You are on page 1of 25

Neuropsychology Review

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9388-2

REVIEW

Cognitive Deficits in Psychotic Disorders: A Lifespan Perspective


Julia M. Sheffield 1 & Nicole R. Karcher 2 & Deanna M. Barch 2,3,4

Received: 28 March 2018 / Accepted: 15 October 2018


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Individuals with disorders that include psychotic symptoms (i.e. psychotic disorders) experience broad cognitive impairments in
the chronic state, indicating a dimension of abnormality associated with the experience of psychosis. These impairments
negatively impact functional outcome, contributing to the disabling nature of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and psychotic
depression. The robust and reliable nature of cognitive deficits has led researchers to explore the timing and profile of impair-
ments, as this may elucidate different neurodevelopmental patterns in individuals who experience psychosis. Here, we review the
literature on cognitive deficits across the life span of individuals with psychotic disorder and psychotic-like experiences,
highlighting the dimensional nature of both psychosis and cognitive ability. We identify premorbid generalized cognitive
impairment in schizophrenia that worsens throughout development, and stabilizes by the first-episode of psychosis, suggesting
a neurodevelopmental course. Research in affective psychosis is less clear, with mixed evidence regarding premorbid deficits, but
a fairly reliable generalized deficit at first-episode, which appears to worsen into the chronic state. In general, cognitive impair-
ments are most severe in schizophrenia, intermediate in bipolar disorder, and the least severe in psychotic depression. In all
groups, cognitive deficits are associated with poorer functional outcome. Finally, while the generalized deficit is the clearest and
most reliable signal, data suggests specific deficits in verbal memory across all groups, specific processing speed impairments in
schizophrenia and executive functioning impairments in bipolar disorder. Cognitive deficits are a core feature of psychotic
disorders that provide a window into understanding developmental course and risk for psychosis.

Keywords Psychosis . Cognitive Impairment . Development . Schizophrenia . Bipolar Disorder . Depression

Introduction

Psychosis refers to the experience of delusions, hallucinations,


and thought disorder. In the context of disorders that almost
* Julia M. Sheffield always or often include psychotic symptoms, such as schizophre-
Julia.sheffield@vumc.org nia and bipolar disorder, psychotic symptoms are significant
enough to cause distress or functional impairment. However psy-
Nicole R. Karcher chotic experiences occur on a spectrum. There is evidence that
nkarcher@wustl.edu approximately 20% of individuals in the general population have
Deanna M. Barch subtle psychotic-like experiences (van Os et al. 2009), while
dbarch@wustl.edu approximately 2-5% meet criteria for schizotypal personality dis-
order (Chemerinski et al. 2013), a disorder characterized by mag-
1
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt ical thinking, ideas of reference, and perceptual aberrations that
University Medical Center, 1601 23rd Ave S, Nashville, TN 37212,
USA do not rise to the level of primary psychosis. In recognition of
2 mental illness as continuous in nature, the NIMH put forth
Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington
University St. Louis, 1 Brookings Dr., St. Louis, MO 63130, USA Research Domain Criteria to conceptualize and research aspects
3 of psychopathology through a dimensional lens (Insel et al.
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, St.
Louis, MO, USA 2010). Under the research domain criteria framework, questions
4 arise regarding whether associated features of psychosis, such as
Department of Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis, St.
Louis, MO, USA cognitive dysfunction, also occur along a spectrum. Cognitive
Neuropsychol Rev

dysfunction represents an excellent opportunity to assess dimen- Cognitive Functioning in Schizophrenia


sional characteristics of mental illness, as cognitive impairment is
present in individuals with putatively Bdistinct^ mental disor- Premorbid Cognitive Function in Schizophrenia
ders that share the feature of psychosis. While research
domain criteria does not directly address the trajectory of There is a general consensus that children and adolescents
phenotypic dimensions, a fact recently critiqued (Mittal exhibit premorbid cognitive impairments prior to the onset
and Wakschlag 2017), understanding the timing of cogni- of schizophrenia. As will be detailed in this section, this con-
tive impairments in psychosis contributes to a richer un- sensus follows decades of empirical research documenting
derstanding of its etiology. Examining the time course of both general (e.g., IQ) and specific (e.g., processing speed)
cognitive impairment in psychotic disorders also allows for impairments in individuals that later develop schizophrenia.
assessment of differential trajectories, introducing an op- Consistent with premorbid impairments, first-degree relatives
portunity to test questions of dissociation. In the spirit of also show cognitive impairments (Ivleva et al. 2012; Keri et al.
research domain criteria, and with an eye towards future 2001; McIntosh et al. 2005; Niendam et al. 2003), and the
neurodevelopmental investigations, we present a review of presence of psychosis in a first-degree relative increases the
broad cognitive dysfunction across the psychosis spec- severity of childhood premorbid impairment in schizophrenia
trum, across the life course. (Seidman et al. 2013). Despite the consistency of research
Although not a symptom of psychotic disorders, cognitive regarding premorbid cognitive impairments in schizophrenia,
dysfunction is a core feature of illnesses that include psychotic there is less consensus regarding their exact nature (i.e., which
symptoms. From the definition of schizophrenia as dementia cognitive domains show premorbid impairments specific to
praecox (Kraepelin 1919), cognitive deficits have been iden- schizophrenia) and course.
tified in individuals in the chronic states of illness. Despite the
co-occurrence of cognitive deficits with psychosis, anti- General Cognitive Impairments Children and adolescents
psychotic medications do not improve cognitive impairment who later develop schizophrenia show deficits in general
(Nielsen et al. 2015). In fact, anti-psychotics demonstrate min- cognitive abilities (Fig. 1). Consistent IQ deficits are ob-
imal efficacy in improving daily functioning, and research served among individuals who subsequently develop
suggests that functional impairment is most strongly associat- schizophrenia (Aylward et al. 1984; Daban et al. 2006;
ed with cognitive impairment, not severity of psychotic symp- Dickson et al. 2012; Khandaker et al. 2011; Mollon and
toms (Velligan et al. 1997). Therefore, cognitive deficits rep- Reichenberg 2017; Woodberry et al. 2008), with evidence
resent an important target for improving the lives of patients that risk for schizophrenia increases by 3.7% for every one
suffering from psychotic disorders. point decrease in IQ (Khandaker et al. 2011). Premorbid
The relevance of understanding cognitive impairment is fur- reduction in IQ has a medium effect size (Khandaker
ther highlighted when considering its presence early in the dis- et al. 2011; Woodberry et al. 2008), which translates to a
ease course. While psychotic symptoms typically emerge be- premorbid IQ deficit of around 8-10 points (Mollon and
tween ages 18 and 25, cognitive deficits are observed much Reichenberg 2017; Seidman et al. 2013). This is approxi-
earlier on in the lifespan of those who go on to develop schizo- mately half of the deficit seen in first-episode and chronic
phrenia (Cornblatt et al. 1999). This suggests that cognitive schizophrenia patients (Reichenberg and Harvey 2007). In
deficits are a marker of abnormal neurodevelopment, especially terms of the relationship between premorbid school
within the context of genetic risk and early developmental in- achievement and schizophrenia, poor premorbid school
sults (Rapoport et al. 2012). While schizophrenia is often con- performance is generally associated with increased risk
sidered a neurodevelopmental disorder, researchers continue to for schizophrenia (Crow et al. 1995; Fuller et al. 2002;
debate this model in bipolar disorder, which some evidence Kendler et al. 2016; MacCabe et al. 2008; Strauss et al.
suggests is neurodegenerative in nature (Goodwin et al. 2012; Watt and Lubensky 1976). However, one meta-
2008). Therefore, understanding the timing, specificity, and analysis found that children who later develop schizophre-
severity of cognitive deficits in psychotic disorders creates a nia do not show deficits in premorbid academic perfor-
window into brain function across the life span. In this review, mance (Dickson et al. 2012), and other research has pointed
we examine the data on cognitive functioning across the life to intact early school functioning but later difficulties in
course of individuals who experience psychosis, starting with schooling that are associated with severity of symptoms
available data on premorbid cognition, and then considering (Helling et al. 2003) and poorer outcomes (Cannon et al.
individuals at ultra-high risk for developing psychosis, first- 1999; Ang and Tan 2004). Therefore, early premorbid cog-
episode psychosis, and finally the chronic state. We work to nitive function in schizophrenia appears to be associated
uncover the current landscape of research on cognitive dysfunc- with some functional impairment, as measured by academ-
tion in psychotic disorders, and briefly discuss implications for ic performance, but this may only become apparent later in
functional outcome, treatment, and developmental course. schooling with increased severity of symptoms.
Neuropsychol Rev

Fig. 1 Estimated general cognitive impairment across the life span in et al. 2013; Martinez-Aran et al. 2004; Schatzberg et al. 2000). No data
schizophrenia, psychotic bipolar disorder, and psychotic depression. is currently available for premorbid or ultra-high risk psychotic depres-
Effect size estimates based on studies presented in this review (e.g. sion. Childhood+ = children who develop disorder; UHR+ = ultra high
Mollon and Reichenberg 2017; Seidman et al. 2016; Heinrichs and risk individuals who develop disorder
Zakzanis 1998; Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009; Daban et al. 2006; Hill

Specific Cognitive Impairments In addition to generalized cog- aspects of cognition, particularly executive functioning, ver-
nitive impairments in terms of IQ and academic achievement, bal fluency and memory (Knowles et al. 2015; Keefe and
specific premorbid impairments exist in a number of cognitive Harvey 2015). In the premorbid state of schizophrenia, pro-
domains (Heinrichs and Zakzanis 1998; Daban et al. 2006; cessing speed impairments are therefore multifaceted, both
Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009; Trotta et al. 2015). These do- relying on other areas of cognition that are impaired and po-
mains include attention (Cannon et al. 2006; Erlenmeyer- tentially contributing to, or interacting with, deficit develop-
Kimling et al. 2000; Cornblatt et al. 1999; Kern et al. 2011; ment of the host of cognitive impairments seen in later stages
Welham et al. 2010), memory (MacCabe et al. 2013; of the disorder, such as verbal abilities (Brebion et al. 2006).
Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al. 2000; McIntosh et al. 2005; Verbal abilities are consistently associated with premorbid
Meier et al. 2014; Reichenberg and Harvey 2007), reasoning impairments (Mollon and Reichenberg 2017; Parellada et al.
(Addington and Addington 2005; Niendam et al. 2003; 2017), particularly receptive language (Addington and
Reichenberg et al. 2010; Reichenberg and Harvey 2007; Addington 2005; Reichenberg et al. 2002; Seidman et al.
Reichenberg et al. 2002; Tiihonen et al. 2005; Welham et al. 2013; Cannon et al. 2002; Jones et al. 1994; Kremen et al.
2010), and executive functioning (Cannon et al. 2006; Meier 2010; Meier et al. 2014; Niendam et al. 2003; Welham et al.
et al. 2014; Reichenberg and Harvey 2007). 2010). For example, children later diagnosed with schizophre-
There is also considerable support for premorbid impair- nia exhibit receptive language impairments in childhood,
ments in processing speed (McIntosh et al. 2005; Kern et al. which predict psychotic symptoms at age eleven (Cannon
2011; Bachman et al. 2010; Seidman et al. 2013; Meier et al. et al. 2002). Among children with receptive language disorder
2014; Niendam et al. 2003), consistent with research indicat- diagnoses, 10% later developed schizophrenia (Howlin et al.
ing a processing speed impairment across the course of 2000), consistent with an early deficit in receptive language as
schizophrenia, including in prodromal, first-episode, and a risk-factor for schizophrenia.
chronic phases of the illness (Heinrichs and Zakzanis 1998;
Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009; Seidman et al. 2010, 2013). Neurodevelopment Model of Cognitive Impairments
Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in almost 2000 chron- Premorbid cognitive deficits support the neurodevelopmental
ic patients, for instance, demonstrates that digit symbol coding model of schizophrenia, which posits that the earliest signs of
ability is significantly more impaired than episodic memory, the disorder are mild abnormalities in cognitive development
executive functioning, and working memory ability, suggest- (Mollon and Reichenberg 2017; Murray and Lewis 1987;
ing processing speed as one of the most sensitive areas of Weinberger 1987). This model conceptualizes cognitive dys-
cognitive impairment in schizophrenia (Dickinson et al. function in schizophrenia as aberrant neurodevelopment dur-
2007). One hypothesis for why tasks like digit symbol coding ing the first two decades of life (Bora 2015; Dickson et al.
are so sensitive to impairment is their reliance on multiple 2012; Khandaker et al. 2011; MacCabe et al. 2013), with
Neuropsychol Rev

deficits in the acquisition of cognitive abilities compared to risk, or prodromal, are identified through dimensions of atten-
normative individuals (Bora and Pantelis 2015; Kremen et al. u a t e d p s y c h o t i c sy m p t o m s a n d / o r fa m i l i a l r i s k .
1998; Reichenberg et al. 2010). The presence of cognitive Approximately 13-23% of individuals meeting these ultra-
deficits years prior to the onset of psychotic symptoms sug- high risk criteria convert to psychosis within two years
gests that cognitive dysfunction is at the core of schizophrenia, (Bechdolf et al. 2010; Lemos-Giraldez et al. 2009; Seidman
with abnormal neurodevelopment manifesting through perfor- et al. 2016). Identification of ultra-high risk individuals in
mative lag as early as preschool age (Seidman and Mirsky longitudinal studies allows for a particularly rich comparison
2017). As outlined elegantly by Kahn and Keefe (2013), mul- of neurocognitive function in those who do and do not convert
tiple lines of evidence converge on the notion that schizophre- to psychosis, providing a window into the functioning of those
nia is a cognitive illness that is neurodevelopmental in nature: at the highest risk.
cognitive deficits exist prior to psychosis onset, cognitive abil-
ity is influenced by genetics, and cognition is an independent Ultra-High Risk Compared to Healthy Controls Based on a
predictor of functional outcome (Kahn and Keefe 2013). multitude of studies, there is reliable evidence that ultra-high
Furthermore, despite the vast heterogeneity in symptom pro- risk individuals are globally cognitively impaired when com-
files of schizophrenic patients, an estimated 98% of schizo- pared to a normative sample (Fusar-Poli et al. 2012; Hawkins
phrenia patients have lower cognitive functioning than expect- et al. 2004; Keefe et al. 2006; Simon et al. 2007). At-risk
ed based on their mother’s education level (Keefe et al. 2005). participants demonstrate deficits in working memory, execu-
Cognitive impairment early in illness is therefore a critical tive function, verbal fluency, attention, and memory (Hawkins
piece of evidence for abnormal neurodevelopment and should et al. 2004; Keefe et al. 2006; Seidman et al. 2016). In a meta-
be considered a core feature and risk-factor for symptom man- analysis, general intelligence and all cognitive sub-domains,
ifestation later in life (Seidman and Mirsky 2017). with the exception of processing speed, were found to be
While the presence of premorbid cognitive impairment in impaired in ultra-high risk participants (Fusar-Poli et al.
schizophrenia is well-described, there are mixed findings re- 2012). This global deficit in ultra-high risk was replicated with
garding the nature of this aberrant neurodevelopment, includ- a small but reliable effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.30) for cognitive
ing whether schizophrenia is associated with static premorbid impairment averaged across 19 neuropsychological tasks
deficits versus increasing cognitive deficits prior to the onset (Seidman et al. 2016). Further, individuals who endorse psy-
of the disorder. There is some evidence for static deficits chotic symptoms between ages 8-12 have a significantly low-
(Cornblatt et al. 1999; Crow et al. 1995; Jones et al. 1994), er predicted age based on their cognitive performance (Gur
including in attention (Cornblatt et al. 1999) and verbal abil- et al. 2014). This impairment is present at 8 years old, be-
ities (Meier et al. 2014; Reichenberg et al. 2010). Yet the comes even more pronounced after age 16, and is observed
majority of research shows increasing cognitive deficits prior in all tested domains (executive function, memory, complex
to the onset of schizophrenia, especially immediately prior to cognition, social cognition, and sensorimotor) with the biggest
the onset of the disorder (Lewandowski et al. 2011b; lag in complex cognition, which includes verbal reasoning,
Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009; Mollon and Reichenberg non-verbal reasoning, and spatial processing, and social
2017; Parellada et al. 2017). One hypothesis helping to ex- cognition.
plain these discrepant findings is that verbal deficits may
emerge early and remain static during childhood, contribute Ultra-High Risk Compared to First-Episode Schizophrenia
to an increasing impairments in non-verbal abilities in adoles- Cognitive ability in individuals at-risk for schizophrenia is
cence, with these non-verbal deficits further exacerbating ver- globally impaired compared to healthy controls, but the mag-
bal deficits during adolescence (Mollon and Reichenberg nitude of impairment is less than can be seen in patients who
2017). Schizophrenia is largely associated with premorbid have recently experienced their first-episode of psychosis
deficits in the acquisition of cognitive abilities during (Keefe et al. 2006; Simon et al. 2012). Ultra-high risk partic-
neurodevelopment (Bora 2015), as opposed to the loss of pre- ipants have performed significantly better than first-episode
viously acquired cognitive abilities, indicating these deficits subjects on all tasks (CPT, verbal recall, digit symbol, verbal
increase in severity throughout development (Mollon and fluency, and working memory) except for finger tapping on
Reichenberg 2017). which they had similar performance (Keefe et al. 2006). This
intermediate impairment between controls and first-episode
Schizophrenia: Ultra-High Risk patients was replicated across all neuropsychological tasks in
a separate analysis of ultra-high risk subjects (Hou et al.
Prior to first-episode psychosis, prediction of who will devel- 2016). Intermediate levels of cognitive impairment have also
op a primary psychotic disorder remains elusive. However, been observed in areas of visual working memory, verbal
researchers have identified markers of increased risk. These memory, executive functioning, visual spatial skills, and men-
individuals, often referred to as ultra-high risk, clinical high tal control (Goghari et al. 2014) (Liu et al. 2015). Impairments
Neuropsychol Rev

