You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/235343547

Neuropsychological Profile in High Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders

Article  in  Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders · December 2012


DOI: 10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0

CITATIONS READS

53 1,345

5 authors, including:

Antonio Narzisi Filippo Muratori


Università di Pisa Università di Pisa
57 PUBLICATIONS   663 CITATIONS    252 PUBLICATIONS   3,736 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Sara Calderoni Franco Fabbro


IRCCS Stella Maris Foundation, Pisa, Italy University of Udine
82 PUBLICATIONS   862 CITATIONS    246 PUBLICATIONS   3,826 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Eating Disorders in the light of clinical and neurobiological findings, typical traits in adolescents with Anorexia Nervosa and brain morphometry studies View project

Traduzione di M-CHAT-R/F View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Cosimo Urgesi on 11 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


J Autism Dev Disord
DOI 10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0

ORIGINAL PAPER

Neuropsychological Profile in High Functioning Autism Spectrum


Disorders
Antonio Narzisi • Filippo Muratori •
Sara Calderoni • Franco Fabbro • Cosimo Urgesi

Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012

Abstract A comprehensive investigation of the neuro- reflect alterations in multiple cognitive domains in
psychological strengths and weaknesses of children with HFASD.
autism may help to better describe their cognitive abilities
and to design appropriate interventions. To this end we Keywords Autism spectrum disorders  NEPSY 
compared the NEPSY-II profiles of 22 children with high- Neuropsychology  Theory of Mind
functioning autism spectrum disorders (HFASD) with
those of 44 healthy control (HC) children 2:1 matched by
gender, age, race and education. Results showed that only Introduction
Visuospatial Processing was relatively spared in HFASD,
while deficits were observed in Attention and Executive Individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) exhibit
Functions, Language, Learning and Memory, and Senso- core impairments in the socio-communication domain and
rimotor Processing. Theory of Mind difficulties were have restricted or stereotyped patterns of behaviors. How-
observed in verbal tasks but not in the understanding of ever ASD vary greatly in the severity of their socio-
emotional contexts, suggesting that appropriate contextual communicative impairments (Lord 2011) and in cognitive
cues might help emotion understanding in HFASD chil- and language development. ASD with normal or high IQ
dren. These widespread neuropsychological impairments are referred to as high functioning autism (HFA) or high
functioning autism spectrum disorders (HFASD). This
latter term is often used to characterize the broader group
of children with HFA, Asperger syndrome and Pervasive
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this Developmental Disorders Not Otherwise Specified
article (doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0) contains supplementary
material, which is available to authorized users.
(PDDNOS) (Volker et al. 2010; Oliveras-Rentas et al.
2012; Scheeren et al. 2012). These individuals demonstrate
A. Narzisi  F. Muratori  S. Calderoni relative strengths in cognitive and language abilities (Klin
Division of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Stella Maris et al. 2000), though pragmatic language deficits are
Scientific Institute, University of Pisa, Via dei Giacinti, 2,
observed (Landa 2000). Thus, they share the two core
56018 Calambrone, Pisa, Italy
features of ASD: social-communication impairment and
A. Narzisi (&) circumscribed pattern of behavior and interest, which,
Division of Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Stella Maris together, contribute to their poor overall social competence
Scientific Institute, University of Pisa, Viale del Tirreno, 331,
(Attwood 2004).
56018 Calambrone, Pisa, Italy
e-mail: antonio.narzisi@inpe.unipi.it Several studies have shown that children with HFASD
are also impaired in different other cognitive functions
F. Fabbro  C. Urgesi (&) (Tsatsanis 2005), including deficits in motor functions
Department of Human Sciences, University of Udine
(Baranek et al. 2005), high-order language aspects, such as
and Eugenio Medea Scientific Institute, Via Margreth, 3,
33100 Udine, Italy semantics and pragmatics (Minshew et al. 1995; Tager-
e-mail: cosimo.urgesi@uniud.it Flusberg 2004), selected executive functions (Liss et al.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

2001; Ozonoff et al. 2004; Ozonoff and Jensen 1999), Narrative Memory were no longer significant after con-
memory for faces (Hauck et al. 1998). On the other hand, trolling for IQ.
there is a growing literature pointing to a relative sparing of Similar results were obtained in the validation study of
visual processing (Samson et al. 2012; Joseph et al. 2009; the NEPSY-II (Korkman et al. 2007). Indeed, HFASD
Ghaziuddin and Mountain-Kimchi 2004), although this is children presented lower scores in all cognitive domains,
an area of debate (see Edgin and Pennington 2005), but particularly severe difficulties in the measures of
selected aspects of memory (Minshew and Goldstein executive functions (sorting, cognitive flexibility, and
2001), and inhibitory functions (Ozonoff and Strayer auditory attention) and language. Moreover, verbal and
1997). These findings have contributed to a better knowl- visuospatial memory, visuomotor coordination, line orien-
edge of the cognitive characteristics of HFASD; however, tation discrimination and block constructions abilities were
the conclusions that can be drawn from these studies are also impaired in HFASD children. Since the latter tests
limited because they used different tests to measure single heavily rely on verbal abilities to understand the task
abilities or a subset of cognitive abilities in different instructions, it is likely that the deficits of HFASD children
samples of HFASD and control individuals. This makes it stem from a lack of language mediation. These findings
impossible to clarify the profile and the relative strengths suggest that the NEPSY can contribute to the neuropsy-
and weaknesses in the same sample, thus introducing an chological description of children with HFASD.
unknown and uncontrollable amount of variability to the However, previous studies did not control for the extent
findings. to which performance of HFASD children in a cognitive
The developmental neuropsychological battery NEPSY domain may be affected by weaknesses in other but in-
(Korkman et al. 1998; 2007) was developed following the tercorrelated domains independently from general cogni-
neuropsychological approach of Luria (1962), who con- tive abilities. The US (Korkman et al. 2007) and Italian
siders multiple brain systems at the base of cognitive (Urgesi et al. 2011) standardization of the NEPSY-II
functioning, and provides a comprehensive overview of the showed large inter-correlations between the NEPSY-II
child’s neuropsychological functioning. The NEPSY is a tests both within (in particular for Language tests) and
unique, co-normed, and multi-domain neuropsychological between domains (in particular for Language and Attention
battery designed specifically for preschoolers, children, and and Executive Function tests). These between-domain
adolescents. Its revised version, the NEPSY-II (Korkman inter-correlations in healthy individuals were probably
et al. 2007), includes measures of social perception abili- caused by the similarity between the required modality of
ties, thus allowing also the evaluation of the core deficits of stimulus presentation and response and by general effects
ASD children in comparison to their functioning in other of attention resources on task performance. These effects
cognitive domains. Until now, validation data of the may suggest the possibility that an impairment in a cog-
NEPSY (Korkman et al. 1998; Hooper et al. 2006) and nitive domain, for example language, may induce second-
NEPSY-II (Korkman et al. 2007) batteries have provided a ary impairment in other tasks, for example executive
comprehensive evaluation of the neuropsychological functions, that involve language mediation to perceive the
functions of HFASD children, and a few independent stimuli or to provide the response (Russel 1997; Russell
studies have used a selection of subtests of the NEPSY to et al. 1999; Joseph et al. 2005).
evaluate specific cognitive functions (Boneh et al. 2008; Here we aimed to provide a comprehensive description
Davidson et al. 2008; Joseph et al. 2005; Miniscalco et al. of the neuropsychological profile of children with HFASD
2007; Planche and Lemonnier 2011). using the NEPSY-II and to explore the possible relations
The validation study of the first version of the NEPSY between the impairments in different cognitive domains. In
(Korkman et al. 1998) reported that HFASD children pre- keeping with previous studies (Korkman et al. 1998, 2007;
sented lower scores than control individuals in the tests of Hooper et al. 2006), we expected to find impaired perfor-
executive functions (Tower), memory (Memory for Faces, mance by HFASD children in multiple cognitive domains,
Narrative Memory), language comprehension (Compre- with particular deficits in attention and executive functions
hension of Instructions), sensorimotor functions (Imitating and in language and social perception abilities. Impor-
Hand Positions, and Visuomotor Precision), and line ori- tantly, taking into account the mutual influence between
entation discrimination (Arrows). A reanalysis of these executive and language impairments, we tested whether or
data, however, controlling for the influence of IQ (Hooper not the executive deficits in HFASD are secondary to pri-
et al. 2006), showed further impairments of HFASD chil- mary deficits in the ability to use inner speech to control
dren in the subtests of Phonological Processing, Auditory and guide behaviors (Hughes and Russell 1993). Indeed, in
Attention and Response Set, and Speeded Naming. In keeping with the so called language mediation hypothesis
contrast, the differences between HFASD and control (Russel 1997), the deficits of HFASD children in executive
children in the Comprehension of Instructions and functions should not be consistent when the relative impact

