You are on page 1of 5

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 83

ogy, Tobin Hall, University of Massa- Clifton, R., Rochat, P., Litovsky, R., & Perris, E.
their knowledge to regulate action, (1991). Object representation guides infants’
chusetts, Amherst, MA 01003.
solve problems, and achieve goals. reaching in the dark. Journal of Experimental
In G. Bremner, A. Slater, & G. But- Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
17, 323–329.
terworth (Eds.), Infant development:
Recent advances (pp. 109–135). Hood, B., Carey, S., & Prasada, S. (2000). Predict-
Hove, England: Psychology Press. ing the outcomes of physical events: Two-
References year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity
and support. Child Development, 71, 1540–1554.
Baillargeon, R. (1993). The object concept revisited:
Acknowledgments— This research was Hood, B., Cole-Davies, V., & Dias, M. (2003). Look-
New directions in the investigation of infants’
supported by Grant HD27714 from the ing and search measures of object knowledge
physical knowledge. In C.E. Granrud (Ed.), Vi-
National Institutes of Health and Re- in pre-school children. Developmental Psychol-
sual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–
ogy, 39, 61–70.
search Scientist Award MH00332 from 315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
the National Institute of Mental Health to Baillargeon, R., Graber, M., DeVos, J., & Black, J. Mash, C., Clifton, R.K., & Berthier, N.E. (2002,
Rachel K. Clifton (now Rachel Keen). I am (1990). Why do young infants fail to search for April). Two-year-olds’ event reasoning and
grateful to Neil Berthier, my collaborator hidden objects? Cognition, 36, 225–284. object search. In L. Santos (Chair), Interpreting
in all of these studies, and to the other col- Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E., & Wasserman, S. (1985). dissociations between infant looking and reaching:
laborators who contributed to various Object permanence in five-month-old infants. A comparative approach. Symposium conducted
phases of this work. Cognition, 20, 191–208. at the meeting of the International Society on
Infant Studies, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Berthier, N.E., DeBlois, S., Poirier, C.R., Novak,
J.A., & Clifton, R.K. (2000). Where’s the ball? Mash, C., Keen, R., & Berthier, N.E. (in press). Vi-
Two- and three-year-olds reason about unseen sual access and attention in two-year-olds’
events. Developmental Psychology, 36, 394–401.
Note event reasoning and object search. Infancy.
Butler, S.C., Berthier, N.E., & Clifton, R.K. (2002).
Two-year-olds’ search strategies and visual Spelke, E.S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Ja-
1. Address correspondence to tracking in a hidden displacement task. Devel- cobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psy-
Rachel Keen, Department of Psychol- opmental Psychology, 38, 581–590. chological Review, 99, 605–632.

Why People Fail to Recognize Their an adequate measure of the good


life, people must have some insight
Own Incompetence into their limitations. To ace an
exam, a college student must know
David Dunning,1 Kerri Johnson, Joyce Ehrlinger, and when he needs to crack open his
Justin Kruger notebook one more time. To provide
Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York (D.D., K.J., and J.E.), adequate care, a physician must
and Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Champaign-Urbana, Illinois (J.K.) know where her expertise ends and
the need to call in a specialist begins.
Recent research we have con-
ducted, however, suggests that peo-
Abstract Because these notions often do ple are not adept at spotting the lim-
Successful negotiation of ev- not correlate with objective per- its of their knowledge and expertise.
eryday life would seem to re- formance, they can lead people Indeed, in many social and intellec-
quire people to possess insight to make judgments about their tual domains, people are unaware of
about deficiencies in their intel- performance that have little to their incompetence, innocent of their
lectual and social skills. How- do with actual accomplishment. ignorance. Where they lack skill or
ever, people tend to be blissfully knowledge, they greatly overesti-
unaware of their incompetence. Keywords mate their expertise and talent, think-
This lack of awareness arises be- self-evaluation; metacognition; ing they are doing just fine when, in
cause poor performers are dou- self-concept; overconfidence; fact, they are doing quite poorly.
bly cursed: Their lack of skill performance evaluation
deprives them not only of the
ability to produce correct re-
sponses, but also of the expertise Real knowledge is to know the extent
of one’s ignorance. IGNORANCE OF
necessary to surmise that they
—Confucius INCOMPETENCE:
are not producing them. People AN EXAMPLE
base their perceptions of perfor-
mance, in part, on their precon- Confucius’ observation rings
ceived notions about their skills. just as true today as it did 26 centu- Consider the following example.
ries ago. To achieve and maintain In a sophomore-level psychology

