You are on page 1of 36

Copyright © 2000 by The Resilience Alliance.

Sutherland, G. D., A. S. Harestad, K. Price, and K. P. Lertzman. 2000. Scaling of natal dispersal distances
in terrestrial birds and mammals. Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16. [online] URL:
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Report
Scaling of Natal Dispersal Distances in Terrestrial Birds and
Mammals
Glenn D. Sutherland1, Alton S. Harestad2, Karen Price2, and Kenneth P. Lertzman2

ABSTRACT. Natal dispersal is a process that is critical in the spatial dynamics of populations, including
population spread, recolonization, and gene flow. It is a central focus of conservation issues for many vertebrate
species. Using data for 77 bird and 68 mammal species, we tested whether median and maximum natal dispersal
distances were correlated with body mass, diet type, social system, taxonomic family, and migratory status. Body
mass and diet type were found to predict both median and maximum natal dispersal distances in mammals: large
species dispersed farther than small ones, and carnivorous species dispersed farther than herbivores and
omnivores. Similar relationships occurred for carnivorous bird species, but not for herbivorous or omnivorous
ones. Natal dispersal distances in birds or mammals were not significantly related to broad categories of social
systems. Only in birds were factors such as taxonomic relatedness and migratory status correlated with natal
dispersal, and then only for maximum distances. Summary properties of dispersal processes appeared to be
derived from interactions among behavioral and morphological characteristics of species and from their linkages
to the dynamics of resource availability in landscapes.

In all the species we examined, most dispersers moved relatively short distances, and long-distance dispersal was
uncommon. On the basis of these findings, we fit an empirical model based on the negative exponential
distribution for calculating minimum probabilities that animals disperse particular distances from their natal areas.
This model, coupled with knowledge of a species' body mass and diet type, can be used to conservatively predict
dispersal distances for different species and examine possible consequences of large-scale habitat alterations on
connectedness between populations. Taken together, our results can provide managers with the means to identify
species vulnerable to landscape-level habitat changes such as forest fragmentation. In addition, our dispersal
models can be used to predict which species in a community are likely to be the most vulnerable to loss of
connectedness and allow managers to test the merits of alternative habitat conservation plans.

INTRODUCTION dispersal occurs regularly, but at a relatively low


frequency. Nonetheless, long dispersal distances are
Distances moved by juvenile animals during natal important in invasion and recolonization processes
dispersal are a fundamental element of demography (Shaw 1995) and in the genetic structuring of
(Arcese 1989), population dispersion, colonization populations (Ibrahim et al. 1996).
(Hengeveld 1994), and gene flow (Neigel and Avise
1993, Nelson 1993). Despite their ecological No consensus exists on the factors that determine the
importance, dispersal movements are among the least distribution of dispersal distances moved by juvenile
understood attributes of both individual animals and animals (Paradis et al. 1998). Dominant hypotheses
populations. Natal dispersal distances vary currently used to explain the ultimate benefits of natal
considerably among species (Swingland 1982). The dispersal are intrasexual competition for resources
frequency of dispersal decreases with increasing (e.g., mates, food, and space) and inbreeding
distance from the natal area (Taylor 1980, Paradis et avoidance (Dobson 1982, Pusey 1987, Wolff 1993).
al. 1998). Available evidence indicates that short However, there is considerable controversy about the
dispersal distances are frequent and strongly influence relative roles of these two processes in structuring
age and sex structure, abundance, and relatedness dispersal patterns within and among species
within populations. In many species, long-distance (Bengtsson 1978, Moore and Ali 1984, Dobson and

1
University of British Columbia; 2Simon Fraser University
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Jones 1985, Shields 1987, Wolff 1993, Kunkele and predictive relationships for birds and mammals.
von Holst 1996). All hypotheses assume that Accordingly, we developed a number of a priori
dispersers incur costs to survival and/or fecundity expectations regarding how attributes of species should
(Bengtsson 1978) and that these costs increase with relate to their ability to disperse a given distance:
distance traveled. Dispersing individuals may face
increased mortality risks associated with unfamiliar 1. Dispersal distance should increase with
habitats, passage through areas of relatively high increasing body size. Scaling functions that
predator densities, or the physiological costs of relate body size to interspecific variation in
extensive movement (Waser et al. 1994, Plissner and biological functions ranging from rates of
Gowaty 1996). cellular metabolism to population dynamics are
common (Peters 1983, Schmidt-Nielsen 1984,
Lack of quantitative methods for predicting the spatial Holling 1992, Silva 1998). Generally, such
scale of dispersal from natal habitats is a major functions can be explained in terms of how
limitation in the development of theories and tools for individuals acquire and use resources as a
forecasting the effects of landscape alteration on function of body size (Brown and Maurer
connectivity between habitats and subpopulations of 1989, Kozlowski and Weiner 1997, West et al.
animals. Recent spatially explicit models for assessing 1997). In particular, allometric scaling
the effects of habitat fragmentation on populations of equations with an exponent value of
animals require knowledge of the dispersal approximately ¾ are expected for size-
characteristics of their target species. For example, two dependent resource utilization relationships
key parameters are the dispersal rate (number of (McNab 1963, Peters 1983, West et al. 1997).
dispersers leaving their natal territory) and the The dominant hypotheses about the ultimate
expected distance dispersed by each disperser (Pulliam benefits of dispersal can also be interpreted
et al. 1992, With and Crist 1995, Schumaker 1996). broadly in terms of the availability of resources
However, dispersal characteristics of this type are (ecological or genetic). Larger animals also
known in detail for only a few species. While simple have more time, on average, to explore (both
mathematical models have been fit to the dispersal between meals and over a lifetime) as longevity
distributions of some of these species (Waser 1987, increases with body mass ( Peters 1983). Both
Miller and Carroll 1989, Caley 1991), generalizing explanations lead to the prediction that larger
them to other species is questionable. Parameter species will disperse farther (assuming some
estimates vary widely among species (Miller and benefit to dispersing); thus, across species we
Carroll 1989), and many models assume particular expected body mass to be significantly
behavioral mechanisms governing the distance moved correlated with dispersal distance.
by individual dispersers that may themselves not be
2. Dispersal distance should increase with
general (Rees 1993). Interstudy differences in the
increasingly exclusive acquisition of resources.
intensity of sampling and the high probability of biases
We examined two correlates of spacing
introduced through restricted searching patterns
behavior: diet type and social system. Diets
(Porter and Dooley 1993) further reduce the generality
vary in resource abundance and predictability,
of these models.
and diet type influences spacing behavior
(Harestad and Bunnell 1979). Because
The prominent role of dispersal in the life history of utilizable energy per unit area is greater at
most species suggests that relationships exist between lower trophic levels (Harestad and Bunnell
dispersal patterns and basic life-history attributes of 1979) and because home range size increases
species. Indeed, some have recently been described: with decreasing resource abundance (Mace et
mean and standard deviation (SD) of natal and al. 1982), we predicted that predators would
breeding dispersal distances in many species of British disperse farther than omnivores and that
birds depend on habitat type and migratory capability, omnivores would disperse farther than
with body size as a covariate (Paradis et al. 1998). herbivores. In both birds and mammals, factors
However, for some species of mammals, median such as type of mating system and form of
dispersal distance appears to be related to diet type and territorial defense can influence the relative
body size (Van Vuren 1998). We wished to place the distance moved by dispersers (Greenwood
analysis of these relationships into a common 1980). For example, if males defend resources,
ecological framework and determine general, females usually disperse farther (Greenwood
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

and Harvey 1982). If both sexes defend (Bunnell and Harestad 1983). For most species and
territories, gender bias in dispersal does not most dispersing individuals, dispersal takes place
occur in some species (Mattysen and Schmidt before first reproduction and is termed natal dispersal
1987, Arcese 1989). Males are likely to (Howard 1960). Natal dispersal is usually the single
disperse farther in polyandrous species (Oring largest (and often only) long-distance movement made
and Lank 1984) and if males defend females by individual animals (Dice and Howard 1951) and is
(Lessels 1985). We predicted that species generally accepted as the major agent of gene flow
defending breeding territories should disperse among populations (Wiklund 1996). Dispersal by
farther than species with overlapping or non- reproductive adults, if it occurs, has consequences for
exclusive territories. Patterns of dispersal the lifetime reproductive success of individuals
distances in more gregarious species (e.g., (Clutton-Brock 1988, Newton 1989). Because of its
colonial nesters) could potentially differ importance for interpopulation genetic structure and
substantially from those of nongregarious local population dynamics, we focused our analyses on
species, depending on the density, geographic patterns of natal dispersal, rather than breeding
range, and aggregation patterns of breeding dispersal.
sites within and among colonies. For example,
median distances may be quite short if colonies We do not address in detail the variety of reasons,
are clumped in distribution, but maximum proximate and ultimate, why an animal might disperse
distances may be very long if colonies are from its natal area, the factors that directly determine
sparsely dispersed at a large (e.g., continental) mortality during dispersal, or year-to-year variation in
geographic scale. dispersal success. However, given that animals do
3. In birds, dispersal distances for migrants disperse and that the chances of dispersers successfully
should exceed those for residents. Recent colonizing a site remain approximately constant from
evidence suggests that the breeding dispersal year to year, we do consider the nature of relationships
distances for migrant species are longer than among distance dispersed, body mass, and other life-
those for resident species (Paradis et al. 1998), history attributes of species.
although this result was not found for natal
dispersal distances. METHODS
4. Dispersal distances for closely related species Data collection
should be more similar than for less closely
related species. Interspecific comparisons of We searched papers published between 1930 and mid-
ecological relationships are often confounded 1998 for reports of natal dispersal movements by birds
because species are part of a hierarchically and mammals, excluding marine species and bats. Our
structured phylogeny and cannot be regarded, geographically unrestricted survey did not include
for statistical purposes, as if drawn papers written in languages other than English or
independently from the same distribution unpublished theses (except in cases where raw data for
(Felsenstein 1985). In our study, evolutionary a species were made available to us). Most
factors determining body size, although likely documented long-distance movements are based either
independent of dispersal, may be strongly on incidental observations of dispersals made by
correlated in closely related species. individual animals or on much more detailed
investigations that yield frequency distributions of
If the dispersal distance varies predictably with these dispersal distances for one or more populations. More
life-history attributes, then researchers could use such observations of a single long distance moved by a
relationships to identify species potentially vulnerable dispersing animal were reported in studies of
to loss of connectivity between habitats, and habitat mammals than in studies of birds.
planners could use these relationships to assess the
potential risks of alternative habitat configurations to Wide variation in study designs, objectives, and
locally vulnerable species. methods of quantifying dispersal required that we
develop screening criteria for selecting dispersal data
In this paper, we define dispersal as the movement of suitable for comparative analyses. Therefore, we
an individual out of an area larger than its home range, accepted dispersal data only if authors:
with no predictable returns, i.e., excluding migrations
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

1. reported individual dispersal distances (or mean size measured in the study. Where available, we
ranges) rather than means or medians only; also recorded median, mean, and maximum dispersal
distances. For each study, we recorded how
2. reported net (minimum straight-line distance)
observations were obtained (e.g., radiotelemetry,
rather than gross distance moved;
tagging, hunter returns, etc.). Within studies, telemetry
3. did not include observations of likely migrants data were chosen over single observations when both
as dispersers. We accepted subsets of data if were given. We ignored data on nondispersal
authors provided some evidence of the movements.
nonmigratory status of some individuals.
Distinguishing dispersal from migration is We determined mean adult body mass for each species
important for several reasons. In birds, and sex from the original studies (if given) or from
postfledging exploratory movements may have standard references: Dunning (1993) for birds and
a function in locating future breeding sites, Silva and Downing (1995a) for mammals. We also
locating sites suitable for overwintering, or identified the following categorical variables as
establishing a navigational target, all of which potential ecological and life-history correlates of
could confound interpretation of dispersal dispersal distance:
movements (Baker 1993);
4. gave single observations of long-distance 1. Diet type. We classified all bird and mammal
dispersal, provided the authors had evidence species into three diet groups based on the
that the disperser was marked at or near its main foods consumed throughout the year:
natal site and that the reported movement met herbivores (including seed eaters), carnivores
our criteria for natal dispersal movements (as (including insectivores), and omnivores.
defined above); or Definitions follow Schoener (1968): herbivores
ingest < 10% by volume animal matter,
5. either observed breeding (or, for banding whereas carnivores ingest > 90% by volume
studies, specified breeding season returns) or, animal matter on an annual basis. All species
for species with delayed maturity, provided ingesting 10–90% animal matter are
evidence of little movement after settlement. omnivores. We used supplemental dietary
information from Ehrlich et al. (1988) for birds
Many studies reporting data on natal dispersal do not or from our source studies.
control biases in measurement, such as the decreased
likelihood of detecting a dispersing animal at the edge 2. Social system during the breeding season. We
of a study area. Therefore, we further screened studies classified this as territorial, nonexclusive (broad
presenting distance-density distributions of dispersal, overlap between home ranges), or gregarious
selecting only those that did not truncate distances by (including herding and colonial species).
using a small study area. We did include studies 3. Migratory status (birds only). We classified
involving several small study areas with a potential for bird species as migrants (including partial
observing movement between sites. We accepted migrants) or residents. All of our dispersal data
statements of the adequacy of site size when surveys for mammals came from nonmigratory periods,
of surrounding areas did not extend the distribution of so we did not include this variable for
distances. Some authors also included correction mammals.
factors for potential biases in their data; we used these
if provided. 4. Phylogeny. To assess the influence of
relatedness on dispersal scale, we included
taxonomic family as a categorical variable in
For each study selected, we recorded all given
our analyses.
dispersal data. If raw distance-density data were
provided in tables, we recorded them, maintaining data
separately for each sex if possible. Numerical values The data and references used in our analyses are
were also estimated from figures. Small figures with summarized in Tables A1 and A2 in Appendix 1.
log scales probably resulted in some measurement
error for long distances. We converted distances given Data analysis
as numbers of territories or home ranges crossed to
kilometers, assuming packed circular territories of the Our general approach was to use robust methods for
estimation wherever possible and to develop simple,
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

empirical relationships for prediction, rather than dispersal data (median and maximum distances) to
constructing process-based models. Dispersal is a stabilize variances, linearize responses, and normalize
complex phenomenon that makes it difficult to residuals. We then examined normal quantile-quantile
develop general models based on interspecific plots of all transformed data to test for departures from
comparisons (Paradis et al. 1998). Besides the normality, finding that most dispersal distance data
previously mentioned potential for sampling biases, remained significantly non-normal after log10
sample sizes differ between studies, fates of dispersers transformation.
are not always known, and dispersers of different
species may be responding to different proximate Second, we used ordinary least squares (Type I)
factors. We therefore treated our data as follows. regression to develop all predictive allometric
relationships, because we did not have error
To examine underlying similarities in dispersal distributions of body mass data for most species of the
patterns between sexes and among species, we needed type proposed by LaBarbera (1989) in his discussion
to rescale the frequency distributions of dispersal of appropriate regression methods in allometric
distances obtained from each study to a common unit analyses. Before finalizing the regressions, we
of measurement. We rescaled each distribution to a examined the influence of each observation on the
ratio of the distance moved by 50% of the observed estimated regression coefficients using Cook's (1977)
dispersers (hereafter referred to as the median distance and eliminated observations with a Cook's
distance). If the distribution was given in intervals, we distance > 1.0. All estimated coefficients derived from
used the midpoint of the interval containing the allometric equations based on log10 were corrected for
median. Subsequent analyses involving frequency bias using the methods of Sprugel (1983). Because of
distributions used these rescaled values. departures from normality, we estimated standard
errors and confidence limits of the allometric equation
To evaluate the relationships between dispersal coefficients using 1000 replicate bootstrap samples of
distances and life-history characteristics, we used the the data. We adjusted for bias following Efron and
following methods based on median and maximum Tibshirani (1993). All analyses were done with S-Plus
distances moved by dispersers of each species. First, (MathSoft 1998).
we logarithmically transformed all body masses and

Table 1. Summary shape statistics (means ± SD) for the original frequency distributions of detected natal dispersal distances
analyzed in this study. Distributions presented here were not transformed or rescaled prior to analysis. The skewness and
kurtosis of each distribution were calculated using Pearson moment statistics. Tests of the deviation of the population of
shape parameters from a normal distribution were done using t0.05 (Sokal and Rolf 1995).

