You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/12340943

Executive Functions in Dyslexia

Article  in  Child Neuropsychology · April 2000


DOI: 10.1076/0929-7049(200003)6:1;1-B;FT037 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

170 2,059

2 authors:

Turid Helland Arve Asbjørnsen


University of Bergen University of Bergen
45 PUBLICATIONS   1,301 CITATIONS    95 PUBLICATIONS   3,115 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

1. The Bergen Longitudinal dyslexia Study View project

Prison education View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Turid Helland on 16 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Child Neuropsychology 0929-7049/00/0601-037$15.00
2000, Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 37-48 © Swets & Zeitlinger

Executive Functions in Dyslexia


Turid Helland1,3 and Arve Asbjørnsen2,3
1Department of Special Education, University of Oslo, Norway, 2Department of Psychosocial Science, Uni-
versity of Bergen, Norway, and 3Centre for Logopedics, Eikelund Resource Centre, Bergen, Norway

ABSTRACT

This study focused on executive functions in dyslexia. A group of 43 heavily-affected young dyslexics,
divided into two groups based on the results of a receptive language test, and 20 non-dyslexic controls,
were tested with a Dichotic Listening Test, the Stroop Color Word Test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test. The dyslexic subjects demonstrated significant impairment on all tasks, but with different patterns of
impairment according to the subgrouping. The subgroups were equally impaired on the Dichotic Listening
Test, but differed on the Stroop and the Wisconsin Tests. The data support a hypothesis suggesting execu-
tive problems in dyslexia, depending on receptive language skills.

Dyslexia is described as a word-decoding im- attention is a basic feature of executive func-


pairment (cf. Liberman, Shankweiler & Liber- tions. Prominent models of attention demon-
man, 1989; Stackhouse, 1996) and a temporal strate this (for reviews, see Halperin, 1996;
processing deficit (Tallal, Allard, Miller & Cur- Hugdahl, 1995; Mirsky, 1996). Lately, the
tiss, 1997). It is also described as a multifaceted attentional model of Mirsky et al. (Mirsky,
disorder (Kinsbourne, Rufo, Gamzu, Palmer & 1996) has been both validated and applied to
Berliner, 1991; Tallal, Allard, Miller, & Curtiss, clinical populations (Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Flet-
1997). The term suffers from a variety of defini- cher, Levin, Miner & Eisenberg, 1998; Loss,
tions reflecting the various professional view- Yeates & Enrile, 1998). According to this
points involved in dyslexia research (see Miles, model, attention can be subdivided into five dis-
1995; Lyon, 1995; Tønnesen, 1995 and 1997, tinct functions, each associated with different
for discussions of the definition). Primary symp- regions of the brain: the capacity to focus/
toms of dyslexia are, by nature, language-based, execute, to encode, to shift, to sustain, and to
but non-linguistic symptoms such as lack of stabilize attention. The organization of the atten-
concentration, disorganization, and forgetful- tional system implies both specialization and
ness are often reported by parents, professionals interaction. In this study three of these executive
and dyslexics themselves. Assessment of such task functions will be focused on: the sustain
functions in dyslexia has gained minor status function (the limbic system and midbrain),
compared to other cognitive constructs like which is defined in terms of vigilance or the ca-
memory and language processing. This study pacity to maintain focus and alertness over time;
will focus on executive functions in a group of the focus/execute function (prefrontal cortex),
heavily-affected dyslexics. which involves the ability to select relevant
One feature of executive functions is the abil- stimuli from a broad array and to complete tasks
ity to focus attention over time and to shift at- involving those stimuli in an efficient manner;
tention according to a behavioral program. Thus and the shift function (midbrain, prefrontal cor-

Address correspondence to: Turid Helland, Eikelund Resource Centre, PO Box 85, Sandbrekkevn. 27, 5232
Paradis, Norway. E-mail: turhel@ks-eikelund.no
Accepted for publication: March 29, 2000.
38 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

