You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/248018317

Agnosias

Article  in  Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science · March 2010


DOI: 10.1002/wcs.42

CITATIONS READS
9 709

2 authors, including:

Marlene Behrmann
Carnegie Mellon University
397 PUBLICATIONS   14,118 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Visual object recognition View project

Word reading View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Marlene Behrmann on 16 January 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Overview

Agnosias
Marlene Behrmann∗ and Mayu Nishimura

The neuropsychological disorder, known as visual agnosia, refers to the impairment


in deriving the meaning of a visually presented stimulus, in spite of the affected
individual having intact sensory and low-level vision, and normal language
and semantic function. This type of disorder is intriguing both clinically and
scientifically, and vision scientists have studied visual agnosia as a means of
shedding light on how the normal visual system functions. Considerable progress
has been made in this domain, in parallel with detailed behavioral and neural
investigations of the visual system of neurologically intact individuals and of
nonhuman primates. Here, we focus specifically on the neuropsychological studies
and provide a broad overview of the wide range of impairments that fall under
the label ‘visual agnosia’, including those acquired following brain damage in
premorbidly normal individuals, those that appear to have been present since
birth, and those whose onset is late in life and is associated with neurodegeneration.
We also outline the different subtypes of visual agnosia, including those that affect
primarily the recognition of faces, words, or objects, and we lay out some of the
key questions currently being addressed by researchers in this domain.  2010 John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd. WIREs Cogn Sci 2010 1 203–213

V isual agnosia refers to a diverse class of


neuropsychological disorders in which the
affected individual is impaired at deriving the meaning
of recognition can be observed in other modalities
such as audition (auditory agnosia) and touch (tactile
agnosia) but, because visual agnosia is perhaps the
of some or all categories of visual stimuli. The term best studied of all the modality-specific agnosias, we
‘agnosia’, which comes from the Greek, means ‘not focus on this particular subset of neuropsychological
knowing’ or ‘without knowledge’, and aptly captures disorders.1–3
the dramatic failures of recognition which characterize
the behavior of individuals with visual agnosia. For
example, some agnosic individuals fail to recognize VISUAL AGNOSIA: OVERVIEW
the faces of close family members, others fail to recog-
nize common everyday objects such as a lamp or a salt The key characteristic of visual agnosia is the patient’s
shaker, and yet others have difficulty recognizing com- failure to access the meaning of perceptual infor-
mon written words, even when the words are printed mation presented to the visual modality, in spite
in the largest and clearest of fonts. Importantly, these of normal sensory vision, language, and intelligence.
visuoperceptual failures occur despite the agnosic indi- Importantly, too, individuals with agnosia demon-
vidual exhibiting normal or near-normal elementary strate normal recognition of objects through modal-
visual functions such as acuity, brightness discrimina- ities other than vision (touch, audition, and verbal
tion, motion perception, and color vision, along with description of its function); that the patients are able
normal or near-normal semantic and memory func- to recognize inputs in other sensory domains further
tioning. These individuals also have intact alertness, attests to the fact that the impairment does not arise
intelligence, and language, thus setting aside ques- from a difficulty in retrieving names or in accessing
tions about whether visual agnosia arises simply from the necessary semantic/knowledge information.
reduced elementary visual function or from compro- Visual agnosia can be general, affecting the
mised intellectual ability. Similar interesting failures recognition of all types of visual stimuli or it can
be more specific: for example, there are agnosias that
∗ Correspondence to: behrmann@cmu.edu appear to be relatively selective, affecting only the
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, recognition of objects and leaving the recognition
PA 15213-3890, USA of other visual stimuli largely intact. Among these
DOI: 10.1002/wcs.42 selective disorders are the agnosia for faces, termed