123
J Autism Dev Disord

of deficits in language abilities is partialled out. In a similar Version of NEPSY-II (Korkman et al. 2007; Urgesi et al.
vein, we expected to find that, when the influence of 2011). NEPSY-II is a battery of neuropsychological tests
impairments in the attention and executive functions and in specifically developed for the evaluation of 3–16 years old
language abilities is controlled for, the impairments in children in 6 cognitive domains: Attention and Executive
visuospatial abilities should be attenuated. Functions, Language, Memory and Learning, Sensorimotor
Functions, Social Perception, and Visuospatial Processing.
Here we used the 31 tests of the Italian version of NEPSY-
Methods II (Urgesi et al. 2011) that can be administered to children
aged 5–16 years.
Participants
Attention and Executive Functions
Twenty-two boys with a diagnosis of HFASD were con-
secutively recruited at the Division of Child Neurology and Six tests are comprised in this domain. The Visual Attention
Psychiatry of the Stella Maris Scientific Institute (Univer- assesses visual scanning and selective visual attention abil-
sity of Pisa) over a 24 months period. Inclusion criteria ities. The participant is required to scan an A3 sheet and mark
were: (a) diagnosis of Autistic Disorder (AD) or Pervasive specified visual targets among several similar distracters
Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (PDD- (face drawings). The Design Fluency test accesses behav-
NOS) according to DSM-IV-TR. The diagnosis was certi- ioral productivity. The participant is required to generate
fied by a senior child psychiatrist (F.M.) with more than unique designs by connecting up to five dots presented in a
25 years of experience in assessing individuals with Aut- structured or random array. In the Auditory Attention and
ism; (b) age between 5 and 16 years; (c) a Full Scale Response Set test the participant listens to a series of words
Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ), evaluated through WISC-III, and points to the appropriate circle when he hears a target
greater than 80; (d) availability for carrying on several word. Auditory Attention is aimed at evaluating selective
neuropsychological evaluation sessions. Exclusion criteria and sustained auditory attention and requires to point to one
were presence or history of any other axis I mental disor- circle when one color name is heard, while ignoring other
der; a history of traumatic brain injury or any other neu- color names. Response Set is aimed at assessing the ability to
rological illness. The boys who composed the final sample shift and maintain a new and complex response set involving
had a diagnosis of AD (n = 10) or PDDNOS (n = 12). inhibition of automatic response and alternating between
The mean age of patients was 9.77 (SD: 3.65; range: matching or contrasting stimuli. Inhibition evaluates the
5–16); mean VIQ was 96.14 (SD: 13.73; range: 80–131); ability to inhibit automatic responses and shift between
mean PIQ was 103.36 (SD: 16.28; range: 72–141); mean congruent or incongruent responses during naming of visual
Total IQ: 99.09 (SD: 14.23; range: 82–126). Four patients stimuli. The Naming condition requires to name the shape of
were left-handed. squares and circles or the up or down direction of arrows; the
The neuropsychological performance of HFASD was Inhibition condition requires to provide the opposite naming
compared to that of a group of 44 healthy control (HC) response on the same stimuli; the Switching condition
children matched 2:1 for age, gender, race, and education. requires to provide the congruent or the incongruent naming
HC children were selected randomly from the standardiza- response according to the color of the stimulus. The Clocks
tion sample of the Italian Version of NEPSY-II (Urgesi et al. test evaluates planning and organization of visuospatial
2011). Two controls were sampled for each HFASD patient perception and response and the concept of time by requiring
to improve the efficiency of the comparison of the control to read or draw the times on analogical or digital clocks.
sample and the estimation of the expected performance at Animal Sorting requires to sort cards into two groups of four
each age (Haviland et al. 2007). Five controls were left- cards each using various self-initiated sorting criteria.
handed. Before inclusion into the standardization sample,
controls were screened with standardized neuropsychologi- Language
cal tests of verbal (Marini et al. 2007) and non-verbal general
cognitive abilities (Goodenough 1975; Raven 1954) in order The Language domain includes six tests. The Compre-
to exclude deficits of verbal or visuospatial abilities. All hension of Instruction evaluates non-contextual language
HFASD and HC children were native Italian speakers. and asks to indicate a given sequence of crosses and circles
of different colors in response to oral instructions of
Neuropsychological Evaluation increasing syntactic complexity. In the Speeded Naming
test, the participant is required to name as quickly as
HFASD and HC underwent a full neuropsychological possible a series of letters and numbers, thus allowing to
evaluation (conducted by A.N. and C.U.) using the Italian evaluate the velocity of lexical retrieval and production.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

The Phonological Processing test assesses phonemic evaluates motor dexterity in repetitive finger movements and
awareness with tasks requiring the phonological segmen- motor programming in sequences of finger movements.
tation at the level of syllables and phonemes (substitute a Imitation of Hand Postures evaluates ideomotor praxis
syllable or a phoneme in a given word to create a new abilities and requires to imitate, with the dominant and non
word). The Word Generation test requires the participant to dominant hand, various meaningful and meaningless hand/
produce, within 1 min for each category, as many words as finger positions demonstrated by the examiner. The Manual
possible on the basis of a semantic or phonological crite- Motor Sequences test assesses manual motor coordination by
rion. The Repetition of Non-sense Words test assesses requiring to imitate for five times a series of rhythmic hand
phonological encoding and decoding by requiring partici- movement sequences demonstrated by the examiner.
pants to repeat nonsense words presented aloud. The
Oromotor Sequences test assesses oromotor coordination Social Perception
and requires to repeat onomatopoeic sounds and tongue
twisters for five times. Two NEPSY-II tests, one with two separate parts, evaluate
social perception. In the verbal items of the Theory of Mind test,
Memory and Learning participants are provided with verbal or pictorial descriptions of
some social situations and are, then, asked questions about
Seven NEPSY-II tests evaluate verbal and visuospatial those situations which require the understanding of the char-
memory abilities. Four tests have both immediate and acters’ point of view. The contextual items of the Theory of
delayed recall conditions to evaluate short- and long-term Mind test evaluate the capacity to understand how certain
memory retrieval. The Memory for Faces test assesses face emotions are linked to given social situations and to recognise
discrimination and recognition. The participant looks at a correctly the emotions that the various social settings generate.
series of faces in an incidental learning procedure and then, Affect Recognition assesses the ability to recognise emotional
in immediate and delayed recall conditions, is shown three expressions (happy, sad, anger, fear, disgust, and neutral) from
photographs at a time from which he selects a face previ- photographs of children’s faces in four different tasks. Partic-
ously seen. Word List Interference assesses verbal working ipants need to match the two faces expressing the same emo-
memory, repetition, and word recall following interference. tions among three or more alternatives.
The participant is presented with two series of words and
asked to repeat each sequence following its presentation. Visuospatial Processing
Then, he recalls each series in the order of presentation.
The Memory for Designs test assesses memory for the form Six NEPSY-II tests are comprised. Design Copying
and spatial location of novel visual material. The partici- assesses constructional abilities asking to copy two-
pant is shown a series of non-figural abstract shapes placed dimensional geometric figures of increasing complexity.
on a grid which are, thereafter, removed from view. In Block Construction assesses the visuospatial and visuo-
immediate or delayed recall conditions after each presen- motor ability to reproduce three-dimensional constructions
tation, he is required to select the appropriate shapes from a from two-dimensional drawings. Picture Puzzles assess
set of cards and place them onto an empty grid in the visual discrimination and the perception of part-whole
positions in which he had seen them in the sample stimu- relationships. The participant identifies on a large picture
lus. List Memory assesses long term verbal memory for the sections from which each of four small pictures, pre-
lists of words. The participant is read a list of words several sented separately, were taken. The Geometric Puzzles test
times and is asked to recall them. The Memory for Names assesses the ability to identify and match geometric shapes
test assesses visuo-verbal association learning and requires of increasing complexity (design recognition) as well as
participants to learn the name of children drawn on eight mental rotation abilities. The Route Finding test assesses
cards. Narrative Memory assesses story recall abilities. The the knowledge of visual-spatial relations and directionality
participant listens to a story and is asked to repeat it. He is and the ability to transfer a route from a simple schematic
then asked questions to elicit missing details from his or map to a more complex one. The Arrows test assesses the
her recall of the story. In the Sentence Repetition test, ability to judge line orientation. The participant looks at an
participants are asked to repeat sentences of increasing array of arrows arranged around a target and indicates the
complexity and lenghth. arrows that points to the centre of the target.