Percent with Skewness Kurtosis


n
1 mode
λ3 P λ4 P

Birds

Females 24 50.0 1.06 (± 1.37) < 0.001 2.21 (±4.88) < 0.050

Males 23 58.8 1.40 (± 0.63) < 0.001 1.85 (±2.49) < 0.002

Sexes not separated 21 40.0 1.50 (± 1.05) < 0.001 2.44 (±5.71) < 0.20

Mammals

Females 22 62.5 1.51 (± 1.08) < 0.001 2.91 (±4.09) > 0.400

Males 20 63.6 1.22 (± 1.09) < 0.001 2.09 (±3.09) < 0.001

Sexes not separated 5 50.0 0.75 (± 0.84) > 0.15 -0.21 (±2.54) > 0.50
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

RESULTS

Usable dispersal data were found for 77 bird and 68 Fig. 1. Empirical distributions of distances moved by
mammal species (see Tables A1 and A2 in Appendix juveniles dispersing from their natal areas. Each data point
1). Dispersal data were reported separately by sex in represents the number of juveniles dispersing that particular
the majority of cases: 52% for birds and 76% for distance according to the original study. All dispersal
mammals. Within each class, the range of natal distances shown are scaled to the median dispersal distance
(D) for the population estimated from the data presented in
dispersal movements varied by at least two orders of
each original study. Sexes are pooled for each class. Also
magnitude for both median distances. In the case of shown for each class is the LOESS smoothed line through
birds, median distances ranged from 0.03 km for the the data (Cleveland et al. 1992) using a quadratic smoothing
House Martin (Delichon urbica) to 10 km for the algorithm.
Glaucous-winged Gull (Larus glaucescens). For
mammals, median distances ranged from 0.03 km for
the common vole (Microtus agrestis) to 129.7 km for
the gray wolf (Canis lupus). Maximum distances for
birds ranged from 1.3 km for the European Magpie
(Pica pica) to 1305 km for the Great Horned Owl
(Bubo virginianus), and, for mammals, from 0.14 km
for the prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) to 930.1
km for the lynx (Lynx lynx).

Distributions of dispersal distances

The distributions of the 107 dispersal distance data


sets exhibited left skewness and platykurtosis (Table
1), indicating that many dispersers cluster near the
natal areas, whereas others disperse relatively far from
their natal areas (Turchin 1998). This pattern was
significant for all class and sex comparisons, except
for mammals in studies where the sexes were not
separated. Shape statistics based on the original
distance-density distributions are highly variable,
particularly for distances dispersed by females (Table
1). This variability, coupled with different methods of
categorizing distances among studies, suggested that
further analysis of sources of variation in the patterns
of dispersal in birds and mammals was warranted.

Once distances were rescaled to units of the median


dispersal distance for each distribution, comparative
analyses revealed that the distributions were
statistically indistinguishable between sexes and
between noncarnivores (herbivores and omnivores
combined) and carnivores. In two sample
Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests, the sex-based values were
D = 0.042, Pα=0.05, 100 > 0.5 for birds and D = 0.023,
Pα=0.05, 100 > 0.5 for mammals. The noncarnivore vs. Two broad patterns were evident in these pooled data.
carnivore values were D = 0.015, Pα=0.05, 100 > 0.5 for First, rescaled dispersal distance distributions for bird
birds and D = 0.029, Pα=0.05, 100 > 0.5 for mammals. and mammal species were very similar in shape (Fig.
Consequently, for both classes we pooled distributions 1). Second, for both classes, the rescaled dispersal
for different sexes and diet types. distributions showed a strong decline in frequency
with increasing distance from the natal area (Fig. 1). In
both birds and mammals, most detected dispersal
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

distances were less than 3 median distances from the However, it is possible that the lack of significance in
natal area; only 9 of 35 bird species and 2 of 19 our findings for the maximum distance comparison in
mammal species showed juveniles dispersing farther birds and mammals was due to small sample sizes.
than 10 median distances. At least part of the decline Except where indicated below, we pooled the sexes
in the numbers of birds and mammals was probably together for the remainder of our interspecific
due to the reduced likelihood of detecting animals far comparisons.
from the natal area. Although we attempted to screen
out data where this problem was clearly evident, subtle Do dispersal distances scale with body mass and other
sampling biases of this type were more difficult to ecological factors that may determine the density and
identify. We therefore treated the rescaled distributions dispersion of breeding sites across the landscape?
as estimates of minimum dispersal distances only, and Based on our initial expectations, we tested the
not as estimates of "true" but unknown dispersal importance of average adult body mass M in
distances. explaining variations in median dispersal distances
(Dmedian) and maximum dispersal distances (Dmax) in
Rare long-distance dispersals often appear at the birds and mammals. Both Dmedian and Dmax increased
extreme right of frequency distributions of dispersal, significantly with increasing body mass in both
although their effects on determining the dispersal classes, and the variation in dispersal distance
scale of species are usually difficult to take into explained by body mass was higher for maximum
account (Turchin 1998). To test for the presence of dispersal distance in mammals (Table 2). By
"fat tails" in the rescaled distributions, we used comparing bootstrapped confidence intervals for each
polynomial regression to examine the significance of measure (median vs. maximum distance), we found
the coefficients (b0, b1, b2) in the relationship that the estimated slopes of these relationships did not
ln(dispersers)x = b0 + b1x + b2x2, where x is distance differ at α = 0.05 between either measure within
dispersed. We found that, in both birds and mammals, classes or each measure between classes. Nevertheless,
the b2 coefficients were significantly greater than 0: for the intercepts between measures differed significantly
birds, this value was t0.05, 19 = 8.93, P < 0.0001; for for each class. The estimated maximum distance that
mammals, t0.05, 12 = 7.67, P < 0.0001. This confirmed dispersing individuals of a species with a given body
that the tails were fatter than expected under an mass were expected to move was 4.9 and 5.8 times the
assumption of a Gaussian distribution (Turchin 1998). median dispersal distance in birds and mammals,
Although more detailed statistical analysis of the tails respectively (based on ratios of intercepts). Over the
is not warranted here because of potential biases in the range of comparable body masses, ratios of predicted
original data, this result is consistent with a dispersal dispersal distances for birds and mammals combined
pattern created by highly variable dispersal distances vary between 5.1 and 9.5 (median distance) and 5.0
that also includes rare long-distance dispersal events. and 56.0 (maximum distance). Overall, birds dispersed
between 0.5 and 1.5 orders of magnitude farther than
Allometric scaling of dispersal distances mammals of equivalent body mass. Although body
mass was a significant predictor of both median and
The frequency of natal dispersal (i.e., the proportion of maximum dispersal distances for mammals, it was
individuals dispersing) is female-biased in birds and clearly weak in predicting maximum dispersal distance
male-biased in mammals (Greenwood 1980), leading for birds.
to the expectation that female birds and male
mammals may also disperse farther than their When we separated species within classes by diet type,
counterparts of the opposite sex. We could not find estimated allometric relationships were consistently
strong enough evidence for these hypotheses in our stronger for carnivorous species than for herbivores or
data to warrant separate analyses for males and omnivores (Figs. 2 and 3). For herbivorous and
females. There are high probabilities that median and omnivorous birds, the percentage of unexplained
maximum dispersal distances show no significant sex variance in median or maximum dispersal distance
bias. Sign tests comparing distances dispersed by ranged between 86% and 98% after accounting for
females and males of the same species showed that, body mass. The slopes of the allometric regressions
for birds, median distances were P = 0.402, n = 16 and were not significantly different from 0 for either diet
maximum distances were P = 0.254, n = 9, whereas, type or dispersal measure in herbivorous and
for mammals, median distances were P = 0.613, n = omnivorous birds (P > 0.154 for all slopes), whereas
12 and maximum distances were P = 0.06, n = 8. the slopes were significantly different from 0 for
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

carnivorous birds (P < 0.0005 for both median and 1.21; maximum, 0.32–0.77), although there was
maximum distances). Furthermore, slopes for considerable residual uncertainty about the slope (Fig.
carnivorous birds did not differ significantly from 0.75 2).
(bootstrapped 95% confidence range for median, 0.49–

Table 2. Summary of equations converting body mass M (in kg) to type of dispersal distance (in km) in birds and mammals.
Shown are separate Type I regression coefficients (± SD) and regression statistics for species of each diet type ("C" =
carnivores, "H+O" = herbivores and omnivores pooled, and "Combined" = all diet types combined).

Class Dispersal distance type Trophic type Equation df r2(adjusted) P

Birds Median C 36.4 (± 1.33) M0.62 (± 0.27) 12 0.24 0.0440

2.1 (± 1.76)
H+O 24 0.04 0.3300
M0.18 (± 0.18)

13.1 (± 1.47)
Combined 36 0.32 0.0001
M0.63 (± 0.15)

Maximum C 199.5 (± 1.38) M0.59 (± 0.13) 34 0.33 0.0003

H+O 36.4 (± 1.55) M0.14(± 0.15) 41 0.02 0.6600

Combined 73.3 (± 1.31) M0.34 (± 0.10) 75 0.09 0.0030

Mammals Median <


C 3.45 (± 1.07) M0.89 (± 0.03) 10 0.88
0.0001

<
H+O 1.45 (± 1.05) M0.54 (± 0.01) 18 0.74
0.0001

Combined 2.04 (± 1.32) M0.67 (± 0.09) 28 0.42 0.0010

Maximum <
C 40.7 (± 1.41) M0.81 (± 0.14) 48 0.73
0.0001

H+O 3.31 (± 1.17) M0.65 (± 0.05) 15 0.75 0.0001

<
Combined 6.46 (± 1.23) M0.68 (± 0.08) 63 0.72
0.0001

For mammals, the unexplained variance in median and Dispersal distances of mammalian carnivores were
maximum dispersal distances after accounting for considerably longer than those of herbivores and
allometric relationships was less than 32% for all omnivores. The predicted ratios of carnivore dispersal
regressions. Analysis of covariance indicated that the distances to herbivore/omnivore distances ranged
intercepts of the relationships for carnivorous between 1.2 and 4.5 (median distance) and 7.1 and
mammals differed significantly from those of 18.3 (maximum distance) for mammals of equivalent
herbivores and omnivores (P < 0.03), although the body mass. As in birds, the slopes of the regressions
slopes of the regressions did not differ. We therefore for carnivorous mammals did not differ significantly
pooled our data for herbivores and omnivores (Fig. 3). from 0.75 (bootstrapped 95% confidence range for
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

median, 0.65–1.11; maximum, 0.64–1.10), although 0.75 (bootstrapped 95% confidence range for median,
the slopes for both measures calculated for the 0.39–0.70; maximum, 0.56–0.72).
combined herbivore and omnivore data were both <

Fig. 2. Allometric relationships in birds between body mass (M), median dispersal distance (Dmedian) on the left, and
maximum dispersal distance(Dmax) on the right. Solid triangles represent data points for herbivores, open circles represent
data points for omnivores, and solid circles represent data points for carnivores. Significant Type I regressions (—) are shown
for carnivores only (see Table 2 for equations), along with their 95% confidence bands (····) and 95% prediction bands (----).
Sexes or other categorical variables are not differentiated in this figure.
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Fig. 3. Allometric relationships in mammals between body mass, median dispersal distance on the left, and maximum
dispersal distance on the right. Symbols are as in Fig. 2. Significant Type I regressions are given for herbivores and
omnivores (pooled) and for carnivores (see Table 2 for equations). Sexes and other categorical variables are not
differentiated. Note the difference in x-axis scale between this figure and Fig. 2.

We examined the relationships between our two distances in birds or mammals (Table 3). We found
measures of dispersal distance (median and maximum) that observed maximum distances dispersed by species
and the categorical life-history variables after we of Corvidae, particularly Gray Jay (Perisoreus
removed the influences of body mass and diet type canadensis) and Magpie (Pica pica), and Fringillidae,
(Table 3). Except for family and migratory type in namely Song Sparrow (Melospiza melodia), Savannah
birds, we found little evidence for the effects of these Sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis), and White-
variables on either median or maximum dispersal crowned Sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys), were
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

shorter, averaging 72% less than the predicted value. maximum distances dispersed by migratory bird
In contrast, those of the Laridae, including the lesser species were significantly farther than for resident
Black-backed Gull (Larus fuscus), the Glaucous- species (Table 3); class averages for migratory species
winged Gull (L. glaucescens), the Ring-billed Gull (L. were 28.4 km, n = 39, and, for resident species, 24.3
delawarensis), and the Herring Gull (L. argentatus), km, n = 34. Our data provided no clear evidence that
and the Motacillidae, particularly the White Wagtail social system was a correlate of the distance of
(Motacilla alba) and the Yellow Wagtail (M. flava)), dispersal in either birds or mammals, although, in
were longer than expected, averaging 60.7% farther general, low power precluded eliminating this variable
than the predicted value based on the general body from further study (Table 3).
mass-diet type relationship for birds. In addition, the

Table 3. Effects of categorical variables on median and maximum dispersal distances of birds and mammals after removing
the effects of body mass and diet type. Shown are all main effects and significant interactions. Effects significant at α = 0.05
are marked with an asterisk (*). Comparisons with too few observations for analysis are indicated by an ellipsis ("...").

