You are on page 1of 20

This article was downloaded by: [University of Miami]

On: 18 September 2013, At: 11:50


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Sports Sciences


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

A review of batting in men’s cricket


a a a
Richard A. Stretch , Roger Bartlett & Keith Davids
a
Sport Bureau, University of Port Elizabeth, South Africa
b
The Centre for Sport and Exercise Science, Sheffield Hallam University,
Collegiate Crescent Campus, Sheffield S10
c
Department of Exercise and Sport Science, Manchester Metropolitan
University, Hassall Road, Alsager ST7 2HL, UK
Published online: 09 Dec 2010.

To cite this article: Richard A. Stretch , Roger Bartlett & Keith Davids (2000) A review of batting in men’s
cricket, Journal of Sports Sciences, 18:12, 931-949, DOI: 10.1080/026404100446748

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026404100446748

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our
licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or
suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are
the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis.
The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with
primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever
caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the
Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can
be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2000, 18, 931± 949

A review of batting in men’ s cricket


RICHARD A. STRETCH,1* ROGER BARTLETT2 and KEITH DAVIDS3
1
Sport B ureau, University of Port Elizabeth, PO Box 1600, Port Elizabeth 6000, South Africa, 2The Centre for Sport and
Exercise Science, Sheý eld Hallam University, Collegiate Crescent Campus, Sheý eld S10 2BP, UK and 3Department
of Exercise and Sport Science, Manchester Metropolitan University, Hassall Road, Alsager ST7 2HL, UK

Accepted 29 November 1999

In this review, we critically evaluate the scienti® c research into the morphology and physiology of cricket bats-
men. We consider all aspects of the motor control of this skill, in the context of research into dynamic interceptive
actions, the biomechanics (kinematics and kinetics) of the various phases of batting strokes and injuries to
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

batsmen. Some attention is also devoted to batting equipment and to psychological factors in batting. Because of
the lack of published scienti® c research into women’ s cricket, this review focuses on the men’ s game and covers
research on batsmen of various playing standards. For the future, we see as a high priority research into injury
mechanisms, rather than simple injury statistics, and the role of cricket equipment design in injury prevention. A
second priority is for multi- or inter-disciplinary research, linking the biomechanics of batting to the underlying
motor control of the movements and the eþ ect of environmental information. Biomechanical studies of the
variability of the batsman’ s movements are needed, and these should be related to the compensatory variability
proposal of ecological psychology. Clearly, there is also a need for scienti® c research into batting in women’ s
cricket, which has been inadequately researched to date.

Keywords: biomechanics, cricket batting, injury, motor skills, physiology, psychology.

Introduction fore, we use the traditional term batsman, rather than


batter, throughout.
Batting in cricket requires motor and psychological
skills to play the best shot from a wide repertoire of
attacking and defensive strokes against bowlers of Morphology
diþ erent types ± fast, spin, seam and swing. A good
innings can last several hours (occasionally, days) and Studies of the morphology of cricket batsmen are
will involve the batsmen running the length of the limited. Most of these have been conducted as part
pitch (about 19 m) for each run scored (other than of research on cricketers in general rather than bats-
boundaries), wearing protective equipment, including men in particular (Foster and Elliott, 1985; Elliott
pads and helmet. The ® tness and morphology of the et al., 1986; Stretch, 1987, 1990; Peens, 1996). A tall,
batsman are, therefore, also important. Previous reviews athletic build, with morphological diþ erences between
of scienti® c research into cricket have concentrated on batsmen, bowlers and all-rounders, has been found for
the biomechanical and injury aspects of fast bowling international cricketers and for those of provincial,
(Bartlett et al., 1996; Elliott et al., 1996). The aim of this county and state standard. Mean somatotype ratings of
review is to evaluate the scienti® c research into the 2.5± 5.3± 2.1 (Stretch, 1987) and 3.1± 5.5± 2.0 (Stretch,
morphology and physiology of batsmen, their motor 1990) for provincial and 3.8± 4.4± 2.4 (Peens, 1996) for
skill, the biomechanics of batting, injuries, the equip- club cricketers have been documented. Provincial
ment used and the psychology of the game. Because of batsmen (Stretch, 1987: 2.5± 5.2± 2.0; Stretch, 1990:
the lack of published scienti® c research into women’ s 3.0± 5.7± 2.0) and club batsmen (Peens, 1996: 3.7±
cricket, this review focuses on the men’ s game; there- 4.5± 2.4) have been found to have endo-mesomorphic
somatotypes. It could, of course, be argued that this
* Author to whom all correspondence should be addressed. e-mail: research shows little more than a bias towards large
sparas@upe.ac.za fast bowlers. Anthropometric diþ erences have been

Journal of Sports Sciences ISSN 0264-0414 print/ISSN 1466-447X online Ó 2000 Taylor & Francis Ltd
http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals
932 Stretch et al.

found between batsmen and bowlers, with the batsmen for the next bout of intensive activity. Foster’ s con-
tending to be shorter and lighter than the bowlers, but tention was that cricketers need to be able to repeat
having a greater relative fat mass. Bowlers have been many intensive eþ orts lasting on average 10± 20 s. These
shown to have a signi® cantly greater androgyny index brief periods of intense activity need to be repeated,
and absolute muscle and bone masses than batsmen although at diþ erent regularities and frequencies, by
(Stretch, 1987, 1990). It may be that bowling selectively batsmen, fast bowlers and ® elders. An interval-training
favours larger and taller players; for batting, size is not programme was recommended by Foster (1978) to
necessarily advantageous. develop both the aerobic and anaerobic energy systems
most eþ ectively.
It could be argued that future research needs to
Physiological factors determine more clearly the demands of batting in par-
ticular rather than cricket in general (see, for example,
The importance of physical conditioning to comple- Davis et al., 1994). Stretch and Buys (1991) reported
ment a cricketer’ s skills has been emphasized by Pyke no signi® cant diþ erences in speed and agility between
et al. (1975), Foster (1978), Davis (1984) and Tyson batsmen, bowlers, all-rounders and wicket-keepers
(1985), particularly in the constraints of limited-overs taking part in the South African Universities Students
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

matches. This aspect of cricket batting has been badly Cricket Week. Comparisons between those who had
neglected by sport scientists, as is evident from the lack played at provincial standard and those who had only
of reported research. Foster (1978), Briggs (1980), played club cricket showed that provincial players
Davis (1984), Tomlin and Popplewell (1991) and Bull tended to perform better in the tests than club players.
et al. (1992) have drawn up guidelines for training The running speed (time to run three runs) of the
and for the evaluation of the ® tness of cricketers. Much batsmen was faster than for the other groups, although
of this work has been based on subjective evaluations, they had the slowest 50 m dash times and were ranked
rather than empirical scienti® c research, with some third in the shuttle run test of agility. These contrasting
application of basic physiological principles. results appear to indicate that the ability to run with
Using a portable calorimeter (Kofranyi Michaelis), pads on ± and the skill of running between the wickets
Fletcher (1955) determined the mean energy expen- (as assessed by the time to run three runs) ± play an
ditures for college cricketers playing in the nets to be important role here; batsmen may have re® ned these
1004 kJ ´m- 2 ´h- 1 for batting and 1012 kJ ´ m- 2 ´ h- 1 for skills through batting in matches.
bowling. Energy expenditure during match-play was
calculated by analysing the time spent in the component
activities and multiplying these times by the experi- Motor skills: Perception and action in cricket
mentally determined energy expenditure for each batting
activity in the nets. During inter-college matches, energy
expenditures of 765 kJ ´ m- 2 ´h- 1 for batting, 665 Batting in cricket is an example of a dynamic inter-
kJ ´ m- 2 ´ h- 1 for bowling and 468 kJ ´m- 2 ´h- 1 for ® elding ceptive action, which Whiting (1969) placed in his
were estimated. In an international Test match, it was second, most complex, category of ball skills ± that
predicted that the energy expenditures for batsmen, encompassing task constraints where a ball has to be
bowlers and ® elders were 648, 531 and 368 kJ ´ m- 2 ´ h- 1, received and sent away in the same movement. Skilful
respectively. The American College of Sports Medicine performance of dynamic interceptive actions is funda-
(1990) classi® ed the energy cost of playing cricket in mental to success in many ball games. To psychologists,
general ± not batting in particular ± as falling within the they have represented a useful vehicle for developing
range of 4.6± 7.4 METS. theoretical understanding of the relationship between
Foster (1978) monitored the activity patterns and perception and action in goal-directed behaviour (e.g.
telemetered heart rates of ® rst-class cricketers. He Whiting, 1969; Bootsma, 1988; Williams et al., 1992,
reported that the activity patterns of batting, bowling 1999; Harris and Jenkin, 1998).
and ® elding showed that players were required to repeat
many intensive activities, which do not normally last
The spatio-temporal constraints of dynamic interceptive
more than 20 s. Cricketers rarely sprint more than 70±
actions in ball games
80 m, with a distance of ~ 20 m the most common: this
is roughly the distance covered by each batsman for a Although there is little speci® c research on the processes
`run’ . The energy for this is supplied by anaerobic of perception and action during cricket batting, some
sources, using ATP-PC and the lactic acid energy sys- previous work has been done on similar dynamic inter-
tem. The aerobic energy source is used during recovery ceptive tasks, including striking in fast ball sports such
as well as to replace the phosphate stores in preparation as baseball and table tennis. Because of the similarities
A review of batting in men’s cricket 933

in task constraints across fast ball games involving weaknesses in judging absolute distance and speed,
striking, it may be instructive to compare how top-class beyond a few metres from the point of observation.
players satisfy the severe demands on their perceptual To understand processes of perception and action
and motor systems. In this section, we review some of in dynamic interceptive actions, it is relevant to ask how
the more general work on interceptive actions that is skilled players pick up visual information to satisfy the
relevant to cricket batting performance. spatio-temporal task constraints of batting.
The key to successful coordination and control
of dynamic interceptive actions is for the intercepting
Processes of perception and action: Theoretical approaches
limb or implement to be in the right place at the right
time (Savelsbergh and Bootsma, 1994; Regan, 1997; In the study of perception and action during dynamic
Williams et al., 1999). When performing dynamic inter- interceptive actions, there have been at least two distinct
ceptive actions, athletes need precise information to avenues for research. One line of work has tended
locate the ball in space (`where’ information) at a to focus on how dynamic interceptive actions can be
speci® c time (`when’ information). Savelsbergh and instantaneously regulated by perceptual information
Bootsma (1994) have argued that three task constraints (e.g. haptic, proprioceptive, auditory and visual) when
need to be satis® ed for successful performance of there is suý cient time available (Solomon and Turvey,
dynamic interceptive actions in sport. Performers need 1988; Savelsbergh and Bootsma, 1994). This approach
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