tex), which involves the ability to change the gested that left hemisphere verbal activation was
focus of executive functions in a flexible and interfered with by right hemisphere attentional
adaptive manner. activation, indicating an attentional control dys-
Executive functions are hypothesized to be function in LD. Also Asbjørnsen and Bryden
closely connected to the frontal lobes, which (1998) found attentional shift problems in read-
indicates a control system of the posterior and ing-disabled children. The authors suggested
basal systems of the brain (Stuss, 1995). There two possible interpretations: a basic attentional
is a general agreement that lesions/impairments deficit associated with a frontal, executive defi-
in one area of the brain can lead to distorted cit, and a deficit in temporal processing hamper-
functions in other areas. Thus, poor planning ing the attentional shift. However, others have
functions may not necessarily mean frontal lobe found little or no deviation in LD by applying
distortion: they may indicate reduced capacity in dichotic listening tests (Obrzut & Boliek, 1988).
other areas of the brain that are crucial to frontal Evarett, Warner, Miles and Thomsen (1997)
lobe functions. According to Luria (1973) there found a marked interference effect on the color/
is a disturbance in planned organized acts in pa- word naming subtest of the Stroop Color Word
tients with frontal lesions, especially in those Test in dyslexic children compared to age-
with impairment to the left frontal hemisphere. matched, but not to reading-age controls. Fisher,
This is due to the loss of inner speech as a regu- DeLuca & Rourke (1997) found impaired per-
lator of behavior. formance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Also, information-processing approaches em- (WCST) among children with non-verbal LD,
phasize the role of inner speech as a regulator of but not among children with verbal LD. Levin
executive functions and a key to learning and (1990) found that dyslexic children exhibited
metacognition (Eslinger, 1996). In spite of this, organizational problems in both linguistic and
attempts at relating dyslexia to attention and visuo-spatial tasks. This could not, however,
executive functions have yielded diverging re- explain dyslexia, but the study pointed to a vari-
sults (Kinsbourne, Rufoe, Gamzu, Palmer & able pattern of organizational impairment in
Berliner, 1991). Brain imaging of language pro- dyslexia. Condor and Saling (1995) found indi-
cesses demonstrates frontal activity (Ryding, cations of delayed development of skills for effi-
Brådvik & Ingvar, 1996). Based on event-re- cient planning on problem solving in two read-
lated potentials (ERPs) Segalowitz, Wagner and ing-disabled samples on the Tower of Hanoi.
Menna (1992) found that attention accounted for According to Halperin (1996), executive
differences among poor readers. They held that functions in early childhood seem to be driven
learning to read is one of the most demanding by stimuli in the environment. The child/parent
tasks encountered by the child in school, placing interaction is crucial to the development of di-
a heavy demand on the child’s attentional capac- rected attention and to the regulation of behavior
ity in general and on the frontal system in partic- through outer speech internalized to inner
ular. McPherson, Ackerman, Holcomb and Dyk- speech (Vygotsky, 1962) or metacognition (Bor-
man (1998) based their two groups on a median kowski & Burke, 1996). The child’s ability to
split of scores from a visual non-word test. By orient his/her attention develops gradually. By
ERPs they found that the recognition of orthog- the age of 5 or 6 years the internally driven abil-
raphy was controlled through frontal processes. ity to scan the environment in an active way is
None of the two groups showed reduced frontal well established; and the ability to focus atten-
activities, but the poorer decoders showed poor tion should be fully established by the age of 7.
left posterior phonological priming. Sustained attention continues to develop into
Applying a dichotic listening test Kershner adolescence (Hugdahl & Andersen, 1986; Hug-
and Morton (1990) demonstrated that learning- dahl, Helland, Færevaag, Lyssand & Asbjørn-
disabled children (LD) were inferior to controls sen, 1995).
in recalling left ear digits and in shifting atten- If a child is deprived of environmental stimuli
tion between channels. The investigators sug- due to either external circumstances or factors
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN DYSLEXIA 39

within the child itself, inner language as a regu- functions vary according to receptive language
lator of behavior may not be well developed. A abilities.
person’s use of inner speech in self-regulation
should, to a certain degree, be inferred or de-
tected through task demands as being rule-gov- METHOD
erned or rule-generating. Rule-governed task
demands means that fixed rules are given to the Participants
subject ahead of task. Rule-generating task de- The participants were 43 dyslexic subjects from 42
mands means that no rules are given in advance different Norwegian schools: 36 males and 7 fe-
males, mean age 12.67 (SD 1.68); and a control
as how to solve the task. Comparing the effects
group of 20 subjects from one school: 16 males
of these two types of task demands, Hayes et al. and 4 females, mean age 12.11 (SD 0.36). All sub-
(1996) held that rule-governed tasks induced jects attended regular classes in Norwegian
less variability and more rigidity of response, schools. Norway has a unitary state school system,
while rule-generating tasks induced more effi- with minor variations between schools as to
cient response patterns. Dyslexic subtyping by agenda and curricula, and with relatively small
language development disorder was done by social differences.
The control group (Con) had normal achieve-
Kinsbourne and Warrington in the early 60’s ments in reading and writing, normal intellectual
(Kinsbourne, 1986). abilities and no records of needs for special educa-
Bishop (1997) refers to several studies of in- tion, according to the classroom teachers and
attentiveness to phonology, morphology, syntax school counselors. One of the pupils was left-
and semantics in children with Specific Lan- handed. At the onset of the school year the class
guage Impairment (SLI). She argues that these consisted of pupils from different classes and
impairments change into subtle and not easily schools, due to entrance into junior high school.
This legitimizes the use of one class as a control
detectable pragmatic and discursive deficits in group.
some older SLI children. Likewise, Korkman The dyslexic group (Dys) consisted of strin-
and Häkkinen-Rihu (1994) found that the test gently selected children with reading and writing
profiles of children with developmental lan- problems. Originally they were referred to the lo-
guage disorders could be clustered into five sub- cal psychology agencies for assessment and coun-
groups, two of which appeared to be good pre- seling. Those with very low skills in reading
and/or writing, or with a lack of response to
dictors of spelling problems. These were the
remediation, were further referred to a regional
Global Language Impairment subtype, consist- logopedic clinic. For inclusion in the study, the
ing of children with deficiencies in both recep- clinical participants were required to be within
tive and naming functions, and the Specific normal range of intelligence as defined by full
Comprehension subtype, showing impairment in scale IQ > 70, with either VIQ > 80 or PIQ > 80,
comprehension of complex verbal instruction. and reading or writing skills at least two years be-
These findings give a theoretical foundation for low actual grade. Pre-school SLI was reported in
23 of the dyslexic subjects. For measurements of
further assessment of the linkage between recep-
single word reading and spelling, the Norwegian
tive language abilities, executive functions and version of the Aston Index (Newton & Thomson,
dyslexia. 1976) was applied. For assessment of silent read-
In this study the following two questions ing a commonly used screening test in Norwegian
were investigated: Are there differences be- schools, the Carlsten Test (1982), was used (see
tween a control group and a dyslexic group in Table 1 for baseline data). All participants had had
their executive functions? Will dyslexic sub- all their schooling in Norway. Subjects showing
signs of any other impairments (ADHD, different
groups, split by receptive language skills, ex- syndromes, neurological impairments, impaired
hibit different profiles of executive functions? sight or hearing) or showing incompatibility with
The hypotheses were that there are differences the definition of dyslexia as offered by The Orton
in executive functions between controls and Dyslexia Society Research Committee (1994), ac-
dyslexics, and that within a dyslexic group these cording to which dyslexia is a specific, language-
40 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