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 203


Overview wires.wiley.com/cogsci

‘prosopagnosia’, the agnosia for words, termed the co-occurrences and dissociations has important
‘agnosic alexia’ or ‘pure alexia’, the agnosia for body implications for understanding how visual representa-
parts, the agnosia for colors, and the agnosia for tions are organized. An additional intriguing question
environmental scenes, including landmarks. A large concerns the psychological mechanisms that underlie
nomenclature exists to define and distinguish between agnosia. For example, some have suggested that a
the different forms of visual agnosia and these different problem in discriminating curvature might give rise to
types of agnosia are listed in Table 1, and described prosopagnosia5 whereas others have suggested that it
briefly in the second half of this review (see also Ref 2). is the failure to carry out holistic or configural pro-
Note that visual agnosia of any form is rather rare cessing that is to be blamed.6 Another, closely related
and, consequently, variants of this disorder are even question concerns the brain mechanism or neural sub-
rarer. strate that is affected and whether the different forms
Independent of the particular form of agnosia, of agnosia and their co-occurrences or dissociations
researchers who examine these disorders as a are systematically predicted by the location of the
means of elucidating the fundamental operational lesion. Questions concerning the potential for recov-
characteristics of the normal visual system typically ery from such impairments, and procedures by which
address a key set of questions. Among these are such recovery can be enhanced, are also of great inter-
whether these apparently different forms of agnosia est to researchers and clinicians alike.7 Below, we
are indeed domain-specific or whether the presenting provide a general overview of visual agnosia and the
symptom is simply the most pronounced of a host range of etiologies that give rise to the impairments,
of recognition failures across multiple domains—that and, thereafter, we describe some of the variants in
is, whether an individual who is impaired at greater details.
word recognition, for example, truly evinces the The standard profile of visual agnosia is that
impairment only for orthographic input or whether, it arises following a lesion sustained by an adult
with careful examination and refined testing, other who possessed normal premorbid perceptual abilities.
related perceptual deficits can also be uncovered. Thus, etiologies such as stroke or tumor or other
The answer to this particular question is central to forms of brain damage such as anoxia (e.g., as
theoretical accounts of category-specificity and the a result of carbon monoxide poisoning) in adults
way in which information is processed and organized have, to date, constituted the majority of reported
at a psychological and neural level. If it is indeed the instances of agnosia. There are, however, several
case that, for example, the representation of words case reports of children who have become agnosic
is mediated by a circumscribed, dedicated process for following a brain lesion sustained early in life,
recognizing words and no other visual stimuli, a lesion a disorder usually referred to as ‘developmental
to this dedicated process will naturally result in alexic agnosia’.8,9 Such cases are even more rare than the
agnosia. If, however, the underlying psychological adult form of the disorder. More recently, there has
and neural substrate supporting word recognition been growing recognition of two other etiologies for
(to continue with the example) is not limited to visual agnosia, both of which may turn out to be
words, and patients show a concurrent impairment more common than the acquired forms described
for numbers and other alphanumeric symbols (or even above. One etiology is the deterioration of perceptual
for common everyday objects), then a lesion to that skills in individuals with progressive posterior cortical
area of ventral visual cortex might result in an agnosia atrophy, which can occur in the absence of dementia.
that cuts across categories rather than being tightly This visually selective degenerative condition is rather
restricted to one domain. Other possible forms of different from more conventional neurodegeneration,
organization and predictions are also possible. as in Alzheimer’s disease, and this disorder is being
Another key, related question concerns the pro- increasingly detected and identified as a separate
file and distribution of the differing forms of agnosia. entity, with several reports of such individuals now
There have been several reported cases of fairly ‘pure’ in the literature.10 Finally, in the last decade or so,
forms of these agnosias but there are also many there have been a growing number of reports of
reported cases in whom more than one form of individuals who demonstrate a lifelong impairment in
agnosia coexist, and certain types of agnosia appear recognition that occurs in the absence of any obvious
to co-occur more frequently than others.4 The obvi- concomitant—that is, where there is no obvious
ous question, then, is why do some forms co-occur alteration in cortical structure evident on conventional
more frequently than others and furthermore, which, MRI scan. This last form of agnosia has been labeled
if any, agnosias occur in the complete absence of ‘congenital’ to distinguish it from the disorder in which
any other neuropsychological deficit? Understanding children have an acquired, obviously discernable

204  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Agnosias

lesion, as mentioned above. There can also be different stimulus is not successfully identified. The failure to
forms of congenital agnosia, including congenital recognize the object occurs under a whole range of
prosopagnosia11 and congenital color agnosia,12 a conditions and independently of whether the stimulus
selective neuropsychological condition in which color is presented as a real three-dimensional object, a
perception per se is intact, while the identification and black-and-white line drawing, or as a photograph.
naming of color is disrupted. Developmental dyslexia Conversely, Lissauer suggested that perception
or the failure to acquire word recognition normally itself was normal in cases of associative agnosia and
(or perhaps just certain subtypes) may also be a form that the disorder arises downstream of perception.
of neurodevelopmental agnosia. Note that there may The hallmark of associative agnosia is that the
be corresponding atypicalities that are present from individual is able to copy and match the object (or
birth in other modalities, such as congenital amusia in a drawing/rendition of it) well despite the inability
the auditory domain.13 to identify it. This disorder is often described as
‘perception stripped of meaning’, a phrase coined
by Teuber.15 Whether the derived percept is truly
VISUAL AGNOSIA: SUBTYPES normal in associative agnosia (and simply cannot be
The term ‘visual agnosia’ was originally coined by used to access meaning) or whether the representation
Freud to refer to object recognition problems observed is still not sufficiently rich and precise in these cases,
in some individuals. Lissauer, however, was the first and hence is the source of the recognition failure,
scientist to provide a detailed account of a patient with is a topic of much contention. The lesions that
visual agnosia and, in so doing, went on to identify give rise to the higher-order associative forms of
two separate stages through which visual information agnosia are usually more circumscribed and affect
passes for successful recognition. The first or cortical structures that are somewhat more anterior,
apperceptive stage constitutes ‘the stage of conscious including the fusiform gyrus, temporal gyrus, or even
awareness of a sensory impression’ whereas the the anterior portions of the temporal lobe (see below
second or associative stage involves the simultaneous for further specification, and Ref 16 for descriptions of
activation of a number of concepts related to the different profiles associated with different lesion sites).
object (i.e., the activation of associated memories).14 The classical dichotomy of apperceptive–asso-
Based on this two-stage theory, an impairment to the ciative agnosia, while useful in delimiting cases at
first stage would result in ‘apperceptive mindblindness one end or the other of the agnosia spectrum, has
(seelenblindheit)’ in which the impaired individual is mostly fallen out of favor given that it captures the
unable to construct a good perceptual representation extreme cases but not the variants falling in between
from the visual input. Damage to the second stage the extremes. This simple dichotomy has been sup-
would result in ‘associative mindblindness’, in which planted by a more fine-grained continuum with a more
the affected individual can derive a reasonably (if not precise characterization of the multiple variants of
entirely) precise description of the input but cannot agnosia.17 Indeed, one reason that the visual agnosias
use the well-specified perceptual representation to are so intriguing as a class of clinical phenomena is
access stored knowledge of the object’s functions and that they have important implications for current the-
associations. ories of high-level vision, and this is consistent with the
Apperceptive agnosia is thought to arise from a increased understanding of the complexity of visual
breakdown at relatively early stages of visual process- cognition. Thus, amending the classical dichotomy
ing at which the elementary features of the stimulus is compatible with the growing evidence that visual
are processed and at which a coherent structural object recognition comprises a number of distinct
description is achieved. Consequently, this individual steps (and multiple brain regions) in transforming the
is unable to identify, copy, or even match a drawing retinal input into representations that reflect invariant
based on the impoverished visual representation properties of objects in the real world.
derived from the image. Individuals like this are The more refined characterization of agnosia
obviously very substantially impaired in representing is obviously still in progress but a number of key
incoming visual information. Consistent with this, points along this more refined continuum have been
the lesions associated with apperceptive agnosia are demarcated. It is also the case that, relative to
generally to the occipital lobe and are large and often Lissauer’s distinction, the extremes of the continuum
diffuse in nature (see Table 1 for further description). have been expanded and elaborated (where one
In such cases, knowledge of the shape, form, and/or endpoint is extreme difficulty deriving the percept,
other stored knowledge of the physical attributes of as in apperceptive agnosia, and the other endpoint
an object cannot be accessed, with the result that the is intact perception but failure to access meaning,

