You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Environmental Psychology (1996) 16, 221–233 0272-4944/96/030221+13$18.

00/0
 1996 Academic Press Ltd

ENVIRONMENTAL
Journalof

PSYCHOLOGY
RESTORATIVE QUALITIES OF FAVORITE PLACES

KALEVI KORPELA* AND TERRY HARTIG†


*Department of Psychology, University of Tampere, Finland †School of Public Health,
University of California, Berkeley, CA, U.S.A.

Abstract

Theory and research dealing with place identity and restorative environments have for the most part
proceeded independently. Assuming that emotional- and self-regulation are processes underlying the develop-
ment of place identity, and that a person’s favorite place is an exemplar of environments used in such regu-
lation processes, the present study goes beyond preliminary observations about restorative aspects of favorite
places to consider how individuals evaluate their favorite places using terms set out in restorative environ-
ments theory. Finnish university students (n=78) evaluated the central square of their city (Tampere) and
favorite and unpleasant places of their own designation using the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS), an
instrument based on attention restoration theory. Consistent with notions of self-regulation, PRS subscale
scores for Being Away, Fascination, Coherence, and Compatibility were all high in the favorite place evalu-
ations, but Coherence and Compatibility were reliably higher than Being Away, which was in turn reliably
higher than Fascination. Also, PRS subscale scores for the favorite places were reliably higher than those for
the central square, which were in turn higher than those for the unpleasant places. Furthermore, differences
were also found in self-reported emotional states associated with each place. The discussion suggests ways to
develop further mutually reinforcing relations between restorative environments research and research on
place identity.  1996 Academic Press Limited

Introduction freedom of expression and escape from social


pressures were reported. Favorite places, it
Questions about place identity and restorative appears, enable experiences like those of interest in
environments are receiving more attention from research on restorative environments (e.g. Hartig et
researchers in the environment–behavior-design al., 1991; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Ulrich et al.,
field (e.g. Stokols, 1995). Work in these areas has 1991). The present study goes beyond these prelimi-
for the most part proceeded independently. How- nary observations to consider how individuals
ever, Korpela (1991) has suggested that restorative evaluate their favorite places using terms set out in
experiences figure in emotion- and self-regulation restorative environments theory. It is thus a more
processes through which individuals develop deliberate effort to bring research on place identity
place identity. Taking a person’s favorite place as into a mutually reinforcing relationship with res-
one to which the person has become attached, torative environments research. Some foundations
studies by Korpela (1989, 1991, 1992) have provided for such a relationship are identified in the follow-
self-report data which suggest that adolescents ing overview of theory in each of the areas to be
use their favorite places to anchor emotion- and joined.
self-regulation (see also Silbereisen et al., 1986;
Noack & Silbereisen, 1988; Owens, 1988;
Silbereisen & Noack, 1988; Sommer, 1990). Korpela Place identity, emotion-regulation, and self-
found that his subjects often went to their favorite regulation
places to relax, to calm down, and to clear their
minds after threatening or emotionally negative Proshansky et al. (1983) conceived of place identity
events. In addition, experiences of beauty, control, as ‘a substructure of self-identity which consists of
221
222 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

. . . cognitions about the physical world in which the fulfilling four fundamental functions. In other
individual lives’ (p. 59). They added: words, four principles governing motivation, cog-
These cognitions are memories, ideas, feelings, atti- nition and action are set forth. These principles are
tudes, values, preferences, meanings, and concep- consistent with psychoanalytical, learning and
tions of behavior and experience which relate to the phenomenological theories and integrate them
variety and complexity of physical settings that (Epstein, 1983, 1985). One principle is the need to
define the day-to-day existence of every human maximize the pleasure/pain balance. It corresponds
being. At the core of such physical environment-
related cognitions is the ‘environmental past’ of the to the pleasure principle in Freudian theory and to
person; a past consisting of places, spaces, and their the principle of reinforcement in learning theory. A
properties which have served instrumentally in the second principle—the unity principle—is the need
satisfaction of the person’s biological, psychological, to assimilate the data of reality into a coherent con-
social, and cultural needs. (p. 59). ceptual system and to maintain this system
In elaborating on this conceptualization, Korpela (Epstein, 1983). This need is consistent with a need
(1989) addressed the criticism Proshansky et al. in all higher animals to establish an adequate rep-
received from Sarbin (1983) for not applying an resentation of their environment so that they can
organizing principle that would explain how con- cope with demands more flexibly and efficiently
duct might be influenced by place identity at any than if their responses were instinctive reactions to
given time. Building on work by Epstein (e.g., 1983, discrete stimuli (Epstein, 1981). A third principle is
1994) and Vuorinen (1990), Korpela proposed that the need to maintain a favorable level of self-
place identity is constituted in on-going processes of esteem; it is derived from the pleasure principle,
emotion- and self-regulation; that is, physical and becomes important once a self-theory has been
environments can be used to regulate pleasure/pain formulated. The fourth principle is the need to
and self-experience, and place identity is partly for- maintain relatedness, or favorable relationships
med by the experiences and cognitions produced in with significant others. The principles are assumed
those regulation processes. 1 to overlap and interact. For example, pleasure is
Ideas about emotion- and self-regulation provide usually experienced when there are elevations in
means to join the place identity concept with res- self-esteem and when previously unassimilable
torative environments theory (Korpela, 1989, 1992, material is assimilated into a person’s self-experi-
1995). Emotion-regulation is not only an inner ence. Displeasure is experienced when self-esteem
homeostatic process, but one that also involves is diminished or when there is a failure in assimi-
interaction with the environment (Vuorinen, 1990; lation (Epstein, 1990). The four principles are all of
Dodge & Garber, 1991; Fuhrer et al., 1993; Kaiser, basic importance, and behavior is viewed as a
1993). Emotion-regulation has been defined with compromise among them (Epstein, 1985). For
reference to the ‘intra- and extraorganismic factors example, individuals are generally motivated to set
by which emotional arousal is redirected, controlled, a level of self-esteem only as high as they can
modulated, and modified to enable an individual to realistically maintain without exposing themselves
function adaptively in emotionally arousing situ- to frequent disappointments (Epstein, 1985).
ations’ (Cicchetti et al., 1991, p. 15). Of the types of Emotion- and self-regulation imply the likelihood
emotion-regulation system discussed by Izard and of periodic changes from negative to more positive
Kobak (1991), it is the intentional and self-initiated emotions and self-cognitions. Such changes are also
type that may best encompass the use of physical characteristic of restoration. In describing processes
environments. Other forms of intentional and self- through which person–environment interactions aid
initiated emotion-regulation include self-soothing change from negative antecedents to more positive
and gratification techniques; techniques for mod- states, theories about restorative experience and
ifying expressive behavior; skeletal-muscular tech- restorative environments thus also reveal processes
niques such as play and exercise; and perceptual- that may promote the development of place identity
cognitive techniques such as attention focusing and (cf. Fuhrer et al., 1993). Furthermore, one perspec-
shifting, self-instructions, or self-monitoring. tive on restoration, attention restoration theory
With respect to self-regulation, Epstein (1983, (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995), goes
1985, 1994) assumes that human beings construct a beyond more momentary changes in psychological
personal theory of themselves and the world. This and physiological states to encompass reflection on
personal theory of reality exists at a preconscious oneself and one’s place in the world as an aspect of
level of awareness and directs behavior. It is not restorative experience. This is presumably import-
developed for its own sake, but for the purpose of ant for self-regulation; reflection may aid in the
Favorite Places 223