Sensorimotor Functions Data Analysis

Three tests evaluate motor planning and execution abilities The scores obtained by each HFASD and HC participant at
and sensorimotor coordination. The Finger Tapping test each NEPSY-II test were expressed as scaled scores (mean:

123
J Autism Dev Disord

10; SD: 3) with respect to the normative values for the cor- A significance threshold of p \ 0.001 was set for all
responding chronological ages (Urgesi et al. 2011), thus parametric and non parametric statistical tests, applying a
improving the approximation of the data to the normal dis- Bonferroni correction to control for false discoveries in
tribution. It is worth noting that scaled scores were derived multiple comparisons (36 neuropsychological variables).
from the normative values with no approximation at the low
or high extremes, thus including also negative numbers for
performance lower than -3.33 SD from the mean. The Results
scaled scores were entered into series of Multivariate Anal-
yses of Covariance (MANCOVAs) with group as between- (1). HFASD versus HC
subject factor and MAEVIQ as covariate of interest. The
Table 1 shows mean and standard deviation (SD) of the
mental age was estimated from the VIQ for HFASD with the
scores obtained by HFASD and HC and the ANCOVAs
standard formula (MAEVIQ = VIQ 9 CA/100 and MAE-
results controlling for MAEVIQ.
PIQ = PIQ 9 CA/100). Although all patients presented a
Full Scale IQ [ 80, they presented different levels of VIQ,
Attention and Executive Functions
ranging from borderline to above mean levels; to ensure the
effect of verbal cognitive abilities was ruled out from the
HFASD children had an overall lower performance
between-group comparison, we entered the MAEVIQ as
(F5,59 = 11.13; p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.485); the effect of
covariate of interest. Since we did not have a VIQ measure
MAEVIQ was marginally significant but did not reach the
for HC, we inserted in the MANCOVA their chronological
Bonferroni corrected threshold (F5,59 = 3.34; p = 0.01;
age, assuming a correspondence between their mental and
gp2 = 0.221). A similar pattern of results was obtained for
chronological ages. The chronological age of controls
the tests that were administered only to children aged
(mean: 9.77 years; SD: 3.61) was not significantly different
7–16 years, with a significant effect of group (F4,48 = 13.2;
from the equivalent mental age of HFASD patients for both
p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.524) and no significant effect of
VIQ (MAEVIQ; mean: 9.95 years; SD: 4.24; t64 = -0.245;
MAEVIQ (F4,48 \ 1, gp2 = 0.051). Post-hoc univariate
p = 0.807) and PIQ (MAEPIQ; mean: 10.65 years; SD:
ANCOVAs revealed that HFASD children, independently
4.28; t64 = 0.448; p = 0.655).
from MAEVIQ, had significant lower performance for all the
Separate domain-specific MANCOVAs or ANCOVAs
subtests except for Visual Attention and Design Fluency.
were used for the NEPSY-II tests that were administered to
all 5–16 years old children or only to 7–16 or 5–12 years old
children. Post-hoc univariate ANCOVAs were performed Language
for each test to explore how they contributed to significant
between-group differences in each cognitive domain. In The MANCOVA revealed a significantly lower perfor-
keeping with the within-domain correlations of the US mance of HFASD as compared to HC (F6,56 = 4.87;
(Korkman et al. 2007) and Italian (Urgesi et al. 2011) nor- p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.343), with non-significant effect of
mative studies, all the variables entered in each MANCOVA MAEVIQ (F6,56 = 1.85; p = 0.105; gp2 = 0.166). Post-
were only moderately correlated (0.14 \ r \ 0.67). hoc univariate analyses showed that HFASD children
Normative studies showed higher levels of correlation tended to show lower performance than controls in all tests
between the tests that are comprised in the same cognitive but the difference was significant only in the Oromotor
domain when compared with those comprised in different Sequences test. No between group difference (F1,49 =
cognitive domains (Korkman et al. 2007; Urgesi et al. 3.04, p = 0.087, gp2 = 0.058) and no effect of MAEVIQ
2011). Furthermore, moderate levels of correlations were (F1,49 \ 1, gp2 \ 0.001) was observed for the Phonologi-
also observed between some tests of the Attention and cal Word Generation test, which was administered only to
Executive Functions and Language domains and the tests 7–16 years old children.
of other domain. To analyze how HFASD children differed
from HC in each cognitive domain independently from Memory and Learning
their performance in other cognitive domains, six domain
scores were obtained by averaging individual scaled scores The MANCOVA revealed significant lower performance
for all the tests comprised in each cognitive domain. All the of HFASD compared to HC (F5,59 = 11.08; p \ 0.001;
domain scores were moderately correlated with each other gp2 = 0.484), with non-significant effect of MAEVIQ
(0.34 \ r \ 0.6). The scores of the tests comprised in each (F5,59 = 2.28; p = 0.03; gp2 = 0.27). Univariate ANCO-
cognitive domain were entered into separate MANCOVAs VAs showed that the MAEVIQ had a significant effect on
with group as between-subject factor and the domain Memory for Designs (F1,63 = 18.51; p \ 0.001), which is
scores of other cognitive domains as covariates of interest. likely due to the influence of verbal intelligence on the

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 1 Mean (SD) NEPSY-II scaled scores (mean: 10; SD: 3) of HFASD and HC and comparison with the univariate ANCOVAs, controlling
for verbal mental age equivalent, and the non parametric Mann–Whitney U test
NEPSY-II test, variable Age HFASD HC HFASD vs. HC
ANCOVA Mann–Whitney U test

Attention and executive functions


Visual attention 5–16 9.47 (5.59) 9.96 (3.55) F1,63 = 0.32, p = 0.572 z = -0.22, p = 0.822
Design fluency 5–16 7.81 (4.31) 9.94 (3.11) F1,63 = 6.04, p = 0.017 z = -2.39, p = 0.017
Auditory attention 5–16 -0.8 (16.35) 10.41 (2.17) F1,63 = 20.04, p \ 0.001* z = -4.72, p \ 0.001*
Auditory response set 7–16 3.33 (10.43) 9.78 (2.54) F1,51 = 12.19, p = 0.001* z = -2.5, p = 0.013
Inhibition, naming 5–16 3.1 (7.49) 9.3 (1.85) F1,63 = 27.94, p \ 0.001* z = -4.23, p \ 0.001*
Inhibition, inhibition 5–16 1.82 (10.88) 9.64 (2.34) F1,63 = 22.63, p \ 0.001* z = -2.88, p = 0.004
Inhibition, switching 7–16 4.99 (5.78) 10.29 (3.24) F1,51 = 19.27, p \ 0.001* z = -3.32, p = 0.001*
Clocks 7–16 -4.84 (18.84) 10.23 (2.35) F1,51 = 22.62, p \ 0.001* z = -4.24, p \ 0.001*
Animal sorting 7–16 5.18 (3.67) 9.68 (2.75) F1,51 = 26.21, p \ 0.001* z = -4.17, p \ 0.001*
Language
Comprehension of instructions 5–16 7.22 (3.56) 9.12 (3.08) F1,63 = 4.35, p = 0.041 z = -2.16, p = 0.031
Speeded naming 5–16 1.27 (29.67) 10.2 (2.95) F1,63 = 4.05, p = 0.049 z = -0.61, p = 0.545
Phonological processing 5–16 6.36 (6.86) 9.9 (2.5) F1,63 = 9.25, p = 0.003 z = -2.78, p = 0.005
Word generation, semantic 5–16 7.71 (4.78) 9.97 (4.32) F1,63 = 3.91, p = 0.053 z = -2.93, p = 0.003
Word generation, phonological 7–16 7.66 (3.92) 9.48 (3.32) F1,51 = 3.04, p = 0.087 z = -2.1, p = 0.036
Repetition of non-sense words 5–16 6.21 (8.32) 10.24 (2.18) F1,63 = 8.71, p = 0.004 z = -1.5, p = 0.133
Oromotor sequences 5–16 3.17 (7.36) 9.96 (2.76) F1,63 = 27.88, p \ 0.001* z = -3.63, p \ 0.001*
Memory and learning
Memory for faces 5–16 7.58 (3.29) 10.33 (2.64) F1,63 = 13.31, p \ 0.001* z = -3.24, p = 0.001*
Word list interference 7–16 3.32 (5.85) 9.96 (2.73) F1,51 = 35.43, p \ 0.001* z = -4.06, p \ 0.001*
Memory for designs 5–16 5.16 (7.48) 9.85 (2.31) F1,63 = 17.6, p \ 0.001* z = -2.67, p = 0.008
List memory 7–16 1.30 (4.48) 9.57 (2.31) F1,51 = 82.66, p \ 0.001* z = -5.43, p \ 0.001*
Memory for names 5–16 7.95 (4.02) 10.02 (2.56) F1,63 = 6.59, p = 0.013 z = -1.9, p = 0.057
Narrative memory 5–16 4.95 (5.97) 10.03 (2.74) F1,63 = 22.17, p \ 0.001* z = -3.91, p \ 0.001*
Sentence repetition 5–16 4.09 (6.81) 10.59 (3.29) F1,63 = 27.19, p \ 0.001* z = -4.77, p \ 0.001*
Sensorimotor functions
Finger tapping 5–16 6.94 (8.65) 10.54 (2.04) F1,63 = 7.13, p = 0.01 z = -1.46, p = 0.142
Imitation of hand postures 5–16 -1.05 (9.14) 8.53 (8.74) F1,63 = 17.67, p \ 0.001* z = -5, p \ 0.001*
Visuomotor precision 5–12 11.62 (1.81) 10.73 (2.23) F1,42 = 1.49, p = 0.229 z = 1.97, p = 0.048
Manual motor sequences 5–16 3.17 (6.16) 9.31 (2.68) F1,63 = 32.65, p \ 0.001* z = -4.37, p \ 0.001*
Social perception
Theory of Mind, verbal 5–16 4.02 (6.15) 9.43 (3.08) F1,63 = 22.55, p \ 0.001* z = -3.67, p \ 0.001*
Theory of Mind, contextual 5–16 9.83 (3.31) 10.57 (3.09) F1,63 = 0.81, p = 0.373 z = -1.05, p = 0.292
Affect recognition 5–16 6.54 (6.76) 10.05 (2.87) F1,63 = 8.99, p = 0.004 z = -3.03, p = 0.002
Visuospatial processing
Design copying 5–16 5.33 (8.57) 11 (2.55) F1,63 = 16.42, p \ 0.001* z = -3.8, p \ 0.001*
Block construction 5–16 9.86 (4.65) 9.67 (2.63) F1,63 = 0.08, p = 0.784 z = 0.13, p = 0.897
Picture puzzles 7–16 9.16 (3.95) 9.96 (2.71) F1,51 = 0.77, p = 0.384 z = -0.49, p = 0.627
Geometric puzzles 5–16 7.83 (4.75) 10.15 (3.14) F1,63 = 5.55, p = 0.022 z = -1.86, p = 0.062
Route finding 5–16 9.48 (4.03) 10.06 (2.75) F1,63 = 0.47, p = 0.494 z = -0.69, p = 0.492
Arrows 5–16 6.11 (4.39) 10.61 (2.41) F1,63 = 28.68, p \ 0.001* z = -4.26, p \ 0.001*
* Significance threshold

