You are on page 1of 7

Brain Research Reviews 31 Ž2000.

106–112
www.elsevier.comrlocaterbres

Interactive report

Schizophrenia: the fundamental questions 1


)
Nancy C. Andreasen
UniÕersity of Iowa Hospitals and Clinics, Mental Health Clinical Research Center, 2911 JPP, 200 Hawkins DriÕe, Iowa City, IA 52242-1057, USA
Accepted 29 September 1999

Abstract

Identifying the correct phentotype of schizophrenia is perhaps the most important goal of modern research in schizophrenia. This
identification is the necessary antecedent of indentifying the pathophysiology and etiology. A working model is proposed, which suggests
that the phenotype should be defined on the basis of abnormalities in neural circuits and a fundamental cognitive process. This type of
unitary model may be more heuristic than early ones that were based on heterogeneous signs and symptoms. q 2000 Elsevier Science
B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Schizophrenia; Neural circuits; Creativity; Cognition; Disease phenotypes

Contents

1. Introduction . ....................................................................... 106

2. The definition of the phenotype ............................................................. 107

3. Clues from epidemiology . ................................................................ 108

4. The etiology and pathophysiology of schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110


4.1. Hypothesis one: The etiologies are multiple . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
4.2 Hypothesis two: The pathophysiology is an abnormality in the regulation and expression of neurodevelopment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
4.3. Hypothesis three: The pathology is a ‘disease of neuronal connectivity’ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
4.4. Hypothesis four: The phenotype is defined by a mental metaprocess rather than by clinical symptoms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110

References .......................................................................... 111

1. Introduction ably, but approximately 2r3 of those who develop


schizophrenia require public assistance from governmental
Schizophrenia is one of the most important public health social security systems within a few years after onset w45x.
problems that human society confronts. It is a very com- The majority of people who develop schizophrenia are
mon disorder, affecting 1% of the world population. Peo- unable to return to work or to school and have relatively
ple who develop schizophrenia experience severe suffer- minimal social interactions. The costs to society are counted
ing: approximately 10% commit suicide. The treatments in billions of dollars w21x.
available in the late 1990s reduce this suffering consider- This serious mental illness manifests itself in signs and
symptoms that encompass the entire range of human men-
)
Tel.: q1-319-356-1553; Fax: q1-319-353-8300; E-mail:
tal activity. Because schizophrenia is a brain disease that
nancy-andreasen@uiowa.edu manifests itself in the activities of the mind, it damages a
1
Published on the World Wide Web on 28 October 1999. variety of functions that we regard as specifically human.

0165-0173r00r$ - see front matter q 2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 1 6 5 - 0 1 7 3 Ž 9 9 . 0 0 0 2 7 - 2
N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112 107