to: (i) ensure that they contact a moving projectile in the typically emphasizes the role of receptor anticipation
environment; (ii) contact it with the intended velocity; processes, in which actions need information for
and (iii) contact it with an intended spatial orientation initiation, completion and on-line guidance (Gibson,
to satisfy the accuracy requirements of the task. Success- 1979; Harris and Jenkin, 1998; Williams et al., 1999).
ful interceptive actions require good coordination not Functional on-line adjustments are necessary because
only between relevant parts of the movement system, early retinal image information on ball ¯ ight is unreli-
but also between relevant motor system components able, as the human visual system has diý culty esti-
and the projectile to be intercepted. The latter is a dif- mating the absolute distance and absolute speed of
® cult task, which can be performed with some success an approaching object (Regan et al., 1998). This is an
by most humans (Bootsma and Van Wieringen, 1990; apparent limitation that skilled bowlers seek to exploit
Regan, 1997). by such strategies as holding the ball loosely or
Quantitative descriptions of projectile speeds in hyperextending the wrist at delivery to vary bowling
cricket have indicated that the temporal constraints on speed and ¯ ight along the same trajectory (Regan,
cricket batsmen are severe. For example, Glencross and 1997). Despite this neurally based limitation, the
Cibich (1977) found that a ball delivered at 40.2 m ´ s- 1 human visual system can instantaneously predict the
took 439 ms to reach the batsman. It was assumed that trajectory of a ball to within less than 0.5° of the visual
the movement time was 250 ms, leaving just 189 ms ® eld and can perceive time-to-contact information
to react to the ball. The batsman also needs to pick up to within ± 2 or 3 ms (Lee, 1980; Bootsma and Van
visual information on late deviations in ¯ ight because Wieringen, 1990; Regan et al., 1998). Skilled cricketers
of added complications, such as spin, swerve or drag learn to cope with these visual system limitations
eþ ects. These ® ndings are broadly in line with those of through the development of a continuous link between
Regan (1997), who demonstrated how cricketers often the perceptual and action systems, which allows them to
have only 230 ms to deal with late ¯ uctuations in the use receptor anticipation processes late in the stroke
¯ ight of a ball approaching at 44.4 m ´s- 1. If one accepts to make ongoing adjustments.
the temporal values from these analyses, it is clear that, Another avenue of research, popular in cognitive
to bat successfully, cricketers have to initiate some psychology, is based upon the notion that the spatio-
movements, such as the backswing in the bi-phasic bat- temporal constraints of fast ball games are often so
ting action, before delivery of the ball. The temporal severe that extensive previous knowledge is required
constraints of spin bowling are a little better, but for a to anticipate perceptually the time-of-arrival of a pro-
ball delivered at 18 m ´ s- 1, Regan (1997) calculated that jectile at a speci® c point. Traditionally, in cognitive
it would hit the ground ~ 940 ms after release. Visual psychology, there has been a tendency to highlight the
reaction time was estimated at 150 ms and movement computational problems involved in performing under
time for the stroke at 200 ms, leaving about 600 ms the constraints of fast ball sports. In particular, the
for perception of ¯ ight characteristics. Regan’ s (1997) limited information-processing capacities of the human
analyses of the temporal constraints of slow bowling brain, modelled as a computer, have been emphasized
proposed an appreciable portion of time for perception (e.g. Fitts and Posner, 1967; Abernethy and Russell,
of ¯ ight characteristics. He argued that this period could 1984; Abernethy, 1987). The limitations of the human
be ® lled with unreliable information, owing to human information-processing system are typically not due
934 Stretch et al.

to threshold properties of the performer’ s perceptual actions, therefore, highlights the role of expert percep-
systems ± so-called `hardware’ factors (see Williams tion in decision-making, planning and stroke execution
et al., 1999). Rather, `software’ factors have been (e.g. Whiting, 1969; Abernethy, 1987). Expertise in
implicated: extensive knowledge and skill in using the knowing where and when to look for advance infor-
mental processes of perception, attention and memory mation is believed to support perceptual anticipation
underpin the inadequacy of the visual system. processes during time-constrained interceptive actions,
The limitations of human information processing such as cricket batting (Abernethy, 1981; Abernethy
were highlighted in early cognitive research on reac- and Russell, 1984). The conclusion that skilled
tion times using static interceptive actions (see also cricketers are more able to infer ball ¯ ight character-
Fischman, 1984; Christina, 1992). For example, mean istics from advance information from the bowler’ s body
values of 195 ms were obtained for simple reaction time movements has been supported by evidence on foot
in an interceptive action in which a statically positioned movement times during batting. For example, data on
tennis ball had to be grasped as quickly as possible the foot movements of skilled cricketers batting against
(Henry and Rogers, 1960). A mean of 36 ms was added fast bowling concur with those of Hubbard and Seng
to simple reaction times when participants were (1954) on step durations of baseball batsmen. Foot
required to perform an additional task component or movements were completed before the point of ball± bat
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

the accuracy of movement demands was increased. contact (Abernethy, 1982).


Given the severe spatio-temporal constraints of fast ball The propositions of the information-processing
games, these results indicate the importance not only of approach have rarely been tested in the context of
visual information from ball ¯ ight but also of pre-¯ ight cricket batting. One issue concerns the relevance of the
information, such as the movements of a bowler (e.g. results from studies of reaction time in the laboratories
Williams et al., 1992; Regan et al., 1998). Abernethy and of experimental psychologists to the time constraints of
Russell (1984) argued that, because of the restrictive natural performance contexts, such as cricket batting.
time constraints of fast ball sports at the highest When the ball deviates, either forward or laterally,
standards of performance, the skilled performer is one during the latter part of ¯ ight, the batsman is forced
who `buys’ time for decision-making and response to modify the originally selected stroke under severe
preparation by exploiting advance information from time constraints. Are skilled cricketers burdened with
an opponent’ s movements. This ® nding explains why reaction times of around 200 ms in response to sudden
highly skilled games players rarely seem to react to unanticipated changes in ball ¯ ight during cricket bat-
unexpected events, but appear to operate `in the future’ . ting? McLeod (1987), using a ball projection machine,
They use what has been termed an `anticipatory mode’ found that three international class batsmen were not
of action (Whiting et al., 1973). able to make any adjustments to their stroke during the
last 180± 200 ms of ¯ ight. Although he raised the issue of
the large moment of inertia of the bat, McLeod (1987)
Skilled cricket batting: An information-processing
concluded that a range of values around 200 ms should
interpretation
be accepted as the minimum time required for visual
A classical information-processing approach to cricket feedback to in¯ uence initiation of an interceptive action.
batting emphasizes that stimulus information from the Abernethy and Russell (1984) used a ® lm-based
preparatory movements of a bowler before delivery may reaction time paradigm to examine diþ erences between
be encoded, together with features of early ball ¯ ight. skilled and unskilled cricketers in batting performance.
When batsmen take up a stance at the crease, they seek The task for the groups was to view ® lmed sequences of
any postural cues from the bowler that may assist in the run-up and delivery of two medium-pace bowlers
perceptually anticipating the line, length and speed of and to verbally report a stroke to play. The skilled group
the delivery. Once the ball has been released, parts required 22 ms less viewing time than the unskilled
of the trajectory may be visually tracked and relevant group to report a response decision. The results were
characteristics ± such as speed, swerve and spin ± have to taken to infer that skilled players use perceptual antici-
be identi® ed to predict when and where the ball will pation strategies to satisfy the severe spatio-temporal
pitch. In an inferential process, cues from the event are constraints of batting against fast bowling. Evidence
compared to an internalized representation of the `tar- of the role of visual search strategies in providing
get’ action already stored in memory from many similar advance information is a common feature of research on
experiences (see, for example, Marteniuk, 1976; Barber, expertise in sports, including those involving dynamic
1988). Based on the available perceptual information, interceptive actions (Williams et al., 1999). In cricket,
a decision is reached about whether there was a match Barras (1990) found that skilled batsmen tended to
with similar items in memory. focus on the bowling hand and ball until the bowler was
An information-processing approach to interceptive in a side-on position in the delivery stride. Thereafter,
A review of batting in men’s cricket 935

the batsman ® xated the anticipated area of ball release not have to be controlled separately by the brain.
above and lateral to the shoulder of the bowling arm. This strategy, based on years of repetitive drills and
A time lag of 0.94 s occurred between the initiation systematic practice, suggests how skilled cricketers can
of the visual ® xation of the release zone and the hand reproduce a consistent and highly reliable stroke under
appearing. Barras (1990) argued that this temporal severe time pressure.
delay ensured that the eyes were stationary when Support for this view emerges from the work of
the hand appeared to allow the pick-up of visual Tyldesley and Whiting (1975), who proposed the con-
information. cept of `operational timing’ as a means by which expert
Although sport scientists have become increasingly games players reduce temporal uncertainty of an inter-
aware of the role of advance perceptual information for ceptive action by practising until it has a highly consist-
performance of interceptive actions, bowlers in cricket ent duration. They argued that the development of
have also learned about its signi® cance before delivery good response consistency through practice signi® es
of a ball. For example, bowlers have developed tech- that expert performance of dynamic interceptive actions
niques to either obscure or confuse the relevant visual only demands attention to the input and decision-
information from the movement system before delivery. making components of performance. For expert games
Such techniques include: (1) delaying the onset of players, it appears that movement patterns can become
critical information, for example by screening the so replicable that the spatio-temporal constraints on
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

position of the bowling hand with the non-bowling hand playing the shot can be reduced to simply one of timing
or other parts of the body during the run-up; (2) the initiation of movement. In their study of striking
presenting false signals; (3) increasing the amount of actions, at the key points of movement initiation
information by adopting unorthodox positions before and bat± ball contact, the between-trials displacement
ball release; and (4) constantly varying the parameters diþ erences in the trajectory of the bat for experts
of ball delivery and ¯ ight, including angle of release, were negligible. From this information-processing
speed, swing, ¯ ight and direction. The batsman’ s task is perspective, trial-to-trial variability was regarded as
made more diý cult when a bowler is able to extract late dysfunctional and due to `noise’ in the central nervous
swing in the air or to get the ball to deviate oþ the pitch. system or perturbations within the environment. Other
This strategy, as noted by Regan (1997), forces the data on response accuracy in cricketers also suggest
batsman to rely on information from early segments of that skilled players are more accurate in reporting shot
ball ¯ ight, which the visual system cannot reliably pick selection, particularly during good-length deliveries
up. What can batsmen do in practice to counter these (Abernethy and Russell, 1984). Taken together, these
strategies? The capacity to pick up relevant information ® ndings can be taken to support the notion of the
from the run-up and delivery stride increases the possi- development and use of a general motor program for
bility of movement initiation before ball release. Practice batting strokes by skilled players.
should generally involve developing perceptual skill In summary, the ® ndings reviewed in this section
in picking up advance information from the relevant illustrate the importance of cricketers developing
body parts of bowlers. More speci® cally, videotapes perceptual anticipation strategies to overcome infor-
of an opponent’ s bowlers may be studied to reveal mation-processing limitations when batting. These
idiosyncrasies in the run-up and delivery stride, which ® ndings have been criticized for being too speci® c
signal intent. to experimental contexts in which participants are
required to react suddenly to the appearance of visual
signals. A clear distinction should be made between
Response programming
these speci® c constraints and typical performance
When information about bowlers’ movements and ball conditions in ball games, where athletes can modulate
¯ ight has been encoded, transformed and perceived, ongoing actions on the basis of continuously available
a motor response can be selected from the skilled information (Bootsma and Van Wieringen, 1990;
cricketer’ s repertoire to intercept the ball (see Tyldesley Whiting, 1991). The methodological techniques used
and Whiting, 1975). Hubbard and Seng (1954) by information-processing theorists have been criticized
observed remarkable consistency in the swing times for other reasons (for a full summary, see Williams
of professional baseball players. The implication is et al., 1992, 1999), including problems with ecological
that expert batsmen were able to coordinate the many validity of the experimental context, an apparent
degrees of freedom of the motor system (i.e. the dif- obsession with reaction-time paradigms, the failure to
ferent parts of the skeletomuscular apparatus that are implement realistic movement response measures, and
free to vary at any instant), by pre-programming the the use of static and small-scale stimulus displays to
motor response (see Bernstein, 1967). That is, each present information in experiments. We now highlight
individual component of the movement system does some of these key criticisms.
936 Stretch et al.