assimilation of new experiences into one’s self- or conceptual environment that is of sufficient scope
theory and in the maintenance of one’s conceptual to sustain exploration and interpretation. Finally,
systems. compatibility refers to a match between personal
inclinations and purposes, environmental supports
Restoration and restorative environments for intended activities, and environmental con-
straints on action (see also Kaplan, 1983).
Attention restoration theory (e.g. Kaplan & Kaplan, Other differences between the stress reduction
1989; Kaplan, 1995) and Ulrich’s psychophysiolog- and attention restoration frameworks have to do
ical stress reduction framework (Ulrich, 1983; with the duration of the restorative process and the
Ulrich et al., 1991) dominate the restoration per- immediacy, depth, and persistence of effects. As
spectives in environmental psychology. These theor- presently formulated, the psychophysiological
ies differ in the emphasis placed on emotional, stress reduction approach emphasizes the first
physiological, and attentional factors in their speci- moments of an encounter with an esthetically pleas-
fications of both antecedent conditions and the res- ing scene. Possible changes in affect and
toration process. Briefly, the stress reduction frame- arousal then initiated are modeled in some detail
work (Ulrich, 1983; Ulrich et al., 1991) assumes an (Ulrich, 1983). Longer-term effects are possible
antecedent condition of psychophysiological stress, (Parsons, 1991), but as yet have been little dis-
defined as a process of responding emotionally, cussed. In contrast, attention restoration theory
physiologically, and behaviorally to a situation in allows for a potentially long duration in a restora-
which well-being is challenged or threatened. tive experience, such that a person may pass
Consequences of stress include negative emotions through successive levels (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989).
and heightened autonomic arousal. Restoration is The first level involves ‘clearing the head’. The
brought about in a visual encounter with a scene second is recovery of directed attention capacity.
having particular properties, such as natural con- The third entails facing accumulated matters on
tents, moderate complexity, and the presence of a one’s mind. The fourth is reflection on one’s priorit-
focal point. Perception of these properties prompts a ies, prospects, actions and goals in life. Penetration
shift toward more positively-toned emotional states, into successive levels requires increasing time and
drives down activity in different physiological sys- intensity of the factors thought to work in restora-
tems, evokes sustained attention, and blocks nega- tive experience. Thus, this theory describes a broad
tive emotions and thoughts. temporal range within which restoration and its
In contrast, attention restoration theory (e.g. effects might be seen.
Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995) posits an In describing possible restoration processes and
antecedent condition of directed attention fatigue, outcomes, both the attention restoration and stress
which is incurred in any prolonged mental effort reduction frameworks add to an understanding of
which requires steady exercise of the inhibitory emotion- and self-regulation phenomena. Both
mechanism assumed to make directed attention frameworks also point to qualities of person–envi-
possible. Among the negative corollaries of directed ronment interactions that might contribute to the
attention fatigue are irritability, an inability to development of place identity. However, by allowing
plan, a reduced sensitivity to interpersonal cues, for greater temporal extension of the restoration
and increased likelihood of errors in performance. process and a greater range of effects, including
Restoration can proceed when four factors charac- changes in perspectives on oneself and one’s place
terize the person–environment interaction. One, in the world (see e.g. Kaplan & Talbot, 1983), atten-
being away, involves getting psychological and poss- tion restoration theory allows greater latitude for
ibly geographical distance from one’s usual context, relating restorative experiences to place identity as
including the work one ordinarily does and the pur- an outcome of environmental self-regulation.
suit of particular goals and purposes. A second fac- Appropriately, attention restoration theory pro-
tor is fascination, or effortless attention. When func- vides means for relating restoration to place ident-
tioning is supported by fascination, efforts at ity. In its specification of operative factors, atten-
inhibiting distractions can be relaxed and directed tion restoration theory acknowledges the interests,
attention capacity can be restored. Fascination can purposes and environmental constraints that can
be engaged by environmental contents such as structure people’s lives and activities. An open
water or by processes of exploring and making sense question has to do with the varying significance of
of an environment. A third factor, extent, refers to the four factors for environmental self-regulation.
the possibility for immersion in a coherent physical Compatibility might seem to be most closely aligned
224 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