ability to understand the task instructions which are par- HC in the Memory for Designs test. Significant differences
ticularly complex for this test of visuospatial memory. between the two groups, with no MAEVIQ effect were also
HFASD children had significantly lower performance than obtained for Narrative Memory, Sentence Repetition, and

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Memory for Faces tests. The analysis of the Word List in which they had a significantly impaired performance. This
Interference and List Memory tests, that were administered suggests that, while all HC individuals had scores close to the
only to 7–16 years old children, showed lower perfor- group mean, which corresponded to the norm-based expec-
mance of HFASD (F2,50 = 46.84; p \ 0.001; gp2 = ted value (Urgesi et al. 2011), HFASD individuals had more
0.652) at both the Word List Interference and List Memory variable performance, with some individuals showing more
tests, with non-significant effect of MAEVIQ (F2,50 = impairments than others. Importantly, more extreme
5.84; p = 0.005; gp2 = 0.189). impairments were obtained by different HFASD children in
different tests, reflecting individual weakness rather than
Sensorimotor Functions overall outlier performance. To ensure that our results are not
influenced by violations of the homogeneity of variance
The MANCOVA showed significant lower performance of assumption, we conducted a supplementary nonparametric
HFASD (F3,61 = 14.93; p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.423) with analysis, comparing the performance of HFASD and HC
non-significant effect of MAEVIQ (F3,61 = 1.21; p = children on each test with the Mann–Whitney U Test. As
0.312; gp2 = 0.056). Post-hoc univariate analyses showed shown in Table 1, the nonparametric comparisons confirmed
that HFASD children had lower performance than HC in a significantly impaired performance of HFASD children on
Imitation of Hand Postures and Manual Motor Sequences, most tests that show significant between-group effects in the
but not in Finger Tapping. No effects were also observed corresponding ANCOVA. The only discrepant results con-
between HFASD and 5–12 years old control children cerned the Auditory Response Set, Inhibition, and Memory
for the Visuomotor Precision test (group: F1,42 = 1.49, for Designs tests, in which the nonparametric analysis
p = 0.229, gp2 = 0.034; MAEVIQ: F1,42 \ 1, gp2 = detected only a non-significant trend toward impaired per-
0.023). formance of HFASD children, probably because of reduced
power. Furthermore, no differences were observed between
Social Perception the NEPSY-II performance of children with a DSM-IV-TR
diagnosis of AD vs. PDDNOS (-1.7 \ all zs \ 1.7,
The MANCOVA showed significantly lower performance p [ 0.8), with the exception of a non significantly worse
of HFASD children (F3,61 = 8.25; p \ 0.001; gp2 = NEPSY-II performance of children with PDDNOS on the
0.289), with no effect of MAEVIQ (F3,61 \ 1; gp2 = verbal items of the Theory of Mind test (AD: 7.16 ± 4.33 vs.
0.044). The effect of group was due to the scores in the PDDNOS: 1.39 ± 6.36; z = 2.31, p = 0.021).
verbal items of the Theory of Mind test, while no differ-
(2). Specificity of each neuropsychological domain in
ence was observed for the Theory of Mind contextual items
HFASD children
and for the Affect Recognition test.
The MANCOVA on the Attention and Executive
Visuospatial Processing Functions domain entering the other domains as covariates
of interest showed that the difference between HFASD and
The MANCOVA showed lower performance in HFASD HC was still significant after removing the variance in
(F5,59 = 9.04; p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.434), with non-signifi- common with the other cognitive domains (F5,55 = 5.02,
cant effect of MAEVIQ (F5,59 = 1.28; p = 0.285; p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.313). The effects of the other cogni-
gp2 = 0.098). Post-hoc univariate analyses showed that tive domains were non-significant (all Fs5,55 \ 4.5,
HFASD children had lower performance than HC in p [ 0.002, gp2 \ 0.29). This suggests that the deficits of
Design Copying and Arrows. On the Design Copying test, HFASD children in attention and executive functions were
the deficits of HFASD children were apparent in the Motor independent from other neuropsychological domains. In
(F1,63 = 27.3; p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.302) and Global Pro- contrast, no effects of group (F4,44 = 2.65, p = 0.046,
cessing (F1,63 = 19.23; p \ 0.001; gp2 = 0.234) while no gp2 = 0.194) and of the other neuropsychological domains
difference was observed for the Local Processing (all Fs4,44 \ 3.8, p [ 0.01, gp2 \ 0.26) were obtained for
(F1,63 = 5.03; p = 0.028; gp2 = 0.074). Non-significant the Attention and Executive Functions tests that were
effects of group (F1,51 \ 1, gp2 = 0.015) and MAEVIQ administered only to 7–16 years old children.
(F1,51 \ 1, gp2 \ 0.001) were obtained for the Picture The MANCOVA on Language domain showed signifi-
Puzzles test in 7–16 years old children. cant effects of Attention and Executive Functions
(F6,52 = 5.59, p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.392) and of Memory
Non-Parametric Tests and Learning (F6,52 = 8.57, p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.497),
with no effects of the other domains and no between-group
Inspection of the data shown in Table 1 reveals that the difference (all Fs6,52 \ 3.9, p [ 0.003, gp2 \ 0.31). Thus,
HFASD had lower mean scores and higher SD in most tests the impairments of HFASD children in language abilities

