You are on page 1of 3

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/9069205

Animal Welfare: Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores

Article  in  Nature · November 2003


DOI: 10.1038/425473a · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

238 5,248

2 authors:

Ros Clubb Georgia J Mason


RSPCA Science Group University of Guelph
14 PUBLICATIONS   901 CITATIONS    118 PUBLICATIONS   5,233 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Boredom-like states in farmed mink View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ros Clubb on 15 April 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


brief communications

Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores


Animals that roam over a large territory in the wild do not take kindly to being confined.

S
ome species — ring-tailed lemurs and trend was evident with body weight
a
snow leopards, for example — appar- (F1,333.23, P0.081; controlling for phy-
70 PB
ently thrive in captivity, whereas others, logeny: F1,314.09, P0.052). With both

Captive-infant mortality
60
such as Asian elephants and polar bears, are terms in a multiple regression, each lost its
50
prone to problems that include poor health, L individual effect on pacing, but the overall

(% births)
40
repetitive stereotypic behaviour and breed- adjusted r 2 value increased to 26.5%, from
30
ing difficulties. Here we investigate this AF 6.2% (for body weight alone) and 18.2%
20
previously unexplained variation in captive (for minimum HR alone; Fig. 1b). Likewise,
10 AM
animals’ welfare by focusing on caged carni- 0
median, but not minimum, daily travel dis-
vores, and show that it stems from con- –1 0 1 2 3
tances were positively correlated with pacing
straints imposed on the natural behaviour of Minimum home–range size (log km2) (F1,189.80, P0.003).
susceptible animals, with wide-ranging life- These results all held when total stereo-
styles in the wild predicting stereotypy and b typical behaviours (including non-pacing)
the extent of infant mortality in captivity. 60
L were analysed5. Naturally wide-ranging ani-
Stereotypy frequency