Criticisms of information-processing research on timing of invariant information about actions and objects can be
dynamic interceptive actions processed and committed to a uni® ed representation of
an event in memory, is rejected. This fallacy has been
Traditionally, there has been a tendency to focus on termed the `Grand Illusion’ (Harris and Jenkin, 1998).
coincidence anticipation or motion prediction tasks Ecological psychology instead emphasizes the biological
in which participants typically have to report verbally constraints on brain functions such as perception
when a (suddenly occluded) object or image arrives at and action (Davids and Bennett, 1998). A functional
a designated target (e.g. Abernethy and Russell, 1984). relationship between human perceptual and action
These paradigms emphasize perceptual anticipation systems has evolved to support the behaviours involved
processes, whereas in the study of natural interceptive in negotiating surfaces and interacting with objects
actions the emphasis is on receptor anticipation processes in typical environments (Gibson, 1979). Fundamental
(Poulton, 1957; Williams et al., 1992; Tresilian, behaviours, such as interceptive actions, are exempli® ed
1995). In dynamic interceptive actions, participants are in many ball games. Here, we focus on the visual system
typically able to view a ball until it is adjacent with the as the vehicle for our discussion of key ideas on the
bat. There is ample opportunity for information to pro- ecological approach to perception and action during
vide a steering or guiding role for action. The implica- interceptive actions.
tion is that the typically distinct constraints of motion
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

prediction and natural interceptive tasks may involve


Information aþ ords actions. The ® rst key idea of the
diþ erent control mechanisms. In natural interceptive ecological approach is that ambient light arriving at
actions, the execution times are far shorter than in the eye provides optical information for the performer
motion prediction tasks. Also, the ball is typically able because it already contains enormous structure. Accord-
to be viewed for the whole ¯ ight (e.g. 300 ms for one- ing to Gibson (1979), light reaches the eye after having
handed catching at 10 m ´s- 1; Alderson et al., 1974). As been re¯ ected oþ surfaces and objects, such as an
evidenced by results on timing from `ordinary people’ approaching cricket ball, in the environment. Light
and infants, the large variability in estimation of time- is re¯ ected in straight lines and exists in a highly
to-contact typically reported in simulation studies is structured distribution called an `array’ . In ecological
often not present in the performance of natural tasks psychology, optic variables refer to the properties of
such as rapid interceptive actions (Williams et al., the light re¯ ected in a lawfully structured way from
1999). Tresilian (1995, p. 237) also highlighted that important objects. These properties are available to be
`the variability (standard deviation of response times) perceived directly by all organisms equipped with a
of responses in CA [coincidence anticipation] tasks visual system. Optic variables come in many forms.
is some ® ve or six times greater than that observed They include information on the direction of motion of
in IAs [interceptive actions] performed under the an object, the distance from an observer that an object
same stimulus conditions’ . This ® nding, particularly will pass, and even the time until it contacts the point
with the practised observers and skilled catchers, of observation (tau) (for detailed reviews, see Gibson,
calls into question the use of coincidence anticipation 1979; Lee, 1980; Todd, 1981; Bootsma and Peper,
methods, such as ® lm-based tasks, for investigating the 1992; Michaels and Oudejans, 1992; Williams et al.,
timing of dynamic interceptive actions. The use of these 1999). Optic variables are characterized by the action
unrealistic paradigms to assess dynamic interceptive possibilities or aþ ordances they oþ er the sport per-
actions in sport also prevents expert performers from former. For example, advance information from body
demonstrating a tight coupling between the perceptual orientation and relative motion of joints in the move-
and action systems. As we shall see next, ecological ment system of a bowler running up to deliver a ball
psychologists propose this idea as the conceptual basis provides aþ ordances for a batsman. As a result of
of the relationship between perception and action. learning, the aþ ordances from the bowler’ s run-up and
delivery, as well as ball ¯ ight, can be exploited by skilled
cricketers as reliable sources of information to constrain
Perception and action in dynamic interceptive actions:
batting actions. With practice, the actions of a bats-
The ecological perspective
man can become tightly geared to the most important
In contrast to information-processing accounts, the eco- informational constraints in speci® c task conditions.
logical approach to perception and action emphasizes
the high quality of information continuously available The evolutionary basis of interceptive timing. A second
for direct perception in the environment (Gibson, important aspect of the ecological view is that informa-
1979). For this reason, information does not have to be tion is unambiguous and can be perceived directly by
embellished by inference with internalized models of the sport performer. This position contrasts strongly
the world. The notion in cognitive psychology, that with indirect accounts of `cues’ as tentative stimuli to be
A review of batting in men’s cricket 937

embellished by internalized representations. From an arguments (McLeod, 1987; Regan, 1997). Typically,
evolutionary perspective, the textured elements of the the most straightforward shot to play is hitting the ball
environment perturb the ¯ ow of light rays in meaning- straight down the line. As the game context changes,
ful ways for humans. The continuous availability of batsmen might need to play more risky shots across
information for pick up by purpose-designed perceptual the line of ¯ ight of the ball, such as the glance, cut or
systems is an important element of ecological explana- hook. In the leg glance, for example, the margin of
tions of the performance of dynamic interceptive error (estimated by the standard deviation around the
actions. It implies an existing sensitivity to the pick-up mean time taken to perform the action) is minimal. The
of optical information from objects during relative frontal plane of the blade of the cricket bat (» 10 cm
approach. The practice and experience of actions in wide) is turned almost 90° to de¯ ect the ball past the
speci® c performance contexts merely attune existing wicket keeper. Yet skilled cricketers were able to satisfy
sensitivity to optical sources of information, which successfully these severe spatio-temporal constraints
constrain action (Michaels and Beek, 1995). against fast bowling at speeds of around 40 m ´s- 1.
What evidence is there, in studies of dynamic inter- In the hook shot, the margin of error in perceiving time
ceptive actions, for the notion of an evolutionary based to contact information has been estimated at 2.5 ms
sensitivity to optical information from approaching (Regan, 1997).
objects? Von Hofsten (1983) videotaped infants aged For Lee and Young (1985, p. 2), the extraordinary
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

34 and 36 weeks reaching towards a brightly coloured demonstration of timing precision in non-games players
object travelling in a circular path in a horizontal plane and human infants `suggests that visual-motor systems
at speeds of 0.3, 0.45 and 0.6 m ´ s- 1. In total, 144 have evolved which are particularly eý cient at detecting
reaches were analysed to show highly functional time to contact and gearing actions to the information’ .
accuracy and precision of timing. Only 17 of the trials Recent modelling and evidence on neural mechanisms
resulted in the target being completely missed by the in the visual system also provide some support for the
infants. In the slowest target speed condition, the mean evolutionary basis of smart mechanisms in interceptive
timing error was only 9.4 ms in an average reaching time timing. Regan (1997) and Regan et al. (1998) have
of 550 ms. For targets moving at 0.45 m ´ s- 1, with a modelled how the detection of looming objects during
faster average reach time of 479 ms, the systematic error visually guided actions is supported by the presence of
fell to a mean of 4.4 ms. It is diý cult to conceive of speci® c neural ® lters in the central nervous system. One
infants being able to develop a representation to support crucial retinal detector is the `motion-in-depth’ ® lter,
such accurate performance of interceptive actions. whose magnitude of output is `inversely proportional
Rather, these ® ndings emphasize the view that per- to time to collision’ with an approaching object, such
ceptual processes are supported by smart (i.e. purpose- as a cricket ball (Regan et al., 1998, p. 186). Wang and
designed) mechanisms, which have evolved in the Frost (1992) have also reported that speci® c neurones
human central nervous system to become attuned in the brain respond with heightened electrical activity
to speci® c optical variables (Runeson, 1977). The idea at a constant time before contact during perception
of `smart visual perception’ can be viewed as the of information from looming objects. The maximum
`selective, task-oriented, gathering of information from response of the neuronal cells always occurred at a
the visual world’ for movement execution (Ballard et al., constant time to contact, regardless of changes in size
1998, p. 93). The role of smart mechanisms in dynamic and speed of the approaching stimuli. Motion of the
interceptive tasks is supported by other data showing stimuli in other directions, apart from straight towards
that precise timing is not just a function of the relatively the point of observation, failed to produce a signi® cant
slow speeds used by von Hofsten (1983) with infants. increase in the rate of ® ring of neurons. Other research
McLeod et al. (1986) asked non-games players to strike has also implicated special-purpose functional divisions
vertically dropping squash balls with paddle bats in the visual cortex with an evolutionary basis for
towards a designated target area. A high consistency supporting certain acts (e.g. Harris and Jenkin,
was soon reached by the participants in this novel inter- 1998; Milner, 1998). This suggests that multiple visual
ceptive action, with standard deviations of 10 ms found systems may exist in the cortex for extracting diþ erent
in 90% of trials and 5 ms in 50% of trials. McLeod and types of information when performing diþ erent
Jenkins (1991, p. 286) have argued that these results tasks. One such cortical pathway is dedicated to sub-
suggest that `ordinary people, without any particular consciously picking up and using visual information for
practice’ can produce ® ne timing in dynamic intercep- the purpose of controlling actions. The implication is
tive actions. From these results, it is easy to interpret that people may be able to exploit a sensitivity to optical
how expert cricketers can satisfy the task constraints of information within the central nervous system when
batting. Observations of expert cricketers playing dif- they need to time their actions to rapidly moving objects
® cult shots, such as the hook or leg glance, ® t these in space (Harris and Jenkin, 1998).
938 Stretch et al.

The studies reviewed above imply that skill in exempli® ed in the study of dynamic interceptive actions
dynamic interceptive actions may be based on an evo- by Bootsma and Van Wieringen (1990). The ability of
lutionary sensitivity to optic variables. It would appear experts to modulate precisely ongoing dynamic inter-
that this sensitivity is a resource that can be ® ne-tuned ceptive actions is predicated on a functional role for
with context-speci® c practice in ball games such as movement variability during interceptive actions. Com-
cricket (Williams et al., 1999). Skilled cricketers can pensatory variability is a functional type of variability. It
satisfy the severe task constraints of batting because is an important means by which skilled performers in
unambiguous optical information sources are con- dynamic environments can produce a tight ® t between
tinuously available to be sub-consciously picked up by the current state of the action system and the task goal
dedicated visual systems to constrain the assembly of at the all-important endpoint of execution, through
successful interceptive actions. From the emerging evi- modulating movements on the basis of ongoing per-
dence, therefore, it is apparent that the earlier account ceptual information. They suggested that action systems
from indirect perception, claiming that cricket batsmen and perceptual systems could be used to compensate
need to somehow encode and transform information for sudden changes through the covariation of move-
before selecting a pre-programmed response, is overly ment duration and initiation time for an interceptive
complex. Rather, the process of perception± action coup- action. Such variability between trials should be viewed
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