Median dispersal Maximum dispersal


Class Variable
F d.f. P Power F d.f. P Power

Birds Family 2.21 9,27 0.05* 0.88 0.64 11,77 0.77 0.35

Migration 0.01 1,27 0.93 0.10 0.68 1,77 0.41 0.15

Social
0.08 2,27 0.27 0.12 0.10 2,77 0.91 0.11
system

Migration x
social ... ... ... ... 191.20 22,77 0.046* 0.97
system

Mammals Family 0.22 10,17 0.99 0.98 1.76 15,48 0.20 0.64

Social
2.14 1,17 0.16 0.45 0.11 2,48 0.89 0.12
system

Family x
0.02 1,17 0.89 0.10 0.80 30,48 0.73 0.41
social system

By studying patterns of residuals after the influences 0.49 for mammals.


of body size and diet type had been removed, we
tested for two possible trends in our data. First, we Second, because birds are more mobile than mammals,
searched for systematic biases introduced by the we considered the possibility that their observed
method of obtaining dispersal data. We found no dispersal distances might also be more variable than
evidence that the observation method biased the results those of mammals. When we looked at how the
in either birds or mammals. One-way ANOVA yielded observed dispersal distance values were dispersed with
a median distance of F0.05, 3,347 = 1.17, P = 0.33 and a regard to their predicted values between classes and
maximum distance of F0.05, 3,74 = 1.42, P = 0.51 for diet types, we found no evidence that this was the
birds, and a median distance of F0.05, 2,10 = 1.72, P = case. In fact, it is possible that dispersal distances for
0.20 and a maximum distance of F0.05, 2,64 = 0.72, P = mammals may be relatively more variable than those
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

for birds: paired t tests of coefficients of variation animal could disperse a given distance using data on
yielded values of t0.05(2),3 = 1.95, P = 0.15. its body mass, diet type, and the relationships in Table
2 and Figs. 2, 3, and 4, we derived a simple
phenomenological model based on the empirical
density-distance curves in Fig. 1. Our intention was to
Fig. 4. Relationship between the median dispersal distance
(Dmedian) and the maximum dispersal distance (Dmax)
develop general relationships for estimating the
observed in birds and mammals. Birds are represented by minimum probability that a given animal would
open circles and mammals by solid circles. Also shown is disperse beyond a particular distance. For the range of
the fitted Type I regression line (——) for the pooled data, species that we examined, potentially there were many
the 95% confidence band (····), and the 95% prediction band varied mechanisms that could govern the decision of a
(----) for the relationship. The sexes are not differentiated. dispersing individual to settle at a site. We chose a
conservative dispersal model based on negative
exponential distribution as the simplest and most
general description of the data. This distribution
assumed that, over a large area and a sufficiently long
time, the probability of predicting the dispersal
distances of individuals could be approximated by a
Poisson process. Other process-based models derived
from alternative hypotheses to determine where
dispersers settle have also been developed (Taylor
1980, Miller and Carroll 1989, Caley 1991, Turchin
1998). In many cases, these models do not describe
dispersal patterns any better than does the negative
exponential, even for single species (Miller and
Carroll 1989, Caley 1991). If X is the potential
distance dispersed by an individual originating in a
natal area, with an expected distance of θ, then the
distribution function of X

Because we could estimate the median and maximum


dispersal distances for a given species from the body (1)
mass and diet type relationships in Figs. 2 and 3 and
Table 2, we wondered if we could also estimate the with the useful property that the probability that X
median distance dispersed from the maximum exceeds some value x is given by
distance, and vice versa. To examine this, we chose
only those species of birds and mammals for which we
had both a maximum dispersal distance observation
and a median dispersal distance. As expected, both (2)
classes showed a significant relationship between when x > 0 (Hogg and Tanis 1997).
maximum and median distances, and, as seen in Fig. 4,
Dmax = 9.77 (± 1.23 SD) x Dmedian = 0.98 (± 0.08 SD), For each class, we fit separate negative exponential
df = 59, adjusted r2 = 0.73, P = 0.0001. For both birds functions to two distributions. The first, called the
and mammals, the slope of the line was not
"base" estimate because the source data probably
significantly different from a 1:1 relationship, although underestimated the true pattern of dispersal due to
birds had a higher intercept than mammals. unknown distance-weighted sampling biases, was the
mean value of the empirical distribution given at each
An empirical probability model for the distance unit in Fig. 1. This value was Pr(distance
minimum distance of dispersal dispersed > D) = e-(D/1.64), r2 = 0.99 for birds, and
Pr(distance dispersed > D) = e-(D/1.5), r2 = 0.99 for
To infer the minimum probability that a dispersing mammals. The second was a "corrected" estimate of
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

mean dispersal made using mean correction factors for The primarily herbivorous red squirrel (Tamiasciurus
birds and mammals based on the factors estimated for hudsonicus) and the carnivorous marten (Martes
individual species by Porter and Dooley (1993). In this americana) are good examples of how these
case, the values were, for birds, Pr(distance dispersed probability models can be used in combination with
> D) = e-(D/2.6), r2 = 0.94, and, for mammals, allometric equations to yield estimates of the minimum
Pr(distance dispersed > D) = e-(D/2.06), r2 = 0.96. The probability that dispersers of selected mammal species
resulting pairs of probability curves are shown in Fig. 5. will disperse a given distance. Female red squirrels
have an average body mass of 0.199 kg (Appendix 1,
Table A2), and the equations in Table 2 predicted
median and maximum dispersal distances of 0.82 and
Fig. 5. Estimated probabilities that the minimum distance 11.0 km, respectively. Using the "base" probability
moved by dispersing birds or mammals exceeds a particular model equation for mammals given above, we found
number of median distance units (D) based on negative- that the estimated probability of dispersing farther than
exponential models fit to the empirical distance density data 11.33 median distance units was less than one in a
(see An Empirical Probability Model for the Minimum thousand. By multiplying the estimate of the median
Distance of Dispersal). Fitted parameters are presented for distance dispersed for this species (0.82 km) by this
"base" assumptions and for estimated distributions using
estimate of the number of median distance units
distance-weighted sampling correction factors representing
"corrected" assumptions. The horizontal dashed lines
(11.33), we found, using our "base" assumptions, that
indicate 0.05 probabilities that dispersing juveniles will dispersing females of this species had a small
move farther than the distance indicated by the matching probability of settling farther than 9.29 km. Similar
vertical dashed lines. calculations for marten, which has a body mass of 1.04
kg, yielded an estimate of 40.5 km as the threshold
distance beyond which dispersers have only a small (P
< 0.001) probability of successfully dispersing. For
marten we also had one long-distance dispersal
observation of 61 km (Appendix 1, Table A2). Given
the conservative assumptions we used, our estimate of
a probable "base" threshold long-distance dispersal
range for marten was, as expected, smaller than this
observation. The equivalent threshold long-distance
dispersal range calculated using the "corrected"
probability model was 51.0 km for this species.

How well do the empirical minimum-probability


negative exponential models predict the observed
maximum extent of dispersal distances in birds and
mammals? The dispersal distance data set used to
estimate parameters for the empirical models (Fig. 1)
did not include the maximum long-distance dispersal
movement used to develop the allometric models
(Figs. 2 and 3). Hence, we compared the probabilities
of observing dispersal distances greater than those
estimated from the empirical model with the
proportion of observed maximum distances from our
data that did exceed the predicted maximum distance
(Fig. 6). As expected given the evidence of "fat tails"
in the distributions of dispersal distances, all the
models underestimated the frequency of long-distance
dispersal events, i.e., all points were above the 1:1
line. This was true for both classes even when the
estimated maximum extent was large enough that the
predicted probability of observing even longer
distances became very small. For this reason, the
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

models are best applied in cases where researchers and DISCUSSION


managers wish to minimize the risk of overestimating
dispersal probabilities, for example, for endangered or We found that a significant proportion of the variation
highly habitat-specific species. in the distances dispersed by juvenile birds and
mammals could be explained by differences in body
mass and diet type, despite known differences among
species in terms of reproductive ecology and movement
Fig. 6. Comparison of the probabilities that dispersers capability. Within a class, large species dispersed
would disperse farther than a given distance (D), estimated farther than small species. In comparisons of bird and
using the negative-exponential models with the percentage mammal species of similar body mass, the dispersal
of observed maximum dispersal distances in birds and distances of carnivorous species were significantly
mammals from our data that exceeded the predicted longer than those of noncarnivorous species. In
distance. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals addition, the dispersal distances of carnivorous species
about the mean values, calculated using 1000 replicate
of both birds and mammals grew at an increasing rate
bootstrap samples of the data. Separate comparisons are
shown for the "base" probability models, which are with increasing body mass. Our finding that the shapes
represented by open symbols, and for the "corrected" of distributions of dispersal distances were similar
probability models, which are represented by solid symbols. among species and classes suggested that the
Also shown are the 1:1 lines indicating an identity between probability of individuals dispersing different distances
model predictions and observations. could be estimated even for poorly known species. For
many research and management situations, it would be
helpful to calculate the approximate likelihood that
organisms could disperse particular distances between
required habitats. One way to do this is to combine a
predictive allometric relationship for estimating long
distances of dispersal with a general phenomenological
model for predicting the minimum probability of
dispersing at least that far. We think that a primary
benefit of our models lies in their ability to identify
attainable distances for populations and species using
easily acquired characteristics of species (attributes of
diet and body mass) if detailed dispersal data are
lacking. Our models can be used to identify dispersal-
limited species and thus focus attention on the
characteristics of the landscapes needed to maintain
viable populations of these species.

Consequences of body size and diet type for


dispersal scale

Do patterns of natal dispersal reflect an underlying


relationship among macroecological variables such as
habitat productivity, patterns of resource use, and the
processes that determine how far animals disperse?
Among bird and mammal species, differences in the
coefficients of the allometric regressions for different
diet types were found to be consistent with expected
differences in the density and spacing of resources.
Intercepts for the equations for carnivores predicted
dispersal distances over two orders of magnitude
farther than those of herbivores and omnivores of
equivalent body mass. The slopes of the relationships
for carnivores were consistent with the ¾ scaling
expected for size-dependent resource utilization
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

relationships (McNab 1963, Peters 1983, West et al. of dispersal in both birds and mammals. For example,
1997). Slopes for herbivores and omnivores were the leptokurtic, fat-tailed shapes of the distributions in
smaller than those for carnivores. We remain cautious, Table 1 suggest highly variable dispersal distances
however, when relating causal processes to these among individuals of a given species, with a few
statistical differences between allometric slopes, individuals dispersing long distances relative to the
treating them instead as summary descriptions of the median of the population. Such rare long-distance
minimum distances of dispersal among species. We events are difficult to observe but are important for the
think that the complex spatial and temporal spread of species (Kot et al. 1996), as well as for
interactions that determine when animals move, their preventing inbreeding in small populations (Mills and
risks of mortality while dispersing, and the processes Allendorf 1996). Simple probability density models
driving habitat availability must all help explain such as the negative exponential, while capturing the
differences in dispersal patterns based on resource average behavior of the population of dispersers,
utilization (see also Speculation section below). significantly underestimate the potential for range
expansion in many organisms (Kot et al. 1996).
Methods of fitting allometric equations differ in their
assumptions and interpretation of results. Reduced By examining the predictive nature of intra- and
major-axis (Type II) regression provides a less biased interspecific patterns of dispersal distances in both
estimate of the underlying functional relation as birds and mammals, we can extend results from other
described by an allometric equation (LaBarbera 1989), research. Earlier work by Peters (1983) found positive
and some authors base their allometric analyses on relationships among body mass, speed of locomotion,
Type II regression (Silva 1998). Error distributions of and maximum distance of migration for both birds and
body mass data for species were provided only mammals. The general allometric patterns that we
occasionally in the sources we used. This prevented us obtained for measures of natal dispersal in mammals
from using Type II regression in most analyses. are similar to those obtained by Van Vuren (1998) for
However, our primary goal was to use the equations median dispersal distances of 40 species of North
for predicting expected dispersal distances, given body America mammals and by J. O. Wolff (unpublished
mass and other criteria. For this purpose, Type I data) for long-distance movements in 74 species of
regression was appropriate (LaBarbera 1989). mammals. We obtained slightly shorter median
dispersal distances for herbivorous mammals than did
The spatial scale of dispersal is a primary determinant Van Vuren (1998), although the differences are within
of its functional role in the dynamics of populations, 95% confidence intervals as calculated from his
even in sedentary species of birds (Koenig et al. 1996, results. In their analysis of natal dispersal patterns in
Martin 1998). The frequency distribution of dispersal 75 terrestrial bird species in the UK, Paradis et al.
distances is key to calculating the probability that (1998) found that body mass was a significant
individuals can move through a given landscape covariate of mean and median dispersal distances, but
(Merriam 1998). Our models can be used to estimate they did not attempt to develop predictive
minimum threshold values of the likelihood that relationships. Body mass, diet type, and other factors,
dispersing individuals can move particular distances. in part, determine dispersal distance. Thus, these
Despite the assumptions in our models, we think that factors assist in linking broad population-level
they can be used to generate distributions of dispersal consequences of dispersal (e.g., lifetime fitness,
distances for species whose dispersal parameters are probabilities of habitat occupancy, genetic relatedness)
poorly known. Our models have heuristic value in with life-history attributes of species and their needs
estimating minimum distances between suitable for resources, including the distribution and
habitats for selected species, e.g., conservatively, availability of these resources within and among
female red squirrels (T. hudsonicus) have a strong habitats (Brown and Maurer 1989, Holling 1992,
probability (P < 0.001) of successfully dispersing at Martin 1998). Specific predictions about the dispersal-
least 9.2 km, whereas marten (M. americana) have the mediated consequences to population dynamics that
same probability of successfully dispersing at least are created by future perturbations of habitats are less
40.5 km; details of these calculations are provided in easy to develop using our results. Factors such as
the Results section. However, our models cannot be density-dependent effects on dispersal rates, mortality
used to calculate directly the rates at which species risks, and the availability of unoccupied areas in
could expand their ranges. Rare, long-distance habitats result from interactions among behavioral
dispersal events influence the overall pattern and scale processes below our level of analysis.
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Life-history correlates of dispersal scale evidence that the range of variation in median or
maximum dispersal distances was greater for birds
We were surprised to find little evidence that the than for mammals, after accounting for body mass and
median or maximum distances of natal dispersal in diet type. It is possible that differences in researchers'
birds or mammals were related to broad categories of ability to detect dispersing birds and mammals may
life-history strategies. Only in birds were taxonomic have confounded this result. However, not only did we
relatedness and migratory status found to be related to fail to find a detectable effect of observation method
maximum distance dispersed. In general, our findings (e.g., radiotelemetry, band-resight, etc.) on the residual
paralleled those of Paradis et al. (1998) despite the variation about the regressions, but we also screened
differences in our statistical approach. Of particular both sets of data to ensure that the effects of small
interest are the differences in dispersal distances study areas were reduced or eliminated. This apparent
between migratory and resident bird species, with the similarity in range of variation for dispersal distances
former dispersing farther. Resident species probably between the two classes may reflect interclass
incur different costs and benefits from dispersing than similarity in types of interactions between dispersing
do long-distance migrant species (e.g., neotropical or animals and their environment that determine their
transequatorial migrants) and may be less probability of settling. Our inability to separate clearly
opportunistic in their choice of a site at which to settle the effects of physiological limits, mortality factors,
(Paradis et al. 1998). However, our questionable and variation of habitat quality on the scale of
ability to separate postfledging exploratory movements dispersal constrains our ability to infer how those
(including dispersal) from migratory movements in interactions might operate.
our data renders this interpretation provisional.
Furthermore, our tests are generally not very effective Martin (1998) posed basic questions about patterns of
at distinguishing putative differences in dispersal dispersal: "how, where, how far, when." Our
patterns attributable to other life-history factors (range interspecific study identified predictive relationships
of β: 0.05-0.48). We therefore suggest that allometric underlying the "how far" question. However, other
scaling of dispersal among species as presented in our data are needed from species-specific studies of
study should be viewed primarily as representing dispersal. From our perspective of dispersal processes
synthetic relationships that integrate many, sometimes and landscape design, an important gap in knowledge
conflicting, fine-scale behavioral and ecological is the lack of information about the survivorship of
processes rather than indicating their lack of relevance. dispersing animals. Simulations by Henein and
Merriam (1990) showed the potential importance of
Several syntheses showed that dispersal rates and survivorship during dispersal movements, and there
distances were often male-biased in mammals and are empirical data from several studies involving the
female-biased in birds (Greenwood 1980, Wolff 1993, dwarf mongoose, Helogale parvula (Waser et al.
Clarke et al. 1997). In our analysis, evidence of a 1994), the Blue-breasted Fairy Wren, Malurus
distinction in median or maximum dispersal distances pulcherrimus, and the White-browed Babbler,
between sexes of the same species was too weak to Pomatostomus superciliosus (Brooker et al. 1999), and
make it worthwhile to treat the genders separately. the Northern Spotted Owl, Strix occidentalis (Miller et
Possible explanations for our failure to detect expected al. 1997). However, studies of dispersal in several
differences between genders in median and maximum grouse species did not indicate a significant survival
distances include small sample sizes (only 16 species cost to dispersing juveniles (Martin 1998). We agree
of birds and eight of species of mammals met our data with Merriam (1998) that studies of habitat-specific
criteria), high variance (in part, because observations survivorship during dispersal periods are required to
of maximum distances dispersed by individual animals assess rates of exchange between fragments in
are often serendipitous), and the fact that our dispersal landscape-habitat models.
data did not come from a random sample of species.
Bird species dispersed much farther than mammal SPECULATION
species of equivalent body mass for both median and
maximum dispersal distances. This result is intuitive, In many vertebrates, body mass is closely correlated
because birds are more vagile than nonvolant with ecological variables that affect the allocation of
mammals. space and nutritional resources, e.g., home range area
(McNab 1963, Harestad and Bunnell 1979, Holling
It was even more surprising that we did not find 1992), local population density (Silva and Downing
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