50
Our findings indicate that the keeping of PB mals did not, however, show more normal
(% observation)

40
naturally wide-ranging carnivores should be 30
activity in captivity (for example, minimum
either fundamentally improved or phased out. 20 HR: F1,150.17, n.s.; DDT: F1,150.01, n.s.),
Preventing natural behaviour patterns 10 AM nor did they move around more overall
in animals can give rise to stress and frustra- AF
within their enclosures (for example, mini-
tion1,2, and impair the development of brain mum HR: F1,180.61, n.s.; DDT: F1,180.10,
0
regions that are involved in behavioural –4 –3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3 4 5 n.s.). Home-range size therefore still pre-
sequencing, thereby reducing the animal’s Minimum home–range size (log km2; dicted pacing, even when controlling for
ability to behave flexibly andappropriately3,4. accounting for body weight) the amount of total activity in captivity
To investigate whether the observed varia- (for example, minimum HR: F1,142.65,
tion in the welfare of different species could Figure 1 Natural ranging behaviour and welfare of species P0.019). The degree of natural foraging
arise from a differential impact of captivity from the order Carnivora in captivity. a, Carnivores’ minimum and general activity, in contrast, did not pre-
on their natural behaviour, we calculated the home-range sizes in the wild predict captive infant mortality dict captive stereotypy or infant mortality5
mean frequency of stereotypic pacing5 by (F1,1912.60, P0.001). b, Together with body weight (see text), (for example, the level of natural activity
35 species of caged carnivore. We focused minimum home-range size also predicts stereotypic pacing in versus pacing: F1,183.53, n.s.). Variations
on pacing because it is the most prevalent captivity (F2,194.79, P0.011; controlling for phylogeny: in husbandry did not account for any of
stereotypy among carnivores (97% of F2,173.11, P0.036). On these cross-species plots, a few these findings5.
reported stereotypies5) and also to avoid species from a range of families and with varying relation to the Our results show, to our knowledge for
comparability problems raised by pooling regression line are highlighted: AF, Arctic fox (Alopex lagopus); PB, the first time, that a particular lifestyle in the
different forms of stereotypy (such as sway- polar bear (Ursus maritimus); AM, American mink (Mustela vison); wild confers vulnerability to welfare prob-
ing and head-nodding). We also quantified L, lion (Panthera leo). Values on the x-axes differ because fitted lems in captivity. Our study also reveals
infant mortality in captivity, which is often values are used in b that incorporate body weight; the y-axis in b species that are inherently likely to fare badly
due to poor maternal care6. shows data back-transformed from an arc-sine transformation. in zoos and similar establishments. Among
As an animal’s natural ranging and forag- the carnivores, naturally wide-ranging
ing activities are particularly constrained not by-products of variation in husbandry. species show the most evidence of stress
by captivity7, we obtained all available field Degrees of freedom varied in subsequent and/or psychological dysfunction in capti-
data on median home-range size, daily travel analyses owing to missing data. vity3,4,12, a finding that is a cause for concern,
distance, time spent in general activity, time Natural home-range size (HR) predicted given the difficulties of conserving such
spent foraging, and reliance on hunting. captive-infant mortality (median HR: species in situ13. Husbandry of these species
We also quantified minimum home-range F1,216.04, P0.012; minimum HR, see Fig. in captivity is therefore in need of improve-
sizes and daily travel distances, as these can 1a).Controlling for body weight did not alter ment, such as provision of extra space (a
be orders of magnitude smaller when food this relationship (median HR: F1,204.35, polar bear’s typical enclosure size, for exam-
is abundant8. Relationships between wild P0.025; controlling for phylogeny: ple, is about one-millionth of its minimum
and captive variables were tested by using F1,1620.46, P0.0001; minimum HR: home-range size). Alternatively, zoos could
one-tailed regressions. F1,189.29, P0.004; controlling for phy- stop housing wide-ranging carnivores and
Body-weight effects were investigated in logeny: F1,1816.94, P0.001). Minimum, concentrate instead on species that respond
analyses involving range size9; phylogenetic but not median, daily distances travelled better to being kept in captivity.
effects were controlled where necessary (and (DDT) gave similar results (F1,183.99, Ros Clubb, Georgia Mason
in all analyses involving body mass) by P0.03). These effects seem to be specific to Animal Behaviour Research Group, Department of
comparative analysis of independent con- captive animals: wild and captive infant- Zoology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road,
trasts10,11. Our inferences about welfare took mortality rates did not covary (F1,60.08, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK
into account natural infant-mortality rates, not significant) and infant mortality in e-mail: georgia.mason@zoology.ox.ac.uk
and the amount of normal activity and total the wild was unrelated to range size (for 1. Mason, G., Cooper, J. & Clarebrough, C. Nature 410,
stereotypy in captivity; we also considered example, minimum HR: F1,70.43, n.s.). 35–36 (2001).
2. Dawkins, M. S. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 20, 209–225 (1988).
feeding regimes, and the size and complexity Home-range size also predicted pacing 3. Robbins, T. W., Jones, G. H. & Wilkinson, L. S.
of enclosures, to check that relationships (median HR: F1,225.78, P0.013; mini- J. Psychopharmacol. 10, 39–47 (1996).
between wild and captive variables were mum HR: F1,205.66, P0.014). A positive 4. Lewis, M. H., Gluck, J. P., Bodfish, J. W., Beauchamp, A. J. &