based disorder of constitutional origin, were ex- to follow the instructions of directed attention
cluded from the study. Ten of the 43 dyslexic pu- (Hugdahl, 1989). Since the log-odds ratio has a
pils were left-handed. No effects of age, gender or known variance (Knoke & Burke, 1980), an error
hand preference were found in baseline data. term can be calculated, and thus a standard score
could be estimated. A z-score (zASI) > 1.645 is
Stimulus Material taken to show a significant attentional shift to the
The Dichotic listening test (DLCV – 108, Hugdahl left hemisphere. A zASI score of 0 means there is
& Asbjørnsen, 1991) is an auditory test commonly no attentional shift difference.
applied both to assess language lateralization and The applied version of the Stroop Color Word
sustained attention. It contains a standardized con- Test (Stroop) (Hugdahl, undated) is a visual test
sonant/vowel listening task with lists of 36 × 3 demanding verbal responses (color naming and
dichotic pairs, including six homonymic pairs on word reading), measuring the ability to focus and
each list. Dichotic listening (DL) was administered shift attention over time. The test consisted of 48
according to the instructions given in ‘‘Dichotic items for each of the three tasks of ‘‘color’’ nam-
listening with CV-syllables. Manual.’’ (Hugdahl & ing (C), ‘‘word’’ reading (W) and ‘‘color/word’’
Asbjørnsen, 1991) with the stop consonants b, p, t, naming (CW), presented in fixed order, and ana-
d, k, g preceding the vowel a. Each syllable was lyzed separately for speed (s) and correct re-
paired with any of the other syllables to make sponses (cr). By nature, the reading task is espe-
pairs. Thus, a total of 36 different pairs could be cially vulnerable to dyslexic impairments. Also,
found. The task consisted of three lists of 36 stim- Golden’s formula (Golden, undated) of calculating
uli presented to the subjects by earphones on a por- the interference scores (INT), was applied on both
table Sony cassette player. The subjects were given naming speed (INT-s) and correct responses (INT-
tasks with two conditions: (1) Non-Forced Atten- cr): INT = CWobs – CWpred, where the formula of
tion, where the task of the subject was to repeat predicament is:
what he or she heard best; (2) Forced Attention,
where attention was directed to the right ear (FR) CWpred. = C × W / C + W.
or the left ear (FL), balanced between subjects.
The subjects had to report for each item which syl- The PC version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
lable they heard. The answers were noted by the (WCST – CV2) (Heaton, Curtiss & Tuttle, 1993)
tester, and responses from stimuli to each ear were with visually presented feedback puts demands on
recorded and transformed to percentages of correct the ability to shift attention (Lezak, 1983). The
responses (Re% and Le% = percent correct re- WCST–CV2 offers a differentiated analysis of
sponses from stimuli to right and left ears, respec- strategies. The following six variables were exam-
tively). To analyze attentional shift efficacy, an ined in this study: total numbers of 1) cards admin-
Attentional Shift Index (ASI) was applied (As- istered (TA), 2) errors (TE), 3) perseverative re-
bjørnsen & Bryden, 1998). The index formula is sponses (PR), 4) categories completed (CC), 5)
based on a logarithmic transformation of the rela- perseverative errors (PE), and 6) nonperseverative
tion between hits and errors of a subject being errors (NPE).
tested under conditions of forced attention, and is In order to put into subgroups according to re-
a measure of the degree to which a subject is able ceptive language abilities, the Receptive Language

Table 1. Baseline Data for the Dyslexic Group: Wisc-r, Reading and Writing.

Valid N = 43 M (SD)
VIQ 85,63 (13,10)
PIQ 95,74 (15,61)
FIQ 89,70 (12,00)
Carlsten silent reading test 55,32 (34,60)
Carlsten sentence dictation 14,51 (4,31)
Aston Index, word reading test –3,00 (1,90)
Aston Index, word spelling test –2,63 (1,64)

Note. VIQ = verbal IQ; PIQ = performance IQ; FIQ = full scale IQ, all from WISC-R. Carlsten reading test:
silence reading, word pr. minute; Carlsten dictation: spelling mistakes on dictation of 5 sentences; Aston Index:
grade score under to actual grade.
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN DYSLEXIA 41

Test (Maul, 1989) was administered only to the the different tests in a Group (2: Con vs. Dys) ×
dyslexia group. The participants were first pre- Task mixed design. Due to unequal variance in the
sented with a series of pictures and asked to point two subgroups, speed scores on the Stroop were
out the pictures matching the sentences being read transformed to square root (Kirk, 1982). The
to them. The next task was to mark correct posi- scores were subjected to analyses of variance ac-
tions on a picture according to the sentences being cording to the basic design. Also, to test the hy-
read to them. The grammatical structures of the potheses on sub-grouping, further analyses were
sentences are simple and linguistically comparable performed using a Group (3: Con vs. d/n vs. d/un)
to English constructs (the genitive, the active and × Task design. An alpha level was set to .05. Sig-
the passive, spatial locations (e.g. ‘‘above’’, ‘‘to nificant main effects and interactions were fol-
the left of’’) and sequencing). One point was given lowed up with the Tukey HSD test.
for each correct sentence response; ½ point for a
partly correct response. The maximum score was
24 points, with a clinical cut off at 22 points (me- RESULTS
dian = 22.5 points). Due to the leveling out of lan-
guage development in early school years, the ex-
pected ceiling effect occurred. Significant correla- General Features
tions (Pearson) were found between the Maul test Preliminary analyses showed no effects of sex
and language tests in the WISC–R (Wechsler, or handedness, but an effect of age was found (p
1974) (Information: r = .343, p =.026; Similarities: < .05) on the speed measures of the Stroop. Ta-
r = .414, p = .006; Arithmetic: r = .351, p = .023), ble 2 shows descriptive statistics of data sub-
and the Aston Index (Vocabulary: r = 623, p =
jected to multivariate ANOVAs.
.000).

