You are on page 1of 10

Covert Categories and Folk Taxonomies

BRENT BERLIN
DENNIS E. BREEDLOVE
University of California, Berkeley
PETER H. RAVEN
Stanford University
Much of the recent work i n ethnoscience has been concerned with the nature of folk taxonomies, a n often
stated definition of which r e p i r e s that &folk taxa be monolexemically labeled. This paper offers evi-
dence that unlabeled categories may also be of crucial taxonomic significance, and we feel that it is in-
appropriate to treat such categories apart from the named taxonomic entities of the system. More im-
portanUy, evidence presented indicates that by recognizing unnamed taxa one may gain an understand-
ing of the structure of a particular semantic domain that is actually obscured if one focuses solely on
lexically labeled units.

MUCH of the productive work in ethno-


science today derives from collabora-
tive research in certain areas of ethnobiology
able, however, whether “ample evidence” is
actually available to support his position. The
purpose of the present paper is in part to cor-
(see Colby 1966, Sturtevant 1964 for a survey rect this deficiency.
of these materials). It is often found that the
semantic structures uncovered by such re- TZELTAL PLANT TAXONOMY
search are hierarchic in their formal char- I n our research in Tzeltal botanical ethnog-
acteristics. Thus they resemble the classifica- raphy,’ we have found i t possible to deter-
tory structures conventionally employed in mine with a high degree of reliability the major
the biological sciences and have generally outlines of the named taxonomic structure of
been termed (jolk) taxonomies (Conklin 1962a, the plant world for Tzeltal speakers (Berlin,
1962b, 1964; Durbin 1966; Frake 1961, 1962; Breedlove and Raven in preparation). In the
Gregg 1954; Kay 1966; Lounsbury 1964; course of this work, we have discovered many
Romney n.d.). meaningful and culturally revealing categories
Most writers agree on a definition of a folk related by inclusion that are not convention-
taxonomy similar to the following: “A system ally, monolexemically labeled. I n fact, many of
of monolexemically labelled folk segregates re- these categories receive no habitual linguistic
lated by hierarchic inclusion . . .” (Conklin designations of any sort. We feel that it is in-
1962a: 128). But must the segregates, or cate- appropriate to treat such categories separately
gories, included in such taxonomies necessarily from the named taxonomic entities of the sys-
be monolexemically labeled? Or is Keesing cor- tem. More importantly, we shall present evi-
rect in asserting “If we insist that the descrip- dence that by recognizing unnamed taxa,
tive units of an ethnography be lexically la- one may gain an understanding of the struc-
belled, we are likely to arrive at a very limited ture of a particular semantic domain that is
sort of description: an ethnography of how actually obscured if one focuses solely on
people talk about what they do, not what they lexically labeled units.
do or expect each other to do. . . . There is A t the highest level of the Tzeltal plant
ample evidence that expectations and distinc- taxonomy, there is no monolexemic taxon, or
tions need not be directly mapped in language” “unique beginner,” in which all other Tzeltal
(1966 :23)? plant taxa are included. Nevertheless, i t has
I n questioning the utility of restricting been possible to establish, by the procedures
ethnographic description to labeled categories, outlined below, that animals and plants are
Keesing has pointed the way toward a reex- distinguished as two separate unnamed classes
amination of one of the most fundamental by Tzeltal speakers.a I n the absence of any
assumptions of ethnoscience. It is question- “unique beginner,” the highest level in Tzeltal
plant taxonomy is represented by four major
Accepted for publication March 13. 1967. plant-class lexemes in which some eighty per-
290
[BERLIN, BREEDLOVE, & RAVEN] Folk Taxonomies 291

%(i=l,Z,. .., 194)indicatesaspedfictaxondominatedbyfe?.


For i<138, xi is immediately preceded byte?. For 139 Si1194, riin not immediately preceded by Is?.
yi(i = 1,Z... ,28) indicates a non-speafictaxon immediately p d e d by fc?.
9

*.(i = 1,2..
3 .
,r) indicatesa taxon in direct cdrust with to?.
FIGURE1. Schematic representation of the Tzeltal plant taxon le? (trees) and its included members
indicating the paucity of midlevel named taxa.