NATURE | VOL 425 | 2 OCTOBER 2003 | www.nature.com/nature 473


brief communications
Mailman, R. B. in Stereotyped Movements (eds Sprague, R. L. in Fig. 1. During event 1, before loading with
& Newell, K. M.) 37–67 (Am. Psychol. Assoc., TNT, a reference voltage pulse (25 volts) is
Washington DC, 1996).
5. Clubb, R. E. The Roles of Foraging Niche, Rearing Conditions and
applied to the piezoresistive heater, causing a
Current Husbandry on the Development of Stereotypies in temporary upward spike in circuit output
Carnivores (Thesis, Univ. Oxford, 2001). that is due to heating. TNT loading (event 2)
6. Meier, J. in Zoo and Wild Animal Medicine (ed. Fowler, M. E.) causes a gradual upward shift in sensor out-
842–851 (Saunders, Philadelphia, 1986).
7. Shepherdson, D. J., Mellen, J. D. & Hutchins, M. (eds) Second
put, which then gradually decreases when the
Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals TNT begins to desorb from the cantilever
(Smithsonian Inst. Press, Washington DC, 1998). (event 3). The second pulse (5 volts) during
8. Gittleman, J. L. & Harvey, P. H. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10,
57–63 (1982).
desorption does not raise the cantilever
9. Gittleman, J. L. J. Mamm. 67, 23–36 (1986). temperature sufficiently for deflagration
10. Purvis, A. & Rambaut, A. Comp. Appl. Biosci. 11, 247–251 (1995). (event 4). The third pulse (25 volts) causes
11. Bininda-Emonds, O. R. P., Gittleman, J. L. & Purvis, A. Biol. Sci. deflagration, as shown by a visible smoke
74, 143–175 (1999).
12. Bahr, N. I., Pryce, C. R., Döbeli, M. & Martin, R. D. Physiol.
plume, and a dramatic mass decrease, which
Behav. 64, 429–437 (1998). is verified by a reduction in circuit output
13. Woodroffe, R. & Ginsberg, J. R. Science 280, 2126–2128 (1998). (event 5) that overwhelms the upward ther-
Competing financial interests: declared (see online version). mal signal evident in event 1. Post-deflagra-
tion reference pulses of 25 volts resulted in
spikes similar to the one seen in event 1.
Explosives The occurrence of deflagration was Figure 1 Deflagration and detection of trinitrotoluene (TNT). a,
inferred from three consistent observations. Sensor output plotted against time. During event 1 (numbered
A microsensor for First, the cantilever returns to its pre-test arrow) of the test, a 25-volt reference pulse is sent to the piezo-

trinitrotoluene vapour resonance frequency after deflagration, sug-


gesting that all of the adsorbed material has
resistor before TNT loading begins; sensor output returns quickly
to almost its pre-pulse value. In event 2, TNT is gradually loaded
been lost. Second, a specific voltage (corre-

