You are on page 1of 10

Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Feeling safe but appearing anxious: Differential effects of alcohol on


anxiety and social performance in individuals with social anxiety
disorder
Stephan Stevens, PhD a, *, Ruth Cooper a, Trisha Bantin b, Christiane Hermann b,
Alexander L. Gerlach a
a
University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
b
Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Social anxiety disorder (SAD) and alcohol use disorders (AUD) co-occur frequently and there is pre-
Received 29 August 2016 liminary evidence that alcohol might reduce social anxiety. It is, however, unclear which mechanisms
Received in revised form contribute to the anxiety reducing effect, particularly regarding key aspects of social anxiety such as
7 March 2017
deficits in social performance. We compared self-rated and physiological measures of anxiety as well as
Accepted 17 April 2017
Available online 18 April 2017
self- and observer-rated social performance in a sample of 62 individuals with SAD and 60 nonanxious
control participants during a speech task after receiving either alcohol, an alcohol-free placebo drink or
orange juice. SAD patients reported more anxiety during the speech task than did control participants.
Keywords:
Social anxiety disorder
Furthermore, SAD patients underestimated their performance in comparison to observer ratings. Alcohol
Comorbidity reduced self-report anxiety only in SAD patients, while observers rated all participants as less competent
Social performance when intoxicated. Although individuals with SAD experience a reduction in anxiety when drinking
Alcohol consumption alcohol, simultaneous decreases in social performance might contribute to negative reactions from
others and consequently increase the risk of further alcohol use to cope with these negative reactions.
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction unanimously confirmed that they had previously used alcohol to


cope with anticipatory anxiety regarding social situations (Randall,
Social anxiety disorder (SAD) and alcohol use disorders (AUD) 2000). Social anxiety among students undergoing a brief alcohol
are highly comorbid (Davidson, Hughes, George, & Blazer, 1993; intervention has been related to poorer outcomes (Terlecki,
Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005). Moreover, both subclini- Buckner, Larimer, & Copeland, 2011), which highlights the need
cal (Crum & Pratt, 2001) and clinical (Kushner, Sher, & Beitman, to understand anxiety-specific consequences of present alcohol
1990) social anxiety often precedes the onset of pathological use.
drinking patterns. This heightened comorbidity is not surprising, Such findings are often conceptualized within the framework of
given that alcohol is often used as a coping mechanism for social general theories of alcohol's stress-dampening effects, for example
fears: more than half of individuals with SAD report to “often or the “self-medication hypothesis” (Chutuape & Dewit, 1995;
always” use alcohol to attenuate symptoms of SAD (Buckner & Quitkin, Rifkin, Kaplan, & Klein, 1972), the “attention allocation
Heimberg, 2010; Thomas, Randall, & Carrigan, 2003). Similarly, model” (Josephs & Steele, 1990), or the “appraisal disruption
the motive to drink alcohol to cope with anxiety in social situations model” (Sayette, 1993). Experimentally, the effects of alcohol on
is higher in socially anxious individuals compared to non-anxious social anxiety were previously assessed in speech tasks, yielding
controls (Cludius, Stevens, Bantin, Gerlach, & Hermann, 2013). In- mixed results regarding the anxiolytic effect of alcohol: While some
dividuals with SAD and comorbid alcohol use disorder studies found an anxiolytic effect (Abrams, Kushner, Medina, &
Voight, 2001; Himle et al., 1999; Kidorf & Lang, 1999), others
failed (Himle et al., 1999; Keane & Lisman, 1980; Naftolowitz,
* Corresponding author. Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Co- Vaughn, Ranc, & Tancer, 1994) or underlined the role of alcohol
logne, Pohligstraße 1, 50969 Ko€ln Germany. expectancies and placebo effects (Abrams & Kushner, 2004).
E-mail address: stephan.stevens@uni-koeln.de (S. Stevens).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.04.008
0005-7967/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
10 S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