cent of all Tzeltal plant names are included. I n and Eikinib do not in themselves indicate their
addition to and coordinate with these major inclusion in hihte?, although these plants are
plant classes are a series of taxa that are for the consistently considered as kinds of hihte? by
most part botanically unusual when judged Tzeltal speakers.
by the morphological criteria that define The remaining three major classes, wumal,
the four major classes: te? (trees), ?ak' (vines), ?uk, and ?ak' show a similar paucity of non-
?ak (grasses), and wumal (herbs). These minor specific taxa.
classes are considered to be separate, un-
affiliated groups, and include, for example, I 'et
certain epiphytes, cacti, agaves, and bamboos.
Within these coordinate major and minor hi&* (oak)
plant classes are grouped all of the Tzeltal
specijc taxa, i.e., those taxa which include no
other members. There are, however, very few
midlevel plant categories in the taxonomy that
are lexemically labeled. We have selected the
major plant class te?, as described by one of
our 50 informants, to illustrate this general
lack of midrange named taxa.
It will be noted in Figure 1 that the taxon
te? immediately includes no less than 166 mem- ----
-
bers ( X I , XZ, . , ZISS; y ~ ,ya, - -
, ~ z s ) . Ex-
actly 138 of the taxa in this contrast set, or 83 Botanical ranges of each spxific taxon:
percent, are specific taxa. The remaining 28 Zibinib: Qiicrcus OcalnwrngnSiS Trel ,Qucrcus mexicam Ilumb.
& Bonpl , & Quacur supotifoli0 Liebm.
terms of the contrast set are nonspecific, each sakyok: Quncus cundiC0n.s Nee. Qiicrciis CrassiJolia Humb. &
immediately including from two to seven spe- Bonpl. in part.
cific taxa. A t no point does one find subhierar- k ' m d hikle?: Quercus srgooinrsis Liebm., Quncus Polymmpha
chies that exceed more than two levels in Schlecht. & Cham.
cu?put hiklo?: Qusrcw)Juncularis Neb, Quncus rugosa N d ,
depth. The total number of specific taxa in & Qunnrs crasrijolio Humb. & Bonpl. in part.
this class, for this informant, is 194. Cis hihlc?: Qucrcus cmrugda Hook.
An example of a midlevel, nonspecific taxon
is hihte? (oak), as seen in Figure 2. Figure 2. An example of the named midlevel taxon
The linguistic structure of the names sakyok hihte? (oak) and its included members.
292 American Anthropologist [70, 1968
&’

A&R\
?ac’amle?
XI m
?akolc?cs Pahoh
xl ?dEd
Yl ?&tat
24 €’if ?ararCndc?
XY 9ckmonef
3

/I \
%am
XI 7ih#’allc9
21 XI - . .,
?iSimtc~, xU8 3,

/I\
x119 2140 ’ * ’ Z1U $146 2146 XI41

E Portion of the contrast set immediately included in the taxon le?. Horizontal
F I G ~ 3.
ordering of the coordinate taxa is arbitrarily alphabetical.