Procedure Dichotic Listening


The control group were tested individually at their A 3-way ANOVA with a Group (2: Con vs. Dys)
school during the school day, being administered × Condition (2: FR, FL) × Ear (2: right, left)
the DL, the Stroop and the WCST. The test order design yielded significant main effects of Group
was randomized over subjects. The dyslexic sub- × Condition (F (1,61) = 4,802, p = .03), Group ×
jects were tested individually as part of a clinical
assessment. The two subgroups were: ‘‘dyslexic
Ear (F (1,61) = 8.936, p = .004), Condition × Ear
norm’’ (d/n), score 22 points on the Receptive (F (1,61) = 40.641, p < .001), and Group × Condi-
Language Test, and ‘‘dyslexic under norm’’ tion × Ear (F (1,61) = 23.228, p < .001). A follow-
(d/un), score < 22 points on the same test. The d/n up test (Tukey HSD) showed that there were
subgroup consisted of 25 subjects (mean age 13.07 significant within-group effects in Con, with FR
(SD 1.51), M/F ratio 19/6) and the d/un subgroup Re% and FL Le% significantly higher than FR
of 18 (mean age 12.11 (SD 1.77), M/F ratio 17/1). Le% and FL Re% ( p < .001). No within-group
The age difference between the two subgroups was
not significant. The two subgroups differed on the effect was seen in Dys. A between-group effect
verbal subtests of the WISC–R (d/n VIQ = 89.88; was seen in FR Re%, with significantly higher
d/un VIQ = 82.50) but not on the performance part overall scores in Con (p < .01). As to the sub-
(d/n PIQ = 96.20; d/un PIQ = 95.11) . However, groups, a 3-way ANOVA with a Group (3: Con
none of the differences were significant. Both sub- vs. d/n vs. d/un) × Condition (2: FR, FL) × Ear
groups exhibited the ACID profile typical of dys- (2: right, left) design yielded significant main
lexia, with low scores on ‘‘arithmetic’’, ‘‘coding’’,
effects of Group (F (2,60) = 3.502, p = .04),
‘‘information’’, and ‘‘digit span’’. Pre-school SLI
was reported in 72.27% of the subjects in the d/un Group × Ear (F (2,60) = 6.888, p = .002), Condi-
subgroup, while this was reported in only 40% of tion × Ear (F (1,60) = 25,207, p < .001), and
the d/n subgroup, which may explain the differ- Group × Condition × Ear (F (2,60) = 11.430, p <
ences on the WISC–R between the subgroups. Due .001). The follow-up test (Tukey HSD) showed
to missing data 7 cases (4 from d/n, 3 from d/un) no within-group effects in the subgroups. A be-
had to be omitted from the Stroop, and 2 cases (1 tween-group effect was seen in FR Re%, with
from d/n, 1 from d/un) from the WCST.
significantly higher overall scores in d/n (p <
Data Scoring and Analysis .01).
Initial analyses were made of the raw-scores from
42 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

Analyses of the zASI showed that Con at- yielded no effect, and age was hence left out as
tained a significant shift score (see Table 3). a covariate. A 2-way ANOVA with a Group (2:
Neither Dys nor any of the subgroups attained a Con vs. Dys) × Task (3: C-s, W-s, CW-s) design
significant shift score; and the scores were sig- yielded significant main effects of Group (F (1,54)
nificantly lower than in Con. There was no sig- = 23.2635, p < .001) and Task (F (2,108) =
nificant difference between the subgroups. The 100.083, p < .001), but no Group by Task effect.
distribution of the zASI scores are shown in Fig- The follow-up test (Tukey HSD) showed that
ure 1. A summary of the DL result is that signif- Dys needed more time (p < .01) compared to
icant differences were found between controls Con on all three tasks. A within-group effect (p
and dyslexics in reports from stimuli to the right < .001) was found in Con, with significant lower
ear, which also gave an impaired attentional time consumption on W-s (p < .001) and higher
shift score in the dyslexia group. on CW-s (p < .001) compared to C-s. A within-
group effect was also found in Dys, with CW-s
Stroop significantly higher (p < .001) than both C-s and
W-s. A separate two-way ANOVA splitting on
Speed the dyslexia subgroups yielded significant main
The square root transformations were done on effects of Group (F (2,53) = 15.9132, p < .001)
the C-s, W-s, and CW-s before the data analy- and Task (F (2,106) = 102.457, p < .001), but no
ses. Preliminary analyses with age as a covariate Group by Task effect. Tukey HSD revealed an

Table 2. DL, Stroop and WCST, Means and SD.

