You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/235372001

Cognitive and Neuroplasticity Mechanisms By Which Congenital or Early


Blindness May Confer a Protective Effect Against Schizophrenia

Article  in  Frontiers in Psychology · January 2012


DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

60 193

3 authors, including:

Steven Silverstein Brian P Keane


University of Rochester Medical Center University of Rochester
406 PUBLICATIONS   12,176 CITATIONS    117 PUBLICATIONS   1,422 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Cognitive Neuroscience Task Reliability and Clinical applications in Serious Mental Illness View project

CAPR- Computerized Assessment of Psychosis Risk (Multi-site Collaborative R01) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Steven Silverstein on 01 June 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE
published: 21 January 2013
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624

Cognitive and neuroplasticity mechanisms by which


congenital or early blindness may confer a protective
effect against schizophrenia
Steven M. Silverstein 1,2 *, Yushi Wang 1 and Brian P. Keane 1,2,3
1
University Behavioral HealthCare, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Piscataway, NJ, USA
3
Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science, Piscataway, NJ, USA

Edited by: Several authors have noted that there are no reported cases of people with schizophre-
Michael Green, University of
nia who were born blind or who developed blindness shortly after birth, suggesting that
California Los Angeles, USA
congenital or early (C/E) blindness may serve as a protective factor against schizophrenia.
Reviewed by:
Yue Chen, McLean Hospital, USA By what mechanisms might this effect operate? Here, we hypothesize that C/E blindness
Jonathan K. Wynn, University of offers protection by strengthening cognitive functions whose impairment characterizes
California Los Angeles, USA schizophrenia, and by constraining cognitive processes that exhibit excessive flexibility
*Correspondence: in schizophrenia. After briefly summarizing evidence that schizophrenia is fundamentally
Steven M. Silverstein, University
a cognitive disorder, we review areas of perceptual and cognitive function that are both
Behavioral HealthCare, University of
Medicine and Dentistry of New impaired in the illness and augmented in C/E blindness, as compared to healthy sighted indi-
Jersey, 151 Centennial Avenue, viduals. We next discuss: (1) the role of neuroplasticity in driving these cognitive changes
Piscataway, NJ 08854, USA. in C/E blindness; (2) evidence that C/E blindness does not confer protective effects against
e-mail: silvers1@umdnj.edu
other mental disorders; and (3) evidence that other forms of C/E sensory loss (e.g., deaf-
ness) do not reduce the risk of schizophrenia. We conclude by discussing implications of
these data for designing cognitive training interventions to reduce schizophrenia-related
cognitive impairment, and perhaps to reduce the likelihood of the development of the
disorder itself.
Keywords: schizophrenia, blindness, perception, cognition, vision, vision disorders, plasticity

INTRODUCTION that protection occurs by (1) strengthening the perceptual and


Over the past 60 years, several authors (Chevigny and Braverman, cognitive functions whose impairment forms the essential nature
1950; Abely and Carton, 1967; Horrobin, 1979; Riscalla, 1980; of schizophrenia; and (2) constraining cognitive processes that are
Feierman, 1982; Sanders et al., 2003) have postulated that con- excessively neuroplastic in the illness.
genital or early (C/E) blindness may serve as a protective factor In the discussion below, after briefly summarizing the position
against the development of schizophrenia. Supporting data derive that schizophrenia is fundamentally a cognitive (i.e., information-
from several sources, including literature reviews, examination of processing) disorder, we review evidence that a cluster of per-
cohorts of blind patients in psychiatric hospitals, and surveys of ceptual and cognitive functions is at once markedly impaired in
agencies that treat large numbers of blind people. The most recent schizophrenia and significantly augmented in C/E blindness, com-
of these Sanders et al. (2003) noted that across all past papers, pared to healthy sighted individuals (see Table 1). In subsequent
there has not been even one reported case of a congenitally blind sections, we explicate the role of neuroplasticity in driving these
person who developed schizophrenia. While we identified one perceptual and cognitive differences, we provide evidence that C/E
reported case of a blind “schizophrenic” child (Stewart and Sardo, blindness does not confer protection against other mental disor-
1965), the 6-year-old described in that paper had many symp- ders (e.g., depression, anorexia nervosa) and finally we argue that
toms of autism and no symptoms of psychosis, and would almost other forms of C/E sensory loss (e.g., deafness) do not reduce
certainly be diagnosed with an autism-spectrum disorder using the risk of schizophrenia. We conclude by noting the implications
DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) or later crite- of these data for designing cognitive training interventions for
ria. In contrast to the lack of cases of people with C/E blindness patients and people at high risk for the disorder, and for developing
who meet modern criteria for schizophrenia, reports do exist of interventions to prevent schizophrenia.
people who develop both blindness and schizophrenia later in life
(Checkley and Slade, 1979). We therefore hypothesize that it is SCHIZOPHRENIA AS A COGNITIVE DISORDER: KRAEPELIN
the brain changes that occur secondary to C/E blindness – rather AND DEMENTIA PRAECOX REVISITED
than blindness per se – that protect against schizophrenia. How- Although the most obvious clinical features of schizophrenia
ever, the mechanisms that confer this apparent protection have include psychotic symptoms such as hallucinations, delusions, and
not yet been identified. In this paper, we advance the hypothesis bizarre behavior, it has long been thought that such symptoms

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 1


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

Table 1 | Summary of cognitive and brain function enhancements (+) underperformance precede the onset of psychotic symptoms (and
and impairments (−), compared to healthy sighted individuals, in C/E therefore the diagnosis) by many years; (3) decline in cognitive
blindness and schizophrenia. functioning continues after psychosis onset; and (4) this pattern
of changes is not found in other major mental illnesses, even those
Function Blindness Schizophrenia that can include psychotic symptoms, such as bipolar disorder
(Neumann et al., 1995; Schenkel and Silverstein, 2004; Khan, in
AUDITORY PERCEPTION
press). Cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are also signifi-
Localization + −
cantly correlated with level of functional disability (Green, 1996;
Acuity + −
Silverstein et al., 1998), and are therefore important treatment
Discrimination + −
targets.
Comprehension + −
Categorization + −
Temporal resolution + −
DOMAINS OF PERCEPTUAL AND COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING
Latency of auditory ERPs +* −**
THAT ARE STRENGTHENED BY C/E BLINDNESS AND
AUDITORY ATTENTION
IMPAIRED IN SCHIZOPHRENIA
AUDITORY PERCEPTION
Preattentive processing (e.g., MMN) + −
Selective attention + −
Compared to sighted people (and in some cases to late-blind peo-
Divided attention + −
ple), C/E blind individuals are characterized by enhanced auditory
MEMORY
acuity for pitch, better pitch discrimination, better pitch-timbre
Working memory + −
categorization (Wan et al., 2010), better speech discrimination
Short-term memory + −
(Hertrich et al., 2009), better sound localization, better temporal
Long-term memory + −
auditory resolution, better noise-embedded speech discrimina-
LANGUAGE
tion (Niemeyer and Starlinger, 1981; Muchnik et al., 1991), lower
Lexical decision making +* −**
sound threshold for identification of pseudo-words (Rokem and
Abstraction − +
Ahissar, 2009), shorter latencies for auditory event-related poten-
Conceptual inclusiveness − +
tials (ERPs; Roder et al., 1996, 1999), and greater tolerance for short
Word inventions − +
stimulus-onset asynchronies in an auditory backward masking
CONSTRUCTION OF SUBJECTIVE EXPERIENCE
task (Stevens and Weaver, 2005). C/E blindness is also associated
Integration via serial processing + −
with comprehension of ultra-fast synthetic speech at a rate of
Holistic processing + −
∼25 syllables per second, which is close to three times the rate at
Olfaction + −
which non-blind individuals can comprehend such speech (Her-
Motor control + −
trich et al., 2009). In addition, C/E blind subjects have shown
Body perception + −
larger mismatch negativity (MMN) components of the ERP, sug-
Plasticity + −
gesting a compensatory improvement in pre-attentional processes
(Kujala et al., 1995). It has been demonstrated that these sensory
*Faster than normal. and perceptual processing differences are due to enhanced basic
**Slower than normal. perceptual skills, not to “higher order” functions such as attention,
memory, language, or executive functions (reviewed in Cattaneo
and Vecchi, 2011).
are secondary, compensatory, features of the disorder. In contrast, The above findings are in contrast to what has been observed
much evidence suggests that the core features of schizophrenia are in schizophrenia, in which, for example, patients are typically
cognitive in nature, involving disturbances in perception, atten- characterized by poor auditory acuity, precision, and discrimina-
tion, memory, learning, language, and context-based modulation tion (including in tone matching) (Javitt et al., 1997; Rabinowicz
of these processes as a function of expectations and action plans et al., 2000; Li et al., 2002; Rojas et al., 2007; Turetsky et al.,
(Kraepelin, 1903; Bleuler, 1950; Weiss, 1989; Phillips and Silver- 2009; Perrin et al., 2010; Kantrowitz et al., 2013), impairments
stein, 2003; Nuechterlein et al., 2012; Tamminga, 2013; Khan, in in sound localization (Perrin et al., 2010), temporal resolution of
press). In this way, schizophrenia can be viewed as similar to brain sound (Foucher et al., 2007), auditory backward masking (Kall-
disorders such as Parkinson’s disease dementia and dementia with strand et al., 2002), pre-attentional processing as indicated by the
Lewy bodies, in which psychotic symptoms can occur, but are often (smaller) MMN ERP component (Umbricht et al., 2003), and
late-developing phenomena, and are not considered the defin- longer latency of post-attentional auditory ERPs (Ward et al.,
ing features of the disorder (Ballard et al., 2001; Williams-Gray 1991; Sevik et al., 2011; Iyer et al., 2012). Individuals with schiz-
et al., 2006; Ffytche, 2007). A large body of research evidence now ophrenia also have abnormal processing of speech sounds (Ngan
indicates that perceptual and cognitive impairments are found in et al., 2003; Hirano et al., 2008), are worse than other groups at
nearly all people with schizophrenia, and occur with greater fre- detecting speech embedded in noise, and are more likely than con-
quency than psychotic symptoms (Palmer et al., 2009). Significant trols to identify non-speech sounds as speech (Vercammen et al.,
cognitive impairment is also easily detectable in most schizo- 2008). Importantly, it has been shown that these impairments in
phrenia patients even when psychotic symptoms are in remission pitch processing in schizophrenia are not secondary to symptoms,
(Altshuler et al., 2004). In addition: (1) low intelligence is a risk medication, or poor attentional functioning (Rabinowicz et al.,
factor for schizophrenia; (2) cognitive decline and intellectual 2000).

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 2


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

The impaired auditory functions noted above are not mere auditory attention (Bleuler, 1950; Green et al., 1994; Heinrichs
laboratory curiosities; they have important clinical and functional and Zakzanis, 1998; Bruder et al., 1999; Light and Braff, 2005a,b;
implications. For example, poor sound localization is thought to be Scholes and Martin-Iverson, 2010). In short, the improvements in
associated with an externalization bias in which there is a reduced sensory, perceptual, and attentional auditory functioning in C/E
ability to recognize that the sound of one’s own voice is coming blindness represent changes that appear to essentially eliminate
from the self, as opposed to an external cause, which may con- the possibility of the development of the set of auditory impair-
tribute to delusional thinking (Johns et al., 2001; Ilankovic et al., ments and their consequences that form an important component
2011). At a more general level, perception has been conceptu- of schizophrenia.
alized as an active, hypothesis-generating function that involves
generating a model of the world, and then updating this model WORKING AND LONG-TERM MEMORY
via coding of prediction error (i.e., the degree to which continued Compared to healthy, sighted individuals, C/E blind individu-
incoming sensory input does not conform to the current per- als have superior working memory capacity, in terms of both
ceptual model of the world; Corlett et al., 2009; Clark, in press). the amount that can be processed, and the ability to recall the
It has been postulated that symptoms of schizophrenia, such as sequence in which information was presented (Hull and Mason,
hallucinations and delusions may result from an impaired abil- 1995; Amedi et al., 2003; Roder and Neville, 2003; Raz et al., 2007).
ity to detect inconsistencies between what would normally be The authors of a recent study that matched perceptual threshold
expected (based on past experience) and incoming sensory infor- of blind and sighted subjects concluded that superior short-term
mation (Corlett et al., 2007, 2009; Clark, in press). Both faulty memory in C/E blind people was due to enhancement of stimulus
bottom-up processing and poor quality top-down feedback, as encoding (related to the processes reviewed above), rather than
have been demonstrated in schizophrenia (Silverstein and Keane, to differences in storage or recall processes (Rokem and Ahissar,
2009, 2011a,b), would degrade the quality of perceptual represen- 2009). In addition, it is thought that, in C/E blindness, primary
tations and the extent to which they are modified by context to reliance on controlled, sequential processes (i.e., via haptic and
most accurately reflect the nature of reality (Corlett et al., 2007, auditory perception) during navigation – as opposed to the more
2009; Clark, in press; Silverstein, in press). The result can be abnor- automatic and parallel processing normally afforded by vision –
mal perceptions, hypersalience of normally ignored stimuli, and leads to superior working memory abilities (Raz et al., 2007; Salillas
new and unusual beliefs to account for these changes in subjective et al., 2009; Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011). C/E blind people have also
experience – in short, an internal representation of the world that demonstrated superior long-term memory compared to sighted
can deviate markedly from reality, and increasingly so over time controls (Bull et al., 1983; Roder et al., 2001). All of this is in
(Clark, in press). In contrast, as C/E blindness is associated with stark contrast to schizophrenia, in which working memory (Silver
enhanced sensory and perceptual processing, it can be seen how et al., 2003; Conklin et al., 2005; Brahmbhatt et al., 2006; Horan
this would reduce the likelihood of the sort of prediction cod- et al., 2008; Silverstein et al., 2010) and long-term memory (Van
ing failures, and their sequelae, associated with schizophrenia. The Snellenberg, 2009) impairments are typically observed, along with
auditory impairments in schizophrenia described above have also problems in stimulus organization during encoding (Harvey et al.,
been shown to have other important consequences, which would 1986; Brebion et al., 1997, 2004; Landgraf et al., 2011).
not occur in people with C/E blindness, such as problems decod- These data have important clinical implications. A critical func-
ing cues to emotion in speech (Leitman et al., 2011; Gold et al., tion of WM is to integrate perceptual information with informa-
2012) and impaired semantic analysis (Leitman et al., 2007). tion stored in long-term memory, to allow for efficient learning,
memory, and reasoning (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011). Enhanced
AUDITORY ATTENTION WM abilities would thus facilitate these other processes, as is the
Blindness has been associated with enhancements in several com- case in C/E blindness (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011), whereas impair-
ponents of attention that are impaired in schizophrenia. For exam- ment would have the opposite effect, as is the case in schizophrenia
ple, on reaction time tasks, blind subjects are superior to sighted (Tek et al., 2002). Thus, it can again be seen how C/E blindness
individuals in responding to auditory and tactile cues (Collignon could protect against development of the cognitive impairments
and De Volder, 2009). On tactile and auditory selective attention that comprise the syndrome of schizophrenia.
tasks involving spatial discrimination, blind people are more effi-
cient than sighted subjects (Roder et al., 1996, 1999; Hotting and LANGUAGE
Roder, 2004; Collignon et al., 2006). Since these studies demon- Congenital or early blind children often experience language delays
strated that blind and sighted participants did not differ in sensory and abnormalities related to semantic, syntactic, and phonological
sensitivity on these tasks, these findings appear to reflect supe- processing (Perez-Pereira and Conti-Ramsden, 1999). One possi-
rior attentional capabilities (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011), although ble consequence of these is a reduction in the risk of developing
teasing apart attentional and perceptual deficits is often difficult. disordered thought, which is commonly seen in schizophrenia. For
Blindness is also associated with superior performance on tasks example, many thought-disordered patients with schizophrenia
of divided attention, such as dichotic listening (Hugdahl et al., have a tendency toward overabstraction and overinclusion in their
2004), and tasks requiring dividing attention across more than one thinking, as well as overly elaborated (and more easily primed)
sensory modality (Kujala et al., 1995). Again, such a pattern of per- semantic networks (Siekmeier and Hoffman, 2002; Lerner et al.,
formance contrasts sharply with what has been long been observed 2012). In addition, compared to healthy controls, individuals with
in schizophrenia, namely, severely impaired selective and divided schizophrenia more often use language in odd ways, including