similar to first-episode patients have been reported for high risk+ group (Seidman et al. 2010), further suggesting that
sustained attention (Francey et al. 2005) and relational mem- ultra-high risk subjects who will convert to psychosis demon-
ory (Greenland-White et al. 2017). Overall, ultra-high risk strate a cognitive profile similar to those with the illness.
participants demonstrate global impairments that are interme- Meta-analysis has found that ultra-high risk+ individuals have
diate in magnitude between healthy controls and first-episode. significantly lower general intelligence, poorer verbal fluency,
That said, there may be domains that are impaired to the same verbal and visual memory, and working memory compared to
degree as first-episode psychosis, even during the prodromal ultra-high risk- individuals (Fusar-Poli et al. 2012). Greater
state, particularly in the domains of memory and attention. impairments in working memory, attention, and worse declar-
ative and verbal memory abilities can also differentiate ultra-
Cognitive Deficits Based on Clinical Versus Familial Risk high risk+ from ultra-high risk- subjects (Lencz et al. 2006)
Definition of ultra-high risk is not uniform, and several studies (Seidman et al. 2016). Finally, conversion to psychosis is as-
have worked to parse out differences in cognitive ability based sociated with poor verbal memory and sustained attention
on the category of risk. For instance, verbal memory is signif- (Seidman et al. 2010) (Keefe et al. 2006). These longitudinal
icantly worse in individuals whose risk is defined based on findings demonstrate that individuals at high risk who will
clinical symptoms compared to those whose risk is defined ultimately develop a psychotic disorder are more cognitively
based on family history (1st degree relative with psychotic impaired than individuals presenting with a similar level of
disorder) (Seidman et al. 2010). Additionally, while their com- risk, who do not develop a psychotic disorder within 1-3
posite cognition scores were similar, their profiles of impair- years. Those who do convert look more similar to first-
ment differed; individuals with clinical risk showed deficits in episode patients, suggesting that the intermediate deficit
executive functioning and verbal memory while individuals discussed earlier may partially reflect the average of two
with familial risk had deficits in vocabulary and visuospatial groups (converters and non-converters). Of further interest is
skills. Children with a lower familial risk (only one affected the potential importance of poor verbal memory and attention,
2nd-degree relative) have cognitive scores similar to healthy both of which predicted conversion to psychosis and both of
controls, but those with higher familial risk (at least one af- which were more impaired in first-episode psychosis than
fected 1st degree relative or at least two affected 2nd-degree ultra-high risk on average.
relatives) demonstrate lower full-scale IQ, scholastic achieve-
ment, verbal comprehension, working memory, verbal mem- More Specific Impairments in Ultra-High Risk Schizophrenia
ory, and executive functioning (Dickson et al. 2014). When Broad domain-level deficits in ultra-high risk patients are
patients are stratified by the level of clinical risk, which may clear, and growing research into this population is working
indicate different stages of the prodrome (Klosterkotter et al. to reveal more specific impairments. For instance, in the con-
2001), those identified as Bbasic symptom at-risk^ had better text of broad memory and IQ impairments, visual reproduc-
working memory and verbal memory than those at ultra-high tion of a stimulus and story recall on a logical memory task
risk and first-episode subjects (Simon et al. 2007). Basic have discriminated ultra-high risk+ patients from those who
symptom participants showed cognitive performance worse did not convert (Brewer et al. 2005) – an association not seen
than healthy controls but similar to other psychiatric controls for other memory, attentional, or executive functioning tasks.
who endorsed no psychotic symptoms, suggesting that cogni- Abnormalities in perceptual processing are also observed in
tive deficits are attenuated, but present at this early prodromal clinical high risk patients, with deficits in visual form percep-
stage. tion (Kimhy et al. 2007) and perceptual organization
(Silverstein et al. 2006). Perceptual processing abnormalities
Ultra-High Risk+ Compared to Ultra-High Risk- Longitudinal may contribute to, or possibly interact with, abnormalities in
studies of ultra-high risk subjects allow for the comparison of attributional bias in the ultra-high risk state (An et al. 2010),
cognitive ability between individuals who do and do not con- which is in turn related to paranoid symptoms. Unpacking
vert to a primary psychotic disorder, usually over the course of executive dysfunction into Bcold^ (e.g. working memory)
1-3 years, providing clues about incremental risk and time and Bhot^ (e.g. decision making) functions, there are specific
course. Research suggests that ultra-high risk+ subjects (i.e., relationship between Bhot^ executive functioning (decision
those who convert) demonstrate greater cognitive impairment making ability) and psychotic symptom severity
than ultra-high risk- subjects (i.e. those who do not convert) (MacKenzie et al. 2017), suggesting that certain aspects of
(Keefe et al. 2006) (Seidman et al. 2010), with some evidence these broad cognitive domains are more critical for under-
of ultra-high risk+ subjects having cognitive deficits similar to standing relationships with current and impending psychotic
first-episode participants, and ultra-high risk- subjects looking experiences. Finally, specific analysis of latent inhibition, a
similar to healthy controls (Keefe et al. 2006). Interestingly, learning process dependent on both attentional and perceptual
the tests most impaired in the first-episode subjects, symbol resources, identified a deficit in latent inhibition in ultra-high
coding and verbal memory, are the most impaired in the ultra- risk individuals, indicating impaired ability to learn from past
Neuropsychol Rev

experiences to adjust expectations for future experiences their impairments were similar to those observed in first-
(Kraus et al. 2016). While not comprehensive, these findings, episode subjects (chronic patients: Cohen’s ds ranged from
particularly those predicting conversion to psychosis and re- -.46 to -1.41). Verbal memory also represented the greatest
lationships with symptom severity, suggest the importance of deficit in chronic schizophrenia subjects (Cohen’s d=-1.41)
unpacking deficits observed within neurocognitive domains. suggesting a stable profile of impairment across the illness
course. Deficits in global cognitive impairment between
Summary Together, these data suggest that prodromal psycho- first-episode and chronic schizophrenia remain similar when
sis is characterized by global cognitive impairment, with some reducing methodological heterogeneity (e.g. medication
specific risk associated with poor memory and attention dur- use; McCleery et al. 2014), as these groups exhibit compara-
ing this stage. Individuals with a strong familial risk, more ble deficits in processing speed, attention or vigilance, verbal
pronounced attenuated psychosis, and those who will ulti- learning, visual learning, and reasoning and problem solving
mately convert to a psychotic disorder, demonstrate the performance. In both groups, processing speed was approxi-
greatest impairment in cognition on average. Cognitive defi- mately 0.5 standard deviations below their mean performance
cits are reliably present at this stage of illness, with some data across all domains, revealing a more pronounced deficit in
suggesting that deficits are similar in magnitude and pattern to processing speed in both early and chronic stages of illness,
those seen in first-episode psychosis. against the backdrop of a generalized deficit.
Evidence is strong that first-episode schizophrenia patients
Schizophrenia: First-Episode demonstrate cognitive deficits similar to those observed in
chronic stages, suggesting that duration of anti-psychotic
Measuring cognitive ability in first-episode schizophrenia al- medication use is not a potent contributor to cognitive impair-
lows for the measurement of cognition prior to long-term ment in schizophrenia. In fact, recent meta-analysis of medi-
treatment with anti-psychotics and prior to any potential cation-naïve first-episode patients found medium to large ef-
degenerating process associated with the illness (e.g. fect sizes in all cognitive domains compared to healthy con-
Kirkpatrick et al. 2008). Although definitions of Bfirst- trols, with the largest impairment in verbal memory, process-
episode^ are variable, the phrase refers to the identification ing speed, and working memory (Fatouros-Bergman et al.
of individual who have recently transitioned to a primary psy- 2014). These findings across 23 studies strongly support the
chotic disorder. The majority of first-episode subjects presence of significant cognitive impairment in the early
discussed in this review were tested following their index stages of psychosis that is independent of medication use.
hospitalization, within 24 months of their first contact with
psychiatric treatment for psychosis. Longitudinal Analysis of First-Episode Schizophrenia
Longitudinal studies of first-episode schizophrenia patients
First-Episode Compared to Chronic Schizophrenia provide important insight into the trajectory of cognitive im-
Comparison of first-episode schizophrenia with chronic pairments into the chronic state. In general, these studies point
schizophrenia and healthy controls has been the primary ques- to a stable cognitive profile in the years following a patient’s
tion of interest for many studies, allowing researchers to assess first break. For instance, over two years, only 10% of a large
whether cognitive deficits emerge through a degenerating pro- cohort of first episode patients demonstrated decline or im-
cess following first-episode psychosis, or are already present provement in cognition, indicating that 90% of patients had
when the psychosis emerges. Meta-analysis of over 2,000 stable cognitive functioning (Sanchez-Torres et al. 2017).
first-episode patients across 47 studies has revealed medium- Over 10 years, in two independent studies, schizophrenia-
to-large impairments in first-episode schizophrenia across 10 spectrum patients exhibited stable cognitive performance on
cognitive domains, with all tested domains revealing signifi- all tests (Bergh et al. 2016; Rund et al. 2016), even in the
cant impairment (Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009). General cog- context of improved clinical symptoms. Duration of untreated
nitive ability had an effect size of -.91, demonstrating almost psychosis was unrelated to cognitive performance (Rund et al.
one standard deviation of overall cognitive impairment in 2016). Global cognitive ability measured at first-episode
first-episode schizophrenia. Areas of greatest impairment in- therefore appears to reflect a patient’s cognitive capacity dur-
cluded immediate verbal memory (Cohen’s d = -1.2) and pro- ing the chronic course, arguing against neurotoxic effects of
cessing speed (Cohen’s d = -0.96). Motor skills exhibited the schizophrenia.
least impairment, but still represented a significant deficit
compared to controls (Cohen’s d = -0.64). The authors com- Additional Cognitive Abnormalities in First-Episode
pared their results to those in chronic patients presented in Schizophrenia Large cognitive batteries, such as the
Heinrichs and Zakzanis (1998) review. Chronic subjects were, MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, provide standard-
on average, 9 years older, had greater illness chronicity, and ized measurement of broad cognitive domains, but do not
had substantially longer duration of medication exposure, yet address a number of more specific aspects of cognitive
Neuropsychol Rev

performance. We therefore mention several more nuanced Chronic Schizophrenia


findings of cognitive ability in first-episode psychosis to
provide a more complete picture. For instance, analysis of At this point, there is no mystery regarding whether cognitive
verbal learning in antipsychotic-naïve first-episode patients impairment is present in chronic schizophrenia. A robust and
revealed significant impairment in both short and long-term reliable cognitive deficit has been observed in up to 80% of
memory recall (Hill et al. 2004). Memory impairment was patients with schizophrenia (Reichenberg et al. 2009) and a
associated with reduced use of organizational strategies to large literature has emerged working to elucidate the specific-
facilitate retrieval of words, based on semantic similarities ity of these deficits as well as their correlates. Here, we broad-
(e.g. animals), suggesting that first-episode patients’ robust ly review that literature.
verbal memory impairment may, in part, be due to minimal
use of memory-facilitating strategies that may be used by Generalized Deficit As previously mentioned, in 1998,
healthy controls. Another area of cognition not yet Heinrichs and Zakzanis produced a striking meta-analysis of
discussed is Jumping to Conclusions, a data-gathering bias cognitive deficits in schizophrenia (Heinrichs and Zakzanis
measured as the amount of information needed for an indi- 1998). The data, analyzed across 204 studies and 22 cognitive
vidual to make a decision (McKay et al. 2006). Jumping to variables, revealed an undeniable generalized deficit in chron-
conclusions studies typically ask participants to determine ic schizophrenia. Patients were significantly impaired for all
which jar colored balls are being pulled from, when re- cognitive variables compared to controls, with moderate-to-
vealed one-by-one, with jars of different color ratios (e.g. large effect sizes. These deficits were not only large in mag-
80:20 vs. 60:40). When tested in first-episode schizophre- nitude, but appear relatively stable over time. For instance, no
nia, 49% of first-episode participants demonstrated the significant differences in cognitive ability have been found
jumping to conclusions bias, compared to 26% of controls, between patients aged 18-70, suggesting that schizophrenia
which was a significant difference. Participant IQ and de- is characterized by static impairments and that cognitive abil-
lusional severity were significant predictors of jumping to ity does not degenerate as part of the disease process
conclusions and both IQ and working memory ability were (Goldberg et al. 1993). Individuals who experience an unsta-
significantly lower in first-episode subjects who jumped to ble remission or are continually psychotic during the first year
conclusions, compared to those who did not. These data have lower overall cognitive scores than those who achieved
suggest that cognitive impairment contributes to a cogni- remission, but this difference remains 10 years later (Rund
tive bias, which is associated with more severe psychotic et al. 2016). In fact, illness remission in the first year was the
symptoms in early stages of psychosis. Finally, in a study only significant predictor of cognitive course, not duration of
on prospective memory, first-episode patients were signif- untreated psychosis, symptom scores, or medication use. It
icantly impaired on both cue identification (identifying appears that at the start of the illness a general cognitive deficit
when they observed a cue that signaled a to-be-performed is present, without strong evidence for a steep decline in the
action) and intentional retrieval (retrieval of the intended context of aging.
action from long-term memory following the specified cue; The generalized deficit is therefore a stable feature of
Liu et al. 2017). Cue identification impairments remained schizophrenia and has been further characterized in multiple
after controlling for working memory and retrospective studies. Shared variance in cognitive ability across 17 cogni-
memory. tive domains is the best predictor of diagnostic group (schizo-
phrenia or healthy control), explaining 63.6% of the
Summary First-episode schizophrenia is characterized by diagnosis-related variance (Dickinson et al. 2008). This gen-
large deficits in overall cognitive ability. Analysis of specific eralized deficit factor has been further replicated across pa-
cognitive domains reveals a similar pattern of impairment as tients well-characterized for schizophrenia, schizoaffective
seen in chronic schizophrenia, with comparable magnitude. disorder, and bipolar disorder with psychotic features, reveal-
Verbal memory and processing speed were most robustly im- ing that a single-factor model is the best fit for characterizing
paired in first-episode patients, consistent with findings in cognitive ability in psychosis (Hochberger et al. 2016). A
both ultra-high risk and chronic stages (discussed below). common factor explained 53% of the variance in cognitive
These deficits are observed even in the absence of neurolep- ability in schizophrenia and 50% in schizoaffective disorder,
tics and prior to chronicity of illness, and therefore are not a suggesting that the broad profile of cognitive impairment
byproduct of anti-psychotics or a result of degeneration as the manifests as dysfunction shared across domains.
illness progresses. Longitudinal studies suggest a stable cog-
nitive profile following one’s first break. Data therefore sug- Specific Deficits Within the context of a robust generalized
gests that global cognitive deficits are a core feature of schizo- deficit, researchers have explored whether pockets of specific-
phrenia that are present in close to their final form at the first- ity can be observed. Processing speed and social cognition
episode of psychosis. have been found to best distinguish patients from controls
Neuropsychol Rev