123
J Autism Dev Disord

were not consistent after removing the variance in common different cognitive domains, ruling out the effects of gen-
with the other cognitive domains and in particular with eral cognitive abilities. Controlling for verbal general
Attention and Executive Functions and Memory and cognitive abilities, several impairments were observed
Learning abilities, which strongly affected performance in across different cognitive domains in HFASD. Our data
language tasks. seem to support the notion that NEPSY-II is able to
The MANCOVA on Memory and Learning domain highlight the profile of neuropsychological functioning of
showed a significant effect of Attention and Executive HFASD. While the results from the initial validation study
Functions (F9,51 = 5.14, p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.476), while reported in the NEPSY-II manual (Korkman et al. 2007)
the effects of the other cognitive domains and of group were were largely replicated, some differences were noted.
non-significant (all Fs9,51 \ 2.4, p [ 0.025, gp2 \ 0.3). In Within the Attention and Executive Functioning
contrast, when the scores in the Word List Interference and domain, we confirmed the deficits of HFASD children in
List Memory tests were analyzed, the effect of group resulted the measures of Inhibition, Animal Sorting and Auditory
significant (F4,44 = 5.71, p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.342), with Attention and Response Set. Furthermore, the pattern of
non-significant effects of the other cognitive domains (all Attention and Executive Function deficits in HFASD is
Fs4,44 \ 2, p [ 0.12, gp2 \ 0.15). Thus, HFASD impair- largely consistent with prior research in HFASD using the
ments in memory for lists of words were consistent even after first version of the NEPSY (Joseph et al. 2005; Hooper
removing the portion of variance in common with other et al. 2006; Korkman et al. 1998). In particular, in the
neuropsychological functions. Joseph et al. (2005)’s study, children with autism exhibited
The MANCOVA on Sensorimotor Functions domain deficits compared to the comparison group across all the 3
showed only a significant effect of Language (F3,57 = domains of executive functions that were assessed,
6.25, p \ 0.001, gp2 = 0.248), while the effect of group including working memory, inhibitory control and plan-
and of the other cognitive domains were not significant (all ning. Several studies, indeed, have demonstrated that
Fs3,57 \ 2.8, p [ 0.049, gp2 \ 0.13). That the HFASD HFASD children present executive dysfunctions as com-
impairments in Sensorimotor Functions were not signifi- pared to age-matched typical developing individuals and
cant after removing the variance in common with Lan- other clinical control groups (Ozonoff et al. 1991; Ozonoff
guage tests was likely due to the fact that many language 1997; Bennetto et al. 1996; Ozonoff and McEvoy 1994;
tasks require motor planning and monitoring abilities Ozonoff 1995; Ozonoff and Jensen 1999; Sergeant et al.
similar to those required by the sensorimotor tasks. In 2002; Geurts et al. 2004; Verté et al. 2005). In a similar
particular, the analysis of the intercorrelations between the vein, in other studies that evaluated planning abilities
NEPSY-II tests revealed highly significant correlation (Hughes et al. 1994; Ozonoff et al. 2004; Pennington and
between the Oromotor Sequences and Manual Motor Ozonoff 1996; Bennetto et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 2009;
Sequences tests (Urgesi et al. 2011). Joseph et al. 2005; Lopez et al. 2005; Happé et al. 2006),
The MANCOVA on Social Perception domain was only HFASD were impaired relative to HC. The NEPSY subtest
influenced by Attention and Executive Functions (F3,57 = ‘Statue’ was also used to study 2 components of motor
5.93, p = 0.001, gp2 = 0.238), while the other effects were control: motor response inhibition and motor persistence in
not significant (all Fs3,57 \ 2.2, p [ 0.09, gp2 \ 0.11). This 24 HFASD aged 7–13 years (Mahone et al. 2006). HFASD
suggests that the deficits of HFASD children in the social achieved lower scores than controls only on measures of
perception abilities evaluated with NEPSY-II were associated motor persistence, with no concomitant impairment on
to attention and executive functions deficits. motor inhibition tasks, in keeping with prior research
In a similar vein, the MANCOVA on Visuospatial Pro- demonstrating relatively spared motor inhibition in autism.
cessing domain was influenced only by Attention and We also confirm the lower performances of HFASD
Executive Functions (F5,55 = 5.58, p \ 0.001, gp2 = children in Language domain (Hooper et al. 2006;
0.336). Although HFASD children tended to show a rela- Korkman et al. 2007). In particular, the impairments in the
tively lower performance in visuospatial tests, the effect of Phonological Processing, Oromotor sequences and Repe-
group did not reach the Bonferroni corrected significance tition of non-sense words are consistent with Tager-
threshold (F5,55 = 2.73, p = 0.029, gp2 = 0.199) after Flusberg (2004), who suggested that children with autism
removing the effects of the other cognitive domains. and phonological deficits may represent a selected subtype
of autism. Whitehouse et al. (2008) suggested that only
some children with autism demonstrate poor non-sense
Discussion word repetition, while Schmidt et al. (2008) pointed out
that problems in phonological processing and repetition of
The main aim of the present study was to describe the non-sense words might be characteristic of the broad aut-
neuropsychological profiles of HFASD children across ism phenotype. Accordingly, some studies have found that

123
J Autism Dev Disord

HFASD have an impairment in the repetition of non-sense the Imitation of hand postures and in the Manual motor
words as compared to controls (Botting and Conti-Rams- sequences tests, thus in those tasks requiring the imitation
den 2003; Riches et al. 2011) and this deficit might reflect a of complex movement sequences. Following Joseph et al.
general phonological processing impairment in HFASD (2005), these tasks require children to combine working
(Bishop et al. 2004; Hooper et al. 2006). memory and inhibitory control so as to withhold a domi-
HFASD had lower performance than HC also in all tasks nant motor response by maintaining a conflicting response
of the Memory and Learning domain. These results are rule (e.g., to knock with one hand while the other hand taps
consistent with the Korkman et al. (2007)’s findings and and vice versa) in active memory. In keeping with our
with other studies showing impaired immediate memory finding, Korkman et al. (2007) and Hooper et al. (2006)
performance in HFASD. In particular, we confirm that showed no significant differences between HFASD versus
HFASD performance on immediate verbal recall of com- controls in the Finger Tapping task, suggesting the absence
plex material, such as a stories or sentences, is significantly of deficits in low level sensorimotor control.
impaired (Boucher and Warrington 1976; Boucher 1981; The findings in Social Perception domain confirm the
Fein et al. 1996; Bennetto et al. 1996; Minshew et al. 1997; Korkman et al. (2007)’s results with lower scores for
Korkman et al. 1998; Minshew and Goldstein 2001; HFASD than controls in Affect recognition and Theory of
Williams et al. 2005, 2006; Hooper et al. 2006). Previous Mind Verbal tasks. Within the Social Perception domain an
studies have found that individuals with HFASD may important result of our work was the intact performances of
display a similar pattern of list learning across 5 repetitions HFASD in the contextual items of the Theory of Mind test.
of the same list words presented in the same order (Bowler These trials evaluate the capacity of children to understand
et al. 2009). However, the impairments of HFASD indi- how certain emotions are linked to given social situations
viduals in verbal learning become particularly manifest and to recognize correctly the emotions that the various
under conditions that require the organization of the social settings generate. The child is shown an illustration
learning material to improve the performance (Bowler of a social situation and he/she has to choose which of four
et al. 2008, 2010). Such organization of the information is facial expressions illustrates appropriately the emotional
also required by the List Memory and Word List interfer- state of one of the characters shown in the photograph (see
ence tests of the NEPSY-II, in which an interferential list is Fig. 1). Our results contrast with those of Da Fonseca et al.
presented and participants need to organize the words (2008) that revealed that children with HFASD are able to
correctly in the two lists avoiding interference. Thus, the use contextual cues to recognize objects but not emotions.
deficits observed in list learning in our HFASD sample However, differently than in the task of Da Fonseca et al.
may be due mainly to difficulties in organizing the verbal (2008), in which the children have to choose the appro-
materials and to resist interference, rather than to word priate emotional state between smile illustrations, in the
learning per se. Our results are also consistent with previ- NEPSY-II tasks the children have to choose the appropriate
ous evidence that performance in the immediate recall of emotional state from real facial photos. Our findings may
complex designs or forms is impaired in ASD children suggest that: (1) contextual elements (the social situation)
(Ameli et al. 1988; Minshew et al. 1992; Minshew and seem to facilitate the processing of the corresponding
Goldstein 2001; Rumsey and Hamburger 1990; Williams emotional state (photo of human face); (2) the human face,
et al. 2006). We confirmed also the marked deficits in face in contrast to smiles illustration (Da Fonseca et al. 2008),
memory which has been widely reported in the literature. could facilitate the activation of an isomorphic process—
Several studies, indeed, found that ASD, as compared to IQ identity between the level of observed experience and that
matched controls, are particularly impaired in this task (de of the physiological processes underlying it—in HFASD
Gelder et al. 1991; Boucher and Lewis 1992; Gepner et al. children (Eagle and Wakefield 2007; Gallese 2003); (3) the
1996; Korkman et al. 1998; Hauck et al. 1998; Klin et al. systematizing inclination of HFASD to analyze details
1999; Blair et al. 2002; Williams et al. 2005; Hooper et al. (Baron-Cohen 2010) could facilitate the trend toward
2006) (for a theoretical explanation see Carver and Dawson greater attention to contextual cues of social situations by
2007). However, our study allows to document that the facilitating the recognition of appropriate emotional states.
visuospatial memory impairments in HFASD are not lim- Our HFASD sample displayed a relative sparing of the
ited to faces, but they extend to abstract geometric forms visuospatial abilities when compared with the deep neu-
(Memory for Designs). ropsychological deficits observed in the other domains. In
According to Hooper et al. (2006), ASD demonstrated this domain, only the Arrows and Design Copy tests
deficits in the Sensorimotor Functions domain. In particu- showed a significant difference between the HFASD and
lar, while no difference between HFASD and control HC children, with scores corresponding to borderline per-
children were observed in the Visuomotor Precision and formance. The deficits in the line orientation discrimination
Finger Tapping tests, specific impairments were found in (Arrows) have been found also in previous studies