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 3


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

generating neologisms (i.e., novel words; Solovay et al., 1987; Kre- formation of coherent mental representations in haptics and audi-
her et al., 2008). In contrast, it has been shown that compared to tion requires that information must be “built up” over time to a
sighted children, blind children are characterized by a lack of over- greater extent than in vision (Geenens, 1999). One result of this
generalization of concepts and categories, and a reduced number imposed learning style is that C/E blind individuals develop abil-
of word inventions (Andersen et al., 1984, 1993). It has also been ities in sequential and controlled processing that may be superior
shown that semantic networks in blind children are more depen- to those of sighted people (Vecchi et al., 2004; Raz et al., 2007).
dent on language, and less dependent on sensory experience, than In addition, as we argue below, compensatory processes involving
sighted children (Pring, 1988). This could be a protective factor working memory capacity, perceptual organization via controlled
against delusion formation secondary to altered sensory experi- processing, multisensory integration, learning, and a preserved
ence, as has been hypothesized to exist in schizophrenia (Maher, form of visual imagery lead to a rich, seemingly automatic, and
1974; Uhlhaas and Mishara, 2007). non-fragmented flow of experience (see Cattaneo et al., 2008; Salil-
las et al., 2009 and above; Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011). In contrast,
OLFACTION in schizophrenia, impairments in working memory capacity, per-
Cuevas et al. (2009) demonstrated that congenitally blind males ceptual organization, and multisensory integration, which develop
were better at discriminating odors, and at naming familiar odors, long after birth and for which there appear to be no compensation,
compared to sighted subjects. Other studies have reported mixed contribute to an over-reliance on serial processing that is often
results, for example, demonstrating superior odor identification inefficient, and that can lead to fragmented subjective experience
but not odor sensitivity (Wakefield et al., 2004). However, stud- (Chapman, 1966; Knight, 1984; Carr and Wale, 1986; Knight and
ies with mixed findings have grouped early blind and late-blind Silverstein, 1998; Silverstein and Keane, 2011a). This is illustrated
subjects together, and did not match groups on gender (women in the following quote from a person with schizophrenia: “Every-
are typically better than men at olfaction tasks; Cattaneo and Vec- thing I see is split up. It’s like a photograph that’s torn in bits and
chi, 2011). While more research is needed on this issue, evidence put together again. If somebody moves or speaks, everything I see
does suggest that people born blind may develop increased olfac- disappears quickly and I have to put it together again” (Chapman,
tory abilities (Kupers et al., 2011). This is in contrast to people 1966, p. 229).
with schizophrenia, in which olfactory impairments have been Interesting examples of how blind individuals construct men-
reported (Nguyen et al., 2010; Kamath et al., 2011). Although the tal representations incrementally via haptic perception come from
functional consequences of olfactory impairment in schizophrenia Helen Keller’s (1908) book The World I Live In; her descriptions
are not well understood, such deficits could impair self-regulation, also illustrate how this process can be facilitated by prior knowl-
learning, attachment, and interpersonal functioning (see Sullivan, edge and augmented with imagery. For example, she noted: “My
2003). For example, the detection of maternal odor can promote fingers cannot, of course, get the impression of a large whole at a
attachment to the mother, reduce stress, and help normalize the glance; but I feel the parts, and my mind puts them together” (p.
sleep-wake cycle in infants (Goodin-Jones et al., 1997; Sullivan and 7). Similarly: “My hand has its share in this multiple knowledge,
Toubas, 1998). In addition, odor perception is important in form- but it must never be forgotten that with the fingers I see only a
ing bonds with peers in childhood (Sullivan, 2000). It is therefore very small portion of a surface, and that I must pass my hand con-
interesting that risk for schizophrenia is associated with problems tinually over it before my touch grasps the whole. It is still more
in maternal attachment (demonstrated at 3 days, 1, and 6 years important, however, to remember that my imagination is not teth-
of age; Naslund et al., 1984; Persson-Blennow et al., 1984; McNeil ered to certain points, locations, and distances. It puts all the parts
and Kaij, 1987), increased stress reactivity (Myin-Germeys and van together simultaneously as if it saw or knew instead of feeling
Os, 2007), and fewer peer relationships (Schenkel and Silverstein, them.” (pp. 28–29). It is true of course that vision also incremen-
2004), as well as olfactory processing deficits (Brewer et al., 2003). tally builds distal representations, viz, through saccades or shifts in
attention (Ullman, 1984), but because this process is not limited
CONSTRUCTION OF SUBJECTIVE EXPERIENCE by the speed of grasping and reaching, it plausibly occurs over a
In this section, we discuss: (1) differences between people who much shorter time scale, lessening the load on working memory.
are C/E blind and people with schizophrenia with regard to the In short, while C/E blindness is characterized by a greater than
relative importance of parallel versus serial data acquisition in the normal reliance on serial processing, compensatory processes lead
generation and quality of subjective experience; and (2) the role to superior serial processing abilities and to integrated subjective
that preservation of a form of visual imagery in C/E blindness experience.
may play in the integration of experience. As noted above, C/E In addition, we speculate that a preserved form of visual
blind people rely to a much greater extent than sighted people on imagery is involved in the spatial integration abilities of C/E blind
serial processing due to primary reliance on haptic and auditory people. A reasonable amount of evidence supports the presence
processing, and lack of visual input. That is, due to the temporal- of imagery in C/E blindness, including: (1) visual cortex areas are
sequential nature of data acquisition about objects in auditory activated by input from other senses in blind and non-blind peo-
and haptic perception, the relatively smaller amounts of informa- ple (Amedi et al., 2001, 2002; Ortiz et al., 2011); (2) the occipital
tion available at any one time in haptic perception compared to lobe helps generate mental representations even when there has
vision (Barber and Lederman, 1988; Amadeo and Speicher, 1995; been no prior visual input (Amedi et al., 2008); (3) after training
Herssens, 2010), and the more rapidly changing nature of infor- with tactile stimulation, blind people reported visual qualia, and
mation flow in audition relative to vision (Shamma et al., 2011), the extent of these reports were related to ERP activity in the lateral

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 4


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

occipital complex (Ortiz et al., 2011); (4) C/E blind people have history) who later developed schizophrenia-spectrum disorders
visual content in their dreams, and can draw it, and the extent of had significantly higher eye exam scale and strabismus scale scores
their dream imagery is negatively correlated with EEG alpha band compared to children who developed other non-psychotic psy-
power, just as it is in sighted individuals (Bertolo et al., 2003); chopathology and children who did not develop a mental illness,
(5) C/E blind people report visual imagery (Cornoldi et al., 1979; while functioning in other sensory domains was far less impaired
Zimler and Keenan, 1983), although this is reduced, and more (Schiffman et al., 2006); and (3) the high specificity of visual
verbally mediated, compared to sighted people (Cornoldi et al., abnormalities for predicting transition to schizophrenia among
1979); (6) however, spatial representation is not completely ver- at-risk youth (Klosterkotter, 1992). In all of these cases, even if
bally mediated in blind people; during the encoding and working the presence of visual dysfunction is a reflection of the diathesis
memory maintenance phases of a haptic mental rotation task, for schizophrenia rather than a primary cause of it, the continued
both sighted and blind subjects demonstrated increased parietal state of abnormal visual input may lead to further perceptual and
activation (as demonstrated in ERP data; Roder et al., 1997); (7) cognitive problems throughout development that increase the risk
spatial images are supramodal, in that they are not simply the sum of developing the full-blown disorder.
of modality-specific inputs; (8) there are several characteristics Impaired visual functioning may also compromise multisen-
of visual experience, such as size, contour, and edge information, sory integration, which, in turn, may lead to other cognitive deficits
shape and texture that can be perceived through touch, and blind in schizophrenia. Efficient integration of different sensory inputs
people can generate visual representations of haptically explored requires remapping these into an external spatial reference frame
objects (reviewed in Cattaneo et al., 2008; Cattaneo and Vecchi, (Roder et al., 2007), which requires normal vision to properly
2011); (9) vision and haptics may share common representations develop (Collignon et al., 2009). However, in schizophrenia, there
(Aleman et al., 2001); and (10) both the dorsal and ventral path- are multiple visual processing impairments (e.g., in gain control,
ways are thought to contain supramodal regions to code shape and perceptual organization, motion perception, etc.; Butler et al.,
location of objects, respectively, regardless of the nature of the sen- 2008). Multisensory integration is also impaired in schizophre-
sory input (Pietrini et al., 2009). We suggest, based on the quotes nia (Williams et al., 2010), and it is thought that deficits in visual
cited above, and others in the literature, that these “visual” experi- processing contribute to this (Landgraf et al., 2012). In contrast,
ences and supramodal representations aid in integrating subjective blind people appear to rely on somatotopic spatial coordinates as
experience in C/E blindness. However, direct evidence for this opposed to external, visual, coordinates (Roder et al., 2004). There-
claim has not yet been reported. fore, in contrast to schizophrenia, in C/E blindness, the primary
sense used to create a reference frame for multisensory integra-
C/E BLINDNESS IS PROTECTED FROM NEGATIVE tion (i.e., touch) is associated with superior perceptual abilities
DEVELOPMENTAL EFFECTS OF VISUAL PROCESSING compared to sighted people.
IMPAIRMENTS Blind people rely primarily on vestibular and somatosensory
Vision allows for the simultaneous perception of a great deal of feedback for motor control. This leads to enhanced voluntary
information, and therefore for attention to be spread over many motor control and motor-kinesthetic processing (Deutschlander
stimuli, or to be switched between stimuli. In schizophrenia, with et al., 2009). In contrast, schizophrenia is associated with impaired
its related sensory gating deficits, this can lead to the experience of motor control (Cortese et al., 2005; Putzhammer and Klein, 2006;
being flooded with stimuli, and of stimuli being more intense than Velasques et al., 2011), and it has been hypothesized that this could
usual (Carr and Wale, 1986). In contrast, blindness is associated be related to passivity phenomena and the development of delu-
with touch as the primary method of exploring the environment. sions of control (e.g., the belief that one is not the agent of one’s
The nature of haptic perception is such that the person can only own actions, and/or that an external entity is controlling one’s
attend to the object currently being touched, and in this way thoughts and/or actions; Danckert et al., 2004). Abnormalities in
attention cannot be involuntarily drawn to other objects (unless movement have also been observed in people at high risk for schiz-
there are auditory or olfactory cues to other objects; Cattaneo and ophrenia (Mittal et al., 2008). Therefore, the enhanced develop-
Vecchi, 2011). ment of motor control and kinesthetic processing in C/E blindness
Of course, we are not saying that vision is, by itself, a risk can be viewed as protective against these schizophrenia-related
factor for schizophrenia. However, in combination with sensory symptom formation processes.
gating, perceptual organization, and working memory impair- Finally, it has been recently proposed that visual processing dis-
ments, visual processing, with its parallel inputs, may contribute turbances in individuals at-risk for schizophrenia may contribute
to sensory flooding and subsequent disorganized cognition and to the later development of the disorder by causing impairments
behavior in schizophrenia. That is, visual impairment may be in body perception, involving the senses of “ego boundaries,” body
a risk factor for schizophrenia, not only in being reflective of ownership, body agency, and sense of self in space. These phe-
abnormal neural development but also by being a cause of it. nomena may then lead to problems in sense of identity, and in
Conversely, C/E blindness may exert its protective effects against awareness of symptoms and insight into illness (Landgraf et al.,
schizophrenia, in part, by preventing the possibility of abnormal 2012). Body ownership and agency can be measured experimen-
visual input. Evidence consistent with this comes from several tally with the “rubber hand illusion” (Ehrsson et al., 2004). In this
sources, including: (1) visual dysfunction in children of mothers paradigm, subjects have the experience that they are touching their
with schizophrenia predicts the later development of schizophre- own right hand with their left index finger, when in reality they
nia (Schubert et al., 2005); (2) children (regardless of parental are touching a rubber hand with their left index finger while the

