corre-
lated with additional measures of physical functioning, health care use, and work status. Regression
analyses indicated that, after pain and patient background variables were statistically controlled,
psychological flexibility accounted for significant variance in eight separate measures of functioning
while pain management strategies accounted for significant variance in none. These results may call
for a shift in our approaches to chronic pain in line with developments taking place in broader areas
of behavioral and cognitive therapy.
Perspective: This study includes development of an instrument for assessing coping, consisting of
traditionally conceived coping strategies and a process that may be unfamiliar to most readers, termed
“psychological flexibility.” Results demonstrated that this process, a blend of acceptance, values-based
action, mindfulness, and cognitive defusion, is significantly related to patient functioning with chronic
pain.
© 2007 by the American Pain Society
Key words: Chronic pain, coping, cognitive-behavioral therapy, acceptance, mindfulness, assessment.

C
hronic pain can seriously affect the lives of those and the suffering experienced, particularly in those who
who have it, leading to significant suffering for seek treatment.
these people and their families and to significant The behavioral and cognitive therapies have effec-
costs to our communities and healthcare systems.5,17 If tively addressed the suffering and disability of many
we are to lighten the burden of chronic pain, we may with chronic pain to a certain degree.28 At the same
require a deeper understanding of the basic psychologi- time, recent theoretical developments from the broader
cal processes underlying the reduced daily functioning behavioral and cognitive therapy tradition advocate an
expansion of current approaches, including unique basic
principles of psychopathology, and a new model of hu-
man suffering.13 This model posits that a primary source
Received February 23, 2007; Revised April 12, 2007; Accepted April 24,
2007 of suffering is what is termed psychological inflexibility.
Address reprint requests to Dr. Lance M. McCracken, Pain Management Unit, This inflexibility occurs when cognitive processes such as
RNHRD, Bath, BA1 1RL, UK. E-mail: lance.mccracken@rnhrd-tr.swest.nhs.uk
learning from instruction or rules, thinking, verbal prob-
1526-5900/$32.00
© 2007 by the American Pain Society lem solving, or reasoning, alter or overwhelm the effects
doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2007.04.008 of other behavioral processes.11 For example, under the

700
ORIGINAL REPORT/McCracken and Vowles 701

influence of these cognitive processes, avoidant behav- amine the combined role of these coping strategies
ior can be rigid, and fail to change even with evidence of along side the behavior patterns entailing psychological
its clearly disabling effects; or otherwise healthy behav- flexibility.
ior can be nonpersistent, stopping whenever pain or a
worry occurs. In either case, the behavior patterns seem Methods
likely to result in significantly reduced functioning in the
long term. Participants for this study were 260 consecutive adult
Psychological inflexibility, in turn, includes a number patients seen for assessment on a specialty pain manage-
of psychological processes, which are described in ment unit, in southwest England, between November
depth elsewhere.12-14 Briefly, these entail unworkable 2004 and July 2006, and completing all of the measures
attempts to control private psychological experiences used in the study. A majority of participants were
(experiential avoidance), failures in what is loosely women, 64.6%, and white European, 98.1%. Their mean
considered attention and awareness (loss of contact age was 47.5 years (SD ⫽ 11.5) and they had completed
with the present), failures of what is held as important an average of 12.6 years of formal education (SD ⫽ 2.4).
to guide action (absence of values-based action), and Reported diagnoses included fibromyalgia, 32.8%, other
excessive or inappropriate regulation of behavior by nonspecific musculoskeletal pain conditions, 21.3%, un-
verbal/cognitive processes (cognitive fusion). Some of known, 15.6%, post lumbar surgery pain, 11.5%, or
the variables described here may look new in the field other, 18.8%. The median duration of pain was 84.0
of pain management; however, they are commonplace months (range, 8.0 –552.0). Only 8.1% of participants
in other literature (see the Discussion section of this were continuing to work full time away from home, and
paper or Hayes et al11-13 for further detail). 77.3% were receiving some type of wage replacement
As psychological inflexibility is a primary source of suf- benefit at the time of the assessment.
fering, “psychological flexibility” is proposed as the aim All patients received a standard set of self-report in-
of treatment. In turn, psychological flexibility is estab- ventories at home in preparation for their assessment
lished through core processes of acceptance, mindful- appointment. They were asked to complete them and
ness, values-based action, and cognitive defusion, bring them in to the clinic on the day of their visit. All
among others.12 As applied to chronic pain, this treat- patients provided written informed consent allowing
ment approach, derived from the model of Acceptance their data to be used in research, and the study was
and Commitment Therapy (ACT),13 is referred to as Con- approved by the appropriate local research committee.