Schizophrenia impairs the ability to think creatively and tions and affective blunting, while another may have delu-
imaginatively, to have close social relationships with other sions and attentional impairment. This non-overlapping
human beings, to use language to express ideas with pattern of symptoms raises an obvious question: is there a
clarity, or to experience and express a variety of emotions core or fundamental theme that unites this diversity of
such as love and fear. People with schizophrenia are symptoms and that can provide a more general conceptual-
tormented by intrusive experiences such as hearing voices ization of the phenotype?
or by beliefs that they are being persecuted or injured by This fundamental question raises an even more funda-
those around them or by alien external forces. No single mental question: What is the correct basis for defining the
sign or symptom defines schizophrenia. It is defined by the phenotype of schizophrenia? While the answer to this
fact that people who suffer from it experience abnormali- question seems simple and obvious at first glance, closer
ties in many different kinds of mental activities. inspection suggests that probing deeply below the surface
Attempts to understand schizophrenia are shaped by may be necessary.
several fundamental questions, which provide themes for The obvious answer is that the phenotype of
this Nobel Symposium. Some questions addressed by this schizophrenia is defined by the criterion-based diagnostic
symposium are: symptoms that have been developed in the past several
What is the correct basis for identifying the phenotype? decades. The fourth Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
What does epidemiology tell us about its etiologyrpa- the American Psychiatric Association Ž1994. ŽDSM IV. w2x
thophysiology? is perhaps the most widely used, although the definition in
How many etiologies are there? the 10th edition of the International Classification of Dis-
This chapter provides a brief introduction to these ques- ease Ž1990. ŽICD 10. w68x provides a similar alternative.
tions, which will be amplified in the presentations of the Both these criterion-based systems take a purely descrip-
other participants. tive approach and define schizophrenia on the basis of
presenting clinical signs and symptoms. Instead of trying
to account for the diversity of symptoms, they emphasize
2. The definition of the phenotype psychotic symptoms as the defining feature Ži.e., delusions
and hallucinations.. While the criterion-based systems are
The first of these questions, and perhaps the most clinically useful, in that they have provided a reliable
fundamental of all of them, is the definition of the pheno- means for physicians around the world to communicate
type Ži.e., the core or true manifestation of the disease.. with one another, their validity is less well established. By
The definition of the phenotype in schizophrenia is particu- focusing on only a few of the symptoms with which
larly difficult because patients with this illness suffer from patients present, they have identified those that are easy to
such a diversity of symptoms. Some mental illnesses, such operationalize, but this narrow approach has probably sac-
as depression, are defined by an abnormality in a single rificed validity for the sake of reliability. If the long-term
mental system — i.e. depressed or dysphoric mood in the goal is to identify the pathophysiology and etiology of
case of depression. Schizophrenia, on the other hand, is schizophrenia, then the definition of the phenotype must
characterized by symptoms that reflect multiple mental emphasize a definition that is heuristic for studying mecha-
processes: hallucinations, or abnormalities in perception; nisms of illness. With their excessive emphasis on positive
delusions, or abnormalities in inferential thinking; disorga- or psychotic symptoms, criterion-based approaches such as
nized speech, or abnormalities in language; disorganized DSM may have distracted investigators away from the
behavior, or abnormalities in behavioral monitoring and ‘real illness’ by over-emphasizing clinical signs and symp-
control. Those four symptoms are the classic ‘positive toms, and perhaps even by over-emphasizing the wrong
symptoms’ of schizophrenia in which normal functions are ones.
distorted or exaggerated. The various ‘negative symptoms’ When schizophrenia was delineated at the beginning of
of schizophrenia are another important aspect of the illness the 20th century by Emil Kraepelin w49x and Eugen Bleuler
w6,7,33x. They represent a diminution or absence of mental w22x, both of these clinician scientists stressed the impor-
functions that are normally present. Alogia Žliterally an tance of defining the illness by attempting to identify a
absence of words. is a decrease in the fluency of ideas and fundamental ‘morbid process’. Both emphasized that psy-
language; affective blunting is a diminution in the ability chotic symptoms, such as delusions and hallucinations, are
to express emotions; avolition is a decrease in the ability to not specific to schizophrenia, but occur in many other
initiate and pursue goal-directed activity; anhedonia is a illnesses such as mood disorders or dementias. For both
decrease in the ability to seek out and experience pleasur- Kraepelin and Bleuler, the most important defining feature
able activities, and attentional impairment is a decrease in was an impairment in the ability to think in a clear, fluent,
the ability to focus attention. and logical way. The person with schizophrenia suffers
These various symptoms are present in patients suffer- primarily from a fragmenting in cognitive processes. Krae-
ing from schizophrenia in patterns that may not overlap at pelin highlighted this aspect when he named the illness
all. One person with schizophrenia may have hallucina- ‘‘dementia praecox’’, or a serious cognitive illness with an
108 N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112

onset at a relatively young age. He saw the diversity of Although the clinicians who originally delineated and
symptoms as unified by the underlying destruction of defined schizophrenia emphasized that the disorder was
cognitive processes: not defined by clinical symptoms, this perspective has
been lost in DSM IV and ICD 10. Both define schizophre-
nia using a polythetic clustering of symptoms. While use-
‘‘Dementia praecox consists of a series of states, the
ful clinically, the improved reliability of these recent stan-
common characteristic of which is a peculiar destruc-
dardized diagnostic manuals may exact a large price in
tion of internal connections of the psychic
terms of validity. Defining schizophrenia as a polythetic
personality . . . . The majority of the clinical pictures are
cluster makes the search for mechanisms more difficult,
the expression of a single morbid process, though out-
since it leads to a fragmented model: i.e., hallucinations
wardly they often diverge very far from one another.’’
are usually explained by one mechanism and negative
symptoms by another. The alternative proposed by Krae-
Bleuler highlighted the same concept when he renamed pelin and Bleuler — that schizophrenia is defined by an
the illness ‘‘schizophrenia’’. Bleuler’s new name referred abnormality in a fundamental mental process — makes the
to the ‘fragmenting of the mind’ that he believed to be the search simpler and more tractable. Consequently, several
fundamental abnormality. Bleuler emphasized that the per- recent investigators have proposed ‘cognitive models’ of
son with schizophrenia had lost the capacity to have his schizophrenia w16x.
thought processes guided by concepts that were correctly
linked together:

3. Clues from epidemiology


‘‘Of the thousands of associative threads that guide our
thinking, this disease seems to interrupt, quite haphaz-
As we search for ‘the holy grail’ — the etiology and
ardly, sometimes single threads, sometimes a whole
pathophysiology of schizophrenia — we can be guided by
group, and sometimes whole segments of them.’’
some simple and very consistent observations concerning
the epidemiology of the disorder w21x. These include the
Thus the ‘founding fathers’ of schizophrenia believed following:
that they had identified a disease that had many different 1. Schizophrenia has a similar clinical presentation and a
kinds of symptomatic manifestations at the clinical level, similar prevalence throughout the world — a lifetime
but that was in fact defined by an abnormality in a more prevalence of approximately 1%. Because schizophre-
basic mental process. Their thinking, reshaped in 21st nia is at present a lifetime disorder, the lifetime preva-
century terms, would be expressed by a ‘working model’ lence and the point prevalence are essentially the same.
similar to that shown below. Schizophrenia is a disease 2. Schizophrenia has been present for centuries, as in-
that is characterized by multiple different symptoms when ferred from literary descriptions.
assessed at the clinical level Že.g., hallucinations, avolition. 3. ‘Classic’ cases begin in young adult life.
and multiple disturbances in cognitive processes when 4. Males are more frequently and severely affected.
assessed at the systems level Že.g., memory, language.. 5. The illness tends to run in families.
However, schizophrenia is ultimately a disease of the mind 6. Despite the fact that the majority of people with
that is characterized by a ‘disturbance in thinking’ — an schizophrenia do not marry or have children, the dis-
abnormality in a fundamental cognitive process. ease persists in the human population.

Working Model
Etiology: Multiple convergent factors such as
DNA Gene Expression Viruses Toxins Nutrition Birth Injury Psychological Experiences

Pathophysiology: Brain development from conception to early adulthood


Že.g., neuron formation, migration, synaptogenesis, pruning, apoptosis, activity dependent changes.

Anatomic and functional disruption in neuronal connectivity and communication

Impairment in a fundamental cognitive process

Impairment in one or more second-order cognitive processes Že.g., attention, memory, language, emotion.

Symptoms of schizophrenia Že.g., hallucinations, delusions, negative symptoms, disorganized speech.


N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112 109

The existence of schizophrenia is not defined by geog- genetic disease, as Huntington’s disease, Tay Sachs dis-
raphy. Unlike some illnesses Že.g., multiple sclerosis., ease, or cystic fibrosis are. Solid and well-designed stud-
prevalence rates are the same throughout the world. The ies, conducted since early in this century, have shown that
similar clinical presentation and lifetime prevalence has the disease tends to run in families w52x. As compared with
been documented in several international studies. The the population prevalence of approximately 1%, the preva-
largest and most influential of these has been the WHO lence among siblings and parents is increased approxi-
International Collaborative Study w60x. Conducted in multi- mately tenfold to around 10%. The prevalence in the
ple countries with different cultures and different levels of adopted offspring of schizophrenic mothers, reared by
industrialization, it documented that the rate of schizophre- ‘normal mothers’, is also around 10%, indicating that the
nia varied slightly but that most countries had similar rates familiality of the disorder cannot be explained by the
in the expected range. family environment or the stress of living with a
Although it has sometimes been suggested that schizophrenic family member. Twin studies have shown a
schizophrenia could be ‘a disease of civilization’, this concordance rate of approximately 40% in monozygotic
seems unlikely. Although schizophrenia was only demar- twins, as compared to a 10% concordance in dizygotic
cated as a specific diagnostic entity at the end of the 19th twins. Thus schizophrenia cannot be due only to genetic
century, earlier accounts appear in literature. Many charac- factors. Further, it does not follow classical Mendelian