COVERT MIDLEVEL CATEGORIES would then have a high information content


with considerable psychological r e l e ~ a n c e . ~
It is possible, of course, to accept the struc- They would proceed directly from the special-
ture in Figure 1 as displaying all of the cul- purpose, highly specific taxonomy of our in-
turally obligatory inclusion relationships formants. (Our use of “general purpose” vs.
among the relevant taxa of the class le? and to “special purpose” taxonomy is outlined more
proceed to other problems. Such a decision fully in Berlin, Breedlove and Raven, 1966:
would depend, in part, on one’s definition of a 275.)
taxonomy. Thus Conklin, in his classic study
of Hanunbo ethnobotany (1954), describes TESTING THE HYPOTHESIS
a similarly shallow plant taxonomy, but goes I n general, we set out to determine the ex-
on to note that other “midgroupings of plants istence of culturally significant subgroupings
are made, of course, but not according to a by employing several field procedures. The
structured terminologically-identifiable sys- first consisted of simple ethnographic observa-
tem” (1954:97). tions and recordings of informants’ comments
If such actual or potential midgroupings of on plants in natural contexts. During the pro-
plant taxa are ignored, however, i t becomes cess of botanical collecting of some 10,000
difficult if not impossible to develop a rationale specimens with native informants, much evi-
for horizontally ordering the individual plant dence was accumulated that suggested that
taxa within each of the many-membered con- some plants in the same named contrast set
trast sets. Let us return our focus to a portion were more closely related, cognitively, than
of the contrast set immediately included in others. These data included discussions of
te7 to clarify this point. If one relies solely on ethnobotanical features of certain groups of
the relationships revealed by the taxonomic plants, visual demonstrations of similar plants,
classification of the named taxa, the horizontal importance as food, herbs, firewood, and so on.
ordering of the immediately included items is Such observations, while sporadic and anec-
necessarily arbitrary. The order could pre- dotal in character, were important checks on
sumably be alphabetical, as seen in Figure 3. the results of the methods of grouping dis-
The items might also be arranged in terms of cussed below. Thus, the many hours of in-
their relationships as displayed by the general- formal discussion with informants in the field
purpose Linnaean system of classification. were extremely helpful in providing insights
Such an arrangement would clearly not be the into what might or might not prove to be a po-
most ethnographically relevant one possible. tential ethnobotanical semantic dimension.‘
One alternative to the arbitrary ordering of A second method we have found useful in
coordinate taxa derives from the intuition, searching for possible subgroupings within
mentioned above, that unlabeled, midlevel contrast sets of large numbers was to deter-
categories exist for our informants. If such mine the extent to which informants sub-
categories can be shown to exist, one might divided lists of plant names. Instruction con-
expect that smaller subsets of terms within a cerning the task of grouping was accom-
named contrast set would be conceptually plished as follows. Several names of plants
grouped together. These unnamed subsets and animals, written on separate slips of paper,
BERLIN, BREEDLOVE, & RAVEN] Folk Taxonomies 293
were presented to an informant. He was then tures. The first was the so-called kiads-test,
instructed to read the names on each slip and used by Romney and D’Andrade (1964) in
to put into separate groups those that were their analysis of American kinship data and
“most like one other.” The Tzeltal interroga- first employed ethnographically by D’Andrade
tion was as follows: (n.d.) in his early Tzeltal ethnobotanical work.
bdik sbil ya shun sba sok The procedure requires informants to specify
(Which names group with one another?) which item in a set of three is “most different”
from the others. The results, when run for all
Informants had no problem in grouping these logical triads in a set of terms, provide some in-
names correctly, animal names being placed dication of the total cognitive similarity of
in one set, plant names in another. These data, each term to all others in the set. This proce-
along with observation in natural contexts, in- dure is clearly restricted to sets of relatively
dicated the existence of an unnamed group, small size because of the larger and larger
“plants,” as mentioned above. numbers of triads that an informant must ex-
Following the grouping of plants and ani- amine as the number of items in the set in-
mals, a set of plant names was presented to creases.’J
each informant, some of the names belonging A second procedure in the discovery of
to one major Tzeltal class, some to another, characteristics of ethnobotanical relevance is
e.g., names for two kinds of trees as opposed to the construction of folk keys by informants.
the name for a particular kind of herb. In- Keys are routinely employed for biological pur-
structions were the same as those given for the poses and consist of a series of successive bin-
grouping of animal and plant names. As ex- ary divisions of a set of organisms, the charac-
pected, informants had no difficulty in group- teristics used for each division being specified,
ing names that belonged to the same major until each organism in the set has been distin-
Tzeltal plant class. guished from all others. I n our field procedure,
Once it became clear that the task was un- an informant was presented with a set of plant
derstood, the names of the immediately in- names that he had earlier isolated as being
cluded taxa of each major class name, written cognitively similar to one another. He was
on slips of paper, were presented to informants then asked to construct a key for this set of
with instructions to read through the lists and organisms, accounting for all the included
place in separate piles those names which ap- forms. By this means, the informant was re-
plied to plants that were judged to be similar quired to verbalize all of the conceptual dis-
to one another. We found that such subgroup- tinctions he has utilized in making the divi-
ings of closely semantically related items sions. Such keys provide useful data when
within the same contrast set is easily accom- compared with the results obtained by other
plished by trained informants. We have also tests.
found the resulting sets to be highly reliable. Our third procedure consists of paired com-
The ability of native informants to perform parisons of all lexical items in a particular de-
such grouping tasks, in conjunction with the limited set of plant names. I n completing this
reliability of the resulting groups, is a good task the informant was requested to compare
indication of the psychological saliency of the all logical pairs in any set in terms of all the
classification.6 similarities and differences that he felt were
Our inquiry did not stop a t merely requiring relevant for any pair. Such characteristics as
informants to subgroup items within the same the manner of stem growth, size and shape of
contrast set of named specifics. We also found the stem and leaves, internode length, and
it possible to determine many of the concep- fruit size and shape have been utilized in these
tual features that were shared among plants discriminations. Responses obtained by this
within each set, as well as to determine certain method can later be converted into a standard
features that distinguished items within a set. notation for distinctive features that allows
The procedures outlined below also indicated the investigator to scan “componential defini-
that some of the first sets isolated by infor- tions]’ of a set of terms, and, as a consequence,
mants included yet other important, unlabeled bring together those terms that are most simi-
conceptual groupings. lar in terms of those features judged important
Having demonstrated the existence of such by the informant.
subgroupings, we used three main procedures All of these simple techniques can be em-
to investigate their defining conceptual fea- ployed by any intelligent] semiliterate infor-
294 American Anthropologist [70,1968
mant. To illustrate their use, we have selected TABLE 2. NUMBER OF TIMESANY Two ITEMS WERE
one informant’s subgrouping of a set of plant JUDGED “MOST SIMILAR’’ IN ANY TRIAD CO~WRISED
OF FIVECONCEPTUALLY RELATED “VINE” NAMES
names for “vines” (7ak’). This classification
was similar to that constructed by other in-
formants presented with the same situation boht cu mayil c’ol t’um
and is selected for illustrative purposes only.
This particular set of plants names was iso-
lated by the sorting procedure described earlier
and consisted of the following Tzeltal terms’:
bohE (large gourd) Lugenaria sucreria
(Mol.)Standl.
cu (bottle gourd) Logenaria sueraria
(Mol.)Standl. c’ol
c’ol (squash) Cucurbita peg0 L. -I __
t’um (pumpkin) Cucurbita moschata t’nim I
Duch.
mayil (perennial squash) Cucurbilaficifolia
Bouch6 readily that the ordering of plant names in the
key is identical in hierarchical structure to
There are ten possible triads for this set of that derived from the triads data.
forms. One informant, when presented with all The results of our third procedure, that of
logical triads, made the responses shown in paired comparisons for all items in the set, are
Table 1. The number of times any two lexemes shown in Table 4. Not all dimensions are
in the set were classed together (i.e., judged equally relevant for all terms in the set. Thus
most similar in any triad) is given in Table 2. dimension G, color of the meat of the fruit, is
Table 2 indicates that bohE and mayil were not relevant, and hence omitted, for the
never classed together in any triad. On the gourds bohE and cu. Fruit size (dimension F ) is
other hand, mayil was classed twice with c’01 relevant for bohE and GU, but these gourds
and twice with t’um; i.e., it was judged to be range over both of the values for the dimen-
equally similar to both. The pairs bohE-cu and sion (large and small). Consequently, the
c’ol-E’um were judged to be closest in three letter for the dimension of size appears with-
triads. out subscripts for bohE and CU.
The divisions made in a folk key con- We noted earlier that the number of shared
structed for the same set of plant names are features between two items in a set provides an
shown graphically in Figure 4.The conceptual index of their overall similarity as judged by
dimensions with their appropriate values ver- the informant. The number of shared features
balized for this key are indicated in Table 3. for every pair of items in the set is shown in a
Small letters with subscripts indicate values
for a particular dimension. It can be seen TABLE
3. CONCEPTUAL
DINENSIONS
UTILIZED
IN
TEE CONSTRUCTION
OF THE TZELTAL
FOLK
1. ALLPOSSIBLE TRIADS
TABLE IN SET OF
KEY GIVENIN FIGURE4
FIVESEMANTICALLYRELATED
TZELTALPLANT NAMES (A) “Kind of vine” [uniting feature for the total
set]: Herbaceous creepers with large leaves
and similar fruits and flowers.
1) baht cu
-
G’Ol
2) boht cu -
t’um (B) vine texture: b1 hairy
3) boht cu
-
may3 ba spiny
4, -
boht c’o1 t’um
5, -
boht 6’01 rnayil