above and beyond the generalized deficit (Kern et al. 2011). chronic schizophrenia, but provides insight into the rich appli-
Furthermore, processing speed, visual learning, and attention cation of cognitive neuroscience research to understanding the
or vigilance best distinguish patients with and without com- generalized cognitive deficit across the life course of schizo-
petitive employment, suggesting that impairments in these phrenia patients.
areas contribute to functional disability (Kern et al. 2011).
Processing speed, and even more specifically digit symbol Impact of Medications on Cognition Associations between
coding, has been found to be particularly sensitive to impair- medications use, primarily anti-psychotics, and cognition has
ments in schizophrenia, leading some to suggest that process- been of great interest to researchers, due both to their influence
ing speed represents a specific deficit in schizophrenia that on the dopamine system (which is critical for cognitive
contributes to the generalized deficit (Dickinson et al. 2007). functioning; Backman et al. 2010) and the hope that medica-
The sensitivity of digit symbol coding may be due to its reli- tions might improve cognitive ability in schizophrenia. In one
ance on multiple cognitive demands, including sustained at- of the most robust tests of this question, Keefe and colleagues
tention, memory, motor speed, and strategy formation tested whether medication influenced cognitive ability in 817
(Glosser et al. 1977). An analysis of digit symbol in schizo- schizophrenia patients in randomized double-blind treatment
phrenia revealed that, although patients are impaired on the trials for anti-psychotics (Keefe et al. 2007). Patients received
test overall, they demonstrate difficulty in benefiting from one of four medications for 18 months and after two months
predictable information about the target, suggesting that a def- there was a small (approximately d=0.20) but significant im-
icit in relational memory fails to aid cognitive processing provement in cognitive ability for all of the anti-psychotics
speed (Bachman et al. 2010). tested. While cognitive improvement was stable after two
Of all the psychotic disorders and stages of illness, chronic years, all areas of cognition improved in those first two
schizophrenia has been most extensively studied for cognitive months, with no specificity for any cognitive domain. Small,
specificity, with consortium such as the Cognitive but positive influence of anti-psychotic use on global cogni-
Neuroscience Test Reliability and Clinical Applications tion has also been observed in early psychosis patients taking
(CNTRACS) working to identify cognitive neuroscience- either haloperidol or second-generation antipsychotics over
driven paradigms for investigating mechanism within broad six months (Davidson et al. 2009). While there is some sug-
cognitive domains (Gold et al. 2012). While a comprehensive gestion that first-generation anti-psychotics have negligible if
description of findings is not within the breadth of this review, not adverse impact of cognitive performance (Elie et al. 2010)
several findings stand out. Under the large umbrella of exec- with second-generation anti-psychotics having a more posi-
utive functioning is cognitive control, the ability to maintain tive impact (Kane et al. 1988), a recent meta-analysis found no
goal representation in the service of performing a specific task. global differences in cognition for first- versus second-
Chronic patients are impaired in cognitive control tasks generation anti-psychotics (Nielsen et al. 2015). One major
(Barch et al. 2003) and more specifically demonstrate a reli- limitation in interpreting these findings is the impact of prac-
ance on reactive versus proactive cognitive control (Barch and tice effects. Recently, practice effects were estimated to have
Ceaser 2012), indicating a Blate correction^ for behavior as an effect size of 0.18 using data from 813 psychotic disorder
opposed to a preparedness and active maintenance of goals. patients across multiple clinical trials (Keefe et al. 2017). This
Deficits in cognitive control have been related to disorganized is a notably similar effect size to the previously reported
symptoms of schizophrenia (Lesh et al. 2013). Working mem- change in cognition following two months of anti-psychotic
ory is also a multifaceted construct, including encoding, vi- treatment (Keefe et al. 2007) but is smaller than the medium
suospatial and verbal buffer systems, representation, and effect sizes for medication effects reported by Davidson and
maintenance (Baddeley 2000). Schizophrenia patients demon- colleagues (Davidson et al. 2009). Inclusion of healthy con-
strate deficits in all of these areas, making specificity challeng- trols at each time point would help address the concern of
ing, however there is some suggestion that the encoding, rep- practice effects, however neither study showing a positive
resentation, and maintenance of information are driving the effect of medications included a healthy comparison group.
observed working memory deficits (Barch and Sheffield Overall, cognitive benefits of anti-psychotics in schizophrenia
2014). In addition to very early perceptual deficits in chronic are mixed and modest at best (Hill et al. 2010) suggesting the
schizophrenia, such as visual integration (Silverstein et al. need for adjunctive or alternative pharmacological treatments
2012), early task-dependent processes have recently been for meaningful improvement.
shown to be impaired in schizophrenia. More specifically,
rapid instructed task learning, the ability to rapidly transfer Summary Together, findings across the lifespan of schizophre-
task instruction into goal-directed behavior, is reduced in nia widely suggest cognitive deficits that can be observed as
schizophrenia, suggesting impaired learning processes early as childhood, are robust in ultra-high risk individuals
(Sheffield et al. 2018). Again, this overview is sparse com- who develop the disorder, and are stable and severe from the
pared to the impressive literature on cognitive impairment in first-episode of psychosis throughout the remainder of the life
Neuropsychol Rev

course (Fig. 1). While deficits in verbal memory and attention study who developed bipolar disorder with psychotic features
may be early markers of abnormal psychiatric development, showed evidence of premorbid cognitive impairment. Yet sev-
there is less specificity at the chronic stage, with the strongest eral studies have failed to find premorbid IQ deficits in bipolar
evidence pointing to particularly robust deficits in processing disorder with psychotic features (Guerra et al. 2002; Simonsen
speed. Cognitive deficits are minimally impacted by anti- et al. 2011; Zanelli et al. 2010). Mixed evidence also comes
psychotics and, as will be discussed in more detail below, from a study finding that both low and high scholastic perfor-
contribute to the functional disability that takes such a large mance are associated with risk for developing bipolar disorder
toll on this patient population. Cognitive deficits in schizo- with psychotic features (MacCabe et al. 2010). Taken togeth-
phrenia appear to be neurodevelopmental in nature, and reve- er, research more consistently points to mild premorbid global
lations about their development are a necessary step towards cognitive impairments in bipolar disorder with psychotic fea-
early intervention. tures in comparison to premorbid cognitive function in bipolar
disorder generally.
Bipolar Disorder
Comparison with Premorbid Cognitive Function in
Premorbid Cognitive Functioning in Bipolar Disorder Schizophrenia Research indicates that schizophrenia and bi-
polar disorder with psychotic features are distinguished by the
In comparison to premorbid cognitive function in schizophre- degree of premorbid impairment, with schizophrenia showing
nia, fewer studies have been conducted on premorbid cogni- more severe premorbid cognitive impairments than bipolar
tive deficits in bipolar disorder. Of these studies, even fewer disorder (Fig. 1; (Daban et al. 2006; Trotta et al. 2015).
have examined premorbid impairments associated with bipo- Even among the studies finding premorbid cognitive impair-
lar disorder with psychotic features (Daban et al. 2006). In ments in bipolar disorder with psychotic features, these im-
terms of research examining bipolar disorder in general (i.e., pairments are generally less severe than found in premorbid
including bipolar disorder with and without psychotic fea- schizophrenia (Daban et al. 2006; Seidman et al. 2013;
tures), studies largely fail to find evidence of significant Kendler et al. 2016). Although schizophrenia is associated
premorbid cognitive impairments (Lewandowski et al. with greater premorbid IQ deficits than bipolar disorder in
2011a; Martino et al. 2015; Mollon and Reichenberg 2017; several studies (Simonsen et al. 2011; Zanelli et al. 2010),
Parellada et al. 2017). For example, several studies failed to others have failed to find this association (Olvet et al. 2010;
find premorbid IQ deficits among children and adolescents Seidman et al. 2002). Thus, while bipolar disorder with psy-
who subsequently developed bipolar disorder (Cannon et al. chotic feature is more consistently associated with premorbid
2002; Sorensen et al. 2012; Zammit et al. 2004). Furthermore, impairments than in bipolar disorder generally, these impair-
there is evidence that higher cognitive functioning is associ- ments are generally less severe than premorbid cognitive def-
ated with the development of bipolar disorder (Tiihonen et al. icits in schizophrenia.
2005; MacCabe et al. 2013). A significant limitation of the
existing research is the lack of studies comparing bipolar dis- Mixed Findings on Premorbid Cognitive Function in Bipolar
order with and without psychotic features on premorbid cog- Disorder Potential reasons for these mixed results regarding
nitive functioning (Parellada et al. 2017). the presence of premorbid cognitive impairments in bipolar
In contrast to premorbid cognitive function in bipolar dis- disorder include both small sample sizes as well as differing
order generally, a mild global cognitive impairment exists in findings depending on how the sample (i.e., conscript, birth
individuals with bipolar disorder with psychotic features, in- cohort, or high-risk) was collected (Daban et al. 2006; Trotta
cluding evidence that the presence of psychotic features may et al. 2015). Furthermore, premorbid cognitive impairments in
worsen IQ deficits (Daban et al. 2006). In early onset bipolar bipolar disorder may be restricted to individuals with earlier
disorder with psychotic features, research demonstrates onset of the disorder (Parellada et al. 2017) and therefore more
premorbid IQ deficits (Paya et al. 2013). Similarly, individuals severe versions of the disorder (i.e., psychotic symptoms; ear-
at high-risk for schizophrenia who later develop bipolar dis- lier onset). One theory that potentially helps explain the lim-
order show global cognitive deficits (Olvet et al. 2010; ited evidence for premorbid cognitive deficits in bipolar dis-
Ratheesh et al. 2013), and medium-sized impairments in pro- order is termed the staging model, which theorizes that as
cessing speed and executive functioning are observed in the symptoms of the illness arise, cognitive functioning deterio-
premorbid phase (Ratheesh et al. 2013). Retrospective exam- rates, and these cognitive deficits are compounded by the ef-
ination of premorbid cognitive functioning in individuals who fects of number of episodes, life stress, and illness progression
later developed bipolar disorder with psychotic features iden- (Kapczinski et al. 2009). However, not all longitudinal studies
tified intermediate impairment between schizophrenia and of cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder support the notion
controls, with no significant differences between either group of deteriorating cognitive functioning over the course of the
(Seidman et al. 2013). However, 22.9% of individuals in this disorder. Several studies have observed static cognitive deficit
Neuropsychol Rev

trajectories in the bipolar disorder (Samame et al. 2014; Bora forms of bipolar disorder. For example, some studies indicate
and Ozerdem 2017; Martino et al. 2018), and several studies that FEBP+ has less severe cognitive deficits in comparison to
demonstrate cognitive improvements after the first manic ep- multiple-episode patients (Elshahawi et al. 2011; Hellvin et al.
isode (Torres et al. 2014; Torrent et al. 2018). It is also plau- 2012), especially regarding attention and executive function
sible that premorbid cognitive functioning impairments in bi- (Elshahawi et al. 2011) and the presence of prior depressive
polar disorder are associated with specific genetic variants episodes does not confer additional cognitive impairments
(Arts et al. 2013; Bryzgalov et al. 2018; Flowers et al. 2016) (Muralidharan et al. 2014). There is also mixed evidence re-
that may be associated with neural development (Tabares- garding whether FEBP+ exhibits significantly greater impair-
Seisdedos et al. 2008) and thus may not be present in all ments compared to bipolar disorder without psychotic features
individuals premorbidly. Overall, the heterogeneity of find- (FEBP-), with some studies demonstrating similar severity of
ings regarding the presence of premorbid cognitive deficits neuropsychological deficits (Demmo et al. 2016;
in bipolar disorder during childhood and adolescence points Lewandowski et al. 2011a; Trisha et al. 2017), and others
to the necessity that future studies incorporate consistent finding greater impairments in FEBP+ (Albus et al. 1996).
methodology using large samples to better understand wheth- Thus, FEBP+, if anything, appears to be intermediate in se-
er premorbid deficits exist in bipolar disorder. verity between FEBP- and chronic bipolar disorder, although
additional research should attempt to resolve disparities in
First-Episode Bipolar Disorder these findings (Bora and Pantelis 2015), including the effect
of depressive symptoms on FEBP+.
Deficits occur across the spectrum of cognitive domains early
after the onset of bipolar disorder with psychotic features, Bipolar Disorder Compared to Schizophrenia The majority of
including in samples of first-episode bipolar disorder with research indicates that first-episode schizophrenia shows
psychotic features (FEBP+) and recent onset psychosis diag- greater cognitive impairments than FEBP+ (Fig. 1;
nosed with bipolar disorder (Barrett et al. 2009; Dickerson Dickerson et al. 2011; Mojtabai et al. 2000; Reichenberg
et al. 2011; Reichenberg et al. 2009; Zabala et al. 2010). et al. 2009; Woodward 2016; Zanelli et al. 2010). First-
These cognitive impairments have generally been localized episode schizophrenia is associated with greater impairments
to attention, processing speed, verbal learning or memory, on most cognitive measures, including measures of verbal
and executive functioning (Albus et al. 1996; Daglas et al. memory, executive functioning, and processing speed
2016; Demmo et al. 2016; Elshahawi et al. 2011; Hellvin (Barrett et al. 2009; Demmo et al. 2016; Hill et al. 2009).
et al. 2012; Trisha et al. 2017). There is also cumulating evi- Yet there is some evidence for similar severity in deficits be-
dence for impairments in verbal memory and fluency early in tween schizophrenia and FEBP+ (Albus et al. 1996; Ayres
the course of bipolar disorder, including in samples of FEBP+ et al. 2007; McClellan et al. 2004; Zabala et al. 2010), espe-
(Albus et al. 1996; Daglas et al. 2016; Demmo et al. 2016; cially in processing speed and attention (Albus et al. 1996;
Elshahawi et al. 2011; Hellvin et al. 2012; Trisha et al. 2017), Dickerson et al. 2011). Taken together, the synthesis of this
majority (~70%) FEBP+ samples (Muralidharan et al. 2014; research indicates that any differences in cognitive impair-
Torres et al. 2014), samples with recent onset psychosis diag- ments between schizophrenia and FEBP+ are quantitative
nosed with bipolar disorder (Ayres et al. 2007; Zanelli et al. rather than qualitative in nature (Hill et al. 2009; Mojtabai
2010), as well as samples of early onset bipolar disorder with et al. 2000; Pena et al. 2011; Reichenberg et al. 2009;
psychotic features (McClellan et al. 2004). Impairments in Zanelli et al. 2010).
executive functioning are also consistently observed, with im-
pairments demonstrated in FEBP+ (Albus et al. 1996; Daglas Specific Deficits Nuanced studies of cognitive dysfunction in
et al. 2016; Demmo et al. 2016; Elshahawi et al. 2011; Hellvin first-episode bipolar disorder with psychotic features is nota-
et al. 2012; Trisha et al. 2017) and majority (~70%) FEBP+ bly limited, as many studies including affective psychosis
samples (Muralidharan et al. 2014; Torres et al. 2014). combine patient groups to study the broader category of Bfirst
Executive functioning impairments have been found most episode psychosis^. Impaired semantic verbal fluency has
consistently in cognitive flexibility (Daglas et al. 2015; been observed in first episode psychotic bipolar patients in
Trisha et al. 2017), with these impairments correctly classify- the context of intact cognitive functioning over a wide range
ing the bipolar diagnosis of 80% of patients with first-episode of cognitive domains (e.g. processing speed, working memo-
psychosis (Pena et al. 2011). ry, executive functioning: Kravariti et al. 2009). On an
antisaccade task, increased error rates were observed in a large
Bipolar Disorder With and Without Psychotic Features While group of first episode psychosis patients, but when separated
research consistently indicates that FEBP+ is associated with by diagnostic group, bipolar disorder patients did not demon-
cognitive impairments, there is a lack of consensus regarding strate significant impairment, as this finding was largely driv-
the relative severity of FEBP+ impairments compared to other en by the schizophrenia group (Harris et al. 2009). Finally,
Neuropsychol Rev

cognitive flexibility is an area of particular deficit in first ep- Aran et al. 2008), with these domains typically showing the
isode psychotic bipolar disorder patients compared to bipolar largest effect sizes (Bora et al. 2010b). Impairments in verbal
patients without a history of psychosis (Trisha et al. 2018). learning or memory are typically seen in CBP+ (Bora 2017;
There is much room for growth in the understanding of spe- Bora et al. 2009, 2010a, b), with verbal memory impairment
cific cognitive deficits in first episode psychotic bipolar disor- relating to global functioning, longer duration of illness, and a
der. One limitation to studying first episode subjects is the higher number of manic episodes (Martinez-Aran et al. 2004).
difficulty in knowing whether a truly bipolar illness will man- However, verbal memory deficits may be a marker for bipolar
ifest, as opposed to schizoaffective disorder or an affectively- disorder more generally, as opposed to specifically related to
charged schizophrenia; however as larger longitudinal studies CBP+ (Aminoff et al. 2013; Bora et al. 2007).
take shape, analysis of first episode bipolar disorder will help There is also significant evidence for impairments in exec-
elucidate aspects of cognition more vulnerable to decline over utive functioning in CBP+ (Bora et al. 2007; Ozdel et al. 2007;
the course of illness. Selva et al. 2007), and these impairments may be specific to
those with psychotic features (Bora et al. 2007). Two aspects
Summary While the findings point to significant mild to mod- of executive functioning implicated in CBP+ are cognitive
erate cognitive impairments across a broad range of cognitive flexibility (Bora et al. 2007; Savitz et al. 2009) and working
domains in FEBP+, questions remain regarding the contribu- memory (Bora 2017; Bora et al. 2010b; Glahn et al. 2007;
tions of several clinical variables to these cognitive impair- Jimenez-Lopez et al. 2017; Kim et al. 2015), although some-
ments. For example, stable impairments in cognitive function- times these impairments show modest (e.g.,Cohen’s d=.29)
ing after first-episode treatment have been observed (Demmo effect sizes (Bora 2017; Jimenez-Lopez et al. 2017).
et al. 2017; Hill et al. 2009), while other research shows im- Working memory impairments in CBP+ may also be localized
provements after first-episode remission (Torres et al. 2014), to more demanding working memory measures (Frydecka
including in one test of processing speed (Daglas et al. 2016). et al. 2014). One study demonstrated that working memory
These improvements are above-and-beyond practice effects impairments are specifically related to the presence of hallu-
observed in the comparative healthy control groups tested at cinations (Jenkins et al. 2017).
similar time intervals, as reflected in significant group by time
interactions. Several methodological issues may contribute to Bipolar Disorder With and Without Psychotic Features There
the heterogeneity of findings, including a lack of consistency is some empirical support for greater cognitive impairments in
regarding what is considered first-episode with psychotic fea- CBP+ in comparison to chronic bipolar disorder without psy-
tures (Demmo et al. 2016). As previously mentioned, these chotic features (CBP-). While a few studies that have found
samples varied in inclusion criteria for first-episode, from a that CBP+ and CBP- show similar cognitive profiles (Ancin
first manic episode with psychotic features (Demmo et al. et al. 2013; Savitz et al. 2009), the majority of studies demon-
2016; Elshahawi et al. 2011; Trisha et al. 2017), to first hos- strate more severe cognitive impairments in CBP+ as com-
pital admission for psychotic symptoms (Barrett et al. 2009; pared to CBP- (Bora 2017; Frydecka et al. 2014; Levy and
Pena et al. 2011; Zanelli et al. 2010). Furthermore, samples Weiss 2010; Simonsen et al. 2011). Importantly, more severe
varied on several characteristics, including active cognitive impairments in CBP+ exist in the domains of verbal
symptomology (versus remission), duration of illness, and memory/learning and executive functioning (Levy and Weiss
the number of untreated depressive episodes. 2010), supporting the notion that mechanisms underlying
CBP+ may be partially independent of those for bipolar dis-
Chronic Bipolar Disorder order more generally (Glahn et al. 2007). Thus, the presence
of a history of psychotic symptoms in bipolar disorder may
In comparison to premorbid and first-episode cognitive func- represent either a more severe subtype of bipolar disorder
tioning, by far the greatest amount of research has been con- (Simonsen et al. 2011), or a separate nosological identity, as
ducted on chronic bipolar disorder. Similar to FEBP+, chronic CBP+ is associated with a younger onset of illness (Bora et al.
bipolar disorder with psychotic features (CBP+) is associated 2010b) and poorer outcome (Bora et al. 2010a; Torres et al.
with impairments across the gamut of cognitive domains 2007).
(Bora 2017; Bora et al. 2010b; Vohringer et al. 2013), espe-
cially in attention, executive function, and memory (Glahn Bipolar Disorder Compared to Schizophrenia Schizophrenia
et al. 2007; Jenkins et al. 2017; Ozdel et al. 2007). In compar- shows greater cognitive impairments compared to CBP+
ison to FEBP+, more studies have examined domains of im- (Fig. 1). Despite some evidence of similar performance across
pairment specifically associated with psychotic features in neurocognitive measures (Ivleva et al. 2012; Jimenez-Lopez
chronic bipolar disorder, with some finding that verbal learn- et al. 2017), studies generally find that schizophrenia is asso-
ing or memory and aspects of executive functioning may be ciated with greater cognitive impairments than CBP+ (Hill
specific trait markers of CBP+ (Bora et al. 2009; Martinez- et al. 2013; Sperry et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2015; Sheffield
Neuropsychol Rev