Copyright © 2003 American Psychological Society


84 VOLUME 12, NUMBER 3, JUNE 2003

class, we asked 141 students to tell


us how well they had done on an
exam, just before they walked out
of the classroom. We asked the re-
spondents to estimate their perfor-
mance and mastery of the course ma-
terial relative to the other students
taking the exam. We also asked
them to estimate their raw score on
the test.
Figure 1 presents the data from
the comparison questions. In the
figure, we have separated respon-
dents into four groups based on
their actual performance on the test,
from the bottom 25% of performers
to the top 25%. As the figure shows,
students in the bottom quartile
greatly overestimated their perfor-
mance on the test. Whereas their
performance actually put them in
the 12th percentile, they estimated Fig. 1. Perceived percentile rankings for mastery of course material and test perfor-
mance as a function of actual performance rank.
their mastery of the course mate-
rial to fall in the 60th percentile
and their test performance to fall
in the 57th. Figure 2 reveals a sim- solving ability in the lab (Haun,
ilar pattern in estimates of raw Zeringue, Leach, & Foley, 2000). THE DOUBLE CURSE
scores, with bottom performers This pattern even appears, un-
overestimating their performance checked, after participants are prom- People fail to recognize their
by roughly 30%. ised up to $100 for accurate as- own incompetence because that in-
This example is not an isolated sessments of their performance competence carries with it a double
case. Participants taking tests in (Ehrlinger et al., 2003). curse. In many intellectual and so-
their ability to think logically, to
write grammatically, and to spot
funny jokes tend to overestimate
their percentile ranking relative to
their peers by some 40 to 50 points,
thinking they are outperforming a
majority of their peers when, in
fact, they are the ones being outper-
formed (Kruger & Dunning, 1999).
This pattern also emerges in more
real-world settings: among debate
teams taking part in a college tour-
nament and hunters quizzed about
their knowledge of firearms just be-
fore the start of hunting season
(Ehrlinger, Johnson, Banner, Dun-
ning, & Kruger, 2003); among med-
ical residents evaluating their pa-
tient-interviewing skills (Hodges,
Regehr, & Martin, 2001); and
among medical lab technicians as-
sessing their knowledge of medical
terminology and everyday problem- Fig. 2. Perceived versus actual test score as a function of actual test performance.

Published by Blackwell Publishing Inc.


CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 85

cial domains, the skills needed to have the skills needed to avoid poor good sense of how well they per-
produce correct responses are vir- performance in the first place. They form in absolute terms, such as
tually identical to those needed to would no longer be incompetent. their raw score on a test (see Fig. 2).
evaluate the accuracy of one’s re- Despite this paradox, we de- Where they err is in their estimates
sponses. The skills needed to pro- cided to put this hypothesis to the of other people—consistently over-
duce logically sound arguments, for test (Kruger & Dunning, 1999, estimating how well other people
instance, are the same skills that Study 4). In a first phase of the are doing on the same test (Fussell
are necessary to recognize when a study, participants were tested on & Krauss, 1992). As a result, they
logically sound argument has been their ability to solve a certain type tend to underestimate how their
made. Thus, if people lack the of logic problem. Not surprisingly, performance compares with that of
skills to produce correct answers, poor performers grossly overesti- others. One can disabuse top per-
they are also cursed with an inabil- mated their performance on the formers of this misperception by
ity to know when their answers, or test. Then, in a second phase, we showing them the responses of
anyone else’s, are right or wrong. gave roughly half of the partici- other people. They then tend to re-
They cannot recognize their re- pants a mini-lecture about how to alize how unique and distinctive
sponses as mistaken, or other peo- solve this type of logic problem, their performances are, providing
ple’s responses as superior to their giving them the skills needed to more positive and accurate self-eval-
own. In short, incompetence means distinguish accurate from inaccu- uations. For example, asking people
that people cannot successfully rate answers. When given their who are particularly proficient in
complete the task of metacogni- original test to look over, the par- grammar to evaluate the grammar
tion, which, among its many mean- ticipants who received the lecture, of others causes them to appropri-
ings, refers to the ability to evalu- and particularly those who were ately raise their perceptions of their
ate responses as correct or incorrect. poor performers, provided much own relative grammar skill. This
A good deal of research demon- more accurate self-ratings than exercise has no effect on the self-
strates that poor performers have they had originally. They judged impressions of poor performers
more difficulty with metacognitive their performance quite harshly— (Kruger & Dunning, 1999, Study 3;
judgments than their more compe- and even lowered their confidence see also Hodges et al., 2001, for sim-
tent peers do. Relative to students in their own general logical reason- ilar findings involving interviewing
who are doing well, students doing ing ability, even though, if any- skills among medical residents).2
poorly on a college exam do not as thing, the mini-lecture had strength-
successfully distinguish which in- ened that ability, not weakened it.
dividual questions they are getting
right from which they are getting
wrong (Sinkavich, 1995). Poor read- WHERE PERCEPTIONS OF
ers are less accurate than more able COMPETENCE COME FROM
readers in judging what they com- THE UNDUE MODESTY OF
prehend from a passage of text TOP PERFORMERS The work we have summarized
(Maki & Berry, 1984). In our own leaves open an important mystery.
research, students unskilled in Top performers also suffer a It explains what does not happen
grammar provided less accurate burden, albeit one that differs from (i.e., people recognizing their in-
“grades” of the grammatical perfor- that of their less skilled counter- competence), but it does not explain
mances of others than did their parts in that they tend to underesti- what does. How do people arrive at
more skilled counterparts (Kruger mate their percentile rank relative the impressions, sometimes nega-
& Dunning, 1999, Study 3). to the people with whom they tive but usually positive, that they
This double-curse explanation compare themselves. Their under- hold of their performances?
also suggests a crucial hypothesis: If estimation is usually statistically sig- In recent research, we have
poor performers are given the nificant (Ehrlinger et al., 2003; Haun identified one important source of
skills necessary to distinguish cor- et al., 2000; Hodges et al., 2001; people’s performance evaluations,
rect from incorrect answers, then Kruger & Dunning, 1999), al- and shown that it can be a potential
they would be in a position to rec- though in the case of Figure 1 it ap- source of error in those evalua-
ognize their own incompetence. Of pears quite small. tions. At first blush, one might
course, this hypothesis comes with This underestimation has a dif- think that people judge how well
a paradox: If poor performers had ferent source than the overestima- they are doing on a test by moni-
the skills needed to distinguish ac- tion of poor performers. Top per- toring their experience with it. Are
curacy from error, they would then formers tend to have a relatively they taking a long time to provide