7)
-
boht
-
GU
t’um
c’ol
mayil
t’um
(C) fruit shape: c1
c2
disk-shaped
elongated

8, -
cu c’ol
t’um
may2
mayil
(D) vine-leaf color: d~ blackish [relatively dark]
dn whitish [relatively white]
9)
10) C’OZ E’um -
may2
(E) flesh color of fruit: el white
cs yellow
Underlined terms w e n judged “most different” in each triad.
BERLIN, BREEDLOVE, & RAVEN] Folk Taxonomies 295
A

A
mayil, c’ol, E’um

\ (dz)
6’01, E’um

bohE

(large gourd)
C1c (CZ)

(bottle gourd)
L! LA
mayil

(perennial squash) (squash)


c ’01 E ’um

(pumpkin)
FIGURE4. Tzeltal folk key constructed for set of five conceptually related “vines.”

matrix in Table 5 for comparison with the equally similar to E’um and c’ol, sharing three
findings made by the triad and key methods of out of seven features with E’um and four out
evaluation. As might be expected, these three of seven features with c’ol. Finally, as our other
sets of data correspond closely. Thus bohE and data would suggest, t’um and 6’01 share five out
cu are similar in that they share five out of six of seven features, showing them to be con-
possible features, but bohE shares only one ceptually very similar.
feature with mayil, E’um, and 201, and cu From the botanical point of view, the Tzel-
shares only two with mayil and none (other tal treatment given this set is quite under-
than the uniting value of the set) with E’um or standable. First, the subset of five Tzeltal
c’oZ. The squash mayil is approximately names, although not included in a named

TABLE4. FOLKFEATURE OF TZELTAL


DEFINITIONS PLANT NAMESOF FIVECONCEPTUALLY
RELATED
“VINES”AS COMPILED
FROM PAIREDCO~~~ARISONS
-.