ters ‘become mad’ in classical tragedy, although the pat- patterns of inheritance. In fact, schizophrenia often co-ex-
terns of illness do not map precisely on the modern ists in familial pedigrees with other psychotic illnesses,
conceptions of schizophrenia — a fact probably due to such as severe mood disorders. Nor do symptoms breed
both literary license and to the fact that the nosology of true within families. Thus, as a genetic disorder,
mental illness was not highly refined at that time. By the schizophrenia is ‘complex’, much as hypertension or can-
era of Elizabethan drama, however, we have portrayals of cer are. It probably cannot be explained by a single gene,
schizophrenia that closely resemble the modern concept or by genetic factors alone.
w5x. The madness of Ophelia in Hamlet is quite similar to The fact that schizophrenia persists in the human popu-
modern schizophrenia, and the portrayal of ‘Poor Tom’, lation despite the fact that the majority of people who
son of Gloucester pretending to be a ‘Bedlam beggar’ develop it do not marry or procreate is fascinating. This
escaped from the large Bethlehem Hospital where mental observation should perhaps be coupled with another that
patients were housed, is a near-perfect portrayal of both has been less frequently noted and has not yet been
hallucinations and disorganized speech: systematically investigated: a substantial number of highly
creative individuals have family members who are in the
Who gives anything to Poor Tom? Whom the foul fiend schizophrenia spectrum. The association between manic-
hath led through fire and through flame . . . ? Tom’s depressive illness and creativity is well-established, but
a-cold,,— O, do de, do de, do de . . . Do poor Tom some most of this work has focused on literary or artistic
charity, whom the foul fiend vexes. There I could have creativity w4,9,47x. In the case of schizophrenia, there may
him now, — and there, and there again, and there.King be a link with scientific creativity instead, although the
Lear III.iv.51 evidence at present is only anecdotal. However, an impres-
sive number of Nobel Laureates have an association with
‘Classic’ schizophrenia usually has its onset in the late schizophrenia. Bertrand Russell had a son and granddaugh-
teens or early twenties, and it occurs more often in males ter who suffered from schizophrenia, as well as an aunt
and affects them more severely than females. These obser- and uncle who probably also suffered from this illness
vations may contain important clues about pathophysiol- when it was simply called ‘insanity’ w53x. Albert Einstein
ogy and etiology. The age of onset is too late for the had a son by his first wife who developed schizophrenia
mechanism to be simply one of fetal or early childhood w27x. John Nash, a recent Nobel laureate in economics,
brain development. Whatever the factors are that lead to developed schizophrenia himself in his early 30s and also
this illness, their effects must be cumulative, or must has a son who suffers from schizophrenia w55x. James
require a ‘releasing phenomenon’, or both. Because human Joyce was emotionally aloof and cold and became increas-
brain development is an ongoing process that continues ingly disorganized artistically; his daughter Lucia suffered
into early adulthood, the age of onset suggests that the from hebephrenic schizophrenia w3x. Isaac Newton was a
development of schizophrenia must be influenced by solitary, chronically suspicious, and socially aloof man
molecular mechanisms involved in the regulation of brain who had a variety of unusual interests and beliefs; he
maturation and that adolescence is a critical period during would probably be called schizotypal using modern
which these mechanisms exert their influence. The male nomenclature; however, he also had a psychotic break at
predominance — a clue that is rarely pursued — suggests age 40 w66x.
that at least one molecular influence may be x-linked. Coupled with the persistence of schizophrenia through-
Schizophrenia is also familial. It clearly has a genetic out history despite decreased fertility and procreation, this
component, although it does not appear to be a purely modest association between schizophrenia and ‘genius’
110 N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112