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 5


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

experimenter touches their right hand in a synchronized manner. tasks (which involve detection of a deviation from a sequential
This effect is enhanced in schizophrenia and is thought to reflect pattern in an attended or unattended sequence of stimuli, respec-
abnormal body perception including a reduction in sense of body tively), indicate normal ERP amplitude, with more activity over the
ownership (Thakkar et al., 2011). In contrast, in people with C/E occipital cortex in blind subjects, and enhanced pre-attentional
blindness, the illusion is not experienced (Petkova et al., 2012). processing in blind compared to sighted subjects (Kujala et al.,
These results have been interpreted as indicating that C/E blind 1995; Roder et al., 1996). This is in marked contrast to abnor-
people have a more veridical perception of self-touch, along with mal ERP amplitudes during oddball and MMN tasks in people
a less flexible and dynamic representation of their own body in with schizophrenia (e.g., Kayser et al., 2001) as noted above. Aside
space compared to sighted individuals (Petkova et al., 2012). We from developmental neuroplasticity, other ERP data from oddball
believe that this reduced flexibility in dynamic representation of tasks in people with schizophrenia suggested reduced short-term
the body, secondary to loss of vision, is a protective factor against neuroplasticity. For example, when people with schizophrenia are
the body perception abnormalities associated with schizophrenia presented with a high-frequency sequence of individual audi-
that were noted above. tory stimulations, their ERP signatures reveal abnormally reduced
long-term potentiation (Mears and Spencer, 2012). In contrast,
NEUROPLASTICITY short-term neuroplasticity, at least involving the occipital cortex,
In this section we review evidence on how and where in the brain may be greater in people with C/E blindness (De Volder et al.,
changes occur in response to C/E blindness, and how these sub- 1999).
serve the changes in cognitive functioning discussed above. There A major focus of research on visual processing in schizophrenia
is a large literature on brain plasticity in blind people, including is impaired functioning in the dorsal and ventral visual streams.
evidence for developmental crossmodal plasticity (see below) – Therefore, we briefly summarize evidence that, in contrast to what
where areas of the occipital lobe are recruited for auditory and is observed in schizophrenia, functioning in these pathways is
haptic perception (see Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011 for a review), intact in C/E blindness, despite the absence of visual input. Much
and also for olfaction (Kupers et al., 2011). Beyond this, however, evidence suggests that the dorsal stream is specialized for deter-
other changes in developmental intramodal plasticity can occur mining where an object is and for guiding action, whereas the
which appear to result in perceptual and cognitive abilities that ventral stream is specialized for analyzing form and color for
are opposite to those found in schizophrenia. For example, a mag- determining what an object is (Goodale, 1993; Goodale et al.,
netic source localization study (Elbert et al., 2002) indicated that 2005). Many studies indicate impaired functioning of both of these
dipoles associated with processing low- and high-frequency tones streams in schizophrenia (e.g., King et al., 2008; Silverstein et al.,
were more distant in blind people than in sighted subjects, sug- 2009; Sehatpour et al., 2010; Lalor et al., 2012; Plomp et al., 2012).
gesting an expansion of regions in the auditory cortex, supporting Surprisingly, these pathways appear to subserve similar functions
the superior pitch discrimination abilities noted above. Further in blind people as they do in psychiatrically healthy sighted people,
evidence for intramodal plasticity in blind individuals is reduced suggesting that they can develop even in the absence of visual expe-
hemodynamic responses to auditory stimulation in comparison rience, and therefore that their representations are supramodal
to sighted subjects, which is thought to reflect increased efficiency (Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011; Ptito et al., 2012). For example, dur-
in auditory signal processing within the temporal lobe, in light of ing spatial imagery tasks using auditory or tactile stimuli, early
the generally superior auditory processing abilities in this popula- blind and sighted individuals demonstrated similar dorsal stream
tion (Stevens and Weaver, 2009). Evidence for changes regarding and frontal eye field activation, as assessed via PET or fMRI (Van-
haptic perception comes from findings of reduced accuracy in lierde et al., 2003; Garg et al., 2007; Bonino et al., 2008). In addition,
identifying which finger is touched during a sensory threshold dorsal areas devoted to processing motion information in sighted
task (Sterr et al., 1998, 2003), within the context of overall supe- people (e.g., V5/MT) are employed in blind people in response
rior tactile perception (Wong et al., 2011). These data suggested to tactile and auditory motion cues (e.g., Ptito et al., 2009; Bedny
altered cortical representations for touch in the sense that the cor- et al., 2010; Matteau et al., 2010). These areas are notably underac-
tical representations of individual fingers are “fused” together due tivated during perception of visual motion in schizophrenia, in the
to their simultaneous use during Braille reading. Moreover, these context of impaired motion perception (Chen, 2011). In the ven-
data are strongest for the hand used to read Braille, and do not gen- tral stream, blind subjects demonstrated similar activation during
eralize to other body parts that are not used for reading, such as tactile object perception to that shown by sighted subjects during
lips. Of note, the “holistic” processing of Braille letter patterns and visual and haptic exploration of the same objects (Pietrini et al.,
reduced precision of individual finger-level input is in contrast to 2004; Ptito et al., 2012).
what is often observed in the visual processing of people with schiz- It should be noted that while occipital lobe activity can occur
ophrenia, where processing of holistic visual patterns is impaired, in sighted subjects during tactile and auditory processing tasks,
sometimes with enhanced processing of individual visual details this has been found to be due to visual imagery (Sathian, 2005),
(reviewed in Uhlhaas and Silverstein, 2005; Silverstein and Keane, which activates early visual cortex areas in a retinotopic fashion
2011a). (Slotnick et al., 2005), and to calibration of head-centered sound
Developmental crossmodal plasticity can lead to superior per- localization (Zimmer et al., 2004). In contrast, occipital cortex
formance compared to sighted subjects, and there are several activity in C/E blind subjects appears to support actual non-visual
interesting instances of this relevant to schizophrenia. For example, stimulus processing, and to represent a true crossmodal reorga-
somatosensory and auditory ERP data from oddball and MMN nization (see Roder et al., 1997; Cattaneo and Vecchi, 2011 for a

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 6


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

review). Consistent with this, transcranial magnetic stimulation an increase in NMDA-receptor activity in C/E blindness could have
(TMS), applied to the occipital cortex, disrupts Braille reading widespread positive effects on cognition, and protective effects
ability in blind individuals, but not in sighted individuals, whereas against symptoms.
the reverse is true when TMS is applied over the somatosensory In short, there are several ways in which the neuroplasticity that
cortex in the parietal lobe (Cohen et al., 1997). Similarly, TMS over results from C/E blindness results in changes that are opposite to
the occipital cortex induces tactile sensations in blind readers’ fin- those observed in schizophrenia, and that may represent protec-
gers, but only phosphene perception for sighted subjects (Ptito tive effects. Specifically, the evidence reviewed above suggests that,
et al., 2008). in C/E blindness: (1) despite the loss of visual input, cortical reor-
As noted above, schizophrenia patients have demonstrated ganization during early development leads to accurate form and
olfactory deficits in several studies, while C/E blind individuals space perception (via auditory and tactile processing) which is
have demonstrated superior olfactory abilities. A recent study, supported by brain areas that function abnormally in schizophre-
comparing congenitally blind subjects to blindfolded sighted con- nia (e.g., dorsal and ventral visual streams); (2) this developmental
trols, demonstrated that this is due to enhanced processing in plasticity also leads to use of occipital areas for functions such as
areas normally dedicated to olfaction (e.g., amygdala, hippocam- preattentive processing (e.g., as reflected in the MMN), language,
pus) and in multiple regions of the occipital cortex (Kupers et al., memory, and olfaction – all of which are impaired in schizophre-
2011). This is further evidence of both the supramodal nature of nia – which may underlie the superior functioning in those areas
representations in the C/E visually deprived occipital lobe, and in C/E blindness reviewed above; and (3) in addition to these
also of crossmodal plasticity in C/E blindness. sequelae of developmental plasticity in C/E blindness, there is also
In addition to subserving enhanced non-visual sensory pro- evidence of normal later-onset plasticity, which may in part reflect
cessing, fMRI data indicate that the occipital lobe in C/E blind prolonged plasticity of the occipital cortex in C/E blindness (De
individuals also supports syntactic and semantic (more so than Volder et al., 1999), and which is the basis for the success of sen-
phonological) aspects of language processing (Roder et al., 2002; sory substitution devices (Ward and Meijer, 2010). In contrast,
Burton et al., 2003), as well as verbal memory, episodic mem- later-onset plasticity has been shown to be reduced in schizophre-
ory, and verbal fluency (Amedi et al., 2003; Raz et al., 2005), all nia in several domains (Fitzgerald et al., 2004; Oxley et al., 2004;
of these being processes that are impaired in schizophrenia (Ten- Daskalakis et al., 2008; Hasan et al., 2011, 2012; Mears and Spencer,
dolkar et al., 2002; Fisher et al., 2009; Costafreda et al., 2011). 2012).
For example, in addition to occipital lobe activation during mem-
ory retrieval, and a correlation between occipital activation and CONGENITAL OR EARLY BLINDNESS IS NOT PROTECTIVE
episodic memory in blind but not sighted subjects (Raz et al., AGAINST MENTAL DISORDERS IN GENERAL
2005), repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over While C/E blindness may protect against schizophrenia, they do
the occipital lobe impaired performance in a verb generation task not protect against mental illness in general. A range of mental and
for blind but not sighted subjects (Amedi et al., 2004). behavioral disorders has been reported in blind people. For exam-
Another consequence of blindness may be increased communi- ple, anxiety levels have been reported to be higher in congenitally
cation between brain regions. Several studies have demonstrated blind than sighted adolescents (Bolat et al., 2011), and autistic
increased effective connectivity between the occipital lobe and symptoms and autism are common in blind children (Keeler,
temporal and prefrontal regions in early blind individuals (e.g., 1958; Wing, 1969; Chess, 1971; Chase, 1972; Fraiberg, 1977; Rogers
Noppeney et al., 2003; Leclerc et al., 2005). This is in contrast and Newhart-Larson, 1989; Brown et al., 1997; Ek et al., 1998;
to schizophrenia, where reduced connectivity is typically found Carvill, 2001). Sharp (1993) reported a case of anorexia nervosa
(e.g., Kim et al., 2008; Dima et al., 2009, 2010), including between and depression in a woman blind since the age of 9 months. A
occipital and temporal and frontal regions (Rigucci et al., 2012). recent case study (Kocourkova et al., 2011) described a case of an
Finally, Sanders et al. (2003), based on evidence of visual adolescent patient, blind from early childhood, who showed symp-
cortex plasticity in dark-reared animals (Chen and Bear, 2007; toms of anorexia, depression, suicidal behavior, and self-harming.
Chen et al., 2007), proposed that an effect of early blindness A conclusion in this report was that body image is a mental con-
is increased NMDA-receptor activity (although accompanied by struct that is not dependent on sensory perception. Finally, Musial
reduced numbers of receptors) in visual cortex, and in regions et al. (2007) reported a case of arachnophobia in a congenitally
with which this has many connections, primarily the anterior blind person, suggesting that visual cues are not necessary for the
cingulate cortex (ACC), which is involved in resolving response development of phobic disorders.
competition and context sensitivity, two cognitive domains that
are impaired in schizophrenia (Cohen and Servan-Schreiber, 1992; CONGENITAL DEAFBLINDNESS IS NOT PROTECTIVE
Dolan et al., 1995; Phillips and Silverstein, 2003). Moreover, schiz- AGAINST SCHIZOPHRENIA
ophrenia has been characterized by NMDA-receptor hypofunction Protective effects of blindness may not exist if other forms of severe
(Phillips and Silverstein, 2003), and, in numerous studies, by ACC sensory loss are present. For example, congenital deafblindness
hypoactivation (Dolan et al., 1995). The consequences of NMDA- is associated with elevated rates of psychosis, as well as mental
receptor hypofunction in schizophrenia have been hypothesized retardation (Dammeyer, 2011). Also, children with Usher syn-
to include impairments in sensory gating, perception, working drome, in which people are typically deaf from birth and then
memory, learning, and the full range of positive and negative develop retinitis pigmentosa leading to blindness in childhood,
symptoms (Phillips and Silverstein, 2003; Coyle, 2012). Therefore, has been associated with psychotic symptoms and schizophrenia