with the idea of self, in that it explicitly draws in environments in general will be more restorative
the individual’s own purposes and inclinations. Fur- than many of the urban environments in which
thermore, high degrees of compatibility are people have concentrated themselves. Some pre-
assumed to be necessary for the kind of reflection liminary experimental evidence supports this
thought to contribute to the deep restoration which assertion (Ulrich, 1979; Hartig et al., 1991; Ulrich et
may most involve one’s sense of self. Yet it is al., 1991). Although the extant theory is open to the
thought that high compatibility cannot be found in idea that other places serve restoration, few of the
situations lacking high degrees of being away, fasci- studies in this area have considered other than
nation, and extent. Moreover, having a sense of natural and urban environments (for an exception,
being away in a particular environment, being fasci- see Kaplan et al., 1993). In contrast, previous
nated by what is found there, and finding it to be of research on favorite places (e.g. Korpela, 1992)
some coherence and extent are all behaviors pre- found that subjects did frequently identify natural
sumably susceptible to processes that also work in environments as their favorite places, but that they
the development of self. Thus, when thinking of res- also named a variety of other settings, such as local
torative experiences available in places to which cafés or their room at home. It was of interest
people are attached, such as favorite places, the whether the favorite places identified in the present
relative importance of the different factors is a study would be similarly variable.
question in need of investigation (cf. Hartig et al., The present study also served a methodological
1991). function. The reliability and validity of the PRS has
been assessed in other studies by having subjects
Study objectives evaluate a set of sites expected to differ in theoreti-
cally meaningful terms, with the sites presented in
Favorite places appear to afford restorative experi- different ways (on-site, video, slides) to subjects
ences that aid emotion- and self-regulation pro- from different populations (Hartig et al., 1996). In
cesses which are basic to the development of place the present study, subjects evaluated the favorite,
identity. Attention restoration theory (Kaplan & everyday, and unpleasant places on the basis of
Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995) provides a set of con- their memory or imagination. Thus, two of the
structs that are useful for understanding restora- environments were the same across subjects only in
tive person–environment interactions. One aim of that they were identified as either a favorite or an
the present study was to examine the absolute unpleasant place. Having each subject refer to his
levels and relative importance of these factors in or her own favorite and unpleasant place allowed
people’s experiences of their favorite places, as for variability in the characteristics of the environ-
reflected in evaluations obtained with the Perceived ments while holding the personal significance of the
Restorativeness Scale (PRS) (Hartig et al., 1996). place roughly constant. Also, not providing any sen-
The study also compared the evaluations of favorite sory information about the environment in question
places with evaluations of a place in the everyday is an appropriate next step beyond simulations;
public life of all subjects and an unpleasant place of arguably, the commonly used (visual) simulation
each subject’s designation. The two additional methods already require that a subject draw on
places were likely to have different, lower restora- memory or imagination to flesh out the visual and
tive potentials; the intent was to examine how the nonvisual aspects of the setting not provided in the
restorative factors varied across environments the simulation. It was of interest whether reliability
subjects might move among and to which they and validity information obtained with the use of
would have different degrees of attachment. Fur- these methods would be consistent with that
thermore, the subjects used an emotional state scale obtained previously (cf. Craik & Feimer, 1987).
to report how they would feel in each place. Aside
from portraying the emotions felt in each place, the
added scale made it possible to examine the Method
relations between those emotions and perceptions of
restorativeness. Design
Another concern here was with the kind of places
that become favored. Building on assumptions All subjects evaluated seven settings. The first was
about psychological and psychophysiological mech- Tampere’s central square, a setting chosen because
anisms evolved in natural environments, restora- of its presumed familiarity. The next two settings
tive environments theory posits that natural were a favorite and an unpleasant place in the
Favorite Places 225

given subject’s life. Some subjects (n=48) evaluated imagine themselves in the setting and to then indi-
a favorite place first, the others (n=30) an cate the extent to which each item applied to their
unpleasant place. Subjects were instructed to experience there.
imagine the setting while completing their evalu- Validation studies in the United States and
ations. The remaining settings were a rock garden, Sweden (Hartig et al., 1996) have separately ana-
an outdoor shopping mall, a study room with plants, lysed the evaluations of each of the four UCI sites
and the bottom floor of a parking garage, all on or mentioned above. With relatively few exceptions,
near the campus of the University of California, adequate internal consistencies were found in each
Irvine (UCI). Each of these was represented with a site in each of three studies for the a priori Being
selection of color slides, and the simulations were Away, Fascination, Coherence, and Compatibility
randomly ordered for each group of subjects being subscales. Principal factor analyses of the same
led through the procedure. The rationale for the use data indicated that the 4-factor solution, obliquely
of these four sites is discussed elsewhere (Hartig et (oblimin) rotated to allow for potentially correlated
al., 1996), as are the results based on their evalu- factors, was less stable across sites than was a
ation by the present sample. solution in which one factor was loaded on primarily
by Being Away, Fascination, and Compatibility
Subjects items and a second was loaded on by Coherence
items. For each of the four sites in each of the three
Undergraduate students (49 female, 29 male) from studies, composite scores based on the 2-factor
the University of Tampere volunteered for partici- solution showed reasonable patterns of positive,
pation. Neither course credit nor monetary compen- negative, and null correlations with other measures
sation was provided. Subjects’ ages ranged from 19 of environmental characteristics and with self-
to 46 years (M=25·46, S.D.=6·19). reported emotions. The composite scores also were
useful in differentiating between the sites in pre-
Measures dicted and theoretically meaningful ways. Further-
more, for each of the sites similar evaluations were
Hartig and colleagues (Hartig et al., 1996) have obtained on-site and with simulations. A fourth
initiated development of a Perceived Restora- study reported by Hartig et al. (1996) involved the
tiveness Scale (PRS) to represent factors set out in present subjects’ evaluations of the four UCI sites
attention restoration theory (Kaplan & Talbot, and produced results consistent with those obtained
1983; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan, 1995). Six- previously.
teen items are intended to tap the factors Being The Zuckerman Inventory of Personal Reactions
Away (e.g. Spending time here gives me a good and Feelings (ZIPERS) was used to assess
break from my day-to-day routine); Fascination (e.g. emotional states (Zuckerman, 1977). Twelve items
My attention is drawn to many interesting things); represent the factors Fear Arousal, Sadness,
Coherence (e.g. There is a great deal of distraction); Anger/Aggression, Positive Affect, and Attentive-
and Compatibility (e.g. I have a sense that I belong ness. The instructions were modified to obtain sub-
here). Note that the Coherence items are intended jects’ views on how they would feel if they were in
to tap an aspect of Extent, and that they all define the given setting, imagining that they were actually
coherence in negative terms (e.g. as with regard to within the setting. Responses were given on 5-point
pervasive distraction). scales (1=Not at all, 5=Very Much). The ZIPERS
In the present study the 16 PRS items were has been a sensitive measure in previous exper-
included in a form comprised of 44 items altogether. imental research on the relative restorative poten-
Some of the additional items were to represent tials of natural and urban environments (Ulrich,
other constructs (e.g. preference, familiarity), and 1979; Hartig et al., 1991; Ulrich et al., 1991). It was
some were included to serve scale development pur- expected that the PRS subscales would correlate
poses. For each item, respondents use a 7-point positively with Positive Affect and Attentiveness
scale (0=Not at all; 6=Completely) to indicate the and negatively with Sadness, Fear Arousal, and
extent to which the statement describes their Anger/Aggression.
experience in the given setting. Four random item A 16-item form was used to obtain descriptions of
orders were balanced across subjects; each subject the favorite and unpleasant places. Subjects used a
used the same version of the form for all evalu- 7-point scale (0=Not at all; 6=Completely) to indi-
ations. For the central square and the favorite and cate the extent to which certain features were pre-
unpleasant places, the subjects were asked to sent or the extent to which the place could be
226 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