123
J Autism Dev Disord

a given domain were influenced by their weaknesses in


other domains. A comprehensive neuropsychological
investigation of the main cognitive functions with the
NEPSY-II is particularly adept to this purpose, allowing to
identify specific patterns of primary and secondary deficits
(Luria 1962). To analyze how HFASD children differed
from controls in each cognitive domain independently from
their performance in the others, MANCOVAs were per-
formed for each set of neuropsychological variables
entering the others domain scores as covariates of interest.
These analyses allowed to show how the performance of
Fig. 1 Example of contextual item of the Theory of Mind test
children in a given set of neuropsychological variables is
influenced by their abilities in other cognitive domains and
(Korkman et al. 2007; Hooper et al. 2006). On the other how much the impairments of HFASD in each neuropsy-
hand, the deficit in the Design Copying test is consistent chological function are associated to their deficits in other
with the initial validation study of the NEPSY-II (Korkman domains. We found that the impairments of HFASD chil-
et al. 2007), but not with the validation study of the NEPSY dren in attention and executive functions and verbal
(Hooper et al. 2006). This discrepancy between the two memory were not associated, at least partially, to the other
tests might be due to the improvement of the sensitivity of neuropsychological functions. In turn, a widespread influ-
the Design Copy test in the NEPSY-II, with the inclusion ence of executive deficits was observed on the performance
of more complex items. Importantly, only the Motor and in the other domains. Furthermore, verbal memory affected
Global Processing scores of the Design Copying test were performance in language tests. The deficits of HFASD
lower in HFASD than HC children, while no difference children in language, visuospatial memory, sensorimotor
was found in the local processing score. Thus, while functions, social perception and visuospatial processing
HFASD children presented difficulties in fine-motor con- were associated to executive and verbal memory deficits
trol, causing inaccurate drawing, and in representing the and did not survive after removing the variance in common
overall configuration of the design, they accurately repre- with other domains. It is worth noting, however, that since
sented the design features. This suggests a bias of HFASD the variability in the performance at the different NEPSY-
children towards a local strategy of stimulus processing II tests was not random in the two groups, being strongly
(Baron-Cohen 2010) which may be detected by analyzing influenced by having or not HFASD, the interpretation of
the specific processing scores of the NEPSY-II Design these results should be cautious. They cannot indicate that
Copying test. Recently, Planche and Lemonnier (2011) HFASD children are not impaired in a given cognitive
have used the NEPSY and WISC-III tests to study visuo- function apart from deficits in other cognitive domains, but
spatial abilities in HFASD and found non-significant dif- that their deficits in a given domain are associated to, and
ferences between HFASD and controls. Nevertheless, as possibly influenced, by deficits in other cognitive domains
compared to controls, HFASD children showed a different (Joseph et al. 2005). In other words, individuals with
response strategy in the visuospatial tasks with motor greater language, sensorimotor and social perception defi-
coordination components, i.e. the ‘‘object assembly’’ and cits also had greater impairments in verbal memory or
‘‘block design’’ tests of the WISC-III, but not in tasks executive functions.
requiring similar visuospatial processing in the absence of A similar account has been used by Joseph et al. (2005)
any motor coordination ability, i.e. the ‘‘arrows’’ NEPSY in a study attempting to verify the hypothesis of Russel
test and the ‘‘picture completion’’ WISC-III test. These (1997) that the functional deficit underlying poor perfor-
results might suggest that HFASD children have spared mance by children with autism on executive function tasks
visuospatial processing abilities, but their difficulties in (Hughes and Russell 1993) could be an impairment in the
sensorimotor functions facilitate the use of local rather than ability to use inner speech to maintain arbitrary response
configural processing strategies when the visuospatial tasks rules in working memory and to guide behavior accord-
require motor coordination. Thus, the study by Planche and ingly. In keeping with the language mediation hypothesis
Lemonnier (2011) suggests that, in keeping with the Luria of the executive dysfunctions in autism, studies have found
(1962)’s approach, the performance of HFASD children in that the deficits of HFASD children on executive functions
a given cognitive domain may be affected by their weak- tasks disappeared when the lower scores of the autistic
nesses in other domains. group in verbal IQ (Liss et al. 2001) and in lexical retrieval
A further aim of the present study was to determine how and access measures (Joseph e al. 2005) were controlled for
the neuropsychological impairments of HFASD children in using ANCOVAs. However, since the HFASD group was

123
J Autism Dev Disord

impaired in both executive functions and language tasks, hypothesis is also corroborated by recent findings derived
these data could not determine the origin of the association. from HFASD neuroimaging studies, in which brain alter-
In other words, it could not be established whether a lack of ations are not limited to specific regions or circuits, but
language mediation may explain the executive function rather are indicative of a global cerebral impairment
deficits or whether language and executive functions are (Müller 2007). Indeed, structural magnetic resonance
simultaneously impaired in HFASD. On the other hand, our imaging (MRI) investigations consistently report an abnor-
findings show that the deficits of HFASD in attention and mal enlargement of total brain volume in patients with
executive functions as compared to HC were still signifi- HFASD that involves gray and white matter (Courchesne
cant after removing the effects of verbal IQ and of neu- et al. 2001; Hazlett et al. 2005). Those alterations are
ropsychological linguistic abilities favor a primary particularly conspicuous in frontal and temporal regions of
dysfunction of executive functions in HFASD. children with autism (Calderoni et al. 2012; Carper and
The widespread impairments in different cognitive Courchesne 2005; Hazlett et al. 2006), with a relatively
domains in our HFASD sample might suggest a deficit in spare of the occipital lobe. Similarly, studies using Diffu-
general cognitive abilities rather than in specific neuro- sion Tensor Imaging (DTI) to investigate the whole brain
psychological functions. However, since we only tested confirm a diffuse and atypical connectivity organization in
HFASD individuals with IQ values within the normal HFASD children (Bloemen et al. 2010; Shukla et al. 2010).
range, we can rule out that a deficit of general cognitive Evidence of extensive brain impairment in HFASD
abilities may explain the results. On the other hand, patients comes also from functional-connectivity MRI data,
although we did not have the IQ values of HC individuals that point to an altered connectivity among several brain
and cannot rule out definitively that we selected partici- areas during the execution of different complex tasks (Just
pants with general cognitive abilities higher than expected et al. 2004, 2007; Koshino et al. 2008; Mason et al. 2008).
from chronological age, it is unlikely that this may explain The above mentioned anatomical and functional brain
the neuropsychological deficits of HFASD individuals. alterations are consistent with the disrupted cortical con-
Indeed, the scaled scores of HC individuals in all NEPSY- nectivity model (Kana et al. 2011), which claims that the
II tests were about 10, thus they were comparable to the autistic deficit in higher level cognitive processes may
values expected from the Italian normative study (Urgesi reflect reduced connectivity among key brain areas. Also,
et al. 2011). Conversely, the scaled scores of HFASD the widespread and global neuropsychological impairments
participants in the texts showing a between-group differ- found in HFASD patients could resemble the neurocogni-
ence ranged from 6 to -4 and were less than 2 SD from the tive profile of negative symptoms in patients with chronic
expected value in many tests (see Table 1) Furthermore, schizophrenia (Frith 1995; Fagerlund et al. 2006; Kravariti
low correlations have been reported between the NEPSY-II et al. 2005; Rhinewine et al. 2005). This adds to the
scores and the IQ values (measured with the WISC-III test recent literature data showing shared features between
in a subsample of children) in both the US (Korkman et al. these two conditions (King and Lord 2011), that involve
2007) and the Italian (Urgesi et al. 2011) normative studies. genetic (Burbach and van der Zwaag 2009; Crespi et al.
This suggests that performance on the NEPSY-II tests was 2010), anatomical (Cheung et al. 2010; Pinkham et al.
partially independent from general cognitive abilities as 2008) and clinical (Couture et al. 2010; Craig et al. 2004)
measured with the WISC-III tests (Korkman et al. 2007; aspects.
Urgesi et al. 2011). Relatively higher correlations were The results of this study should be interpreted in the
found for the Attention and Executive Functions domains, light of important limitations. First, the relatively small
where HFASD individuals showed an impaired perfor- sample size of HFASD children may limit the generaliza-
mance as compared to HC, but also for the Visuospatial tion of the finding to the general population of HFASD.
Processing domain tests, where HFASD showed a relative Second, the inclusion of children with PDDNOS who had
sparing. The intact performance in many tests (e.g., Visual comparable social dysfunctions but did not satisfy all
Attention, contextual items of Theory of Mind, Block DSM-IV-TR criteria for AD should be considered when
Construction, Picture Puzzle and Route Finding) and in comparing the results to studies testing only children who
some performance indexes of other tests (e.g., intact Global satisfy the more stringent criteria for AD. However, this
Processing score but impaired Local Processing score in limitation is lessened by the absence of significant differ-
the Design Copying tests) points to alterations in multiple ences on the NEPSY-II profile when children with AD are
cognitive subsystems rather than to general cognitive compared to PDDNOS.
deficits. Finally, the results of the analyses comparing the per-
Our results could be consistent with the emerging view formance of HFASD and HC children on each cognitive
of autism as a disorder of the interactions among multiple domain when the variance in common with the other
brain systems (Frith 2004; Mundy and Sigman 2006). This domains is partialled out should be cautious, since we