ling is advocated in ecological psychology as the basis of as compensatory, because early movement initiations
skilled interceptive timing behaviour (Savelsbergh and would be locked to slower drives and later ones to faster
Bootsma, 1994; Williams et al., 1999). drives. When the interceptive action was broken up into
® rst and second parts, the higher negative correlations
Acting is not reacting (Whiting, 1991). The notion for the ® rst part with mean speed suggested that adapta-
of perception± action coupling questions traditional tions were taking place from trial to trial. Evidence
assumptions that the performance of skilled batsmen in for intra-trial variability was also provided in two parti-
cricket is limited by simple reaction times of around cipants in their study. Smaller movement variability
200 ms, as discussed earlier (McLeod, 1987). Bootsma (estimated by the coeý cient of variation) was found
and Van Wieringen (1990) criticized previous research during the middle to late components of their inter-
postulating visual-motor delays of around 200 ms as ceptive actions, not the earliest parts, suggesting that
arti® cial. They drew a crucial distinction between the these `subjects were still altering their movement during
role of visual information to initiate a movement, execution’ (Bootsma and Van Wieringen, 1990, p. 27).
typically required in laboratory reaction time studies, The visual-motor delays of 105± 122 ms observed in
and its regulatory role in adapting, steering and guiding four participants suggested that they were still able to
ongoing `real-world’ movements, such as cricket batting pick up movement-regulatory information at a relatively
(see also Carlton, 1981). Their ® ndings on dynamic late stage of performance.
interceptive actions in expert table-tennis players, These ® ndings provided a theoretical explanation
revealed visual-motor delays of 105± 122 ms when for the data of Hubbard and Seng (1954) on the ability
modifying a forehand drive. Another study of inter- of expert baseball batsmen to covary movement initi-
ceptive actions by Lee et al. (1983) found evidence of ation time in relation to ball speed. Furthermore, the
visual-motor delays of 55± 130 ms. The implication is results contradicted traditional information-processing
that periods of around 200 ms for visual information assumptions that expert batsmen rely on the known
processing cannot be justi® ed outside the laboratory, duration of an interceptive action as the basis for
particularly during natural actions when visual infor- timing (e.g. Tyldesley and Whiting, 1975; Abernethy,
mation was used primarily to adapt and modify on- 1981). Rather, it is apparent that an ongoing process
going movement. Moreover, these results are in line of perception± action coupling allows experts to update
with revised estimates from newer, more biological interceptive actions continuously, with concurrent
approaches to the study of cognitive processes, which perceptual information, until a very late stage of
are less `black-box-oriented’ than their predecessors. performance. These explanations call into question
For example, temporal constraints on the pick-up and previous interpretations of how skilled cricketers satisfy
comparison of perceptual information with `cortical the time constraints of batting against fast bowlers
memories’ have been modelled around 80± 100 ms, (e.g. Glencross and Cibich, 1977; Abernethy, 1981;
based on what neuroscientists now know about ® ring Abernethy and Russell, 1984). The shorter estimates
times for neurons and local cortical circuitry in the brain of visual-motor delay suggest that ongoing adaptations
(e.g. Ballard et al., 1998). may be feasible at a much later stage of performance
than previously believed.
A functional role for variability. A third proposal in eco- It is clear that, because of its theoretical emphasis on
logical psychology is that of compensatory variability, coupling of perception and action systems in dynamic
A review of batting in men’s cricket 939

interceptive actions, the ecological framework has The stance and backlift. Stretch et al. (1998a) reported
tended to emphasize kinematic measures, using bio- that the grip and stance were similar for the forward
mechanical techniques of analysis. This approach defensive stroke and front foot drive, with the feet twice
lends itself naturally to a close relationship between the the recommended distance apart (0.30± 0.35 m apart)
sub-disciplines of biomechanics and motor control in for both strokes (Greig, 1974; Reddick, 1979; Tyson,
experimental design. In the next section, we examine 1985; MCC, 1987; Khan, 1989). The front knee and
the ® ndings of biomechanical studies of cricket batting. hip were directly over the front foot, with the back knee
(0.05 m), hip (0.07 m) and shoulder (0.09 m) forward
of the back foot. The front shoulder was forward of
Biomechanics of batting the front foot (0.08 m) and above the back shoulder
(0.07 m); this resulted in the body’ s centre of mass
Several biomechanical studies have been conducted being displaced 0.08 m forward of the midpoint
on the swing of a cricket ball and the technique of fast between the feet, and towards the line of ¯ ight of the
bowling; these are well documented in a review by ball. This concurs with the recommendations of the
Bartlett et al. (1996). In contrast, only Davis (1983), coaching literature (e.g. MCC, 1987) and the ® ndings
Baker (1992), Elliott et al. (1993), Stretch (1993c) of Elliott et al. (1993) and supports the principle of
and Stretch et al. (1995, 1998a) have illustrated bio- weight distribution for an expected forward movement.
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

mechanical principles related to cricket batting and Stretch et al. (1998a) reported that the backlift for the
evaluated the eþ ectiveness of coaching points. The drive was 0.09 m higher than for the forward defensive,
research to date on the biomechanics of batting has been similar to recommendations in the coaching literature
essentially descriptive. For example, no research has (Greig, 1974; Reddick, 1979; Tyson, 1985; MCC, 1987;
yet evaluated how coaching style in¯ uences the kine- Khan, 1989) and to the ® ndings of Elliott et al. (1993).
matics of batting. The principle of distal-to-proximal This observation suggests that the batsman made an
sequencing has not been clearly addressed for cricket initial choice of stroke before the start of the downswing
batting. Perhaps most importantly, the role of com- of the bat (Stretch et al., 1998a). The pick up of the bat
pensatory variability (see previous section) in the skill of in the drive brought the front elbow ahead of the body
striking a moving cricket ball with a moving cricket bat and ¯ exed at an angle of 144° (FD = 142°), somewhat
has not received attention from biomechanists. Having less than the 121° and 124° for the oþ - and on-drives
identi® ed the kinematic and kinetic principles involved reported by Elliott et al. (1993). At no stage in the back-
in batting, as in the research reviewed below, coaches lift did Stretch et al. (1998a) ® nd that the bat was kept
should be aware of individual diþ erences in the tech- stationary at the top of the backlift, as suggested by
niques, as skills can be performed in a variety of Gibson and Adams (1989); rather, the downswing of
ways. These limitations should be borne in mind when the stroke ¯ owed from the backswing in a continuous
reading the following sub-sections, in which we have movement.
sought to relate the mostly descriptive research ® ndings Gibson and Adams (1989) used a split-image ® lming
to biomechanical principles or to views on batting technique to examine the time-course of the batting
technique in the coaching literature. action of an international cricketer when facing a fast-
medium bowler or a ball projection machine set at
the same speed. When batting against the bowler, the
The kinematics of batting
batsman initiated his backswing at a consistent time
Davis (1983) provided insight into the technique of before ball release. The bat was lifted slowly, held at the
batting and questioned some of the established top of the backswing and then the downswing was
principles of coaching cricket skills; however, these performed while completing the front foot movement.
results were not obtained under well-controlled experi- When facing the bowling machine, the backlift began at
ments. For a group of 14 ® rst-class batsmen (two of widely varying times, with the front foot movement
whom went on to play Test cricket), Stretch (1993c) starting before the backswing was completed. The back-
and Stretch et al. (1998a) compared the kinematics of swing was held for a short time, the downswing was
the front foot drive (D) to those of the forward defensive faster and the front foot movement was completed
stroke (FD), which forms the basis of the drive (Greig, before the end of the downswing. The pause at the top
1974; Reddick, 1979; Tyson, 1985; MCC, 1987; Khan, of the backswing was used to make ® nal adjustments to
1989). Their ® ndings con® rmed several descriptions in the stroke-selection decision. The batsman thus initiates
the coaching literature (Greig, 1974; Reddick, 1979; the stroke in response to temporal cues, modifying
Tyson, 1985; Andrew, 1987; MCC, 1987; Khan, 1989) it later as a result of continued input of spatial and
and highlighted diþ erences between the two stroke temporal information. Gibson and Adams (1989)
patterns. recommended that batsmen should bat against bowling
940 Stretch et al.

machines for the learning of techniques and against back foot oþ the ground, also as recommended in the
bowlers for the development of stroke timing and coaching literature (MCC, 1987). The horizontal
modi® cations. forward displacement, measured from the front of the
boot (D = 0.10 m; FD = 0.05 m), of the toe of the back
The stride and downswing to impact. The diþ erences foot at the instant of contact diþ ered from the recom-
reported by Stretch et al. (1998a), between the kine- mendations of the MCC (1987). Whereas the MCC
matics of the stride and downswing of the bat for the two recommended that the toe should not drag forwards,
strokes, are as expected for successful execution of the moving the back foot forwards may, in fact, help to
two strokes and are generally supportive of the coaching transfer weight into the stroke.
literature. Both the instant at which batsmen began During the stride phase of the drive, no statistically
playing the drive and the start of the stride occurred signi® cant diþ erences were noted between the strokes
later than for the forward defensive; the front foot for the forward (D = 0.35 m; FD = 0.31 m) or down-
movement occurred 0.58 s pre-impact (FD = 0.64 s) ward (D = 0.12 m; FD = 0.09 m) displacement of the
and the downswing of the bat started 0.36 s pre-impact head (Stretch et al., 1998a). During the last 0.06 s before
(FD = 0.38 s). Delaying the forward movement of impact in the drive, the head moved 0.09 m downwards,
the foot and the downswing of the bat, when playing ensuring that the batsman’ s weight did not shift away
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

the attacking stroke, allows the batsman to assimilate from the point of bat± ball contact, which could have
additional ball ¯ ight information and, therefore, make resulted in the ball being hit upwards (Tyson, 1985).
® nal decisions about the execution of the stroke from The downswing of the bat to execute the drive began
this temporal and spatial information. This is supported with the movement of the hands, forearm and upper
by Glencross and Cibich (1977), Abernethy (1981, arm; this was followed by the movement of the bat
1982), Abernethy and Russell (1984) and Gibson and forwards and downwards (Stretch et al., 1998a). The
Adams (1989), who suggested that, only after the front upper arm then accelerated to reach a peak
batsman has made his initial general response, such as horizontal velocity 0.22 s before impact; the forearm
playing forward, can he make the ® nal adjustment to the accelerated until 0.20 s before impact, with the back
stroke. upper arm, forearm and the centre of mass of the bat
The front foot strides for the two strokes showed no accelerating into the stroke. The bat reached a peak
statistically signi® cant diþ erences. This lack of statistical horizontal velocity (11.8 ± 4.61 m ´ s- 1) at impact, after
signi® cance, and the limited number of batsmen in the which all segments decelerated. During the downswing
study, hinders the generality of the ® ndings of Stretch of the drive, the elbow ¯ exed to 113° at the instant the
et al. (1998a). Nevertheless, the results do allow some front foot was raised, after which it extended to 129° at
quanti® cation of batting technique and the drawing of impact (Stretch et al., 1998a). This ¯ exion is in line with
tentative comparisons with opinions in the coaching the coaching view that the downswing of the bat should
literature and with biomechanical principles. Although be carried out with the front elbow ¯ exed and with the
not statistically signi® cant, the stride for the drive bat moving down in a straight line, as close to the body
was shorter than the forward defensive (D = 0.68 m; as possible (Tyson, 1985). In cricket batting, the exten-
FD = 0.72 m), the front foot was raised higher (D = sion of the front elbow might result in `its [the bat’ s] arc
0.09 m; FD = 0.08 m) and the stride began 0.06 s later, ¯ attening’ (Tyson, 1985), leading to an increase in the
with the front foot placement occurring later (D = radius of the arm and bat segment, which would then
0.06 s pre-impact; FD = 0.14 s pre-impact). The longer increase the tangential velocity of the distal end of the
front foot movement time for the drive bene® ted the segment (Hay and Reid, 1988), resulting in an increase
summation of forces in the direction of the stroke, in the speed of the bat and, therefore, the post-impact
leading to a greater impact speed of the bat on the ball, ball speed.
unlike the forward defensive, where the batsman was
in a stationary position well before bat± ball impact. Impact. The aim of the drive is to hit the ball with
At the instant when the front foot was lifted oþ the suý cient force to score runs, while still maintaining
ground in moving forwards to play the drive, Stretch control of the ball (Stretch et al., 1998a). The success
et al. (1998a) reported that the front knee was ¯ exed of the stroke can be ascertained from the resultant
slightly to 173° (FD = 175°). The front knee angle then end-point linear speeds and the interaction between
reduced to 152° (FD = 159°) at front foot placement, bat and ball. Stretch et al. (1998a) reported that, in the
147° at impact (FD = 156°) and 150° during the follow- drive, which requires the ball to be hit with both power
through (FD = 149°). This front knee ¯ exion helped the and accuracy, the diþ erence between the horizontal
weight of the body to move forward, as suggested in velocity vectors of the bat (11.8 m ´ s- 1) and ball (20.5
the coaching literature (e.g. MCC, 1987). This weight m ´ s- 1) at impact (32.3 ± 5.06 m ´s- 1) was signi® cantly
transfer was further enhanced by raising the heel of the greater (P < 0.05) than for the forward defensive
A review of batting in men’s cricket 941