More explicit social anxiety-relevant models were only recently revealed a complex pattern of results (see Battista, Stewart, & Ham,
established and empirical support for them is still being developed 2010), we again examined the influence of alcohol in a common
(Buckner, Heimberg, Ecker, & Vinci, 2013). These models suggest social speech task. Although the influence of alcohol on self-
that when explaining the complex relationship between social awareness in individuals with SAD has been shown, evidence for
anxiety and alcohol use disorders, not only the level of negative its effects on social performance is quite rare. Based on the social
affect due to social anxiety is relevant, but also other pivotal anxiety literature, we predicted that sober individuals (i.e. persons
components such as low positive affect, fear of scrutiny, and social in the orange juice condition) with SAD would judge their social
avoidance. In line with the idea that specific components of social performance as worse compared to controls and blind raters (e.g.,
anxiety contribute to the risk of alcohol related problems is the Voncken & Bogels, 2008). If anxiety inhibits performance, de-
“avoidance coping” model (Bacon & Ham, 2010), which states that creases in anxiety in individuals with SAD in the alcohol group may
the reduction of attentional biases through alcohol use increases lead to increases in self-rated and perhaps even observer rated
the risk for alcohol dependency in individuals with social anxiety. social performance. While remembering that the relation between
Indeed, this is underscored by the fact that alcohol reduces atten- social anxiety and alcohol is complex and additionally based on
tional bias toward threat as measured with attentional probe tasks expectancy effects, drinking motives and attentional processes
(Gerlach, Schiller, Wild, & Rist, 2006; Stevens, Rist, & Gerlach, (Battista et al., 2010), we nonetheless hypothesize that social per-
2009). To summarize, the effects of alcohol on state social anxiety formance would increase in individuals with SAD after ingesting
in individuals with SAD are much more complex than a simple alcohol. Finally, we expected that participants with SAD receiving
direct relation (more alcohol e less anxiety) and explicitly should placebo would benefit less as compared to those receiving alcohol,
consider cognitive processes like expectancy effects, attentional as a pharmacological effect on anxiety should be more profound
biases, drinking motives and comorbid depression. than a pure expectancy based effect.
As suggested by the above-mentioned models, there is potential
value in evaluating the components of social anxiety that may be 2. Method
influenced by the use of alcohol and which may contribute to the
etiology and/or maintenance of SAD. In addition to biased cognitive 2.1. Participants
processing of social situations, self-focused attention in social sit-
uations, self-awareness and safety behaviors, cognitive models of Participants suffering from social anxiety were invited to the
SAD (i.e. Clark & Wells, 1995) emphasize the role of social perfor- experiment through newspaper ads, flyers, and emails (sent to all
mance deficits in the maintenance of social anxiety. Whereas pa- students of the University of Giessen) that specifically targeted
tients usually possess adequate skills to perform in social situations, individuals who feel insecure in social situations. Participants in the
their high level of state anxiety in feared social situations, or actual SAD group could choose between monetary compensation (20 V
physiological arousal, often interferes with situational demands for the OJ condition, 30 V for the ALC and PLA conditions) or a 2-
(anxiety inhibition) and may lead to a significant decrease in social hour counseling session concerning their social fears and treat-
performance (Hofmann, Gerlach, Wender, & Roth, 1997). Support- ment options. Participants were included if they fulfilled the DSM-
ing this notion, individuals with SAD rate themselves as performing IV criteria for a primary SAD diagnosis. The diagnosis of a substance
worse compared to healthy controls, expect to be rated more use (including alcohol use disorders), bipolar, or psychotic disorder
negatively by others and observers rate the performance of in- resulted in exclusion from the study. Further general exclusion
dividuals with SAD as worse compared to non-anxious controls criteria included suffering from coronary heart diseases; undergo-
(Baker & Edelmann, 2002; Norton & Hope, 2001; Stevens et al., ing current psychiatric, neurological, or psychological treatment;
2010; Voncken & Bogels, 2008). Furthermore, individuals with and the current intake of drugs affecting the central nervous system
SAD even underestimate their actual performance when compared or the cardiovascular system. Due to possible alcohol intake,
to observer performance ratings in speech (Rapee & Lim, 1992) and pregnant or breast-feeding female participants were excluded. In
interaction situations (Stopa & Clark, 1993). Against this back- addition, participants who were alcohol naïve (i. e., reported to
ground it is not surprising that the probability of negative reactions have never consumed alcohol) or reported a history of were
from others in social situations is increased in individuals with SAD excluded. To recruit control participants (CONT), flyers and emails
(Alden & Taylor, 2004). In a study by Keane and Lisman (1980), the were sent out that targeted people who do not feel anxious in social
authors investigated the effects of alcohol and alcohol expectancy situations. Control participants were compensated monetarily as
on social anxiety in males. They did not find an anxiolytic effect of explained above. In addition to the general exclusion criteria, spe-
alcohol or alcohol expectancy on self-reported anxiety. On the cific exclusion criteria for control subjects consisted of a diagnosis
contrary, alcohol had detrimental effects on social performance. of any current mental disorder.
However, in this landmark study social skills were only assessed Initially, we screened 99 individuals for the SAD and 78 in-
regarding verbal responses (i.e. total speaking time and how often a dividuals for the control group. Sixty-two SAD participants and 60
test subject asked a question). Self-ratings of social performance healthy control participants (CONT) participants were included. 19
were not measured. Against this background, the aim of the current individuals with SAD were excluded as they failed to fulfill DSM-IV
study is to explicitly disentangle the effects of alcohol on social criteria for SAD, 10 refused to drink alcohol during the experiment
anxiety and social performance to better understand the height- and 10 had additional diagnoses (seven substance use, two bipolar
ened comorbidity between the two disorders. disorder and one somatic liver disease). In the control group, 10
In summary, it is not well-known if and how alcohol influences participants fulfilled criteria for a DSM-IV diagnosis, six refused to
social performance, and how a possible core feature of social anx- drink alcohol during the experiments and 3 moved after the
iety, i.e. decreased social performance, is influenced by alcohol completion of the initial screening. Detailed sociodemographic
intake in both healthy controls and socially anxious individuals. information is presented in Table 1.
Thus, the aims of the present study were to explore the acute effects
of alcohol during a speech task on social performance in individuals 2.2. Measures
with SAD and non-anxious control participants. We additionally
assessed self-reported anxiety and physiological arousal. 2.2.1. Demographics questionnaire
As research on a state social anxiety reducing effect of alcohol Participants were asked to self-report age, gender, education,
S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18 11

Table 1
Sociodemographic and questionnaire data of the sample.

Individuals with SAD Controls p

Gender Male: 21 Male: 20 n.s.


Female: 41 Female: 40
Education University: 16 University: 20 n.s.
High school: 41 High school: 36
Lower education: 5 Lower education: 4
Marital status Single: 39 Single: 30 n.s.
Relationship: 13 Relationship: 18
Married: 8 Married: 9
Divorced: 2 Divorced: 3
Comorbid Diagnoses MDE: 40% n.a.
Other Anxiety: 33%
Somatoform: 25%
OCD: 1.6%
Eating Disorder: 1.6%
Total: 66%

M (SD) M (SD)

Age 30.5 (11.8) 30.5 (11.4) p ¼ 0.46


BDI 12.1 (7.0) 2.4 (2.7) p < 0.001
SPS 36.8 (11.3) 5.1 (4.8) p < 0.001
SIAS 50.3 (10.8) 10.2 (5.9) p < 0.001
LSAS-F 38.8 (12.1) 10.9 (8.0) p < 0.001
LSAS-A 32.5 (11.8) 10.0 (8.8) p < 0.001
AUDIT 5.9 (3.7) 4.5 (3.0) p < 0.05
DDSAQ 29.2 (20.1) 6.1 (7.3) p < 0.001

Note: SAD ¼ Social Anxiety Disorder; MDE: Major Depressive Episode (current and past); OCD: Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. BDI ¼ Beck Depression Inventory; SPS ¼ Social
Phobia Scale; SIAS ¼ Social Interaction Anxiety Scale; LSAS-F ¼ Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale e Fear Subscale; LSAS-A ¼ Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale e Avoidance Subscale;
AUDIT ¼ Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; DDSAQ ¼ Drinking Due to Social Anxiety Questionnaire.