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 205


Overview wires.wiley.com/cogsci

TABLE 1 Classification of Types of Visual Agnosia, their Underlying Neuropathology, and Clinical Manifestation
Neuropathology Clinical Manifestation
Apperceptive agnosia
Visual form agnosia Stroke, anoxia, carbon monoxide poisoning Unable to copy, match, or identify visual stimuli
affecting occipital, parietal, or posterior
temporal regions bilaterally
Integrative agnosia Extensive extrastriate damage bilaterally or Able to copy and match stimuli, may even be
just to the right hemisphere able to provide verbal description of aspects of
input but still fails to recognize object
Associative agnosia
Impaired access to structural Both types: usually bilateral infarction of the Fails to access stored representation of object
knowledge posterior cerebral arteries but unilateral structural description
temporo-occpital damage may suffice
Impaired access to/within Fails to access or activate stored semantic
semantic knowledge knowledge of objects either just from vision or
from all modalities
Perceptual categorization Not yet definitively specified Impaired recognition of objects from unusual
deficit views or under unusual lighting conditions
Prosopagnosia Acquired form: bilateral infero-mesial visual Failure to recognize faces
association cortices (lingual and fusiform
gyri) and subjacent white matter
Agnosia for words Left occipitotemporal cortex ‘Pure’ alexia—fails to read words normally
despite normal language and sensory visual
function
Agnosia for scenes Bilateral or right posterior artery infarction Failure to recognize landmarks or known scenes
involving the fusiform and lingual gyri,
extending to the parahippocampal gyrus
Developmental agnosia No obvious neural concomitant on Difficulty in acquiring mastery over word or face
conventional CT or MRI scanning recognition (developmental dyslexia or
developmental/congenital prosopagnosia)
Posterior cortical agnosia Focal right temporal and/or occipital lobe Progressive decline in complex visual functions
atrophy and recognition

as in associative agnosia). The nomenclature below individuals cannot recognize, copy, match, or dis-
proceeds from the most severe perceptual impairment criminate simple visual stimuli, and cannot recognize
to the most intact perception. even simple shapes such as triangles or circles. Some
individuals may also have achromatopsia,19 in which
Apperceptive Agnosia case they cannot match stimuli by color either. These
Patients with apperceptive agnosia are unable to patients usually do better with real images than with
construct a stimulus-specific structural description. line drawings—for example, one such patient recog-
Two predominant forms exist, as detailed below.17 nized only 17% of black-and-white line drawings but
when the same images were presented in color they
Visual Form Agnosia were recognized with 26% accuracy.20 Performance
Visual form agnosia, a term introduced by Benson also improves when motion accompanies the input,
and Greenberg,18 and synonymous with severe form presumably because this provides additional cues
perception, refers to the class of individuals who to assist in shape perception. Tracing the contours
complain of blurred or unclear vision but, on formal also aids recognition, because the traced information
examination, their acuity is more than adequate to can be represented internally through the hand
recognize objects. In addition, color, brightness, and movements, perhaps by visual imagery processes,
movement discrimination are typically preserved, which provides a compensatory approach to support
while the ability to perceive figural properties such recognition. The lesions in visual form agnosia are
as size, orientation, and shape is typically lost. These typically diffuse and posterior. The prominence of