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 7


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

(Dammeyer, 2012). The reason why adding deafness to blindness as noted above is involved in multiple perceptual and cognitive
removes the apparent effect of the latter against schizophrenia processes – is thicker than in sighted people, and is thought to
is not clear at present. One possibility is that blindness by itself be characterized by a less than normal amount of pruning, due to
presents a surmountable challenge to cope with the environment deprivation of visual experience (Jiang et al., 2009). A consequence
and thereby fosters compensatory sensory, perceptual, and cogni- of this thickening is that, even if genes related to schizophre-
tive changes that lead to a surprisingly high level of functioning. nia cause excessive pruning in someone with C/E blindness, the
Deafblindness, on the other hand, may so seriously restrict the remaining number of neurons may still be greater than normal in
opportunity for environmental interaction that it also stunts the some areas. This, in combination with the cortical reorganization
development of cognitively based coping strategies. For example, noted above, leading to occipital regions being used for non-visual
congenital deafblindness typically leads to significant delays in cognitive processes, may protect blind people against crossing the
achieving several cognitive milestones, and to profound impair- threshold needed for psychotic symptoms to emerge (i.e., an ade-
ments in speed of processing, the ability to integrate disparate quate number of neurons devoted to specific types of processing
items of information, attention, communication, and the capac- may be present even if pruning is excessive).
ity for symbolic understanding and expression (Geenens, 1999;
Bruce, 2005; Shamma et al., 2011). It is worth noting here that CONCLUSION
although C/E blindness and C/E deafblindness are both relatively This paper builds on previous reports of a lack of schizophrenia
rare, there are no published cases of the former being comorbid in people with C/E blindness by advancing the hypothesis that the
with schizophrenia, whereas there are many reported cases of this latter alters cognition and fosters neuroplasticity in ways that con-
comorbidity with the latter. fer protective effects against schizophrenia. The reviewed evidence
indicates that: (1) schizophrenia is primarily a cognitive disorder;
SENSORY LOSS IN GENERAL DOES NOT PROTECT AGAINST (2) many of the cognitive functions that are impaired in schiz-
SCHIZOPHRENIA ophrenia are enhanced among the C/E blind; (3) C/E blindness
Another important consideration is that sensory loss per se does involves reduced flexibility in language and in dynamic repre-
not protect against schizophrenia. For example, deafness occurs sentation of the body, and these reductions may protect against
in schizophrenia at as high a rate as in the general population thought disorder and alterations in experience of the self, respec-
(Kitson and Fry, 1990), and congenital deafness is a risk factor for tively; (4) the mechanisms noted in 2 and 3 above are significantly
the development of psychotic symptoms (Thewissen et al., 2005; less affected if the onset of blindness is after the first few months
Atkinson, 2006), including, interestingly, higher rates of visual and of life; (5) other forms of C/E sensory loss do not protect against
somatic hallucinations than are found in the non-deaf people with schizophrenia; and (6) C/E blindness does not protect against
schizophrenia (Cutting, 1985), which often co-occur with reports other disorders, suggesting that there is a special link between
of hearing voices (Du Feu and McKenna, 1999). And, while deaf- schizophrenia and visual processing.
ness may be associated with compensatory cognitive changes (see Of course, schizophrenia is characterized by cognitive impair-
Bross, 1979; Dye and Bavelier, 2010; e.g., heightened attention to ments in addition to those described here, such as in executive
visual peripheral cues in people who use sign language; Proksch functioning, that are thought to rely heavily on functioning of
and Bavelier, 2002), these appear to not protect against schizo- the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). There appears to be
phrenia, although they may lead to subtle differences in profiles of almost no research comparing blind (congenital or acquired)
cognitive impairment between deaf and hearing people with schiz- and sighted people on these functions, however, and so whether
ophrenia (Horton and Silverstein, 2007, 2011). Moreover, in some blindness confers any protective effects on them remains an open
cases, compensatory changes may not develop until adulthood question. Note, however, that the DLPFC does not fully develop
(Rettenbach et al., 1999). until late adolescence or early adulthood. In contrast, cognitive and
academic decline, and interpersonal dysfunction, typically begin in
MEDIATING BIOLOGICAL VARIABLES late childhood or early adolescence in people who go on to develop
The hypothesized protective effects of blindness on schizophre- schizophrenia, and this age range corresponds to the time frame in
nia may be mediated by other factors. For example, Horrobin which the visual system in humans is most plastic (i.e., until ∼14–
(1979) proposed that abnormal pineal gland function and result- 16 years of age; Cohen et al., 1999). This suggests that the protective
ing prostaglandin activity are etiological factors in schizophrenia. sequelae of C/E blindness involve changes in more basic sensory,
This is supported by evidence of increased inflammation and perceptual, and cognitive (i.e., non-executive) functions.
reduced melatonin levels in schizophrenia patients (Anderson and While the idea of one abnormal condition having a protec-
Maes, 2012; Muller et al., 2012). Conversely, blind individuals tive effect against another condition may seem unusual, it is not
have elevated melatonin levels (Bellastella et al., 1995), which has unknown. For example, several studies have now replicated the
been shown to attenuate cognitive impairments (Peck et al., 2004) finding that schizophrenia appears to have an inverse relationship
and reduce inflammation (Cuzzocrea et al., 1999; Sharma et al., with rheumatoid arthritis (RA), with some estimates putting the
2012). Another potential mechanism involves cortical thickness risk for RA among schizophrenia patients at 30% that of con-
and pruning. Schizophrenia is typically characterized by cortical trol counterparts (Eaton et al., 1992; Mors et al., 1999; Oken
thinning (Cobia et al., 2012), and it has been hypothesized that and Schulzer, 1999). Conversely, there may also be a reduced risk
excessive neuronal pruning takes place during adolescence (Boksa, for developing schizophrenia among individuals with RA (Gor-
2012). In contrast, the visual cortex of C/E blind people – which wood et al., 2004). This is especially low considering smoking

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 8


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

is a risk factor for developing RA (Stolt et al., 2003) and that a success in improving functioning in these specific areas in humans
disproportionally large percentage (60–90%) of individuals with (Temple et al., 2003; Mahncke et al., 2006; Genevsky et al., 2010)
schizophrenia smoke (Dervaux and Laqueille, 2008). As noted, we and animals (Polley et al., 2006; Zhou et al., 2011). In addition,
hypothesize that the cognitive benefits conferred by C/E blindness although schizophrenia patients often demonstrate reduced plas-
act as a prophylactic for schizophrenia but not other mental dis- ticity, it is not absent, and capacity for perceptual and cognitive
orders. This hypothesis, if confirmed, has implications for deter- change has been demonstrated (Fisher et al., 2009, 2010; Silver-
mining when cognitive training should occur for people with, or stein and Keane, 2009) both in and outside the context of specific
at-risk for, schizophrenia, and for the nature of such training. It interventions. Therefore, it is possible that even greater plasticity
also has implications for whether insights from C/E blindness can can be harnessed earlier in the developmental course of the ill-
be used to design interventions to prevent schizophrenia. Each of ness, before psychotic symptoms emerge and near the onset of,
these issues will be addressed in turn below. or prior to, cognitive and functional decline. In addition, it has
Evidence from animal studies indicates that prevention of already been demonstrated in schizophrenia that improving audi-
schizophrenia-related cognitive impairment can be achieved by tory precision by repeated practicing of tone discrimination leads
early cognitive training. For example, in one study, rats received a to improvements in auditory and (more interestingly) higher level
ventral hippocampus lesion that induces a spatial working mem- cognitive functions (e.g., verbal learning and memory; Adcock
ory deficit similar to that found among schizophrenia patients. et al., 2009; Fisher et al., 2009). Therefore, broader improvements
It was found that among animals that had been provided with (beyond the trained task) may occur in at-risk youth with similar
pre-lesioning cognitive training, the typical post-lesion working practice.
memory deficit did not arise (Lee et al., 2012). While similar pre- An additional strategy for people at-risk for schizophrenia
ventive data do not exist in humans, a recent study of cognitive involves increasing reliance on non-visual sensory modalities to
remediation in first-episode schizophrenia demonstrated that it enhance their functioning. Already it has been shown in peo-
significantly slowed the typical gray matter loss associated with ple with established schizophrenia, for example, that steady-state
the first years after an initial psychotic episode (Eack et al., 2010). auditory responses, which are typically abnormally reduced and
Therefore, it is reasonable to study whether the long-term effects associated with an increase in broadband noise, can be signifi-
of cognitive training in people at high risk for the disorder could be cantly improved, and to a greater degree than occurs in healthy
at least as strong, if not stronger, than effects of cognitive training people, by closing the eyes during task performance (Griskova-
introduced after a diagnosis of schizophrenia is made, given that Bulanova et al., 2012). Fostering cognitive reserve, and trying to
the effects are occurring in the context of a healthier and younger prevent schizophrenia, or at least minimize later illness-related
brain. cognitive impairment, by strengthening capacity and efficiency
But when should training occur? Evidence indicates that inter- in non-visual domains is an as yet untested strategy. How-
vening when people first begin to show psychotic symptoms or ever, the goal of creating a schizophrenia-specific, acquired cog-
functional decline (i.e., in the “ultra high-risk” state) can delay, nitive reserve, in a manner guided by compensatory changes
but does not prevent, schizophrenia (Yung and Nelson, 2011). observed in C/E blindness, would appear to be a reasonable
Therefore, it has recently been proposed that intervening at a and promising direction to pursue, given that premorbid cogni-
“pluripotent risk stage” – the time of the earliest emergence of tive reserve predicts later cognitive functioning in normal aging
behavioral and cognitive difficulties (typically 9–13 years of age) (Steffener and Stern, 2012), neurodegenerative disorders (Koerts
that are non-specific as to final diagnosis – would be more effec- et al., 2012; Stern, 2012), and schizophrenia (de La Serna et al.,
tive (Agius et al., 2010; McGorry, 2010; McGorry et al., 2010; 2013).
Morgan et al., in press). Whether or not such a strategy can pre- Can an understanding of cognition and neuroplasticity in
vent schizophrenia, it may be an ideal time for intensive cognitive C/E blindness be used to reduce the likelihood of schizophre-
training, in terms of affording protection against later and severe nia? At first glance, the answer seems to be “no” since: (1) only
schizophrenia-related cognitive impairment. Of course, even ear- C/E blindness (but not blindness acquired after the first few
lier training could be effective, but the feasibility of identifying months of life) is associated with a protective effect; (2) it is
people at high risk for mental illness at younger than 9-years old difficult to identify most people who will develop schizophre-
has not been well-established, except in cases where an identical nia during the first year of life; (3) it is not clear what type
twin or first-degree relative has the disorder. of training could be given to people less than 1-year old, given
What sort of training would be effective? Based on the evidence limited attention span and undeveloped verbal abilities; and (4)
reviewed above, we suggest that cognitive training in young peo- the presence of vision is likely to reduce the extent of devel-
ple at-risk for schizophrenia should be comprehensive in nature, opmental neuroplasticity. However, the ultimate answer to this
and should focus to a greater degree than it has in the past on question may not be this simple. For example, much remains to
sensory and perceptual functioning. For example, in addition to be learned about biological and computational changes associ-
a focus on improving aspects of attention, it should focus on ated with C/E, and later-developing, blindness, and so further
improving sensory tuning and the accuracy of sensory represen- investigation into these issues may reveal clues for intervention
tations, perceptual organization, multisensory integration, and development. Also, it is not known beyond what age fostering neu-
controlled processing including integration of bottom-up sen- roplasticity and cognitive enhancement might no longer be able
sory data with stored information (see Heller quotes above, and to prevent schizophrenia, but only reduce schizophrenia-related
Hemsley, 1996). Some interventions have already demonstrated cognitive impairment. Third, since it is not C/E blindness per se,

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 9


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

but rather, the corresponding brain and cognitive changes that ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
are protective against schizophrenia, it is possible that methods We thank Thomas Papathomas, Matt Roche, Keith Feigenson,
other than naturally occurring C/E blindness can confer protec- James Gold, and Judy Thompson for their helpful comments on
tive effects. For example, an open question is the extent to which earlier drafts of this manuscript. We also thank Ms. Arwen Lockley
particularly intensive sensory-perceptual-cognitive interventions for her helpful insights on blindness and schizophrenia, and the
delivered to at-risk youth past the first year of life can create some two reviewers for their helpful suggestions. This work was sup-
of the protective changes associated with naturally occurring C/E ported by a National Research Service Award (F32MH094102) to
blindness. Brian P. Keane, and R01MH093439 to Steven M. Silverstein.