123
J Autism Dev Disord

cannot establish whether associated impairments simply Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic
co-occur in HFASD children or are dependent from the difficulties and specific language impairment: Can we distin-
guish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental
same primary deficit. Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 515–524.
In conclusion, our study supports the utility of NEPSY- Boucher, J. (1981). Immediate free recall in early childhood autism:
II as a sensitive evaluation tool to describe the neuropsy- Another point of behavioral similarity with the amnesic
chological profile of HFASD children. It can identify syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 72, 211–215.
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in
weaknesses and strengths of their cognitive abilities and to relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and
disentangle between primary and secondary deficits. Fur- Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 843–859.
thermore, our findings, in particular the lower perfor- Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early
mances on verbal versus contextual items of Theory of infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome.
British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87.
Mind, may pave the way for empirical studies on larger Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Subjective
sample of HFASD. Indeed, a deep investigation of the organisation in the free recall of adults with Asperger’s
neuropsychological impairments in HFASD might help the syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
identification of the pathophysiological mechanisms asso- 38, 103–114.
Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2010). Multiple list
ciated with the disorder and to design appropriate inter- learning in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Parallels with
ventions aimed at improving cognitive capacities. frontal lobe damage or further evidence of diminished relational
processing? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40,
Acknowledgments This work was supported by the Italian Ministry 179–187.
of Health under Strategic Program ‘Inquiry into Disruption of Inter- Bowler, D. M., Limoges, E., & Mottron, L. (2009). Different verbal
subjective Equipment in Autism Spectrum Disorder in Childhood learning strategies in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from
(IDIA)’ (Principal Investigator F.M.) and partially by grant from the the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Autism and
IRCCS ‘‘E. Medea’’ (Ricerca Corrente, Ministry of Health to C.U.). Developmental Disorders, 39, 910–915.
Burbach, J. P., & van der Zwaag, B. (2009). Contact in the genetics of
autism and schizophrenia. Trends in Neurosciences, 32, 69–72.
Calderoni, S., Retico, A., Biagi, L., Tancredi, R., Muratori, F., &
Tosetti, M. (2012). Female children with autism spectrum
References disorder: An insight from mass-univariate and pattern classifi-
cation analyses. NeuroImage, 59, 1013–1022.
Ameli, R., Courchesne, E., Lincoln, A., Kaufman, A. S., & Grillon, C. Carper, R. A., & Courchesne, E. (2005). Localized enlargement of the
(1988). Visual memory processes in high-functioning individuals frontal cortex in early autism. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 126–133.
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Carver, L. J., & Dawson, G. (2007). Development and neural bases of
18(4), 601–615. face recognition in autism. Molecular Psychiatry, 7(2), 18–20.
Attwood, T. (2004). Cognitive behaviour therapy for children and Cheung, C., Yu, K., Fung, G., Leung, M., Wong, C., Li, Q., et al.
adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Behaviour Change, 21(3), (2010). Autistic disorders and schizophrenia: related or remote?
147–162. An anatomical likelihood estimation. PLoS One, 5(8), 122–133.
Baranek, G., Parham, L. D., & Bodfish, J. W. (2005). Sensory and Courchesne, E., Karns, C., Davis, H. R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R.,
motor features in autism: Assessment and intervention. In Tigue, Z., et al. (2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early
F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook life in patients with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology,
of autism and pervasive developmental disorders, Volume 2: 57, 245–254.
Assessment, intervention, and policy (3rd ed.). New York: Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Losh, M., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R., &
Wiley. Piven, J. (2010). Comparison of social cognitive functioning in
Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Empathizing, systemizing, and the extreme schizophrenia and high functioning autism: More convergence
male brain theory of autism. Progress in Brain Research, 186, than divergence. Psychological Medicine, 40(4), 569–579.
167–175. Craig, J. S., Hatton, C., Craig, F. B., & Bentall, R. P. (2004).
Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and Persecutory beliefs, attributions and Theory of Mind: Compar-
impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, ison of patients with paranoid delusions, Asperger’s syndrome
1816–1835. and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Research, 69(1), 29–33.
Bishop, D., Mayberry, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., Hill, W., & Crespi, B., Stead, P., & Elliot, M. (2010). Evolution in health and
Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are phonological processing deficits part medicine Sackler colloquium: Comparative genomics of autism
of the broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Medical and schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Genetics, 128(1), 54–60. Sciences of the United States of America, 107(Suppl 1),
Blair, R. J., Frith, U., Smith, N., Abell, F., & Cipolotti, L. (2002). 1736–1741.
Fractionation of visual memory: Agency detection and its Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Bastard-Rosset, D., Rondan, C., Poinso,
impairment in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40(1), 108–118. F., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Can children with autistic spectrum
Bloemen, O. J., Deeley, Q., Sundram, F., Daly, E. M., Barker, G. J., disorders extract emotions out of contextual cues? Research in
Jones, D. K., et al. (2010). White matter integrity in Asperger Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 50–56.
syndrome: A preliminary diffusion tensor magnetic resonance Davidson, A. J., McCann, M. E., Morton, N. S., & Myles, P. S.
imaging study in adults. Autism Research, 3, 203–213. (2008). Anesthesia and outcome after neonatal surgery. Anes-
Boneh, A., Beauchamp, M., Humphrey, M., Watkins, J., Peters, H., & thesiology, 109, 941–944.
Yaplito-Lee, J. (2008). Newborn screening for glutaric aciduria de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., & van der Heide, L. (1991). Face
type I in Victoria: Treatment and outcome. Molecular Genetics recognition and lip reading in autism. European Journal of
and Metabolism, 94, 287–291. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 69–86.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Eagle, M. N., & Wakefield, J. M. (2007). Gestalt psychology and the Joseph, R. M., McGrath, L. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005).
mirror neuron discovery. Gestalt Theory, 29(1), 54–64. Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in
Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism verbal school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuro-
spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of psychology, 27(3), 361–378.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 729–745. Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., Kana, R. K., &
Fagerlund, B., Pagsberg, A. K., & Hemmingsen, R. P. (2006). Minshew, N. J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical under
Cognitive deficits and levels of IQ in adolescent onset schizo- connectivity in autism: Evidence from an fMRI study of an
phrenia and other psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia Research, executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry.
85, 30–39. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 951–961.
Fein, D., Dunn, M., Allen, D. A., Aram, D. M., Hall, N., Morris, R., Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J.
et al. (1996). Language and neuropsychological findings. In (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence
I. Rapin & L. Wing (Eds.), Preschool children with inadequate comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of under
communication: Developmental language disorder, autism, low connectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.
IQ. Clinics in Developmental Medicine, 139 (pp. 123–154). Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E., & Moore, M. S. (2011). Disrupted cortical
London: Mac Keith. connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum
Frith, C. D. (1995). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. disorders. Physics of Life Review, 8(4), 410–437.
Hove, UK: Psychology Press. King, B. H., & Lord, C. (2011). Is schizophrenia on the autism
Frith, C. (2004). Is autism a disconnection disorder? Lancet Neurol- spectrum? Brain Research, 22(1380), 34–41.
ogy, 3, 577. Klin, A. K., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen,
Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face
quest for a common mechanism. Philosophical Transactions of recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism
the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biologica Sciences, 358, and Developmental Disorders, 29(6), 499–508.
517–528. Klin, A., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Asperger
Gepner, B., de Gelder, B., & de Schonen, S. (1996). Face processing syndrome. New York: The Guilford Press.
in autistics: Evidence for a generalized deficit? Child Neuropsy- Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Fellman, V. (1998). NEPSY: A develop-
chology, 2, 123–139. mental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The
Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. Psychological Corporation.
(2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY-II: A develop-
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of mental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45(4), Psychological Corporation.
836–854. Koshino, H., Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Minshew,
Ghaziuddin, M., & Mountain-Kimchi, K. (2004). Defining the N. J., & Just, M. A. (2008). fMRI investigation of working
intellectual pro file of Asperger syndrome: Comparison with memory for faces in autism: Visual coding and underconnectiv-
high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental ity with frontal areas. Cerebral Cortex, 18(2), 289–300.
Disorders, 34, 279–284. Kravariti, E., Dixon, T., Frith, C., et al. (2005). Association of
Goodenough, F. L. (1975). Measurement of intelligence by drawings. symptoms and executive function in schizophrenia and bipolar
New York: New York Press. disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 74, 221–231.
Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome and
function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention- high-functioning autism. In A. Klin, F. Volkmar, & S. Sparrow
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 125–158). New York, NY:
domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 25–39. Guilford Press.
Hauck, M., Fein, D., Maltby, N., Waterhouse, L., & Feinstein, C. Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., et al.
(1998). Memory for faces in children with autism. Child (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with
Neuropsychology, 4, 187–198. autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 261–270.
Haviland, A., Nagin, S. D., & Rosenbaum, P. R. (2007). Combining Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining
propensity score matching and group-based trajectory analysis in the relationship between executive functions and restricted,
an observational study. Psychological Methods, 12, 247–267. repetitive symptoms of Autistic Disorder. Journal of Autism and
Hazlett, H. C., Poe, M. D., Gerig, G., Smith, R. G., & Piven, J. (2006). Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 445–446.
Cortical gray and white brain tissue volume in adolescents and Lord, C. (2011). Epidemiology: How common is autism? Nature,
adults with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 1–6. 8(474), 166–168.
Hazlett, H. C., Poe, M., Gerig, G., Smith, R. G., Provenzale, J., Ross, Luria, A. R. (1962). Higher cortical functions in man. Moscow:
A., et al. (2005). Magnetic resonance imaging and head Moscow University Press.
circumference study of brain size in autism: birth through age Mahone, E. M., Powell, S. K., Loftis, C. W., Goldberg, M. C.,
2 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 1366–1376. Denckla, M. D., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Motor persistence
Hooper, S. R., Poon, K. K., Marcus, L., & Fine, C. (2006). and inhibition in autism and ADHD. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological characteristics of school-age children with Neuropsychological Society, 12, 622–631.
high-functioning autism: performance on the NEPSY. Child Marini, A., Lorusso, M. L., D’Angelo, G., Civati, F., Turconi, A. C.,
Neuropsychology, 12(4–5), 299–305. Fabbro, F., et al. (2007). Evaluation of narrative abilities in
Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with patients suffering from Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Brain
mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for and Language, 102(1), 1–12.
theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 498–510. Mason, R. A., Williams, D. L., Kana, R. K., Minshew, N., & Just,
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for M. A. (2008). Theory-of-Mind disruption and recruitment of the
executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492. right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism.
Joseph, R. M., Keehn, B., Connolly, C., Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, Neuropsychologia, 46, 269–280.
T. S. (2009). Why is visual search superior in autism spectrum Miniscalco, C., Hagberg, B., Kadesjö, B., Westerlund, M., &
disorder? Developmental Science, 12, 1083–1096. Gillberg, C. (2007). Narrative skills, cognitive profiles and