(24.2 ± 4.65 m ´s- 1). This variability is in part attri- limb, rotating at the shoulder, elbow and wrist joints,
butable to diþ erent ball release speeds, even though the resulted in the sequential peaking of the linear speeds
same bowler was used. The bat± ball impact speed for of the segment end-points (Stretch et al., 1998a). These
the study of Stretch et al. (1998a) is slightly lower than results agree with research in tennis and baseball, in
the values reported by Elliott et al. (1993) for the oþ - which greater racket and bat impact speeds were found
(36.4 m ´ s- 1) and on-drives (38.6 m ´ s- 1). The reasons from the striking limb moving as a multi-segmental set
for these discrepancies are not clear; however, it should of levers (Breen, 1967; Milburn, 1982; Shibayama and
be noted that both of the values from Elliott et al. (1993) Ebashi, 1983; Tyson, 1985; Hay and Reid, 1988; Elliott
lie within one standard deviation from the mean et al., 1989). In addition, the back elbow reached its
reported by Stretch et al. (1998a). peak linear speed at impact, supporting the coaching
The bat± ball impact was 0.20 ± 0.13 m behind the literature, which suggests that the bottom hand gives
front ankle for the drive (FD = 0.09 ± 0.17 m) and power to the stroke (MCC, 1987). The front elbow
0.04 ± 0.36 m (FD = 0.11 ± 0.25 m) behind the line of moved forwards and upwards in anticipation of the
the head (Stretch et al., 1998a). These ® ndings con¯ ict movement of the ball after bouncing on the pitch,
in part with both the coaching literature (Greig, thereby ensuring that the ball was played into the
1974; Reddick, 1979; Tyson, 1985; Andrew, 1987; ground (Stretch et al., 1998a).
MCC, 1987; Khan, 1989) and the ® ndings of Elliott
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

et al. (1993); the ball was not played close to the front Follow-through. The follow-through for the drive is
foot but, at impact, the line of the head was over the essential for the bat to reach optimal speed at impact.
ball. At impact, the angular displacement of the bat at In the study of Stretch (1993c), all batsmen followed-
77.8 ± 7.1° to the ground in the drive diþ ered signi® - through as suggested in the coaching literature (e.g.
cantly from that of the forward defensive (62.6 ± 6.5°). MCC, 1987). The post-impact bat horizontal velocity
These results of Stretch et al. (1998a) for the drive for the drive (11.3 ± 4.21 m ´s- 1) was signi® cantly greater
support the coaching literature, which recommends than that for the forward defensive stroke (2.73 ±
that bat± ball impact should occur `just before the bat 2.88 m ´ s- 1) where there is less of a follow-through, as it
reaches the perpendicular’ (Khan, 1989, p. 21), with the is a defensive stroke. During the follow-through of the
top of the bat ahead of its toe at impact (Tyson, 1985). drive, the bat retains 95% of its pre-impact speed and
This should enhance both ball speed, by maximizing requires time and distance to allow the body segments
the bat speed at impact, and control ± the impact in this to decelerate without interfering with the application of
position would not tend to raise the ball into the air. force to the ball.
The bat reached its peak horizontal velocity in the
drive (11.8 ± 4.61 m ´s- 1) 0.02 s before impact (Stretch
Kinetics of batting
et al., 1998a). This is in line with the results of research
in golf (Shibayama and Ebashi, 1983), softball (Messier Stretch (1993a) and Stretch et al. (1995, 1998a) studied
and Owen, 1984) and baseball (McIntyre and Pfautsch, the grip forces of the top and bottom hand while batting
1982). Stretch et al. (1998a) agreed with the suggestion on a turf pitch against a medium-fast bowler. Their
that the body segments slow down just before contact to main ® ndings supported the coaching literature, which
prepare for the force of impact. suggests that the top hand is the dominant hand in the
In the drive, the forward step of the front foot ± and drive and is reinforced by the bottom hand at impact
the sequence in which the segments reached their peak (Greig, 1974; Reddick, 1979; Tyson, 1985; Andrew,
linear speeds ± resulted in a peak horizontal velocity 1987; MCC, 1987; Khan, 1989). Similar grip force
for the bat 0.02 s pre-impact that produced much of patterns for the drive and forward defensive strokes
the post-impact ball speed (Stretch et al., 1998a). The were recorded throughout the initial part of the stroke,
¯ exion of the front knee in the drive lowered the centre with greater forces applied by the top and bottom hands
of mass. This increased the stability of the body (Hay just before impact in the drive.
and Reid, 1988), so that the stroke could be played Signi® cant diþ erences were found in the intra-
powerfully while still maintaining a correctly balanced individual means of the top and bottom grip forces
position. The back knee ¯ exed slightly to an angle when playing the two strokes, as well as for the intra-
of 145° when playing the drive. During the downswing individual means of the top hand (range = 87.5± 138 N)
of the bat, the back foot moved forwards 0.10 m and the bottom hand (range = 32.8± 103 N) when
(FD = 0.05 m) to ensure the forward movement of the playing the drive. This variability may relate both
body’ s centre of mass; this accords with the principle of to slight variations in the pace, line and length of the
summation of forces in the direction of the stroke delivery and to diþ erences between players’ styles. The
(Stretch et al., 1998a). initial grip pressure pattern for the drive was very similar
The pendulum movements of the front upper to that of the forward defensive, as the batsman is only
942 Stretch et al.

able to make his ® nal stroke selection based on ball arti® cial and turf pitches and spin and medium-paced
¯ ight information (Abernethy and Russell, 1984; bowlers were collected from only two batsmen, both
Gibson and Adams, 1989). Fairly constant diþ erences experienced elite cricketers. When playing the drive
were found between the grip force patterns of the top against medium-paced bowlers on an arti® cial pitch,
and bottom hands throughout the early part of the smaller forces were generated by the top hand than
drive. Diþ erences occurred at 0.10 s pre-impact as when playing on a turf pitch. In the drive, forces at
the top hand applied more force than the bottom hand impact of 195 N were recorded (FD = 93.6 N) with the
during the drive. peak force of 195 N being reached 0.02 s post-impact
During the drive, the grip force of the top hand (FD = 102 N at 0.04 s pre-impact). The force patterns
increased, with peak forces of 199 ± 41 N being reached for the bottom hand before and at impact were similar
0.02 s before impact, reducing to 158 ± 56 N at impact to those when batting on turf, while a relaxation and
and 126 ± 29 N at 0.02 s after impact. A similar pattern re-gripping of the bat during the follow-through was
occurred in the forward defensive stroke (Stretch et al., demonstrated when batting on the arti® cial pitch. The
1998a). The pattern is similar to that reported for the drive played on a turf pitch against a spin bowler
speed of the body segments in golf (Shibayama and demonstrated a peak force of 158 N, which occurred
Ebashi, 1983), softball (Messier and Owen, 1984) and 0.02 s post-impact for the top hand. A similar grip± force
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

baseball batting (McIntrye and Pfautsch, 1982). The pattern was demonstrated for the bottom hand (peak
upper limb segments reached their peak linear speeds force of 102 N at 0.02 s pre-impact), except that, after
before impact and were decelerating at impact. impact, a slight re-gripping of the bat occurred as the
The relaxation of the grip continued until 0.06 s post- necessary hand forces were generated to regain control
impact and was followed by a slight increase in grip of the bat and to control the inertia of the bat during the
force as the necessary hand forces were generated to follow-through. When playing the forward defensive,
regain control of the bat after impact and to control the the peak forces for the bottom hand (58.4 N) occurred
inertia of the bat (Stretch et al., 1998a). A more gradual 0.08 s after impact (Stretch, 1993c). These diþ erences
relaxing of the grip force followed, as the bat was swung may be attributable to the arti® cial pitch being faster
through to ® nish high in front of the batsman. However, than turf, with a more consistent and predictable
the top hand plays the dominant role in the stroke and bounce and with the ball not spinning or seaming as
needs to grip the bat ® rmly to control the stroke (Greig, much as on turf.
1974; Reddick, 1979; Andrew, 1987; MCC, 1987). When playing the drive against a spin bowler on an
The grip force patterns for the bottom hand during arti® cial surface, the top hand demonstrated a similar,
the drive were similar to the top hand, except the forces although smaller, grip± force pattern than when batting
were smaller, with a peak force of 91.8 ± 41.1 N reached on turf, with the exception that the peak forces were
0.02 s pre-impact, reducing to 86.2 ± 58.2 N at impact reached 0.04 s post-impact (FD peak force = 74.3 N
and 82.4 ± 28.6 N at 0.02 s post-impact (Stretch et al., at 0.06 s post impact). The grip± force pattern for the
1998a). A rapid relaxation of the bottom hand occurred bottom hand again showed a similar force pattern,
up to 0.05 s post-impact, followed by a slight increase as but with smaller forces than those for the top hand,
the necessary hand forces were generated to regain con- with a relaxation and then re-gripping post-impact to
trol of the bat after impact. This was then followed by a control the bat during the follow-through. In the
more gradual relaxing of the grip force as the bat was forward defensive, the bottom hand showed little
swung through to ® nish high in front of the head. change in grip forces throughout the stroke, with a force
Centripetal forces up to 320 N have been reported at impact of 30.1 N, reaching a peak force of 34.4 N
when a golf club is swung, eþ ectively increasing the at 0.06 s post-impact (Stretch, 1993c).
weight of the golf club about 160 times (Daish, 1972).
Similarly, the centripetal forces built up during the
downswing and the follow-through phases of the drive Injuries
in cricket batting may be quite large. Thus, although a
particularly strong grip is not likely to add power to the In addition to possessing good technical skills, the
shot, an eþ ective grip is necessary to sustain the centri- modern batsman needs to be very ® t, to avoid not only
petal forces generated in the execution of the attacking the `traditional’ direct injuries of being struck by the
strokes in cricket. Such a grip may lead to an increase in ball, but also indirect and overuse injuries as a result
the control of the bat, resulting in greater consistency of repetitive training for a sport that is becoming more
of stroke reproduction. explosive in nature. The various studies of injury
Stretch (1993c) reported diþ erences in the grip forces incidence in cricket, highlighted below, do not always
when batting on diþ erent surfaces and against diþ erent distinguish injuries to batsmen from those to other
paced bowlers, although results for the comparison of players.
A review of batting in men’s cricket 943