characterized in a particular way. The items square and to then proceed to the evaluations of
referred to the presence of shops, other people, per- favorite and unpleasant places. In each of these
sonal belongings, traffic, water, plants/greenery, they were to draw on their memory of place and to
peace and quiet, beautiful views, sunlight, and fac- imagine being in it while completing their evalu-
tories, and the extent to which it could be described ation. When all subjects were finished with the first
as urban or natural, inside or outside. An open- three sites, a brief pause was taken. The subjects
ended item enabled mention of other features or were then shown the slides of the first UCI site in
qualities that a subject considered salient. Finally, the given random sequence. After the slides for the
subjects were asked how often they visited their site had been shown, the subjects were directed to
favorite place. fill out the questionnaires while bearing in mind the
place they had just seen. This process was repeated
Procedure without pause until the subjects had evaluated all
four of the simulated sites.
Groups of subjects (n=3–11) were scheduled for ses-
sions of approximately 65 minutes duration. At the
beginning of each session subjects were given a Results
packet which included an instruction sheet and
seven sets of forms, with the ZIPERS stapled in Favorite and unpleasant places and their
front of the PRS. The form for describing favorite characteristics
and unpleasant places was stapled in front of the
ZIPERS in the second and third sets. Subjects were For both favorite and unpleasant places, the sub-
asked to first complete the evaluation of the central jects’ selections varied considerably in their physi-

TABLE 1
Favorite and unpleasant places identified by Finnish university students (n=78)
Favorite places Unpleasant places
Home n Traffic or Crowds n
home or own room 14 a busy street or tunnel 15
home yard 5 a large store 12
childhood home 1 a rush-hour crowd 7
the bus-railway station 3
Water a parking lot 3
a lake shore 13 a train bridge 2
at sea in a boat 2 a traffic jam 1
an island 2 a bus stop 1
on lake ice 1 being with people 1

Greenery Urban Locations


a wooded area 8 a suburb 8
a recreation area 4 an industrial area 3
a city park 3 a street 2
a square 1
Urban Locations a yard outside a high-rise 1
a restaurant or cafe 4 a disco 1
a building 4 the inland revenue 1
a suburb 3 a library 1
an ice hockey hall 1
the old town 1 Various Functional Settings
a bus stop 1 a university hall 3
a cinema 1 a lunch room 2
a library 1 a dentist’s waiting room 2
a workplace 2
Summer Cottage 6 a women’s bathroom 1
a lecture hall 1
Scenic View 3 a hospital ward 1
in an exam 1
a student dormitory 1
Favorite Places 227

cal and social characteristics (see Table 1). Many of which they visited their favorite places, with a
the favorite places were natural settings. range from zero to 21 visits in a typical week. One
Unpleasant places often had traffic or crowds. half reported that they visited only once weekly,
For both favorite and unpleasant places, categor- while 24 per cent indicated that they visited daily.
ies in Table 1 overlap to some degree. For example, As for the unpleasant places, 33 per cent of the sub-
the Finnish summer cottage is typically located in jects visited at least seven times each week, while
the countryside or near the coast, and may be 31 per cent visited from two to six time weekly
favored in large because of its natural surroundings (median=4).
and scenic views. This overlap is reflected in the
ratings of the various characteristics of the places Psychometric properties of the Perceived
each subject identified. So, for example, with Restorativeness Scale
respect to favorite places, 83·3 per cent of the sub-
jects noted the presence of a beautiful view (vs 2·6% Validation studies have yielded evidence of suf-
for unpleasant places); 83·1 per cent (vs 16·7%) ficient reliability and validity for the Perceived Res-
noted that sunlight was at least rather much pre- torativeness Scale (PRS) (Hartig et al., 1996). How-
sent; 73·1 per cent (vs 17·9%) indicated that water ever, the previous studies analysed evaluations
was at least rather much present, and 56·4 per cent obtained on-site or with simulations. Of interest
(vs 2·6%) noted that personal belongings were here was whether the psychometric properties of
rather much or present. Conversely, factories were the PRS as used to evaluate imagined places would
rather much or more present in the unpleasant be consistent with those described previously.
places of 48·1 per cent of the subjects (vs 2·6% for The results do in fact largely agree with those
the favorite places), and unpleasant places were from the earlier validation studies. Analyses of the
described as rather much or more urban by 84·6 per internal consistency of each a priori subscale in the
cent of subjects (vs 26% for favorite places). evaluations of each place show 11 of the 12 Cron-
Finally, subjects varied in the frequency with bach’s α values falling in the range 0·74 to 0·92

TABLE 2
For the favorite place evaluations, Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) item communalities, factor structure matrices,
and factor correlation matrices from the 4- and 2-factor solutions
PRS items arranged 4-Factor Solution 2-Factor Solution
by a priori subscale
h2 I II III IV h2 I II
Being Away 1 0·46 0·27 −0·05 0·24 0·67 0·17 0·41 −0·12
2 0·88 0·47 −0·24 0·29 0·91 0·39 0·61 −0·25