206  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Agnosias

white matter lesions suggests that disconnection, often equally with displays of two- and three-dimensional
of very local intralaminar connections, rather than stimuli and with black-and-white and chromatic dis-
neuronal loss, may cause the visual deficit. Stroke, plays, although in some cases, the presence of depth,
anoxia, carbon monoxide poisoning are common color, and/or surface cues may be of some assistance
causes of the disorder; demyelination, tumor, and to the patients in segmenting the display.28,29 These
mercury poisoning are also causes but somewhat less integration/segmentation problems are most clearly
common.21,22 A particularly well-known patient who demonstrated when there are multiple items present,
fits the visual form agnosia description is DF, a patient such that there is competition in assigning elements
who was unable to determine whether a square and between shapes, as in displays with overlapping
rectangle, matched for total flux, were the same shape shapes or ones that require boundary assignment.
or not.23,24 DF also was impaired at segmenting figure Interestingly, in some patients, the presence of local
from ground, deciding whether shapes were symmet- information is so captivating that it reduces the effi-
rical or not and, not surprisingly, was profoundly ciency of visual recognition; thus, counterintuitively,
impaired at object recognition. DF has garnered in contrast with normal perceivers, some patients with
special scientific attention because, in spite of her integrative agnosia identified silhouettes better than
profound perceptual disorder, she is nevertheless able line drawings, whose internal details apparently led to
to utilize shape information to correctly orient her incorrect segmentation.30 Figure-ground segregation
hand to match the orientation of a slot, and to scale and other forms of perceptual grouping, such as
her grasp appropriately to objects of different sizes. deriving a unified whole based on Gestalt heuristics,
Her pattern of performance has generated a deep may be especially challenging for such individuals.
debate about the independence of information used Also, the rapid and efficient access to the local
for perception and action, and many intriguing studies information may impede the patients’ ability to gain
have been conducted with DF and with similar apper- access subsequently to the global information.31
ceptive agnosic individuals25 to assess the integrity of
their reaching behavior in the absence of perceptual
precision provided by the ventral visual system. Associative Agnosia
Further along the continuum of visual agnosia are the
Integrative Agnosia associative types who show normal or near-normal
While some consider individuals with ‘integrative performance on tests of perception but are still
agnosia’ to be a less severe form of visual form agnosia, impaired at object recognition. In some cases,
others consider its distinguishing features sufficiently recognition of all objects is abnormal whereas, in
different from visual form agnosia and prefer to rec- others, recognition of visually simpler objects such
ognize it as an independent entity. The key feature in as a pen or watch may be better than recognition of
integrative agnosia is that the coding of single, simple objects that are more complex visually (and perhaps
shapes may be relatively unimpaired, with the deficits also less familiar) like mushroom, harmonica, or
being revealed only under conditions that particularly harp. The most common cause of associative visual
stress visual segmentation and grouping. Critically, agnosia is bilateral infarction of the posterior cerebral
patients with integrative agnosia are able to process arteries. Demyelination, hemorrhage, and tumor are
the basic features or elements present in a display but uncommon causes whereas bilateral lesions involving
appear unable to group or integrate all aspects into the inferior temporo-occipital junction and subjacent
a meaningful whole.26,27 These patients operate in white matter are the most common anatomic lesions
a ‘piecemeal’ manner to identify objects, sometimes associated with this form of agnosia.22 Note, however,
oversegmenting the single input object into several that this disorder can also occur with unilateral dam-
different objects (for example, identifying the handle age to the left or right temporo-occipital region.32,33
of a fork as one object and the tines as a second). As with apperceptive agnosia, the associative form
These individuals may perform normally on visual can be subdivided into two variants.17
matching and copying tasks (thus, are similar to the
associative agnosia subtype described below) but their Impairment in Structural Knowledge
perception is compromised (and thus, they are similar One form of associative agnosia arises from the
to the apperceptive agnosia subtype). Crucially, these difficulty in accessing the perceptual knowledge or
patients perform poorly on tasks that require the structural description of an object. These individuals
binding of visual elements in a spatially parallel fash- would likely also have difficulty accessing the
ion across a visual display containing multiple stimuli. semantics associated with the observed object but not
The failure to integrate the disparate elements occurs vice versa. For example, one of the ways of assessing

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 207


Overview wires.wiley.com/cogsci

the integrity of access to the structural description and a screw, or a pyramid, a palm tree and a pine
is to ask patients to decide whether objects are real tree.38 In those cases, in whom the deficit is in the
or not (when they are not real, they are ‘legitimate’, semantic system, the problem may extend beyond their
in other words made of real parts of objects just knowledge of the visual or perceptual properties of
incorrectly combined such as the head of a horse on stimuli and affect knowledge of functional attributes
the body of a dog). Given the sensitivity of these too (for example, the function of a hammer vs.
individuals to the perceptual nature of the input, it is a screwdriver). There are also cases who evince
not surprising that recognition is adversely affected by disproportionate impairment in some categories over
increasing complexity of the object. Some of these others, for example, performing more poorly when
individuals evince a category-specific impairment, recognizing living or animate objects than inanimate
being disproportionately poorer in recognition of objects and others in whom the disorder affects
living things. A possible explanation for this is that mainly recognition of nonliving or inanimate objects.
many living things have a similar perceptual structure, Interesting theoretical exchanges about the type of
and this creates increased competition for recognition information (functional vs. perceptual) tapped for the
and naming. The competition results in longer reaction recognition of living versus nonliving objects,39 as
times as well as higher error rates (and this pattern well as questions about differential lesion localization
may also be seen in normal individuals, albeit to a in these two cases abound in the literature.40 Patients
much milder extent). with disorders within the semantic system will have
Some of these associative agnosic patients still damaged long-term representations,41 and imagery
retain the long-term representation of the object (i.e., should not be possible in these cases.
can provide a verbal description of the objects) but,
nevertheless, fail to recognize the stimulus presented.
Perceptual Categorization Deficit
It has been possible, for example, to demonstrate
Warrington and colleagues described a set of patients
reasonably good performance in visual imagery tasks,
who performed poorly at recognizing or matching
demonstrating that stored visual knowledge may be
objects seen from unusual views or illuminated in ways
at least relatively preserved.34,35 Thus, some patients
that produced confusing shadows.42 These individu-
may be able to visualize in their ‘mind’s eye’ a
als were neither apperceptive nor associative agnosics
particular object but, presented with the same object,
but experienced difficulty in object recognition under
may fail to identify it from vision. Consistent with
these particular conditions. The term ‘perceptual cat-
this is the claim that neuronal templates that match
egorization deficits’ was coined to describe this deficit
a visual stimulus with a visual memory are stored
in object shape constancy that impaired specifically
primarily in ventral visual association cortex and the
the processes that allow us to recognize the equiv-
failure to update or refresh these representations in
alence of an object’s three-dimensional appearance
individuals with a long history of agnosia can affect
when viewed from different perspectives and under
the integrity of these internal representations such that
different lighting conditions rather than a problem
they then begin to decay.36 Note that even patients
in shape perception per se. The recognition deficit is
who have impaired semantic knowledge, may still be usually apparent only when an object is seen from an
able to make correct object decisions, indicating that unusual view that eliminates important features and
access to structural descriptions may be intact and is often identified in patients who have no problems
independent of the status of semantic knowledge,17 with object recognition in their normal environments.
supporting the dissociation between structural and Impairment of perceptual categorization may there-
semantic knowledge. fore be considered an abnormality in the systems that
solve visual problems, perform mental rotation, and
Impairment in Accessing Semantics support visual imagination—not in the visual systems
Semantic knowledge specifies information about that mediate object recognition per se.
object function and interobject associations, and this
forms the basis of our full concept of an object. There
are some cases of associative agnosia in whom the Prosopagnosia
problem is closer to semantics37 and others in whom Individuals with prosopagnosia are unable to recog-
the problem arises in the semantic system itself. In nize individual faces although they are almost always
both cases, patients may be unable to make decisions capable of detecting whether a face is present in the
about which two of three visually presented objects input.43 The disorder may be so profound that the
from the same category belong together and which affected individual may fail to recognize him/herself
one ‘odd’ as when presented with a hammer, a nail from photographs and may fail to recognize family