and marital status on a standard demographic questionnaire. Stangier & Heidenreich, 2003) assesses fear (LSAS-F) and avoidance
(LSAS-A) of 11 common social performance and interaction situa-
2.2.2. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) tions. The LSAS was originally developed as a clinician-
As depression frequently co-occurs with SAD, we added a administered scale, but Fresco et al. (2001) found high compara-
measure of depressive symptoms to our assessment battery. The bility between the self-report and the clinician-administered
BDI (Beck, Erbaugh, Ward, Mock, & Mendelsohn, 1961; German version. Stangier and Heidenreich (2003) reported adequate over-
version:; Hautzinger, Bailer, Worall, & Keller, 1994) is a 21-item all internal consistency for the German version (a ¼ 0.91) and good
measure of depression symptoms. The German version reliably support for convergent validity, as well as a two-factor structure
assesses depressive symptoms (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.92; Hautzinger with acceptable internal consistencies for both factors (fear:
et al., 1994). Additionally, it evidences favorable convergent val- a ¼ 0.82; avoidance: a ¼ 0.80). In the current sample, Cronbach's as
idity with other measures of depression (0.72 - 0.89, Hautzinger were 0.96 for the fear subscale and 0.94 for the avoidance subscale.
et al., 1994). In the current sample, it evidenced good internal
consistency (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.90). Retest-reliability has been 2.2.5. Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)
investigated only for the English version, showing satisfactory to The AUDIT was included as a measure to assess problematic
good stability for a retest interval of 1e2 weeks (r ¼ 0.74 - 0.96). drinking. Given the focus of this study, the propensity for prob-
lematic alcohol use is most relevant. The AUDIT (Saunders, Aasland,
2.2.3. Social Phobia Scale (SPS) and Social Interaction Anxiety Scale Babor, Delafuente, & Grant, 1993; German version: Rist, Glockner-
(SIAS) Rist, & Demmel, 2009) was developed as a method of screening
To assess interaction as well as performance anxiety as pivotal for excessive drinking behavior. It consists of 10 items and the
aspects of SAD, two frequently used self-report measures of social German version has an acceptable internal consistency (a ¼ 0.77;
anxiety were included in the study. The SPS and SIAS (Mattick & Rist et al., 2009) and a good retest-reliability (r ¼ 0.95 across 31
Clarke, 1998; German version:; Stangier, Heidenreich, Berardi, days; Dybek et al., 2006) and convergent validity with other mea-
Golbs, & Hoyer, 1999) measure fear in performance situations and sures of drinking behavior and biological markers of alcoholism
fear in interactional social situations, respectively. Both scales (Allen, Litten, Fertig, & Babor, 1997). In the current sample, Cron-
consist of 20 items. The German versions of the scales evidence bach's a was 0.80.
acceptable internal consistency (SPS: a ¼ 0.82; SIAS: a ¼ 0.77;
Stangier et al., 1999), good retest reliability among 3 weeks (SPS: 2.2.6. Drinking Due to Social Anxiety Questionnaire (DDSAQ)
r ¼ 0.96; SIAS: r ¼ 0.93; Stangier et al., 1999) and adequate As drinking motives have been previously shown to highly vary
construct validity (Eidecker, Glockner-Rist, & Gerlach, 2010). In the between individuals with SAD, we decided to additionally include a
current sample, Cronbach's as were 0.95 for the SPS scale and 0.96 measure that directly assesses drinking motives due to social
for the SIAS scale. anxiety. The DDSAQ (Wagner, Stangier, Heidenreich, & Schneider,
2004) is a 28-item measure assessing the motive to drink alcohol
2.2.4. Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale e self-report version (LSAS-SR) in order to relieve social fears. Although originally developed based
The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale was also specially developed on data from patients with alcohol use disorders, Stevens and
in order to assess anxiety in both performance and social interac- Gerlach (2009) reported very good internal consistency (a ¼ 0.96)
tion situations. The LSAS (Liebowitz, 1987; German version:; and good convergent validity with measures of alcohol
12 S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

expectancies, amount, and frequency of alcohol use (r ¼ 0.42 - 0.52) Two of these electrodes were active, one placed on the right
in a sample of SAD patients. In the current sample, Cronbach's a collarbone and one below the left nipple on the lowest rib. The
was 0.96. Unfortunately, no indices of retest-reliability have yet third electrode on the left collarbone grounded the electric circuit.
been reported. For the ECG analysis, three intervals of 3 min duration each were
marked according to the experimental phases of the task: In a
2.2.7. Social Performance Rating Scale (SPRS) baseline period, the participants were asked to sit quietly, refrain
The SPRS (German version: Fydrich & Bürgener, 1999; Fydrich, from moving and talking and directing their gaze to a fixation cross
Chambless, Perry, Buergener, & Beazley, 1998) is a standardized on the computer screen. During the speech task, participants were
rating system for social performance in social situations. Gaze di- instructed to concentrate on the speech, and during recovery,
rection, voice quality, length of individual talking units (mono- baseline instructions were repeated.
syllabic/overly long responses preventing discourse vs. adequate
responses encouraging discourse), discomfort (agitation and 2.5. Administration of alcohol
nervousness), and conversation flow are rated on five 5-point
Likert-type scales (1 ¼ very good; 5 ¼ very poor), with higher Participants in the ALC condition received an amount of alcohol
scores indicating poorer social performance. The total score was that was meant to induce a final BAC of 0.07%. We aimed for this
calculated as the summed score of the five items. Each participant's BAC level based on findings that individuals with this BAC are still
performance was rated by him/herself (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.86) as well able to perform experimental or behavioral tasks without signifi-
as by two observers (Cronbach's as ¼ 0.83 and 0.80) who were cant performance decrements (Battista, Macdonald, & Stewart,
blind to the participant's diagnosis and assigned condition. The 2012). The necessary amount of alcohol was estimated following
inter-rater reliability of the two observers' ratings was r ¼ 0.89. a modified version of the Widmark formula (Widmark, 1932). This
Observers were master level students of clinical psychology, who calculation takes into account the participant's height, weight, age,
were trained two days to use the SPRS rating system in standard- and gender. The calculated amount of 37 vol.-% vodka was equally
ized test videos to reach an interrater reliability of at least ICC ¼ 0.8 distributed between three drinks of vodka-orange juice (ratio of
in the ratings of the test videos. They were blind to the diagnoses of 1:3). The vodka-orange juice mixture is indistinguishable from a
the participants. drink consisting of pure orange juice (OJ), which helped to ensure
the credibility of the placebo condition. This procedure has been
2.2.8. Visual analogue scales (VASs) successfully used in previous investigations by our group (i.e.,
Participants were asked to repeatedly indicate their level of Gerlach et al., 2006; Stevens, Gerlach, & Rist, 2008; Stevens et al.,
anxiety, tension, and nervousness (e.g., “How anxious do you feel 2009).
right now?”) on three 10 cm VASs ranging from 0 (“not at all”) to
100 (“extremely”). 2.6. Procedure (Fig. 1)