Fascination 3 0·56 0·72 −0·07 0·26 0·44 0·54 0·73 −0·06


4 0·67 0·78 0·16 0·24 0·43 0·68 0·76 0·17
5 0·56 0·73 −0·06 0·30 0·42 0·56 0·74 −0·06
6 0·79 0·83 0·16 0·33 0·51 0·81 0·85 0·13
7 0·69 0·79 −0·27 0·36 0·43 0·65 0·80 −0·26

Coherence 8 0·79 −0·11 0·88 −0·26 −0·04 0·64 −0·17 0·80


9 0·20 0·13 0·39 −0·16 0·09 0·21 0·08 0·43
10 0·41 −0·16 0·62 −0·27 −0·15 0·40 −0·23 0·62
11 0·40 −0·11 0·61 −0·06 −0·12 0·26 −0·14 0·51

Compatibility 12 0·42 0·61 −0·13 0·37 0·14 0·35 0·59 −0·16


13 0·82 0·40 −0·27 0·90 0·17 0·43 0·55 −0·46
14 0·72 0·29 −0·21 0·85 0·26 0·33 0·47 −0·42
15 0·48 0·33 −0·13 0·68 0·25 0·28 0·47 −0·32
16 0·69 0·79 −0·17 0·27 0·11 0·45 0·67 −0·12

I II III I
Factor Correlations II −0·06 −0·19
III 0·34 −0·24
IV 0·39 −0·04 0·21
An item’s highest factor-variable correlation is given in bold type.
228 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

(median=0·765). 2 The exception is with the Coher- as before but with the extraction of two factors
ence subscale in the favorite place, for which α= specified. The result was much the same as was
0·68. To examine the correspondence between the a seen in the previous studies; Being Away, Fasci-
priori subscales and possible empirical factors, prin- nation, and Compatibility items load primarily on
cipal factor analyses were carried out as in the pre- one factor and Coherence items load on the second
vious studies, with squared multiple correlations as (see Tables 2 and 3, Note 2). The 2-factor solution
the starting values for the communality estimate accounted for 56·9, 44·7, 35·5 per cent of the total
procedure, four factors specified for extraction, and, variance in the evaluations of the central square,
allowing for correlations among the factors, oblique favorite place, and unpleasant place, respectively;
(oblimin) rotation. This solution accounted for 63·9, thus, there were reductions in variance accounted
59·5, and 50·0 per cent of the total variance in the for of 7, 14·8, and 14·5 per cent, respectively, over
evaluations of the central square, favorite place, the 4-factor solution for each site. Note that if a
and unpleasant place, respectively. The items defin- standard extraction criterion (eigenvalues >1·00) is
ing the different factors in the 4-factor solution applied, the number of factors extracted varies
obtained for each place were for the most part across places as follows: central square=two;
grouped according to a priori subscale. Results of favorite places=4; unpleasant places=5 (an
the factor analyses for the favorite and unpleasant additional 5·1% of variance accounted for as com-
places are given in Tables 2 and 3 (regarding the pared to the 4-factor solution).
results for the central square see note 2). The results of the factor analyses are somewhat
As in the previous studies, the 4-factor solution equivocal with respect to the calculation of com-
included cross-loadings >0·40 and correspondingly posite scores for use in subsequent analyses; the
substantial correlations between some factors. This scores for different sites could be based on either the
was particularly the case for the central square. a priori subscales or on the 2-factor solution. The
Consequently, following with the previous studies, a latter strategy was more clearly indicated in the
second factor analysis was completed for each site, earlier validation studies by Hartig et al. (1996).

TABLE 3
For the unpleasant place evaluations, Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) item communalities, factor structure
matrices, and factor correlation matrices from the 4- and 2-factor solutions
PRS items arranged 4-Factor Solution 2-Factor Solution
by a priori subscale
h2 I II III IV h2 I II
Being Away 1 0·63 0·55 −0·07 0·70 0·25 0·37 0·61 0·00
2 0·78 0·07 0·05 0·86 0·11 0·08 0·28 0·08

Fascination 3 0·40 0·50 −0·17 0·18 0·53 0·38 0·61 −0·11


4 0·56 0·39 −0·02 0·25 0·73 0·39 0·62 0·04
5 0·41 0·28 −0·07 0·40 0·57 0·28 0·53 0·00
6 0·29 0·07 −0·07 −0·03 0·51 0·07 0·26 −0·04
7 0·57 0·50 −0·03 0·34 0·69 0·51 0·71 0·04

Coherence 8 0·45 −0·10 0·66 0·05 −0·13 0·44 −0·13 0·65


9 0·39 0·25 0·56 0·00 0·00 0·33 0·14 0·56
10 0·61 −0·23 0·74 −0·08 −0·27 0·63 −0·31 0·73
11 0·62 −0·09 0·71 −0·04 0·27 0·50 0·14 0·69

Compatibility 12 0·17 0·31 −0·13 0·27 0·29 0·17 0·41 −0·08


13 0·73 0·85 −0·13 0·27 0·36 0·54 0·74 −0·06
14 0·34 0·53 0·09 0·07 0·35 0·27 0·50 0·13
15 0·76 0·86 −0·01 0·25 0·20 0·41 0·64 0·04
16 0·29 0·43 −0·19 0·34 0·33 0·29 0·52 −0·14

I II III I
Factor Correlations II −0·04 −0·02
III 0·27 −0·06
IV 0·37 −0·09 0·20
An item’s highest factor-variable correlation loading is given in bold type.
Favorite Places 229