The risk of injuries to club cricket players has been hand (Jones and Tullo, 1986). A survey over 18 months
estimated to be 2.6 per 10,000 man hours played carried out at the Sussex Eye Hospital revealed ® ve
(Weightman and Browne, 1971; Temple, 1982). These minor eye injuries caused by playing cricket, which
® gures exclude injuries thought to be trivial, as well as accounted for 5.4% of all the eye injuries recorded in
many chronic overuse injuries. The rate of injury to sports within that period (Gregory, 1986).
® rst-class cricketers in Australia has been estimated Many cricket injuries have been found to be either
to be 1 per 30 man-hours played (Payne et al., 1987). recurring injuries from the previous season or injuries
Provincial cricketers (71.6%) were reported to be at a that recurred later in the season. Twenty percent of
greater risk of injury than schoolboy (49.0%) and club the injuries sustained on an international tour were
cricketers (28.4%; Stretch, 1993b). re-aggravated injuries from the previous season (Smith,
Deaths from cricket date back to the passing 1991), while 23.9% of the injuries to club and provincial
of Frederick Louis, the Prince of Wales and father of cricketers were reported to be recurrent injuries and
George III, who died hours after being struck on the 22.7% of the new injuries sustained were re-aggravated
head by a cricket ball (Temple, 1982). Blonstein (1966) again during that season (Stretch, 1993b). Of the
suggested that six deaths per year occurred in the UK injuries sustained by schoolboy cricketers, 29.8%
as a result of playing cricket. From the self-report involved a recurrence of an old injury, while 36.8% of
questionnaire responses of 183 (59%) of 308 cricketers the new injuries sustained recurred again during the
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

canvassed, Stretch (1993b) found that serious injuries same season. Injuries have been reported as occurring
are sustained more frequently during matches by club fairly regularly throughout the season, with a slight
and provincial cricketers (69.3%) than at practices increase during the early and latter part of the season
(26.1%). In schoolboy cricketers, 45.6% of the injuries (Stretch, 1989, 1993b). Overuse injuries have been
were sustained both in matches and at practices. In both reported to be more common towards the end of the
groups, the balance of the injuries were chronic injuries season (Corrigan, 1984), which would appear to be a
or injuries that occurred during training. result of both the concentration of cricket and players
Although bowling is the major cause of injuries at all not fully recovering from previous injuries.
standards of the game, batting accounted for 17.1% The upper limb accounted for 24.6% of the injuries
of the injuries to club and provincial cricketers, with in schoolboy cricketers, 25% of injuries in club
muscle pulls and impact injuries the most common cricketers (Temple, 1982), 32% in provincial cricketers
(Stretch, 1993b). In schoolboy cricketers, the batsmen (Stretch, 1989) and 34% in provincial and club
sustained 29.8% of the injuries, of which 17.5% were cricketers (Stretch, 1993b). Batting injuries consisted
impact injuries to the head and face. The incidence of primarily of fractures, dislocations and contusions of the
injury to the head, neck and face has been reported ® ngers, which were found to be the most vulnerable
as 17.5% in schoolboy cricketers, 9% in club and pro- site for injuries (Corrigan, 1984; Gregory, 1986; Jones
vincial players, 17.5± 20% in provincial players (Stretch, and Tullo, 1986; Stretch, 1989, 1993b).
1989) and 25% in club cricketers (Temple, 1982). The danger of a fracture, as a result of being struck
Most head injuries were a result of being struck by the by the ball while batting, to the distal third of the ulna,
ball while trying to hook, by the ball de¯ ecting oþ the rib fractures and soft tissue injuries, especially to the
top-edge of the bat on to the head while playing upper leg, abdomen and testicles, have been reported by
a horizontal-bat stroke, or as a result of being struck Corrigan (1984). The case of a splenic rupture, and
by the ball rearing oþ the pitch. These injuries were another of a young player requiring splenectomy for a
sustained when the batsmen were batting either without ruptured spleen after blunt trauma from a cricket ball,
a helmet or with a helmet with earpiece side-guards were reported by Du Toit and Rademan (1987).
only; they included concussions, a broken nose and Top- and middle-order batsmen have been found
cheekbones, and lacerations requiring stitches around to be more susceptible to impact injuries to the head,
the eyes, mouth and chin. phalanges, metacarpals and to lower arm injuries
Eye injuries in cricket were ® rst reported at the than lower-order batsmen (Stretch, 1989). Payne et al.
beginning of the century (Ogilvie, 1900), with later (1987) reported that bowlers sustained most impact
cases of chronic glaucoma secondary to trauma injuries while batting. Lower limb injuries were mainly
(D’ Ombrain, 1945) and ocular concussion (Littlewood, hamstring, quadriceps and calf muscle pulls as a
1982) having been reported. Four cases of severe eye result of running between the wickets (Weightman
injuries, including retina detachment and rupture of and Browne, 1971; Temple, 1982; Payne et al., 1987;
the globe, were documented in batsmen, accounting for Stretch, 1989).
9.0% of sport-related eye injuries. These resulted from Forward (1988) recorded all the indoor cricket
the ball de¯ ecting oþ the top-edge of the bat while hook- injuries that were reported at the Royal Perth Hospital
ing, and striking the eye on the side of the dominant over a 6-month period. He found that the 64 patients
944 Stretch et al.

treated were 19± 34 years old (50 males, 14 females). was further from the handle than that for the wooden
Fielding (72%) and batting (17%) resulted in the major bats, which was similar to the ® ndings of Wood
share of these injuries: no injuries were sustained while and Dawson (1977). The experimental centres of per-
bowling. The most common injuries were to the ® ngers cussion of the junior-sized wooden and aluminium
and thumb. Fifty percent of the injured players required bats were very similar. Both groups of aluminium
time oþ work, with 30% being oþ work for more than bats showed three to four times greater recoil impulses
1 week and 10% being hospitalized. and, on average, produced a greater rebound coeý cient
Coroneo (1985) recommended that ocular pro- than the wooden bats. Wooden bats demonstrated
tective devices should be worn when playing indoor better rebound characteristics than aluminium bats.
cricket after reporting four cases of ocular injuries in Elliott and Ackland (1982) contended that more
cricketers. From the many case reports and studies on research needs to be done into aluminium bats
the incidence of injuries to cricketers, the indications before they can become a viable replacement for
are that the major areas of concern to batsmen are wooden bats; at present, aluminium bats are rarely used
impact injuries to the head, face and ® ngers. Although in cricket.
cricket injuries have not reached serious proportions,
cognisance needs to be taken of these patterns so that
Batting gloves
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

the risks of injury can be reduced even further.


Stretch and Tyler (1995) compared the impact charac-
teristics and the eý ciency to absorb impact forces
of four types of cricket batting gloves using the drop
Equipment test to simulate four impact speeds: slow-medium, fast-
medium, fast and express. In this test, a weighted ball
Bats
was dropped vertically onto the surface of the batting
The centre of percussion is important in the design of glove with the vertical forces measured by sensors in
all sports equipment used to hit balls, including cricket a cricket bat handle. The structure and composition
bats. If the bat-to-ball impact occurs through the centre of the protective part of the gloves over the index and
of percussion, then no force is transmitted to the hands. middle ® ngers were diþ erent in all four makes of gloves.
The centre of percussion will alter with the position of Three of the pairs of gloves (G1, G2 and G3) were com-
the grip on the handle. This sensitivity is reduced if the posed of two protective components, whereas the fourth
centre of mass is closer to the centre of percussion; for (G4) had only one. In the gloves for which there were
example, by the build up of the mass of the cricket bat two protective components, the outer part consisted of
around its centre of percussion (Bartlett, 1997). It is, sponge-foam-polyurethane that was 10, 10 and 8 mm
perhaps, surprising, given the amount of research into thick for G1, G2 and G3, respectively. The density of
the positioning of the `sweet spot’ on tennis rackets, that this padding appeared to be less in G3 than in the
such little research has been reported on the optimal others. The main diþ erence, however, was that, in
design of cricket bats. There is little published informa- addition to the protection oþ ered by this, G3 had addi-
tion on how the position of the centre of percussion is tional protection in the form of a 3 mm thick ¯ at plastic
aþ ected by diþ erent weights or shapes of bat and little reinforcement over the outer part of the padding of the
research into other aspects of optimal bat design. index and middle ® ngers.
Modern wooden cricket bats have been assessed for The second protective layer of the gloves varied, with
their eþ ectiveness as implements to strike the ball by G2 and G4 made up of a similar component to their
Wood and Dawson (1977). The ® ve bats tested showed outer layers, 16 and 10 mm thick, respectively. Gloves
distinct diþ erences in hitting eþ ectiveness because of G1 and G3 were made up of a layer of tightly compacted
their physical properties. Consistent diþ erences were padding in each of the ® ngers. In G1 and G3, a thin
found between the theoretically obtained centres of sponge covering formed the innermost layer of pro-
percussion. The total impact time between bat and ball tection. The only glove with one protective layer, G4,
was 40± 70 ms, with the major shock dissipated in the diþ ered from the other three gloves in that this section
® rst 20± 30 ms after impact. A weak relationship was consisted of four layers. First, it had a thin plastic
found between the weight of the bat and the moments of reinforcement 1 mm thick, with a 2 mm foam covering
inertia. on the outer side, over each of the ® ngers. Secondly, in
Elliott and Ackland (1982) compared aluminium and each ® nger, the middle layer consisted of a 10 mm thick
wooden cricket bats and found that they had similar composition similar to the outer and middle layers in
dimensions, weight, static balance and resistance to G2. Finally, the innermost layer, which was 7 mm thick,
rotational motion. The location of the experimental was made from a component that appeared to be less
centres of percussion of the senior-sized aluminium bats dense than the previous layer.
A review of batting in men’s cricket 945

Signi® cant diþ erences between the force absorption of the protective parts of the pads, which are less
characteristics were found at all speeds except slow- rigid.
medium. At fast-medium, signi® cant diþ erences were These rebound characteristics of pads could be
found between G1 and G2. At fast speed, G2 and G4 either an advantage or a disadvantage when batting,
diþ ered signi® cantly, while at express speed, G4 diþ ered depending on the match; this is not to suggest that
signi® cantly from both G2 and G3, and G3 diþ ered batsmen select their pads according to rebound charac-
signi® cantly from G2. These diþ erences were a result teristics rather than for protection, comfort or freedom
of the diþ erences in the structure and composition of of movement. In Test cricket, where it is more common
this protective part of the gloves. Manufacturers are, for ® elders to ® eld close to the batsman, pads with a
however, in the diý cult position of having to balance large post-impact rebound distance of the ball could
the impact absorption characteristics of the gloves with result in the batsman being caught `bat± pad’ (the ball
the comfort required to wear them for long periods. de¯ ected from the bat onto the pads and then being
At the two fastest impact speeds, the gloves that caught by a ® elder). Wearing the more traditional
showed the greatest ability to absorb the impact forces pads would reduce this rebound distance, lessening the
of the ball (G3 and G4) were those with a thin plastic risk of dismissal in this manner. The converse could,
reinforcement placed in the outer part of the padding of however, apply in limited-overs cricket matches, where
the index and middle ® nger (G3) and separately over the ® elders are normally further from the batsman.
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

each of the ® ngers (G4). This design feature helped A batsman wearing pads with a greater post-impact
to dissipate the impact forces of the cricket ball over rebound distance might have an advantage by being
a greater area, thus reducing the impact forces on the able to score runs or leg byes from balls de¯ ecting oþ
® ngers. These diþ erences were a result of the structure the pads.
and composition of the protective part of the batting Manufacturers of batting pads are in the diý cult
glove, which diþ ered for all four makes of glove. From position of having to balance the impact absorption and
the results, it would appear that the batting gloves best rebound characteristics of the pads with the comfort
able to absorb forces at the fastest impact speeds were required to wear them for long periods. Cricket pad
those that had a thin plastic reinforcement over an extra manufacturers, aware that the batting pads behave
padded layer of the ® ngers, plus the normal padding on diþ erently under various impact conditions because of
the ® ngers. the structure and composition of the protective layers
of the pads, need to investigate further the impact
properties of the components or combinations of com-
Batting pads
ponents they use in their batting pads.
Hrysomalis (1996) reported how such factors as thick-
ness, construction, temperature and humidity aþ ect the
Ball and sightscreen colour
shock absorbency of cricket pads. Several of the pads
tested showed inadequate protection to repeated drops From the batsman’ s point of view, either an orange ball
on the knee roll, especially at higher temperatures and used against a black sightscreen or a white ball against
humidities. A high correlation was reported between a black sightscreen is better than the traditional red
peak deceleration and the thickness of both the pad knee ball against a white sightscreen (Martin-Jenkins, 1988).
roll and shin region. The reaction time for the former combination was
Stretch et al. (1998b) found a signi® cant diþ erence 414 ms, compared with 420 ms for a white ball against
between the impact kinetics of cricket batting pads at a black sightscreen and 456 ms for a red ball against
various impact speeds. These diþ erences were con- a white sightscreen. More recently, Langley et al. (1999)
sidered to be a result of the diþ erent structure and com- found that neither ball colour (red or white) nor
position of the protective part of the pads. Some makes illuminance (571, 1143 or 1714 lux) aþ ected the slip
of pads were better able to absorb the impact forces of catching performance of ® ve male ® rst-class cricketers.
the ball, giving the batsman greater protection, while More research of this kind is required to determine
others were better able to reduce the rebound distance scienti® cally the eþ ects of such changes to the game on
of the ball after impact. From their ® ndings, it would batting performance.
appear that the batting pads best able to absorb the
impact forces of a ball bowled at fast to express speeds
are manufactured from polyurethane. The traditional Psychological factors
batting pads that are in current use have a greater ability
to reduce the rebound distance of the ball after impact Traditionally, cricket players and administrators have
with the pads. This reduced ball± pad coeý cient of neglected the contribution that the sport psychologist,
restitution is a result of the structure and composition and current trends in sport psychology, can make in the
946 Stretch et al.