textual Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CCBT).18 Indeed, There were a number of cases excluded beforehand and
research with chronic pain sufferers has demonstrated therefore not considered a part of the current sample of
that interrelated processes of acceptance,20,21,25,29,34 260, including 67 who either did not complete all mea-
mindfulness (McCracken LM, Gauntlett-Gilbert, Vowles K: sures, did not give consent, or whose data were lost due
The role of mindfulness in a contextual cognitive-behav- to errors in reproducing the forms or other administra-
ioral analysis of chronic pain-related suffering and disabil- tive errors.
ity. Pain 2007 [E-pub ahead of print, available at http://
www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03043959]) and Measures
values-based processes,27 are significantly related to im- A brief patient background inventory asked partici-
portant aspects of patient functioning, as the present pants to report their general personal characteristics,
model would predict. Further support for this approach work status, current pain medications, ratings of their
to chronic pain is provided by a small randomized treat- pain and pain-related distress on a 0 (none) to 10 (worse
ment trial,7 a nonrandomized wait-list controlled trial,26 imaginable) scale, number of GP, specialist, and emer-
and analyses of clinically significant change in complex gency department visits in the past 6 months related to
chronic pain sufferers.23 pain, and an estimate of time spent standing or walking
The purpose of the present study was to further inves- on an average day in the past week (uptime). The medi-
tigate the role of psychological flexibility in patients with cation information was used to calculate a summary
chronic pain. For this study, the content of a previously score for the number of different classes of medication
designed measure of pain coping, the Brief Pain Coping being taken related to pain (ie, weak opioids, strong
Inventory (BPCI),22 was expanded to include additional opioids, nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs [NSAIDS],
items related to acceptance, mindfulness, values-based tricyclic antidepressants, muscle relaxants, sedatives, an-
action, and cognitive defusion. A preliminary aim was to ticonvulsants, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors,
demonstrate adequate reliability and validity for this ex- over-the-counter analgesics), as classified by a research
panded measure. The primary aim then was to test the assistant using the British National Formulary (BNF). Sim-
prediction that psychological flexibility would positively ilarly, the GP, specialist, and emergency department vis-
correlate with measures of patients’ emotional, social, its for pain were summed to form an overall pain-related
and physical functioning and negatively correlate with medical visit score.
health care use. The original content within the BPCI
included a range of traditionally conceived pain coping Brief Pain Coping Inventory-2 (BPCI-2)
strategies, such as pacing, relaxation, and positive self- The BPCI-2 was administered as a pool of 29 items,
statements, and this allowed us the opportunity to ex- including the 18 original items of the BPCI,22 with the
702 Psychological Flexibility in Chronic Pain
intent to further develop the measure based on the cur- scores from the Sickness Impact Profile (SIP).3 As with the
rent analyses. The BPCI was designed as a brief clinical validation measures, each of these measures has demon-
instrument to assess a range of responses to pain includ- strated adequate reliability and validity and has been
ing acceptance-based (eg, “realized that pain does not used in multiple studies of patients with chronic pain.
prevent activity,” or “struggled to get control of the The total score of the PASS-20 is the sum of items rated
pain,” which is negatively keyed) and typical cognitive- 0 –5. The BC-MDI provided a total score from 16 depres-
behaviorally based (“used a relaxation strategy to re- sive symptoms, rated 1 (very mild problem) to 5 (very
duce feelings of pain,” or “changed my activity to keep severe problem). Finally, the SIP also provided 2 scores:
myself focused on something other than pain”), each Composite scores for Physical Disability and Psychosocial
assessed by individual item responses. For each item, pa- Disability. The SIP is a 136-item inventory in which pa-
tients are asked to indicate how many days in the past tients endorse problems with functioning related to
week they responded to their pain in the way described their health in 12 different domains. The physical do-
on a scale from 0 –7. Previous data demonstrate ade- main is made up of separate Ambulation, Mobility, and
quate item intercorrelations and correlations with mea- Body Care and Movement scales. The psychosocial do-
sures of acceptance of pain and patient functioning to main is made up of Social Interaction, Alertness, Emo-
support validity of the BPCI items as a measure of accep- tional Behavior, and Communication.