208  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Agnosias

members and close friends too. In many instances, more on local features such as the mouth to identify
these patients can describe the face they are looking at a given face, and they focus very precisely on local
in detail, including the age and gender of the person, features rather than seeing the whole of a face from
but are unable to say whose face it is. A prosopagnosic its diagnostic center.
patient’s self-report, provided by Pallis,44 is illustra- The lesion that gives rise to prosopagnosia is usu-
tive: ally secondary to temporo-occipital lesions, affecting
the fusiform and lingual gyri, and whereas a bilateral
I can see the eyes, nose and mouth quite clearly but lesion was thought to be necessary, there is growing
they just don’t add up. They all seem chalked in, like consensus that a unilateral right lesion to these regions
on a blackboard. I have to tell by the clothes or voice
may be sufficient to give rise to this disorder.19
whether it is a man or woman, as the faces are all
neutral, a dirty gray color (he also had achromatopsia
or a cortical color vision problem). The hair may help
a lot, or if there is a moustache. . .All the men appear Agnosia for Words
unshaven. . .I cannot recognize people in photographs, Agnosia for words is also known as pure alexia, alexia
not even myself. At the club I saw someone strange without agraphia (as in the original label assigned
staring at me and asked the steward who it was. by the French neurologist Jules Dejerine), perceptual
You’ll laugh at me. I’d been looking at myself in the word-form agnosia, or pure word blindness. Although
mirror. . .I later went to London and visited several this phenomenon is usually discussed in the context
cinemas and theaters. I couldn’t make head or tail of
of language impairments, it is an agnosic symptom,
the plots. I never knew who was who. . .I can shut my
eyes and can well remember what my wife looked like as subjects who suffer from this deficit show a
or the kids. language impairment limited to visually presented
stimuli (e.g., reading), but not to auditorily presented
The ability to extract information about emo- stimuli.48 These patients may be able to spell words
tional expression is preserved in some cases but out loud and to write well but fail to read their own
impaired in others. Invariably, these patients rely handwriting subsequently. This disorder has also been
on non-face characteristics such as gait or voice to called ventral simultanagnosia to indicate the failure
recognize individuals in their daily lives. The deficit is to process multiple letters simultaneously and this
typically perceptual in nature, rather than arising from failure manifests most dramatically in word reading
a memory problem (although there are prosopamnesic where individuals laboriously and sequentially process
cases for whom memory failure for faces is the core one letter at a time (giving rise to the label ‘letter-by-
of the deficit45 ) or semantic deficit, nor from a fail- letter’ reading). Pure alexia is one of the more common
ure to label the face (an anomia). In most cases, the forms of agnosia and can be severely debilitating, with
affected individual also performs poorly at discrimi- some patients requiring 1.4 s to process each letter in
nating between two faces, even novel faces and even a string. Whether this form of agnosia is entirely
under conditions in which they are given unlimited limited to word recognition or whether other classes
time in which to make the discrimination decision, of visual objects are also affected, perhaps to a lesser
although the extent to which the deficit is more apper- degree, remains controversial. In one recent study
ceptive versus more associative may determine the with four such individuals, recognition of single letters
exact nature of the failures.46 Many studies suggest and digits in the central visual field was impaired in
that these patients with acquired prosopagnosia pro- all patients. In addition, visual apprehension span
cess faces differently than controls, focusing more on was also reduced for both letters and digits in all
individual features than on the holistic or second-order patients.49 A similar study has shown that seven
relations between the features, and on some accounts, such patients are even impaired at deciding whether
the ‘configural deficit’ that impairs perception of spa- two black-and-white checkerboards are the same or
tial structure, affects not just faces but also other different (with one cell of the matrix flipped) and
non-facial patterns (for recent example, see Ref 6). that this was especially true as the size of the matrix
There are now several studies in the recent literature increased.50 These recent findings, along with those
showing that individuals with acquired prosopagnosia of other researchers, offer strong support for a deficit,
are less sensitive to the information in the eye region which is perceptual in nature and rather widespread
than their counterparts and perhaps focus unduly on in impact, and affecting not just orthographic input
the mouth region (for a recent example, see Ref 47). but other forms of fine-grained visual discrimination.
The idea is that these individuals have lost the ability The lesion site in these patients is typically in the left
to represent multiple elements of an individual face as ventral occipitotemporal cortex, sometimes, but not
a perceptual unit (holistic face perception) and so rely always, involving the splenium of the corpus callosum.