REFERENCES correlates of drawing in an early Bertolo, H., Paiva, T., Pessoa, L., detection analysis of a visual dis-
Abely, P., and Carton, C. (1967). [The blind painter: a case study. Brain Res. Mestre, T., Marques, R., and San- crimination task. Percept. Mot. Skills
man of night and the man of 1242, 252–262. tos, R. (2003). Visual dream content, 48, 187–194.
silence. Prologue to an attempted Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, graphical representation and EEG Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A.,
comparative psychopathologic and R., and Zohary, E. (2003). Early alpha activity in congenitally blind and Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there
psychiatric study of the blind and ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates subjects. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. “autistic-like” features in congeni-
deaf]. Ann. Med. Psychol. (Paris) 125, with superior verbal memory per- 15, 277–284. tally blind children? J. Child Psychol.
111–125. formance in the blind. Nat. Neurosci. Bleuler, E. (1950). Dementia Prae- Psychiatry 38, 693–703.
Adcock, R. A., Dale, C., Fisher, M., 6, 758–766. cox or the Group of Schizophrenias. Bruce, S. M. (2005). The impact of con-
Aldebot, S., Genevsky, A., Simpson, American Psychiatric Association. New York: International Universities genital deafblindness on the struggle
G. V., et al. (2009). When top- (1980). Diagnostic and Statisti- Press. to symbolism. Int. J. Disabil. Dev.
down meets bottom-up: auditory cal Manual of Mental Disorders, Boksa, P. (2012). Abnormal synap- Educ. 52, 223–251.
training enhances verbal memory in 3rd Edn. Washington: American tic pruning in schizophrenia: urban Bruder, G., Kayser, J., Tenke, C., Amador,
schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 35, Psychiatric Association. myth or reality? J. Psychiatry Neu- X., Friedman, M., Sharif, Z., et al.
1132–1141. Andersen, E. S., Dunlea, A., and Keke- rosci. 37, 75–77. (1999). Left temporal lobe dysfunc-
Agius, M., Goh, C., Ulhaq, S., and lis, L. S. (1984). Blind children’s lan- Bolat, N., Dogangun, B., Yavuz, M., tion in schizophrenia: event-related
McGorry, P. (2010). The staging guage: resolving some differences. J. Demir, T., and Kayaalp, L. (2011). potential and behavioral evidence
model in schizophrenia, and its clin- Child Lang. 11, 645–664. Depression and anxiety levels and from phonetic and tonal dichotic lis-
ical implications. Psychiatr. Danub. Andersen, E. S., Dunlea, A., and Keke- self-concept characteristics of ado- tening tasks. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry
22, 211–220. lis, L. S. (1993). The impact of input: lescents with congenital complete 56, 267–276.
Aleman, A., Van Lee, L., Mantione, M. language acquisition in the visually visual impairment. Turk. Psikiyatri. Bull, R., Rathborn, H., and Clifford, B. R.
H., Verkoijen, I. G., and De Haan, impaired. First Lang. 13, 23–49. Derg. 22, 77–82. (1983). The voice-recognition accu-
E. H. (2001). Visual imagery with- Anderson, G., and Maes, M. (2012). Bonino, D., Ricciardi, E., Sani, L., Gen- racy of blind listeners. Perception 12,
out visual experience: evidence from Melatonin: an overlooked factor in tili, C., Vanello, N., Guazzelli, M., 223–226.
congenitally totally blind people. schizophrenia and in the inhibition et al. (2008). Tactile spatial working Burton, H., Diamond, J. B., and
Neuroreport 12, 2601–2604. of anti-psychotic side effects. Metab. memory activates the dorsal extras- McDermott, K. B. (2003). Dissoci-
Altshuler, L. L., Ventura, J., Van Gorp, Brain Dis. 27, 113–119. triate cortical pathway in congen- ating cortical regions activated by
W. G., Green, M. F., Theberge, D. C., Atkinson, J. R. (2006). The per- itally blind individuals. Arch. Ital. semantic and phonological tasks:
and Mintz, J. (2004). Neurocognitive ceptual characteristics of Biol. 146, 133–146. a FMRI study in blind and
function in clinically stable men with voice-hallucinations in deaf people: Brahmbhatt, S. B., Haut, K., Cser- sighted people. J. Neurophysiol. 90,
bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia insights into the nature of subvocal nansky, J. G., and Barch, D. M. 1965–1982.
and normal control subjects. Biol. thought and sensory feedback loops. (2006). Neural correlates of verbal Butler, P. D., Silverstein, S. M., and
Psychiatry 56, 560–569. Schizophr. Bull. 32, 701–708. and nonverbal working mem- Dakin, S. C. (2008). Visual percep-
Amadeo, D., and Speicher, K. (1995). Ballard, C. G., O’Brien, J. T., Swann, ory deficits in individuals with tion and its impairment in schizo-
Essential environmental and spatial A. G., Thompson, P., Neill, D., and schizophrenia and their high- phrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 40–47.
concerns for the congenitally visu- McKeith, I. G. (2001). The natural risk siblings. Schizophr. Res. 87, Carr, V., and Wale, J. (1986). Schizo-
ally impaired. J. Plan. Educ. Res. 14, history of psychosis and depression 191–204. phrenia: an information processing
113–122. in dementia with Lewy bodies and Brebion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M. J., model. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 20,
Amedi, A., Floel, A., Knecht, S., Zohary, Alzheimer’s disease: persistence and and Gorman, J. M. (1997). Mecha- 136–155.
E., and Cohen, L. G. (2004). Tran- new cases over 1 year of follow-up. J. nisms underlying memory impair- Carvill, S. (2001). Sensory impairments,
scranial magnetic stimulation of the Clin. Psychiatry 62, 46–49. ment in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. intellectual disability and psychi-
occipital pole interferes with verbal Barber, P. O., and Lederman, S. J. (1988). 27, 383–393. atry. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 45,
processing in blind subjects. Nat. Encoding direction in manipula- Brebion, G., David, A. S., Jones, H., 467–483.
Neurosci. 7, 1266–1270. tory space. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 82, and Pilowsky, L. S. (2004). Seman- Cattaneo, Z., and Vecchi, T. (2011).
Amedi, A., Jacobson, G., Hendler, T., 99–106. tic organization and verbal mem- Blind Vision. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Malach, R., and Zohary, E. (2002). Bedny, M., Konkle, T., Pelphrey, K., ory efficiency in patients with Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Cornoldi, C.,
Convergence of visual and tactile Saxe, R., and Pascual-Leone, A. schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 18, Mammarella, I., Bonino, D., Riccia-
shape processing in the human lat- (2010). Sensitive period for a mul- 378–383. rdi, E., et al. (2008). Imagery and
eral occipital complex. Cereb. Cortex timodal response in human visual Brewer, W. J., Wood, S. J., McGorry, P. D., spatial processes in blindness and
12, 1202–1212. motion area MT/MST. Curr. Biol. 20, Francey, S. M., Phillips, L. J., Yung, visual impairment. Neurosci. Biobe-
Amedi, A., Malach, R., Hendler, T., 1900–1906. A. R., et al. (2003). Impairment hav. Rev. 32, 1346–1360.
Peled, S., and Zohary, E. (2001). Bellastella, A., Sinisi, A. A., Criscuolo, of olfactory identification ability in Chapman, J. (1966). The early symp-
Visuo-haptic object-related activa- T., De Bellis, A., Carella, C., Iorio, individuals at ultra-high risk for toms of schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychi-
tion in the ventral visual pathway. S., et al. (1995). Melatonin and psychosis who later develop schiz- atry 112, 225–251.
Nat. Neurosci. 4, 324–330. the pituitary-thyroid axis status in ophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, Chase, J. B. (1972). Retrolental Fibro-
Amedi, A., Merabet, L. B., Camprodon, blind adults: a possible resetting after 1790–1794. plasia and Autistic Symptomatology.
J., Bermpohl, F., Fox, S., Ronen, I., puberty. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 43, Bross, M. (1979). Residual sensory New York, NY: New York America
et al. (2008). Neural and behavioral 707–711. capacities of the deaf: a signal Foundation for the Blind.

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 10


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

Checkley, S. A., and Slade, A. P. (1979). Conklin, H. M., Curtis, C. E., Calkins, parietal cortex. Schizophr. Res. 70, Eaton, W. W., Hayward, C., and Ram, R.
Blindness and schizophrenia. Lancet M. E., and Iacono, W. G. (2005). 241–261. (1992). Schizophrenia and rheuma-
313, 730–731. Working memory functioning in Daskalakis, Z. J., Christensen, B. K., toid arthritis: a review. Schizophr.
Chen, Q., He, S., Hu, X. L., Yu, J., schizophrenia patients and their Fitzgerald, P. B., and Chen, R. (2008). Res. 6, 181–192.
Zhou, Y., Zheng, J., et al. (2007). first-degree relatives: cognitive func- Dysfunctional neural plasticity in Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., and Pass-
Differential roles of NR2A- and tioning shedding light on etiology. patients with schizophrenia. Arch. ingham, R. E. (2004). That’s my
NR2B-containing NMDA receptors Neuropsychologia 43, 930–942. Gen. Psychiatry 65, 378–385. hand! Activity in premotor cortex
in activity-dependent brain-derived Corlett, P. R., Frith, C. D., and de La Serna, E., Andres-Perpina, S., Puig, reflects feeling of ownership of a
neurotrophic factor gene regulation Fletcher, P. C. (2009). From drugs O., Baeza, I., Bombin, I., Bartres-Faz, limb. Science 305, 875–877.
and limbic epileptogenesis. J. Neu- to deprivation: a Bayesian frame- D., et al. (2013). Cognitive reserve as Ek, U., Fernell, E., Jacobson, L.,
rosci. 27, 542–552. work for understanding models a predictor of two year neuropsycho- and Gillberg, C. (1998). Relation
Chen, W. S., and Bear, M. F. (2007). of psychosis. Psychopharmacology logical performance in early onset between blindness due to retinopa-
Activity-dependent regulation of (Berl.) 206, 515–530. first-episode schizophrenia. Schizo- thy of prematurity and autistic spec-
NR2B translation contributes to Corlett, P. R., Honey, G. D., and Fletcher, phr. Res. 143, 125–131. trum disorders: a population based
metaplasticity in mouse visual P. C. (2007). From prediction error De Volder, A. G., Catalan-Ahumada, study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 40,
cortex. Neuropharmacology 52, to psychosis: ketamine as a phar- M., Robert, A., Bol, A., Labar, D., 297–301.
200–214. macological model of delusions. Coppens, A., et al. (1999). Changes Elbert, T., Sterr, A., Rockstroh, B., Pan-
Chen, Y. (2011). Abnormal visual J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxford) 21, in occipital cortex activity in early tev, C., Muller, M. M., and Taub, E.
motion processing in schizophre- 238–252. blind humans using a sensory sub- (2002). Expansion of the tonotopic
nia: a review of research progress. Cornoldi, C., Calore, D., and Pra-Baldi, stitution device. Brain Res. 826, area in the auditory cortex of the
Schizophr. Bull. 37, 709–715. A. (1979). Imagery rating and recall 128–134. blind. J. Neurosci. 22, 9941–9944.
Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children in congenitally blind subjects. Per- Dervaux, A., and Laqueille, X. (2008). Feierman, J. R. (1982). Nocturnalism:
with congenital rubella. J. Autism cept. Mot. Skills 48, 627–639. [Smoking and schizophrenia: epi- an ethological theory of schizophre-
Child. Schizophr. 1, 33–47. Cortese, L., Caligiuri, M. P., Malla, demiological and clinical features]. nia. Med. Hypotheses 9, 455–479.
Chevigny, H., and Braverman, S. (1950). A. K., Manchanda, R., Takhar, J., Encephale 34, 299–305. Ffytche, D. H. (2007). Visual hallucina-
The Adjustment of the Blind. New and Haricharan, R. (2005). Relation- Deutschlander, A., Stephan, T., Hufner, tory syndromes: past, present, and
Haven: Yale University Press. ship of neuromotor disturbances K., Wagner, J., Wiesmann, M., future. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 9,
Clark, A. (in press). Whatever (next)? to psychosis symptoms in first- Strupp, M., et al. (2009). Imagined 173–189.
Predictive brains, situated agents, episode neuroleptic-naive schizo- locomotion in the blind: an fMRI Fisher, M., Holland, C., Merzenich,
and the future of cognitive science. phrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 75, study. Neuroimage 45, 122–128. M. M., and Vinogradov, S. (2009).
Behav. Brain Sci. 65–75. Dima, D., Dietrich, D. E., Dillo, W., and Using neuroplasticity-based audi-
Cobia, D. J., Smith, M. J., Wang, L., and Costafreda, S. G., Fu, C. H., Picchioni, Emrich, H. M. (2010). Impaired top- tory training to improve verbal
Csernansky, J. G. (2012). Longitudi- M., Toulopoulou, T., McDonald, C., down processes in schizophrenia: a memory in schizophrenia. Am. J.
nal progression of frontal and tem- Kravariti, E., et al. (2011). Pat- DCM study of ERPs. Neuroimage 52, Psychiatry 166, 805–811.
poral lobe changes in schizophrenia. tern of neural responses to ver- 824–832. Fisher, M., Holland, C., Subrama-
Schizophr. Res. 139, 1–6. bal fluency shows diagnostic speci- Dima, D., Roiser, J. P., Dietrich, D. niam, K., and Vinogradov, S.
Cohen, J. D., and Servan-Schreiber, ficity for schizophrenia and bipo- E., Bonnemann, C., Lanfermann, (2010). Neuroplasticity-based cog-
D. (1992). Context, cortex, and lar disorder. BMC Psychiatry 11:18. H., Emrich, H. M., et al. (2009). nitive training in schizophrenia: an
dopamine: a connectionist approach doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-18 Understanding why patients with interim report on the effects 6
to behavior and biology in schizo- Coyle, J. T. (2012). NMDA receptor schizophrenia do not perceive the months later. Schizophr. Bull. 36,
phrenia. Psychol. Rev. 99, 45–77. and schizophrenia: a brief history. hollow-mask illusion using dynamic 869–879.
Cohen, L. G., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, Schizophr. Bull. 38, 920–926. causal modelling. Neuroimage 46, Fitzgerald, P. B., Brown, T. L., Marston,
A., Corwell, B., Falz, L., Dambrosia, Cuevas, I., Plaza, P., Rombaux, P., 1180–1186. N. A., Oxley, T., De Castella, A.,
J., et al. (1997). Functional relevance De Volder, A. G., and Renier, Dolan, R. J., Fletcher, P., Frith, C. D., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2004).
of cross-modal plasticity in blind L. (2009). Odour discrimination Friston, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S., and Reduced plastic brain responses in
humans. Nature 389, 180–183. and identification are improved Grasby, P. M. (1995). Dopaminergic schizophrenia: a transcranial mag-
Cohen, L. G., Weeks, R. A., Sadato, N., in early blindness. Neuropsychologia modulation of impaired cognitive netic stimulation study. Schizophr.
Celnik, P., Ishii, K., and Hallett, M. 47, 3079–3083. activation in the anterior cingulate Res. 71, 17–26.
(1999). Period of susceptibility for Cutting, J. (1985). The Psychology of cortex in schizophrenia. Nature 378, Foucher, J. R., Lacambre, M., Pham,
cross-modal plasticity in the blind. Schizophrenia. Edinburgh: Living- 180–182. B. T., Giersch, A., and Elliott, M.
Ann. Neurol. 45, 451–460. stone. Du Feu, M., and McKenna, P. J. (1999). A. (2007). Low time resolution in
Collignon, O., and De Volder, A. G. Cuzzocrea, S., Costantino, G., Mazzon, Prelingually profoundly deaf schiz- schizophrenia Lengthened windows
(2009). Further evidence that con- E., and Caputi, A. P. (1999). Regu- ophrenic patients who hear voices: of simultaneity for visual, auditory
genitally blind participants react lation of prostaglandin production a phenomenological analysis. Acta and bimodal stimuli. Schizophr. Res.
faster to auditory and tactile spa- in carrageenan-induced pleurisy by Psychiatr. Scand. 99, 453–459. 97, 118–127.
tial targets. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, melatonin. J. Pineal Res. 27, 9–14. Dye, M. W., and Bavelier, D. (2010). Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the
287–293. Dammeyer, J. (2011). Mental and Attentional enhancements and Blind. London: Souvenir.
Collignon, O., Renier, L., Bruyer, R., behavioral disorders among people deficits in deaf populations: an Garg, A., Schwartz, D., and Stevens, A.
Tranduy, D., and Veraart, C. (2006). with congenital deafblindness. Res. integrative review. Restor. Neurol. A. (2007). Orienting auditory spatial
Improved selective and divided spa- Dev. Disabil. 32, 571–575. Neurosci. 28, 181–192. attention engages frontal eye fields
tial attention in early blind subjects. Dammeyer, J. (2012). Children with Eack, S. M., Hogarty, G. E., Cho, R. Y., and medial occipital cortex in con-
Brain Res. 1075, 175–182. Usher syndrome: mental and behav- Prasad, K. M., Greenwald, D. P., Hog- genitally blind humans. Neuropsy-
Collignon, O., Voss, P., Lassonde, M., ioral disorders. Behav. Brain Funct. arty, S. S., et al. (2010). Neuroprotec- chologia 45, 2307–2321.
and Lepore, F. (2009). Cross-modal 8, 16. tive effects of cognitive enhancement Geenens, D. L. (1999). “Neurobio-
plasticity for the spatial process- Danckert, J., Saoud, M., and Maruff, therapy against gray matter loss in logical development and cognition
ing of sounds in visually deprived P. (2004). Attention, motor control early schizophrenia: results from a in the deafblind,” in A Guide to
subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 192, and motor imagery in schizophre- 2-year randomized controlled trial. Planning and Support for Individ-
343–358. nia: implications for the role of the Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 674–682. uals Who are Deafblind, ed. J. M.