suggests that, perhaps like sickle cell anemia, a predisposi- about pathophysiology and etiology by suggesting the
tion to schizophrenia may convey a biological advantage. importance of examining the molecular processes that
regulate and shape brain development and the external
factors that may influence those processes w1x. The most
4. The etiology and pathophysiology of schizophrenia typical age of onset is during the late teens and early
twenties, a time when brain maturation is reaching comple-
We can formulate several hypotheses about the illness, tion w19x, suggesting that one component of the etiology
based on its epidemiology, although these are necessarily must involve a neurodevelopmental process related to these
speculative. These hypotheses provide a structure on which final stages of ‘brain sculpturing’, such as pruning or
‘the search for the holy grail’ may be based. activity-dependent changes Žpsychological experiences that
affect plasticity..
4.1. Hypothesis one: The etiologies are multiple
4.3. Hypothesis three: The pathology is a ‘disease of
Because the prevalence appears to be distributed fairly neuronal connectiÕity’
evenly throughout space and time, the etiology of
schizophrenia is not linked to a particular place or to a The study of the neuropathology of schizophrenia is
particular time in history. This suggests, but does not sometimes called a ‘graveyard’. The search for a single
require, that the etiologies are probably multiple. This neuropathology, which began with Kraepelin’s team Žand
concept is portrayed in the Working Model. Most cases included Alzheimer, Nissl, and Brodmann., has not been
probably occur as a consequence of multiple hits, much successful in finding a ‘marker’. Perhaps this is because
like cancer — an accumulation of genetic and non-genetic we have been looking in the wrong place or thinking in the
influences ranging from genes coded within DNA to expo- wrong way. The brain abnormalities that have been identi-
sure to toxins or drugs Že.g., radiation, amphetamines., fied in schizophrenia, using techniques that range from
viruses and other pathogens, injuries to the brain occurring neuropathology to neuroimaging, have implicated multiple
at the time of birth or later, nutrition, or psychological regions throughout the brain: frontal cortex, temporal cor-
experiences that produce somatic effects such as exposure tex, thalamus, hippocampal complex, basal ganglia, and
to stress. Conceivably, some forms in some individuals even the cerebellum w10–15,20,24,28–31,36,40,44,50,
could be due to a single ‘necessary and sufficient’ factor. 57,58,61–63x. This suggests that the pathology of the
More likely Žand more daunting in terms of identifying illness is probably not focal or ‘localizable’ to a single
etiologies of schizophrenia. is the possibility that the phe- region. Coupled with the absence of a pathological neural
notype of schizophrenia is produced by the influence of marker such as plaques and tangles, research to date
multiple factors that lead to a final common pathway in the suggests that the pathology may involve distributed neural
brain. That is, the schizophrenia of ‘poor Tom’ and of the circuits and neurotransmitter systems w15,17,18,31,38,41,
last patient that I saw in Iowa could be caused by a 42,61x. That is, schizophrenia is ‘a disease of neuronal
different and even non-overlapping constellation of factors connectivity’. Perhaps the search must refocus on those
that produce a similar brain injury, hypothesized in the mechanisms that influence and continually shape and re-
Working Model to be an abnormality in neural connectiv- shape neural connectivity at the level of dendrites, spines,
ity that occurs as the brain develops and modifies itself in synapses, neurotransmitters, and signal transducers. These
response to a variety of internal and external influences. mechanisms cannot be visualized and measured using the
traditional methods of neuropathology, which is why those
4.2. Hypothesis two: The pathophysiology is an abnormal- methods led to a ‘graveyard’. However, a host of newer
ity in the regulation and expression of neurodeÕelopment tools are now being applied to this question, with increas-
ingly interesting results, as evidenced by many presenta-
Schizophrenia differs from the classical dementias in tions at this symposium.
that there is no visible neuropathological marker such as
plaques, tangles, or Lewy bodies. Importantly, the gliosis 4.4. Hypothesis four: The phenotype is defined by a mental
that is seen as a marker of neuronal death in neurodegener- metaprocess rather than by clinical symptoms
ative diseases is not present in schizophrenia w59x. This
suggests that the pathophysiology and etiology must be When the connectivity and communication within neu-
related to a maturational or developmental brain process ral circuitry is disrupted, patients display diverse impair-
Že.g., neurite formation, synaptogenesis, neuronal pruning, ments in cognition and diverse symptoms, the complex and
apoptosis. w8,23,34,35,48,54,64,65,69 x. This sets the time varied signs and symptoms of schizophrenia. This clini-
period for the etiology of schizophrenia as some time cally observed presentation of schizophrenia may not in
between the beginning of neuron formation and migration fact represent the true phenotype, since it is comprised of
Žaround the second trimester. and young adult life. Al- varied symptoms that derive from diverse systems, occurs
though this is a long time period, it focuses our thinking in non-overlapping patterns, and does not breed true. In-
N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112 111