tance-oriented and traditional cognitive-behavioral cop-
ing responses.22 The purpose of the additional 11 items Analyses
added to form the BPCI-2 was an attempt to derive multi- The analyses for the present study proceeded in 4
item summary scores from the inventory, as the original stages. First, the adequacy of the item pool for the BPCI-2
single item format was open to criticism regarding inad- was examined with frequency, descriptive, and correla-
equate reliability and stability. The items were written by tion analyses. The purpose of these analyses was to elim-
1 of the authors (LM), in consultation with 3 other clinical inate items that provide little information or adversely
psychologists working in pain management, and de- affect scale internal consistency. Second, factor and reli-
signed to encompass psychological flexibility, including ability analyses were conducted to derive summary
acceptance, mindfulness, values-based action, and cog- scores from the final BPCI-item pool. Third, correlations
nitive defusion; or a traditional CBT approach, including between the BPCI-2 summary scores and measures of
activity pacing, exercise, relaxation, positive self-state- acceptance of pain, avoidance, mindfulness, and pain
ments, distraction, and pain coping. Deriving and vali- were calculated to test the validity of the factor-derived
dating these summary scales (psychological flexibility scales. Finally, predictive analyses, including correlation
and traditional pain management) was 1 of the purposes and multiple regression, were completed to compare the
of the present study. (Note: Further information, the relative values of differing coping scales in relation to
original item pool, or a copy of the final measure can be measures of patient functioning and test the primary
obtained from the first author.) prediction of the study. A useful description of the type
of scale development methods used here is available
Validation Measures and Measures of from Jensen.10
Patient Functioning
Three measures were used to validate the psychologi- Results
cal flexibility summary score. These included a measure
of acceptance of pain, the 20-item Chronic Pain Accep- Preliminary Item Frequency and
tance Questionnaire (CPAQ),25 a measure of avoidance, Correlation Analyses
the 5-item Avoidance subscale of the 20-item Pain Anxi- Preliminary analyses led to elimination of 10 items
ety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20),19 and a measure of mind- from the BPCI-2. Two items, “saw my reactions to pain as
fulness, the 15-item Mindful Attention Awareness Scale just reactions and not true facts,” and “practiced mind-
(MAAS).6 Each of these has demonstrated reliability, is fulness,” each showed approximately 15.0% missing
well-validated, and has been used in previous studies of data. Four additional items showed highly skewed fre-
chronic pain (McCracken LM, Gauntlett-Gilbert, Vowles quency distributions. These included the items, “used a
K: The role of mindfulness in a contextual cognitive-be- relaxation strategy to help me focus and act produc-
havioral analysis of chronic pain-related suffering and dis- tively,” “sought attention, advice, or treatment from a
ability. Pain 2007 [E-pub ahead of print, available at http:// health care provider,” and “accepted the pain and real-
www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/03043959]). The ized I did not need to change it,” which each received
CPAQ items are rated 0 (never true) to 6 (always true). extremely low endorsement rates, with less than 35.0%
The PASS items are rated on a scale from 0 (never) to 5 endorsing these at any frequency, and the item, “used
(always). The MAAS items are rated on a scale from 1 pain medication,” for which 74.2% of the sample en-
(almost always) to 6 (almost never). dorsed the highest possible rating (ie, 7 of 7 days. Finally,
Three measures were used to assess patient function- a series of inter-item and item-total correlation, and pre-
ing and adjustment to chronic pain. These included the liminary reliability analyses were conducted as a test of
total scores from the PASS-20,19,30 the British Columbia correlation between items and potential summary scales.
Major Depression Inventory (BC-MDI),14 and summary Four items did not correlate with other items or with the
ORIGINAL REPORT/McCracken and Vowles 703

proposed item clusters adequately enough to support items as well as items related to doing activities without
further analyses. These included “contacted a friend of pain being a barrier, and was labeled “Pain Acceptance.”
family member for support,” “asked for or accepted help The third factor included items related to awareness of
with a task,” “tried not to think of the pain,” and “used painful thoughts and feelings and taking goal- or values-
a sedative medication, had a drink of alcohol, or a smoke, directed action and was labeled “Awareness and Values-
to try to feel better.” Based on these analyses, 19 of 29 based Action.” The factor loadings from the rotated fac-
BPCI-2 items were retained for further study, 11 items tor solution are shown in Table 1.
from the original BPCI and 8 of the newly added items. Reliability analyses demonstrated that a scale made up
of items from Factor 1 achieved a Cronbach’s ␣ of .73.
Factor Analyses Cronbach’s ␣ for the items from Factors 2 and 3 were
A Principal Components Analyses (PCA) with orthogo- lower at .68 and .63, respectively. Both of these techni-
nal rotation was conducted for data reduction purposes cally fall below the .70-level typically regarded as a rule
and to explore potential subscales within the BPCI-2 item of thumb lower boundary for acceptable reliability.