2.3. Interview measures 2.6.1. First laboratory visit


During the initial laboratory visit, participants read study in-
2.3.1. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) formation and provided informed consent. Next, they underwent a
The SCID (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1996; German SCID diagnostic assessment that lasted approximately two hours,
version:; Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz, & Zaudig, 1997) is a and were randomly assigned to the experimental conditions (ALC,
semi-structured interview based on DSM-IV criteria. Two masters- PLA, and OJ). Participants were then scheduled for the second
level clinicians administered the SCID interviews and were trained laboratory visit and were given nutrition instructions intended to
and supervised in administering the SCID by the senior author of optimize any administration of alcohol during that second visit.
the study. Interrater reliability of the DSM-IV diagnosis of SAD was Specifically, they were instructed to eat a light meal four hours
r ¼ 0.8. For the number of comorbid diagnosis, please refer to before the experiment and to refrain from eating or drinking
Table 1. caffeine after the meal. They were also asked to abstain from
alcohol 24 h prior to the second laboratory visit. In order to ensure
2.4. Physiological measures the safety of participants, they were informed that they could
choose to either arrange for a designated driver if they wanted to
2.4.1. Breath alcohol concentration (BAC) leave the laboratory immediately after the upcoming visit, or wait
In order to ensure the effectiveness of the alcohol manipulation, in the laboratory until their BAC was equal to or lower than 0.02%.
all participants were repeatedly measured with a breathalyzer
(Dra€ger Alcotest 7410, Dra €ger Sicherheitstechnik GmbH, Lübeck, 2.6.2. Second laboratory visit
Germany), which is checked and calibrated annually by the At the beginning of the second laboratory visit (i.e., the exper-
manufacturer. Participants in the ALC condition were tested for a imental session), female participants in the PLA and ALC conditions
targeted breath alcohol concentration of 0.07%. Participants in the were asked to conduct a routine self-administered urine pregnancy
PLA condition were “assessed” with a device that looked identical test (HCG Ultra Strip, Ascimed Germany), but no women were
to the valid breathalyzer but falsely indicated an elevated BAC of. excluded because of a positive result. All participants were then
0.04% during the first, of 0.5% (indicating a rising blood alcohol weighed in order to calculate the amount of alcohol needed for
level) during the second assessment. Before each breath alcohol reaching the appropriate BAC level or the amount of orange juice
test, both ALC and PLA participants were additionally asked to take needed for those in the PLA and OJ conditions. To ensure that all
a guess regarding their current BAC, which was recorded by the participants did not drink any alcohol before the administration of
experimenter (“BAC-self”). drinks, the first BAC estimation (BAC-self) and actual measurement
(BAC-actual) were conducted. The ECG electrodes were then
2.4.2. Electrocardiogram (ECG) attached and a 2-min pre-drink physiological baseline assessment
Heart rate (HR) was continuously recorded using the Varioport was initiated. To assess for baseline anxiety, participants completed
system (Becker Meditec, Karlsruhe). The ECG was recorded with a VASs.
sample rate of 512 Hz using three electrodes attached to the chest. Next, participants underwent the administration of alcohol.
S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18 13

Participants in the ALC condition were told that they would be (r ¼ 0.89) to be combined into a mean observer SPRS score
drinking alcohol, and were given three separate drinks containing a (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.82). Consequently, one self-rated and one
vodka-orange juice mixture. They were given the three drinks observer-derived SPRS score were entered into the analyses. Finally,
consecutively and asked to finish each one within five minutes. for the analyses regarding biased self-perceptions in individuals
Participants in the PLA condition were also told that they would be with SAD, we calculated a performance rating difference score
drinking alcohol, and were also given three separate drinks. These (self-rating e observer-rating).
drinks were served in glasses that had been rubbed with a minimal
amount of vodka before the orange juice was poured in, and then 3.2. Preliminary analyses
one drop of vodka was added; these measures were taken to create
the smell of alcohol and thus increase the credibility of the placebo. Sample characteristics for the SAD and control groups, including
They were given the three drinks consecutively and asked to finish sociodemographic variables and scores on self-report measures are
each one within five minutes. Participants in the OJ condition were reported in Table 1. We ran a series of t-tests and c2 tests to
told that their drinks contained only orange juice, and were also examine whether groups differed on age, gender, education and
given the three drinks consecutively and asked to finish each marital status. Results indicated that groups did not differ in any
within five minutes. sociodemographic variable. We also ran a one-way (Group: SAD,
After the drinking phase, which lasted a total of fifteen minutes, control) MANOVA on questionnaire scores. SAD participants scored
participants sat quietly for another five minutes to allow for the significantly higher than control participants on all self-report
absorption of alcohol. Next, ALC and PLA participants rinsed their scales (see Table 1).
mouths and the second BAC-self and BAC-actual assessment was All the following analyses were also run including gender.
conducted. For the PLA group, a manipulated BAC tester was used However, since gender was not significant in any of these analyses
that first falsely indicated an elevated BAC of 0.04%. This BAC was and gender was not the focus of the present study, we decided to
selected because on the one hand, it indicates a relevant BAC, but on not include the respective analyses here.
the other still is believable for participants in a placebo condition.
Participants were then given instructions concerning the up- 3.3. Manipulation checks
coming speech that highlighted the evaluative characteristics of the
task (i.e., “please remember that your performance will be video- 3.3.1. Efficacy of the alcohol administration
taped and judged”). The topic was on the pros and cons of the To check whether our drinking procedure actually induced the
death penalty. Directly before beginning the speech task, partici- expected BAC in the SAD and control participants who actually
pants completed the VASs again and underwent the third BAC drank alcohol, a repeated measures ANOVA on BAC-actual with
assessment. Participants in the PLA condition were told that their Group (SAD, control) and Time (baseline, pre-drink, speech, re-
BAC was now at 0.05%, (indicating the rising arm of the breath covery) yielded a significant multivariate effect of time (F(3,
alcohol curve). During the 3-min speech task in front of a small 37) ¼ 256.8, p < 0.01, h2p ¼ 0.96). BAC levels between the two groups
audience of two individuals, the second physiological HR assess- were not significantly different (see Fig. 2; F(1,39) ¼ < 0.01, p ¼ 0.96,
ment took place, followed by the fourth BAC assessment (BAC-self h2p < 0.01). Breath alcohol levels increased from pre-to post-drink as
and BAC-actual). Here, neither participants in the PLA condition nor well as from post-drink to speech (mean differences ¼ 0.6 and 0.1,
in the ALC condition received feedback with regard to their actual respectively; ps < 0.01). Mean BAC levels were raised to 0.072%
BAC. Following this, participants were asked to sit quietly, direct (SD ¼ 0.02) for SAD patients and to 0.071% (SD ¼ 0.02) for CONT
their gaze towards a fixation cross on a computer screen in front of participants.
them to assess the HR measurement for the recovery period.
Finally, participants rated their previous performance on the SPRS, 3.3.2. Efficacy of the placebo manipulation
and the two audience members completed the SPRS scales. Next, we examined whether participants in the alcohol condi-
Upon completion of the SPRS scales, participants in the PLA tion evidenced differences in BAC-self ratings relative to partici-
group were informed of their drinks’ actual content. All participants pants in the placebo condition. A 2 (Group: SAD, control)  2
were debriefed concerning the purposes of the experiment. (Condition: alcohol, placebo)  4 (Time: pre-drink, post-drink,
speech, recovery) mixed model ANOVA on BAC-self levels indicated
3. Results a significant interaction effect Condition  Time (F(3, 81) ¼ 25.3,
p < 0.01, h2p ¼ 0.51), such that participants in the alcohol condition
3.1. Data reduction evidenced higher BAC levels during post-drink, speech and recov-
ery but not during baseline (see Fig. 2). A main effect of time
ECG raw data were visually inspected and corrected for artifacts. (F(1,3) ¼ 397.5, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.94) also indicated that, irrespective
Subsequently, an algorithm using MATLAB™ (Gerlach, Wilhelm, of group and condition, estimated BAC increased across assessment
Gruber, & Roth, 2001) calculated mean HR scores for each partici- points but did not differ between SAD and control participants.
pant and measurement period (pre-drink, post-drink, and speech) Concerning the credibility of the placebo manipulation, all partic-
by counting the number of R-spikes per minute. Detected R-spikes ipants in the PLA condition estimated their BAC to be higher than
were checked visually to ensure the validity of the algorithm. zero during the speech task. In addition, 16 of 21 SAD patients (76%)
The three anxiety-related VASs (anxiety, tension, and nervous- and 18 of 21 CONT participants (86%) reported a BAC of at least
ness) were combined into a single mean anxiety rating for each of 0.04% in the placebo group.
the three measurement points (pre-drink, speech, and recovery), as
per Andor, Gerlach, & Rist, 2008). Correlations between the single 3.3.3. Efficacy of the speech anxiety induction
VASs ranged between r ¼ 0.77 and r ¼ 0.95 and were thus suffi- Regarding the effects of our speech task, we wanted to ensure
ciently high to calculate a mean VAS score for each assessment. that it increases self-report anxiety and HR. Regarding self-report
Cronbach's a for VAS ratings were: a ¼ 0.95 (pre-drink), a ¼ 0.97 anxiety, a repeated measures ANOVA with assessment (pre-drink
(speech), and a ¼ 0.95 (recovery). vs. speech vs. recovery) as within-subjects factor and group (SAD,
Additionally, correlations between the two observers’ social control) as between-subjects factors on self-reported anxiety
competence ratings for each participant were sufficiently high revealed a significant main effect of group (F (1,116) ¼ 67.5,
14 S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