stead, the phenotype may perhaps be best defined by a w13x N.C. Andreasen, L. Flashman, M. Flaum, S. Arndt, V. Swayze, D.S.
process that lies behind the symptoms, or a metaprocess, O’Leary, J.C. Ehrhardt, W.T.C. Yuh, Regional brain abnormalities
in schizophrenia measured with magnetic resonance imaging, JAMA
sometimes also referred to as an endophenotype, which is 272 Ž1994. 1763–1769.
a ‘final common pathway’ that defines the illness. This w14x N.C. Andreasen, S. Arndt, V. Swayze, T. Cizadlo, M. Flaum, D.
way of thinking about schizophrenia is very similar to the O’Leary, J. Ehrhardt, W.T.C. Yuh, Thalamic abnormalities in
way that we think about cancer, which is also a disease schizophrenia visualized through magnetic resonance image averag-
with diverse clinical presentations that are due to a more ing, Science 266 Ž1994. 294–298.
w15x N.C. Andreasen, D.S. O’Leary, T. Cizadlo, S. Arndt, K. Rezai, L.L.
fundamental process. For cancer it is misregulation of cell Ponto, G.L. Watkins, R.D. Hichwa, Schizophrenia and cognitive
proliferation. For schizophrenia it is misregulation of infor- dysmetria: A positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional
mation processing in the brain. Several different models prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
have been proposed to define or describe this mental 93 Ž18. Ž1996. 9985–9990.
w16x N.C. Andreasen, Linking mind and brain in the study of mental
metaprocess. Following the search for unitary processes
illnesses: A project for a scientific psychopathology, Science 275
originally suggested by Bleuler and Kraepelin, these mod- Ž1997. 1586–1593.
els seek to identify a single fundamental process that w17x N.C. Andreasen, S. Paradiso, D.S. O’Leary, Cognitive dysmetria as
defines the illness. Examples provided in this Nobel sym- an integrative theory of schizophrenia: A dysfunction in cortical-
posium include the work of Frith w39x, Holzman w46x, and cerebellar circuitry? Schizophr. Bull. 24 Ž2. Ž1998. 203–218.
w18x N.C. Andreasen, Understanding the causes of schizophrenia, N.
Goldman-Rakic w43x. Our work and that of others provide
Engl. J. Med. 340 Ž1999. 645–647.
additional alternative examples w17,25,26,32,37,51,56,67x. w19x E. Armstrong, A. Schleicher, H. Omran, M. Curtis, K. Zilles, The
This strategy may offer a more powerful method for ontogeny of human gyrification, Cereb. Cortex 1 Ž1995. 56–63.
identifying the neural and molecular mechanisms of the w20x P.E. Barta, G.D. Pearlson, R.E. Powers, S.S. Richards, L.E. Tune,
illness and for developing animal models for molecular Auditory hallucinations and smaller superior temporal gyrus volume
and pharmacologic research. in schizophrenia, Am. J. Psychiatry 147 Ž1990. 1457–1462.
w21x D.W. Black, N.C. Andreasen, Schizophrenia, Schizophreniform, and
Delusional ŽParanoid. Disorders, in: The American Psychiatric As-
sociation Textbook of Psychiatry, Washington, 1999, pp. 425–478.
References w22x E. Bleuler, ŽTranslated by J. Zinkin, 1950., Dementia Praecox or the
Group of Schizophrenias, International Universities Press, New York,
w1x S. Akbarian, N.J. Sucher, D. Bradley, A. Tafazzoli, D. Trinh, W.P. 1911.
Hetrick, S.G. Potkin, C.A. Sandman, W.E. Bunney, E.G. Jones, w23x F. Bloom, Advancing a neurodevelopmental origin for schizophre-
Selective alterations in gene expression for NMDA receptor subunits nia, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50 Ž1993. 224–227.
in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics, J. Neurosci. 16 Ž1. Ž1996. w24x B. Bogerts, Recent advances in the neuropathology of schizophrenia,
19–30. Schizophr. Bull. 19 Ž1993. 431–445.
w2x American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual w25x D.L. Braff, M.A. Geyer, Sensorimotor gating and schizophrenia:
of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition ŽDSM-IV., American Psychi- Human and animal model studies, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47 Ž1990.
atric Press, Washington, DC, 1994. 181–188.
w3x N.J.C. Andreasen, James Joyce: A portrait of the artist as schizoid, w26x D.L. Braff, Information processing and attention dysfunctions in
JAMA 224 Ž1973. 67–77. schizophrenia, Schizophr. Bull. 19 Ž1993. 233–259.
w4x N.J.C. Andreasen, A. Canter, The creative writer: Psychiatric symp- w27x D. Brian, Einstein: A Life, Wiley, New York, 1996.
toms and family history, Comp. Psychiatry 15 Ž1974. 123–131. w28x M.S. Buchsbaum, D.H. Ingvar, R. Kessler, R.N. Waters, J. Capel-
w5x N.J.C. Andreasen, The artist as scientist: Psychiatric diagnosis in letti, D.P. Kammen, C. King, J. Johnson, R.G. Manning, R.W.
Shakespeare’s Tragedies, JAMA 235 Ž1976. 1868–1872. Flynn, L.S. Mann, W.E. Bunney, L. Sokoloff, Cerebral glucography
w6x N.C. Andreasen, Negative symptoms in schizophrenia: Definition with positron tomography, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 39 Ž1982. 251–259.
and reliability, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 39 Ž1982. 784–788. w29x M.S. Buchsbaum, K.H. Neuchterlein, R.J. Haier, J. Wu, N. Sicotte,
w7x N.C. Andreasen, S. Olson, Negative versus positive schizophrenia: E. Hazlett, R. Asarnow, S. Potkin, S. Guich, Glucose metabolic rate
Definition and validation, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 39 Ž1982. 789–794. in normals and schizophrenics during the continuous performance
w8x N.C. Andreasen, H.A. Nasrallah, V.D. Dunn, S.C. Olson, W.M. test assessed by positron emission tomography, Br. J. Psychiatry 156
Grove, J.C. Ehrhardt, J.A. Coffman, J.H.W. Crossett, Structural Ž1990. 216–227.
abnormalities in the frontal system in schizophrenia: A magnetic w30x M.S. Buchsbaum, T. Someya, C.Y. Teng, L. Abel, A. Najafi, R.J.
resonance imaging study, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 43 Ž1986. 136–144. Haier, J. Wu, W.E. Bunney Jr., PET and MRI of the thalamus in
w9x N.C. Andreasen, Creativity and mental illness: Prevalence rate in never-medicated patients with schizophrenia, Am. J. Psychiatry 153
writers and their first-degree relatives, Am. J. Psychiatry 144 Ž1987. Ž1996. 191–199.
1288–1292. w31x M. Carlsson, A. Carlsson, Schizophrenia: A subcortical neurotrans-
w10x N.C. Andreasen, Brain imaging: Applications in psychiatry, Science mitter imbalance syndrome? Schizophr. Bull. 16 Ž1990. 425–432.
239 Ž1988. 1381–1388. w32x B. Crespo-Facorro, S. Paradiso, N.C. Andreasen, D.S. O’Leary, G.L.
w11x N.C. Andreasen, J.C. Ehrhardt, V.W. Swayze, R.J. Alliger, W.T.C. Watkins, L.L.B. Ponto, R.D. Hichwa, Recalling word list reveals
Yuh, G. Cohen, S. Ziebell, Magnetic resonance imaging of the brain cognitive dysmetria in schizophrenia patients. A PET study, Am. J.
in schizophrenia: The pathophysiological significance of structural Psychiatry 156 Ž1999. 386–392.
abnormalities, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47 Ž1990. 35–44. w33x T.J. Crow, Positive and negative schizophrenic symptoms and the
w12x N.C. Andreasen, K. Rezai, R. Alliger, V.W. Swayze, M. Flaum, P. role of dopamine, Br. J. Psychiatry 137 Ž1980. 383–386.
Kirchner, G. Cohen, D.S. O’Leary, Hypofrontality in neuroleptic- w34x I. Feinberg, Schizophrenia: Caused by a fault in programmed synap-
naive and chronic schizophrenic patients: Assessment with xenon-133 tic elimination during adolescence? J. Psychiatr. Res. 17 Ž1982.
single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of 319–334.
London, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49 Ž1992. 943–958. w35x B. Fish, Neurobiological antecedents of schizophrenia in children:
112 N.C. Andreasenr Brain Research ReÕiews 31 (2000) 106–112