Con Dys d/n d/un


N = 20 N = 43 n = 25 n = 18

DL M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)


FR Re% 57,00 (9,91) 41,04 (10,78)a 44,33 (11,05)a 36,48 (8,74)a, b
FR Le% 32,00 (9,64) 40,92 (11,23)a 40,66 (11,55) 41,30 (11,09)
FL Re% 39,00 (10,87) 40,06 (8,78) 42,77 (8,55) 36,30 (7,83)
FL Le% 47,33 (10,24) 44,57 (9,37) 44,00 (9,86) 45,37 (8,87)

Stroop N = 20 N = 36 n = 22 n = 15
C-s 34,36 (4,90) 52,22 (23,67)a 43,86 (9,89) 64,48 (31,98)a,b
W –s 19,64 (3,12) 49,35 (45,49)a 43,26 (40,60)a 57,06 (51,01)a
CW –s 58,26 (6,90) 90,42 (32,44)a 83,80 (27,56)a 100,10 (37,38)a
C –cr 47,00 (1,12) 46,39 (1,55) 47,00 (1,69) 46,27 (1,39)
W –cr 47,90 (0,31) 47,27 (1,34) 47,23 (1,22) 47,33 (1,54)
CW –cr 44,15 (1,95) 40,08 (6,05)a 39,23 (6,21)a 41,27 (5,82)

WCST N = 20 N = 42 n = 25 n = 17
PE 14,55 (7,29) 18,31 (9,82) 16,51 (7,21) 20,82 (7,68)
NPE 17,00 (7,62) 27,90 (14,85)a 24,29 (14,70) 33,00 (13,92)a

Note. Con = control group; Dys = dyslexia group; d/n = dyslexic subgroup, norm on receptive language test; d/un
= dyslexic subgroup, under norm on receptive language test. DL = Dichotic Listening: FR = forced right, FL =
forced left, Re = Right ear, Le = Left ear. Stroop = Stroop Colour and Word Test: C = colour, W = word, CW =
colour/word, s = speed, cr = correct responses. WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: TA = total administered,
TE = total errors, PR = perseverative responses, CC = categories completed. Significant between group differ-
ences are marked: a = significant difference to controls, b = significant difference between the d/n and d/un
subgroups. Alfa level: p < .05.
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN DYSLEXIA 43

Table 3. DL, z-Scores of Attentional Shift Index (zASI), Means and STD.

Con Dys d/n d/un


DL N = 20 N = 43 F(1,61) n = 25 n = 18 F(2,60)
zASI 2,47 0,37a 22,36 0,40a 0,33a 11,01
(2,30) (1,23) (1,38) (1,03)

Note. Significant effects of 1-way ANOVA are marked. Other abbreviations as in Table 2.

expected significant overall lower score on the ferentiated by the subgrouping, where d/un ex-
W-s in Con (p < .01) compared to all other hibited significantly higher speed interference
scores. The CW-s score on each of the two sub- than Con. No significant difference was seen in
groups were equally significantly higher (p < Con vs. d/n.
.05) than the other scores. A between-group ef-
fect was seen in Con vs. d/un (p < .001) and d/n Correct responses
vs. d/un on the C-s (p = .02) due to higher time A 2-way ANOVA with a Group (2: Con vs. Dys)
consumption in d/un. × Task (3: C-cr, W-cr, CW-cr) design yielded
These effects were further confirmed by the significant main effects of Group (F(1,54) =
INT-s (see Table 4). Dys showed significantly 11.133, p = .002) and Task (F(2,108) = 50.647, p
more speed interference than Con. This was dif- < .001), and a Group by Task effect (F(2,108) =

Fig. 1. DL. Plot of individual zASI scores (= z-scores, Attentional Shift Index) separate for the three groups.
Dotted line represents 1.65, the limit for significant attentional shift. Other abbreviations as in Table 3.
44 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

5.830, p = .004). The Tukey HSD test showed effect was seen in Con, while this was seen in
that the effects were mainly due to significant Dys (p = .01).
overall between- and within-group effects of the Further, a 2-way ANOVA with a Group (3:
CW-cr (p < .001), with more errors being made Con, d/n, d/un) × Errors (2: PE, NPE) design
in Dys. There was a within-group effect (p < was performed. The analysis yielded a main ef-
.05) in Con due to more mistakes on the CW-cr fect of Group, (F(2,58) = 6.855, p = .002), and
task. For the subgroups a 2-way ANOVA with a Errors, (F(1,58) = 29.553, p < .001), and also a
Group (3: Con vs. d/n vs. d/un) × Task (3: C-cr, Group by Errors effect, (F(2.58) = 3.749, p = .03).
W-cr, CW-cr) design yielded a significant main The Tukey HSD follow-up test showed that the
effect of Group (F(2,53) = 6.060, p = .004) and effects were due to overall significant differ-
Task (F(2,106) = 66.287, p < .001), and a Group ences to NPE in d/un (p < .001), and within-
by Task effect (F(4,106) = 3.685, p = .008). The groups effects in both d/n and d/un (p < .01).
Tukey HSD test revealed significant between- This result was underlined by the separate 1-way
and within-group effects (p < .001) of CW-cr ANOVA with a Group (Con vs. Dys and Con vs.
due to more errors being made in d/n, but no d/n vs. d/un) × Scores (4: TA, TE, PR, CC) de-
effect of d/n vs. d/un or of Con vs. d/un. sign. As can be seen in Table 5, Dys as a whole
These effects were also seen in the INT-cr had significantly more trials and errors than
(see Table 4). Dys exhibited significantly lower Con, but with no effect as to perseveration (PR).
interference score, indicating more mistakes Con completed significantly more category sets
being made in Dys than in Con. As to the sub- (CC) than Dys. The subgrouping yielded a dif-
groups, the d/n subgroup yielded significantly ferentiated picture with significant differences
lower scores than Con, with no significant ef- between Con and d/un on all scores, but not be-
fects of d/un vs. Con or d/un vs. d/n. tween Con and d/n. A significant difference in
d/n vs. d/un emerged on the CC score.
WCST The findings can be summarized as follows.
Perseverative and non-perseverative error scores There were significant differences between Con
were analyzed separately in a 2-way ANOVA and Dys on all three tasks. The subgrouping by
with a Group (2: Con, Dys) × Errors (2: PE, receptive language abilities yielded a differenti-
NPE) design. This revealed a main effect of ated pattern of executive functions. The d/n sub-
Group (F(1,59) = 8.119, p = .006), a main effect group was impaired on the DL and the correct
of Errors (F(1,59) =16.419, p < .001) and a signif- responses on the Stroop, while the d/un group
icant Group by Errors effect (F(1,59) = 5.771, p = was impaired on the DL and the WCST with
.019). The Tukey HSD follow-up test showed impaired speed scores on the Stroop.
that the effect was due to an overall significantly
higher NPE in Dys than in Con (p < .001), with
no significant effects for PE. No within-group