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 209


Overview wires.wiley.com/cogsci

Agnosia for Landmarks and Environment in existence from birth but there seems no reason
An impairment in recognizing landmarks or scenes, to believe otherwise).54 Developmental dyslexia, or at
referred to as topographic agnosia, can co-occur least one subtype thereof in which perceptual encoding
with agnosia, prosopagnosia, or it can also occur is affected, may also be a form of childhood agnosia,
in isolation. As with other forms of agnosia, these and it is the case that while some individuals appear to
patients perform well on a wide range of memory and have an obvious underlying neural concomitant, this
basic perception tasks. In these topographic cases, is not the case for all individuals. Recently, there have
known landmarks and buildings lose their familiarity been a substantial number of reports of individuals
to the patients. Unsurprisingly, these patients get lost who are congenitally prosopagnosic.11 Some of these
and are unable to learn new routes. They show undue individuals appear to be as impaired as cases in whom
reliance on street names and numbers of buildings to a clear lesion is evident. For example, one of the
identify the landmarks. Topographic agnosia can be individuals who has participated in several of the
differentiated from other disorders affecting spatial investigations in our lab said the following:
orientation in a large-scale environment. In one I grew up in a small town of about 7000 people
recent study,51 the authors described a patient who and most everyone knew everyone else. During High
got lost in familiar environments but could readily School I worked in the local movie theater and was
read and draw maps, describe familiar routes, and quite good at selling the tickets, making change
provide correct directions. On a familiar route, this by hand and doing the reports that went with it.
particular patient could recognize major landmarks However, my Mom would say things like, ‘Mr. and
but could not recognize route configurations made Mrs. X (vocal emphasis), our neighbors, or Mary
up of combinations of visual features each lacking (vocal emphasis) from our church told me that you
individual distinctiveness. On a test of route learning, are stuck up. They went to the movies and you sold
he learned landmarks that differed in minor details them a ticket; but, you didn’t say hello to them.’ I
never realized that they were people I knew or was
and could use them to orient himself along a route
suppose to know. I’ve always been amazed at how
but had difficulty in recognizing and learning scenes people recognized other people so easily.
lacking salient landmarks. The lesion giving rise to
topographic agnosia most commonly follows bilateral Because of the availability of high resolution imaging,
or right posterior artery infarction and the lesions and because these congenitally prosopagnosic indi-
(as in prosopagnosia) may implicate the fusiform and viduals do not have any vascular limitations making
lingual gyri, extending to the parahippocampal gyrus. them ideal candidates for imaging, many neural stud-
Neuroimaging studies provide converging evidence for ies have been conducted with such individuals. Among
topographic-specific representations in these cortical the results obtained is that many, albeit not all, of
regions, such as in the so-called parahippocampal these individuals evince normal cortical activation,
place area.52 in functional magnetic resonance imaging investiga-
tions, in the ‘fusiform face area’, the pre-eminent
occipitotemporal region of cortex associated with
DEVELOPMENTAL FORMS face perception.55 They do, however, in contrast with
OF AGNOSIA matched counterparts, fail to show activation in more
anterior regions perhaps associated with biographical
The taxonomy and illustration of the agnosias and individual identity representations,56 and, consis-
described above refer to those instances in which tent with this, the white matter tracts that project from
the object recognition failures occurred in adults who the more posterior to more anterior regions appear
were premorbidly normal. As mentioned at the outset to be compromised in the individuals, to an extent
of this review, however, there is growing interest in commensurate with their face recognition difficulty.57
the type of agnosia that appears to be present from Finally, there have been several reports of
childhood. These childhood agnosias may themselves individuals who are congenitally agnosic for color.
be subdivided into two subgroups: ‘developmental These individuals are unable to recognize colors
agnosia’ in which individuals who are born normal despite intact color perception, semantic memory for
become agnosic following a lesion, such as stroke, color information, and color naming.12
sustained during childhood (for example, see Refs 9 Much research remains to be done with
and 53), and ‘congenital agnosia’ in which the disorder individuals with the congenital variants of agnosia to
is congenital or, at a minimum, lifelong, in the absence characterize the disorders more fully and to uncover
of any obvious neurological concomitants (of course the underlying neural mechanism that may give rise to
it is difficult to determine whether the disorder is truly these disorders. It is also the case that there appears to