Fig. 1. Procedure.
SCID I ¼ Structured Clinical Interview for Mental Disorders 1; VAS ¼ Visual Analogue Scales Anxiety; BACeS ¼ Breath Alcohol Level-self report; BAC-O ¼ Breath Alcohol Level-
objective; SPRS ¼ Social Performance Rating Scale.

p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.37). Individuals with SAD had higher VAS scores
than controls during the assessment (see Fig. 3). A significant main
effect of time (F (2,122) ¼ 28.4, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.20) was found due
to self-reported anxiety being higher during the speech task than
during baseline (Mspeech ¼ 28.2; SDspeech ¼ 28.5; Mbaseline ¼ 18.5;
SDbaseline ¼ 19.7) and recovery across groups (Mrecovery ¼ 16.21;
SDrecovery ¼ 22.7; compare Fig. 2). For heart rate (see Fig. 4), we
found a main effect of time (F (2,121) ¼ 325.1, p < 0.01, h2p ¼ 0.77),
such that HR increased from baseline to speech (1, 122) ¼ 99.8;
p < 0.05; h2p ¼ 0.3), and decreased from speech to recovery (1,
122) ¼ 78.42; p < 0.05; h2p ¼ 0.47), irrespective of group. In addition,
a main effect of group (F (1,121) ¼ 5.1, p ¼ 0.03, h2p ¼ 0.05) indicated
that individuals with SAD had higher HR compared to controls. No
effects of condition or any interactions on HR were found.

3.4. The effects of alcohol on anxiety and social performance

All significant effects regarding our main hypotheses were fol-


lowed up hierarchically with further ANOVAs and for comparison
Fig. 2. Measured and estimated BAC.
ALC ¼ alcohol condition; PLA ¼ placebo condition; BAC-Actual ¼ measured breath for two means with planned simple contrasts.
alcohol concentration in ALC condition; BAC-self ¼ participants' estimated BAC before
each measurement/false feedback of BAC. Bars indicate standard errors.
3.4.1. Effects of alcohol on self-report anxiety and HR
We examined whether alcohol reduced anxiety as measured by
self-report (see Fig. 3) or HR (see Fig. 4) in two separate repeated
measures ANOVAs. The 2 group (SAD, control)  3 condition
S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18 15

Fig. 3. Anxiety Scale scores between groups, condition, and time.


SAD ¼ Social Anxiety Disorder group; CONT ¼ control group; OJ ¼ orange juice condition; PLA ¼ placebo condition; ALC ¼ alcohol condition. Bars indicate standard errors.

Fig. 4. Mean HR between groups, condition, and time.


SAD ¼ Social Anxiety Disorder group; CONT ¼ control group; OJ ¼ orange juice condition; PLA ¼ placebo condition; ALC ¼ alcohol condition. Bars indicate standard errors.

(control, placebo, alcohol)  3 assessment (baseline, speech, re- anxiety increased from baseline to speech (F (1, 60) ¼ 234.35;
covery) repeated measures analysis for self-report anxiety resulted p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.5) and decreased from speech to recovery (F (1,
in a significant 3 way interaction (F (4, 122) ¼ 3.2; p ¼ 0.01; 60) ¼ 199.75; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.44). Thus, no effect of condition on
h2p ¼ 0.05). In follow-up repeated measures ANOVAs separate for anxiety was found for controls (F (2, 60) ¼ 0.98; p ¼ 0.45;
groups, the analysis for controls only revealed a main effect of h2p ¼ 0.01). For individuals with SAD, we found a significant
assessment (F (2, 60) ¼ 20.0; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.41), such that assessment  condition interaction (F (2, 62) ¼ 3.8; p ¼ 0.01;
16 S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