pool. The PCA yielded 5 factors with eigen values greater Upon further analysis we also found that summary scores
than one. This included 2 small factors, and the solution for these 2 factors were significantly correlated, r ⫽ .42,
was considered uninterpretable. Examination of the P ⬍ .001. We therefore elected to combine Factors 2 and
scree plot suggested a 3-factor solution. A 3-factor solu- 3, and we labeled the combined scale “Psychological
tion also appeared highly interpretable and accounted Flexibility,” as the content of the factors together in-
for a modest 41.0% of the variance in the variable set. cludes acceptance, mindfulness, values-based action,
The first factor included a range of commonly practiced and defusion elements of this overall process, as origi-
pain management techniques and was thus labeled sim- nally hypothesized (Table 1). This combined scale
ply “Pain Management Strategies.” The second factor achieved a Cronbach’s ␣ within an adequate range, at
included several reversed avoidance and pain control .73. Hence, 2 summary scales were derived for further
analyses: “Pain Management Strategies” (8 items) and
“Psychological Flexibility” (11 items). The scales were
Table 1.Results From Principal Components correlated at r ⫽ .27, P ⬍ .001. The mean scores for the
Analyses With Orthogonal Rotation of the Pain Management Strategies and Psychological Flexibil-
Items of the Brief Pain Coping Inventory-2 ity scales were 3.32 (SD ⫽ 1.60) and 3.40 (SD ⫽ 1.35),
respectively.
FACTOR LOADINGS

ITEM CONTENT SUMMARIES 1 2 3 Validity Analyses: Correlations


with Acceptance, Avoidance,
10. Paced myself .69 ⫺.02 .09
3. Used physical exercise .61 .22 ⫺.08
Mindfulness, and Pain
14. Changed activity to focus away from pain .58 ⫺.10 .15 Correlation analyses of the pain management and psy-
1. Encouraged self or changed thinking .56 .06 .29 chological flexibility scores from the BPCI-2 with patient
24. Rested briefly then resumed activity .54 ⫺.01 .15 background characteristics showed that the pain man-
29. Tried to think positive before taking action .53 .16 .42 agement score was positively correlated with duration of
15. Used ice, heat, massage, or TENS .52 .06 ⫺.28 pain, r ⫽ .15, P ⬍ .05, and negatively correlated with
8. Used relaxation .50 ⫺.08 ⫺.01
strong opioid use, r ⫽ ⫺.15, P ⬍ .05, but uncorrelated
7. Rested most of the day* .10 .74 ⫺.03
with age or gender. The flexibility score was positively
2. Avoided a painful activity* ⫺.22 .64 .07
4. Kept doing activity without pain stopping .09 .57 .39 correlated with age, r ⫽ .22, P ⬍ .001, female gender (1 ⫽
it men, 2 ⫽ women), r ⫽ .18, P ⬍ .01, and duration of pain,
11. Realized pain does not prevent activity .41 .55 .26 r ⫽ .16, P ⬍ .01, and negatively correlated with strong
23. Used pain as a reason not to do activity* ⫺.04 .53 .19 opioid use, r ⫽ ⫺.18, P ⬍ .01. Neither score was corre-
17. Struggled to get control of pain* .00 .52 ⫺.13 lated with years of education.
22. Chose not to struggle with .05 .07 .68 Table 2 includes correlation results from the pain man-
thoughts/feelings agement and psychological flexibility scales of the BPCI-2
25. Chose to act on what I value, not pain .12 .18 .64
with the measures used to examine validity. The pain
20. Remained aware of pain in a wider .24 ⫺.03 .56
situation
management strategies score significantly correlated
27. Did what works despite thoughts/feelings .27 .39 .51 with acceptance of pain and avoidance but not with
19. Noticed pain without doing anything ⫺.12 ⫺.02 .49 mindfulness or pain intensity. The psychological flexibil-
about it ity score significantly correlated with acceptance of pain,
Percent variance 15.69 12.78 12.41 avoidance, mindfulness, and pain intensity, supporting
the validity of the scale as a measure of psychological
Abbreviation: TENS, transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation.
flexibility. Although these correlations with acceptance
NOTE. The 3 factors here were labeled “Pain Management Strategies,” “Pain
Acceptance,” and “Awareness and Values-based Action.” Factors 2 and 3 and avoidance were relatively large, suggesting nearly
were subsequently combined and labeled “Psychological Flexibility.” 40% and 24% overlapping variance, respectively, the
*These items reversed before entry into the factor analysis. correlation with mindfulness was much smaller.
704 Psychological Flexibility in Chronic Pain

Table 2.Correlations Between Summary larger. These correlations were each in the predicted di-
Scores From the Brief Pain Coping Inventory- rection.
2 With Measures of Acceptance of Pain,
Mindfulness, and Patient Functioning Multiple Regression Analyses
(N ⴝ 260) A series of 9 hierarchical multiple regression analyses
were conducted to examine the potential combined in-
SUBSCALES FROM THE BPCI-2
fluences of pain management strategies and psycholog-
ical flexibility on patient functioning after controlling for
PAIN PSYCHOLOGICAL pain and patient background variables, where relevant.