Dimensions ValULs

(A) stem texture a1 hairy; a2 spiney


(B) leaf texture bl hairy; b2 spiney
(C) flower color c1 white; ct yellow
(D) fruit shape dl disk-like;dn elongated
(E) leaf color el relatively light; cz relatively dark
(I;) fruit size j1large;jz small
( G ) color of meat of fruit g, yellow; g2 white

Dejinilions o j planl names


boht
CU
mayil
t’um
C’Ol
296 American Anthropologist [70,1968
TABLE5. MAT- INDICATING NUMBER OF SHARED the subtribe Benincasinae, whereas Cucurbita
FEATURES FOR ALL LOGICAL PAIRSOF PLANT belongs to a different subtribe, Cucurbitinae.
NAaaEs DEFINEDIN TABLE 4 It is interesting now to compare the taxo-
nomic structure of this subset when one re-
bohE GU mayil t’um c’ol stricts it to labeled taxa, on the one hand, and
when one includes empirically validated un-
named taxa, on the other. It is clear that the
amount of culturally revealing information dis-
played in Figure 6 is significantly greater than
that in Figure 5.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
To summarize, we have presented empirical
C’lll evidence that unlabeled taxa do exist for Tzel-
tal speakers. The nodes of the hierarchy gener-
Figuresinparentheses indieate potential number of dimension8 ated by our eliciting procedures are not arbi-
on which two plants might be compared. trarily spaced and not permutable in their
vertical or sequential ordering; therefore they
Tzeltal taxon, does form a morphologically clearly imply a taxonomic structure. This
coherent group. Of all members of the family structure is not an arbitrary key generated in
Cucurbitaceae familiar to Tzeltal speakers, terms of culturally irrelevant oppositions of
these five are the only members of the tribe our own invention (cf. Conklin 1962a:135-
Cucurbiteae (Jeffrey 1962). They have larger 136; 1962b:90-91; 1964:39-41). Much addi-
flowers than any other member of the family tional evidence that we have accumulated
known to these people, and are the only ones supports the existence of these unnamed taxa
having large fruits with tough rinds. Within and clearly points to their cultural validity,
this group, the first obligatory division cor- both in the particular example discussed in de-
responds to a recognition of the major ‘differ- tail and in many analogous instances. (The
ences between the group comprizing boht and taxon te7, for example, now is believed to im-
cu and that including mayil, t’um, and c’ol. mediately include no less than thirty un-
The gourds boht and cu both belong to the named taxa.)
species Lagenaria siceraria, although their
fruits are very different and provide the basis
for their being given distinctive Tzeltal names.
---
‘ak’ (vines)
The other three items in the subset-mayil,
Pum, and c’ol-represent different species of
the genus Cucurbita. Lagenaria is a member of Tribe Cuawbiteac