Copyright © 2003 American Psychological Society


86 VOLUME 12, NUMBER 3, JUNE 2003

the answers? Are they sure there selves quite negatively). Although Study 4). Perception of performance,
are no competitors to the answers participants in the two conditions not reality, influenced decisions
they give? Such an approach would took the same test, and achieved on about future activities.
be termed bottom-up, as it refers to average essentially the same score,
the specific experiences people those who thought they had taken
have with the test. an abstract reasoning test estimated
However, we have found that that they had achieved higher scores
people’s estimates of their perfor- than did those who thought they had CONCLUDING REMARKS
mance arise, at least in part, from a taken a computer programming test
top-down approach. People start (Ehrlinger & Dunning, 2003). This research, combined with pre-
with their preconceived beliefs The top-down nature of perfor- vious work (for a review, see Fal-
about their skill (e.g., “I am good at mance estimates can have important chikov & Boud, 1989), calls into
logical reasoning”) and use those behavioral consequences. Women, question the ability people have to
beliefs to estimate how well they for example, tend to disproportion- form accurate views of their skills
are doing on any specific test. This ately leave science careers along and expertise. But more than that,
strategy at first seems to be a good every step of the educational and it calls into question whether peo-
one—people who believe they professional ladder (Seymour, ple are, or ever can be, in a position
have logical reasoning skill should 1992). We began to wonder if top- to form accurate self-impressions.
have some basis for that claim—ex- down influences on performance es- If incompetent individuals do not
cept for one fly in the ointment. timates might contribute to this pat- have the skills necessary to achieve
People’s impressions of their intel- tern. Starting in adolescence, women insight into their plight, how can
lectual and social skills often corre- tend to rate themselves as less scien- they be expected to achieve accu-
late only modestly, and sometimes tifically talented than men rate rate self-views? How can anybody
not at all, with measures of their themselves (Eccles, 1987). Because be sure that he or she is not in the
actual performance (Falchikov & of this, women might start to think same position?
Boud, 1989). Indeed, and perhaps they are doing less well on specific This research also potentially ex-
more important, people just tend scientific tasks than men tend to plains, in part, a mystery that peo-
to hold overinflated views of their think, even when there is no gender ple regularly confront in their ev-
skills that cannot be justified by difference in performance. Thinking eryday dealings. Everyone knows
their objective performance (Dun- they are doing less well, women people who just seem to accept their
ning, Meyerowitz, & Holzberg, might become less enthusiastic about deficiencies, failing to work to im-
1989; Weinstein, 1980). Therefore, participating in scientific activities. prove upon them. Perhaps these in-
preconceived notions of skill can We put these notions to a test by dividuals “accept” their deficiencies
lead people to err in their perfor- giving male and female college stu- because they are unaware that they
mance estimates. dents a pop quiz on scientific rea- have them. As Alfred North White-
In several studies, we have shown soning. Before the quiz, the students head once observed, it is not igno-
that people tend to make top-down were asked to rate themselves on rance, but ignorance of ignorance,
performance estimates that have their scientific skills, and the women that is the death of knowledge.
little to do with objective perfor- rated themselves more negatively Our work suggests many differ-
mance. In one study, we measured than the men did. The students’ es- ent avenues of follow-up, but one
participants’ views of their “ab- timates of their performance on the particularly important future ave-
stract reasoning ability” before quiz showed the same pattern, with nue would focus on how, or whether,
they sat down and took a quiz on the women thinking that they had people can become aware of their in-
logic. Later, their estimates of how done less well than the men thought, tellectual and social deficiencies.
well they had done on the test (e.g., even though there was no gender What are the domains in which peo-
how many items they got right) difference in actual performance. ple naturally intuit their deficits, and
correlated just as highly with their Later, when asked if they would how do those domains differ from
preexisting self-views as with their like to participate in a science compe- the ones we have studied? Are there
actual performance. In another tition for fun and prizes, the women rules of thumb that people can fol-
study, we gave participants a test were more likely than the men to de- low to ferret out their areas of in-
that was purported to assess abstract cline the invitation. This reluctance competence? Can people ever be
reasoning ability (on which partici- correlated significantly with their expected to uncover their pockets
pants rated themselves highly) or perceptions of performance on the of incompetence on their own, or
computer programming skills (on quiz, but not at all with actual perfor- is outside intervention always nec-
which participants rated them- mance (Ehrlinger & Dunning, 2003, essary? Removing barriers to self-

Published by Blackwell Publishing Inc.


CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 87

improvement may rest on answers imperfect, and that any measurement assessment in higher education: A meta-analy-
flaw would lead perceptions of perfor- sis. Review of Educational Research, 59, 395–430.
to these questions. Fussell, S.R., & Krauss, R.M. (1992). Coordination
mance to correlate less than perfectly of knowledge in communication: Effects of
with objective performance. This im- speakers’ assumptions about what others
Recommended Reading perfect correlation would then cause know. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
the perceptions of poor performers to ogy, 62, 378–391.
Ehrlinger, J., & Dunning, D. (2003). be more positive than their objective per- Haun, D.E., Zeringue, A., Leach, A., & Foley, A.
(See References) (2000). Assessing the competence of specimen-
formance (see Krueger & Mueller, 2002). processing personnel. Laboratory Medicine, 31,
Falchikov, N., & Boud, D. (1989). (See However, across several studies, we 633–637.
References) have found that statistically estimating Hodges, B., Regehr, G., & Martin, D. (2001). Diffi-
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). (See and then correcting for imperfections culties in recognizing one’s own incompetence:
References) in our measures leaves our original pat- Novice physicians who are unskilled and un-
Metcalfe, J. (1998). Cognitive opti- aware of it. Academic Medicine, 76, S87–S89.
tern of misperception almost wholly in-
mism: Self-deception or memory- Krueger, J., & Mueller, R.A. (2002). Unskilled, un-
tact (Ehrlinger et al., 2003; Kruger & aware, or both? The contribution of social-per-
based processing heuristics. Person- Dunning, 2002). ceptual skills and statistical regression to self-
ality and Social Psychology Review, 2, enhancement biases. Journal of Personality and
100–110. Social Psychology, 82, 180–188.
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and
unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing
one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-
Acknowledgments— The research de- References assessments. Journal of Personality and Social
scribed in this report was supported fi- Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.
nancially by National Institute of Mental Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J.A., & Holzberg, A.D. Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (2002). Unskilled and
Health Grant RO1 56072, awarded to (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: The unaware—but why? A reply to Krueger and
David Dunning. role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self- Mueller. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
serving assessments of ability. Journal of Per- chology, 82, 189–192.
sonality and Social Psychology, 57, 1082–1090. Maki, R.H., & Berry, S.L. (1984). Metacomprehen-
Eccles, J.S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s sion of text material. Journal of Experimental
achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Notes Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172. 10, 663–679.
Ehrlinger, J., & Dunning, D. (2003). How chronic Seymour, E. (1992). Undergraduate problems with
1. Address correspondence to David self-views influence (and potentially mislead) teaching and advising in SME majors: Explain-
Dunning, Department of Psychology, estimates of performance. Journal of Personality ing gender differences in attrition rates. Journal
Uris Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY and Social Psychology, 84, 5–17. of College Science Teaching, 21, 284–292.
14853-7601; e-mail: dad6@cornell.edu. Ehrlinger, J., Johnson, K., Banner, M., Dunning, D., Sinkavich, F.J. (1995). Performance and metamem-
& Kruger, D. (2003). Why the unskilled are un- ory: Do students know what they don’t know?
2. In other analyses, we have ruled aware: Further explorations of (absent) self-insight Instructional Psychology, 22, 77–87.
out another explanation for our find- among the incompetent. Unpublished manu- Weinstein, N.D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about
ings. It is almost a given that our mea- script, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY. future life events. Journal of Personality and So-
sures of objective performance were Falchikov, N., & Boud, D. (1989). Student self- cial Psychology, 39, 806–820.

Cognitive Activity and Risk of Keywords


Alzheimer’s disease; cognitive
Alzheimer’s Disease activity; longitudinal studies
Robert S. Wilson1 and David A. Bennett
Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center (R.S.W., D.A.B.) and Departments of Neurological Recent scientific data suggest
Sciences (R.S.W., D.A.B.) and Psychology (R.S.W.), Rush-Presbyterian-St. Luke’s that people with higher educational
Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois
and occupational attainment tend
to have a lower risk of developing
Alzheimer’s disease than do peo-
ple with lower educational and oc-
cupational attainment (Stern et al.,
Abstract count for the association between
1994). The mechanism underlying
Recent research suggests that higher educational and occupa-
this pattern is unknown. One hy-
frequent participation in cogni- tional attainment and reduced
pothesis is that the effects of educa-
tively stimulating activities may risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Fi-
tion and occupation are due to
reduce risk of Alzheimer’s dis- nally, we discuss the behavioral
their association with frequency of
ease in old age. We review epide- and neurobiological mecha-
participation in cognitively stimu-
miological evidence of such an nisms that may underlie the asso-
lating activities (Evans et al., 1997).
association. We then consider ciation between cognitive activity
Although the idea that frequent in-
whether cognitive activity can ac- and risk of Alzheimer’s disease.
tellectual activity might help one’s

Copyright © 2003 American Psychological Society

You might also like