MANAGEMENT FLEXIBILITY The criterion variables included the rating of pain-re-
Validity analyses lated distress, pain-related anxiety, depression, physical
Acceptance of pain (CPAQ) .25‡ .63‡ and psychosocial disability, analgesic medication use,
Avoidance (PASS) ⫺.13* ⫺.49‡ pain-related medical visits, uptime, and work status. The
Mindfulness (MAAS)1 .17 .25* pain rating and patient background variables were
Pain intensity (0–10 rating) .04 ⫺.19† tested for entry in consecutive initial steps based on sta-
Predictive analyses tistical entry criteria (probability of F to enter ⬍.05; prob-
Pain-related distress (0–10 ⫺.08 ⫺.36‡ ability of F to remove ⬎.10). The 2 summary scores from
rating) the BPCI-2 were entered together on a single step after
Pain-related anxiety (PASS) ⫺.20† ⫺.50‡
that. The results of the regression analyses are included
Depression (BCMDI) ⫺.21‡ ⫺.46‡
in Table 3.
Physical disability (SIP) ⫺.03 ⫺.29‡
Psychosocial disability (SIP) ⫺.17† ⫺.36‡
We were able to generate 9 of 9 significant prediction
Medication (number different ⫺.05 ⫺.20† equations with regression. Pain intensity was a signifi-
analgesics) cant predictor in 8 of these equations, with the exception
Medical visits (visits related to ⫺.07 ⫺.20† of the equation for daily uptime. The variance accounted
pain last 6 mo) for by pain intensity ranged from 2.5% in the equation
Uptime (hours per day standing .10 .27‡ for psychosocial disability to 40.0% in the equation for
or walking) pain-related distress. Pain was also a relatively large pre-
Work status (0 ⫽ not working, 1 .02 .15* dictor of physical disability, accounting for 12.0% or vari-
⫽ working) ance. The patient background variables were generally
n ⫽ 111 for analyses including the MAAS.
1 small and inconsistent predictors of patient functioning
*P ⬍ .05. and were selected as significant just 3 times out of 36
†P ⬍ .01. tests. Together the BPCI-2 summary scores added a sig-
‡P ⬍ .001. nificant increment in explained variance in 8 of 9 equa-
tions, failing to significantly predict work status. Signifi-
cant variance accounted for by the BPCI-2 summary
scores ranged from 2.7% in the equation for pain-re-
Correlation Results From BPCI-2
lated medical visits to 21.0% in the equation for pain-
Summary Scores With Measures of related anxiety. The summary scores also accounted for a
Patient Functioning notable 16.0% of variance in depression. Interestingly,
Examination of the correlations between the pain the regression coefficients for psychological flexibility
management strategies scale and 9 measures of patient were significant in 8 of 9 equations, whereas the regres-
functioning in Table 2 showed a surprisingly small num- sion coefficients for pain management strategies were
ber of significant correlations, just 3, of relatively small significant in none. Psychological flexibility most fre-
magnitude, none above r ⫽ –.21. Nonetheless, the pain quently achieved the largest regression coefficient over-
management strategies score was significantly corre- all across the 9 analyses. It appeared as the strongest
lated with pain-related anxiety, depression, and psycho- predictor of pain-related anxiety, depression, psychoso-
social disability. In each case, the relationship was nega- cial disability, medication use, uptime, and medical visits,
tive suggesting that when patients report more frequent although on 4 of these occasions it was only minimally
performance of the strategies in this scale, they also re- larger than pain intensity. In each case, psychological
port less emotional distress and problems with psychos- flexibility was related to patient functioning in the pre-
ocial functioning. dicted direction: Patients who reported behavior pat-
The correlations from the psychological flexibility scale terns entailing greater psychological flexibility also re-
were generally stronger than those achieved by the pain ported better functioning.
management strategies scale. Each of the 9 correlations
between the psychological flexibility scale and the mea-
sure of pain and patient functioning were significant.
Discussion
The correlations with work status, medication use, and The present study examined a range of patient re-
medical visits for pain were small, while correlations with sponses to chronic pain, some designed to reflect tradi-
general pain-related distress, pain-related anxiety, de- tional pain management strategies and the others de-
pression, and psychosocial disability were relatively signed to reflect what is termed psychological flexibility.
ORIGINAL REPORT/McCracken and Vowles 705

Table 3.Hierarchical Multiple Regression background variables were controlled, psychological


Analyses of Pain Management Strategies and flexibility significantly predicted eight key aspects of pa-
Psychological Flexibility in Relation to tient functioning while pain management strategies,
Patient Functioning in Chronic Pain surprisingly, predicted none.