et al. 2012; Vohringer et al. 2013). For example, in compari- (Donaldson et al. 2003). Overall, it appears that bipolar disor-
son to 57.7% of psychotic bipolar patients, 84% of schizo- der with psychotic features is intermediate on the spectrum of
phrenia patients were cognitively impaired (Reichenberg cognitive impairment between bipolar disorder without psy-
et al. 2010). Yet as with FEBP+, even among studies finding chotic features and schizophrenia, with evidence of increasing
more severe impairments in schizophrenia compared to CBP+ cognitive impairments, including in verbal abilities and exec-
, most studies find that the groups differ in magnitude, but not utive functioning, as the disorder progresses from early to
pattern, but impairment (Barch and Sheffield 2014; Jimenez- chronic stages (Fig. 1).
Lopez et al. 2017; Sheffield et al. 2012; Van Rheenen et al.
2016). Major Depressive Disorder, with Psychotic Features

Specific Deficits Investigation of specific deficits in psychotic In the United States, approximately 14% of individuals diag-
bipolar disorder pales in comparison to the breadth of studies nosed with Major Depressive Disorder report psychotic expe-
in chronic schizophrenia, but databases such as the Bipolar riences (Johnson et al. 1991). Depression with psychotic fea-
and Schizophrenia Spectrum on Intermediate Phenotypes tures signifies individuals who endorse delusions or halluci-
(B-SNIP) are helping to fill this gap (Keshavan et al. 2011). nations during, but not independent of, a depressive episode.
For instance, in a task of context processing, psychotic bipolar Although psychotic features are a diagnostic specifier in the
patients demonstrated reduced target sensitivity and increase DSM-5, there is debate about whether depression with psy-
false alarms, largely due to impaired response inhibition chotic features represents a distinct psychiatric disorder
(Reilly et al. 2017), which was specific to bipolar patients (Keller et al. 2007). Those with psychotic features have been
compared to schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. In an found to experience more frequent and severe psychomotor
executive function task of set-shifting, psychotic bipolar dis- abnormalities (Glassman and Roose 1981), longer episodes of
order patients had increased rates of regressing to a previously depression (Coryell et al. 1987), and greater likelihood of
established response (i.e., regressive errors: Hill et al. 2015). depression recurrence (Aronson et al. 1988) compared to
Importantly, while increased perseverative errors were also those with non-psychotic depression. An additional feature
observed in psychotic bipolar disorder, only regressive errors of psychotic depression that distinguishes it from non-
remained a significant deficit after controlling for general cog- psychotic depression is the profile of cognitive impairment.
nitive ability, suggesting a specific deficit within the larger
impairment of executive functioning. Within the realm of First-Episode Psychotic Depression
working memory, psychotic bipolar participants demonstrate
impairment for high-demand, but not low-demand measures, To our knowledge there is no research on individuals at high
suggesting intact working memory functioning that breaks risk for psychotic depression or premorbid cognition function-
down as demand increases (Frydecka et al. 2014). Finally, in ing in psychotic depression. However, even at their first-
a study of dichotic listening, in which individuals had to focus episode of psychosis, psychotic depression patients have def-
attention on auditory stimuli in one ear or the other to guide icits in full-scale IQ and demonstrate impairments in verbal
behavior, psychotic bipolar patients were significantly im- learning, category fluency, and Trails B even after adjusting
paired and demonstrated performance similar to patients with for IQ (Zanelli et al. 2010). Across 16 tasks, effect sizes range
schizophrenia (Bozikas et al. 2014). Interestingly, this impair- from -0.2 to -0.9 compared with healthy controls, but psychot-
ment was present at the start and end of their hospitalization, ic depression’s cognitive profile is comparable to individuals
indicating the presence of this cognitive deficit in the context from other psychotic disorder groups, indicating a level of
of symptom improvement. severity similar to first-episode schizophrenia (Zanelli et al.
2010). Hill and colleagues also observed a broad impairment
Summary Research generally points to cognitive impairments in psychotic depression, in a small sample of antipsychotic
in CBP+ being trait-like features present even during periods naïve first-episode psychotic depression patients (Hill et al.
of symptom remission (Bora et al. 2010b; Glahn et al. 2007). 2004). Similar to schizophrenia, psychotic depression patients
There is cumulating research that cognitive deficits in chronic were impaired in all areas of cognition including visual mem-
bipolar disorder may be stable over time, including from el- ory, verbal memory, and spatial abilities.
derly chronic bipolar disorder populations, which failed to
find evidence substantiating accelerated cognitive decline Chronic Psychotic Depression
(Schouws et al. 2012). However, more empirical studies need
to be conducted specifically examining the longitudinal In more chronic stages of psychotic depression, a global def-
course of cognitive deficits in CBP+, as well as the contribu- icit emerges. For instance, psychotic depression participants
tion of several factors to cognitive impairment, including the have been shown to be significantly impaired on tests of at-
contribution of chronic antipsychotic medication usage tention, working memory, executive functioning, verbal
Neuropsychol Rev

memory recall (both immediate and delayed), and delayed participants and analyzed whether cortisol levels were associ-
logical memory recall and recognition, although they demon- ated with cognitive functioning (Gomez et al. 2006). This in-
strate intact simple attention (Gomez et al. 2006). Effect sizes vestigation was based on previous research revealing associa-
between -0.82 and -1.86 have also been observed on Trails B, tions between higher cortisol and lower cognitive ability in
WAIS Vocabulary, WAIS Block Design, Stroop, Paragraph depression (O'Brien et al. 1996) as well as hyperactivity of
Recall Test, and immediate visual memory when compared the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis in psychotic de-
to healthy controls (Schatzberg et al. 2000). Although both pression (Belanoff et al. 2001) – a biological system that regu-
studies had relatively small sample sizes (<30 in each group), lates the production of cortisol. Psychotic depression partici-
these data support the notion that psychotic depression partic- pants performed significantly worse than non-psychotic de-
ipants have a fairly generalized cognitive impairment in rela- pression participants on measures of working memory, execu-
tion to non-psychiatric controls. tive functioning, processing speed, and verbal memory.
Cortisol levels were found to be elevated in psychotic depres-
MDD With and Without Psychotic Features sion and correlated negatively with verbal memory and pro-
cessing speed across all subjects. This finding was recently
Research in the field of psychotic depression has not only ad- replicated and extended to implicate genetic variation in psy-
dressed cognitive ability compared with healthy subjects, but chotic depression as a mechanism for the relationship between
has also explored cognitive ability in psychotic depression cortisol and cognitive performance (Keller et al. 2017).
compared with non-psychotic depression. This question works
to isolate the contribution of psychosis to the cognitive profile, Summary
as individuals with non-psychotic depression also experience
global cognitive impairment (Goodall et al. 2018). One meta- Although more work is needed, current evidence in psychotic
analysis of five studies found that psychotic depression patients depression reveals a broad profile of cognitive impairment
were impaired in all areas of cognition when compared to non- when compared to both healthy subjects as well as patients
psychotic depression (effect sizes ranging from -0.37 to - with non-psychotic depression (Fig. 2). There is some sugges-
0.73: Fleming et al. 2004). Areas of cognition that revealed tion from meta-analyses that processing speed, verbal memo-
the strongest impairment in psychotic depression were verbal ry, and executive functioning are most impaired in the context
memory, executive functioning, and processing speed. A more of psychotic depression. The majority of studies match psy-
recent meta-analysis that included 12 studies observed a signif- chotic depression and non-psychotic depression participants
icant global cognitive impairment in psychotic depression com- on endogenous depressive symptoms, reducing the likelihood
pared with non-psychotic depression, as calculated by a sum- that the more severe cognitive impairment observed in psy-
mary measure of effect sizes across all domains (Zaninotto chotic depression is due to more severely depressed mood. In
et al. 2015). Within the context of a global impairment, signif- fact, in DSM-5, severity of depression and presence of psy-
icant specific deficits in verbal learning, visual learning, and chotic features have been separated, reflecting that psychotic
processing speed were observed. Overall, these meta-analyses symptoms are not simply a manifestation of exceptionally
reveal that the presence of psychosis in depression contributes depressed mood. Instead, psychotic features in the context of
to greater cognitive impairment than the experience of depres- depressed mood are associated with a broad cognitive impair-
sion without psychotic features. ment that is present at the first-episode of psychosis, is poten-
Further analysis of psychotic depression compared with tially less severe with appropriate treatment, and may be re-
non-psychotic depression has revealed the impact of medica- lated to elevated cortisol.
tion use and cortisol on cognitive impairment. For instance, the
Zaninotto meta-analysis found a greater global impairment in Psychotic-Like Experiences
drug-free psychotic depression than was observed in the whole
psychotic depression sample, suggesting that untreated psy- A small literature exists on cognitive deficits in individuals
chotic depression is associated with greater cognitive impair- who endorse psychotic-like experiences. These individuals
ment than psychotic depression that is treated with medications exemplify how conceptualization of symptoms as dimension-
(Bmedications^ varied across studies and included anti-depres- al can yield richer understanding of brain-behavior relation-
sants, anti-psychotics, mood stabilizers, and anxiolytics). This ships. Psychotic-like experiences are often self-reported expe-
finding may also be influenced by anti-depressant use in the riences of perceptual abnormality or reality distortion in the
medicated patients, as anti-depressants have a modest, positive general population that, by definition, cause minimal func-
effect on a wide range of cognitive domains (e.g. processing tional difficulties (van Os et al. 2009). Due to the robust find-
speed, attention, memory) (Prado et al. 2018). Gomez and col- ings of cognitive impairment in primary psychotic disorders,
leagues (Gomez et al. 2006) measured cortisol levels in psy- researchers have started investigating cognitive functioning in
chotic depression, non-psychotic depression, and healthy those who endorse psychotic-like experiences.
Neuropsychol Rev

Fig. 2 Illustration of cognitive


deficits in MDD patients with and
without psychotic symptoms,
compared with controls (shown as
effect size of 0). While non-
psychotic MDD patients demon-
strate deficits in cognition, those
with psychotic symptoms exhibit
even greater deficits in all areas of
cognition. This pattern suggest
that the presence of psychosis
contributes to greater cognitive
impairment. Estimates based on
data from (Bora et al. 2013; Rock
et al. 2014; Zaninotto et al. 2015)

In one of the first studies to directly ask whether cognition diagnosing a mental health disorder, and psychotic disorders
is intact in individuals who endorse psychotic-like experi- are among the most disabling (Rossler et al. 2005), causing
ences, Barnett and colleagues (Barnett et al. 2012) utilized a significant burden to families and society at large (Ohaeri
population-based sample in Great Britain of 2918 participants 2003). Understanding illness-related factors that contribute
(Barnett et al. 2012). Of those, 22% reported psychotic-like to functional disability is of great interest, as it carries the hope
experiences at age 53, and those who reported psychotic-like of specific interventions to support patients in achieving great-
experiences had poorer childhood cognitive test scores at ages er independence. Somewhat surprisingly, severity of positive
8 and 15. Cognitive deficits were also associated with greater psychotic symptoms is minimally associated with functional
general health problems as reported on a self-report question- outcome (e.g Tabares-Seisdedos et al. 2008), suggesting that
naire at age 53, however this association was not significant delusions and hallucinations are not the primary barrier to
after controlling for psychotic-like experiences, suggesting work and social functioning. In fact, of those who were ad-
that childhood cognitive deficits are a specific risk factor for mitted to a hospital for first-episode psychosis, 65% of those
the experience of psychosis in adulthood. Presence of sub- who achieved syndromal recovery (i.e. resolution of symp-
clinical psychosis has been associated with processing speed toms) failed to achieve functional recovery (Tohen et al.
deficits as measured by the digit symbol task (Rossler et al. 2000). Instead of pointing to symptom resolution as a marker
2015). In data from the Human Connectome Project, 21.6% of of functioning, research consistently demonstrates a critical
individuals endorsed at least one psychotic-like experience, role of cognitive deficits in functional capacity.
and psychotic-like experiences were significantly negatively
associated with general cognitive ability, replicating previous
findings (Sheffield et al. 2016). Further, individuals who en- Cognition and Function Status in Schizophrenia
dorse symptoms of schizotypy in the general population have
below average cognitive performance (Dinn et al. 2002) (Park Relationships between functional outcome and cognitive abil-
et al. 1995). ity can be seen even in individuals at high-risk of developing
Research in psychotic-like experiences, although in a rela- psychosis, suggesting they shape the trajectory of that individ-
tively early stage, suggests that cognitive impairment can be ual's capabilities prior to disease onset. Poor verbal memory
observed even in individuals from the general population who has been associated with poor concurrent social functioning in
experience subtle, non-impairing psychotic experiences. ultra-high risk individuals (Niendam et al. 2006) as well as
These data support the research domain criteria conceptuali- poor functional outcome 3-5 years (Carrion et al. 2013) and 13
zation of psychosis-risk as being related to dimensional im- years (Lin et al. 2011) after assessment of memory. Processing
pairments in global cognitive functioning. speed, attention, and verbal fluency measures at baseline also
predict functioning several years later (Carrion et al. 2013; Lin
Functional Implications for Cognitive Impairment et al. 2011), both in the realms of social functioning and role
in Psychotic Disorders functioning. In first-episode psychosis, patients considered to
have Bgood^ premorbid adjustment show better overall cog-
A primary interest for understanding cognitive deficits across nitive functioning (Rabinowitz et al. 2002); however
the psychosis spectrum is understanding their role in function- premorbid functioning is not a perfect predictor of psychotic
al impairment. Functional limitations are a critical facet of disorder status, as 40% of ultra-high risk subjects identified as
Neuropsychol Rev