Table 4. Stroop Interference Scores (INT), Means and SD.

Con Dys d/n d/un

N = 20 N = 36 F(1,54) n = 21 n = 15 F(2,53)
a a
INT s 5,11 5,97 8,13 5,82 6,19 4,60
(0,41) (1,32) (1,45) (1,12)
INT cr 20,43 16,68a 7,40 15,82a 17,88 4,50
(1,89) (5,89) (6,13) (5,77)

Note. Significant effects of 1-way ANOVA are marked. Abbreviations as in Table 2.


EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN DYSLEXIA 45

Table 5. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), Means and STD.

WCST Con Dys d/n d/un

N = 20 N = 41 F(1,59) n = 24 n = 17 F(2,58)
a a
TA 111,40 120,63 5,65 117,63 124,88 4,24
(17,98) (12,06) (13,14) (9,10)
TE 31,55 46,22a 8,12 40,83 53,82a 6,85
(15,90) (20,14) (20,14) (18,06)
PR 15,60 19,56 2,94 17,54 22,41a 3,20
(7,87) (8,82) (8,25) (9,05)
CC 5,55 4,41a 9,94 4,88 3,76a,b 9,47
(0,83) (1,50) (1,39) (1,39)

Note. Significant effects of 1-way ANOVA are marked. Other abbreviations as in Table 2.

DISCUSSION The focus/execute functions (Mirsky,


1996) involve the ability to select relevant stim-
The main findings of this study were that im- uli and to complete tasks involving those stimuli
paired executive functions were found in the in an efficient manner. These functions were
dyslexia group as a whole, and that these func- assessed by the Stroop test, which, like the DL,
tions were differentiated by the subgrouping is a verbal test, but which differs from it in that
according to receptive language impairment. the Stroop test gives visual cueing demanding
The dyslexic subgroup with receptive language verbal responses. Dyslexics could react in two
deficits was more impaired than the subgroup ways on the interference task of the Stroop.
without receptive language deficits. This was They could show no interference, due to lack of
seen mainly in the WCST, but was also evident automaticity in reading, or suppress the reading
in the other tests. Impaired executive functions process, either by double interference or by a
in dyslexia has been referred to in other studies, possible priming effect of the reading task. One
but the linkage to linguistic abilities yielding main feature of the test results was the variabil-
differentiations as to executive functions in dys- ity within the dyslexia group as to speed, with
lexia has, to our knowledge, not been reported overall slower responses in both subgroups. This
previously. should not be surprising in a clinical group that
The sustain function (Mirsky, 1996), as as- is vulnerable to verbal processing and reading
sessed by the DL, was impaired in both sub- (McDougall & Hulme, 1994). In contrast to the
groups, with significantly lower shift scores other language-based test, DL, the subgroups
compared to the control group. These results differed on the Stroop. The d/n subgroup
were as expected, lending support to the studies showed little impairment as to the naming test
of both Asbjørnsen and Bryden (1998) and (C), in opposition to the d/un subgroup. Signifi-
Keshner and Morton (1990). However, it is open cant speed impairment was most distinct in the
to discussion whether the impaired scores are d/un group. As to the analyses of correct re-
due to impaired executive functions or to a tem- sponses, only the d/n subgroup diverged signifi-
poral processing impairment. The absence of cantly from the control group. This indicates a
within-group variation in the dyslexia group difference as to the focus/execute function be-
gives support to both possibilities. In dyslexia a tween the two dyslexic subgroups; one exhibit-
combination of the two should be a very plausi- ing impaired processing as to speed, the other
ble interpretation, especially when these func- exhibiting impaired processing as to correct re-
tions are viewed as interactive parts of a neuro- sponses. Both results are compatible with the
logical and cognitive network. hypothesis of Everatt, Warner, and Miles (1997)
46 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