h2p ¼ 0.01). No differences between conditions were revealed for report and physiological measures of anxiety. Alcohol reduced
baseline (F (2, 62) ¼ 1.3; p ¼ 0.3; h2p ¼ 0.07)), while during speech anxiety during a speech task in individuals with SAD. Since patients
and recovery, individuals in the verum (F (1, 62) ¼ 425.35; in the placebo group felt more anxious compared to those in the
p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.7) and placebo conditions (F (1, 60) ¼ 310.55; alcohol group, the reduction in self-report anxiety can partly be
p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.54) reported less anxiety compared to those in the attributed to the pharmacological effects of alcohol. However, SAD
control condition (see Fig. 3) and those in the verum condition participants receiving placebo also reported less anxiety compared
reported less anxiety compared to individuals with SAD in the to the control (orange juice) condition lending support to the
placebo condition (F (1, 60) ¼ 123.98; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.3). notion, that alcohol expectancy effects also may reduce social
Next, we examined whether alcohol reduced HR in the alcohol anxiety.
compared to the placebo and verum conditions initially using a 2 Individuals with SAD rated their performance worse compared
group (SAD, controls)  3 condition (control, verum, placebo)  3 to the ratings of neutral observers. Most interestingly, although
assessment (baseline, speech, recovery) repeated measures alcohol led to a decrease in self-report anxiety in individuals with
ANOVA. This analysis only revealed two main effects of group (F (1, SAD, no beneficial effects of alcohol on self-rated performance were
122) ¼ 5.2; p ¼ 0.025; h2p ¼ 0.1) and time (F (2, 122) ¼ 325.46; found. In contrast, alcohol intake had detrimental effects on
p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.77). HR increased from baseline to speech (F (1, observer rated social performance in both groups (compare Keane
122) ¼ 303.56; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.69) and decreased from speech to & Lisman, 1980; for a similar effect in socially anxious males).
recovery (F (1, 122) ¼ 228.64; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.62) irrespective of Interestingly, a detrimental effect of placebo on observer rated
condition and group (compare Fig. 4). social performance was also revealed. At this point it is unclear
what mechanism may be responsible for such an effect. Possibly,
3.4.2. Effects of alcohol on social performance measures fear of being hindered by intoxication to give the speech skillfully
To examine the effects of alcohol on social performance, we may have actually hampered performance (Cludius et al., 2013).
conducted a 2 (Group: SAD, controls)  3 (Condition: alcohol, In addition to the replication of deficits in social performance in
placebo, OJ)  2 (source of the ratings: self vs. observer) repeated SAD (Baker & Edelmann, 2002; Norton & Hope, 2001; Stevens et al.,
measurement ANOVA on social performance ratings provided by 2010; Voncken & Bogels, 2008), and anxiety reduction in in-
participants and observers with the SPRS as dependent variable. dividuals with SAD in a speech task (e.g., Abrams et al., 2001), the
This ANOVA resulted in a significant “group  rating source” key finding of the current study is two-fold: While participants
interaction (F (1,121) ¼ 25.1, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.19), such that only in suffering from SAD reported less anxiety after drinking alcohol
individuals with SAD, self-ratings of social performance were lower compared to those who drank a placebo beverage or orange juice,
compared to observer ratings (see Fig. 5; p < 0.05). In controls, their performance was judged more negatively by group- and
there was no difference between self- and observer-rated social condition-blind observers. This key finding is somewhat surprising,
performance measures (p > 0.05). Next, a significant rating as, arguably, a reduction in anxiety should lead to increases in social
source  condition interaction (F (2,121) ¼ 10.23, p < 0.001, performance, according to modern cognitive models of SAD (Clark
h2p ¼ 0.16) revealed that observer-rated performance decreased & Wells, 1995). Our results, however, suggest a rather independent
from orange juice to placebo conditions (F (1, 122) ¼ 366.42; influence of alcohol on social performance and anxiety, which
p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.7), and from placebo to alcohol conditions (1, might increase the risk for hazardous drinking in individuals with
122) ¼ 197.82; p < 0.001; h2p ¼ 0.38), irrespective of the clinical SAD. Primarily, alcohol reduces social anxiety in individuals with
status of the participants. No influence of alcohol use was found for SAD, qualifying negative reinforcement as one possible mechanism
social performance as rated by the individuals themselves. that increases the risk of further alcohol intake (Battista et al.,
2010). This anxiety reducing effect does not, however, directly
4. Discussion lead to improvements in social performance, as the physiological
consequences of alcohol intake tend to include typical behaviors
We investigated the impact of alcohol use on social performance associated with reduced social performance (e.g., having problems
in individuals with SAD and healthy controls, as well as on self- concentrating, slurring words, see also Keane & Lisman, 1980).

Fig. 5. Self- and observer-rated SPRS scales between groups and condition.
Higher scores indicate poorer performance ratings. SPRS ¼ Social Performance Rating Scale; Self ¼ self-rated SPRS; observer ¼ observer-rated SPRS; ALC ¼ alcohol condition;
PLA ¼ placebo condition; OJ ¼ orange juice condition; SAD ¼ social anxiety disorder group; CONT ¼ control group. Bars indicate standard errors.
S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18 17