having poor functional outcome did not transition to having a instance, across 12 studies, 50% of them revealed verbal
primary psychotic disorder (Carrion et al. 2013). memory ability as a significant predictor of psychosocial func-
In chronic schizophrenia, cross-sectional work has also re- tioning, and in several of those studies, verbal memory was
vealed associations between cognitive capacity and current the only cognitive domain associated with functional outcome
functioning. For instance, functional capacity as measured (Wingo et al. 2009). Additionally, low-functioning euthymic
by a performance-based skills test is associated with relational bipolar patients had significantly lower verbal memory and
memory, cognitive control, and processing speed abilities, executive functioning compared to high-functioning bipolar
while verbal memory is associated with self-reported func- patients (Martinez-Aran et al. 2007). In a study on depression,
tioning (Sheffield et al. 2014). Associations between cognitive patients whose cognition worsened from baseline to 6-month
performance and real-world functioning appear to be mediat- follow-up represented those patients who remained signifi-
ed by functional capacity. Patients with poor cognitive ability cantly disabled at 6-months (Jaeger et al. 2006), although it
have poorer functional capacity, which predicts more impaired is unclear how many of those patients suffered from psychotic
real-world functioning in three areas (interpersonal skills, symptoms.
work skills, and community activities; Bowie et al. 2006). Cognitive deficits across the psychosis spectrum have crit-
Therefore, cognitive deficits in schizophrenia appear to con- ical implications for patients’ ability to function in their daily
tribute to functional impairment through deficits in the ability life. In all psychotic disorders, cognitive deficits are associated
to perform skills of daily living. with poor current functioning, but also predict functional out-
Longitudinal studies have further exposed cognitive capac- come up to 13 years later. The longevity and predictability of
ity as a robust predictor of future functional outcome. A re- these functional impairments may not be surprising, given the
view of longitudinal studies from Green and colleagues chronicity and stability of cognitive deficits across the course
(Green et al. 2004) described medium effect sizes (Cohen’s of illness. The presence of cognitive deficits prior to first-ep-
d=.50) for the relationship between cognitive constructs and isode, and association between those early cognitive abilities
functional outcome in schizophrenia (Green et al. 2004). In and functioning, further highlights the importance of interven-
these studies, cognitive ability predicted the rate of improve- ing on cognitive impairment early, to help these individuals
ment in work performance (Bell and Bryson 2001; Bryson achieve and maintain a higher lifetime level of functioning.
and Bell 2003), change in social and community outcomes
(Dickerson et al. 1999) and the number of hours worked 12
and 24 months after cognitive testing (Gold et al. 2002). Discussion
Baseline cognition predicted activities of daily living
(ADLs) four years later, and change in cognition was related In this review, we outline decades of research indicating the
to change in ADLs. Further, attention, working memory, and presence of cognitive deficits in psychotic disorders across the
verbal fluency predicted community outcome, as measured by life course. Considering these findings as a whole, we docu-
work/school functions and independent living (Jaeger et al. ment robust, reliable, and stable cognitive impairment in
2003). The relatively stable cognitive deficits observed in chronic psychosis, regardless of affective status, suggesting
schizophrenia are therefore predictive of functional outcomes that psychotic disorders are incontrovertibly associated with
in a range of areas, including work, social functioning and cognitive dysfunction (Fig. 1). Differences among diagnostic
independent living across the course of illness. categories emerge when considering timing and magnitude,
and possibly specificity, although the data is somewhat mixed.
Cognition and Functional Status in Affective Psychosis In schizophrenia, IQ deficits are observed during childhood,
become more severe during adolescence, and reach a stable
Cognitive deficits also have functional implications in affec- level of impairment (approximately 1 standard deviation be-
tive disorders, although few studies have investigated this as- low the norm for global cognition) by the first-episode of
sociation in affective psychosis specifically. In bipolar disor- psychosis. This general impairment remains consistent during
der generally, social functioning deficits are associated with the chronic state, contributes to functional impairment, and is
deficits in planning and problem solving, suggesting a role of minimally influenced by psychiatric medications. In affective
executive difficulties in problems with daily living (Laes and psychosis, there is some evidence of mild global impairment
Sponheim 2006). Change in cognitive scores over the course during childhood (particularly for those later developing psy-
of one year predicted functioning changes over that follow-up chotic bipolar disorder), but these premorbid impairments are
period, as did the overall deficit in processing speed (Tabares- less severe than in schizophrenia. By the first-episode of psy-
Seisdedos et al. 2008). Interestingly, the majority of studies chosis, bipolar and depression patients demonstrate a global
assessing cognition and functioning in bipolar disorder have deficit similar in pattern to schizophrenia, but less severe in
implicated verbal memory as a particularly strong predictor of magnitude (Fig. 1). In schizophrenia, there is some evidence
functional outcome (Sanchez-Moreno et al. 2009). For of specific impairments in verbal memory and processing
Neuropsychol Rev

speed, while affective psychosis may demonstrate greater im- episode schizophrenia, but in the chronic state, these differ-
pairment in the domains of verbal memory and executive ences are not as strong. Differential timing of cognitive impair-
functioning; however these deficits exist in the context of a ment provides a marker of different disease states, and begs the
generalized deficit across all psychotic disorders and even in question of whether the mechanism that causes or contributes
those experiencing psychotic-like experiences. to cognitive impairment in both affective and non-affective
psychosis is the same, just with different timing, or whether
Covariation Between Cognitive Deficits and Psychotic cognitive impairment is simply a shared phenotype with dis-
Symptom Severity tinct etiologies. Working to elucidate this question are large-
scale studies such as the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental
Surprisingly, despite the clear co-occurrence of psychotic Cohort (Gur et al. 2014), which addresses early development
symptoms and cognitive impairment, there is little evidence of psychotic disorders, and the Bipolar and Schizophrenia
to suggest that severity of psychotic experiences is directly Network on Intermediate Phenotype (B-SNIP: Hill et al.
associated with severity of cognitive deficits (e.g. Bell et al. 2013), which includes large cohorts of chronic patients with
1994; Heydebrand et al. 2004). Global IQ, as well as perfor- both affective and non-affective psychosis. Studies of this na-
mance on tasks of cognitive flexibility, verbal fluency, and ture allow for the continued analysis of differential and shared
processing speed, do not demonstrate significant linear asso- phenotypes, including possible neurobiological mechanisms
ciations with positive symptoms, suggesting that those with underlying cognitive impairment across the psychosis
more frequent and severe delusions and hallucinations are not spectrum.
those also experiencing the worst cognitive impairment (Bell
et al. 1994; Berman et al. 1997) (Heydebrand et al. 2004). The Improving Cognitive Function Across the Spectrum
lack of association between severity of cognitive and psychot- of Psychotic Disorders
ic symptoms is elusive, and diminishes the frequent assump-
tion that presence of psychotic symptoms impairs perfor- Finally, given the clear presence of cognitive deficits across
mance on cognitive tasks. Instead, data suggests that negative the psychosis spectrum, and its contribution to functional im-
symptoms are more closely related to cognitive ability pairment, how can cognition be improved? As noted earlier,
(Berman et al. 1997; Heydebrand et al. 2004; Harvey et al. anti-psychotic medications have minimal efficacy in improv-
2006), as well as functional outcome (Evans et al. 2004), ing cognitive ability (Nielsen et al. 2015), leading many to
possibly related to impaired reward processing (Barch and consider non-pharmacological alternatives. Cognitive remedi-
Dowd 2010), although these data are largely specific to ation is the leading avenue of research, in the hopes that sys-
schizophrenia. While it is not in the scope of this review to tematic training in cognitive tasks might improve cognitive
explore mechanism, the lack of relationship between psychot- functioning. To date, the results are mixed. In chronic patients,
ic symptoms and cognitive impairment is notable. The timing meta-analysis reveals small to moderate improvement in glob-
of when these symptoms arise may provide some insight into al cognition, which appears stable over follow-up (Wykes
their relationship, as cognitive impairment in schizophrenia et al. 2011). Cognitive remediation is most effective when
appears prior to onset of psychotic symptoms, suggesting that adjunctive rehabilitation support is provided, particularly with
presence of positive symptoms does not directly cause cogni- supportive, as opposed to drill and practice, remediation pro-
tive impairment. tocols. In first-episode patients, meta-analysis demonstrates
smaller, non-significant effect sizes for general cognition, pos-
Non-Affective Versus Affective Psychosis sibly due to a small number of trials with fewer participants
than is seen with chronic subjects (Revell et al. 2015).
When considering timing of cognitive and psychotic symp- Cognitive remediation is getting more attention for use in
toms, there is a notable discrepancy between affective and bipolar disorder (Miskowiak et al. 2018), but current results
non-affective disorders. Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a on efficacy are mixed (Demant et al. 2015; Bonnin et al.
fairly reliable deficit in IQ during childhood, well before their 2016). In general, cognitive remediation represents an impor-
first-episode of psychosis. Bipolar and depression patients, tant avenue of continued research, and more data is needed in
however, appear to develop cognitive deficits closer to their ultra-high risk subjects to assess efficacy of remediation on
first-episode, showing much less robust premorbid impair- buffering the effects of first-episode psychosis. Combination
ments. These differences in cognitive development have con- of cognitive and functional remediation is also a promising
tributed to two hypotheses: 1) that schizophrenia is a disorder direction (Torrent et al. 2013), providing a more comprehen-
of neurodevelopment, and 2) that bipolar disorder takes a neu- sive approach to cognitive and functional improvement.
rodegenerative course (Lewandowski et al. 2011b). These hy- Lastly, there is also a growing body of research examining
potheses are further bolstered by the fact that first-episode bi- the effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in psy-
polar patients demonstrate less impaired cognition than first- chosis (Falkai et al. 2017; Firth et al. 2017; Su et al. 2016).
Neuropsychol Rev

This research suggests that regular aerobic exercise improves Ancin, I., Cabranes, J. A., Santos, J. L., Sanchez-Morla, E., & Barabash,
A. (2013). Executive deficits: a continuum schizophrenia-bipolar
global cognition in schizophrenia, possibly through the mech-
disorder or specific to schizophrenia? Journal of Psychiatric
anism of increased brain volume, particularly within the hip- Research, 47(11), 1564–1571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.
pocampus. The most effective duration, frequency, and type of 2013.07.008.
exercise is still being investigated, as is the persistence of Ang, Y. G., & Tan, H. Y. (2004). Academic deterioration prior to first
episode schizophrenia in young Singaporean males. Psychiatry
cognitive improvement following completion of the exercise
Research, 121(3), 303–307.
intervention. Aronson, T. A., Shukla, S., Gujavarty, K., Hoff, A., DiBuono, M., & Khan,
E. (1988). Relapse in delusional depression: a retrospective study of
the course of treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29(1), 12–21.
Arts, B., Simons, C. J., & Os, J. (2013). Evidence for the impact of the
Summary CACNA1C risk allele rs1006737 on 2-year cognitive functioning in
bipolar disorder. Psychiatric Genetics, 23(1), 41–42. https://doi.org/
Cognitive impairment is a dimensional phenotype present 10.1097/YPG.0b013e328358641c.
across the psychosis spectrum. Although not linearly related Aylward, E., Walker, E., & Bettes, B. (1984). Intelligence in schizophre-
nia: meta-analysis of the research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10(3),
in terms of severity, there is no doubt that the presence of or
430–459.
vulnerability to psychotic experiences confers risk for cogni- Ayres, A. M., Busatto, G. F., Menezes, P. R., Schaufelberger, M. S.,
tive deficits. In terms of development, schizophrenia patients Coutinho, L., Murray, R. M., et al. (2007). Cognitive deficits in
exhibit cognitive impairment during childhood that continues first-episode psychosis: a population-based study in Sao Paulo,
Brazil. Schizophrenia Research, 90(1-3), 338–343. https://doi.org/
to decline, and then stabilize, during the chronic state. Bipolar
10.1016/j.schres.2006.09.026.
patients, on the other hand, demonstrate minimal impairment Bachman, P., Reichenberg, A., Rice, P., Woolsey, M., Chaves, O.,
early on in the life course, but show deficits by the first-epi- Martinez, D., et al. (2010). Deconstructing processing speed deficits
sode, that appear to worsen as the disease progresses. Across in schizophrenia: application of a parametric digit symbol coding
psychotic disorders, a global deficit is present, with impair- test. Schizophrenia Research, 118(1-3), 6–11. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.schres.2010.02.1029.
ments apparent in nearly all areas of cognition. Assessment of Backman, L., Lindenberger, U., Li, S. C., & Nyberg, L. (2010). Linking
domain specificity within psychotic disorders, on a backdrop cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter func-
of global impairment, reveals the largest deficit in processing tioning: recent data and future avenues. Neuroscience and
speed in schizophrenia (e.g. Dickinson et al. 2007) and exec- Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 670–677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neubiorev.2009.12.008.
utive functioning in bipolar disorder (e.g. Bora et al. 2010b) as Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working
well relatively greater impairment in verbal memory across all memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.
groups (Mesholam-Gately et al. 2009; Daglas et al. 2016; Barch, D. M., & Ceaser, A. (2012). Cognition in schizophrenia: core
Gomez et al. 2006). Research on associations between psy- psychological and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 16(1), 27–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.015.
chotic symptoms and cognitive impairment, differential devel- Barch, D. M., & Dowd, E. C. (2010). Goal representations and motiva-
opmental mechanisms across affective and non-affective psy- tional drive in schizophrenia: the role of prefrontal-striatal interac-
chosis, and treatment options for cognitive impairment are tions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(5), 919–934. https://doi.org/10.
important steps for improving the lives of individuals with 1093/schbul/sbq068.
Barch, D. M., & Sheffield, J. M. (2014). Cognitive impairments in psy-
psychosis. chotic disorders: common mechanisms and measurement. World
Psychiatry, 13(3), 224–232. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20145.
Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., MacDonald 3rd, A. W., Braver, T. S., &
Cohen, J. D. (2003). Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia:
diagnostic specificity, 4-week course, and relationships to clinical
References symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(1), 132–143.
Barnett, J. H., McDougall, F., Xu, M. K., Croudace, T. J., Richards, M., &
Addington, J., & Addington, D. (2005). Patterns of premorbid function- Jones, P. B. (2012). Childhood cognitive function and adult psycho-
ing in first episode psychosis: relationship to 2-year outcome. Acta pathology: associations with psychotic and non-psychotic symp-
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112(1), 40–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. toms in the general population. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
1600-0447.2005.00511.x. 201, 124–130. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.111.102053.
Albus, M., Hubmann, W., Wahlheim, C., Sobizack, N., Franz, U., & Barrett, S. L., Mulholland, C. C., Cooper, S. J., & Rushe, T. M. (2009).
Mohr, F. (1996). Contrasts in neuropsychological test profile be- Patterns of neurocognitive impairment in first-episode bipolar dis-
tween patients with first-episode schizophrenia and first-episode af- order and schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(1),
fective disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94(2), 87–93. 67–72. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.054874.
Aminoff, S. R., Hellvin, T., Lagerberg, T. V., Berg, A. O., Andreassen, O. Bechdolf, A., Thompson, A., Nelson, B., Cotton, S., Simmons, M. B.,
A., & Melle, I. (2013). Neurocognitive features in subgroups of Amminger, G. P., et al. (2010). Experience of trauma and conversion
bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 15(3), 272–283. https://doi. to psychosis in an ultra-high-risk (prodromal) group. Acta
org/10.1111/bdi.12061. Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121(5), 377–384. https://doi.org/10.
An, S. K., Kang, J. I., Park, J. Y., Kim, K. R., Lee, S. Y., & Lee, E. (2010). 1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01542.x.
Attribution bias in ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode Belanoff, J. K., Kalehzan, M., Sund, B., Fleming Ficek, S. K., &
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 118(1-3), 54–61. https:// Schatzberg, A. F. (2001). Cortisol activity and cognitive changes
doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.025. in psychotic major depression. The American Journal of
Neuropsychol Rev