However, in the present study the remaining both at moderate levels and not reliably different
results are based on composite scores for the four a from one another. Coherence (and so Fascination)
priori subscales. The 4-factor solution more was, however, reliably higher than Compatibility
adequately represents the variability in evaluations (t (72)=5·44, p<0·001), which was in turn reliably
of the favorite and unpleasant places, and it also higher than Being Away (t (75)=2·79, p<0·01). In
allows a qualitatively richer description of the contrast, in the unpleasant places mean Coherence
experiences available in the different places. was at a moderate level but was reliably greater
than Fascination (t (73)=6·12, p<0·001). Fascination
Experiential qualities of favorite and unpleasant was in turn only marginally higher than Being
places Away (t (74)=1·97, p=0·05), which was in turn
higher than Compatibility (t (76)=2·62, p=0·01).
On a scale from 1–7 (recoded from the original 0–6) These results not only point to the varying signifi-
scale, the favorite place ratings are near the high cance of the different factors across experiences in
end for all of the PRS subscales (see Table 4). Com- different places, but also indicate that the PRS is
patibility and Coherence appear to be most charac- sensitive to differences at the subscale level; the
teristic of the subjects’ experiences in their favorite subscales do not merely move up or down in tandem
places, and in about equal degree; a paired-samples across environments.
t-test suggests that the mean scores for those subs- With respect to differences between the environ-
cales do not reliably differ from one another. Com- ments in terms of the PRS subscale scores, paired-
patibility (and so by implication Coherence) is, how- samples t-tests confirm that the favorite places
ever, significantly higher than Being Away (and so were given higher mean ratings than the central
Fascination) (t (76)=1·98, p=0·05). Finally, Being square in terms of Being Away (t (76)=13·80), Fasci-
Away seems to be more characteristic of the sub- nation (t (73)=8·31), Coherence (t (72)=14·23), and
jects’ favorite place experiences than is Fascination Compatibility (t (75)=18·66) (for all, p <0·0001). The
(t (74)=2·31, p<0·05). tests likewise confirm that the central square
Parallel analyses were conducted for the central received higher evaluations than the unpleasant
square and the unpleasant places to determine places in terms of Being Away (t (76)=3·57, p <
whether the relative standing of the PRS subscale 0·001), Fascination (t (73)=7·90, p<0·0001), and
scores was the same as in the favorite place evalu- Compatibility (t (74)=8·10, p<0·0001). However, the
ations. As seen in Table 4, the mean Fascination difference between the central square and the
and Coherence scores for the central square were unpleasant places in terms of Coherence scores only

TABLE 4
Descriptive statistics for the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) and Zuckerman Inventory of Personal Reactions
(ZIPERS) subscales for each site
Subscale Site
Favorite Place Central Square Unpleasant Place
M (S.D.) M (S.D.) M (S.D.)
a b
PRS Being Away 5·47 (1·26) 2·42 (1·28) 1·88 (0·98)
Fascination 5·16 (1·13)a 3·50 (1·28)b 2·13 (0·76)
Coherence 5·89 (0·89)a 3·46 (1·09) 3·10 (1·19)
Compatibility 5·77 (0·81)a 2·71 (1·22)b 1·59 (0·54)

ZIPERS Positive Affect 4·04 (0·66)c 2·26 (0·80)d 1·54 (0·45)


Sadness 1·94 (0·95) 1·83 (0·83)d 2·74 (1·07)
Attentiveness 3·06 (1·20) 2·97 (1·04)e 2·64 (1·07)
Anger/Aggression 1·25 (0·40)c 1·96 (0·78)d 3·17 (0·87)
Fear Arousal 1·69 (0·73)c 1·97 (0·73)d 2·60 (0·95)
PRS values fall on a scale from 1–7, where lower values indicate, for example, lower fascination. ZIPERS values fall on
a scale from 1–5, where lower values indicate, for example, lower sadness. The n’s range from 74–78. aReliably greater
than corresponding score for the central square, p<0·0001; bReliably greater than corresponding score for the
unpleasant place, p<0·001; cReliably different from corresponding score for the central square, p<0·01; dReliably
different from corresponding score for the unpleasant place, p<0·0001; eReliably different from corresponding score for
the unpleasant place, p<0·05.
230 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

verges on statistical significance (p=0·06). Note that Arousal scores. These results are largely consistent
the order in which the favorite and unpleasant with expectations and with results from Hartig et
places were evaluated did not affect the evalu- al. (1996), which were discussed with respect to the
ations; analysis of variance in the evaluations of criterion validity of the PRS.
each site produced no statistically significant F-
ratio for any of the subscales.
Differences between the places are also reflected Discussion
in subjects’ reports of the emotions they would feel
in each, as assessed with the Zuckerman Inventory The environmental self-regulation hypothesis
of Personal Reactions (ZIPERS). Subjects associated (Korpela, 1989, 1995) provides a bridge between
higher Positive Affect (t (73)=16·00, p<0·0001) and restorative environments research and research on
lower Anger/Aggression (t (77)=7·55, p<0·0001) and place identity. Taking a person’s favorite place as
Fear Arousal (t (75)=2·70, p=0·009) with their favor- an exemplar of environments used in emotion- and
ite place in comparison to the central square. They self-regulation, the present study was an initial
associated higher Positive Affect (t (74)=8·02, p< empirical examination of how experiences in favor-
0·0001) and Attentiveness (t (77)=2·14, p=0·035), ite places might be evaluated using terms set out in
and lower Sadness (t (77)=6·60, p<0·0001), attention restoration theory (Kaplan and Kaplan,
Anger/Aggression (t (76)=10·23, p<0·0001), and 1989; Kaplan, 1995). That favorite place experi-
Fear Arousal (t (75)=6·08, p<0·0001) with the cen- ences are characterized by high levels of being
tral square as compared to the unpleasant places. away, fascination, coherence, and compatibility
Finally, as shown in Table 5, the various PRS affirms that it is useful to think of favorite places as
subscale scores were reliably correlated with diff- sources of restorative experience. Of the restorative
erent subscale scores for the ZIPERS. Two sets of factors, the results suggest that, on average, coher-
relationships were found in all three sites. One con- ence and compatibility are most characteristic of
sisted of positive correlations between Being Away, favorite place experiences, followed by being away
Fascination, and Compatibility scores on the one and then fascination. This ranking conforms to the
hand and the Positive Affect score on the other. idea that maintenance of a coherent conceptual sys-
The other involved negative correlations between tem is a fundamental aspect of environmental self-
Coherence and ZIPERS Anger/Aggression and Fear regulation, one which may rely on opportunities for