1995b), and geographic range (Brown and Maurer are not always apparent from studies of individual
1989). We recognize that there are still substantial species. If the ultimate evolutionary function of
problems related to the derivation, interpretation, and dispersal is to improve reproductive success (Sinclair
predictive power of allometric scaling when applying 1992) and if interspecific allometric relationships
it to interspecific ecological analyses. capture the responses of species to patterns of resource
availability at different scales (Holling 1992), then a
First, whereas allometric relationships provide a species' dispersal capability may be linked to the
concise description of the percentage changes of one spatial and temporal grain as well as the extent of the
structural character (usually body mass) with other resources required for survival and reproduction.
life-history characteristics, their functional Generally, interpretations of an animal's areal
interpretation remains unclear. At root, this uncertainty decisions about utilization of resources are related to
is founded upon the question of whether a pervasive the density of resources at the scales of individual
causal process underlies allometric patterns or whether foraging items, patches, and their seasonal integration
interspecific allometries arise as mere statistical at the larger scale of a home range (Holling 1992,
epiphenomena out of syntheses of intraspecific Peterson et al. 1998). Our finding of structurally
patterns (Cates and Gittleman 1997). On the one hand, similar relationships between body mass and the
West et al. (1997) propose that constraints on energy spatial scale of dispersal in birds and mammals
allocation within and among organisms in a phylogeny suggests that an individual's dispersal decisions have a
are the fundamental explanation for interspecific similar ecological basis, serving to locate the animal in
allometric relationships (including macroecological habitats that are likely to provide resources needed for
ones such as home range size and patterns of its long-term reproductive success. We suggest that, at
population density). On the other, Koslowski and the large spatial and long time scales of dispersal
Weiner (1997) argue that, because the target of phenomena, the summary properties of dispersal
evolution is individual variation within a species, processes that determine their spatial extent may be
interspecific patterns may combine different derived from interactions between behavioral and
evolutionary solutions to the problem of energy morphological variables of species and their linkages
allocation. Consequently, they think that interspecific to the dynamics of resource availability in landscapes.
allometries have only weak biological significance.
The results of our study can be used to develop and
Second, analytical methods for calculating allometric assess potential solutions for problems involving
exponents can be biased (LaBarbera 1989), habitat management and landscape planning. We
particularly in their assumption that the processes suggest two broad types of applications and offer
being compared are similar across the taxa and body examples from forests. First, the basic parameters of
mass ranges under consideration (Prothero 1986). the frequency-distance distributions are fundamental to
Thus, inferences about the meaning of exponents assessing the potential effects of habitat loss and
relating ecological patterns to body mass must fragmentation on selected species. Many conservation
carefully consider whether underlying processes are issues involve natal dispersal because it is a primary
truly comparable among the species included (Holling mechanism for colonization and genetic exchange
1992). between populations (Hedrick 1996). As well,
population declines in many species have been linked
Third, by the nature of their derivation, scaling directly to loss and fragmentation of habitats
relationships (e.g., exponents) have little capability to (Robinson et al. 1992) and indirectly to reduced
discriminate across the broad classes of stochastic or interpatch dispersal (Whitcomb et al. 1981, Fahrig and
conditionally stochastic processes that generated them. Merriam 1985, Doak et al. 1992, Pulliam et al. 1992,
Additionally, because they are implicitly conditional Lamberson et al. 1994, Schumaker 1996). For species
(i.e., they arise from studies conducted under specific in which preserving key habitats is a management
conditions), using them to forecast patterns is priority, researchers and managers can combine the
problematic if future conditions are expected to differ allometric equations with the probability model to help
substantially from those under which the original assess the ability of alternative configurations of
relationships evolved. habitat patches to provide adequate connectedness
among populations (Van Vuren 1998).
Despite these difficulties, broad cross-taxon
relationships reveal characteristics of processes that Managers must have a knowledge of dispersal because
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

it is a critical variable for modeling the effects of capability among the species of a community,
landscape change on the long-term viability of members of the community can be identified that are
metapopulations (Beissinger and Westphal 1998). potentially more vulnerable to habitat fragmentation
Concerns about habitat fragmentation and landscape than others. Such analyses can be used to plan
design are based, in part, on the ability of wildlife to landscapes of managed forests over the long term,
disperse between the blocks of habitat types that they supporting frequently used theoretical arguments for
require (Schumaker 1996, Fahrig 1997). Long-term the inclusion of broad-scale movement patterns in the
population declines of forest-dependent species are management of wildlife habitat (Schumaker 1996,
thought to occur because large-scale forest harvesting Beissinger and Westphal 1998). By combining
changes habitats by destroying old-growth forest, information on patch size with data on distances
reducing the number of areas of contiguous mature between patches, forest managers can determine which
forest, and eliminating many structural components in forest-dwelling species are vulnerable in existing and
stands (Bunnell and Kremsater 1990, Hansen et al. future landscapes.
1991, Spies et al. 1994). Forestry practices can alter
and fragment habitats over spatial and temporal scales Modeling of dispersal distances is useful at the scale of
important for population survival, e.g., 10 m–1000 km landscapes in assessing the connectedness between
and 1 month–1 century (Bunnell and Huggard 1999). habitats for the species and populations they host.
These scales place a priority on identifying the Such analyses must include detailed species-specific
equivalent extent of population movements that will habitat relationships (Brooker et al. 1999). Besides
reduce the risk of population loss. Our study provides fragmentation, managers should consider processes
estimates of dispersal distances and can be used to that occur within the matrix between habitat
predict the effects of fragmentation and alteration of fragments. The number of individuals dispersing a
habitats on individual species. particular distance depends on the survivorship of
dispersing animals as they travel through this matrix.
The second broad type of application is at the level of However, there is little information on the landscape
communities in managed landscapes. Our results can features that increase the risks of mortality for
be used to help identify dispersal-limited species that dispersing animals (Beissinger and Westphal 1998).
may face long-term risks from large-scale habitat To facilitate dispersal and reduce risks to vulnerable
alterations. The allometric relationships that we species, it is essential to maintain the quality of the
present can be used to estimate dispersal distances for habitat encountered by dispersing animals. Two key
each species of bird and mammal in a community and questions are central to issues of forest fragmentation:
assess their potential vulnerability to fragmentation how hostile is the intervening habitat to a species and
based on dispersal capability. Loss and fragmentation for how long is it hostile? In forest landscapes
of forests, especially late-seral stands, is touted as a modified by logging, managers could decrease the
critical issue in the management of temperate forests "hostility" of the matrix by maintaining structural
(Lehmkuhl and Ruggiero 1991). Predicting the effects heterogeneity within cutblocks (Franklin et al. 1997).
of habitat changes due to forest practices on By providing suitable cover in cutblocks, survivorship
communities is contentious because of the biological of dispersing individuals could increase, which would
interdependence of forest ecosystems; forestry and enhance dispersal between fragments. Appropriate
land-use practices create several changes silvicultural practices in the matrix between forest
simultaneously (Bunnell 1999). To meet the habitat fragments could reduce the vulnerability of species to
needs of the community of species dwelling in late- habitat fragmentation.
seral stands, managers must consider the amount of
late-seral forest that is retained, the size of forest Responses to this article can be read online at:
patches, and the distances between forest patches. It is http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16/responses/index.html.
difficult to optimize the spatial distribution of later
seral forests for the suite of species that depends on
them because of the complex relationship between
habitat area, patch size, and distance between patches Acknowledgments:
(Harris 1984). Our models can be used as a planning
filter for assigning priorities to conservation initiatives We thank Fred Bunnell, Dave Daust, Glen Dunsworth, Sue
and more directly evaluating management options for Glenn, David Huggard, Kathy Martin, Doug Runde, and Ian
these vulnerable species. By examining dispersal Thompson for helpful discussions during the development of
this paper. Fred Bunnell, Clay Elder, Neil Surrey, and Elke
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Wind assisted us with the literature search. Comments by Canada Green Plan research grant to ASH, KL, and Fred
the Forest Ecology Group at SFU, Sue Glenn, David Bunnell, as well as Forest Renewal British Columbia, and
Huggard, C. S. Holling, Phil Taylor, and three anonymous an EcoResearch Doctoral Fellowship to GDS. This is
reviewers greatly improved earlier versions of the research contribution R-33 from the Centre for Applied
manuscript. Research funding was provided by a Forestry Conservation Biology.

APPENDIX 1
Sources of dispersal data for birds (Table A1) and mammals (Table A2) used for analysis. Species are listed in
taxonomic order. For each study, the type of data extracted is coded as "S" (single observation only—generally a
maximum natal dispersal distance) or "D" (distance-density distribution). Categorical life-history variables shown
here are: (1) diet type: H, herbivore; O, omnivore; C, carnivore; (2) social system in breeding season: T,
territorial; N, non-exclusive territories or neighborhood; G, gregarious; and (3) migratory status of the population
or species: M, migratory; R, non-migratory. If sample sizes (n) are given, they refer to the number of dispersing
animals meeting our criteria that were reported in the study; otherwise, a " ... " is given. Where possible, data for
males (m) and females (f) are shown separately. Data from studies in which the sexes were not differentiated are
indicated by "m/f."

Table A1. Birds

Natal Natal
dispersal dispersal
Body Obs
Species median maximum 1 2 3 n Source
mass (kg) type
distance distance
(km) (km)

Phalacrocorax 2.03 m
S ... 150.0 m/f C G M ... Hobson (1997)
pelagicus 1.7 f

11.80 m
Cygnus olor S ... 64.0 m/f H T R ... Ciaranca et al. (1997)
9.67 f

Cygnus 11.40 m
S ... 128.0 m/f O T M ... Mitchell (1994)
buccinator 10.30 f

Branta 3.814 m 2.0 m 28.4 m 19 m


D H T R Lessells (1985)
canadensis 3.314 f 1.5 f 11.2 f 20 f

0.681 m Hepp and Bellrose


Aix sponsa S ... 3.8 m/f O N M ...
0.685 f (1995)

Bucephala 1.00 m Dow and Fredga


D 0.75 f 6.0 f O N M 138 f
clangula 0.80 f (1983)

Bucephala
0.334 f S ... 4.5 f C T M ... Gauthier (1993)
albeola

Lophodytes 0.68 m
S ... 5.6 m/f C T M 10 f Dugger et al. (1994)
cucullatus 0.54 f

Eudocimus 1.04 m Kushlan and Bildstein


S ... 100.0 m/f C G M ...
albus 0.76 f (1992)

Elanus leucurus 0.325 m/f S ... 160.0 m/f C N R ... Dunk (1995)
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Marcström and
Accipiter 0.912 m 32 m 87 m Kenward (1981),
D 1150 m/f C T M
gentilis 1.137 f 17 f 66 f Mueller and Berger
(1967)

Accipiter 0.349 m 9.2 m Rosenfield and


D 35.2 m C T M 6m
cooperi 0.529 f 14.4 f Bielefeldt (1992)

0.150 m 9.0 m 108.0 m 99 m Newton and Marquiss


Accipiter nisus D C T R
0.280 f 18.5 f 166.0 f 65 f (1983)

Parabuteo 0.69 m
S ... 160.0 m/f C N R ... Bednarz (1995)
unicinctus 0.99 f

0.475 m
Buteo lineatus S ... 24.0 m/f C T M 4 m/f Crocoll (1994)
0.643 f

Buteo Preston and Beane


1.069 f S ... 320.0 f C T M 46 m/f
swainsoni (1993)

Aquila
3.0 m/f D 100.0 m/f 430.0 m/f C T R 24 m/f Ferrer (1993)
adalberti

Mearns and Newton


Falco 0.611 m 58.0 m 357.0 m
D C T R 11 m (1982), James et al.
peregrinus 0.952 f 83.0 f 324.0 f
(1989)

Falco 0.111 m 4.8 m 32.45 m Miller and Smallwood


D C T M 21 f
sparverius 0.120 f 5.06 f 38.79 f (1997)

5.7 m
Bonasa bonasia ... S ... H N R 9 m/f Fang and Sun (1997)
4.8 f

Dendragapus 0.492 m 0.6 m 6.0 m Hines (1986),


D H T M 88 m
canadensis 0.456 f 5.0 f 6.0 f Schroeder (1986)

Beaudette and Keppie


Dendragapus 1.188 m 0.9 m 2.6 m 24 m
D H T R (1992), Jamieson and
obscurus 0.891 f 1.4 f 11.0 f 42 f
Zwickel (1983)

Tympanuchus 0.999 m Bowman and Robel


D 1.0 m/f 10.8 m/f H N M 24 m/f
cupido 0.772 f (1977)

Meleagris 7.40 m
S ... 48.0 f O N R ... Eaton (1992)
gallopavo 4.22 f

Lagopus 0.359 m 1.0 m 7.5 m 258 m Giesen and Braun


D H T R
leucurus 0.516 f 4.0 f 29.0 f 68 f (1993)

Lagopus 0.601 m 1.0 m 4.0 m 60 m Martin and Hannon


D H T R
lagopus 0.516 f 2.9 f 7.5 f 17 f (1987)

Actitis 0.477 m
S ... 146.7 m/f C T P 5 m/f Oring et al. (1997)
macularia 0.394 f

Numenius 0.36 m Skeel and Mallory


S ... 1.63 m O N M ...
phaeopus 0.40 f (1996)

Recurvirostra 5m Robinson and Oring


0.32 m/f S ... 19.4 f O G M
americana 3f (1997)
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Charadrius 596.0 m
0.552 m/f D 11.8 m/f C T M 34 m/f Haig and Oring (1988)
melodus 146 f

Larus 0.57 m 2427


D 98.0 m/f 700 m/f O G M Gabrey (1996)
delawarensis 0.47 f m/f

Butler et al. (1980),


Larus 581
1.010 m/f D 100.0 m/f 400.0 m/f C G R Belant and Dolbeer
glaucescens m/f
(1993)

1836
Larus fuscus 0.71 m/f D 42 m/f 202 m/f C G R Paradis et al. (1998)
m/f

Columba 751
0.49m/f D 4 m/f 150 m/f O T R Paradis et al. (1998)
palumbus m/f

Zenaida 0.123 m 4.8 m Tomlinson et al.