Indeed, reduction of anxiety in individuals with SAD and decreases 2013). Nonetheless, in this study a speech task was chosen since
in social performance were particularly high in the alcohol this task is more easily controlled experimentally, has been studied
compared to the placebo and orange juice groups. These results most extensively, and most reliably induces social anxiety in both
suggest that individuals with SAD are explicitly susceptible to the socially anxious and healthy individuals. Also, arguably, at least
effects of alcohol with regards to its anxiety-reducing properties, pharmacological effects of alcohol on anxiety should not be
and might be particularly vulnerable to observable decreases in differentially affected by the situation in which anxiety occurs.
social performance: Social rejection in performance situations due However, future research should also focus on other common social
to worse social performance might further increase the vicious situations like small talk or eating, where alcohol use may better fit
circle of anxiety, alcohol use, decreases in social performance, and into the social situation and ambulatory assessment might be used
negative reactions from others (Alden & Taylor, 2004). to measure real life effects of alcohol on social anxiety. Finally, the
Regarding our placebo design, we found more pronounced ef- speech did not induce much anxiety in the control group, which
fects of alcohol on anxiety and social performance in the alcohol might explain why we did not find an anxiety reducing effect of
compared to the placebo and the orange juice conditions. As sug- alcohol in the control group.
gested by the recent literature on alcohol effects in SAD (Battista In summary, our study reveals a complex interaction of social
et al., 2010), a relatively large amount of alcohol is necessary to anxiety, social performance, and alcohol use. While alcohol atten-
investigate the effect of alcohol on anxiety in the laboratory (tar- uates social anxiety on a self-report level, social performance de-
geting at least a blood alcohol level of 0.06%). However, as the creases in intoxicated individuals. Negative reactions from others
placebo group in individuals with SAD consistently differed from based on these performance deficits might, in turn, increase social
the orange juice group in the current study, considerations of the anxiety, which would then contribute to the maintenance of the
psychological effects of alcohol use should not be neglected. vicious circle of anxiety and alcoholism.
Recently, alcohol expectancies have been regularly linked to the
anxiety reducing effects of alcohol (Abrams & Kushner, 2004). In Conflict of interest
particular, expectations regarding tension reduction and improve-
ment in social performance after drinking alcohol have been found The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.
to influence the anxiety reduction properties of alcohol. In addition
to alcohol expectancies, further psychological aspects such as the
motive to drink alcohol to reduce social anxiety (Cludius et al., References
2013), and the adequacy of alcohol use depending on the social
Abrams, K., & Kushner, M. G. (2004). The moderating effects of tension-reduction
situation (i.e., speech vs. interaction), should be considered when alcohol outcome expectancies on placebo responding in individuals with so-
attempting to explain placebo effects in social anxiety research. cial phobia. Addictive Behaviors, 29(6), 1221e1224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
In a clinical sense, our results point to the pivotal role of in- j.addbeh.2004.03.020.
Abrams, K., Kushner, M., Medina, K. L., & Voight, A. (2001). The pharmacologic and
terventions that address problematic alcohol use in social anxiety. expectancy effects of alcohol on social anxiety in individuals with social phobia.
On the one hand, alcohol leads to immediate anxiety reduction and Drug Alcohol Depend, 64(2), 219e231. dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(01)00125-
is comparable to other safety behaviors as it prevents learning in 9.
Alden, L. E., & Taylor, C. T. (2004). Interpersonal processes in social phobia. Clinical
social situations, i.e. that social situations are not dangerous Psychology Review, 24(7), 857e882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.07.006.
without the use of alcohol. On the other hand, while alcohol use Allen, J. P., Litten, R. Z., Fertig, J. B., & Babor, T. (1997). A review of research on the
might help to alleviate anxiety symptoms, it decreases social per- alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT). Alcoholism-clinical and
Experimental Research, 21(4), 613e619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00000374-
formance, and thus might contribute to increased social isolation of 199706000-00008.
individuals with SAD. Andor, T., Gerlach, A. L., & Rist, F. (2008). Superior perception of phasic physiological
Naturally, our study has some limitations, which have to be arousal and the detrimental consequences of the conviction to be aroused on
worrying and metacognitions in GAD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(1),
carefully considered. Individuals in the placebo condition believed
193e205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.117.1.193.
that they consumed less alcohol compared to those in the alcohol Bacon, A. K., & Ham, L. S. (2010). Attention to social threat as a vulnerability to the
condition. Ideally, both the alcohol group and the placebo group development of comorbid social anxiety disorder and alcohol use disorders: An
avoidance-coping cognitive model. Addictive Behaviors, 35(11), 925e939. http://
should not differ in this regard. However, as has been pointed out
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.002.
by Martin and Sayette (1993), this cannot be accomplished if higher Baker, S. R., & Edelmann, R. J. (2002). Is social phobia related to lack of social skills?
BAC levels (i. e., 0.07%) are used, because of the strong pharmaco- Duration of skill-related behaviours and ratings of behavioural adequacy. British
logical effect of this level of alcohol. Nonetheless, our placebo group Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 243e257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/
014466502760379118.
significantly differed in perceived BAC from the control group, Battista, S. R., Macdonald, D., & Stewart, S. H. (2012). The effects of alcohol on safety
allowing to at least partially disentangle the pharmacological vs. behaviors in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol-
expectancy effects of alcohol intake. Furthermore, due to ethical ogy, 31(10), 1074e1094. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2012.31.10.1074.
Battista, S. R., Stewart, S. H., & Ham, L. S. (2010). A critical review of laboratory-based
consideration, we excluded participants who currently or previ- studies examining the relationships of social anxiety and alcohol intake. Current
ously suffered from alcohol related disorders, as these groups may Drug Abuse Reviews, 3(1), 3e22. http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/
be particularly susceptible to the effects of alcohol in social situa- 1874473711003010003.
Beck, A. T., Erbaugh, J., Ward, C. H., Mock, J., & Mendelsohn, M. (1961). An inventory
tions. In addition, as our experimental conditions might have for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6). http://dx.doi.org/
differed regarding the stress they caused our participants, i.e. 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004, 561-&.
drinking alcohol or not, we were asked by the ethical review board Buckner, J. D., & Heimberg, R. G. (2010). Drinking behaviors in social situations
account for alcohol-related problems among socially anxious individuals. Psy-
to compensate differentially depending on the experimental group. chology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(4), 640e648. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Unfortunately, this might be a meaningful threat to the internal a0020968.
validity of our study. Similarly problematic, only female partici- Buckner, J. D., Heimberg, R. G., Ecker, A. H., & Vinci, C. (2013). A biopsychosocial
model of social anxiety and substance use. Depression and Anxiety, 30(3),
pants in the ALC and PLA condition were given a urine pregnancy
276e284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/da.22032.
test. Also, we used a speech task as social stress task. Individuals Chutuape, M. A. D., & Dewit, H. (1995). Preferences for ethanol and diazepam in
with SAD may, however, use alcohol more often in social interac- anxious individuals - an evaluation of the self-medication hypothesis. Psycho-
tion situations because in social interactions impairment of per- pharmacology, 121(1), 91e103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/Bf02245595.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg
formance may be of less concern and positive effects of alcohol may (Ed.), Social phobia - diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 69e93). New
be more salient (e.g. higher social assertiveness; Cludius et al., York: The Guilford Press.
18 S. Stevens et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 94 (2017) 9e18