mental preparation of batsmen. The emphasis has vision has not been aþ ected by wearing a helmet with
largely been placed on the technical, physical and bars.
tactical components; recently, however, more research At the end of a programme examining the relation-
has been conducted into mental preparation. Batsmen ship of mental preparation and performance, club
and coaches are becoming more aware of the role that cricketers were able to determine personal objectives
sport psychology can play in the enhancement of with regard to control, commitment and mood analysis
performance (see, for example, Bull et al., 1992), (Shilbury, 1989). They were also able to isolate factors
although there is plenty of scope for further, good- such as anxiety, nervousness, lack of motivation
quality research into the various psychological aspects and poor concentration by gaining a greater under-
of batting. standing of themselves as cricketers and individuals.
No diþ erences between anxiety and the performance Only recently have attempts been made to identify
of skilled and semi-skilled cricket batsmen were found and develop the necessary mental skills required for
in tests when these batsmen were required to discrimi- competition by integrating the psychological with the
nate between various bowling deliveries from a video- technical and physical aspects of the game. We would
tape presentation (Morris et al., 1985). When required argue that, although some studies ± such as the one just
to respond by selecting the appropriate batting stroke, discussed ± have not distinguished between batsmen
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

the skilled batsmen recorded lower state anxiety scores, and other players, batsmen have the most to gain from
although no diþ erences in cue discrimination were research into the eþ ects of mental preparation on their
found between the groups. High speci® city in the causes performance.
and eþ ects of stress in batting have been reported
(Morris et al., 1985); these authors suggested that
practice in muscle relaxation might reduce stress in Conclusions and recommendations
batsmen with relatively high state anxiety. Dippenaar
and Potgieter (1986) found no signi® cant relationship The scienti® c research into aspects of batting in men’ s
for anxiety between or within ability groups in ® rst cricket has included the morphology and physiology
league batsmen and bowlers. They did, however, ® nd of cricket batsmen, motor skills, the biomechanics
signi® cant diþ erences between and within the groups of the various phases of batting strokes, injuries to
on measures of attentional and interpersonal style. batsmen and their association with cricket equipment,
When looking at the more pro® cient cricketers in each and psychological factors in batting. The research is
group, spin bowlers were less impulsive and had more patchy in coverage and many topics merit further
self-control than batsmen. Fast bowlers were found investigation. These topics include aspects of ® tness
to be more intellectually expressive than seam bowlers, speci® city, for example weight training based on
and more positively expressive and in¯ uenced to a electromyographic evidence, and psychological coping
greater extent by external stimuli than spin bowlers. skills.
Jones et al. (1988) assessed the aþ ect of anxiety on A high priority should be given to research into injury
psychomotor performance of 12 cricketers, 4 days, mechanisms, rather than simple injury statistics, and
1 day, 1 h and immediately before batting. Their results the role of cricket equipment design in injury preven-
showed no change in cognitive anxiety across the tion. A second priority is for multi- or inter-disciplinary
four test sessions, whereas somatic anxiety increased research, linking the biomechanics of batting to the
immediately before batting with a corresponding reduc- underlying motor control of the movements and the
tion in self-con® dence. Simple and discrimination eþ ect of environmental information. Biomechanical
reaction time did not alter, but the error percentage studies of the variability of the batsman’ s movements
was greater on the discrimination task immediately are needed, and these should be related to the com-
before batting, which the authors ascribed to increased pensatory variability proposal of ecological psychology.
somatic anxiety. Additional areas of research should focus on methods
Many batsmen have been under the impression that of performance enhancement through vision training
wearing a helmet when facing fast bowling helps to and the use of virtual reality. Clearly, there is also a need
reduce anxiety by reducing the possibility of serious for scienti® c research into batting in women’ s cricket,
head and facial injuries. Davids and Morgan (1988) which has been inadequately researched to date.
found that batsmen showed little change in per-
formance-related anxiety, heart rate and the ability
to track and play fast bowling when wearing a References
helmet with bars, a visorless helmet or no helmet at all.
These ® ndings imply that batsmen can now face fast Abernethy, B. (1981). Mechanisms of skill in cricket batting.
bowlers with con® dence in the knowledge that their Australian Journal of Sports Medicine, 13, 3± 10.
A review of batting in men’s cricket 947

Abernethy, B. (1982). Skill in cricket batting: Laboratory Carlton, L.G. (1981). Processing visual feedback information
and applied evidence. In Proceedings of the Kinesiological for movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Sciences Section of the VIIth Commonwealth Conference Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1019± 1030.
on Sports, Recreation and Dance (edited by M.L. Howell Christina, R.W. (1992). Unravelling the mystery of the
and B.D. Wilson), pp. 35± 40. Brisbane: University of response complexity eþ ect in skilled movements. Research
Queensland. Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 63, 218± 230.
Abernethy, B. (1987). Selective attention in fast ball sports. Coroneo, M.T. (1985). An eye for cricket ± ocular injuries in
I: General principles. Australian Journal of Science and indoor cricketers. Medical Journal of Australia, 142, 469± 471.
Medicine in Sport, 19, 7± 16. Corrigan, A.B. (1984). Cricket injuries. Australian Family
Abernethy, B. and Russell, D.G. (1984). Advance cue utilisa- Physician, 13, 558± 562.
tion by skilled cricket batsmen. Australian Journal of Science Daish, C.B. (1972). The Physics of B all Games. London:
and Medicine in Sport, 16, 2± 10. English Universities Press.
Alderson, G.J.K., Sully, D.J. and Sully, H.G. (1974). An Davids, K. and Bennett, S.J. (1998). The dynamical hypo-
operational analysis of a one-handed catching task using thesis: The role of biological constraints on cognition.
high speed photography. Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 636± 644.
217± 226. Davids, K. and Morgan, M. (1988). The eþ ect of helmet
American College of Sports Medicine (1990). Guidelines for design and bowling speed on indices of stress in cricket
Graded Exercise Testing and Prescription. Philadelphia, PA: batting. Ergonomics, 31, 1665± 1671.
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

Lea & Febiger. Davis, J.A., Brewer, J. and Atkin, D. (1994). A physiological
Andrew, K. (1987). Skills in Cricket. Marlborough: Crowood comparison of ® rst class male batsmen and bowlers. Journal
Press. of Sports Sciences, 12, 159.
Baker, J. (1992). A biomechanical analysis of the cricket Davis, K. (1983). Discovering biomechanical principles
oþ -drive and on-drive played under open and closed skill of batting in cricket. In Biomechanics VIII-B (edited by
conditions. Unpublished Master’ s thesis, University of H. Matsui and K. Kobayashi), pp. 915± 922. Champaign,
Western Australia, Nedlands. IL: Human Kinetics.
Ballard, D.H., Salgian, G., Rao, R. and McCallum, A. (1998). Davis, K. (1984). Training principles related to cricket. Sports
On the role of timing in brain computation. In Vision and Coach, 8(3), 23± 25.
Action (edited by L.R. Harris and M. Jenkin), pp. 82± 119. Dippenaar, G.J and Potgieter, J.R. (1986) Angs, Aandag-
New York: Cambridge University Press. en Interpersoonlike style en rieketprestasie. South African
Barber, P. (1988). Applied Cognitive Psychology: An Information Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and
Processing Framework. London: Methuen. Recreation, 9, 27± 31.
Barras, N. (1990). Looking while batting in cricket: What a D’ Ombrain, A. (1945). Traumatic monocular chronic
coach can tell batsmen. Sports Coach, 13(2), 3± 7. glaucoma. Transactions of the Ophthamological Society of
Bartlett, R.M. (1997). Introduction to Sports Biomechanics. Australia, 5, 116± 120.
London: E & FN Spon. Du Toit, D.F. and Rademan, F. (1987). Splenic rupture
Bartlett, R.M., Stockill, N.P., Elliott, B.C. and Burnett, caused by a cricket ball. South African Medical Journal, 71,
A.F. (1996). The biomechanics of fast bowling in 796.
men’ s cricket: A review. Journal of Sports Sciences, 14, Elliott, B.C. and Ackland, T.R. (1982). Physical and impact
403± 424. characteristics of aluminium and wooden cricket bats.
Bernstein, N.A. (1967). The Control and Regulation of Journal of Human Movement Studies, 8, 149± 157.
Movements. London: Pergamon Press. Elliott, B.C., Foster, D.H. and Gray, S. (1986). Bio-
Blonstein, J.L (1966). Medical aspects of amateur boxing. mechanical and physical factors in¯ uencing fast bowling.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 59, 64± 99. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18,
Bootsma, R.J. (1988). The Timing of Rapid Interceptive Actions. 16± 21.
Amsterdam: Free University Press. Elliott, B.C., Marsh, A.P. and Overheu, P. (1989). A
Bootsma, R.J. and Peper, C.E. (1992). Predictive visual biomechanical comparison of the multisegmental and single
information sources for the regulation of action with special unit topspin forehand drives in tennis. International Journal
emphasis on catching and hitting. In Vision and Motor of Sport Biomechanics, 5, 350± 364.
Control (edited by L. Proteau and D. Elliott), pp. 285± 314. Elliott, B.C., Baker, J. and Foster, D. (1993). The kinematics
Amsterdam: North-Holland. and kinetics of the oþ -drive and on-drive in cricket.
Bootsma, R. J. and Van Wieringen, P.W.C. (1990). Timing Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 25,
an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. Jour nal of 48± 54.
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Elliott, B.C., Burnett, A., Stockill, N.C. and Bartlett, R.M.
16, 21± 29. (1996). The fast bowler in cricket: A review. Sports, Exercise
Breen, J.L. (1967). What makes a good hitter? Journal of and Injury, 1, 201± 206.
Health, Physical Education and Recreation, 38, 36± 39. Fischman, M.G. (1984). Programming time as a function
Briggs, C. (1980). Ideas to us in warm-up for cricket. Sports of number of movement parts and changes in movement
Coach, 4(1), 19± 23. direction. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 405± 423.
Bull, S.J., Fleming, S. and Doust, J. (1992). Play Better Cricket. Fitts, P.M. and Posner, M.I. (1967). Human Performance.
Eastbourne: Sports Dynamics. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
948 Stretch et al.