TABLE 5
Correlations between Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) subscales and Zuckerman Inventory of Personal Reactions
(ZIPERS) subscales, calculated for each site
Site and PRS Subscale ZIPERS Subscale
PA S A A/A FA
Central Square
Being Away 0·57*** 0·10 0·16 −0·22 −0·06
Fascination 0·65*** 0·00 0·27* −0·28* 0·12
Coherence 0·36*** −0·03 −0·08 −0·49*** −0·34**
Compatibility 0·58*** 0·10 0·32** −0·25 0·05

Favorite Place
Being Away 0·38*** −0·05 0·23 −0·24 0·16
Fascination 0·49*** −0·11 0·11 −0·11 0·22
Coherence 0·14 0·03 −0·20 −0·56*** −0·46***
Compatibility 0·37*** −0·05 0·06 −0·11 −0·01

Unpleasant Place
Being Away 0·31** 0·09 −0·11 −0·16 0·02
Fascination 0·37*** 0·29* 0·17 −0·04 0·10
Coherence 0·13 −0·05 0·01 −0·28* −0·31**
Compatibility 0·50*** 0·04 0·06 −0·24 −0·04
The n’s range from 69 to 78. PA=Positive Affect; S=Sadness; A=Attentiveness; A/A=Anger/Aggression; FA=Fear
Arousal.
*p<0·01; **p<0·005; ***p<0·001.
Favorite Places 231

reflection in nondistracting circumstances to which research, additional studies using alternative item
one may withdraw. Conceivably, some environ- combinations and analytic strategies (e.g. confirma-
ments with less personal significance might serve tory factor analysis) will be needed to refine the
restoration, yet have different profiles of the four PRS further.
operative factors; they might thus afford outcomes The present study suggests several ways to inte-
differing in temporal extension, depth and other grate further research on place identity and restora-
qualities. For example, a video game might enable tive environments. Views on restorative person–
high levels of being away and fascination and some- environment interactions might be modified in par-
what lower levels of compatibility and coherence; allel with the further development of ideas about
time spent playing one might change mood without emotion- and self-regulation. Such development can
having profound effects on the person’s perspectives help elaborate the relations between place identity
on the world and their place in it. and constructs such as place attachment. It can
Arguments about the restorativeness of favorite simultaneously consider the possibility that a per-
place experiences are strengthened by three other son’s experience of psychological bonds with a
components of the results. First, the favorite places specific locale may generalize to the experience of
identified by subjects were, in keeping with the psychological bonds with that type of locale
literature on restorative environments, most often (Feldman, 1990). It should also consider Lalli’s
places with greenery, water, and scenic quality. (1992) argument that each spatial level on which
Second, the reliable differences between evaluations place attachment (and place identity) may be mani-
of the favorite place, Tampere’s central square, and fest needs its own theoretical account and specific
the unpleasant place were as expected, with levels empirical operationalizations (see also Altman &
of being away, fascination, coherence, and compati- Low, 1992). This possibility bears on a larger set of
bility highest in the favorite places, intermediate in questions concerning the ways in which evolution-
the central square, and, with the exception of coher- ary, cultural, social, and individual level processes
ence, lowest in the unpleasant places. Thus, the combine to influence the restorativeness of environ-
four restorative factors provided a meaningful basis ments and so their suitability for service in
for differentiating favorite place experiences from emotion- and self-regulation (cf. Hartig, 1993;
experiences of the central square and unpleasant Hartig & Evans, 1993).
places. Third, the self-reported emotional states
associated with each place were about as expected,
in that the pattern of differences between places in Acknowledgements
terms of positive affect, anger/aggression, and fear
arousal paralleled that seen with the PRS scores. This study was supported by grants to the first
The seeming match between self-reported emotions author from the Academy of Finland and the Scien-
and evaluations in terms of the restorative factors is tific Foundation of the City of Tampere. Preparation
substantiated to an extent by the various corre- of this article was partially supported by Grant T32
lations between PRS and ZIPERS scores. HL07365-14 from the U.S. National Heart, Lung,
Characterization of the favorite places as restora- and Blood Institute to the second author. The
tive environments is here based on scores from an authors are grateful to Tommy Gärling for com-
instrument still in an early stage of development. ments on the study design and to Florian Kaiser,
Previous studies (Hartig et al., 1996) and the results Daniel Stokols, and the anonymous reviewers for
presented here show that the present version of the their constructive criticisms.
PRS has generally reliable subscales, is sensitive to
differences between environments, and has
adequate criterion validity (as reflected in corre- Notes
lations between PRS and ZIPERS scores). Ques-
tions remain, however, about the factor structure in Reprint requests and correspondence should be addressed
responses obtained with the PRS. As in the previous to Kalevi Korpela, Institute of Migration, Piispankatu 3,
studies, the results presented here suggest that a 2- SF-205 00 Turku, Finland.
factor solution is more stable across sites, although (1) The common features of Epstein’s and Vuorinen’s the-
here it accounts for a considerably smaller portion ories of self-regulation are described in detail elsewhere
(Korpela, 1989, 1995).
of the variance in evaluations of the favorite and (2) To conserve space, some results bearing on the
unpleasant places than does the 4-factor solution. psychometric properties of the PRS are not reported here
As with many instruments used in psychological in detail. For the full set of results from this study and
232 K. Korpela and T. Hartig