210  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Agnosias

be a familial hereditary aspect associated with most of This rare syndrome is part of the fronto-temporal
these neurodevelopmental disorders and this promises dementias (FTDs), which may present as focal atro-
a fruitful avenue for further genotyping studies in phy in any combination of the right and left frontal or
agnosic individuals and their family members. temporal cortices. Other forms of progressive agnosia
may include agnosia for words and agnosia for objects,
but further description and systematic data collection
PROGRESSIVE POSTERIOR CORTICAL in such cases are required.
ATROPHY
Just as there appear to be forms of agnosia that arise in
SUMMARY
childhood so there are forms of agnosia that can occur
relatively selectively as a form of progressive cortical Much progress has been made in recent years in
deterioration associated with aging. Posterior cortical describing and characterizing the neuropsychologi-
atrophy refers to the progressive decline in complex cal disorders that fall under the heading of ‘visual
visual processing ability along with relative sparing agnosia’. While these disorders are intriguing clin-
of other cognitive and perceptual functions.58,59 Later ically, they have also offered scientists a unique
on, memory and other cognitive dysfunctions may also window into the workings of the visual system.
be involved. Progressive visual agnosia is associated Coupled with the findings from recent neuroimaging
with occipitoparietal atrophy and hypometabolism investigations, the information gleaned from studying
on single photon emission computed tomography those with visual agnosia has added substantially to
(SPECT) and on positron emission tomography (PET) our understanding of how the brain transforms the
scans,60 especially in the right hemisphere, and can be impoverished signals from the eyes into meaningful
independent of Alzheimer’s disease. and coherent representations. Although considerable
Progressive prosopagnosia is a degenerative progress has been made, many outstanding questions
agnosia, too, in which there is a more selective remain and a definitive nomenclature and classifica-
progressive impairment in the recognition of faces.61 tion of the visual agnosias is still evolving.

REFERENCES
1. Devinsky O, Farah MJ, Barr WB. Chapter 21 Visual 9. Farah MJ, Rabinowitz C, Quinn GE, Liu GT. Early
agnosia. Handb Clin Neurol 2008, 88:417–427. commitment of neural substrates for face recognition.
Cogn Neuropsychol 2000, 17:117–123.
2. Farah MJ. Visual Agnosia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press;
2004. 10. Giovagnoli AR, Aresi A, Reati F, Riva A, Gobbo C,
3. Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ. To See But Not To et al. The neuropsychological and neuroradiological
See: A Case-Study of Visual Agnosia. Hillsdale, NJ: correlates of slowly progressive visual agnosia. Neurol
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1987. Sci 2009, 30:123–131.

4. Farah MJ. Patterns of co-occurrence among the associa- 11. Behrmann M, Avidan G. Congenital prosopag-
tive agnosias: implications for visual object recognition. nosia: face-blind from birth. Trends Cogn Sci 2005,
Cogn Neuropsychol 1991, 8:1–19. 9:180–187.

5. Kosslyn SM, Hamilton SE, Bernstein JH. The per- 12. Nijboer TC, Van Zandvoort MJ, De Haan EH. Covert
ception of curvature can be selectively disrupted in colour processing in colour agnosia. Neuropsychologia
prosopagnosia. Brain Cogn 1995, 27:36–58. 2006, 44:1437–43.

6. Barton JJS. What is meant by impaired configural pro- 13. Peretz I, Hyde KL. What is specific to music process-
cessing in acquired prosopagnosia? Perception 2009, ing? Insights from congenital amusia. Trends Cogn Sci
38:242–260. 2003, 7:362–367.
7. Leff AP, Behrmann M. Treatment of reading 14. Lissauer H. Ein fall von seelenblindheit nebst einem
impairment after stroke. Curr Opin Neurol 2008, beitrage zur theorie derselben. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr
21:644–648. 1890, 21:222–270.
8. Barton JJS, Cherkasova M, O’Connor M. Covert recog- 15. Teuber H-L. Alteration of perception and memory in
nition in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia. man. In: Weiskrantz L, ed. Analysis of Behavioral
Neurology 2001, 57:1161–1168. Change. New York: Harper and Row; 1968.

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 211


Overview wires.wiley.com/cogsci

16. Barton JJ. Structure and function in acquired prosopag- 34. Behrmann M, Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Intact visual
nosia: lessons from a series of 10 patients with brain imagery and impaired visual perception in a patient
damage. J Neuropsychol 2008, 2:197–225. with visual agnosia. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Per-
17. Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW. Visual agnosia. Neurol form 1994, 20:1068–1087.
Clin 2003, 21:501–520. 35. Behrmann M, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Dissociation
18. Benson DF, Greenberg JP. Visual form agnosia. Annu between mental imagery and object recognition in a
Rev Neurosci 1969, 20:82–89. brain-damaged patient. Nature 1992, 359:636–637.