S ... H N M ...
macroura 0.115 f 4.8 f (1960)

Belthoff and Ritchison


0.167 m (1989), VanCamp and
Otus asio D 10.7 m/f 158.5 m/f C N R 17 m/f
0.194 f Henny (1975),
Gehlbach (1986)

0.454 m
Strix aluco D 4.0 m/f 22.4 m/f C T R 9 m/f Southern (1970)
0.478 f

0.79 m Bull and Duncan


Strix nebulosa S ... 50.0 m/f C N R 21 m/f
1.16 f (1993)

Gutiérrez et al. (1985),


Strix 0.582 m
D 29.0 m/f 87.4 m/f C T R 11 m/f Miller and Meslow
occidentalis 0.637 f
(1985)

Speotyto
0.150 m/f S ... 30.0 m/f C G M ... Haug et al. (1993)
cunicularia

Wallin and Andersson


(1981), Löfgren et al.
23 m/f
Aegolius 0.101 m 12.8 m 67.5 m (1986), Korpimäki
D C N R 13 m
funereus 0.167 f 56.0 f 300.0 f (1987), Korpimäki
37 f
and Lagerström
(1988)

Bubo 1.154 m 111 Adamcik and Keith


S ... 1305.0 m/f C T R
virginianus 1.555 f m/f (1978)

Picoides
0.044 m/f S ... 90.0 m/f O T M ... Jackson (1994)
borealis

Dryocopus 0.31 m Bull and Jackson


S ... 32.0 m/f O T R 8 m/f
pileatus 0.27 f (1995)

Hirundo fulva 0.020 m/f S ... 30.0 m/f C G M ... West (1995)

Allen and Nice


Hirundo rustica 0.19 m/f D 6.4 m/f 8.1 m/f C G R 7 m/f
(1952), Shields (1982)

Iridoprocne
0.02 m/f D 1.0 m/f 6.4 m/f O N M 41 m/f Chapman (1955)
bicolor
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

3046
Riparia riparia 0.015 m/f D 6.0 m/f 144.0 m/f C N M Mead (1979)
m/f

Progne subis 0.049 m/f D 40 m/f 336.0 m/f O G M 46 m/f Allen and Nice (1952)

Delichon
0.020 m/f D 0.03 m/f 4.3 m/f C N M 49 m/f Rheinwald (1975)
urbica

Aphelocoma 0.7 m 1.1 m 38 m Woolfenden and


0.080 m/f D H N R
coerulescens 0.3 f 1.8 f 32 f Fitzpatrick (1978)

Malurus 0.20 m Russell and Rowley


0.010 m/f D 2.40 m/f C G R 94 f
splendens 0.20 f (1993)

Gymnorhinus 1064 Marzluff and Balda


0.103 m/f S ... 640.0 m/f O N R
cyanocephalus m/f (1989)

Sayornis 0.02 m 34.4 m 33 m


S ... C T R Wolf (1997)
nigricans 0.018 f 20.0 f 30 f

Perisoreus 0.0 m
0.071 m/f D 11.3 m/f O T R 15 m/f Strickland (1991)
canadensis 2.8 f

0.189 m 0.35 m 1.3 m


Pica pica D O N R ... Eden (1987)
0.166 f 0.5 f 0.8 f

Parus 58 m Weise and Meyer


0.011 m/f D 1.1 f 11.2 f O N R
atricapillus 45 f (1979)

Berndt and Sternberg


(1968), Greenwood et
0.02 m 0.6 m 3.3 m 92 m
Parus major D O T R al. (1979), Weise and
0.018 f 0.9 f 3.3 f 98 f
Meyer (1979), Nilsson
(1989)

1.1 m 4.6 m 65 m
Parus palustris 0.014 m/f D O T R Nilsson (1989)
2.6 f 7.3 f 53 f

Parus Berndt and Sternberg


0.011 m/f D 0.7 m/f 470.0 m/f O T R 72 f
caeruleus (1968)

Berndt and Sternberg


1.1 m 29 m
Sitta europea 0.022 m/f D ... O T M (1968), Mattysen and
0.8 f 26 f
Schmidt (1987)

Vireo griseus 0.011 m/f S ... 20.0 m/f C T M ... Hopp et al. (1995)

Lanius Collister and De Smet


0.047 m/f D 7.3 m/f 79.7 m/f O T M 69 m/f
ludovicianus (1997)

Allen and Nice


74 m
Turdus merula 0.095 m/f D 5.0 m/f 355.0 m/f O T R (1952), Greenwood
50 f
and Harvey (1977)

2204
Turdus merula 0.095 m/f D 4.0 m/f 355 m/f O T R Paradis et al. (1998)
m/f

Toxostoma
0.079 m/f S ... 30.0 m/f C T R ... Tweit (1996)
curvirostre

Hylocichla 0.044 m 680


S ... 4.68 m/f C T M Anders et al. (1998)
mustelina 0.050 f m/f
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Acrocephalus
0.012 m/f D 51 m/f 271 m/f C N M 78 m/f Paradis et al. (1998)
scirpaceus

Motacilla alba 0.24 m/f S ... 100.0 m/f C N M ... Badyaev et al. (1996)

Seiurus 0.020 m
S ... 4.0 m/f C T M ... Robinson (1995)
motacilla 0.021 f

Passerculus 0.248 f 1.4 f 65 m Wheelwright and


0.019 m/f D H N M
sandwichensis 0.202 m 1.6 m 76 f Mauck (1998)

Nice (1937), Johnston


Melospiza 0.20 m 13.2 m 36 m/f (1956), Halliburton
0.021 m/f D H T R
melodia 0.20 f 1.3 f 34 m/f and Mewaldt (1976),
Arcese (1989)

Zonotrichia 0.3 m 138 m Baker and Mewaldt


0.294 m/f D 2.6 m/f O T M
leucophrys 0.4 f 173 f (1978)

Passerina
0.108 m/f S ... 470.0 m/f O N M ... Payne (1991)
cyanea

Carduelis 7.0 m 150.0 m 15 m Greenwood and


0.03 m/f D H T R
chloris 3.0 f 37.5 f 11 f Harvey (1977)

Ficedula 473 Berndt and Sternberg


0.015 m/f D 0.9 m/f 75.0 m/f O T M
hypoleuca m/f (1968), Pärt (1990)

Quiscalus 0.21 m 35.0 m


S ... O G R ... Post et al. (1996)
major 0.12 f 7.0 f

0.05 m
Molothrus ater S ... 40.0 m/f O N M 15 m/f Lowther (1993)
0.04 f

Table A2. Mammals

Natal Natal
Body
Obs dispersal dispersal
Species mass 1 2 n Source
type median maximum
(kg)
distance (km) distance (km)

Didelphis 2.839 m 4.3 m


S ... O T 8 VanDruff (1969)
virginianus 1.99 f 5.152 f

Phascogale 0.199 m Soderquist and Lill


S ... 6.8 m C T ...
tapotafa 0.145 f (1995)

Trichosurus
2.93 m/f D 5.4 m 12.8 m C T 13 m Cowan et al. (1996)
vulpecula

Hanski et al. (1991),


29 m
Sorex araneus 0.004 m/f S ... 0.869 m/f C N Peltonen and Hanski
21 f
(1991)

Scapanus 0.722 m 44
0.148 m/f S ... O G Carraway et al. (1993)
townsendii 0.856 f m/f

Ursus 130.0 m 225.0 m 19 m


S ... O T Rogers (1987)
americanus 78.9 f 28.8 f 15 f
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

134.0 m 35 m Glenn and Miller


Ursus arctos 331.0 m/f S ... O T
82.0 f 26 f (1980)

222
5.5 m
Ursus arctos 331.0 m/f D ... O T m Pearson (1972)
9.5 f
343 f

8.1 m 34 Priewert (1951),


Procyon lotor S ... 265.5 m/f O T
7.5 f m/f Lynch (1967)

Martes 15
1.04 m/f S ... 61.0 m/f C T Lofroth (1993)
americana m/f

Martes 3.70 m 23.0 m 35


S ... C T Arthur et al. (1993)
pennanti 2.10 f 22.6 f m/f

Mustela 0.080 m 5.6 m 36 m


S ... C T Erlinge (1977)
erminea 0.054 f 1.0 f 29 f

1.225 m 45.1 m 6m
Mustela vison S ... C T Mitchell (1961)
1.112 f 45.0 f 2f

Lutra 39 Melquist and


9.07 m S ... 42.0 m C T
canadensis m/f Hornocker (1983)

Pasitschniak-Arts and
Gulo gulo 14.4 m/f S ... 300 m/f C T ...
Lariviere (1995)

7.80 m 110.0 m 1m
Taxidea taxus S ... C T Lindzey (1978)
6.15 f 52.0 f 1f

11.6 m 8.3 m
Meles meles S ... O G ... Cheesman et al. (1988)
10.1 f 7.8 f

Mephitis 3.176 m 10.1 m 1m


S ... O T Bjorge et al. (1981)
mephitis 2.642 f 21.7 f 4f

Helogale 0.25 m 44 m
0.350 m/f D ... C T Rood (1987)
parvula 0.75 f 22 f

176.0 m Andrews and Boggess


Canis latrans 14.06 m/f S ... C N 125
232.2 f (1978)

16.6 m 7m
Canis latrans 14.06 m/f D ... C N Bowen (1982)
42.2 f 5f

42.54 m 432.0 m 5m Berg and Kuehn


Canis lupus S ... C T
40.22 f 79 f 2f (1982), Mech (1987)

42.54 m 125.0 m 9m
Canis lupus D ... C T Ballard et al. (1987)
40.22 f 125.0 f 5f

42.54 m 225.0 m 40 m
Canis lupus D ... C T Gese and Mech (1991)
40.22 f 75.0 f 29 f

394.5 m 1m Ables (1965), Allen


Vulpes vulpes 5.20 m/f S ... C T
302.0 f 72 f and Sargent (1993)

8.6 m 31 m
Vulpes vulpes 5.20 m/f D ... C T Jensen (1973)
4.9 f 31 f
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

298
24.2 m
Vulpes vulpes 5.20 m/f D ... C T m Storm et al. (1976)
8.1 f
207 f

171
8.1 m
Vulpes vulpes 5.20 m/f D ... C T m Phillips et al. (1972)
8.1 f
124 f

12.0 m 16 m
Vulpes vulpes 5.20 m/f D ... C T Zimen (1984)
12.4 f 4f

Urocyon
4.1 m
cinereoargente S ... 83.68 f C T ... Sullivan (1956)
3.9 f
us

Logan et al. (1986),


61.6 m 274.0 m 33 m
Felis concolor S ... C T Ross and Jalotsky
44.5 f 155.0 f 13 f
(1992)

Lynx lynx 10.149 f S ... 9.7 f C T 1 Saunders (1963)

Robinson and Grant


Lynx rufus 8.2 f S ... 56.0 f C T 10
(1958)

Lariviere and Walton


Lynx rufus 9.6 m S ... 182 m C T ...
(1997)

Castor 19
24.3 f S 2.1 m/f 40.6 m/f H T Leege (1963)
canadensis m/f

Armitage (1974),
Marmota
5.448 m S ... 1.4 m H G 2 Armitage and
flaviventris
Downhower (1974)

Marmota 3.511 m 0.685 m 46


S ... H N Swihart (1992)
monax 3.486 f 0.768 f m/f

Spermophilus 0.05 m 0.975 m 26 m Mitchener and


0.405 m/f D H G
richardsonii 0.05 f 0.525 f 26 f Mitchener (1977)

Spermophilus 1.44 m 56 m Evans and Holdenried


0.475 m/f S ... H G
beecheyi 1.312 f 25 f (1943)

Spermophilus 25 m
0.400 m/f S ... 0.328 m/f H G Holekamp (1984)
beldingi 2f

Spermophilus 0.515 m 65 m Allred and Beck


0.192 m/f S ... H N
leucopus 0.321 f 83 f (1963)

Spermophilus
46 m
tridecemliniatu 0.140m S ... 0.239 m H T Rongstad (1965)
45 f
s

Sciurus niger 0.790 m/f S ... 3.37 m/f H T 2 Fitch (1958)

Tamiasciurus 0.204 m 0.485 m 0.60 m


D H T 8 m/f Sun (1997)
hudsonicus 0.199 f 0.24 f 0.60 f

Thomomys 2m Daly and Patton


0.150 m/f S ... 0.30 m/f C T
bottae 1f (1990)
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Perognathus 28 m Allred and Beck


0.023 m S ... 0.853 H T
formosa 18 f (1963)

Dipodomys 0.044 m 0.13 m 0.25 m 13 m


D H T Jones (1989)
merriami 0.0426 f 0.03 f 0.158 f 13 f

159
Dipodomys 0.411 m Allred and Beck
0.065 m/f S ... H T m
microps 0.434 f (1963)
172 f

Dipodomys 0.9 m
0.115 m/f S ... H T ... Jones (1987)
spectabilis 2.0 f

Dipodomys 0.03 m 82 m Waser and Elliott


0.115 m/f D ... H T
spectabilis 0.03 f 84 f (1991)

Dipodomys 0.03 m 22 m
0.115 m/f D ... H T Jones (1987)
spectabilis 0.03 f 18 f

Dipodomys 0.087 m 13 m
0.064 m/f D ... H T Price et al. (1994)
stephensi 0.045 f 18 f

Neotoma 0.42 m
S ... 2.2 m H T ... Smith (1997)
cinerea 0.30 f

Clethrionomys 58 Dickman and


0.023 m/f S ... 0.40 m/f H T
glareolus m/f Doncaster (1989)

Onychomys 0.329 m 1m Allred and Beck


26.8 m/f S ... H T
torridus 0.325 f 1f (1963)

Microtus 0.03 m 0.159 m 39 m


0.023 m/f D H T Sandell et al. (1990)
agrestis 0.03 f 0.193 f 19 f

Microtus Boyce and Boyce


0.022 f D 0.03 f 0.2 f H N 31 f
arvalis (1988)

Microtus 0.028 m 0.136 m 79 m


S ... H T McGuire et al. (1993)
ochrogaster 0.033 f 0.127 f 78 f

Microtus
0.032 m/f D 0.75 m 1.0 m H T 5m Steen (1994)
oeconomus

Microtus
0.040 m/f S ... 0.245 m/f H T 1 Mihok et al. (1988)
pennsylvanicus

Microtus 0.008 m 0.068 m 52 m


0.032 m/f D H T Lambin (1994)
townsendi 0.003 f 0.054 f 300 f

Microtus Wolff and Lidicker


135 m/f S ... 0.3 m/f H G ...
xanthognathus (1980)