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 11


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

McInnes (Toronto: University of plasticity in schizophrenia revealed attentional sensitivity for identifica- in Blind Children with Retrolen-
Toronto Press), 150–174. by transcranial direct current stimu- tion of speech sounds. Brain Res. tal Fibroplasia. New York: Grune &
Genevsky, A., Garrett, C. T., Alexander, lation. Behav. Brain Res. 224, 15–22. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 28–32. Stratton.
P. P., and Vinogradov, S. (2010). Heinrichs, R. W., and Zakzanis, K. Hull, T., and Mason, H. (1995). Per- Keller, H. (1908). The World I Live In.
Cognitive training in schizophrenia: K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in formance of blind children on digit New York: Grosset and Dunlop.
a neuroscience-based approach. schizophrenia: a quantitative review span tests. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 89, Khan, R. (in press). “Why Kraepelin
Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 12, of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 166–169. was right: schizophrenia as a cogni-
416–421. 426–445. Ilankovic, L. M., Allen, P. P., Engel, tive disorder,” in Schizophrenia: Evo-
Gold, R., Butler, P., Revheim, N., Leit- Hemsley, D. R. (1996). Schizophrenia. R., Kambeitz, J., Riedel, M., Muller, lution and Synthesis, eds S. Silver-
man, D. I., Hansen, J. A., Gur, R. C., et A cognitive model and its implica- N., et al. (2011). Attentional mod- stein, B. Moghaddam, and T. Wykes
al. (2012). Auditory emotion recog- tions for psychological intervention. ulation of external speech attribu- (Cambridge: MIT Press).
nition impairments in schizophre- Behav. Modif. 20, 139–169. tion in patients with hallucinations Kim, D., Burge, J., Lane, T., Pearl-
nia: relationship to acoustic features Herssens, J. (2010). “Haptic design and delusions. Neuropsychologia 49, son, G. D., Kiehl, K. A., and Cal-
and cognition. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, research: a blind sense of 805–812. houn, V. D. (2008). Hybrid ICA-
424–432. place,” in Proceedings of the 7th Iyer, D., Boutros, N. N., and Zouri- Bayesian network approach reveals
Goodale, M. A. (1993). Visual pathways ARCC/EAAE 2010 International dakis, G. (2012). Single-trial distinct effective connectivity differ-
supporting perception and action in Conference on Architectural analysis of auditory evoked ences in schizophrenia. Neuroimage
the primate cerebral cortex. Curr. Research (Washington, DC: potentials improves separation 42, 1560–1568.
Opin. Neurobiol. 3, 578–585. EAAE-ARCC). Available at: http: of normal and schizophrenia King, J. P., Christensen, B. K., and
Goodale, M. A., Kroliczak, G., and West- //www.aia.org/aiaucmp/groups/ subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, Westwood, D. A. (2008). Grasping
wood, D. A. (2005). Dual routes aia/documents/pdf/aiab087187.pdf 1810–1820. behavior in schizophrenia suggests
to action: contributions of the dor- [accessed December 27, 2012]. Javitt, D. C., Strous, R. D., Gro- selective impairment in the dorsal
sal and ventral streams to adap- Hertrich, I., Dietrich, S., Moos, A., Trou- chowski, S., Ritter, W., and Cowan, visual pathway. J. Abnorm. Psychol.
tive behavior. Prog. Brain Res. 149, vain, J., and Ackermann, H. (2009). N. (1997). Impaired precision, but 117, 799–811.
269–283. Enhanced speech perception capa- normal retention, of auditory sen- Kitson, N., and Fry, R. (1990). Prelin-
Goodin-Jones, B. L., Eiben, L. A., and bilities in a blind listener are asso- sory (“echoic”) memory informa- gual deafness and psychiatry. Br. J.
Anders, T. F. (1997). Maternal, well- ciated with activation of fusiform tion in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Hosp. Med. 44, 353–356.
being and sleep-wake behaviors in gyrus and primary visual cortex. Psychol. 106, 315–324. Klosterkotter, J. (1992). The meaning
infants: an intervention using mater- Neurocase 15, 163–170. Jiang, J., Zhu, W., Shi, F., Liu, Y., Li, J., of basic symptoms for the genesis
nal odor. Infant Ment. Health J. 18, Hirano, S., Hirano, Y., Maekawa, T., Qin, W., et al. (2009). Thick visual of the schizophrenic nuclear syn-
378–393. Obayashi, C., Oribe, N., Kuroki, cortex in the early blind. J. Neurosci. drome. Jpn. J. Psychiatry Neurol. 46,
Gorwood, P., Pouchot, J., Vinceneux, T., et al. (2008). Abnormal neural 29, 2205–2211. 609–630.
P., Puechal, X., Flipo, R. M., De oscillatory activity to speech sounds Johns, L. C., Rossell, S., Frith, C.,Ahmad, Knight, R. A. (1984). “Converging mod-
Bandt, M., et al. (2004). Rheumatoid in schizophrenia: a magnetoen- F., Hemsley, D., Kuipers, E., et al. els of cognitive deficits in schiz-
arthritis and schizophrenia: a neg- cephalography study. J. Neurosci. 28, (2001). Verbal self-monitoring and ophrenia,” in Nebraska Symposium
ative association at a dimensional 4897–4903. auditory verbal hallucinations in on Motivation: Theories of Schizo-
level. Schizophr. Res. 66, 21–29. Horan, W. P., Braff, D. L., Nuechterlein, patients with schizophrenia. Psychol. phrenia and Psychosis, ed. J. C. W.
Green, M. F. (1996). What are the func- K. H., Sugar, C. A., Cadenhead, K. S., Med. 31, 705–715. Spaulding (Lincoln, NE: University
tional consequences of neurocogni- Calkins, M. E., et al. (2008). Verbal Kallstrand, J., Montnemery, P., Nielzen, of Nebraska Press), 93–156.
tive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. working memory impairments in S., and Olsson, O. (2002). Auditory Knight, R. A., and Silverstein, S. M.
Psychiatry 153, 321–330. individuals with schizophrenia and masking experiments in schizophre- (1998). “The role of cognitive psy-
Green, M. F., Hugdahl, K., and Mitchell, their first-degree relatives: findings nia. Psychiatry Res 113, 115–125. chology in guiding research on cog-
S. (1994). Dichotic listening during from the Consortium on the Genet- Kamath, V., Turetsky, B. I., Calkins, M. nitive deficits in schizophrenia,” in
auditory hallucinations in patients ics of Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. E., Kohler, C. G., Conroy, C. G., Origins and Development of Schiz-
with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 103, 218–228. Borgmann-Winter, K., et al. (2011). ophrenia: Advances in Experimental
151, 357–362. Horrobin, D. F. (1979). Schizophre- Olfactory processing in schizophre- Psychopathology, ed. R. H. D. M.
Griskova-Bulanova, I., Dapsys, K., nia: reconciliation of the dopamine, nia, non-ill first-degree family mem- Lenzenweger (Washington, DC: APA
Maciulis, V., and Arnfred, S. M. prostaglandin, and opioid concepts bers, and young people at-risk for Press), 247–295.
(2012). Closed eyes condition and the role of the pineal. Lancet 1, psychosis. World J. Biol. Psychia- Kocourkova, J., Soltysova, M.,
increases auditory brain responses 529–531. try. PMID:22070564. [Epub ahead Mohaplova, M., and Hrdlicka,
in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. doi: Horton, H. K., and Silverstein, S. M. of print]. M. (2011). Anorexia nervosa in a
10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.04.004 (2007). Cognition and functional Kantrowitz, J. T., Leitman, D. I., blind girl. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett.
Harvey, P. D., Earle-Boyer, E. A., Weil- outcome among deaf and hearing Lehrfeld, J. M., Laukka, P., Juslin, 32, 748–750.
gus, M. S., and Levinson, J. C. people with schizophrenia. Schizo- P. N., Butler, P. D., et al. (2013). Koerts, J., Tucha, L., Lange, K. W.,
(1986). Encoding, memory, and phr. Res. 94, 187–196. Reduction in tonal discriminations and Tucha, O. (2012). The influ-
thought disorder in schizophrenia Horton, H. K., and Silverstein, S. M. predicts receptive emotion process- ence of cognitive reserve on cog-
and mania. Schizophr. Bull. 12, (2011). Visual context processing ing deficits in schizophrenia and nition in Parkinson’s disease. J.
252–261. deficits in schizophrenia: effects of schizoaffective disorder. Schizophr Neural Transm. PMID:23212723.
Hasan, A., Nitsche, M. A., Herrmann, deafness and disorganization. Schiz- Bull. 39, 86–93. [Epub ahead of print].
M., Schneider-Axmann, T., Mar- ophr. Bull. 37, 716–726. Kayser, J., Bruder, G. E., Tenke, C. E., Kraepelin, E. (1903). Lehrbuch der Psy-
shall, L., Gruber, O., et al. (2012). Hotting, K., and Roder, B. (2004). Hear- Stuart, B. K., Amador, X. F., and chiatrie. Leipzig: Barth.
Impaired long-term depression in ing cheats touch, but less in congen- Gorman, J. M. (2001). Event-related Kreher, D. A., Holcomb, P. J., Goff,
schizophrenia: a cathodal tDCS pilot itally blind than in sighted individu- brain potentials (ERPs) in schiz- D., and Kuperberg, G. R. (2008).
study. Brain Stimul. 5, 475–483. als. Psychol. Sci. 15, 60–64. ophrenia for tonal and phonetic Neural evidence for faster and
Hasan, A., Nitsche, M. A., Rein, B., Hugdahl, K., Ek, M., Takio, F., Rin- oddball tasks. Biol. Psychiatry 49, further automatic spreading acti-
Schneider-Axmann, T., Guse, B., tee, T., Tuomainen, J., Haarala, 832–847. vation in schizophrenic thought
Gruber, O., et al. (2011). Dysfunc- C., et al. (2004). Blind individu- Keeler, W. R. (1958). Autistic Pat- disorder. Schizophr. Bull. 34,
tional long-term potentiation-like als show enhanced perceptual and terns and Defective Communication 473–482.