Fletcher, J.G. (1955). Calories and cricket. Lancet, 1, Marteniuk, R.G. (1976). Information Processing in Motor Skills.
1165± 1166. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Forward, G.R. (1988). Indoor cricket injuries. Medical Journal Martin-Jenkins, C. (1988). Cricketing facts. Wisden Cricket
of Australia, 148, 560± 561. Monthly, July, p. 5.
Foster, D. (1978). Management ± how to get the best out of MCC (1987). The MCC Cricket Coaching Book. London:
your cricket team. Sports Coach, 2(4), 40± 47. Heinemann.
Foster, D. and Elliott, B. (1985). Fast bowling ± an impact McIntyre, D.R. and Pfautsch, E.W. (1982). A kinetic analysis
sport: A pro® le of D.K. Lillee. Sports Coach, 9(3), 3± 8. of the baseball swings involved in opposite-® eld and same-
Gibson, A.P. and Adams, R.D. (1989). Batting stroke timing ® eld hitting. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 53,
with a bowler and a bowling machine: A case study. 206± 213.
Australian Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 21, 3± 6. McLeod, P. (1987). Visual reaction time and high speed ball
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Percep- games. Perception, 16, 49± 59.
tion. Boston, MA: Houghton Miü in. McLeod, P. and Jenkins, S. (1991). Timing accuracy and
Glencross, D.J. and Cibich, B.J. (1977). A decision analysis of decision time in high-speed ball games. International Journal
games skills. Australian Journal of Sports Medicine, 9, 72± 75. of Sport Psychology, 22, 279± 295.
Gregory, P.T.S. (1986). Sussex Eye Hospital sports injuries. McLeod, P., McLaughlin, C. and Nimmo-Smith, I. (1986).
British Journal of Ophthalmology, 70, 748± 750. Information encapsulation and automaticity: Evidence
Greig, T. (1974). Greig on Cricket. London: Stanley Paul. from timed actions. In Attention and Performance (edited by
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

Harris, L.R. and Jenkin, M. (eds) (1998). Vision and Action. M.I. Posner and O. Marin), pp. 217± 230. Hillsdale, NJ:
New York: Cambridge University Press. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hay, J.G. and Reid, J.G. (1988). Anatomy, Mechanics and Messier, S.P. and Owen, M.G. (1984). Bat dynamics of female
Human Motion. Englewood Cliþ s, NJ: Prentice-Hall. fast pitch softball players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Henry, F.M. and Rogers, D.E. (1960). Increased response for Sport, 55, 141± 145.
complicated movements and a `memory drum’ theory of Michaels, C.F. and Beek, P. (1995). The state of ecological
neuromotor reaction. Research Quarterly, 31, 448± 458. psychology. Ecological Psychology, 7, 259± 278.
Hrysomalis, C. (1996). Shock absorption of cricket leg guards Michaels, C.F. and Oudejans, R.R.D. (1992). The optics of
and batting gloves. In Abstracts of the Australian Conference catching ¯ y balls: Zeroing out optical acceleration. Ecological
of Science and Medicine in Sport (edited by A.C.T. Bruce), Psychology, 4, 199± 222.
pp. 406± 407. Canberra, ACT: Sports Medicine Australia. Milburn, P.D. (1982). Summation of segmental velocities in
Hubbard, A.W. and Seng, S.N. (1954). Visual movements of the golf swing. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
batters. Research Quarterly of the American Association of 14, 60± 64.
Health and Physical Education, 25, 42± 57. Milner, D.A. (1998). Streams and consciousness: Visual
Jones, J.G., Cale, A. and Kerwin, D.G. (1988). Multi- awareness and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2,
dimensional competitive state anxiety and psychomotor 25± 30.
performance. Australian Journal of Science and Medicine Morris, P., Renshaw, I. and Murray, S. (1985). Stress and
in Sport, 20, 3± 7. batting in cricket. Carnegie Research Papers, 1(7), 4± 11.
Jones, N.P. and Tullo, A.B. (1986). Severe eye injuries in Ogilvie, F.M. (1900). On one of the results of concussion
cricket. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 20, 178± 179. injuries of the eye (`holes’ at the macula). Transactions of
Khan, I. (1989). Imran Khan’s Cricket Skills. Gra® cromo, the Ophthalmological Society of the UK, 20, 202± 229.
Spain: Hamlyn. Payne, W.R., Hoy, G., Laussen, S.P. and Carlson, J.S. (1987).
Langley, M., Scott, K., Davids, K. and Bennett, S. (1999). What research tells the cricket coach. Sports Coach,
The eþ ect of diþ erent coloured cricket balls and illumi- 10(4), 17± 22.
nance on the catching performance and timing of two- Peens, J. (1996). Kinantropometriese pro® el van Suid-
handed slip catching in ® rst class cricketers. In Abstracts of Afrikaanse klubkampioens-kapkrieketspelers. Unpublished
the 1st World Congress on Science and Medicine in Cricket Master’ s thesis, Potchefstroom Universiteit vir Christelike
(edited by R.M. Bartlett), p. 46. Sheý eld: Sport Science Hoer Onderwys, Potchefstroom, South Africa.
Research Institute. Poulton, E.C. (1957). On prediction in skilled movements.
Lee, D.N. (1980). Visuo-motor coordination in space± time. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 467± 478.
In Tutorials in Motor Behavior (edited by G.E. Stelmach Pyke, F.S., Crouch, G.C. and Davis, K.H. (1975). The testing
and J. Requin), pp. 281± 295. Amsterdam: North-Holland. and training of an international fast bowler ± Dennis Lillee.
Lee, D.N. and Young, D.S. (1985). Visual timing of intercep- British Journal of Sports Medicine, 4, 152± 154.
tive action. In Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision (edited Reddick, T. (1979). Play Cricket the Right Way. Cape Town:
by D. Ingle, M. Jeannerod and D.N. Lee), pp. 1± 30. Seven Seas Publications.
Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoþ . Regan, D. (1997). Visual factors in hitting and catching.
Lee, D.N., Young, D.S., Reddish, P.E., Lough, S. and Clayton, Journal of Sports Sciences, 15, 533± 558.
T.M.H. (1983). Visual timing in hitting an accelerating Regan, D., Gray, R., Portfors, C.V., Hamstra, S.J., Vincent,
ball. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35A, A., Hong, X.H., Kohly, R. and Beverley, K. (1998).
333± 346. Catching, hitting and avoidance collision. In Vision and
Littlewood, K.R. (1982). Blunt ocular trauma and hyphaema. Action (edited by L.R. Harris and M. Jenkin), pp. 181± 214.
Australian Journal of Ophthalmology, 10, 263± 266. New York: Cambridge University Press.
A review of batting in men’s cricket 949

Runeson, S. (1977). On the possibility of `smart’ perceptual Stretch, R.A., Buys, F., Du Toit, D.E. and Viljoen, G.
mechanisms. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 18, (1998a). Kinematics and kinetics of the drive oþ the
172± 179. front foot in cricket batting. Journal of Sports Sciences, 16,
Savelsbergh, G.J.P. and Bootsma, R.J. (1994). Perception± 711± 720.
action coupling in hitting and catching. International Journal Stretch, R.A., Du Toit, D.E., Pretorius, B. and Edwards, T.
of Sport Psychology, 25, 331± 343. (1998b). The force absorption and rebound characteristics
Shibayama, H. and Ebashi, H. (1983). Development of a of cricket batting pads at four impact velocities. In From
motor skill using the golf swing from the viewpoint of the Community Health to Elite Sport: Proceedings of the Third
regulation of muscle activity. In B iomechanics VIII-B (edited Annual Congress of the European College of Sport Science
by H. Matsui and K. Kobayashi), pp. 895± 902. Champaign, (edited by A.J. Sargeant and H. Siddons), p. 465. Liverpool:
IL: Human Kinetics. Centre for Health Care Development.
Shilbury, D. (1989). An analysis of ® elding patterns of an Temple, R. (1982). Cricket injuries: Fast pitches change
`A’ grade cricket team. Sports Coach, 13(4), 41± 44. the gentleman’ s sport. Physician and Sportsmedicine, 10,
Smith, C. (1991). Sports injuries encountered on a ® rst-class 186± 192.
international cricket tour. Sports Medicine, 6, 10± 15. Todd, J.T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects.
Solomon, H.Y. and Turvey, M.T. (1988). Haptically per- Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
ceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. Performance, 7, 795± 810.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Tomlin, C. and Popplewell, G. (1991). Fit for the Game:
Downloaded by [University of Miami] at 11:50 18 September 2013

Performance, 14, 404± 427. Cricket. London: Ward Lock.


Stretch, R.A. (1987). Anthropometric pro® le of ® rst-class Tresilian, J.R. (1995). Perceptual and cognitive processes in
cricketers. South African Journal for Research in Sport, time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion
Physical Education and Recreation, 10, 65± 75. and relative judgement tasks. Perception and Psychophysics,
Stretch, R.A. (1989). Injuries to South African cricketers 57, 231± 245.
playing at ® rst-class level. South African Journal of Sports Tyldesley, D. and Whiting, H.T.A. (1975). Operational timing.
Medicine, 4, 3± 20. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 1, 172± 177.
Stretch, R.A. (1990). Somatotype and body composition Tyson, F. (1985). The Cricket Coaching Manual. Melbourne:
changes in ® rst-class cricketers. Communication to the Nelson Publishers.
International Congress on Youth, Leisure and Physical von Hofsten, C. (1983). Catching skills in infancy. Journal of
Activity and Kinanthropometry IV, Brussels, Belgium, Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
21± 25 May. 9, 75± 85.
Stretch, R.A. (1993a). A biomechanical analysis of the Wang, Y. and Frost, B. (1992). Time to collision is signalled
double-handed grip forces. Journal of Sports Sciences, 11, in neurons in the nucleus rotundus of pigeons. Nature,
543± 558. 356, 236± 238.
Stretch R.A. (1993b). The incidence and nature of injuries in Weightman, D. and Browne, R.C. (1971). Injuries in eleven
® rst-league and provincial cricketers. South African Medical selected sports. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 1, 27.
Journal, 83, 339± 342. Whiting, H.T.A. (1969). Acquiring Ball Skill: A Psychological
Stretch, R.A. (1993c). A biomechanical analysis of batting in Interpretation . London: G. Bell.
cricket. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Whiting, H.T.A. (1991). Action is not reaction! International
Port Elizabeth, Port Elizabeth, South Africa. Journal of Sport Psychology, 22, 296± 303.
Stretch, R.A. and Buys, F. (1991). Physical ® tness pro® le of Whiting, H.T.A., Alderson, G.J.K. and Sanderson, F.H.
South African Universities cricketers. South African Journal (1973). Critical time intervals for viewing and individual
for Research in Sport, Physical Education and Recreation, 14, diþ erences in performance of a ball-catching task. Inter-
73± 80. national Journal of Sport Psychology, 4, 155± 156.
Stretch, R.A. and Tyler, J. (1995). Force absorption Williams, A.M., Davids, K., Burwitz, L. and Williams, J.G.
characteristics of cricket batting gloves at four impact (1992). Perception and action in sport. Journal of Human
velocities. South African Journal of Sports Medicine, 3, Movement Studies, 22, 147± 204.
22± 29. Williams, A.M., Davids, K. and Williams, J.G. (1999). Visual
Stretch, R.A., Buys, F.J. and Viljoen, G. (1995). The kinetics Perception and Action in Sport. London: Routledge.
of the drive oþ the front foot in cricket batting: Hand grip Wood, G.A. and Dawson, M. (1977). Physical properties of
forces. South African Journal for Research in Sport, Physical cricket bats and their implications for hitting. Sports Coach,
Education and Recreation, 18, 83± 93. 1(3), 28± 34.

You might also like