Lagenaria Cucurbita

W’ (vines) Cucurbita pep0

bohE GU mayil 6’01

FIGURE5. Taxonomic structure that indicates FIGURE6. Taxonomic structure that indicates both
labeled taxa only for a subset of “vines.” labeled and unlabeled taxa for a subset of “vines.”
BERLIN, BREEDLOVE, & RAVEN] Folk Taxonomies 297
There is a widely held view that the “con- position. Thus, conceptually, each of our mul-
cept of the genus is as old as folk-science it- timembered “named” contrast sets are in actu-
sell” (Davis & Heywood 1963:103). It seems ality subdivided into several more cognitively
clear that this and other midlevel categories in amenable contrast sets, many of which are in-
taxonomic hierarchies have grown up syn- cluded in unnamed taxa. By recognizing un-
thetically, by the grouping of kinds of plants labeled taxa, our data show clearly that no
and animals. Specifically, the labeling of ini- contrast set exceeds 64 items, while most con-
tially unnamed categories that prove t o tain considerably fewer taxa than ten.
have cultural validity and predictiveness I n conclusion, we would like to urge the test-
about the included forms would appear to ing of superficially shallow taxonomic hierar-
have been a principal route to the verbaliza- chies for the presence of unlabeled midlevel
tion of more and more complex hierarchies. We taxa. We believe that our evidence shows that
have already considered a probable later step such taxa cannot be ignored as insignificant. I n
in this process in the example of hihtel. I n the the present paper we have outlined several
development and rationalization of formal procedures by which the investigation of such
Linnaean taxonomy, genus, family, order and hierarchies can be carried out, but doubtless
many other midlevel categories have been de- many others can and eventually will be de-
fined and named a s a means of reflecting in- vised. A recognition of the existence and a n
creasing amounts of information about the evaluation of the taxonomic status of such un-
organisims being classified. It is extremely in- labeled categories will usually be found to lead
structive to examine the structure of a folk t o a more psychologically revealing and cul-
taxonomy such as that of the Tzeltal, in which turally meaningful description of the under-
these midlevel categories are present in an in- lying conceptual structure of a particular do-
cipient, unlabeled form. I n fact, these findings main, a structure that may otherwise remain
help us to better understand the development obscure.*
and structure of our own “general-purpose”
taxonomy. NOTES
Our data can also be interpreted as having 1This research has been financed by the National
some bearing on the somewhat neglected hy- Science Foundation under the following grants:
NSF GS 383, “Comparative ethnobotany of two
pothesis of Miller (1956) and Wallace (1961) Tzeltal speaking communities,” A. Kimball Romney
concerning the capacity of Homo sapiens to and Peter H. Raven, Co-principal investigators;
store and process information. Wallace’s hy- NSF GS 1183, “Studies in Tzeltal botanical eth-
pothesis states that “irrespecfive of race, cul- nograpby,” Brent Berlin and Peter H. Raven, Co-
rincipa investigators.This support is gratefully ac-
ture, or evolutionary level, culturally institu-
tionalized folk taxonomies will lzot contain more
E nowledged. The work is being carried out primarily
in the Tzeltal-speaking municipio of Tenejapa,
than 26 entities and consequently will not require Chiapas, Mexico, although new research is being
more than six orthogonally related binary dimen- undertaken in several additional Tzeltal dialects. A
general statement of the physical characteristics of
sionsjor the definitions of all of the terms” (Wal- the area is given in Berlin, Breedlove, and Raven
lace 1961:462). The maximum number of en- (1966). Relevant bibliographic material on linguis-
tities that can be contained in a space of six tic, ethnographic, or botanical work completed or
binary dimensions is sixty-four and presum- now in progrese in highland Chiapas may be ob-
tained by writing the authors.
ably no folk taxonomy should contain more ZThere is some evidence that the noun+noun
than this number of folk taxa. compounds tepak’ (Ic’ [tree]+%ak’ [vine]) and tan-
However, i t is clear that Wallace is not re- balam (tan [snaRc]~b&m [tiger])function as lexical
items indicating lants” and “animals” respec-
ferring to the folk taxonomy in toto, but to any
particular contrast set contained within i t
tE
tively. A re ort to is effect is given in Metzger and
Williams 6966). We have been able to verify
(Colby 1966:lS). (See Wallace’s [1961:462] this finding for two Tzeltal informants. However,
definition of a taxonomy as “a group of sym- long periods of elicitation with many additional in-
bols . . . which denote mutually exclusive but formants lead us to question the widespread appli-
cability of these forms with the general meanings
jointly exhaustive subsets of referents within a “plants” and “animals.” The vast majority of our
set denoted by a cover symbol [immediately informants indicate that tepaak’ more appropriately
including taxon?].”) designates “forest” or “woods” while tadalum re-
Should this interpretation be correct, i.e., a fers only to a small subset of large four-legged mam-
mals.
maximum number of 64 items in a n y particular a An interest in unlabeled categories is not new to
contrast set, our data concerning unnamed ethnoscience,although the taxonomic signiliurnceof
categories would tend to support Wallace’s such categoriesha8 not been systematicallyexplored.
298 American Anthropologist [70,1968
D’Andrade (n.d.) in one of the first papers on Tzeltal 6 I t is assumed, of course, that the informant can
botanical ethnogra hy searched for unnamed cate- read and write his native language with relative