Peromyscus 0.06 m 0.45 m 27 m


0.018 m/f D H T Ribble (1992)
californicus 0.15 f 0.791 f 24 f

Peromyscus 0.228 m 1m Allred and Beck


0.008 m/f S ... H T
longicaudus 0.235 f 1f (1963)

Peromyscus 0.05 m 0.883 m 71 m Dice and Howard


0.015 m/f D O T
maniculatus 0.15 f 1.005 f 64 f (1951)
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Apodemus 261 Dickman and


0.020 m/f S ... 0.5 m/f H N
sylvaticus m/f Doncaster (1989)

Ondatra
1.048 m/f S ... 3.37 m/f H T ... Fitch (1958)
zibethicus

Ochotona 15 Peacock and Smith


... D 0.09 m/f 0.396 m/f H G
princeps m/f (1997)

Lepus
1.43 m/f S ... 20.1 m/f H T 3 O’Farrell (1965)
americanus

Broekhuisen and
Lepus europea 4.50 m/f S ... 9.0 m/f H T 11
Maaskamp (1982)

99 Angerbjörn and Flux


Lepus timidus 2.750 m/f S 3.0 m/f 200 m/f H N
m/f (1995)

Lepus
2.2 m/f S ... 45 m/f H N ... Best (1996)
californicus

Sylvilagus 148
0.841 m/f S ... 0.352 m/f H N Shields (1960)
bachmani m/f

Sylvilagus 1.10 m 3.9 m 65 Chapman and


S ... H T
floridanus 1.20 f 2.3 f m/f Trethewey (1972)

Odocoileus
64.638 4.0 m 15.2 m 26 m Bunnell and Harestad
hemionus D H G
m/f 2.0 f 12.2 f 14 f (1983)
columbianus

Odocoileus
82.5 m 7.34 m 65 m
hemionus S ... H G Robinette (1966)
51.75 f 8.22 f 33 f
hemionus

Odocoileus Nelson and Mech


86.41 f S ... 11.74 f H G 7f
virginianus (1992)

Odocoileus 6.0 m 26 m
86.41 f D ... H G Nelson (1993)
virginianus 24.0 f 4f

481.83
Alces alces S ... 118.0 H T 87 Pullianen (1974)
m/f

Cervus 315 m
S ... 18.5 m/f H T 5 m/f Brazda (1953)
canadensis 225 f

LITERATURE CITED American Midland Naturalist 47: 606-645.

Allen, S. H., and A. B. Sargent. 1993. Dispersal patterns of


Ables, E. D. 1965. An exceptional fox movement. Journal
red foxes relative to population density. Journal of Wildlife
of Mammalogy 46: 102.
Management 57: 526-533.
Adamcik, R. S., and L. B. Keith. 1978. Regional
Allred, D. M., and D. E. Beck. 1963. Range of movement
movements and mortality of great horned owls in relation to
and dispersal of some rodents at the Nevada atomic test site.
snowshoe hare fluctuations. Canadian Field-Naturalist 92:
Journal of Mammalogy 44: 190-200.
228-234.
Anders, A. D., J. Faaborg, and F. Thompson III. 1998.
Allen, R. W., and M. M. Nice. 1952. A study of the
Postfledging dispersal, habitat use, and home-range size of
breeding biology of the purple martin (Progne subis).
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

juvenile wood thrushes. Auk 115: 349-358. Belthoff, J. R., and G. Ritchison. 1989. Natal dispersal of
Eastern Screech-Owls. Condor 91: 254-265.
Andrews, R. D., and E. K. Boggess. 1978. Ecology of
coyotes in Iowa. Pages 249-265 in M. Bekoff, editor. Bengtsson, B. O. 1978. Avoiding inbreeding, at what cost?
Coyotes: biology, behavior, and management. Academic Journal of Theoretical Biology 73: 439-444.
Press, New York, New York, USA.
Berg, W. E., and D. W. Kuehn. 1982. Ecology of wolves
Angerbjörn, A., and J. E. C. Flux. 1995. Lepus timidus. in north-central Minnesota. Pages 4-11 in F. C. Harrington
Mammalian Species 495: 1-11. and P. C. Paquet, editors. Wolves of the world: perspectives
of behavior, ecology and conservation. Noyes, Park Ridge,
Arcese, P. 1989. Intrasexual competition, mating system, New Jersey, USA.
and natal dispersal in song sparrows. Animal Behaviour 38:
958-979. Berndt, R., and H. Sternberg. 1968. Terms, studies and
experiments on the problems of bird dispersion. Ibis 110:
Armitage, K. B. 1974. Male behaviour and territoriality in 256-269.
the yellow-bellied marmot. Journal of Zoology (London)
172: 233-265. Best, T. L. 1996. Lepus californicus. Mammalian Species
530: 1-10.
Armitage, K. B., and J. F. Downhower. 1974.
Demography of yellow-bellied marmot populations. Bjorge, R. R., J. B.Gunson, and W. M. Samuel. 1981.
Ecology 55: 1233-1245. Population characteristics and movements of striped skunks
(Mephitis mephitis) in Central Alberta. Canadian Field-
Arthur, S. M., T. F. Paragi, and W. B. Krohn. 1993. Naturalist 95: 149-155.
Dispersal of juvenile fishers in Maine. Journal of Wildlife
Management 57: 868-874. Bowen, W. D. 1982. Home range and spatial organization
of coyotes in Jasper National Park, Alberta. Journal of
Badyaev, A. V., D. D. Gibson, and B. Kessel. 1996. White Wildlife Management 46: 201-216.
Wagtail, Montacilla alba. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors.
The Birds of North America. Number 236/237. The Birds of Bowman, T. J., and R. J. Robel. 1977. Brood break-up,
North America, Inc., Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA. dispersal, mobility, and mortality of juvenile prairie
chickens. Journal of Wildlife Management 41: 27-34.
Baker, M. C., and L. R. Mewaldt. 1978. Song dialects as
barriers to dispersal in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia Boyce, C. C. K., and J. L. Boyce III. 1988. Population
leucophrys nuttalli. Evolution 32: 712-722. biology of Microtus arvalis. II. Natal and breeding dispersal
of females. Journal of Animal Ecology 57: 723-736.
Baker, R. R. 1993. The function of post-fledging
exploration: a pilot study of three species of passerines Brazda, A. R. 1953. Elk migration patterns and some of the
ringed in Britain. Ornis Scandinavica 24: 71-79. factors affecting movements in the Gallatin River Drainage,
Montana. Journal of Wildlife Management 17: 9-23.
Ballard, W. B., J. S. Whitman, and C. L. Gardner. 1987.
Ecology of an exploited wolf population in south-central Broekhuizen, S., and F. Maaskamp. 1982. Movement,
Alaska. Wildlife Monographs 98: 1-54. home range, and clustering in the European hare. Zeitschrift
fur Saugertierkunde 47: 22-32.
Beaudette, P. D., and D. M. Keppie. 1992. Survival of
dispersing spruce grouse. Canadian Journal of Zoology 70: Brooker, L., M. Brooker, and P. Cale. 1999. Animal
693-697. dispersal in fragmented habitat: measuring habitat
connectivity, corridor use, and dispersal mortality.
Bednarz, J. C. 1995. Harris' Hawk, Parabuteo unicinctus. Conservation Ecology 3(1): 4. [online] URL:
In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. http://www.consecol.org/vol3/iss1/art4
Number 146. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union, Brown, J. H., and B. A. Maurer. 1989. Macroecology: the
Washington, D.C., USA. division of food and space among species on continents.
Science 243: 1145-1150.
Beissinger, S. R., and M. I. Westphal. 1998. On the use of
demographic models of population viability in endangered Bull, E. L., and J. R. Duncan. 1993. Great Gray Owl, Strix
species management. Journal of Wildlife Management 62: nebulosa. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of
821-841. North America. Number 41. Academy of Natural Sciences,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Belant, J. L., and R. A. Dolbeer. 1993. Migration and Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
dispersal of Laughing Gulls in the United States. Journal of
Field Ornithology 64: 557-565. Bull, E. L., and J. A. Jackson. 1995. Pileated Woodpecker,
Dryocopus pileatus. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Birds of North America. Number 148. Academy of Natural & Brooks, Pacific Grove, California, USA.
Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA. Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1988. Reproductive success. Pages
472-485 in Clutton-Brock, T. H., editor. Studies of
Bunnell, F. L. 1999. What habitat is an island? Pages 1-31 individual variation in contrasting breeding systems.
in J. A. Rochelle, L. A. Lehmann, and J. Wisniewski, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.
editors. Forest fragmentation: wildlife and management
implications. Brill, Boston, Massachusetts, USA. Collister, D. M., and K. De Smet. 1997. Breeding and
natal dispersal in the Loggerhead Shrike. Journal of Field
Bunnell, F. L., and A. S. Harestad. 1983. Dispersal and Ornithology 68: 273-282.
dispersion of black-tailed deer: models and observations.
Journal of Mammalogy 64: 201-209. Cook, R. D. 1977. Detection of influential observation in
linear regression. Technometrics 19: 15-18.
Bunnell, F. L., and D. J. Huggard. 1999. Biodiversity
across spatial and temporal scales: problems and Cowan, P. E., R. E. Brockie, G. D. Ward, and M. G.
opportunities. Forest Ecology and Management 115: 113- Efford. 1996. Long-distance movements of juvenile
126. brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) on farmland,
Hawke’s Bay, New Zealand. Wildlife Research 23: 237-244.
Bunnell, F. L., and L. L. Kremsater. 1990. Sustaining
wildlife in managed forests. Northwest Environmental Crocoll, S. T. 1994. Red-shouldered Hawk, Buteo lineatus.
Journal 6: 243-269. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America.
Number 107. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
Butler, R. W., N. A. M. Verbeek, and R. G. Foottit. 1980. Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Mortality and dispersal of the glaucous-winged gulls of Washington, D.C., USA.
southern British Columbia. Canadian Field-Naturalist 94:
315-320. Daly, J. C., and J. L. Patton. 1990. Dispersal, gene flow,
and allelic diversity between local populations of Thomomys
Caley, J. M. 1991. A null model for testing distributions of bottae pocket gophers in the coastal ranges of California.
dispersal distances. American Naturalist 138: 524-532. Evolution 44: 1283-1294.

Carraway, L. N., L. F. Alexander, and B. J. Verts. 1993. Dice, L. R., and W. E. Howard. 1951. Distance of
Scapanus townsendii. Mammalian Species 434: 1-7. dispersal by prairie deermice from birthplaces to breeding
sites. University of Michigan, Contributions from the
Cates, S. E., and J. L. Gittleman. 1997. Reading between Laboratory of Vertebrate Biology 50: 1-13.
the lines—is allometric scaling useful? Trends in Ecology
and Evolution 12: 338-339. Dickman, C. R., and C. P. Doncaster. 1989. The ecology
of small mammals in urban habitats. II. Demography and
Chapman, J. A., and D. E. C. Trethewey. 1972. dispersal. Journal of Animal Ecology 58: 119-127.
Movements within a population of introduced eastern
cottontail rabbits. Journal of Wildlife Management 36: 155- Doak, D. F., P. C. Marino, and P. M. Karieva. 1992.
158. Spatial scale mediates the influence of habitat fragmentation
on dispersal success: implications for conservation.
Chapman, L. B. 1955. Studies of a tree swallow colony. Theoretical Population Biology 41: 315-336.
Bird-Banding 26: 45-70.
Dobson, F. S. 1982. Competition for mates and
Cheesman, C. L., W. J. Creswell, S. Harris, and P. J. predominant juvenile male dispersal in mammals. Animal
Mallinson. 1988. Comparison of dispersal and other Behaviour 30: 1183-1192.
movements in two badger (Meles meles) populations.
Mammal Review 18: 51-59. Dobson, F. S., and W. T. Jones. 1985. Multiple causes of
dispersal. American Naturalist 126: 855-858.
Ciaranca, M. A., C. C. Allin, and G. S. Jones. 1997. Mute
Swan, Cygnus olor. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Dow, H., and S. Fredga. 1983. Breeding and natal
Birds of North America. Number 273. Academy of Natural dispersal of the goldeneye, Bucephala clangula. Journal of
Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American Animal Ecology 52: 681-695.
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Dugger, B. D., K. M. Dugger, and L. H. Fredrickson.
Clarke, A. L., B.-E. Saether, and E. Roskaft. 1997. Sex 1994. Hooded Merganser, Lophodytes cucullatus. In A.
biases in avian dispersal: a reappraisal. Oikos 79: 429-438. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America.
Number 98. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
Cleveland, W. S., E. Grosse, and W. M. Shyu. 1992. Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Local regression models. Pages 309-376 in J. M. Chambers Washington, D.C., USA.
and T. J. Hastie, editors. Statistical models in S. Wadsworth
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Dunk, J. R. 1995. White-tailed Kite, Elanus leucurus. In A. 111-139 in K. A. Kohm and J. F. Franklin, editors. Creating
Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. a forestry for the 21st century. Island Press, Washington,
Number 178. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, D.C., USA.
Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Washington, D.C., USA. Gabrey, S. W. 1996. Migration and dispersal in Great
Lakes Ring-billed and Herring Gulls. Journal of Field
Dunning, J. B. 1993. CRC handbook of avian body masses. Ornithology 67: 327-339.
CRC Press, Boca Raton, Florida, USA.
Gauthier, G. 1993. Bufflehead, Bucephala albeola. In A.
Eaton, S. W. 1992. Wild Turkey, Meleagris gallopavo. In Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America.
A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. Number 67. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
Number 22. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Washington, D.C., USA.
Gehlbach, F. R. 1986. Odd couples of suburbia. Natural
Eden, S. F. 1987. Natal philopatry of the magpie Pica pica. History 6: 56-66.
Ibis 129: 477-490.
Gese, E. M., and L. D. Mech. 1991. Dispersal of wolves
Efron, B., and R. J. Tibshirani. 1993. An introduction to (Canis lupus) in northeastern Minnesota, 1969-1989.
the bootstrap. Chapman and Hall, San Francisco, California, Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 2946-2955.
USA.
Giesen, K. M., and C. E. Braun. 1993. Natal dispersal and
Ehrlich, P. R., D. S. Dobkin, and D. Wheye. 1988. The recruitment of juvenile White-tailed Ptarmigan in Colorado.
birder's handbook: a field guide to the natural history of Journal of Wildlife Management 57: 72-77.
North American birds. Simon and Schuster, New York,
New York, USA. Glenn, L. P., and L. H. Miller. 1980. Seasonal movements
of an Alaska peninsula brown bear population. Pages 307-
Erlinge, S. 1977. Spacing strategy in stoat Mustela erminea. 312 in Fourth International Conference on Bear Research
Oikos 28: 32-42. and Management (Kalispell, Montana, USA, 1977). The
Bear Biology Association and the Bureau of Land
Evans, F. C., and R. Holdenried. 1943. A population study Management, Tonto, Arizona, USA.
of the Beechey ground squirrel in central California. Journal
of Mammalogy 24: 231-260. Greenwood, P. J. 1980. Mating systems, philopatry, and
dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour 28:
Fahrig, L. 1997. Relative effects of habitat loss and 1140-1162.
fragmentation on population extinction. Journal of Wildlife
Management 61: 603-610. Greenwood, P. J., and P. J. Harvey. 1977. Feeding
strategies and dispersal of territorial passerines: a
Fahrig, L., and H. G. Merriam. 1985. Habitat patch comparative study of the blackbird Turdus merula and the
connectivity and population survival. Ecology 66: 1762- greenfinch Charduelis chloris. Ibis 119: 528-531.
1768.
Greenwood, P. J., and P. J. Harvey. 1982. The natal and
Fang, Y., and Y.-H. Sun. 1997. Brood movement and natal breeding dispersal of birds. Annual Review of Ecology and
dispersal of hazel grouse Bonasa bonasia at Changbai Systematics 13: 1-21.
Mountain, Jilin Province, China. Wildlife Biology 3: 261-
264. Greenwood, P. J., P. J. Harvey, and C. M. Perrins. 1979.
The role of dispersal in the great tit (Parus major): the
Felsenstein, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative causes, consequences and heritability of natal dispersal.
method. American Naturalist 125: 1-15. Journal of Animal Ecology 48: 123-142.
Ferrer, M. 1993. Ontogeny of dispersal distances in young Gutiérrez, R. J., A. B. Franklin, W. Lahaye, V. J .
Spanish Imperial Eagles. Behavioral Ecology and Meretsky, and J. P. Ward. 1985. Juvenile spotted owl
Sociobiology 32: 259-263. dispersal in northwestern California: preliminary results.
Pages 60-65 in R. J. Gutiérrez, and A. B. Carey, editors.
Fitch, H. S. 1958. Home ranges, territories, and seasonal Ecology and management of the spotted owl in the Pacific
movements of vertebrates of the Natural History
Northwest. U.S. Forest Service General Technical Report
Reservation. University of Kansas Publications, Museum of PNW-GTR-185.
Natural History 11: 63-326.
Haig, S. M., and L. W. Oring. 1988. Distribution and
Franklin, J. F., D. R. Berg, D. A. Thornburgh, and J. C. dispersal in the Piping Plover. Auk 105: 630-638.
Tappeiner. 1997. Alternative silvicultural approaches to
timber harvesting: variable retention harvest systems. Pages Halliburton, R., and L. R. Mewaldt. 1976. Survival and
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