123
J Autism Dev Disord

neuropsychiatric disorders in 7–8-year-old children with late Raven, J. C. (1954). Standard progressive matrices. Firenze: Giunti
developing language. International Journal of Language & OS.
Communication Disorders, 42(6), 665–681. Rhinewine, J. P., Lencz, T., Thaden, E. P., et al. (2005). Neurocog-
Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2001). The pattern of intact and nitive profile in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia:
impaired memory functions in autism. Journal of Child Clinical correlates. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 705–712.
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 1095– Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E.
1101. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific lan-
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L. R., & Payton, J. B. (1992). guage impairment and autism plus language impairments: A
Neuropsychological functioning in nonmentally retarded autistic qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(1),
individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy- 23–36.
chology, 14, 749–761. Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P.
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. (1995). Speech and (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum
language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychol- disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 362–368.
ogy, 9, 255–261. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuropsychological
Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsycho- divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of
logic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(2), 155–168.
processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsycho- Russel, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring aboutan
logical Society, 3, 303–316. adeguate ‘‘Theory of Mind’’. In J. Russel (Ed.), Autism as an
Müller, R. A. (2007). The study of autism as a distributed disorder. executive disorder (pp. 256–304). Oxford: Oxford University
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Press.
Review, 13(1), 85–95. Russell, J., Jarrold, C., & Hood, B. (1999). Two intact executive
Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (2006). Joint attention, social competence capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core
and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and
(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, second edition, volume Developmental Disorders, 29, 103–112.
one: Theory and methods. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Samson, F., Mottron, L., Soulières, I., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2012).
Oliveras-Rentas, R. E., Kenworhty, L., Roberson, R. B., Martin, A., & Enhanced visual functioning in autism: An ALE meta-analysis.
Wallace, G. L. (2012). WISC-IV profile in high functioning Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1553–1581.
autism spectrum disorders: Impaired speed is associated with Scheeren A.M., Koot H.M., & Begeer S. (2012). Social interaction
increased autism communication symptoms and decreased style of children and adolescent with high functioning spectrum
adaptive communication abilities. Journal of Autism and Devel- disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Feb
opmental Disorders, 42(5), 655–664. 1. (Epub ahead of print).
Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card Schmidt, G. L., Kopelioff, L., Winterrowd, E., Pennington, B. F.,
Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491–500. Hepburn, S. L., & Rojas, D. C. (2008). Impairments in
Ozonoff, S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and phonological processing and non-verbal intellectual function in
other disorders. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive parents of children with autism. Journal of Clinical and
disorder (pp. 179–211). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 557–567.
Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, P. (2002). How specific is a
et al. (2004). Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 3–28.
function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the Shukla, D. K., Keehn, B., Lincoln, A. J., & Müller, R. A. (2010).
Collaborative Programs of Excellence in Autism network. White matter compromise of callosal and subcortical fiber tracts
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), in children with autism spectrum disorder: A diffusion tensor
139–150. imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(12), 1269–1278.
function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177. language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Ozonoff, S., & McEvoy, R. E. (1994). A longitudinal study of Disorders, 34, 75–80.
executive function and Theory of Mind development in autism. Tsatsanis, K. D. (2005). Neuropsychological characteristics in autism
Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 415–431. and related conditions. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. KIin, & D.
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental
function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Rela- disorders: Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior
tionship to Theory of Mind. Journal of Child Psychology and (3rd ed.). Wiley: New York.
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081–1105. Urgesi, C., Campanella, F., & Fabbro, F. (2011). NEPSY-II,
Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory function in Contributo alla Taratura Italiana. Firenze: Giunti OS.
nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism and Verté, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A.
Developmental Disorders, 27, 59–77. (2005). Executive functioning in children with autism and
Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and Tourette syndrome. Development and Psychopatholy, 17(2),
developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology 415–445.
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 51–87. Volker, M. A., Lopata, C., Smerbeck, A. M., Knoll, V. A., Thomeer,
Pinkham, A. E., Hopfinger, J. B., Pelphrey, K. A., Piven, J., & Penn, M. L., Toomey, J. A., et al. (2010). BASC-2 PRS Profile for
D. L. (2008). Neural bases for impaired social cognition in students with high functioning autism spectrum disorders.
schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 188–199.
Research, 99, 164–175. Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further
Planche, P., & Lemonnier, E. (2011). Does the islet of ability on defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific
visuospatial tasks in children with high-functioning autism really language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication
indicate a deficit in global processing? Encephale, 37(1), 10–17. Disorders, 41, 319–346.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2005). Impaired Williams, D. L., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). The profile
memory for faces and social scenes in autism: Clinical impli- of memory function in children with autism. Neuropsychology,
cations of the memory disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy- 20(1), 21–29.
chology, 20, 1–15.

123

View publication stats

You might also like