19. Bouvier SE, Engel SA. Behavioral deficits and cortical 36. Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW, Gannon T, Blott W,
damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia. Cereb Cortex Jones V. Memories are made of this: the effects of time
2006, 16:183–191. on stored visual knowledge in a case of visual agnosia.
Brain 1999, 122:537–559.
20. Mapelli D, Behrmann M. The role of color in object
37. Charnallet A, Carbonnel S, David D, Moreaud O. Asso-
recognition: evidence from visual agnosia. Neurocase
ciative visual agnosia: a case study. Behav Neurol 2008,
1997, 3:237–247.
19:41–44.
21. Landis T, Graves R, Benson F, Hebben N. Visual recog-
38. Howard D, Patterson KE. The Pyramids and Palm
nition through kinaesthetic mediation. Psychol Med
Trees Test. Suffolk, UK: Thames Valley Test Company;
1982, 12:515–531.
1992.
22. De Renzi E. Disorders of visual recognition. Semin
39. Warrington EK, Shallice T. Category specific semantic
Neurol 2000, 20:479–485.
impairments. Brain 1984, 107:829–853.
23. Milner AD, Goodale M. The visual brain in action.
40. McClelland JL, Rogers TT, Patterson K, Dilkina KN,
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1995.
Lambon Ralph MR. Semantic cognition: its nature, its
24. Milner AD, Perrett DI, Johnston RS, Benson PJ, Jor- development, and its neural basis. In: Gazzaniga, M,
dan TR, et al. Perception and action in ‘visual form ed. The Cognitive Neurosciences IV. Boston, MA: MIT
agnosia’. Brain 1991, 114(Pt 1B):405–428. Press; 2009, chapter 72, 1–20.
25. Karnath HO, Ruter J, Mandler A, Himmelbach M. The 41. Ohtake H, Fujii T, Yamadori A, Fujimori M,
anatomy of object recognition—visual form agnosia Hayakawa Y, et al. The influence of misnaming on
caused by medial occipitotemporal stroke. J Neurosci object recognition: a case of multimodal agnosia. Cor-
2009, 29:5854–5862. tex 2001, 37:175–186.
26. Humphreys GW. Integrative agnosia. In: Humphreys, 42. Warrington EK. Neuropsychological studies of object
GW, ed. Case Studies in Vision. London: Psychology recognition. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B 1982, 298:15–33.
Press; 1999.
43. De Renzi E. Current issues in prosopagnosia. In: Ellis,
27. Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW. A case of integrative H, Jeeves, MA, Newcombe, F, Young, AW, eds. Aspects
visual agnosia. Brain 1987, 110:1431–1462. of face processing. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff; 1986.
28. Behrmann M, Kimchi R. What does visual agnosia 44. Pallis CA. Impaired identification of faces and places
tell us about perceptual organization and its relation- with agnosia for colours: report of a case due to cere-
ship to object perception?. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept bral embolism. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 1955,
Perform 2003, 29:19–42. 18:218–224.
29. Jankowiak J, Kinsbourne M, Shalev RS, Bachman DL. 45. Tippett LJ, Miller LA, Farah MJ. Prosopamnesia: a
Preserved visual imagery and categorization in a case selective impairment in face learning. Cogn Neuropsy-
of associative visual agnosia. J Cogn Neurosci 1992, chol 2000, 17:241–255.
4:119–131.
46. De Renzi E, Faglioni P, Grossi D, Nichelli P. Apper-
30. Lawson R, Humphreys GW. The effects of view in ceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex
depth on the identification of line drawings and silhou- 1991, 27:213–221.
ettes of familiar objects. Vis Cogn 1999, 6:165–195. 47. Rossion B, Kaiser MD, Bub D, Tanaka JW. Is the
31. Behrmann M, Williams P. Impairments in part-whole loss of diagnosticity of the eye region of the face a com-
representations of objects in two cases of integrative mon aspect of acquired prosopagnosia? J Neuropsychol
visual agnosia. Cogn Neuropsychol 2008, 24:701–730. 2009, 3:69–78.
32. Farah MJ. Dissociable systems for visual recognition: a 48. Behrmann M, Plaut DC, Nelson J. A literature review
cognitive neuropsychology approach. In: Kosslyn SM, and new data supporting an interactive account of
Osherson D, eds. Invitation to Cognitive Science. Cam- letter-by-letter reading. Cogn Neuropsychol 1998,
bridge, MA: MIT Press; 1994. 15:7–51.
33. McCarthy R, Warrington EK. Visual associative 49. Starrfelt R, Habekost T, Leff AP. Too little, too late:
agnosia: a clinico-anatomical study of a single case. reduced visual span and speed characterize pure alexia.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 1986, 49:1233–1240. Cereb Cortex 2009.

212  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010


WIREs Cognitive Science Agnosias

50. Mycroft RH, Behrmann M, Kay J. Visuoperceptual network in congenital prosopagnosia. Curr Biol 2009,
deficits in letter-by-letter reading? Neuropsychologia 19:1146–1150.
2009, 47:1733–1744.
57. Thomas C, Avidan G, Humphreys K, Jung KJ, Gao
51. Mendez MF, Cherrier MM. Agnosia for scenes F, Behrmann M. Reduced structural connectivity in
in topographagnosia. Neuropsychologia 2003, ventral visual cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Nat
41:1387–1395. Neurosci 2009, 12:29–31.
52. Epstein RA. Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contri-
58. Mendez MF, Shapira JS, Clark DG. ‘‘Apperceptive’’
butions to human spatial navigation. Trends Cogn Sci
alexia in posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex 2007,
2008, 12:388–396.
43:264–270.
53. Hadjikhani N, de Gelder B. Neural basis of prosopag-
nosia: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2002, 59. McMonagle P, Deering F, Berliner Y, Kertesz A. The
16:176–182. cognitive profile of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurol-
ogy 2006, 66:331–338.
54. Galaburda AM, Duchaine BC. Developmental disor-
ders of vision. Neurol Clin 2003, 21:1–16. 60. Whitwell JL, Jack CR Jr, Kantarci K, Weigand
55. Avidan G, Hasson U, Malach R, Behrmann M. Detailed SD, Boeve BF, et al. Imaging correlates of poste-
exploration of face-related processing in congenital rior cortical atrophy. Neurobiol Aging 2007, 28:
prosopagnosia: 2. Functional neuroimaging findings. 1051–1061.
J Cogn Neurosci 2005, 17:1150–1167. 61. Joubert S, Felician O, Barbeau E, Sontheimer A, Guedj
56. Avidan G, Behrmann M. Functional MRI reveals E, et al. Progressive prosopagnosia: clinical and neu-
compromised neural integrity of the face processing roimaging results. Neurology 2004, 63:1962–1965.

Vo lu me 1, March /April 2010  2010 Jo h n Wiley & So n s, L td. 213

View publication stats

You might also like