mobility in a population of Pacific Coast Song Sparrows Sociobiology 16: 21-30.


(Melospiza melodia gouldii). Condor 78: 499-504.
Holling, C. S. 1992. Cross-scale morphology, geometry,
Hansen, A. J., T. A. Spies, F. J. Swanson, and J. L. and dynamics of ecosystems. Ecological Monographs 62:
Ohmann. 1991. Conserving biodiversity in managed 447-502.
forests. BioScience 41: 382-392.
Hopp, S .L., A. Kirby, and C. A. Boone. 1995. White-eyed
Hanski, I., A. Peltonen, and L. Kaski. 1991. Natal Vireo, Vireo griseus. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The
dispersal and social dominance in the common shrew Sorex Birds of North America. Number 168. Academy of Natural
araneus. Oikos 62: 48-58. Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Harestad, A. S., and F. L. Bunnell. 1979. Home range and
body weight—a re-evaluation. Ecology 60: 389-402. Howard, W. E. 1960. Innate and environmental dispersal of
individual vertebrates. American Midland Naturalist 63:
Harris, L. D. 1984. The fragmented forest. University of 152-161.
Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.
Ibrahim, K. M., R. A. Nichols, and G. M. Hewitt. 1996.
Haug, E. A., B. A. Millsap, and M. S. Martell. 1993. Spatial patterns of genetic variation generated by different
Burrowing Owl, Speotyto cunicularia. In A. Poole and F. forms of dispersal during range expansion. Heredity 77:
Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. Number 61. 282-291.
Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania,
USA, and American Ornithologists' Union, Washington, Jackson, J. A. 1994. Red-cockaded Woodpecker, Picoides
D.C., USA. borealis. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North
America. Number 85. Academy of Natural Sciences,
Hedrick, P. H. 1996. Genetics of metapopulations: aspects Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
of a comprehensive perspective. Pages 29-51 in D. R. Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
McCullough, editor. Metapopulations and wildlife
conservation. Island Press, Washington, D.C., USA. James, P. C., I. G. Warkentin, and L. W. Oliphant. 1989.
Turnover and dispersal in urban merlins Falco columbarius.
Henein, K., and G. Merriam. 1990. The elements of Ibis 131: 425-447.
connectivity where corridor quality is variable. Landscape
Ecology 4: 157-170. Jamieson, I. G., and F. C. Zwickel. 1983. Dispersal and
site fidelity in blue grouse. Canadian Journal of Zoology
Henein, K., J. Wegner, and G. Merriam. 1998. 61: 570-573.
Population effects of landscape model manipulation on two
behaviourally different woodland small mammals. Oikos Jensen, B. 1973. Movements of the red fox (Vulpes vulpes
81:168-186. L.) in Denmark investigated by marking and recovery.
Danish Review of Game Biology 8: 1-20.
Hengeveld, R. 1994. Small step invasion research. Trends
in Ecology and Evolution 9: 339-342. Johnson, R. F. 1956. Population structure in salt marsh
song sparrows. Part I. Environment and annual cycle.
Hepp, G. R., and F. C. Bellrose. 1995. Wood Duck, Aix Condor 58: 24-44.
sponsa. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North
America. Number 169. Academy of Natural Sciences, Jones, W. T. 1987. Dispersal patterns in kangaroo rats
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American (Dipodomys spectabilis). Pages 119-127 in B. D. Chepko-
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA. Sade and Z. T. Halpin, editors. Mammalian dispersal
patterns: the effects of social structure on population
Hines, J. E. 1986. Survival and reproduction of dispersing genetics. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois,
Blue Grouse. Condor 88: 43-49. USA.
Hobson, K. A. 1997. Pelagic Cormorant, Phalacrocorax Jones, W. T. 1989. Dispersal distance and the range of
pelagicus. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of nightly movements in Merriam's kangaroo rats. Journal of
North America. Number 282. Academy of Natural Sciences, Mammalogy 70: 27-34.
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA. Koenig, W. D., D. V. Vuren, and P. N. Hooge. 1996.
Detectability, philopatry, and the distribution of dispersal
Hogg, R. V., and E. A. Tanis. 1997. Probability and distances in vertebrates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution
statistical inference. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, 11: 514-517.
New Jersey, USA.
Korpimäki, E. 1987. Selection for nest-hole shift and
Holekamp, K. E. 1984. Natal dispersal in Belding's ground tactics of breeding dispersal in Tengmalm's owl Aegolius
squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). Behavioral Ecology and funereus. Journal of Animal Ecology 56: 185-196.
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Korpimäki, E., and M. Lagerström. 1988. Survival and Lofroth, E. C. 1993. Scale dependent analyses of habitat
natal dispersal of fledglings of Tengmalm's owl in relation selection by marten in the sub-boreal spruce biogeoclimatic
to fluctuating food conditions and hatching date. Journal of zone, British Columbia. Thesis. Simon Fraser University,
Animal Ecology 57: 433-441. Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada.

Kot, M., M. A. Lewis, and P. van den Driessche. 1996. Logan, K. A., L. L. Irwin, and R. Skinner. 1986.
Dispersal data and the spread of invading organisms. Characteristics of a hunted mountain lion population in
Ecology 77: 2027-2042. Wyoming. Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 648-654.

Kozlowski, J., and J. Weiner. 1997. Interspecific Lowther, P. E. 1993. Brown-headed Cowbird, Molothrus
allometries are by-products of body size optimization. ater. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North
American Naturalist 149: 352-380. America. Number 47. Academy of Natural Sciences,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Kunkele, J., and D. von Holst. 1996. Natal dispersal in the Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
European wild rabbit. Animal Behaviour 51: 1047-1059.
Lynch, G. M. 1967. Long-range movement of a raccoon in
Kushlan, J. A., and K. L. Bildstein. 1992. White Ibis, Manitoba. Journal of Mammalogy 48: 659-660.
Eudocimus albus. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds
of North America. Number 9. Academy of Natural Sciences, Mace, G. M., P. H. Harvey, and T. H. Clutton-Brock.
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American 1982. Vertebrate home-range size and energetic
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA. requirements. Pages 32-53 in I. R. Swingland and P. J.
Greenwood, editors. The ecology of animal movement.
LaBarbera, M. 1989. Analyzing body size as a factor in Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
ecology and evolution. Annual Review of Ecology and
Systematics 20: 97-117. Marcström, V., and R. Kenward. 1981. Movements of
wintering goshawks in Sweden. Viltrevy 12: 1-36.
Lamberson, R. H., B. R. Noon, C. Voss, and K.
McKelvey. 1994. Reserve design for territorial species: the Martin, K. 1998. The role of animal behavior studies in
effects of patch size and spacing on the viability of the wildlife science and management. Wildlife Society Bulletin
Northern Spotted Owl. Conservation Biology 8: 185-195. 26: 911-920.

Lambin, X. 1994. Natal philopatry, competition for Martin, K., and S. J. Hannon. 1987. Natal philopatry and
resources, and inbreeding avoidance in Townsend's voles recruitment of willow ptarmigan in north central and
(Microtus townsendii). Ecology 75: 224-235. northwestern Canada. Oecologia 71: 518-524.

Lariviere, S., and L. R. Walton. 1997. Lynx rufus. Marzluff, J. M., and R. P. Balda. 1989. Causes and
Mammalian Species 563: 1-8. consequences of female-biased dispersal in a flock-living
bird, the Pinyon Jay. Ecology 70: 316-328.
Leege, T. A. 1963. Natural movements of beavers in
southeastern Idaho. Journal of Wildlife Management 32: Mathsoft, Inc. 1998. SPLUS 4 Guide to Statistics.
973-976. Mathsoft, Seattle, Washington, USA.

Lehmkuhl, J. F., and L. F. Ruggiero. 1991. Forest Mattysen, E., and K.-H. Schmidt. 1987. Natal dispersal in
fragmentation in the Pacific Northwest and its potential the nuthatch. Ornis Scandinavica 18: 313-316.
effects on wildlife. Pages 35-46 in L. F. Ruggiero, K. B.
Aubry, A. B. Carey, and M. F. Huff. Wildlife and vegetation McGuire, B., L. L. Getz, J. E. Hofman, T. Pizzuto, and
of unmanaged Douglas-fir forests. U.S. Forest Service, B. Frase. 1993. Natal dispersal and philopatry in prairie
Pacific Research Station, General Technical Report PNW- voles (Microtus ochrogaster) in relation to population
GTR-285. density, season, and natal social environment. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology 32: 293-302.
Lessells, C. M. 1985. Natal and breeding dispersal of
Canada geese Branta canadensis. Ibis 127: 31-41. McNab, B. T. 1963. Bioenergetics and the determination of
home range size. American Naturalist 97: 133-140.
Lindzey, P. G. 1978. Movement patterns of badgers in
northwestern Utah. Journal of Wildlife Management 42: Mead, C. J. 1979. Colony fidelity and interchange in the
418-422. sand martin. Bird Study 26: 99-106.

Löfgren, O., B. Hörnfeldt, and B.-G. Carlsson. 1986. Site Mearns, R., and I. Newton. 1982. Turnover and dispersal
tenacity and nomadism in Tengmalm’s owl (Aegolius in a peregrine Falco peregrinus population. Ibis 126: 347-
funereus (L.)) in relation to cyclic food production. 355.
Oecologia 69: 321-326.
Mech, L. D. 1987. Age, season, distance, direction, and
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

social aspects of wolf dispersal from a Minnesota pack. observations and comments on the periodic invasions of
Pages 55-74 in B. D. Chepko-Sade and Z. T. Halpin, goshawks. Auk 84: 183-191.
editors. Mammalian dispersal patterns: the effects of social
structure on population genetics. University of Chicago Neigel, J. E., and J. C. Avise. 1993. Application of a
Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA. random walk to geographic distributions of animal
mitochondrial DNA variation. Genetics 135: 1209-1220.
Melquist, W. E., and H. G. Hornocker. 1983. Ecology of
river otters in west central Idaho. Wildlife Monographs 83: Nelson, M. E. 1993. Natal dispersal and gene flow in white-
1-60. tailed deer in northeastern Minnesota. Journal of
Mammalogy 74: 316-322.
Merriam, G. 1998. Biodiversity at the population level: a
vital paradox. Pages 45-65 in F. L. Bunnell and J. F. Nelson, M. E., and L. D. Mech. 1992. Dispersal in female
Johnson, editors. Policy and practices for biodiversity in white-tailed deer. Journal of Mammalogy 73: 891-894.
managed forests: the living dance. University of British
Columbia Press, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. Newton, I., editor. 1989. Lifetime reproduction in birds.
Academic Press, London, UK.
Mihok, S., T. Lawton, and B. Schwartz. 1988. Fates and
movements of meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) Newton, I., and M. Marquiss. 1983. Dispersal of
sparrowhawks between birthplace and breeding place.
following a population decline. Canadian Journal of
Zoology 66: 323-328. Journal of Animal Ecology 52: 463-477.

Miller, G. L., and B. W. Carroll. 1989. Modeling dispersal Nice, M. M. 1937. Studies in the life history of the song
sparrow. I. A population study of the song sparrow.
distances: alternatives to the geometric distribution. Ecology
70: 977-986. Transactions of the Linnean Society of New York 6: 1-328.

Miller, G. S., and E. C. Meslow. 1985. Dispersal data for Nilsson, J. A. 1989. Causes and consequences of natal
dispersal in the marsh tit, Parus palustris. Journal of Animal
juvenile spotted owls: the problem of small sample size.
Pages 69-73 in R. J. Gutiérrez and A. B. Carey, editors. Ecology 58: 619-636.
Ecology and management of the spotted owl in the Pacific O'Farrell, T. P. 1965. Home range and ecology of
Northwest. U.S. Forest Service General Technical Report snowshoe hares in interior Alaska. Journal of Mammalogy
GTR PNW-185. 46: 406-418.
Miller, G. S., R. J. Small, and E. C. Meslow. 1997. Oring, L. W., E. M. Gray, and J. M. Reed. 1997. Spotted
Habitat selection by spotted owls during natal dispersal in Sandpiper, Actitis macularia. In A. Poole and F. Gill,
western Oregon. Journal of Wildlife Management 61: 140- editors. The Birds of North America. Number 289. Academy
150. of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and
American Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Miller, K. E., and J. A. Smallwood. 1997. Natal dispersal
and philopatry of southeastern American Kestrels in Florida. Oring, L. W., and D. B. Lank. 1984. Breeding area
Wilson Bulletin 109: 226-232. fidelity, natal philopatry, and the social systems of
Mills, L. S., and F. W. Allendorf. 1996. The one-migrant- sandpipers. Pages 125-147 in J. Burger and B. L. Olla,
per-generation rule in conservation and management. editors. Shorebirds: breeding behavior and populations.
Plenum Press, New York, New York, USA.
Conservation Biology 10: 1509-1518.