The findings presented here are similar to the results
STEP PREDICTOR ␤ (FINAL) ⌬R2 TOTAL R2 from numerous studies over the years demonstrating that
Pain-related distress (0–10) reliable and potent adaptive strategies are relatively diffi-
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .58‡ .40‡ cult to identify from within the class of traditionally con-
2. Pain management strategies ⫺.044 ceived pain coping strategies while “maladaptive” strat-
Psychological flexibility ⫺.23‡ .058‡ .46‡ egies appear readily identifiable, reliable, and more
Pain-related anxiety (PASS) strongly related to patient functioning (McCracken LM,
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .17** .062‡ Samuel VM: The role of avoidance, pacing, and other ac-
2. Gender ⫺.064 .024* tivity patterns in chronic pain. Pain 2007 [E-pub ahead of
3. Pain management strategies ⫺.081 print, available at http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/
Psychological flexibility ⫺.45‡ .21‡ .30‡
journal/03043959]).8,9,31,32 They are also consistent with
Depression (BC-MDI)
studies demonstrating that one element of psychological
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .21‡ .079‡
2. Gender ⫺.046 .016*
flexibility, acceptance of pain, is substantially better than
3. Pain management strategies ⫺.099 traditionally conceived coping variables at predicting the
Psychological flexibility ⫺.37‡ .16‡ .25‡ emotional, physical, and social functioning of patients
Physical disability (SIP) with chronic pain.20 The present study expands these
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .30‡ .12‡ results by including additional elements of psychological
2. Pain management strategies .072 flexibility beyond acceptance alone.
Psychological flexibility ⫺.24‡ .050‡ .17‡ It is perhaps surprising that the pain management
Psychosocial Disability (SIP) items did not perform better in the prediction analyses,
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .11 .025* given that the strategies captured by these items are
2. Gender ⫺.064 .016*
often trained in treatment programs. For example,
3. Pain management strategies ⫺.092
thinking positively, activity pacing, relaxation, and dis-
Psychological flexibility ⫺.29‡ .099‡ .14‡
Medication use (number of different analgesics being used) traction are common components of many cognitive-be-
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .16* .039† havioral treatment programs,28,33 yet the present results
2. Pain management strategies .00 suggest they are associated with little benefit, at least
Psychological flexibility ⫺.18† .031* .070‡ when assessed in treatment naïve patients. It may be that
Medical visits (pain-related GP, specialist, or emergency visits last 6 these strategies improve with treatment, such as
months) through changes in the situations where, or the skillful-
1. Pain intensity (0–10) .14* .029† ness with which, they are done. On the other hand, it
2. Pain management strategies ⫺.018 may be that, although these behaviors look like they
Psychological flexibility ⫺.16* .027* .056†
should help, they may actually result in less benefit, or
Uptime (hours per day)
more restriction of functioning, than previously thought.
1. Pain management strategies .014
Psychological flexibility .26‡ .071‡ .071‡
Clearly, further investigation is required before firm con-
Work status (0 ⫽ not working, 1 ⫽ working) clusions can be drawn.
1. Pain intensity (0–10) ⫺.17* .035† The theoretical approach employed here is based on
2. Pain management strategies .00 the model underlying ACT.13 As indicated earlier, we call
Psychological flexibility .095 .010 .045* our application of this model to chronic pain CCBT.18 ACT
includes 6 core therapeutic processes: Acceptance, being
NOTE: In each of these regression analyses age, gender (1⫽ men, 2 ⫽
present (eg, an aspect of mindfulness), values, cognitive
women), years of education, duration of pain, and pain intensity were tested
for entry based on statistical criteria (probability of F to enter ⬍.05; probability defusion, committed action, and self as context.12 Our
of F to remove ⬎.10), then the 2 summary scores from the BPCI-2 were particular research and clinical developments have fo-
entered simultaneously after that. cused on 4 of these: Acceptance (see McCracken and
*P ⬍ .05. Vowles for a review),24 mindfulness (McCracken LM,
†P ⬍ .01. Gauntlett-Gilbert, Vowles K: The role of mindfulness in a
‡P ⬍ .001.
contextual cognitive-behavioral analysis of chronic pain-
related suffering and disability. Pain 2007 [E-pub ahead of
print, available at http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/
In correlation analyses, both pain management strate- journal/03043959]) values,27 and cognitive defusion (in
gies and psychological flexibility scores were associated clinical developments only). These processes focus on re-
with patients’ emotional functioning and psychosocial ducing the ways that language and cognitive influences
disability. Psychological flexibility was associated with interact with direct nonverbal contingencies and create
additional aspects of physical functioning, healthcare behavior patterns that do not continue or do not change
use, and work status, and overall achieved the larger as purposes of important long term goals would re-
correlations of the 2 predictors. Based on regression quire.12 When an individual’s actions are caught up with
analyses in which pain intensity and relevant patient processes of language and cognition and fail to achieve
706 Psychological Flexibility in Chronic Pain

important goals, they demonstrate psychological inflex- The existing literature on acceptance of chronic pain is
ibility. promising and relatively well established, with results
According to the model presented here psychological coming from both our Unit and elsewhere.7,24,29,35,36
flexibility occurs when behavior patterns demonstrate pro- Mindfulness, values-related processes, and cognitive de-
cesses of (a) contact with painful psychological experiences fusion, on the other hand, have not been extensively
without being restricted by, or attempting to control, them studied. Each of these processes deserves more study in
(acceptance), (b) contact with the present moment in a way the context of chronic pain.