the other validation studies completed to date, (see Har- Hartig, T., Korpela, K. M., Evans, G. W. & Gärling, T.
tig et al., 1996). Validation of a measure of perceived (1996). Validation of a measure of perceived environ-
environmental restorativeness (Göteborg Psychological mental restorativeness. (Göteborg Psychological
Reports, 26:7). Göteborg: Göteborg University, Depart- Reports, 26:7). Göteborg: Göteborg University,
ment of Psychology. Department of Psychology.
Hartig, T., Böök, A., Garvill, J., Olsson, T. & Gärling, T.
(in press). Environmental influences on psychological
restoration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology.
References Izard, C. E. & Kobak, R. R. (1991). Emotions systems
functioning and emotion-regulation. In J. Garber & K.
Altman, I. & Low, S. (1992). Place Attachment. New York: A. Dodge, Eds, The Development of Emotion-regu-
Plenum Press. lation and Dysregulation. Cambridge: Cambridge
Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J. & Barnett, D. (1991). Contri- University Press, pp. 303–321.
butions from the study of high-risk populations to Kaiser, F. (1993). Mobilität als Wohnproblem: Ortsbin-
understanding the development of emotion-regu- dung im Licht der emotionalen Regulation (Mobility
lation. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge, Eds, The Develop- as a dwelling problem: Place attachment as emotional
ment of Emotion-regulation and Dysregulation. regulation) Bern: Lang.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 15–48. Kaplan, R. & Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature:
Craik, K. & Feimer, N. (1987). Environmental assess- A psychological perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge
ment. In D. Stokols & I. Altman, Eds, Handbook of University Press.
Environmental Psychology Vol 2. New York: Wiley, Kaplan, S. (1983). A model of person-environment com-
pp. 891–918. patibility. Environment & Behavior, 15, 311–332.
Dodge, K. A. & Garber, J. (1991). Domains of emotion- Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature:
regulation. In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge, Eds, The toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environ-
Development of Emotion-regulation and Dysregul- mental Psychology, 15, 169–182.
ation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. Kaplan, S. & Talbot, J. F. (1983). Psychological benefits of
3–11. a wilderness experience. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill
Epstein, S. (1981). The unity principle versus the reality Eds, Human Behavior and Environment: Advances in
and pleasure principles, or the tale of the scorpion and theory and research, Vol. 6: Behavior and the Natural
the frog. In M. D. Lynch, A. A. Norem-Hebeisen, & K. Environment. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 163–203.
J. Gergen, Eds, Self-concept. Cambridge, MA: Kaplan, S., Bardwell, L. V. & Slakter, D. B. (1993). The
Ballinger, pp. 27–37. museum as a restorative environment. Environ-
Epstein, S. (1983). The unconscious, the preconscious, ment & Behavior, 25, 725–742.
and the self-concept. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald, Korpela, K. M. (1989). Place-identity as a product of
Eds, Psychological Perspectives on the Self Vol. 2. environment self-regulation. Journal of Environmen-
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 219–247. tal Psychology, 9, 241–256.
Epstein, S. (1985). The implications of cognitive-experien- Korpela, K. M. (1991). Are favorite places restorative
tial self-theory for research in social psychology and environments? In J. Urbina-Soria, P. Ortega-
personality. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, Andeane & R. Bechtel, Eds, Healthy Environments.
15, 283–310. Oklahoma City, OK: Environmental Design Research
Epstein, S. (1990). The self-concept, the traumatic neur- Association, pp. 371–377.
osis, and the structure of personality. In D. Ozer, J. Korpela, K. M. (1992). Adolescents’ favourite places and
M. Healy, Jr & A. J. Stewart, Eds, Perspectives on environmental self-regulation. Journal of Environ-
Personality, Vol. 3. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, pp. mental Psychology, 12, 249–258.
63–98. Korpela, K. M. (1995). Developing the environmental self-
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the regulation hypothesis (Doctoral dissertation). (Acta
psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, Universitatis Tamperensis, Series A, Vol. 446).
49, 709–724. Vammala: University of Tampere.
Feldman, R. M. (1990). Settlement-identity: psychological Lalli, M. (1992). Urban-related identity: theory, measure-
bonds with home places in a mobile society. Environ- ment, and empirical findings. Journal of Environmen-
ment & Behavior, 22, 183–229. tal Psychology, 12, 285–303.
Fuhrer, U., Kaiser, F. & Hartig, T. (1993). Place attach- Noack, P. & Silbereisen, R. K. (1988). Adolescent develop-
ment and mobility during leisure time. Journal of ment and choice of leisure settings. Children’s
Environment Psychology, 12, 187–198. Environments Quarterly, 5, 25–33.
Hartig, T. (1993). Nature experience in transactional Owens, P. E. (1988). Natural landscapes, gathering
perspective. Landscape and Urban Planning, 25, places, and prospect refuges: characteristics of out-
17–36. door places valued by teens. Children’s Environments
Hartig, T. & Evans, G. W. (1993). Psychological foun- Quarterly, 5, 17–24.
dations of nature experience. In T. Gärling & R. G. Parsons, R. (1991). The potential influences of environ-
Golledge, Eds, Behavior and Environment: Psycho- mental perception on human health. Journal of
logical and geographical approaches. Amsterdam: Environmental Psychology, 11, 1–23.
North-Holland, pp. 427–457. Proshansky, H. M., Fabian, A. K. & Kaminoff, R. (1983).
Hartig, T., Mang, M. & Evans, G. W. (1991). Restorative Place-identity: physical world socialization of the self.
effects of natural environment experiences. Environ- Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 57–83.
ment & Behavior, 23, 3–26. Sarbin, T. R. (1983). Place identity as a component of self:
Favorite Places 233

an addendum. Journal of Environmental Psychology, Ulrich, R. S. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological
3, 337–342. well-being. Landscape Research, 4, 17–23.
Silbereisen, R., Noack, P. & Eyferth, K. (1986). Place for Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to
development: Adolescents, leisure settings, and devel- natural environment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill,
opmental tasks. In R. K. Silbereisen, K. Eyferth & G. Eds, Human Behavior and Environment: Advances in
Rudinger, Eds, Development as Action in Context: theory and research, Vol. 6: Behavior and the natural
Problem behavior and normal youth development. environment. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 85–125.
Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 87–107. Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E.,
Silbereisen, R. & Noack, P. (1988). Adolescence and Miles, M. A. & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery
environment. In D. Canter, M. Krampen & D. Stea, during exposure to natural and urban environments.
Eds, Environmental Policy, Assessment and Com- Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 201–230.
munication. Aldershot: Avebury, pp. 19–34. Vuorinen, R. (1990). Persoonallisuus ja minuus
Sommer, B. (1990). Favorite places of Estonian ado- (Personality and self). Juva: WSOY.
lescents. Children’s Environments Quarterly, 7, Zuckerman, M. (1977). Development of a situation-
32–36. specific trait-state test for the prediction and
Stokols, D. (1995). The paradox of environmental psy- measurement of affective responses. Journal of Con-
chology. American Psychologist, 50, 821–837. sulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 513–523.

You might also like