Psychiatry, 158(10), 1612–1616. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp. paradigm. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(5), 1212–1219. https://
158.10.1612. doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.02.014.
Bell, M. D., & Bryson, G. (2001). Work rehabilitation in schizophrenia: Brebion, G., David, A. S., Bressan, R. A., & Pilowsky, L. S. (2006).
does cognitive impairment limit improvement? Schizophrenia Processing speed: a strong predictor of verbal memory performance
Bulletin, 27(2), 269–279. in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Bell, M. D., Lysaker, P. H., Milstein, R. M., & Beam-Goulet, J. L. (1994). Neuropsychology, 28(3), 370–382. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Concurrent validity of the cognitive component of schizophrenia: 13803390590935390.
relationship of PANSS scores to neuropsychological assessments. Brewer, W. J., Francey, S. M., Wood, S. J., Jackson, H. J., Pantelis, C.,
Psychiatry Research, 54(1), 51–58. Phillips, L. J., et al. (2005). Memory impairments identified in peo-
Bergh, S., Hjorthoj, C., Sorensen, H. J., Fagerlund, B., Austin, S., Secher, ple at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode
R. G., et al. (2016). Predictors and longitudinal course of cognitive psychosis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(1), 71–78.
functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, 10years after base- https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.71.
line: The OPUS study. Schizophrenia Research, 175(1-3), 57–63. Bryson, G., & Bell, M. D. (2003). Initial and final work performance in
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.03.025. schizophrenia: cognitive and symptom predictors. The Journal of
Berman, I., Viegner, B., Merson, A., Allan, E., Pappas, D., & Green, A. I. Nervous and Mental Disease, 191(2), 87–92. https://doi.org/10.
(1997). Differential relationships between positive and negative 1097/01.NMD.0000050937.06332.3C.
symptoms and neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Bryzgalov, L. O., Korbolina, E. E., Brusentsov, I. I., Leberfarb, E. Y.,
Schizophrenia Research, 25(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Bondar, N. P., & Merkulova, T. I. (2018). Novel functional variants
S0920-9964(96)00098-9. at the GWAS-implicated loci might confer risk to major depressive
Bonnin, C. M., Torrent, C., Arango, C., Amann, B. L., Sole, B., disorder. bipolar affective disorder and schizophrenia. BMC
Gonzalez-Pinto, A., et al. (2016). Functional remediation in bipolar Neurosci, 19(Suppl 1), 22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-018-
disorder: 1-year follow-up of neurocognitive and functional out- 0414-3.
come. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(1), 87–93. https:// Cannon, M., Jones, P., Huttunen, M. O., Tanskanen, A., Huttunen, T.,
doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.162123. Rabe-Hesketh, S., et al. (1999). School performance in Finnish chil-
Bora, E. (2015). Neurodevelopmental origin of cognitive impairment in dren and later development of schizophrenia: a population-based
schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 45(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/ longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(5), 457–463.
10.1017/s0033291714001263. Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Taylor, A., Murray,
Bora, E. (2017). Neurocognitive features in clinical subgroups of bipolar R. M., et al. (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-
disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 229, 125– developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder:
134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.057. results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General
Bora, E., & Ozerdem, A. (2017). Meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of Psychiatry, 59(5), 449–456.
cognition in bipolar disorder: comparison with healthy controls and Cannon, M., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Murray, R. M., Harrington, H., &
schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 47(16), 2753–2766. https:// Poulton, R. (2006). Neuropsychological performance at the age of
doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001490. 13 years and adult schizophreniform disorder: prospective birth co-
Bora, E., & Pantelis, C. (2015). Meta-analysis of Cognitive Impairment in hort study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 463–464. https://
First-Episode Bipolar Disorder: Comparison With First-Episode doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.105.020552.
Schizophrenia and Healthy Controls. Schizophrenia Bulletin, Carrion, R. E., McLaughlin, D., Goldberg, T. E., Auther, A. M., Olsen, R.
41(5), 1095–1104. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu198. H., Olvet, D. M., et al. (2013). Prediction of functional outcome in
Bora, E., Vahip, S., Akdeniz, F., Gonul, A. S., Eryavuz, A., Ogut, M., individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry,
et al. (2007). The effect of previous psychotic mood episodes on 70(11), 1133–1142. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.
cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients. Bipolar 1909.
Disorders, 9(5), 468–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618. Chemerinski, E., Triebwasser, J., Roussos, P., & Siever, L. J. (2013).
2007.00469.x. Schizotypal personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders,
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes of 27(5), 652–679. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2012_26_053.
bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in Cornblatt, B., Obuchowski, M., Roberts, S., Pollack, S., & Erlenmeyer-
euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. Journal of Kimling, L. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral precursors of schizo-
Affective Disorders, 113(1-2), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad. phrenia. Development and Psychopathology, 11(3), 487–508.
2008.06.009. Coryell, W., Endicott, J., & Keller, M. (1987). The importance of psy-
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2010a). Cognitive impairment in chotic features to major depression: course and outcome during a 2-
affective psychoses: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, year follow-up. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 75(1), 78–85.
36(1), 112–125. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp093. Crow, T. J., Done, D. J., & Sacker, A. (1995). Childhood precursors of
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2010b). Neurocognitive markers of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins. European Archives of
psychosis in bipolar disorder: a meta-analytic study. Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 245(2), 61–69.
Affective Disorders, 127(1-3), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad. Daban, C., Martinez-Aran, A., Torrent, C., Tabares-Seisdedos, R.,
2010.02.117. Balanza-Martinez, V., Salazar-Fraile, J., et al. (2006). Specificity
Bora, E., Harrison, B. J., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2013). Cognitive of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder versus schizophrenia. A sys-
impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. tematic review. Psychother Psychosom, 75(2), 72–84. https://doi.
Psychological medicine, 43(10), 2017–2026. org/10.1159/000090891.
Bowie, C. R., Reichenberg, A., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R. K., & Harvey, Daglas, R., Yucel, M., Cotton, S., Allott, K., Hetrick, S., & Berk, M.
P. D. (2006). Determinants of real-world functional performance in (2015). Cognitive impairment in first-episode mania: a systematic
schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional ca- review of the evidence in the acute and remission phases of the
pacity, and symptoms. The American Journal of Psychiatry, illness. Int J Bipolar Disord, 3, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40345-
163(3), 418–425. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.418. 015-0024-2.
Bozikas, V. P., Kosmidis, M. H., Giannakou, M., Kechayas, P., Tsotsi, S., Daglas, R., Allott, K., Yucel, M., Pantelis, C., Macneil, C. A., Berk, M.,
Kiosseoglou, G., et al. (2014). Controlled shifting of attention in et al. (2016). The trajectory of cognitive functioning following first
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder through a dichotic listening episode mania: A 12-month follow-up study. The Australian and
Neuropsychol Rev

New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50(12), 1186–1197. https://doi. Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Rock, D., Roberts, S. A., Janal, M.,
org/10.1177/0004867415622272. Kestenbaum, C., Cornblatt, B., et al. (2000). Attention, memory,
Davidson, M., Galderisi, S., Weiser, M., Werbeloff, N., Fleischhacker, W. and motor skills as childhood predictors of schizophrenia-related
W., Keefe, R. S., et al. (2009). Cognitive effects of antipsychotic psychoses: the New York High-Risk Project. The American
drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: Journal of Psychiatry, 157(9), 1416–1422. https://doi.org/10.1176/
a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST). The American appi.ajp.157.9.1416.
Journal of Psychiatry, 166(6), 675–682. https://doi.org/10.1176/ Evans, J. D., Bond, G. R., Meyer, P. S., Kim, H. W., Lysaker, P. H.,
appi.ajp.2008.08060806. Gibson, P. J., et al. (2004). Cognitive and clinical predictors of suc-
Demant, K. M., Vinberg, M., Kessing, L. V., & Miskowiak, K. W. (2015). cess in vocational rehabilitation in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
Effects of Short-Term Cognitive Remediation on Cognitive Research, 70(2-3), 331–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2004.
Dysfunction in Partially or Fully Remitted Individuals with 01.011.
Bipolar Disorder: Results of a Randomised Controlled Trial. PLoS Falkai, P., Malchow, B., & Schmitt, A. (2017). Aerobic exercise and its
One, 10(6), e0127955. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone. effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Current Opinion in
0127955. Psychiatry, 30(3), 171–175. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.
Demmo, C., Lagerberg, T. V., Aminoff, S. R., Hellvin, T., Kvitland, L. R., 0000000000000326.
Simonsen, C., et al. (2016). History of psychosis and previous epi- Fatouros-Bergman, H., Cervenka, S., Flyckt, L., Edman, G., & Farde, L.
sodes as potential explanatory factors for neurocognitive impairment (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in drug-naive pa-
in first-treatment bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 18(2), 136– tients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 158(1-3), 156–
147. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12377. 162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.034.
Demmo, C., Lagerberg, T. V., Aminoff, S. R., Hellvin, T., Kvitland, L. R., Firth, J., Stubbs, B., Rosenbaum, S., Vancampfort, D., Malchow, B.,
Simonsen, C., et al. (2017). Course of neurocognitive function in Schuch, F., et al. (2017). Aerobic Exercise Improves Cognitive
first treatment bipolar I disorder: One-year follow-up study. Functioning in People With Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review
Psychiatry Research, 249, 286–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. and Meta-Analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(3), 546–556. https://
psychres.2016.12.048. doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw115.
Dickerson, F., Boronow, J. J., Ringel, N., & Parente, F. (1999). Social Fleming, S. K., Blasey, C., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2004).
functioning and neurocognitive deficits in outpatients with schizo- Neuropsychological correlates of psychotic features in major de-
phrenia: a 2-year follow-up. Schizophrenia Research, 37(1), 13–20. pressive disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Journal of
Dickerson, F., Stallings, C., Vaughan, C., Origoni, A., Khushalani, S., Psychiatric Research, 38(1), 27–35.
Dickinson, D., et al. (2011). Cognitive functioning in recent onset Flowers, S. A., Ryan, K. A., Lai, Z., McInnis, M. G., & Ellingrod, V. L.
psychosis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(6), (2016). Interaction between COMT rs5993883 and second genera-
367–371. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31821cd0ff. tion antipsychotics is linked to decreases in verbal cognition and
Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M. E., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Overlooking the cognitive control in bipolar disorder. BMC Psychol, 4, 14. https://
obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and doi.org/10.1186/s40359-016-0118-3.
other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of General Francey, S. M., Jackson, H. J., Phillips, L. J., Wood, S. J., Yung, A. R., &
Psychiatry, 64(5), 532–542. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. McGorry, P. D. (2005). Sustained attention in young people at high
Dickinson, D., Ragland, J. D., Gold, J. M., & Gur, R. C. (2008). General risk of psychosis does not predict transition to psychosis.
and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: Goliath defeats Schizophrenia Research, 79(1), 127–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
David? Biological Psychiatry, 64(9), 823–827. https://doi.org/10. schres.2005.06.023.
1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.005. Frydecka, D., Eissa, A. M., Hewedi, D. H., Ali, M., Drapala, J., Misiak,
Dickson, H., Laurens, K. R., Cullen, A. E., & Hodgins, S. (2012). Meta- B., et al. (2014). Impairments of working memory in schizophrenia
analyses of cognitive and motor function in youth aged 16 years and and bipolar disorder: the effect of history of psychotic symptoms
younger who subsequently develop schizophrenia. Psychological and different aspects of cognitive task demands. Frontiers in
Medicine, 42(4), 743–755. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001693. Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 416. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.
Dickson, H., Cullen, A. E., Reichenberg, A., Hodgins, S., Campbell, D. 2014.00416.
D., Morris, R. G., et al. (2014). Cognitive impairment among chil- Fuller, R., Nopoulos, P., Arndt, S., O'Leary, D., Ho, B. C., & Andreasen,
dren at-risk for schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 50, N. C. (2002). Longitudinal assessment of premorbid cognitive func-
92–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.003. tioning in patients with schizophrenia through examination of stan-
Dinn, W. M., Harris, C. L., Aycicegi, A., Greene, P., & Andover, M. S. dardized scholastic test performance. The American Journal of
(2002). Positive and negative schizotypy in a student sample: Psychiatry, 159(7), 1183–1189. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.
neurocognitive and clinical correlates. Schizophrenia Research, 159.7.1183.
56(1-2), 171–185. Fusar-Poli, P., Deste, G., Smieskova, R., Barlati, S., Yung, A. R., Howes,
Donaldson, S., Goldstein, L. H., Landau, S., Raymont, V., & Frangou, S. O., et al. (2012). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a
(2003). The Maudsley Bipolar Disorder Project: the effect of med- meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 562–571.
ication, family history, and duration of illness on IQ and memory in https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592.
bipolar I disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(1), 86–93. Glahn, D. C., Bearden, C. E., Barguil, M., Barrett, J., Reichenberg, A.,
Elie, D., Poirier, M., Chianetta, J., Durand, M., Gregoire, C., & Grignon, Bowden, C. L., et al. (2007). The neurocognitive signature of psy-
S. (2010). Cognitive effects of antipsychotic dosage and chotic bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 62(8), 910–916.
polypharmacy: a study with the BACS in patients with schizophre- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.001.
nia and schizoaffective disorder. Journal of Psychopharmacology, Glassman, A. H., & Roose, S. P. (1981). Delusional depression. A distinct
24(7), 1037–1044. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881108100777. clinical entity? Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(4), 424–427.
Elshahawi, H. H., Essawi, H., Rabie, M. A., Mansour, M., Beshry, Z. A., Glosser, G., Butters, N., & Kaplan, E. (1977). Visuoperceptual processes
& Mansour, A. N. (2011). Cognitive functions among euthymic in brain damaged patients on the digit symbol substitution test. The
bipolar I patients after a single manic episode versus recurrent epi- International Journal of Neuroscience, 7(2), 59–66.
sodes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 130(1-2), 180–191. https:// Goghari, V. M., Brett, C., Tabraham, P., Johns, L., Valmaggia, L., Broome,
doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.027. M., et al. (2014). Spatial working memory ability in individuals at
Neuropsychol Rev

ultra high risk for psychosis. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 50, patients recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders,
100–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.12.010. 14(3), 227–238. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01004.x.
Gold, J. M., Goldberg, R. W., McNary, S. W., Dixon, L. B., & Lehman, Heydebrand, G., Weiser, M., Rabinowitz, J., Hoff, A. L., DeLisi, L. E., &
A. F. (2002). Cognitive correlates of job tenure among patients with Csernansky, J. G. (2004). Correlates of cognitive deficits in first
severe mental illness. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(8), episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 68(1), 1–9.
1395–1402. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1395. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00097-5.
Gold, J. M., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Dakin, S., Luck, S. J., Hill, S. K., Beers, S. R., Kmiec, J. A., Keshavan, M. S., & Sweeney, J. A.
MacDonald 3rd, A. W., et al. (2012). Clinical, functional, and (2004). Impairment of verbal memory and learning in antipsychotic-
intertask correlations of measures developed by the Cognitive naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
Neuroscience Test Reliability and Clinical Applications for Research, 68(2-3), 127–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-
Schizophrenia Consortium. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(1), 144– 9964(03)00125-7.
152. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr142. Hill, S. K., Reilly, J. L., Harris, M. S., Rosen, C., Marvin, R. W., Deleon,
Goldberg, T. E., Hyde, T. M., Kleinman, J. E., & Weinberger, D. R. O., et al. (2009). A comparison of neuropsychological dysfunction
(1993). Course of schizophrenia: neuropsychological evidence for in first-episode psychosis patients with unipolar depression, bipolar
a static encephalopathy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19(4), 797–804. disorder, and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 113(2-3),
Gomez, R. G., Fleming, S. H., Keller, J., Flores, B., Kenna, H., 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2009.04.020.
DeBattista, C., et al. (2006). The neuropsychological profile of psy- Hill, S. K., Bishop, J. R., Palumbo, D., & Sweeney, J. A. (2010). Effect of
chotic major depression and its relation to cortisol. Biological second-generation antipsychotics on cognition: current issues and
Psychiatry, 60(5), 472–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych. future challenges. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 10(1), 43–
2005.11.010. 57. https://doi.org/10.1586/ern.09.143.
Goodall, J., Fisher, C., Hetrick, S., Phillips, L., Parrish, E. M., & Allott, K. Hill, S. K., Reilly, J. L., Keefe, R. S., Gold, J. M., Bishop, J. R., Gershon,
(2018). Neurocognitive Functioning in Depressed Young People: A E. S., et al. (2013). Neuropsychological impairments in schizophre-
Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychology Review, nia and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-
28(2), 216–231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9373-9. Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP)
Goodwin, G. M., Martinez-Aran, A., Glahn, D. C., & Vieta, E. (2008). study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(11), 1275–1284.
Cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: neurodevelopment or https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12101298.
neurodegeneration? An ECNP expert meeting report. European
Hill, S. K., Reilly, J. L., Ragozzino, M. E., Rubin, L. H., Bishop, J. R.,
Neuropsychopharmacology, 18(11), 787–793. https://doi.org/10.
Gur, R. C., et al. (2015). Regressing to Prior Response Preference
1016/j.euroneuro.2008.07.005.
After Set Switching Implicates Striatal Dysfunction Across
Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., & Heaton, R. K. (2004). Longitudinal studies of
Psychotic Disorders: Findings From the B-SNIP Study.
cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(4), 940–950. https://doi.org/10.1093/
MATRICS. Schizophrenia Research, 72(1), 41–51. https://doi.org/
schbul/sbu130.
10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009.
Hochberger, W. C., Hill, S. K., Nelson, C. L., Reilly, J. L., Keefe, R. S.,
Greenland-White, S. E., Ragland, J. D., Niendam, T. A., Ferrer, E., &
Pearlson, G. D., et al. (2016). Unitary construct of generalized cog-
Carter, C. S. (2017). Episodic memory functions in first episode psy-
nitive ability underlying BACS performance across psychotic disor-
chosis and clinical high risk individuals. Schizophrenia Research,
ders and in their first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Research,
188, 151–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.035.
170(1), 156–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.022.
Guerra, A., Fearon, P., Sham, P., Jones, P., Lewis, S., Mata, I., et al.
(2002). The relationship between predisposing factors, premorbid Hou, C. L., Xiang, Y. T., Wang, Z. L., Everall, I., Tang, Y., Yang, C., et al.
function and symptom dimensions in psychosis: an integrated ap- (2016). Cognitive functioning in individuals at ultra-high risk for
psychosis, first-degree relatives of patients with psychosis and pa-
proach. European Psychiatry, 17(6), 311–320.
tients with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research,
Gur, R. C., Calkins, M. E., Satterthwaite, T. D., Ruparel, K., Bilker, W. B.,
174(1-3), 71–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.04.034.
Moore, T. M., et al. (2014). Neurocognitive growth charting in psy-
chosis spectrum youths. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(4), 366–374. https:// Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmen-
doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4190. tal receptive language disorder–a follow-up comparison in early
Harris, M. S., Reilly, J. L., Thase, M. E., Keshavan, M. S., & Sweeney, J. adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes.
A. (2009). Response suppression deficits in treatment-naive first- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 561–578.
episode patients with schizophrenia, psychotic bipolar disorder and Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K.,
psychotic major depression. Psychiatry Research, 170(2-3), 150– et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new clas-
156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.031. sification framework for research on mental disorders. The
Harvey, P. D., Koren, D., Reichenberg, A., & Bowie, C. R. (2006). American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748–751. https://doi.org/
Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of their 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.
relationship? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 250–258. https://doi. Ivleva, E. I., Morris, D. W., Osuji, J., Moates, A. F., Carmody, T. J.,
org/10.1093/schbul/sbj011. Thaker, G. K., et al. (2012). Cognitive endophenotypes of psychosis
Hawkins, K. A., Addington, J., Keefe, R. S., Christensen, B., Perkins, D. within dimension and diagnosis. Psychiatry Research, 196(1), 38–
O., Zipurksy, R., et al. (2004). Neuropsychological status of subjects 44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.08.021.
at high risk for a first episode of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, Jaeger, J., Czobor, P., & Berns, S. M. (2003). Basic neuropsychological
67(2-3), 115–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.007. dimensions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 65(2-3),
Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in 105–116.
schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Jaeger, J., Berns, S., Uzelac, S., & Davis-Conway, S. (2006).
Neuropsychology, 12(3), 426–445. Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder.
Helling, I., Ohman, A., & Hultman, C. M. (2003). School achievements Psychiatry Research, 145(1), 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
and schizophrenia: a case-control study. Acta Psychiatrica psychres.2005.11.011.
Scandinavica, 108(5), 381–386. Jenkins, L. M., Bodapati, A. S., Sharma, R. P., & Rosen, C. (2017).
Hellvin, T., Sundet, K., Simonsen, C., Aminoff, S. R., Lagerberg, T. V., Working memory predicts presence of auditory verbal hallucina-
Andreassen, O. A., et al. (2012). Neurocognitive functioning in tions in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with psychosis.
Neuropsychol Rev