gories in another fialect of Tzeltal. His intuitions ease. Much of the most productive work in ethno-
were stated as follows: science de ends, in fact, on the use of literate in-
Are then auh-gmupings in the taxonomic system at a lower
formants b e Metzger and Williams 1963a, 1963b,
1966; Berlin in press; Berlin, Kay, Metzger and
level of contrast than the lowest set of general terms, hut at a Williams n.d., and many other unpublished manu-
higher level of contrast than the most specific terms? . . .Is it
really true that no further subdivisions are consistently made
scripts of the work in progress in Chiapas growing
since there are no [labels] for these subdivisions. . . . It would
out of the Chicago, Harvard, and Stanford projects
Mem likely that these large groups of plants arc to some ex-
in that area over the last ten years). Clearly, con-
siderable effort on the part of the ethnographer is re-
tent subdivided, or at least cross-indexed, to make for a more
efficient mapping [D’Andrade n.d.:l].
quired in develo ing a practical phonemic orthogra-
phy, training inirmants to use it, and making cer-
D’Andrade’s conclusions were tentative, and recog- tain that many quasi-experimental tasks are under-
nized as such, but there were clear indications that stood. We feel the results far outweigh the loss of re-
midlevel categories could be isolated. These cate- search time. The use of skilled informants has drawn
gories appeared to be determined primarily in terms criticism from certain areas of cultural anthropology.
of sets of certain morphological features of individual There are those who look askance at the reliance on
plant taxa. highly trained native assistants for fear that their
Whiting (1939) presents some tentative evidence training somehow effects the validity of the results.
for unnamed categories of plants in Hopi. French We are aware of these problems and do not take
(1956) describes Wasco folk botany as lacking mid- them lightly. However, without such training an in-
level categories of plants but states that “implicit formant will be unable to make available to the in-
categories” may be present. Bulmer and Tyler’s (in vestigator many of his native intuitions about his own
press) work on frogs and Acheson’s (n.d.) materials culture, intuitions that are perhaps forever outside
on birds are recent examples dealing with ethnozoo- the grasp of even the most perceptive ethnographer.
logical unlabeled taxa. Black (1968) argues for A. Rimball Romney is currently developing a
the presence of an unlabeled taxon in her recent computer program that will allow significantly large
work on Ojibwa. Goodenough (1956) explicitly iden- numbers of items to be grouped into smaller con-
tifies unnamed categories of a paradigmatic nature in ceptual subsets. After the research reported here was
his treatment of Trukese kinship and later (1965) completed, we learned of a field procedure de-
presents evidence for unlabeled classes with taxo- veloped by William Geoghegan that allows one to
nomic significance for American English kinship utilize the triads mentioned on very large numbers
terms. After this paper had been drafted, Paul Kay of items without the aid of mechanical processing.
brought to our attention a personal communication ’ It so happens that the Tzeltal forms in this set
from William Geoghegan that is analogous to correspond fairly closely to recognized botanical
Keesing’s (1966:23) remarks and identical to the species, with the obvious exception of Lagenaria
point a t issue in this pa er. Geoghegan, taking issue siceraria, which is overdifferentiated. Such one-to-
with a point develop2 in Kay (1966:22), notes: one correspondence is rather rarely the case (Berlin,
“Why must every node in a taxonomy correspond Breedlove, and Raven 1966). The English and Latin
to a lexeme? Analytically this may be so, due to the glosses for the Tzeltal names are approximations
way in which analysis proceeds. But cognitively only and apply specifically to the forms of these
(my bias again) there is no reason to suppose this, exceedingly variable species most familiar to Tzeltal
and it probably isn’t true in fact.” speakers.
Finally, several recent papers have shown that im- I n Berlin, Breedlove, and Raven (1966) we re-
portant domains such as color (Conklin 1955) and ported that Lagmaria siceraria corresponded to the
categories of eating (Landar 1964; Berlin 1967) need Tzeltal taxa k’alk’ad bohE, sepsep boht, cu, and
not be labeled linguistically by a single mono- E’ahk’o7. This data represented a normalization for
lexemic head-term. all informants consulted. The illustrative data uti-
The discovery of semantic features, or “char- lized for the present a er were derived from an in-
acters,” that are utilized by native speakers in mak- formant who lacks suicLsses of boht (the shape vari-
ing judgments of similarity is one of the major goals etals k’alk’al bohE and sepsep boht not being recog-
of ethnoscientific research. We do not believe, how- nized) and does not grou t’ahk’o? as a member of
ever that a prior knowledge of these features is a the set of conceptually reited gourds and squashes.
prerequisite to the recognition of semantically re- We speculate that its exclusion from the set is due to
lated sets of items. Informants, in the field and in the fact that the individuals of L q m a r i a siceraria
quasi-experimental situations, continually volun- that are called t’ahk’o? are “genetic throwbacks”
teered the fact that items “A, B, and C go together,” that are not easily recognizable as gourds.
or “are compamons.’) Consequently we feel that as * W e greatly a preciate the criticisms of H. C.
an operational procedure it is desirable a t the outset Conklin, George &wgill, Marshall Durbin, William
to request that informants make judgments in terms Geoghegan, Richard W. Holm, Terrence Kaufman,