S
ensing devices designed to detect onto the cantilever and the resonance frequency decreases, causing
explosive vapours are bulky, expensive sponding to a threshold, or deflagration- the sensor output to increase. After loading, the TNT begins to
and in need of technological improve- point, temperature) is necessary to cause desorb (event 3) and a 5-volt pulse (event 4) is insufficient to heat
ment — dogs remain the most effective deflagration. Third, the measurement of the explosive on the cantilever to its deflagration point. However, a
detectors1 in the fight against terrorism heat added to the cantilever during deflagra- 25-volt pulse is sufficient to cause deflagration (event 5). b, Left,
and in the removal of land-mines2,3. Here tion shows that the reaction is exothermic, diagram of the microcantilever, showing the integrated piezoelec-
we demonstrate the deflagration of tri- ruling out other possible reactions such as tric sensor, piezoresistive heater, and TNT loaded on the cantilever
nitrotoluene (TNT) in a small localized melting, vaporization or decomposition. surface. Scale bar, 100 m. Right, deflagration is confirmed in
explosion on an uncoated piezoresistive Our method currently detects the defla- magnified high-speed video images; the smoke plume is visible,
microcantilever. This explosive-vapour sen- gration of as little as 70 picograms (1.91011 particularly when illuminated by red laser light.
sor, which has a detection capability that is molecules) of TNT (calculated from the shift
comparable to that of a dog, should enable in cantilever resonance). This limit of detec-
extremely sensitive, miniature detection tion is the same as that of an improved ver- *Oak Ridge National Laboratory, Oak Ridge,
devices to be used on a large scale. sion9 of the ion-mobility mass-spectrometry Tennessee 37831, USA
Microcantilevers with specific coatings technology now used for airport security. †Department of Physics, University of Tennessee,
can be used for chemical detection4,5, but Calculations show that the detection limit Knoxville, Tennessee 37996, USA
TNT molecules, which are intrinsically could be improved by up to three orders of ‡Transportation Security Administration,
‘sticky’, readily adhere to uncoated cantilever magnitude by using optimized cantilevers. US Department of Homeland Security,
surfaces6. When the source of TNT is An explosive-detection technique based Atlantic City, New Jersey 08405, USA
removed, the TNT molecules slowly desorb. on deflagration is advantageous, because §Department of Mechanical Engineering and the
We found that applying a voltage pulse to deflagration is a characteristic property of Nevada Ventures Nanoscience Program, University
an integrated piezoresistor during this de- these substances. Other potentially interfer- of Nevada, Reno, Nevada 89557, USA
sorption process causes the cantilever tem- ing but non-explosive substances tested — e-mail: jdadams@unr.edu
perature to rise beyond the deflagration including water, acetone, ethyl alcohol and 1. Furton, K. G. & Myers, L. J. Talanta 54, 487–500 (2001).
point of TNT, resulting in a miniature defla- gasoline — all desorbed in our system before 2. Colton, R. J. & Russell, J. N. Science 299, 1324–1325 (2003).
3. Czarnik, A. W. Nature 394, 417–418 (1998).
gration. Our cantilevers did not degrade the voltage pulse was applied. In the absence 4. Thundat, T., Chen, G. Y., Warmack, R. J., Allison, D. P. &
after hundreds of pulses (10–25 volts) and of a voltage pulse, desorption of nanogram Wachter, E. A. Anal. Chem. 67, 519–521 (1995).
deflagrations. quantities of deposited TNT takes tens of 5. Lang, H. et al. Appl. Phys. Lett. 72, 383 (1998).
6. Muralidharan, G. et al. Ultramicroscopy 97, 433–439 (2003).
We monitored deflagration by capturing minutes; comparable amounts of water or
7. Minne, S. C., Manalis, S. R. & Quate, C. F. Appl. Phys. Lett. 67,
magnified high-speed video images, by mea- alcohol desorb in seconds. 3918–3920 (1995).
suring the deflection due to released heat Other explosive molecules can be detect- 8. Rogers, B. et al. Rev. Sci. Instruments (in the press).
using an optical beam-bounce technique ed by this method and differentiated by their 9. Khayamian, T., Tabrizchi, M. & Jafari, M.T. Talanta 59,
327–333 (2003).
that is used in atomic-force microscopy, and compound-specific deflagration points and Competing financial interests: declared none.
by measuring resonance-frequency shifts desorption times (results not shown). Our
and heat added with a piezoelectric/piezo- technique could also be used in conjunction
resistive cantilever7 operated in self-sensing with coated-cantilever chemical-sensing brief communications is intended to provide a forum
mode by means of an a.c. bridge circuit8. The arrays5, in which the arrays serve as an initial for brief, topical reports of general scientific interest and
circuit’s output increases with mass loading indicator, and positive readings are verified for technical discussion of recently published material of
of adsorbed TNT and increasing tempera- by deflagration on an uncoated cantilever. particular interest to non-specialist readers (communi-
ture; it decreases with mass desorption and L. A. Pinnaduwage*†, A. Gehl*†, D. L. cations arising). Priority will be given to contributions
decreasing temperature. Hedden*†, G. Muralidharan*, T. Thundat*†, that have fewer than 500 words, 10 references and only
A five-event TNT-detection test,using the R. T. Lareau‡, T. Sulchek§, L. Manning§, one figure. Detailed guidelines are available on Nature’s
self-sensing platform described, is illustrated B. Rogers§, M. Jones§, J. D. Adams§ website (www.nature.com/nature).

474 NATURE | VOL 425 | 2 OCTOBER 2003 | www.nature.com/nature

View publication stats

You might also like