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 12


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

Kujala, T., Alho, K., Kekoni, J., in an auditory detection and dis- Schizophrenia: Evolution and Synthe- cognitive deficits. Dev. Psychopathol.
Hamalainen, H., Reinikainen, K., crimination task: exploring the ‘self- sis, eds M. B. Silverstein and T. Wykes 24, 529–536.
Salonen, O., et al. (1995). Audi- monitoring’ model of hallucination. (Cambridge: MIT Press). Oken, R. J., and Schulzer, M. (1999).
tory and somatosensory event- Schizophr. Res. 55, 115–128. Mors, O., Mortensen, P. B., and At issue: schizophrenia and rheuma-
related brain potentials in early Light, G. A., and Braff, D. L. (2005a). Ewald, H. (1999). A population- toid arthritis: the negative associ-
blind humans. Exp. Brain Res. 104, Mismatch negativity deficits are based register study of the asso- ation revisited. Schizophr. Bull. 25,
519–526. associated with poor functioning in ciation between schizophrenia and 625–638.
Kupers, R., Beaulieu-Lefebvre, M., schizophrenia patients. Arch. Gen. rheumatoid arthritis. Schizophr. Res. Ortiz, T., Poch, J., Santos, J. M., Requena,
Schneider, F. C., Kassuba, T., Paul- Psychiatry 62, 127–136. 40, 67–74. C., Martinez, A. M., Ortiz-Teran,
son, O. B., Siebner, H. R., et Light, G. A., and Braff, D. L. (2005b). Muchnik, C., Efrati, M., Nemeth, E., L., et al. (2011). Recruitment of
al. (2011). Neural correlates of Stability of mismatch negativity Malin, M., and Hildesheimer, M. occipital cortex during sensory sub-
olfactory processing in congeni- deficits and their relationship to (1991). Central auditory skills in stitution training linked to subjec-
tal blindness. Neuropsychologia 49, functional impairments in chronic blind and sighted subjects. Scand. tive experience of seeing in people
2037–2044. schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, Audiol. 20, 19–23. with blindness. PLoS ONE 6:e23264.
Lalor, E. C., De Sanctis, P., Krakowski, 1741–1743. Muller, N., Myint, A. M., and Schwarz, doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023264
M. I., and Foxe, J. J. (2012). Visual Maher, B. A. (1974). Delusional think- M. J. (2012). Inflammation in schiz- Oxley, T., Fitzgerald, P. B., Brown, T.
sensory processing deficits in schiz- ing and perceptual disorder. J. Indi- ophrenia. Adv. Protein Chem. Struct. L., De Castella, A., Daskalakis, Z. J.,
ophrenia: is there anything to the vid. Psychol. 30, 98–113. Biol. 88, 49–68. and Kulkarni, J. (2004). Repetitive
magnocellular account? Schizophr. Mahncke, H. W., Connor, B. B., Appel- Musial, F., Kolassa, I. T., Sulzenbruck, S., transcranial magnetic stimulation
Res. 139, 246–252. man, J., Ahsanuddin, O. N., Hardy, J. and Miltner, W. H. (2007). A case reveals abnormal plastic response
Landgraf, S., Amado, I., Berthoz, A., L., Wood, R. A., et al. (2006). Mem- of spider phobia in a congenitally to premotor cortex stimulation in
Krebs, M., and Van Der Meer, ory enhancement in healthy older blind person. Psychiatry Res. 153, schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 56,
E. (2012). Cognitive identify in adults using a brain plasticity-based 97–101. 628–633.
schizophrenia: vision, space, and training program: a randomized, Myin-Germeys, I., and van Os, J. (2007). Palmer, B. W., Dawes, S. E., and Heaton,
body perception from prodrome to controlled study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Stress-reactivity in psychosis: evi- R. K. (2009). What do we know
syndrome. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 8, Sci. U.S.A. 103, 12523–12528. dence for an affective pathway to about neuropsychological aspects of
119–139. Matteau, I., Kupers, R., Ricciardi, E., psychosis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, schizophrenia? Neuropsychol. Rev.
Landgraf, S., Steingen, J., Eppert, Y., Nie- Pietrini, P., and Ptito, M. (2010). 409–424. 19, 365–384.
dermeyer, U., Van Der Meer, E., and Beyond visual, aural and haptic Naslund, B., Persson-Blennow, I., Peck, J. S., Legoff, D. B., Ahmed, I., and
Krueger, F. (2011). Temporal infor- movement perception: hMT+ is McNeil, T., Kaij, L., and Malmquist- Goebert, D. (2004). Cognitive effects
mation processing in short- and activated by electrotactile motion Larsson, A. (1984). Offspring of of exogenous melatonin adminis-
long-term memory of patients with stimulation of the tongue in sighted women with nonorganic psychosis: tration in elderly persons: a pilot
schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 6:e26140. and in congenitally blind individu- infant attachment to the mother study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 12,
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026140 als. Brain Res. Bull. 82, 264–270. at one year of age. Acta Psychiatr. 432–436.
Leclerc, C., Segalowitz, S. J., Des- McGorry, P. D. (2010). Risk syndromes, Scand. 69, 231–241. Perez-Pereira, M., and Conti-Ramsden,
jardins, J., Lassonde, M., and Lep- clinical staging and DSM V: new Neumann, C. S., Grimes, K., Walker, G. (1999). Social Interaction and
ore, F. (2005). EEG coherence in diagnostic infrastructure for early E. F., and Baum, K. (1995). Devel- Language Development in Blind Chil-
early-blind humans during sound intervention in psychiatry. Schizo- opmental pathways to schizophre- dren. Hove: Psychology Press.
localization. Neurosci. Lett. 376, phr. Res. 120, 49–53. nia: behavioral subtypes. J. Abnorm. Perrin, M. A., Butler, P. D., Dicostanzo,
154–159. McGorry, P. D., Nelson, B., Goldstone, Psychol. 104, 558–566. J., Forchelli, G., Silipo, G., and Javitt,
Lee, H., Dvorak, D., Kao, H.-Y., Duffy, A. S., and Yung, A. R. (2010). Clini- Ngan, E. T., Vouloumanos, A., Cairo, T. D. C. (2010). Spatial localization
M., Scharfman, H. E., and Fenton, A. cal staging: a heuristic and practi- A., Laurens, K. R., Bates,A. T.,Ander- deficits and auditory cortical dys-
A. (2012). Early cognitive experience cal strategy for new research and son, C. M., et al. (2003). Abnormal function in schizophrenia. Schizo-
prevents adult deficits in a neurode- better health and social outcomes processing of speech during oddball phr. Res. 124, 161–168.
velopmental schizophrenia model. for psychotic and related mood target detection in schizophrenia. Persson-Blennow, I., Naslund, B.,
Neuron. 75, 714–724. disorders. Can. J. Psychiatry 55, Neuroimage 20, 889–897. McNeil, T. F., Kaij, L., and
Leitman, D. I., Hoptman, M. J., Foxe, J. 486–497. Nguyen, A. D., Shenton, M. E., and Malmquist-Larsson, A. (1984).
J., Saccente, E., Wylie, G. R., Nieren- McNeil, T. F., and Kaij, L. (1987). Levitt, J. J. (2010). Olfactory dys- Offspring of women with nonor-
berg, J., et al. (2007). The neural sub- Swedish high-risk study: sample function in schizophrenia: a review ganic psychosis: mother-infant
strates of impaired prosodic detec- characteristics at age 6. Schizophr. of neuroanatomy and psychophys- interaction at three days of
tion in schizophrenia and its sen- Bull. 13, 373–381. iological measurements. Harv. Rev. age. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 70,
sorial antecedents. Am. J. Psychiatry Mears, R. P., and Spencer, K. M. (2012). Psychiatry 18, 279–292. 149–159.
164, 474–482. Electrophysiological assessment of Niemeyer, W., and Starlinger, I. (1981). Petkova, V. I., Zetterberg, H., and
Leitman, D. I., Wolf, D. H., Laukka, P., auditory stimulus-specific plasticity Do the blind hear better – inves- Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Rubber
Ragland, J. D., Valdez, J. N., Turetsky, in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 71, tigations on auditory processing in hands feel touch, but not in blind
B. I., et al. (2011). Not pitch per- 503–511. congenital or early acquired blind- individuals. PLoS ONE 7:e35912.
fect: sensory contributions to affec- Mittal, V. A., Neumann, C., Saczawa, ness. II. Central functions. Audiology doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0035912
tive communication impairment in M., and Walker, E. F. (2008). Lon- 20, 510–515. Phillips, W. A., and Silverstein, S. M.
schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 70, gitudinal progression of movement Noppeney, U., Friston, K. J., and (2003). Convergence of biological
611–618. abnormalities in relation to psy- Price, C. J. (2003). Effects of visual and psychological perspectives on
Lerner, I., Bentin, S., and Shriki, O. chotic symptoms in adolescents at deprivation on the organization of cognitive coordination in schizo-
(2012). Excessive attractor instabil- high risk of schizophrenia. Arch. the semantic system. Brain 126, phrenia. Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 65–82;
ity accounts for semantic priming in Gen. Psychiatry 65, 165–171. 1620–1627. discussion 82–137.
schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 7:e40663. Morgan, C., O’Donovan, M., Bittner, R., Nuechterlein, K. H., Subotnik, K. L., Pietrini, P., Furey, M. L., Ricciardi,
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040663 Cadenhead, K., Jones, P., McGrath, Ventura, J., Green, M. F., Gretchen- E., Gobbini, M. I., Wu, W. H.,
Li, C. S., Chen, M. C., Yang, Y. Y., J., et al. (in press). “How can risk Doorly, D., and Asarnow, R. F. Cohen, L., et al. (2004). Beyond sen-
and Tsay, P. K. (2002). Altered per- and resilience factors be leveraged (2012). The puzzle of schizophre- sory images: object-based represen-
formance of schizophrenia patients to optimize discovery pathways,” in nia: tracking the core role of tation in the human ventral pathway.