of overall similarity without specifying some set of Paul Kay, A. Kimball Romney, David Schneider,
specific features. Berlin (in press) and Berlin and and William Sturtevant. Kay has been especially
Romney (1964) offer discussions of these rocedures helpful in the preparation of the final draft.
in reference to other kinds of linguistic &a. Rom-
ney and D’Andrade (1964) discuss this problem in REFERENCES CITED
reference to the applicability of the method of triads- ACHESON, NICHOLAS H.
testing discussed earlier. Sokal (1966) presents a n.d. The ethnozoology of the Zinacantan In-
lucid account of problems dealing with over-all dians. ms.
similarity in reference to biological classification in B E R ~BRENT ,
general. I n press Tzeltal numeral classifiers: a study in
BERLIN, BREEDLOVE, & RAVEN] Folk Taxonomies 299
ethnographic semantics. Janua Linguarum 70 FRENCH,
DAVID
(series practica). The Hague, Mouton and 1956 An exploration of WWO ethnoscience.
Company. Yearbook of the American Philosophical Soci-
1967 Categories of eating in Tzeltal and Na- ety. Pp. 224-226.
vaho. International Tournal of American GOODENOUGE, WARDH.
Linguistics 33:l-6. “ 1956 Componential analysis and the study of
BERLIN, BRENT, DENNIS 15. BREEDLOVE,AND meaning. Language 32: 195-216.
PETERH. RAVEN 1%5 Yankee kinshi terminology: a problem in
1966 Folk taxonomies and biological classifica- com onential anagsis. In Formal Semantic
tion. Science 154:273-275. A n a b i . E. A. Hammel, ed. American Anthro-
In preparation. Studies in Tzeltal botanical pologist 67(5), pt. 2:259-287.
ethnography: principles of classification. GREGG,J. R.
BERLIN,BRENT,PAULKAY,DUANEMETZGER, AND 1954 The language of taxonomy: an application
GERALD WILLIAMS of symbolic logic to the study of classificatory
n.d. A proposal for a summer institute of eth- systems. New York, Columbia University Press.
nography. Unpublished ms. JEFFERY, C. E.
BERLIN,BRENT,AND A. KIMBALL ROMNEY 1%2 Notes on Cucurbitaceae, including a p n r
1964 The descriptive semantics of Tzeltal nu- posed new classification of the family. Kew
meral classifiers. In Transcultural studies in Bulletin 15:337-371.
cognition. A. Kimball Romney and Roy Good- KAY,PAUL
win D’Andrade, eds. American Anthropologist 1966 Comment [on Colby 19661. Current An-
66(3), pt. 2:79-98. thropology 7 :20-23.
BLACK, MARY KEESING, ROGERM.
1968 Eliciting folk taxonomy in Ojibwa. In 1%6 Comment [on Colby 19661. Current An-
Cognitive anthropology. Steven Tyler, ed. thropology 7 :23.
New York, Holt, Rinehart & Winston. LANDAR, HERBERT
RULMER,RALPH,AND MICHAEL J. TYLER 1964 Seven Navaho verbs of eating. Interna-
I n press. Theclassification of frogs by the Karam tional Journal of American Linguistics 30:94-
of the Kaironk Valley, New Guinea. Journal of 96.
the Polvnesian Societv. LOUNSBWY, FLOYD G.
COLBY, B. N. 1964 The structural analysis of kinshipseman-
1966 Ethnographic semantics: a preliminary sur- tics. In Proceeding of the Ninth International
.
vey. Current AnthroDoloev
I_
7:3-32. Congress of Linguistics. Cambridge, M.I.T.
CONKL~N, HAROLD C. Press. Pp. 1073-1099.
1954 The relation of Hanun60 culture to the METZGER, DUANE AND GERALD E. WILLIAMS
plant world. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation in 1963a Tenejapa medicine I: the curer. South-
anthropology, Yale University. western Journalof Anthropology 19:216-234.
1955 Hanun60 color categories. Southwestern 1963b A formal ethnographic analysis of Tene-
Journal of Anthropology 11:339-344. ja Ladino weddings. American Anthropologist
1962a Lexicographical treatment of folk taxon- 6gO76-1101.
omies. In Problems in Lexicography. F. W. 1966 Some procedures and results in the study of
Householder and S. Saporta, eds. Indiana native categories: T d t a l “firewood.” Amer-
University Research Center in Anthropology, ican Anthropologist 6k389-407.
Folklore, and Linguistics Publication 21. Pp. MILLER,GEORGEA.
119-141. 1956 The maRical number seven, plus or minus
19621, Comment [on Frake 19621. In Anthropol- two: some limits on our capacity of processing
ogyand Human Behavior. T. Gladwin and W. C. information. Psychological Review 63 :81-97.
Sturtevant, eds. Washington, Anthropological ROMNEY, A. KR,U~ALL
Society of Washington. Pp. 86-91. n.d. Multidimensional scaling and semantic
1964 Ethnogenealogical method. In Explora- domains. Unpublished ms.
tions in Cultural Anthropology: Essay5 Pre- ROMNEY, A. KIMBALLAND ROY GOODWIN
sented to George Peter Murdock. W. H. Good- D’ANDRADE
enough, ed.New York, McGraw-Hill. Pp. 25-56. 1964 Cognitive aspects of English kin terms. In
D’ANDRADE, ROYG. Transcultural studies in cognition. A. Kimball
n.d. [Re ort on Aguacatenango Tzeltal ethno- Romney and Roy Goodwin D’Andrade, eds.
botanyfunpublished ms. American Anthropologist 6C(3), pt. 2:146-170.
DAVIS, P. H. AND V. H. HEYWOOD S o u , ROBERTR.
1963 Principles of angiosperm taxonomy. Edin- 1966 Numerical taxonomy. Scientific American
hurgh and London, Oliver and Boyd. 215(6):106-116.
DURBIN,MARSHALL STURTEVANT, WILLIAM c.
1966 The goals of ethnoscience. Anthropological 1964 Studies in ethnoscience. In Transcultural
Linguistics 8(8) :22-41. studies in copition. A. Kimball Romney and
FRAKE,CHARLES 0. Roy Goodwm D’Andrade, eds. American
1961 The diagnosis of disease among the Sub- Anthro ologist 66(3) pt. 299-131.
anun of Mindanao. American Anthropologist WALLACE, 1.F.C.
63:113-132. 1961 On being just complicated enough. Pro-
1962 The ethnographic study of cognitive sys- ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
tems. I n Anthropology and Human Behavior. 47 :45844.
T. Gladwin and W. C. Sturtevant. eds. Wash- WHITING,A. F.
ington, Anthropologid Society of Washington, 1939 Ethnobotanv of the HoDi. Museum of
Pp. 72-85, 91-93. Northern Arizons Bulletin 15.

You might also like