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1–11. psychosis spectrum between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia:
https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2017.1321106. the Schizo-Bipolar Scale. Schizophrenia Research, 133(1-3), 250–
Jimenez-Lopez, E., Aparicio, A. I., Sanchez-Morla, E. M., Rodriguez- 254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.09.005.
Jimenez, R., Vieta, E., & Santos, J. L. (2017). Neurocognition in Khandaker, G. M., Barnett, J. H., White, I. R., & Jones, P. B. (2011). A
patients with psychotic and non-psychotic bipolar I disorder. A com- quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid
parative study with individuals with schizophrenia. Journal of intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 132(2-3),
Affective Disorders, 222, 169–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad. 220–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.017.
2017.07.014. Kim, D., Kim, J. W., Koo, T. H., Yun, H. R., & Won, S. H. (2015). Shared
Johnson, J., Horwath, E., & Weissman, M. M. (1991). The validity of and distinct neurocognitive endophenotypes of schizophrenia and
major depression with psychotic features based on a community psychotic bipolar disorder. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci, 13(1),
study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48(12), 1075–1081. 94–102. https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2015.13.1.94.
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., & Marmot, M. (1994). Child devel- Kimhy, D., Corcoran, C., Harkavy-Friedman, J. M., Ritzler, B., Javitt, D.
opment risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth C., & Malaspina, D. (2007). Visual form perception: a comparison
cohort. Lancet, 344(8934), 1398–1402. of individuals at high risk for psychosis, recent onset schizophrenia
Kahn, R. S., & Keefe, R. S. (2013). Schizophrenia is a cognitive illness: and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 97(1-3), 25–
time for a change in focus. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(10), 1107–1112. 34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.022.
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.155. Kirkpatrick, B., Messias, E., Harvey, P. D., Fernandez-Egea, E., & Bowie,
Kane, J., Honigfeld, G., Singer, J., & Meltzer, H. (1988). Clozapine for the C. R. (2008). Is schizophrenia a syndrome of accelerated aging?
treatment-resistant schizophrenic. A double-blind comparison with Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 1024–1032. https://doi.org/10.1093/
chlorpromazine. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(9), 789–796. schbul/sbm140.
Kapczinski, F., Dias, V. V., Kauer-Sant'Anna, M., Frey, B. N., Grassi- Klosterkotter, J., Hellmich, M., Steinmeyer, E. M., & Schultze-Lutter, F.
Oliveira, R., Colom, F., et al. (2009). Clinical implications of a staging (2001). Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase.
model for bipolar disorders. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(2), 158–164.
9(7), 957–966. https://doi.org/10.1586/ern.09.31.
Knowles, E. E., Weiser, M., David, A. S., Glahn, D. C., Davidson, M., &
Keefe, R. S., & Harvey, P. D. (2015). Understanding symbol coding in
Reichenberg, A. (2015). The puzzle of processing speed, memory,
schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 78(11), 744–746. https://doi.
and executive function impairments in schizophrenia: fitting the
org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.005. pieces together. Biological Psychiatry, 78(11), 786–793. https://
Keefe, R. S., Eesley, C. E., & Poe, M. P. (2005). Defining a cognitive
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.01.018.
function decrement in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 57(6),
Kraepelin, E., Barclay, R. M., & Robertson, G. M. (1919). Dementia
688–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.003.
praecox and paraphrenia. New York: Robert E Krieger Publishing
Keefe, R. S., Perkins, D. O., Gu, H., Zipursky, R. B., Christensen, B. K., &
Co Inc.
Lieberman, J. A. (2006). A longitudinal study of neurocognitive func-
tion in individuals at-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, Kraus, M., Rapisarda, A., Lam, M., Thong, J. Y. J., Lee, J., Subramaniam,
M., et al. (2016). Disrupted latent inhibition in individuals at ultra
88(1-3), 26–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.041.
Keefe, R. S., Bilder, R. M., Davis, S. M., Harvey, P. D., Palmer, B. W., high-risk for developing psychosis. Schizophr Res Cogn, 6, 1–8.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2016.07.003.
Gold, J. M., et al. (2007). Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic
medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE Kravariti, E., Reichenberg, A., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., Morgan, C.,
Trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(6), 633–647. https://doi. Zanelli, J. W., et al. (2009). Selective deficits in semantic verbal
org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.633. fluency in patients with a first affective episode with psychotic
Keefe, R. S. E., Davis, V. G., Harvey, P. D., Atkins, A. S., Haig, G. M., symptoms and a positive history of mania. Bipolar Disorders,
Hagino, O., et al. (2017). Placebo Response and Practice Effects in 11(3), 323–329. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00673.x.
Schizophrenia Cognition Trials. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(8), 807–814. Kremen, W. S., Buka, S. L., Seidman, L. J., Goldstein, J. M., Koren, D.,
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1574. & Tsuang, M. T. (1998). IQ decline during childhood and adult
Keller, J., Schatzberg, A. F., & Maj, M. (2007). Current issues in the psychotic symptoms in a community sample: a 19-year longitudinal
classification of psychotic major depression. Schizophrenia study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(5), 672–677.
Bulletin, 33(4), 877–885. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm065. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.155.5.672.
Keller, J., Gomez, R., Williams, G., Lembke, A., Lazzeroni, L., Murphy Kremen, W. S., Vinogradov, S., Poole, J. H., Schaefer, C. A., Deicken, R.
Jr, G. M., et al. (2017). HPA axis in major depression: cortisol, F., Factor-Litvak, P., et al. (2010). Cognitive decline in schizophre-
clinical symptomatology and genetic variation predict cognition. nia from childhood to midlife: a 33-year longitudinal birth cohort
Molecular Psychiatry, 22(4), 527–536. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp. study. Schizophrenia Research, 118(1-3), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.
2016.120. 1016/j.schres.2010.01.009.
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Mezuk, B., Sundquist, K., & Sundquist, J. Laes, J. R., & Sponheim, S. R. (2006). Does cognition predict community
(2016). A Swedish National Prospective and co-relative study of function only in schizophrenia?: a study of schizophrenia patients,
school achievement at age 16, and risk for schizophrenia, other bipolar affective disorder patients, and community control subjects.
nonaffective psychosis, and bipolar illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, Schizophrenia Research, 84(1), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
42(1), 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv103. schres.2005.11.023.
Keri, S., Kelemen, O., Benedek, G., & Janka, Z. (2001). Different trait Lemos-Giraldez, S., Vallina-Fernandez, O., Fernandez-Iglesias, P.,
markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a neurocognitive Vallejo-Seco, G., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Paino-Pineiro, M., et al.
approach. Psychological Medicine, 31(5), 915–922. (2009). Symptomatic and functional outcome in youth at ultra-
Kern, R. S., Gold, J. M., Dickinson, D., Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., high risk for psychosis: a longitudinal study. Schizophrenia
Baade, L. E., et al. (2011). The MCCB impairment profile for Research, 115(2-3), 121–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.
schizophrenia outpatients: results from the MATRICS psychometric 2009.09.011.
and standardization study. Schizophrenia Research, 126(1-3), 124– Lencz, T., Smith, C. W., McLaughlin, D., Auther, A., Nakayama, E.,
131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.008. Hovey, L., et al. (2006). Generalized and specific neurocognitive
Keshavan, M. S., Morris, D. W., Sweeney, J. A., Pearlson, G., Thaker, G., deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 59(9),
Seidman, L. J., et al. (2011). A dimensional approach to the 863–871. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.005.
Neuropsychol Rev

Lesh, T. A., Westphal, A. J., Niendam, T. A., Yoon, J. H., Minzenberg, M. impairment in bipolar patients with and without history of psycho-
J., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2013). Proactive and reactive cognitive sis. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(2), 233–239.
control and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in first episode Martino, D. J., Samame, C., Ibanez, A., & Strejilevich, S. A. (2015).
schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin, 2, 590–599. https://doi.org/10. Neurocognitive functioning in the premorbid stage and in the first ep-
1016/j.nicl.2013.04.010. isode of bipolar disorder: a systematic review. Psychiatry Research,
Levy, B., & Weiss, R. D. (2010). Neurocognitive impairment and psy- 226(1), 23–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.044.
chosis in bipolar I disorder during early remission from an acute Martino, D. J., Igoa, A., Marengo, E., Scapola, M., & Strejilevich, S. A.
episode of mood disturbance. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, (2018). Longitudinal relationship between clinical course and
71(2), 201–206. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.08m04663yel. neurocognitive impairments in bipolar disorder. Journal of
Lewandowski, K. E., Cohen, B. M., Keshavan, M. S., & Ongur, D. Affective Disorders, 225, 250–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.
(2011a). Relationship of neurocognitive deficits to diagnosis and 2017.08.011.
symptoms across affective and non-affective psychoses. McCleery, A., Ventura, J., Kern, R. S., Subotnik, K. L., Gretchen-Doorly,
Schizophrenia Research, 133(1-3), 212–217. https://doi.org/10. D., Green, M. F., et al. (2014). Cognitive functioning in first-episode
1016/j.schres.2011.09.004. schizophrenia: MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB)
Lewandowski, K. E., Cohen, B. M., & Ongur, D. (2011b). Evolution of Profile of Impairment. Schizophrenia Research, 157(1-3), 33–39.
neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.04.039.
and bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine, 41(2), 225–241. McClellan, J., Prezbindowski, A., Breiger, D., & McCurry, C. (2004).
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291710001042. Neuropsychological functioning in early onset psychotic disorders.
Lin, A., Wood, S. J., Nelson, B., Brewer, W. J., Spiliotacopoulos, D., Schizophrenia Research, 68(1), 21–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Bruxner, A., et al. (2011). Neurocognitive predictors of functional s0920-9964(03)00058-6.
outcome two to 13 years after identification as ultra-high risk for McIntosh, A. M., Harrison, L. K., Forrester, K., Lawrie, S. M., &
psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 132(1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10. Johnstone, E. C. (2005). Neuropsychological impairments in people
1016/j.schres.2011.06.014. with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives.
Liu, C. C., Hua, M. S., Hwang, T. J., Chiu, C. Y., Liu, C. M., Hsieh, M. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 378–385. https://doi.org/10.
H., et al. (2015). Neurocognitive functioning of subjects with puta- 1192/bjp.186.5.378.
tive pre-psychotic states and early psychosis. Schizophrenia McKay, R., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Need for closure,
Research, 164(1-3), 40–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015. jumping to conclusions, and decisiveness in delusion-prone individ-
03.006. uals. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194(6), 422–426.
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.nmd.0000221353.44132.25.
Liu, D., Ji, C., Zhuo, K., Song, Z., Wang, Y., Mei, L., et al. (2017).
Meier, M. H., Caspi, A., Reichenberg, A., Keefe, R. S., Fisher, H. L.,
Impaired cue identification and intention retrieval underlie prospec-
Harrington, H., et al. (2014). Neuropsychological decline in schizo-
tive memory deficits in patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
phrenia from the premorbid to the postonset period: evidence from a
The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51(3),
population-representative longitudinal study. The American Journal
270–277. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867416640097.
of Psychiatry, 171(1), 91–101. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.
MacCabe, J. H., Lambe, M. P., Cnattingius, S., Torrang, A., Bjork, C.,
2013.12111438.
Sham, P. C., et al. (2008). Scholastic achievement at age 16 and risk
Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., &
of schizophrenia and other psychoses: a national cohort study.
Seidman, L. J. (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophre-
Psychological Medicine, 38(8), 1133–1140. https://doi.org/10.
nia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 23(3), 315–336.
1017/s0033291707002048.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014708.
MacCabe, J. H., Lambe, M. P., Cnattingius, S., Sham, P. C., David, A. S., Miskowiak, K. W., Burdick, K. E., Martinez-Aran, A., Bonnin, C. M.,
Reichenberg, A., et al. (2010). Excellent school performance at age Bowie, C. R., Carvalho, A. F., et al. (2018). Assessing and address-
16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study. The ing cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder: the International
British Journal of Psychiatry, 196(2), 109–115. https://doi.org/10. Society for Bipolar Disorders Targeting Cognition Task Force rec-
1192/bjp.bp.108.060368. ommendations for clinicians. Bipolar Disorders. https://doi.org/10.
MacCabe, J. H., Wicks, S., Lofving, S., David, A. S., Berndtsson, A., 1111/bdi.12595s.
Gustafsson, J. E., et al. (2013). Decline in cognitive performance Mittal, V. A., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2017). Research domain criteria
between ages 13 and 18 years and the risk for psychosis in adult- (RDoC) grows up: Strengthening neurodevelopment investigation
hood: a Swedish longitudinal cohort study in males. JAMA within the RDoC framework. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216,
Psychiatry, 70(3), 261–270. https://doi.org/10.1001/2013. 30–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.011.
jamapsychiatry.43. Mojtabai, R., Bromet, E. J., Harvey, P. D., Carlson, G. A., Craig, T. J., &
MacKenzie, L. E., Patterson, V. C., Zwicker, A., Drobinin, V., Fisher, H. Fennig, S. (2000). Neuropsychological differences between first-
L., Abidi, S., et al. (2017). Hot and cold executive functions in youth admission schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. The
with psychotic symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 47(16), 2844– American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(9), 1453–1460. https://doi.
2853. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001374. org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1453.
Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., Colom, F., Torrent, C., Sanchez-Moreno, J., Mollon, J., & Reichenberg, A. (2017). Cognitive development prior to
Reinares, M., et al. (2004). Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipo- onset of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 1–12. https://doi.org/
lar patients: implications for clinical and functional outcome. 10.1017/s0033291717001970.
Bipolar Disorders, 6(3), 224–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399- Muralidharan, K., Torres, I. J., Silveira, L. E., Kozicky, J. M., Bucker, J.,
5618.2004.00111.x. Fernando, N., et al. (2014). Impact of depressive episodes on cog-
Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., Torrent, C., Sanchez-Moreno, J., Goikolea, nitive deficits in early bipolar disorder: data from the Systematic
J. M., Salamero, M., et al. (2007). Functional outcome in bipolar Treatment Optimization Programme for Early Mania (STOP-EM).
disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive factors. Bipolar The British Journal of Psychiatry, 205(1), 36–43. https://doi.org/10.
Disorders, 9(1-2), 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618. 1192/bjp.bp.113.135525.
2007.00327.x. Murray, R. M., & Lewis, S. W. (1987). Is schizophrenia a
Martinez-Aran, A., Torrent, C., Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Salamero, M., neurodevelopmental disorder? British Medical Journal (Clinical
Daban, C., Balanza-Martinez, V., et al. (2008). Neurocognitive Research Ed.), 295(6600), 681–682.
Neuropsychol Rev