that dyslexics have fewer resources available for but the subgrouping yielded two different pro-
controlling automatic word reading. However, files of the task demands.
the study also points to a differentiation in dys- Assets of the d/n group were the attentional
lexics in strategies for handling their processing shift function and rule-generating behavior; def-
impairment. icits were the focus/execute function and rule-
The ability to shift attention in a flexible governed behavior. This indicates ability to use
manner was assessed by the visual test WCST. attentional shifts in a flexible way, to maintain
The dyslexia group exhibited significant impair- focus over time in an efficient manner, and to
ments compared to the control group by needing apply inner language as an organizer of behav-
more trials, making more errors and completing ior. The subgroup’s response pattern to tasks
fewer categories. There were no significant dif- where external rules had to be followed indi-
ferences in perseverative errors (PE), but signif- cates a didactic problem for, e.g., classroom
icant differences in non-perseverative errors teaching of this subgroup.
(NPE). The dyslexia group needed more trials Assets of the d/un subgroup were the abilities
and errors to find, maintain and shift strategies. to focus attention, select relevant stimuli, exe-
The d/n subgroup did not differ significantly cute tasks in a relevant way and being respon-
from the control group on any of the variables, sive to rule-governed task demands. The sub-
except for the variable measuring non-perse- group demonstrated impaired attentional shift
verative errors (NPE). The d/un, on the other functions, indicating impaired abilities to main-
hand, differed on all measures except for perse- tain focus and alertness over time and to change
verative errors (PE). According to Mirsky focus of attention in an adaptive way. The rela-
(1996), this indicates impaired shift abilities at a tive strength of this subgroup in a learning situa-
cognitive level, leaving problem solving more or tion should be to follow instructions, while more
less to luck. The elevated number of perseve- creative academic problem solving (such as in
rative responses (PR) could be interpreted as mathematics and composition) should be diffi-
impaired frontal activity in d/un. However, the cult.
lack of elevation on perseverative errors (PE) To summarize, significant differences were
did not support this hypothesis. The results in seen between the control group and the dyslexia
the d/n subgroup lend support to the study of group as a whole. The subgrouping yielded vari-
Fisher et al. (1997) finding close to normal ations within the dyslexic group rather than
scores on the WCST in dyslexia. On the other clear-cut dichotomies. To find valid subgroups
hand, the subgrouping demonstrated significant has not been the focus of this study, but rather to
impairment in the d/un subgroup, which is not show heterogeneity in dyslexia in accordance
consistent with the same study. From an infor- with theories of continuity (Kinsbourne, 1986).
mation-processing approach, it can be inferred However, significant differences between the
that the d/n subgroup were well able to use inner subgroups emerged in subtests that are essential
speech as a regulator of behavior, whereas the in the assessment of both dyslexia and executive
d/un were not. If it is right that different task functions. Generalizations from the results in
demands induce different problem solving strat- this study are debatable most of all due to the
egies, subjects with good abilities at using inner sampling techniques being used. It has been ar-
speech as a regulator of behavior should respond gued that in order to find symptoms of interest
positively to rule-generating tasks. Subjects with for the definition of dyslexia, one has to start by
poor abilities at using inner speech in problem assessing the poorest readers (Tønnesen, 1997).
solving may be expected to exhibit difficulties The findings in this study indicate the nature of
with rule-generating tasks, with a better chance executive assets and deficits in a group of
at succeeding with rule-governed tasks. The dys- heavily-affected reading and writing impaired
lexia group as a whole demonstrated impairment children. In a randomized sample of dyslexia,
on both rule-governed and rule-generating tasks, subjects in the d/un category may be under-
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS IN DYSLEXIA 47

represented and represented as outliers, having Halperin, J. M. (1996). Conceptualizing, describing,


little or no effect on the statistical analysis of and measuring components of attention. A sum-
mary. In R.G. Lyon & N.A. Krasnegor (Eds.), At-
executive functions.
tention, Memory, and Executive Function (pp.
The final conclusion of the study is that exec- 263–278). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
utive impairment is seen in dyslexia, specifically Hayes, S. C., Gifford, E. V., & Ruckstuhl, Jr. (1996).
in subjects with receptive language impairment. Relational frame theory and executive function: A
The deficit cannot be related solely to impaired behavioral approach. In R.G. Lyon & N.A.
Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, Memory, and Execu-
temporal linguistic or phonological functions, as tive Function (pp. 263–278). Baltimore: Paul H.
assessed by the DL and Stroop tests, since the Brookes.
language impaired subgroup showed impairment Heaton, R. K., Curtiss, G., & Tuttle, K. (1993). Wis-
on the visual test (WCST) also. consin Card Sorting Test: Computer version – 2.
Research Edition. Odessa, FL: Psychological As-
sessment Resources, Inc.
Hugdahl, K. (undated). Stroop Test. University of
REFERENCES Bergen.
Hugdahl, K. (1989). Handbook of Dichotic Listening:
Asbjørnsen, A.E. & Bryden, M.P. (1998). Auditory Theory, Methods and Research. Chichester, U. K.:
attentional shifts in reading-disabled students: Wiley & Sons.
Quantification of attentional effectiveness by the Hugdahl, K. (1995). Psychophysiology. The Mind–
Attentional Shift Index. Neuropsychologia, 36, 2, Body Perspective. Cambridge, MA, London, UK:
143–148 Harvard University Press.
Bishop, D. (1997). Uncommon Understanding. Devel- Hugdahl, K. & Andersson, L. (1986). The ‘‘forced-
opment and Disorders of Language Comprehen- attention paradigm’’ in dichotic listening to CV-
sion in Children. East Sussex: Psychology Press. syllables: A comparison between adults and chil-
Borowski, J. G. & Burke, J. E. (1996). Theories, mod- dren. Cortex, 22, 417–432.
els, and measurements of executive functioning: Hugdahl, K., & Asbjørnsen, A. (1991). Dikotisk
An information processing perspective. In R.G. lyssning til CV-stavelser. Manual. Stockholm:
Lyon & N.A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, Memory, Psykologiförlaget AB.
and Executive Function (pp. 263–278). Baltimore: Hugdahl, K., Helland, T., Færevaag, M. F., Lyssand,
Paul H. Brookes. E. T., & Asbjørnsen, A. (1995). Absence of ear
Carlsten, C. T. (1982). Norsk rettskrivings- og advantage on the Consonant-vowel Dichotic Lis-
leseprøver. Oslo, Norway: Universitetsforlaget. tening Test in adolescent and adult dyslexics: Spe-
Condor, A., Anderson, V., & Saling, M. (1995). Do cific auditory-phonetic dysfunction? Journal of
reading disabled children have planning problems? Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 6,
Developmental Neuropsychology, 11, 4, 485–502. 833–840.
Eslinger, P. J. (1996). Conceptualizing, describing, Kershner, J.R., & Morton, L. L. (1990). Directed at-
and measuring components of executive functions. tention in dichotic listening in reading disabled
A summary. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor children: A test of four models of maladaptive
(Eds.), Attention, Memory, and Executive Function lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 28, 181–198.
(pp. 263–278). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Kinsbourne, M. (1986). Models of fyslexia and its
Evarett, J., Warner, J., Miles, T. R., & Thomsen, M. subtypes. In Pavlidis, G. Th., & Fisher, D. F.
E. (1997). The incidence of Stroop interference in (Eds.), Dyslexia: Its Neuropsychology and Treat-
dyslexia. Dyslexia, 3, 222–228. ment. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M., Fletcher, J. M. Levin, Kinsbourne, M., Rufoe, D.T., Gamzu, E., Palmer,
H. S., Miner, M. E., & Eisenberg, H. M. (1998). R.L., & Berliner, A. K. (1991). Neuropsychologi-
Attention after pediatric traumatic brain injury: A cal deficits in adults with dyslexia. Developmental
multidimensional assessment. Child Neuropsychol- Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, 763–775.
ogy, 4, 1. Kirk, R. E (1982). Experimental design: Procedures
Fisher, N. J., DeLuca, J. W., & Rourke, B. P. (1997). for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Belmont,
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Halstead Cate- CA: Brooks-Cole.
gory Test performances of children and adoles- Knoke, D. and Bur, P. J. (1980). Log-linear Models.
cents who exhibit the syndrome of nonverbal learn- Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
ing disabilities. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 1, Korkman, M. & Häkkinen-Rihu, P. (1994). A new
61–70. classification of Developmental Language Disor-
Golden, C. J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. A der (DLD). Brain and Language, 47, 96–116.
manual for clinical and experimental uses. Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological Assessment
Cat.no.30150M. Illinois: Stoelting. (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
48 TURID HELLAND AND ARVE ASBJØRNSEN