Mitchell, J. L. 1961. Mink movements and populations on Paradis, E., S. R. Baillie, W. J. Sutherland, and R. D.
a Montana river. Journal of Wildlife Management 25: 48-54. Gregory. 1998. Patterns of natal and breeding dispersal in
birds. Journal of Animal Ecology 67: 518-536.
Mitchell, C. D. 1994. Trumpeter Swan, Cygnus buccinator.
In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. Pärt, T. 1990. Natal dispersal in the collared flycatcher:
Number 105. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, possible causes and reproductive consequences. Ornis
Scandinavica 21: 83-88.
Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Washington, D.C., USA. Pasitschniak-Arts, M., and S. Lariviere. 1995. Gulo gulo.
Mitchener, G. R., and D. R. Mitchener. 1977. Population Mammalian Species 499: 1-10.
structure and dispersal in Richardson's ground squirrels. Payne, R. B. 1991. Natal dispersal and population structure
Ecology 58: 359-368. in a migratory songbird, the Indigo Bunting. Evolution 45:
Moore, J., and R. Ali. 1984. Are dispersal and inbreeding 49-62.
avoidance related? Animal Behaviour 32: 94-112. Peacock, M. M., and A. T. Smith. 1997. The effect of
Mueller, H. C., and D. D. Berger. 1967. Some habitat fragmentation on dispersal patterns, mating
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

behavior, and genetic variation in a pika (Ochonota Europe. Naturaliste Canadien 101: 379-392.
princeps) metapopulation. Oecologia 112: 524-533.
Pusey, A. E. 1987. Sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding
Pearson, A. M. 1972. Population characteristics of the avoidance in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology and
northern interior grizzly in the Yukon Territory, Canada. Evolution 2: 295-299.
Pages 32-35 in Second International Conference of Bear
Research and Management (Calgary, Alberta, Canada, Rees, M. 1993. Null models and dispersal distributions: a
1970). International Union for Conservation of Nature and comment on an article by Caley. American Naturalist 141:
Natural Resources, Morges, Switzerland. IUCN Publication, 812-815.
New Series, Number 23.
Rheinwald, G. 1975. The pattern of settling distances in a
Peltonen, A., and I. Hanski. 1991. Patterns of island population of House Martins Delichon urbica. Ardea 63:
occupancy explained by colonization and extinction rates in 136-145.
shrews. Ecology 72: 1698-1708.
Ribble, D. O. 1992. Dispersal in a monogamous rodent.
Peters, R. H. 1983. The ecological implications of body Ecology 73: 859-866.
size. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Robinette, W. L. 1966. Mule deer home range and
Peterson, G., C. R. Allen, and C. S. Holling. 1998. dispersal in Utah. Journal of Wildlife Management 30: 335-
Ecological resilience, biodiversity and scale. Ecosystems 1: 349.
6-18.
Robinson, G. R., R. D. Holt, M. S. Gaines, S. P. Hamburg,
Phillips, R. L., R. D. Andrews, G. L. Storm, and R. A. M. L. Johnson, H. S. Fitch, and E. S. Martinko. 1992.
Bishop. 1972. Dispersal and mortality of red foxes. Journal Diverse and contrasting effects of habitat fragmentation.
of Wildlife Management 36: 237-248. Science 257: 524-527.

Plissner, J. H., and P. A Gowaty. 1996. Patterns of natal Robinson, J. A., and L. W. Oring. 1997. Natal and
dispersal, turnover, and dispersal costs in eastern bluebirds. breeding dispersal in American Avocets. Auk 114: 416-430.
Animal Behaviour 51: 1307-1322.
Robinson, W. B., and E. F. Grant. 1958. Comparative
Porter, J. H., and J. L. Dooley. 1993. Animal dispersal movements of bobcats and coyotes as disclosed by tagging.
patterns: a reassessment of simple mathematical models. Journal of Wildlife Management 22: 117-122.
Ecology 74: 2436-2443.
Robinson, W. D. 1995. Louisiana Waterthrush, Seiurus
Post, W., J. P. Poston, and G. T. Bancroft. 1996. Boat- motacilla. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of
tailed Grackle, Quiscalus major. In A. Poole and F. Gill, North America. Number 151. Academy of Natural Sciences,
editors. The Birds of North America. Number 207. Academy Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
American Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Rogers, L. L. 1987. Effects of food supply and kinship on
Preston, C. R., and R. D. Beane. 1993. Red-tailed Hawk, social behavior, movements, and population growth of black
Buteo jamaicensis. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The bears in northeastern Minnesota. Wildlife Monographs 97:
Birds of North America. Number 52. Academy of Natural 1-72.
Sciences, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Rongstad, O. J. 1965. A life history study of thirteen-lined
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
ground squirrels in southern Wisconsin. Journal of
Price, M. V., P. A. Kelly, and R. L. Goldingay. 1994. Mammalogy 46: 76-87.
Distances moved by Stephens' kangaroo rat (Dipodomys
Rood, J. P. 1987. Dispersal and intergroup transfer in the
stephensi Merriam) and implications for conservation.
Journal of Mammalogy 75: 929-939. dwarf mongoose. Pages 85-103 in B. D. Chepko-Sade and
Z. T. Halpin, editors. Mammalian dispersal patterns.
Priewert, F. W. 1951. Record of an extensive movement by University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.
a raccoon. Journal of Mammalogy 42: 113.
Rosenfield, R. N., and J. Bielefeldt. 1992. Natal dispersal
Prothero, J. 1986. Methodological aspects of scaling in and inbreeding in the Cooper's Hawk. Wilson Bulletin 104:
biology. Journal of Theoretical Biology 118: 259-286. 182-184.

Pulliam, H. R., J. B. Dunning, Jr., and J. Liu. 1992. Ross, P. I., and M. G. Jalotsky. 1992. Characteristics of a
Population dynamics in complex landscapes: a case study. hunted population of cougars in southwestern Alberta.
Ecological Applications 2: 165-177. Journal of Wildlife Management 56: 417-426.

Pullianen, E. 1974. Seasonal movements of moose in Russell, E. M., and I. Rowley. 1993. Philopatry or
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

dispersal: competition for territory vacancies in the splendid Skeel, M. A., and E. Mallory. 1996. Whimbrel, Numenius
fairy-wren Malurus splendens. Animal Behaviour 45: 519- phaeopus. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of
539. North America. Number 219. Academy of Natural Sciences,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American
Sandell, M., J. Agrell, S. Erlinge, and J. Nelson. 1990. Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Natal dispersal in relation to population density and sex
ratio in the field vole, Microtus agrestis. Oecologia 83: 145- Smith, A. A. 1997. Dispersal and movements in a Swedish
149. Willow Grouse, Lagopus lagopus population. Wildlife
Biology 3: 279.
Saunders, J. K. J. 1963. Movements and activities of the
lynx in Newfoundland. Journal of Wildlife Management 27: Smith, F. A. 1997. Neotoma cinerea. Mammalian Species
390-400. 564: 1-8.

Schoener, T. W. 1968. Sizes of feeding territories among Soderquist, T., and A. Lill. 1995. Natal dispersal and
birds. Ecology 49: 123-141. philopatry in the carnivorous marsupial, Phasogale tapoafa
(Dasyuridae). Ethology 99: 297-312.
Schmidt-Nielsen, K. 1984. Scaling: why is animal size so
important? Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK. Sokal, R. H., and F. J. Rolf. 1995. Biometry: the principles
and practice of statistics in biological research. Freeman,
Schroeder, M. A. 1986. The fall phase of dispersal in New York, New York, USA.
juvenile spruce grouse. Canadian Journal of Zoology 64:
16-20. Southern, H. N. 1970. The natural control of a population
of tawny owls (Strix aluco). Journal of the Zoological
Schumaker, N. H. 1996. Using landscape indices to predict Society of London 162: 197-285.
habitat connectivity. Ecology 77: 1210-1225.
Spies, T. A., W. J. Ripple, and G. A. Bradshaw. 1994.
Shaw, M. W. 1995. Simulation of population expansion and Dynamics and patterns of a managed coniferous forest
spatial pattern when individual dispersal distributions do not landscape in Oregon. Ecological Applications 4: 555-568.
decline exponentially with distance. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London Series B. Biological Sciences 259: Sprugel, D. G. 1983. Correcting for bias in log-transformed
243-248. allometric equations. Ecology 64: 209-210.

Shields, P. W. 1960. Movement patterns of brush rabbits in Steen, H. 1994. Low survival of long distance dispersers of
northwestern California. Journal of Wildlife Management the root vole (Microtus oeconomus). Annales Zoologica
24: 381-386. Fennici 31: 271-274.

Shields, W. M. 1982. Optimal inbreeding and the evolution Storm, G. L., R. D. Andrews, R. L. Phillips, R. A.
of philopatry. Pages 132-159 in I. R. Swingland and P. J. Bishop, D. B. Siniff, and J. R. Tester. 1976. Morphology,
Greenwood, editors. The ecology of animal movement. reproduction, dispersal and mortality of midwestern red fox
Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. populations. Wildlife Monographs 49: 1-72.

Shields, W. M. 1987. Dispersal and mating systems: Strickland, D. 1991. Juvenile dispersal in gray jays:
investigating their causal connections. Pages 3-26 in B. D. dominant brood member expels siblings from natal territory.
Chepko-Sade and Z. T. Halpin, editors. Mammalian Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 2935-2945.
dispersal patterns. University of Chicago Press, Chicago,
Illinois, USA. Sullivan, E. G. 1956. Gray fox reproduction, denning,
range and weights in Alabama. Journal of Mammalogy 37:
Silva, M. 1998. Allometric scaling of body length: elastic or 346-351.
geometric similarity in mammalian design. Journal of
Mammalogy 79: 20-32. Sun, C. 1997. Dispersal of young in red squirrels
(Tamiasciurus hudsonicus). American Midland Naturalist
Silva, M., and J. A. Downing. 1995a. CRC Handbook of 138: 252-259.
mammalian body masses. CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, USA.
Swihart, R. K. 1992. Home-range attributes and spatial
Silva, M., and J. A. Downing. 1995b. The allometric structure of woodchuck populations. Journal of Mammalogy
scaling of density and body mass: a nonlinear relationship 73: 604-618.
for terrestrial mammals. American Naturalist 145: 704-727.
Swingland, I. R. 1982. Intraspecific differences in
Sinclair, A. R. E. 1992. Do large mammals disperse like movement. Pages 32-53 in I. R. Swingland and P. J.
small mammals? Pages 229-242 in N. C. Stenseth and W. Z. Greenwood, editors. The ecology of animal movement.
Lidicker, Jr., editors. Animal dispersal: small mammals as a Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
model. Chapman and Hall, New York, New York, USA.
Conservation Ecology 4(1): 16.
http://www.consecol.org/vol4/iss1/art16

Taylor, R. A. J. 1980. A family of regression equations West, G. B., J. H. Brown, and B. J. Enquist. 1997. A
describing the density distribution of dispersing organisms. general model for the origin of allometric scaling laws in
Nature 286: 53-55. biology. Science 276: 122-126.

Tomlinson, R. E., H. M. Wight, and T. S. Baskett. 1960. West, S. 1995. Cave Swallow, Hirundo fulva. In A. Poole
Migrational homing, local movement, and mortality of and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America. Number
mourning doves in Missouri. Pages 253-267 in Transactions 141. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
of the 25th North American Wildlife Conference (Dallas, Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
Texas, USA, 1960). Wildlife Management Institute, Washington, D.C., USA.
Washington, D.C., USA.
Wheelwright, N. T., and R. A. Mauck. 1998. Philopatry,
Turchin, P. 1998. Quantitative analysis of movement: natal dispersal, and inbreeding avoidance in an island
measuring and modeling population redistribution in population of Savannah Sparrows. Ecology 79: 755-767.
animals and plants. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland,
Massachusetts, USA. Whitcomb, R. F., J. F. Lynch, M. K. Klimkiewicz, C. S.
Robbins, B. L. Whitcomb, and D. Bystrak. 1981. Effects
Tweit, R. C. 1996. Curve-billed Thrasher, Toxostoma of forest fragmentation on avifauna of the eastern deciduous
curvirostre. In A. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of forest. Pages 125-206 in R. L. Burgess and D. M. Sharpe,
North America. Number 235. Academy of Natural Sciences, editors. Forest island dynamics in man-dominated
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA, and American landscapes. Springer-Verlag, New York, New York, USA.
Ornithologists' Union, Washington, D.C., USA.
Wiklund, C. G. 1996. Determinants of dispersal in
VanCamp, L. F., and C. J. Henny. 1975. The screech owl: breeding merlins (Falco columbarius). Ecology 77: 1920-
its life history and population ecology in northern Ohio. 1927.
North American Fauna 71: 1-65.
With, K., and T. O. Crist. 1995. Critical thresholds in
VanDruff, L. W. 1969. Movements of opposums in marsh species' responses to landscape structure. Ecology 76: 2446-
and wetland habitats. Pages 81-89 in Transactions of 2459.
Northeast Fish and Wildlife Conference (White Sulphur
Springs, West Virginia, USA, 1969). Northeast Section of Wolf, B. O. 1997. Black Phoebe, Sayornis nigricans. In A.
the Wildlife Society, Amherst, Massachusetts, USA. Poole and F. Gill, editors. The Birds of North America.
Number 268. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia,
Van Vuren, D. 1998. Mammalian dispersal and reserve Pennsylvania, USA, and American Ornithologists' Union,
design. Pages 369-393 in T. Caro, editor. Behavioral Washington, D.C., USA.
ecology and conservation biology. Oxford University Press,
New York, New York, USA. Wolff, J. O. 1993. What is the role of adults in mammalian
juvenile dispersal? Oikos 68: 173-176.
Villard, M.-A., G. Merriam, and B. A. Maurer. 1995.
Dynamics in subdivided populations of neotropical Wolff, J. O., and W. Z. Lidicker, Jr. 1980. Population
migratory birds in a fragmented temperate forest. Ecology ecology of the taiga vole, Microtus xanthognathus, in
76: 27-40. interior Alaska. Canadian Journal of Zoology 58: 1800-
1812.
Wallin, K., and M. Andersson. 1981. Adult nomadism in
Tengmalm's owl Aegolius funereus. Ornis Scandinavica 12: Woolfenden, G. E., and J. W. Fitzpatrick. 1978. The
125-126. inheritance of territory in group-breeding birds. BioScience
28: 104-108.
Waser, P. M. 1987. A model predicting dispersal distance
distributions. Pages 251-256 in B. D. Chepko-Sade and Z. Zimen, E. 1984. Long range movements of the red fox,
T. Halpin, editors. Mammalian dispersal patterns. Vulpes vulpes L. Acta Zoologica Fennici 171: 267.
University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA.

Waser, P. M., S. R. Creel, and J. R. Lucas. 1994. Death


and disappearance: estimating mortality risks associated
with philopatry and dispersal. Behavioral Ecology 5: 135-
141.

Waser, P. M., and L. F. Elliott. 1991. Dispersal and


genetic structure in kangaroo rats. Evolution 45: 935-943.

Weise, C. M., and J. R. Meyer. 1979. Juvenile dispersal


and development of site-fidelity in the Black-capped
Chickadee. Auk 96: 40-55.

You might also like