Evidence for an inherited, congenital neurointegrative defect, Arch. w53x C. Moorehead, Bertrand Russell: A Life, Viking, New York, 1992.
Gen. Psychiatry 125 Ž1977. 1–24. w54x R.M. Murray, S.W. Lewis, Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental
w36x M. Flaum, D.S. O’Leary, V.W. Swayze, D.D. Miller, S. Arndt, N.C. disorder? Br. Med. J. 295 Ž1987. 681–682.
Andreasen, Symptom dimensions and brain morphology in w55x S. Nasar, A Beautiful Mind, Simon and Shuster, New York, 1998.
schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders, J. Psychiatr. Res. 29 w56x K.H. Neuchterlein, M.E. Dawson, Information processing and atten-
Ž1995. 261–276. tional function in the developmental course of schizophrenic disor-
w37x R. Freedman, L.E. Adler, M.C. Waldo, E. Pachtman, R.D. Franks, ders, Schizophr. Bull. 10 Ž1988. 160–203.
Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in inhibitory pathways in w57x P. Nopoulos, I. Torres, M. Flaum, N.C. Andreasen, J.C. Ehrhardt,
schizophrenia: comparison of medicated and drug-free patients, Biol. W.T.C. Yuh, Brain morphology in first episode schizophrenia, Am.
Psychiatry 18 Ž1983. 537–551. J. Psychiatry 152 Ž12. Ž1995. 1721–1724.
w38x K.J. Friston, C.D. Frith, P. Fletcher, P.F. Liddle, R.S.J. Frakowiak, w58x B. Pakkenberg, Pronounced reduction of total neuron number in
Functional tomography: multidimensional scaling and functional mediodorsal thalamic nucleus and nucleus accumens in schizophren-
connectivity in the brain, Cereb. Cortex 6 Ž1996. 156–164. ics, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 47 Ž1990. 1023–1028.
w39x C.D. Frith, The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, w59x G.W. Roberts, N. Colter, R. Lofthouse, E.C. Johnstone, T.J. Crowe,
Lawrence Erlbaum, East Sussix, 1992. Is there gliosis in schizophrenia? Investigation of the temporal lobe,
w40x C.D. Frith, K.J. Friston, S. Herold, D. Silbersweig, P. Fletcher, C. Biol. Psychiatry 22 Ž1987. 1459–1468.
Cahill, R.J. Dolan, R.S.J. Frackowiak, P.F. Liddle, Regional brain w60x N. Sartorius, A. Jablensky, A. Korten, Early manifestations and first
activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during performance of a contact incidence of schizophrenia in different cultures: a prelimi-
verbal fluency task, Br. J. Psychiatry 167 Ž1995. 343–349. nary report on the evaluation of the WHO collaborative study in the
w41x P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Changing concepts of cortical connectivity: determinants of outcome of severe mental disorders, Psychol. Med.
Parallel distributed cortical networks, in: P. Rakic, W. Singer ŽEds.., 16 Ž1986. 909–928.
Neurobiology of Neocortex, Wiley, New York, 1988, pp. 177–202. w61x L.D. Selemon, P.S. Goldman-Rakic, The reduced neuropil hypothe-
w42x P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Topography of cognition: Parallel distributed sis: A circuit based model of schizophrenia, Biol. Psychiatry 45
networks in primate association cortex, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11 Ž1999. 17–25.
Ž1988. 137–156. w62x M.E. Shenton, R. Kikinis, F.A. Jolesz, S.D. Pollak, M. LeMay, C.G.
w43x P.S. Goldman-Rakic, Working memory dysfunction in schizophre- Wible, H. Hokama, J. Martin, D. Metcalf, M. Coleman, R.W.
nia, J. Neuropsych. Clin. Neurosci. 6 Ž1994. 348–357. McCarley, Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought
w44x R.E. Gur, P. Cowell, B.I. Turetsky et al., A follow-up magnetic disorder in schizophrenia: A quantitative magnetic resonance imag-
resonance imaging study of schizophrenia: Relationship of neu- ing study, N. Engl. J. Med. 327 Ž1992. 604–612.
roanatomical changes to clinical and neurobehavioral measures, w63x D.R. Weinberger, K.F. Berman, R.F. Zec, Physiological dysfunction
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55 Ž1998. 145–152. of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia I: regional cerebral
w45x B.C. Ho, N.C. Andreasen, M. Flaum, Dependence on public finan- blood flow ŽrCBF. evidence, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 43 Ž1986.
cial support early in the course of schizophrenia, Psychiatr. Serv. 48 114–124.
Ž1997. 948–950. w64x D. Weinberger, Implications of normal brain development for the
w46x P.S. Holzman, D.L. Levy, L.R. Proctor, Smooth pursuit eye move- pathogenesis of schizophrenia, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44 Ž1987.
ments, attention, and schizophrenia, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45 Ž1976. 660–669.
641–647. w65x D.R. Weinberger, B.K. Lipska, Cortical maldevelopment, anti-psy-
w47x K.R. Jamison, Mood disorders and patterns of creativity in British chotic drugs, and schizophrenia: a search for common ground,
writers and artists, Psychiatry 52 Ž2. Ž1989. 125–134. Schizophr. Res. 16 Ž1995. 87–110.
w48x P. Jones, R.M. Murray, The genetics of schizophrenia is the genetics w66x M. White, Isaac Newton: The Last Sorcerer, Addison-Wesley, Read-
of neurodevelopment, Br. J. Psychiatry 158 Ž1991. 615–623. ing, Mass, 1997.
w49x E. Kraepelin, R.M. Barclay, G.M. Robertson, Dementia Praecox and w67x A.K. Wiser, N.C. Andreasen, D.S. O’Leary, G.L. Watkins, L.L.B.
Paraphrenia, E and S Livingstone, Edinburgh, 1919. Ponto, R.D. Hichwa, Dysfunctional cortico-cerebellar circuits cause
w50x P.F. Liddle, K.J. Friston, C.D. Frith, S.R. Hirsch, T. Jones, R.S.J. ‘cognitive dysmetria’ in schizophrenia, NeuroReport 9 Ž1998. 1895–
Frackowiak, Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia, Br. J. 1899.
Psychiatry 60 Ž1992. 179–186. w68x World Health Organization, Chapter V, Mental and Behavioral
w51x R.W. McCarley, S.F. Faux, M.E. Shenton, P.G. Nestor, J. Adams, Disorders Žincluding disorders of psychological development.: Diag-
Event-related potentials in schizophrenia: Their biological and clini- nosis Criteria for Research, International Classification of Diseases,
cal correlates and a new model of schizophrenic pathophysiology, Tenth Edition ŽICD-10., Geneva, 1990.
Schizophr. Res. 4 Ž1991. 209–231. w69x B.T. Woods, Is schizophrenia a progressive neurodevelopmental
w52x S.O. Moldin, I.I. Gottesman, At issue: genes, experience, and chance disorder? Toward a unitary pathogenetic mechanism, Am. J. Psychi-
in schizophrenia-positioning for the 21st century, Schizophr. Bull. atry 155 Ž12. Ž1998. 1661–1670.
23 Ž4. Ž1997. 547–561.

You might also like