Levin, B. (1990). Organizational deficits in dyslexia: difficulties. Wisbech: Learning developmental


Possible frontal lobe dysfunction. Developmental aids.
Neuropsychology, 6, 95–110. Obrzut, J. E. & Boliek, C. A. (1988). Dichotic Listen-
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. ing: Evidence from learning and reading disabled
(1998). The alphabetic principle and learning to children. In K. Hugdahl (Ed.), Handbook of Dich-
read. In Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. otic Listening: Theory, Methods and Research
(Eds.), Phonology and Reading Disability. Ann (475–511). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Ryding, E. , Brådvik, B., & Ingvar, D. H. (1996). Si-
Loss, N., Yeates, K. O., & Enrile, B.G. (1998). Atten- lent speech activates prefrontal cortical regions
tion in children with myelomeningocele. Child asymmetrically, as well as speech-related areas in
Neuropsychology, 4, 1. the dominant hemisphere. Brain and Language, 52,
Luria, A.R. (1973). The Working Brain. New York: 435–451.
Basic Books. Segalowitz, S., Wagner, J.W. & Menna, R. (1992).
Lyon G. R. (1995). Towards a definition of dyslexia. Lateral versus frontal ERP predictors of reading
Annals of Dyslexia. XLV. skill. Brain and Cognition, 20, 85–103.
Maul, J. (1989). Screening af lærning og forudsætning Stackhouse, J. (1996). Speech, spelling and reading:
for læsning. Herning: Special-Pædagogisk forlag Who is at risk and why? In Snowling & Stackhouse
A-S. (1996). Dyslexia, Speech and Language: A practi-
McDougall, S. & Hulme, C. (1994). Short-term mem- tioner’s handbook. London: Whurr Publishers.
ory, speech rate and phonological awareness as Stuss, D. T. (1995). Neuropsychology of the frontal
predictors of learning to read. In Hulme, C. & lobes. Lecture/handout at the INS conference in
Snowling, M. (Eds.), Reading Development and Uppsala, Sweden.
Dyslexia. London: Whurr Publishers. Tallal, P., Allard, L., Miller, S., & Curtiss, S. (1997).
McPherson, W. B., Ackerman, P. T., Holmcomb, P. Academic outcomes of language impaired chil-
J., & Dykman, R. A. (1998). Event-related poten- dren. In C. Hulme, & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dys-
tials elicited during phonological processing differ- lexia: Biology, cognition and intervention. Lon-
ential subgroups of reading disabled. Brain and don: Whurr Publishers.
Language, 62, 163–185. Tønnesen, F. E. (1995). On defining dyslexia. Scandi-
Miles, E. (1995). Can there be a single definition of navian Journal of Education Research, 39, No. 2.
dyslexia? Dyslexia 1, 36–45. Tønnesen, F. E. (1997). How can we best define
Mirsky, A. F. (1996). Disorders of attention. A neuro- ‘‘dyslexia’’? Dyslexia, 3, 78–92.
psychological perspective. In R. G. Lyon & N. A. Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and Language. Cam-
Krasnegor (Eds): Attention, Memory, and Execu- bridge, MA: MIT Press.
tive Function (263–278). Baltimore: Paul H. Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Brookes. Children – Revised. Norwegian Edition by
Newton, M. S. & Thomson, M. E. (1976). The Aston Undheim, J.O. Jaren, Norway: Vigga-trykk.
Index: A screening procedure for written language

View publication stats

You might also like