Nielsen, R. E., Levander, S., Kjaersdam Telleus, G., Jensen, S. O., Reichenberg, A., & Harvey, P. D. (2007). Neuropsychological impair-
Ostergaard Christensen, T., & Leucht, S. (2015). Second- ments in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-based and brain
generation antipsychotic effect on cognition in patients with imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 833–858. https://
schizophrenia–a meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. Acta doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833.
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 131(3), 185–196. https://doi.org/10. Reichenberg, A., Weiser, M., Rabinowitz, J., Caspi, A., Schmeidler, J.,
1111/acps.12374. Mark, M., et al. (2002). A population-based cohort study of
Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Rosso, I. M., Sanchez, L. E., Hadley, T., premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in pa-
Nuechterlein, K. H., et al. (2003). A prospective study of childhood tients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychot-
neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients and their sib- ic bipolar disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(12),
lings. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(11), 2060–2062. 2027–2035. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.2027.
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2060. Reichenberg, A., Harvey, P. D., Bowie, C. R., Mojtabai, R., Rabinowitz,
Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Johnson, J. K., McKinley, M., Loewy, J., Heaton, R. K., et al. (2009). Neuropsychological function and
R., O'Brien, M., et al. (2006). Neurocognitive performance and dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders.
functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophrenia Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(5), 1022–1029. https://doi.org/10.1093/
Research, 84(1), 100–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006. schbul/sbn044.
02.005. Reichenberg, A., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Houts, R., Keefe, R. S.,
O'Brien, J. T., Ames, D., Schweitzer, I., Colman, P., Desmond, P., & Murray, R. M., et al. (2010). Static and dynamic cognitive deficits
Tress, B. (1996). Clinical and magnetic resonance imaging corre- in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. The
lates of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function in depression American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(2), 160–169. https://doi.org/
and Alzheimer's disease. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 168(6), 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040574.
679–687. Reilly, J. L., Hill, S. K., Gold, J. M., Keefe, R. S., Clementz, B. A.,
Ohaeri, J. U. (2003). The burden of caregiving in families with a mental Gershon, E., et al. (2017). Impaired Context Processing is
illness: a review of 2002. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 16(4), Attributable to Global Neuropsychological Impairment in
457–465. Schizophrenia and Psychotic Bipolar Disorder. Schizophrenia
Olvet, D. M., Stearns, W. H., McLaughlin, D., Auther, A. M., Correll, C. Bulletin, 43(2), 397–406. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw081.
U., & Cornblatt, B. A. (2010). Comparing clinical and Revell, E. R., Neill, J. C., Harte, M., Khan, Z., & Drake, R. J. (2015). A
neurocognitive features of the schizophrenia prodrome to the bipolar systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in
prodrome. Schizophrenia Research, 123(1), 59–63. https://doi.org/ early schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 168(1-2), 213–222.
10.1016/j.schres.2010.07.005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.08.017.
Ozdel, O., Karadag, F., Atesci, F. C., Oguzhanoglu, N. K., & Cabuk, T. Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. D. (2014).
(2007). Cognitive functions in euthymic patients with bipolar disor- Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and me-
ta-analysis. Psychological medicine, 44(10), 2029–2040.
der. Annals of Saudi Medicine, 27(4), 273–278.
Rossler, W., Salize, H. J., van Os, J., & Riecher-Rossler, A. (2005). Size
Parellada, M., Gomez-Vallejo, S., Burdeus, M., & Arango, C. (2017).
of burden of schizophrenia and psychotic disorders. European
Developmental Differences Between Schizophrenia and Bipolar
Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(4), 399–409. https://doi.org/10.
Disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(6), 1176–1189. https://doi.
1016/j.euroneuro.2005.04.009.
org/10.1093/schbul/sbx126.
Rossler, W., Ajdacic-Gross, V., Muller, M., Rodgers, S., Kawohl, W.,
Park, S., Holzman, P. S., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (1995). Individual dif-
Haker, H., et al. (2015). Association between processing speed and
ferences in spatial working memory in relation to schizotypy.
subclinical psychotic symptoms in the general population: focusing
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(2), 355–363.
on sex differences. Schizophrenia Research, 166(1-3), 316–321.
Paya, B., Rodriguez-Sanchez, J. M., Otero, S., Munoz, P., Castro- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.026.
Fornieles, J., Parellada, M., et al. (2013). Premorbid impairments Rund, B. R., Barder, H. E., Evensen, J., Haahr, U., ten Velden Hegelstad,
in early-onset psychosis: differences between patients with schizo- W., Joa, I., et al. (2016). Neurocognition and Duration of Psychosis:
phrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 146(1-3), A 10-year Follow-up of First-Episode Patients. Schizophrenia
103–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.029. Bulletin, 42(1), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv083.
Pena, J., Ojeda, N., Segarra, R., Eguiluz, J. I., Garcia, J., & Gutierrez, M. Samame, C., Martino, D. J., & Strejilevich, S. A. (2014). Longitudinal
(2011). Executive functioning correctly classified diagnoses in pa- course of cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder: a meta-analytic
tients with first-episode psychosis: evidence from a 2-year longitu- study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 164, 130–138. https://doi.
dinal study. Schizophrenia Research, 126(1-3), 77–80. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.028.
org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.019. Sanchez-Moreno, J., Martinez-Aran, A., Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Torrent,
Prado, C. E., Watt, S., & Crowe, S. F. (2018). A meta-analysis of the C., Vieta, E., & Ayuso-Mateos, J. L. (2009). Functioning and dis-
effects of antidepressants on cognitive functioning in depressed and ability in bipolar disorder: an extensive review. Psychotherapy and
non-depressed samples. Neuropsychology Review, 28(1), 32–72. Psychosomatics, 78(5), 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1159/
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9369-5. 000228249.
Rabinowitz, J., De Smedt, G., Harvey, P. D., & Davidson, M. (2002). Sanchez-Torres, A. M., Moreno-Izco, L., Lorente-Omenaca, R., Cabrera,
Relationship between premorbid functioning and symptom severity B., Lobo, A., Gonzalez-Pinto, A. M., et al. (2017). Individual tra-
as assessed at first episode of psychosis. The American Journal of jectories of cognitive performance in first episode psychosis: a 2-
Psychiatry, 159(12), 2021–2026. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp. year follow-up study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical
159.12.2021. Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-017-0857-z.
Rapoport, J. L., Giedd, J. N., & Gogtay, N. (2012). Neurodevelopmental Savitz, J., van der Merwe, L., Stein, D. J., Solms, M., & Ramesar, R.
model of schizophrenia: update 2012. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(12), (2009). Neuropsychological status of bipolar I disorder: impact of
1228–1238. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.23. psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(3), 243–251.
Ratheesh, A., Lin, A., Nelson, B., Wood, S. J., Brewer, W., Betts, J., et al. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.052001.
(2013). Neurocognitive functioning in the prodrome of mania–an Schatzberg, A. F., Posener, J. A., DeBattista, C., Kalehzan, B. M.,
exploratory study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 147(1-3), 441– Rothschild, A. J., & Shear, P. K. (2000). Neuropsychological deficits
445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.09.017. in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental
Neuropsychol Rev

illness. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(7), 1095–1100. schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 761–771.
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.7.1095. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm018.
Schouws, S. N., Stek, M. L., Comijs, H. C., Dols, A., & Beekman, A. T. Simon, A. E., Grädel, M., Cattapan-Ludewig, K., Gruber, K., Ballinari,
(2012). Cognitive decline in elderly bipolar disorder patients: a P., Roth, B., et al. (2012). Cognitive functioning in at-risk mental
follow-up study. Bipolar Disorders, 14(7), 749–755. https://doi. states for psychosis and 2-year clinical outcome. Schizophrenia re-
org/10.1111/bdi.12000. search, 142(1–3), 108–115.
Seidman, L. J., & Mirsky, A. F. (2017). Evolving Notions of Simonsen, C., Sundet, K., Vaskinn, A., Birkenaes, A. B., Engh, J. A.,
Schizophrenia as a Developmental Neurocognitive Disorder. Faerden, A., et al. (2011). Neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar and
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(9-10), schizophrenia spectrum disorders depends on history of psychosis
881–892. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717001114. rather than diagnostic group. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(1), 73–83.
Seidman, L. J., Kremen, W. S., Koren, D., Faraone, S. V., Goldstein, J. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp034.
M., & Tsuang, M. T. (2002). A comparative profile analysis of Sorensen, H. J., Saebye, D., Urfer-Parnas, A., Mortensen, E. L., & Parnas,
neuropsychological functioning in patients with schizophrenia and J. (2012). Premorbid intelligence and educational level in bipolar
bipolar psychoses. Schizophrenia Research, 53(1-2), 31–44. and unipolar disorders: a Danish draft board study. Journal of
Seidman, L. J., Giuliano, A. J., Meyer, E. C., Addington, J., Cadenhead, K. Affective Disorders, 136(3), 1188–1191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
S., Cannon, T. D., et al. (2010). Neuropsychology of the prodrome to jad.2011.12.007.
psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history Sperry, S. H., O'Connor, L. K., Ongur, D., Cohen, B. M., Keshavan, M.
and conversion to psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(6), S., & Lewandowski, K. E. (2015). Measuring Cognition in Bipolar
578–588. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.66. Disorder with Psychosis Using the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive
Seidman, L. J., Cherkerzian, S., Goldstein, J. M., Agnew-Blais, J., Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
Tsuang, M. T., & Buka, S. L. (2013). Neuropsychological perfor- 21(6), 468–472. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617715000442.
mance and family history in children at age 7 who develop adult Strauss, G. P., Allen, D. N., Miski, P., Buchanan, R. W., Kirkpatrick, B.,
schizophrenia or bipolar psychosis in the New England Family & Carpenter Jr, W. T. (2012). Differential patterns of premorbid
Studies. Psychological Medicine, 43(1), 119–131. https://doi.org/ social and academic deterioration in deficit and nondeficit schizo-
10.1017/s0033291712000773. phrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 135(1-3), 134–138. https://doi.
Seidman, L. J., Shapiro, D. I., Stone, W. S., Woodberry, K. A., Ronzio, org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.007.
A., Cornblatt, B. A., et al. (2016). Association of Neurocognition Su, C. Y., Wang, P. W., Lin, Y. J., Tang, T. C., Liu, M. F., & Chen, M. D.
With Transition to Psychosis: Baseline Functioning in the Second (2016). The effects of aerobic exercise on cognition in schizophre-
Phase of the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study. JAMA nia: A 3-month follow-up study. Psychiatry Research, 244, 394–
Psychiatry, 73(12), 1239–1248. https://doi.org/10.1001/ 402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.011.
jamapsychiatry.2016.2479. Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Balanza-Martinez, V., Sanchez-Moreno, J.,
Selva, G., Salazar, J., Balanza-Martinez, V., Martinez-Aran, A., Rubio, Martinez-Aran, A., Salazar-Fraile, J., Selva-Vera, G., et al. (2008).
C., Daban, C., et al. (2007). Bipolar I patients with and without a Neurocognitive and clinical predictors of functional outcome in pa-
history of psychotic symptoms: do they differ in their cognitive tients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder at one-year follow-
functioning? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41(3-4), 265–272. up. Journal of Affective Disorders, 109(3), 286–299. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.03.007. 10.1016/j.jad.2007.12.234.
Sheffield, J. M., Williams, L. E., Cohen, N., & Heckers, S. (2012). Tiihonen, J., Haukka, J., Henriksson, M., Cannon, M., Kieseppa, T.,
Relational memory in psychotic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Laaksonen, I., et al. (2005). Premorbid intellectual functioning in
Disorders, 14(5), 537–546. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618. bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: results from a cohort study of
2012.01036.x. male conscripts. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(10),
Sheffield, J. M., Gold, J. M., Strauss, M. E., Carter, C. S., MacDonald 1904–1910. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1904.
3rd, A. W., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2014). Common and specific Tohen, M., Strakowski, S. M., Zarate Jr, C., Hennen, J., Stoll, A. L.,
cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: relationships to function. Suppes, T., et al. (2000). The McLean-Harvard first-episode project:
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 161–174. 6-month symptomatic and functional outcome in affective and
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0211-5. nonaffective psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 48(6), 467–476.
Sheffield, J. M., Kandala, S., Burgess, G. C., Harms, M. P., & Barch, D. Torrent, C., Bonnin Cdel, M., Martinez-Aran, A., Valle, J., Amann, B. L.,
M. (2016). Cingulo-opercular network efficiency mediates the asso- Gonzalez-Pinto, A., et al. (2013). Efficacy of functional remediation
ciation between psychotic-like experiences and cognitive ability in in bipolar disorder: a multicenter randomized controlled study. The
the general population. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(8), 852–859. https://doi.org/
Neuroimaging, 1(6), 498–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070971.
03.009. Torrent, C., Reinares, M., Martinez-Aran, A., Cabrera, B., Amoretti, S.,
Sheffield, J. M., Ruge, H., Kandala, S., & Barch, D. M. (2018). Rapid Corripio, I., et al. (2018). Affective versus non-affective first episode
instruction-based task learning (RITL) in schizophrenia. Journal of psychoses: A longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders,
Abnormal Psychology, 127(5), 513–528. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 238, 297–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.06.005.
abn0000354. Torres, I. J., Boudreau, V. G., & Yatham, L. N. (2007).
Silverstein, S., Uhlhaas, P. J., Essex, B., Halpin, S., Schall, U., & Carr, V. Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a me-
(2006). Perceptual organization in first episode schizophrenia and ta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116, 17–26.
ultra-high-risk states. Schizophrenia Research, 83(1), 41–52. https:// Torres, I. J., Kozicky, J., Popuri, S., Bond, D. J., Honer, W. G., Lam, R.
doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.003. W., et al. (2014). 12-month longitudinal cognitive functioning in
Silverstein, S. M., Keane, B. P., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., Gold, J. M., patients recently diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Bipolar
Kovacs, I., et al. (2012). Optimization and validation of a visual Disorders, 16(2), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12154.
integration test for schizophrenia research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, Trisha, C., Golnoush, A., Jan-Marie, K., Torres, I. J., & Yatham, L. N.
38(1), 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbr141. (2017). Cognitive functioning in first episode bipolar I disorder pa-
Simon, A. E., Cattapan-Ludewig, K., Zmilacher, S., Arbach, D., Gruber, tients with and without history of psychosis. Journal of Affective
K., Dvorsky, D. N., et al. (2007). Cognitive functioning in the Disorders, 227, 109–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.003.
Neuropsychol Rev

Trisha, C., Golnoush, A., Jan-Marie, K., Torres, I. J., & Yatham, L. N. Wingo, A. P., Harvey, P. D., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2009). Neurocognitive
(2018). Cognitive functioning in first episode bipolar I disorder pa- impairment in bipolar disorder patients: functional implications.
tients with and without history of psychosis. Journal of Affective Bipolar Disorders, 11(2), 113–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-
Disorders, 227, 109–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.003. 5618.2009.00665.x.
Trotta, A., Murray, R. M., & MacCabe, J. H. (2015). Do premorbid and Woodberry, K. A., Giuliano, A. J., & Seidman, L. J. (2008). Premorbid IQ
post-onset cognitive functioning differ between schizophrenia and in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. The American Journal of
bipolar disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry, 165(5), 579–587. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.
Psychological Medicine, 45(2), 381–394. https://doi.org/10.1017/ 07081242.
s0033291714001512. Woodward, N. D. (2016). The course of neuropsychological impairment
van Os, J., Linscott, R. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., & Krabbendam, and brain structure abnormalities in psychotic disorders.
L. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis Neuroscience Research, 102, 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence- neures.2014.08.006.
impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine, Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., & Czobor, P. (2011). A
39(2), 179–195. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708003814. meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: method-
Van Rheenen, T. E., Bryce, S., Tan, E. J., Neill, E., Gurvich, C., Louise, ology and effect sizes. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5),
S., et al. (2016). Does cognitive performance map to categorical 472–485. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855.
diagnoses of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and bipolar dis- Zabala, A., Rapado, M., Arango, C., Robles, O., de la Serna, E.,
order? A discriminant functions analysis. Journal of Affective Gonzalez, C., et al. (2010). Neuropsychological functioning in
Disorders, 192, 109–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.022. early-onset first-episode psychosis: comparison of diagnostic sub-
Velligan, D. I., Mahurin, R. K., Diamond, P. L., Hazleton, B. C., Eckert, groups. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical
S. L., & Miller, A. L. (1997). The functional significance of symp- Neuroscience, 260(3), 225–233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-
tomatology and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia 009-0046-9.
Research, 25(1), 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(97)
Zammit, S., Allebeck, P., David, A. S., Dalman, C., Hemmingsson,
00010-8.
T., Lundberg, I., et al. (2004). A longitudinal study of premorbid
Vohringer, P. A., Barroilhet, S. A., Amerio, A., Reale, M. L., Alvear, K.,
IQ Score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder,
Vergne, D., et al. (2013). Cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder
severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of
and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4,
General Psychiatry, 61(4), 354–360. https://doi.org/10.1001/
87. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00087.
archpsyc.61.4.354.
Watt, N. F., & Lubensky, A. W. (1976). Childhood roots of schizophrenia.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44(3), 363–375. Zanelli, J., Reichenberg, A., Morgan, K., Fearon, P., Kravariti, E.,
Weinberger, D. R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for Dazzan, P., et al. (2010). Specific and generalized neuropsycholog-
the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, ical deficits: a comparison of patients with various first-episode psy-
44(7), 660–669. chosis presentations. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1),
Welham, J., Scott, J., Williams, G. M., Najman, J. M., Bor, W., 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010118.
O'Callaghan, M., et al. (2010). The antecedents of non-affective Zaninotto, L., Guglielmo, R., Calati, R., Ioime, L., Camardese, G., Janiri,
psychosis in a birth-cohort, with a focus on measures related to L., et al. (2015). Cognitive markers of psychotic unipolar depres-
cognitive ability, attentional dysfunction and speech problems. sion: a meta-analytic study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 174,
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 121(4), 273–279. https://doi.org/ 580–588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.027.
10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01470.x.

You might also like