that is not solely based in language and thought (mindful- The present findings are limited. First, our analysis was
ness), (c) influence from important long-term goals and cross-sectional in design and statistical methods were
values (values), and (d) freedom from unhelpful response- based on correlational analyses. Thus, inferences regard-
narrowing influences of thoughts or beliefs (cognitive de- ing causality are not possible. In addition, although the
fusion). Treatment approaches employing the processes of items of the BPCI-2 were designed to measure behavior
this model have demonstrated good results in randomized patterns, it should be noted that they rely on self-reports
trials for depression,38 worksite stress,4 psychosis,1 and of these behavior patterns. As with any results based on
other conditions.12 self-report, the relations observed in the current data
Most of the work in chronic pain from a contextual must be interpreted with care, as outside factors can
model has focused on acceptance. Acceptance includes influence patient reporting. We suggest further instru-
willingness to have the experience of pain without at-
ment development efforts for the processes of psycho-
tempts to control or avoid it. The other processes from
logical flexibility presented here. Finally, these results
the model are perhaps worth clarifying, at least briefly.
are preliminary and require further exploration to estab-
Mindfulness is a way of directing attention to remain in
lish whether they are replicable and generalizable to
contact with each present moment, in a way that is ac-
other settings and other individuals suffering from pain.
cepting, and free from the influences of interpretations
In conclusion, a measure of psychological flexibility in-
and judgments.2,16 Values clarification and values-based
cluding four processes of acceptance, mindfulness, val-
action are designed to produce behavior that is guided
ues, and cognitive defusion, demonstrated significant
by desired long terms purposes, explicitly chosen by the
positive relations with important aspects of functioning
patient, rather than purposes of not feeling pain or from
for patients with chronic pain. These results support a
social pressures, for example.13,37 And finally, cognitive
defusion concerns ways in which thoughts have their need for additional research and further development of
impact on behavior, ways that are modifiable on the a contextual cognitive-behavioral model of treatment
level of context (the psychological situation that gives for chronic pain.
them their impact), separate from the level of their con-
tent (their form or frequency).13 Defusion includes the
“delinking” or “unblending” of cognitive content from the
Acknowledgment
events it describes and a reduction in restrictive or re- Thanks to Dr. Jeremy Gauntlett-Gilbert for reading and
sponse-narrowing influences on the person’s responses. commenting on the final draft of this paper.

References 7. Dahl J, Wilson KG, Nilsson A: Acceptance and Commit-


ment Therapy and the treatment of persons at risk for long-
term disability resulting from stress and pain symptoms: A
1. Bach PB, Hayes SC: The use of Acceptance and Commit- preliminary randomized trial. Behav Ther 35: 785-802, 2004
ment Therapy to prevent the rehospitalization of psychotic
patients: A randomized controlled trail. J Consult Clin Psy- 8. Ersek M, Turner JA, Kemp CA: Use of the Chronic Pain
chol 70:1129-1139, 2002 Coping Inventory to assess older adults’ pain coping strate-
gies. J Pain 7: 833-842, 2006
2. Baer RA: Mindfulness-Based Treatment Approaches. Lon-
don, UK, Academic Press, 2006 9. Geisser ME, Robinson ME, Riley JL III: Pain beliefs, coping,
and adjustment to chronic pain: Let’s focus more on the
3. Bergner M, Bobbitt RA, Carter WB, Gilson BS: The Sick- negative. Pain Forum 8: 161-168, 1999
ness Impact Profile: Development and final revision of a
health status measure. Med Care 29:787-805, 1981 10. Jensen MP. Questionnaire validation: A brief guide for
readers of the research literature. Clin J Pain 19:345-352,
4. Bond FW, Bunce D: Mediators of change in emotion-fo- 2003
cused and problem focused worksite stress management in-
terventions. J Occup Health Psychol 5:156-163, 2000 11. Hayes SC, Barnes-Holmes D, Roche B: Relational Frame
Theory: A Post Skinnerian Account of Human Language and
5. Breivik H, Collett B, Ventafridda V, Cohen R, Gallacher D: Cognition. New York, NY, Kluwer Academic, 2001
Survey of chronic pain in Europe: Prevalence, impact on
daily life, and treatment. Eur J Pain 10:287-333, 2006 12. Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, Lillis J: Accep-
tance and Commitment Therapy: Model, process and out-
6. Brown KW, Ryan RM: The benefits of being present: come. Behav Res Ther 44:1-25, 2006
mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers
Soc Psychol 84:822-848, 2003 13. Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG: Acceptance and Com-
ORIGINAL REPORT/McCracken and Vowles 707

mitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior tual cognitive-behavioral approach to chronic pain. Pain
Change. New York, NY, The Guilford Press, 1999 123:137-45, 2006
14. Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Strosahl K, Gifford EV, Follette VM: 28. Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A: Systematic review and
Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A func- meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials of cognitive
tional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. J behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding
Consult Clin Psychol 64:1152-1168, 1996 headache. Pain 80:1-13, 1999
15. Iverson GL, Remick R: Diagnostic accuracy of the British 29. Nicholas MK, Asghari A: Investigating acceptance in ad-
Columbia Major Depression Inventory. Psychol Rep 95:1241- justment to chronic pain: Is acceptance broader than we
1247, 2004 thought. Pain 124:269-279, 2006
16. Kabat-Zinn J: An outpatient program in behavioral 30. Roelofs J, McCracken LM, Peters ML, Crombez G, van
medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of Breukelen G, Vlaeyen JWS: Psychometric evaluation of the
mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS) in chronic pain patients.
preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiat 4: 33-47, 1982 J Behav Med 27:167-183, 2004
17. Maniadakis N, Gray A: The economic burden of back 31. Snow-Turek AL, Norris MP, Tan G: Active and passive
pain in the UK. Pain 84:95-103, 2000 coping strategies in chronic pain patients. Pain 64: 455-462,
18. McCracken LM: Contextual Cognitive-Behavioral Ther- 1996
apy for Chronic Pain. Seattle, WA, IASP Press, 2005 32. Tan G, Jensen MP, Robinson-Whelen S, Thornby JI,
19. McCracken LM, Dhingra L: A short version of the Pain Monga TN: Coping with chronic pain: A comparison of two
Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS-20): Preliminary develop- instruments. Pain 90:127-133, 2001
ment and validity. Pain Res Manag 7:45-50, 2002
33. Turk DC. A cognitive-behavioral perspective on treat-
20. McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Coping or acceptance: What ment of chronic pain patients, in Turk DC, Gatchel RJ, (ed):
to do with chronic pain? Pain 105:197-204, 2003 Psychological Approaches to Pain Management: A Practitio-
ner’s Handbook, 2nd ed. New York, NY, The Guilford Press,
21. McCracken LM, Eccleston C: A prospective study of ac- 2002, pp 138-158
ceptance of pain and patient functioning with chronic pain.
Pain 118:164-169, 2005 34. Viane I, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Poppe C, Devulder J,
Van Houdenhove B, De Corte W: Acceptance of pain is an
22. McCracken LM, Eccleston C, Bell L: Clinical assessment of independent predictor of mental well-being in patients
behavioral coping responses: Preliminary results from a brief with chronic pain: empirical evidence and reappraisal. Pain
inventory. Eur J Pain 9:69-78, 2005 106:65-72, 2003
23. McCracken LM, MacKichan F, Eccleston C: Contextual 35. Wicksell RK, Dahl J, Olsson GL: Using Acceptance and
cognitive-behavioral therapy for severely disabled chronic Commitment Therapy in the rehabilitation of an adolescent
pain sufferers: Effectiveness and clinically significant female with chronic pain: A case example. Cognit Behav
change. Eur J Pain 11:314-322, 2007 Pract 12:415-423, 2005
24. McCracken LM, Vowles KE: Acceptance of chronic pain.
36. Wicksell RK, Melin L, Olsson GL: Exposure and accep-
Cur Pain Headache Rep 10: 90-94, 2006
tance in the rehabilitation of adolescents with idiopathic
25. McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C: Acceptance of chronic pain: A pilot study. Eur J Pain 11;267-274, 2007
chronic pain: Component analysis and a revised assessment
37. Wilson KG, Murrell AR: Values work in Acceptance and
method. Pain 107:159-166, 2004
Commitment Therapy: Setting a course for behavioral treat-
26. McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C: Acceptance- ment, in Hayes SC, Follette VM, Linehan MM. Mindfulness
based treatment for persons with complex, long standing and Acceptance: Expanding the Cognitive-Behavioral Tradi-
chronic pain: A preliminary analysis of treatment outcome in tion. New York, NY, The Guilford Press, 2004, pp 120-151
comparison to a waiting phase. Behav Res Ther 43:1335-
1346, 2005 38. Zettle RD, Rains JC: Group cognitive and contextual
therapies in treatment of depression. J Clin Psychol 5:436-
27. McCracken LM, Yang S-Y: The role of values in a contex- 445, 1989

You might also like