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 13


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, search compensate in adult but not individuals. Neuropsychologia 47, by phosphamidon. Fundam. Clin.
5658–5663. in juvenile subjects. J. Cogn. Neu- 843–848. Pharmacol. doi:10.1111/j.1472-
Pietrini, P., Ptito, M., and Kupers, R. rosci. 11, 560–583. Salillas, E., Grana, A., El-Yagoubi, R., 8206.2011.00977.x
(2009). Blindness and Consciousness: Rigucci, S., Rossi-Espagnet, C., Ferra- and Semenza, C. (2009). Numbers in Sharp, C. W. (1993). Anorexia nervosa
New Light from the Dark. Amster- cuti, S., De Carolis, A., Corigliano, V., the blind’s “eye”. PLoS ONE 4:e6357. and depression in a woman blind
dam: Elsevier. Carducci, F., et al. (2012). Anatomi- doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006357 since the age of nine months. Can.
Plomp, G., Roinishvili, M., Chkonia, cal substrates of cognitive and clini- Sanders, G. S., Platek, S. M., and Gallup, J. Psychiatry 38, 469–471.
E., Kapanadze, G., Kereselidze, M., cal dimensions in first episode schiz- G. G. (2003). No blind schizophren- Siekmeier, P. J., and Hoffman, R. E.
Brand, A., et al. (2012). Electrophys- ophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. doi: ics: are NMDA-receptor dynamics (2002). Enhanced semantic priming
iological evidence for ventral stream 10.1111/acps.12051 involved? Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 103. in schizophrenia: a computer model
deficits in schizophrenia patients. Riscalla, L. M. (1980). Blindness and Sathian, K. (2005). Visual cortical activ- based on excessive pruning of local
Schizophr. Bull. PMID:22258884. schizophrenia. Med. Hypotheses 6, ity during tactile perception in the connections in association cortex.
[Epub ahead of print]. 1327–1328. sighted and the visually deprived. Br. J. Psychiatry 180, 345–350.
Polley, D. B., Steinberg, E. E., and Roder, B., Kusmierek, A., Spence, C., Dev. Psychobiol. 46, 279–286. Silver, H., Feldman, P., Bilker, W., and
Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Percep- and Schicke, T. (2007). Developmen- Schenkel, L. S., and Silverstein, S. Gur, R. C. (2003). Working memory
tual learning directs auditory cor- tal vision determines the reference M. (2004). Dimensions of premor- deficit as a core neuropsychological
tical map reorganization through frame for the multisensory control bid functioning in schizophrenia: dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am. J.
top-down influences. J. Neurosci. 26, of action. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. a review of neuromotor, cogni- Psychiatry 160, 1809–1816.
4970–4982. 104, 4753–4758. tive, social, and behavioral domains. Silverstein, S. M. (in press). Cogni-
Pring, L. (1988). The ‘reverse- Roder, B., and Neville, H. (2003). Devel- Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. tive coordination and psychotic phe-
generation’ effect: a comparison opmental Functional Plasticity. Ams- 130, 241–270. nomena pose challenges to Clark’s
of memory performance between terdam: Elsevier. Schiffman, J., Maeda, J. A., Hayashi, theory emphasizing propagation of
blind and sighted children. Br. J. Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Hennighausen, K., Michelsen, N., Sorensen, H. prediction error and inhibition of
Psychol. 79(Pt 3), 387–400. E. (1997). Different cortical activa- J., Ekstrom, M., et al. (2006). correlated stimuli. Behav. Brain Sci.
Proksch, J., and Bavelier, D. (2002). tion patterns in blind and sighted Premorbid childhood ocular align- Silverstein, S. M., Berten, S., Essex, B.,
Changes in the spatial distribution of humans during encoding and trans- ment abnormalities and adult Kovacs, I., Susmaras, T., and Little, D.
visual attention after early deafness. formation of haptic images. Psy- schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. M. (2009). An fMRI examination of
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 687–701. chophysiology 34, 292–307. Schizophr. Res. 81, 253–260. visual integration in schizophrenia.
Ptito, M., Fumal, A., De Noordhout, Roder, B., Rosler, F., Hennighausen, Scholes, K. E., and Martin-Iverson, M. J. Integr. Neurosci. 8, 175–202.
A. M., Schoenen, J., Gjedde, A., and E., and Nacker, F. (1996). Event- T. (2010). Disturbed prepulse inhi- Silverstein, S. M., Jaeger, J., Donovan-
Kupers, R. (2008). TMS of the occip- related potentials during auditory bition in patients with schizophrenia Lepore, A. M., Wilkniss, S. M., Savitz,
ital cortex induces tactile sensations and somatosensory discrimination is consequential to dysfunction of A., Malinovsky, I., et al. (2010). A
in the fingers of blind Braille readers. in sighted and blind human subjects. selective attention. Psychophysiology comparative study of the MATRICS
Exp. Brain Res. 184, 193–200. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 4, 77–93. 47, 223–235. and IntegNeuro cognitive assess-
Ptito, M., Matteau, I., Gjedde, A., and Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Neville, H. Schubert, E. W., Henriksson, K. M., and ment batteries. J. Clin. Exp. Neu-
Kupers, R. (2009). Recruitment of J. (1999). Effects of interstimulus McNeil, T. F. (2005). A prospective ropsychol. 32, 937–952.
the middle temporal area by tac- interval on auditory event-related study of offspring of women with Silverstein, S. M., and Keane, B. P.
tile motion in congenital blindness. potentials in congenitally blind and psychosis: visual dysfunction in early (2009). Perceptual organization in
Neuroreport 20, 543–547. normally sighted humans. Neurosci. childhood predicts schizophrenia- schizophrenia: plasticity and state-
Ptito, M., Matteau, I., Zhi Wang, Lett. 264, 53–56. spectrum disorders in adulthood. related change. Learn. Percept. 1,
A., Paulson, O. B., Siebner, H. Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Neville, Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 112, 229–261.
R., and Kupers, R. (2012). Cross- H. J. (2001). Auditory memory 385–393. Silverstein, S. M., and Keane, B.
modal recruitment of the ventral in congenitally blind adults: a Sehatpour, P., Dias, E. C., Butler, P. D., P. (2011a). Perceptual organiza-
visual stream in congenital blind- behavioral-electrophysiological Revheim, N., Guilfoyle, D. N., Foxe, tion impairment in schizophrenia
ness. Neural Plast. 2012, 304045. investigation. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain J. J., et al. (2010). Impaired visual and associated brain mechanisms:
Putzhammer, A., and Klein, H. E. Res. 11, 289–303. object processing across an occipital- review of research from 2005 to
(2006). Quantitative analysis of Roder, B., Rosler, F., and Spence, C. frontal-hippocampal brain network 2010. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 690–699.
motor disturbances in schizophrenic (2004). Early vision impairs tactile in schizophrenia: an integrated neu- Silverstein, S. M., and Keane, B. P.
patients. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 8, perception in the blind. Curr. Biol. roimaging study. Arch. Gen. Psychia- (2011b). Vision science and schizo-
123–130. 14, 121–124. try 67, 772–782. phrenia research: toward a re-view
Rabinowicz, E. F., Silipo, G., Goldman, Roder, B., Stock, O., Bien, S., Neville, Sevik, A. E., Anil Yagcioglu, A. E., Yag- of the disorder editors’ introduction
R., and Javitt, D. C. (2000). Auditory H., and Rosler, F. (2002). Speech cioglu, S., Karahan, S., Gurses, N., to special section. Schizophr. Bull. 37,
sensory dysfunction in schizophre- processing activates visual cortex in and Yildiz, M. (2011). Neuropsycho- 681–689.
nia: imprecision or distractibility? congenitally blind humans. Eur. J. logical performance and auditory Silverstein, S. M., Schenkel, L. S., Valone,
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 1149–1155. Neurosci. 16, 930–936. event related potentials in schizo- C., and Nuernberger, S. W. (1998).
Raz, N., Amedi, A., and Zohary, E. Rogers, S. J., and Newhart-Larson, S. phrenia patients and their siblings: Cognitive deficits and psychiatric
(2005). V1 activation in congeni- (1989). Characteristics of infantile a family study. Schizophr. Res. 130, rehabilitation outcomes in schizo-
tally blind humans is associated with autism in five children with Leber’s 195–202. phrenia. Psychiatr. Q. 69, 169–191.
episodic retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 15, congenital amaurosis. Dev. Med. Shamma, S. A., Elhilali, M., and Micheyl, Slotnick, S. D., Thompson, W. L., and
1459–1468. Child Neurol. 31, 598–608. C. (2011). Temporal coherence and Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Visual men-
Raz, N., Striem, E., Pundak, G., Orlov, Rojas, D. C., Slason, E., Teale, P. D., and attention in auditory scene analysis. tal imagery induces retinotopically
T., and Zohary, E. (2007). Superior Reite, M. L. (2007). Neuromagnetic Trends Neurosci. 34, 114–123. organized activation of early visual
serial memory in the blind: a case of evidence of broader auditory corti- Sharma, A. K., Mehta, A. K., Rathor, areas. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1570–1583.
cognitive compensatory adjustment. cal tuning in schizophrenia. Schizo- N., Chalawadi Hanumantappa, M. Solovay, M. R., Shenton, M. E., and
Curr. Biol. 17, 1129–1133. phr. Res. 97, 206–214. K., Khanna, N., and Bhattacharya, Holzman, P. S. (1987). Compara-
Rettenbach, R., Diller, G., and Sireteanu, Rokem, A., and Ahissar, M. (2009). S. K. (2012). Melatonin attenuates tive studies of thought disorders. I.
R. (1999). Do deaf people see bet- Interactions of cognitive and audi- cognitive dysfunction and reduces Mania and schizophrenia. Arch. Gen.
ter? Texture segmentation and visual tory abilities in congenitally blind neural oxidative stress induced Psychiatry 44, 13–20.

Frontiers in Psychology | Psychopathology January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 14


Silverstein et al. Blindness and schizophrenia

Steffener, J., and Stern,Y. (2012). Explor- working memory impairments in Vanlierde, A., De Volder, A. G., Wing, L. (1969). The handicaps of autis-
ing the neural basis of cognitive schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry Wanet-Defalque, M. C., and Ver- tic children – a comparative study. J.
reserve in aging. Biochim. Biophys. 59, 146–153. aart, C. (2003). Occipito-parietal Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 10, 1–40.
Acta 1822, 467–473. Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, cortex activation during visuo- Wong, M., Gnanakumaran, V., and
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., spatial imagery in early blind Goldreich, D. (2011). Tactile spatial
in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2003). humans. Neuroimage 19, 698–709. acuity enhancement in blindness:
Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. Neural deficits in children with Vecchi, T., Tinti, C., and Cornoldi, evidence for experience-dependent
Sterr, A., Green, L., and Elbert, T. dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral C. (2004). Spatial memory and mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 31,
(2003). Blind Braille readers mislo- remediation: evidence from func- integration processes in congen- 7028–7037.
cate tactile stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 63, tional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. ital blindness. Neuroreport 15, Yung, A. R., and Nelson, B. (2011).
117–127. U.S.A. 100, 2860–2865. 2787–2790. Young people at ultra high risk for
Sterr, A., Muller, M. M., Elbert, T., Tendolkar, I., Ruhrmann, S., Brock- Velasques, B., Machado, S., Paes, F., psychosis: a research update. Early
Rockstroh, B., Pantev, C., and Taub, haus, A., Pukrop, R., and Klosterkot- Cunha, M., Sanfim, A., Budde, H., et Interv. Psychiatry 5(Suppl 1), 52–57.
E. (1998). Perceptual correlates ter, J. (2002). Remembering or al. (2011). Sensorimotor integration Zhou, X., Panizzutti, R., De Villers-
of changes in cortical representa- knowing: electrophysiological evi- and psychopathology: motor control Sidani, E., Madeira, C., and
tion of fingers in blind multifin- dence for an episodic memory deficit abnormalities related to psychiatric Merzenich, M. M. (2011). Natural
ger Braille readers. J. Neurosci. 18, in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 32, disorders. World J. Biol. Psychiatry restoration of critical period plastic-
4417–4423. 1261–1271. 12, 560–573. ity in the juvenile and adult primary
Stevens, A. A., and Weaver, K. (2005). Thakkar, K. N., Nichols, H. S., McIn- Vercammen, A., De Haan, E. H., and auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 31,
Auditory perceptual consolidation tosh, L. G., and Park, S. (2011). Aleman, A. (2008). Hearing a voice 5625–5634.
in early-onset blindness. Neuropsy- Disturbances in body ownership in the noise: auditory hallucinations Zimler, J., and Keenan, J. M. (1983).
chologia 43, 1901–1910. in schizophrenia: evidence from and speech perception. Psychol. Med. Imagery in the congenitally blind:
Stevens, A. A., and Weaver, K. E. (2009). the rubber hand illusion and case 38, 1177–1184. how visual are visual images? J.
Functional characteristics of audi- study of a spontaneous out-of-body Wakefield, C. E., Homewood, J., and Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 9,
tory cortex in the blind. Behav. Brain experience. PLoS ONE 6:e27089. Taylor, A. J. (2004). Cognitive com- 269–282.
Res. 196, 134–138. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027089 pensations for blindness in children: Zimmer, U., Lewald, J., Erb, M., Grodd,
Stewart, R. H., and Sardo, R. (1965). Thewissen, V., Myin-Germeys, I., an investigation using odour nam- W., and Karnath, H. O. (2004). Is
The psychotherapy of a blind schiz- Bentall, R., De Graaf, R., Volle- ing. Perception 33, 429–442. there a role of visual cortex in spa-
ophrenic child. J. Am. Acad. Child bergh, W., and Van Os, J. (2005). Wan, C. Y., Wood, A. G., Reutens, D. C., tial hearing? Eur. J. Neurosci. 20,
Psychiatry 4, 123–132. Hearing impairment and psy- and Wilson, S. J. (2010). Early but 3148–3156.
Stolt, P., Bengtsson, C., Nordmark, B., chosis revisited. Schizophr. Res. 76, not late-blindness leads to enhanced
Lindblad, S., Lundberg, I., Klareskog, 99–103. auditory perception. Neuropsycholo-
L., et al. (2003). Quantification of Turetsky, B. I., Bilker, W. B., Siegel, S. J., gia 48, 344–348. Conflict of Interest Statement: The
the influence of cigarette smoking on Kohler, C. G., and Gur, R. E. (2009). Ward, J., and Meijer, P. (2010). Visual authors declare that the research was
rheumatoid arthritis: results from a Profile of auditory information- experiences in the blind induced conducted in the absence of any com-
population based case-control study, processing deficits in schizophrenia. by an auditory sensory substitu- mercial or financial relationships that
using incident cases. Ann. Rheum. Psychiatry Res. 165, 27–37. tion device. Conscious. Cogn. 19, could be construed as a potential con-
Dis. 62, 835–841. Uhlhaas, P. J., and Mishara, A. L. (2007). 492–500. flict of interest.
Sullivan, R. (2000). Review: Olfac- Perceptual anomalies in schizophre- Ward, P. B., Catts, S. V., Fox, A.
tion in the Human Infant. nia: integrating phenomenology and M., Michie, P. T., and McConaghy, Received: 03 November 2012; accepted:
New York: The Fragrance cognitive neuroscience. Schizophr. N. (1991). Auditory selective atten- 31 December 2012; published online: 21
Foundation. Available at: http: Bull. 33, 142–156. tion and event-related potentials in January 2013.
//www.senseofsmell.org/research/R. Uhlhaas, P. J., and Silverstein, S. M. schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry 158, Citation: Silverstein SM, Wang Y and
Sullivan-White-Paper.pdf (2005). Perceptual organization in 534–539. Keane BP (2013) Cognitive and neu-
Sullivan, R. M. (2003). Developing schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Weiss, K. M. (1989). Advantages of roplasticity mechanisms by which con-
a sense of safety: the neurobi- empirical research and theoreti- abandoning symptom-based diag- genital or early blindness may con-
ology of neonatal attachment. cal implications. Psychol. Bull. 131, nostic systems of research in schiz- fer a protective effect against schizo-
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1008, 618–632. ophrenia. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 59, phrenia. Front. Psychology 3:624. doi:
122–131. Ullman, S. (1984). Visual routines. Cog- 324–330. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00624
Sullivan, R. M., and Toubas, P. (1998). nition 18, 97–159. Williams, L. E., Light, G. A., Braff, D. This article was submitted to Frontiers in
Clinical usefulness of maternal odor Umbricht, D., Koller, R., Schmid, L., L., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2010). Psychopathology, a specialty of Frontiers
in newborns: soothing and feeding Skrabo, A., Grubel, C., Huber, T., et Reduced multisensory integration in in Psychology.
preparatory responses. Biol. Neonate al. (2003). How specific are deficits patients with schizophrenia on a tar- Copyright © 2013 Silverstein, Wang and
74, 402–408. in mismatch negativity generation get detection task. Neuropsychologia Keane. This is an open-access article dis-
Tamminga, C. (2013). Psychosis is to schizophrenia? Biol. Psychiatry 53, 48, 3128–3136. tributed under the terms of the Creative
emerging as a learning and mem- 1120–1131. Williams-Gray, C. H., Foltynie, T., Lewis, Commons Attribution License, which
ory disorder. Neuropsychopharma- Van Snellenberg, J. X. (2009). Work- S. J., and Barker, R. A. (2006). Cogni- permits use, distribution and reproduc-
col. Rev. 38, 247. ing memory and long-term memory tive deficits and psychosis in Parkin- tion in other forums, provided the original
Tek, C., Gold, J., Blaxton, T., Wilk, C., deficits in schizophrenia: is there a son’s disease: a review of pathophys- authors and source are credited and sub-
McMahon, R. P., and Buchanan, R. common substrate? Psychiatry Res. iology and therapeutic options. CNS ject to any copyright notices concerning
W. (2002). Visual perceptual and 174, 89–96. Drugs 20, 477–505. any third-party graphics etc.

www.frontiersin.org January 2013 | Volume 3 | Article 624 | 15

View publication stats

You might also like