Cludius, B., Stevens, S., Bantin, T., Gerlach, A. L., & Hermann, C. (2013). The motive to and Therapy, 36(4), 455e470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-
drink due to social anxiety and its relation to hazardous alcohol use. Psychology 6.
of Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), 806e813. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032295. Naftolowitz, D. F., Vaughn, B. V., Ranc, J., & Tancer, M. E. (1994). Response to alcohol
Crum, R. M., & Pratt, L. A. (2001). Risk of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in in social phobia. Anxiety, 1(2), 96e99.
social phobia: A prospective analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(10), Norton, P. J., & Hope, D. A. (2001). Kernels of truth or distorted perceptions: Self and
1693e1700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1693. observer ratings of social anxiety and performance. Behavior Therapy, 32(4),
Davidson, J. R. T., Hughes, D. L., George, L. K., & Blazer, D. G. (1993). The epidemi- 765e786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(01)80020-4.
ology of social phobia - findings from the duke epidemiologic catchment-area Quitkin, F. M., Rifkin, A., Kaplan, J., & Klein, D. F. (1972). Phobic anxiety syndrome
study. Psychological Medicine, 23(3), 709e718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ complicated by drug dependence and addiction - treatable form of drug-abuse.
S0033291700025484. Archives of General Psychiatry, 27(2). http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/arch-
Dybek, I., Bischof, G., Grothues, J., Reinhardt, S., Meyer, C., Hapke, U., et al. (2006). psyc.1972.01750260013002, 159-&.
The reliability and validity of the alcohol use disorders identification test Randall, C. L. (2000). Alcoholism and social anxiety (Denver, CO). In Paper presented
(AUDIT) in a German general practice population sample. Journal of Studies on at the annual meeting of the research society on alcoholism.
Alcohol, 67(3), 473e481. Rapee, R. M., & Lim, L. (1992). Discrepancy between self and observer ratings of
Eidecker, J., Glockner-Rist, A., & Gerlach, A. L. (2010). Dimensional structure of the performance in social phobics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(4), 728e731.
Social Interaction Anxiety Scale according to the analysis of data obtained with http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.101.4.728.
a German version. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(6), 596e605. http:// Rist, F., Glockner-Rist, A., & Demmel, R. (2009). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identi-
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.020. fication Test revisited: Establishing its structure using nonlinear factor analysis
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). Structured clinical and identifying subgroups of respondents using latent class factor analysis.
interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 100(1e2), 71e82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
American Psychiatric Press, Inc. j.drugalcdep.2008.09.008.
Fresco, D. M., Coles, M. E., Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Hami, S., Stein, M. B., Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., Delafuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993).
et al. (2001). The Liebowitz social anxiety scale: A comparison of the Development of the alcohol-use disorders identification test (audit) - who
psychometric properties of self-report and clinician-administered formats. collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol-
Psychological Medicine, 31(6), 1025e1035. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ consumption .2. Addiction, 88(6), 791e804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-
S0033291701004056. 0443.1993.tb02093.x.
Fydrich, T., & Bürgener, F. (1999). Ratingskalen für soziale Kompetenz. In J. Margraf, Sayette, M. A. (1993). An appraisal-disruption model of alcohols effects on stress
& K. Rudolf (Eds.), Vol. 2. Soziale kompetenz - soziale Phobie: Anwendungsfelder, responses in social drinkers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 459e476. http://
entwicklungslinien, erfolgsaussichten (pp. 81e98). Baltmannsweiler: Schneider- dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.459.
Verlag. Stangier, U., & Heidenreich, T. (2003). Die Liebowitz soziale angst-skala (LSAS). In
Fydrich, T., Chambless, D. L., Perry, K. J., Buergener, F., & Beazley, M. B. (1998). C. I. P. Scalarum (Ed.), Internationale Skalen für Psychiatrie. Go € ttingen: Hogrefe.
Behavioral assessment of social performance: A rating system for social phobia. Stangier, U., Heidenreich, T., Berardi, A., Golbs, U., & Hoyer, J. (1999). Assessment of
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(10), 995e1010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ social phobia by the social interaction anxiety scale and the social phobia scale.
S0005-7967(98)00069-2. Zeitschrift Fur Klinische Psychologie-Forschung Und Praxis, 28(1), 28e36. http://
Gerlach, A. L., Schiller, A., Wild, C., & Rist, F. (2006). Effects of alcohol on the pro- dx.doi.org/10.1026//0084-5345.28.1.28.
cessing of social threat-related stimuli in socially phobic women. British Journal Stevens, S., & Gerlach, A. L. (2009). Self-medication and social phobia: Psychometric
of Clinical Psychology, 45, 279e295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ properties of the questionnaire “drinking due to social anxiety”. Zeitschrift Fur
014466505x49862. Klinische Psychologie Und Psychotherapie, 38(2), 118e125. http://dx.doi.org/
Gerlach, A. L., Wilhelm, F. H., Gruber, K., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Blushing and physi- 10.1026/1616-3443.38.2.118.
ological arousability in social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), Stevens, S., Gerlach, A. L., & Rist, F. (2008). Effects of alcohol on ratings of emotional
247e258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.110.2.247. facial expressions in social phobics. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(6), 940e948.
Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H., & Keller, F. (1994). Beck-depressions-inventar http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.09.007.
(BDI). Bern: Huber. Stevens, S., Hofmann, M., Kiko, S., Mall, A. K., Steil, R., Bohus, M., et al. (2010). What
Himle, J. A., Abelson, J. L., Haghightgou, H., Hill, E. M., Nesse, R. M., & Curtis, G. C. determines observer-rated social performance in individuals with social anxi-
(1999). Effect of alcohol on social phobic anxiety. American Journal of Psychiatry, ety disorder? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(8), 830e836. http://dx.doi.org/
156(8), 1237e1243. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.005.
Hofmann, S. G., Gerlach, A. L., Wender, A., & Roth, W. T. (1997). Speech disturbances Stevens, S., Rist, F., & Gerlach, A. L. (2009). Influence of alcohol on the processing of
and gaze behavior during public speaking in subtypes of social phobia. Journal emotional facial expressions in individuals with social phobia. British Journal of
of Anxiety Disorders, 11(6), 573e585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6185(97) Clinical Psychology, 48, 125e140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466508x368856.
00040-6. Stopa, L., & Clark, D. M. (1993). Cognitive-processes in social phobia. Behaviour
Josephs, R. A., & Steele, C. M. (1990). The 2 faces of alcohol myopia - attentional Research and Therapy, 31(3), 255e267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(93)
mediation of psychological stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(2), 90024-O.
115e126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.99.2.115. Terlecki, M. A., Buckner, J. D., Larimer, M. E., & Copeland, A. L. (2011). The role of
Keane, T. M., & Lisman, S. A. (1980). Alcohol and social anxiety in males - behavioral, social anxiety in a brief alcohol Intervention for heavy-drinking college stu-
cognitive, and physiological-effects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89(2), dents. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25(1), 7e21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/
213e223. 0889-8391.25.1.7.
Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, Thomas, S. E., Randall, C. L., & Carrigan, M. H. (2003). Drinking to cope in socially
and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity anxious individuals: A controlled study. Alcoholism:Clinical and Experimental
survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 617e627. http:// Research, 27(12), 1937e1943. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. 01.ALC.0000100942.30743.8C.
Kidorf, M., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Effects of social anxiety and alcohol expectancies on Voncken, M. J., & Bogels, S. M. (2008). Social performance deficits in social anxiety
stress-induced drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13(2), 134e142. disorder: Reality during conversation and biased perception during speech.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.13.2.134. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(8), 1384e1392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Kushner, M. G., Sher, K. J., & Beitman, B. D. (1990). The Relation between alcohol- j.janxdis.2008.02.001.
problems and the anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(6), Wagner, C., Stangier, U., Heidenreich, T., & Schneider, R. (2004). “Drinking due to
685e695. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.147.6.685. social anxiety” and “social anxiety due to Drinking”: Development of two scales
Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. In T. A. Ban, T. Pichot, & W. Poldinger (Eds.), for alcohol-dependency. Zeitschrift Fur Klinische Psychologie Und Psychotherapie,
Modern problems of pharmacopsychiatry (pp. 141e173). New York: Karger. 33(4), 308e315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443.33.4.308.
Martin, C. S., & Sayette, M. A. (1993). Experimental-design in alcohol administration Widmark, E. M. P. (1932). Die theoretischen Grundlagen und die praktische Ver-
research - limitations and alternatives in the manipulation of dosage-set. wendbarkeit der gerichtlich-medizinischen Alkoholbestimmung. Berlin: Urban &
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 54(6), 750e761. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/ Schwarzenberg.
jsa.1993.54.750. Wittchen, H. U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID-I -
Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV - achse I: Psychische Sto €rungen.
social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research Go €ttingen: Hogrefe-Verlag.

You might also like